ArticlePDF Available

Reactions to ideal body shapes

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Difficult-to-attain beauty standards that are promulgated by the media could contribute to body dissatisfaction, but their potential impact upon body image remains unclear. The present study examined reactions to thin-ideal and muscular-ideal images, and examined the effects of ideal image exposure on preferred body shape and time spent deliberating about ideal shape. In a 2 × 2 experimental design, 200 Internet users completed the Kessler psychological distress scale (K10) and were randomly assigned to view idealistic body images or houses (and rated them on semantic differential dimensions). Females viewed thin ideal images and males viewed muscular ideal images, while the control groups viewed images of houses. Between group measures analysis of variance revealed women reacted negatively to thinspirational images. Women were more driven for thinness and idealized a thinner shape for the female body than men. Psychologically distressed participants had a stronger drive for thinness and greater body dissatisfaction compared to low-distress participants. In addition, although beauty ideal imagery had no significant impact on males, females spent more time in choosing ideal body figure. Viewing thin bodies for females created a trend toward desiring a slimmer figure, as well as increased preoccupation with size as they spent more time deliberating over a photorealistic figure rating scale. Therefore, exposure to thinspiration promotes dissatisfaction with self and leads women to dwell more upon their body image.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=vgen20
The Journal of General Psychology
ISSN: 0022-1309 (Print) 1940-0888 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vgen20
Reactions to ideal body shapes
Katarina Prnjak, Stephanie Pemberton, Eric Helms & James G. Phillips
To cite this article: Katarina Prnjak, Stephanie Pemberton, Eric Helms & James G.
Phillips (2019): Reactions to ideal body shapes, The Journal of General Psychology, DOI:
10.1080/00221309.2019.1676190
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2019.1676190
Published online: 12 Oct 2019.
Submit your article to this journal
View related articles
View Crossmark data
Reactions to ideal body shapes
Katarina Prnjak
a
, Stephanie Pemberton
b
, Eric Helms
b
, and
James G. Phillips
b
a
Western Sydney University;
b
Auckland University of Technology
ABSTRACT
Difficult-to-attain beauty standards that are promulgated by
the media could contribute to body dissatisfaction, but their
potential impact upon body image remains unclear. The pre-
sent study examined reactions to thin-ideal and muscular-ideal
images, and examined the effects of ideal image exposure on
preferred body shape and time spent deliberating about ideal
shape. In a 2 2 experimental design, 200 Internet users com-
pleted the Kessler psychological distress scale (K10) and were
randomly assigned to view idealistic body images or houses
(and rated them on semantic differential dimensions). Females
viewed thin ideal images and males viewed muscular ideal
images, while the control groups viewed images of houses.
Between group measures analysis of variance revealed women
reacted negatively to thinspirational images. Women were
more driven for thinness and idealized a thinner shape for the
female body than men. Psychologically distressed participants
had a stronger drive for thinness and greater body dissatisfac-
tion compared to low-distress participants. In addition,
although beauty ideal imagery had no significant impact on
males, females spent more time in choosing ideal body figure.
Viewing thin bodies for females created a trend toward desir-
ing a slimmer figure, as well as increased preoccupation with
size as they spent more time deliberating over a photorealistic
figure rating scale. Therefore, exposure to thinspiration pro-
motes dissatisfaction with self and leads women to dwell
more upon their body image.
ARTICLE HISTORY
Received 12 June 2019
Accepted 28 September 2019
KEYWORDS
Body image; thinness;
beauty; bodybuilding;
attentional bias
Body image can influence quality of life (Nayir et al., 2016) and can be
negatively influenced by distress and cultural ideals (Campbell &
Hausenblas, 2009). Modern western culture values excessive thinness as an
ideal for females (Lawler & Nixon, 2011), but these ideals are largely
unrealistic as only 2% of women in the United States have the weight
required to meet these modeling inspired criteria (Strahan, Wilson,
Cressman, & Buote, 2006). The pressure to meet these standardscan
harm self-esteem and body image (Polivy & Herman, 2002) and also cause
CONTACT Katarina Prnjak katarinaprnjak@gmail.com Translational Health Research Institute (THRI),
School of Medicine, Western Sydney University, Sydney, Australia.
Color versions of one or more of the figures in the article can be found online at www.tandfonline.com/vgen.
ß2019 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY
https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2019.1676190
feelings of rejection (Blodorn, Major, Hunger, & Miller, 2016; Ravary,
Baldwin, & Bartz, 2019), helplessness, inadequacy, and anxiety (Halliwell &
Dittmar, 2004). These hard-to-achieve beauty standards depicted by mass
media can lead to the internalization of such ideals posing a strong risk
for the development of eating disorders (Piran, 2015). Although clinical
eating disorders affect a small segment of the population, nonclinical risk-
related symptoms such as body image dissatisfaction are widespread,
with around 50% of girls and young women being affected (Ferguson,
Mu~
noz, Garza, & Galindo, 2014). In addition, many males also experience
body image dissatisfaction as a consequence of drive for muscularity
(Murray et al., 2016).
The media created a preoccupation with body shape (Ravary et al.,
2019). The rise of thin-ideal and bodybuilding imagery has been observed
on social media platforms (Fardouly & Vartanian, 2016), whose usage
seems to be associated with negative body image (Holland & Tiggemann,
2016). Specifically, time spent online highly correlated with the internaliza-
tion of the thin ideal, body surveillance, and drive for thinness (Tiggemann
& Slater, 2013). Lawler and Nixon (2011) defined this internalization as
the extent to which an individual cognitively buys into socially defined
ideals of attractiveness, incorporates these standards into ones personal
belief system and commits to behaviors intended to produce an approxima-
tion of these ideals(p. 3). It may be that the internalization of beauty
ideals is the crucial link between media and body discontent (Lawler &
Nixon, 2011). Furthermore, individuals who experience eating disorder
symptoms commonly use social media to motivate and perpetuate their
harmful thoughts and behavior (Borzekowski, Schenk, Wilson, & Peebles,
2010). Pro-eating disorder websites were also found to have an impact on
body image dissatisfaction, dieting and negative affect (Rodgers, Lowy,
Halperin, & Franko, 2016), probably due to sharing thinspiration posts
containing images or quotes intended to inspire weight loss. Similarly, there
is an increasing number of pro-muscularity websites with bodybuilding
content that may encourage some unhealthy eating and exercising behav-
iors and thus provide a greater risk for the development of male eating dis-
orders (Murray et al., 2016). In view of such observations, it seems the
Internet can increase risks of developing eating concerns and negative body
image by providing constant easy access to images and information related
to beauty ideals.
It is well known that body shape ideals differ for men and women, with
the sociocultural influences presenting muscular bodies as ideal for men
and thin bodies as ideal for women (Knauss, Paxton, & Alsaker, 2007). In
recent years, males have experienced growing sociocultural pressure as
male beauty ideals appeared increasingly in media, encouraging less fat and
2 K. PRNJAK ET AL.
more muscle (Knauss et al., 2007). Also, images depicting this muscular
ideal have been found to adversely affect males who internalize the meso-
morphic ideal (Blond, 2008). Furthermore, many studies (Cohn & Adler,
1992; Fallon & Rozin, 1985; Phelps et al., 1993; Rozin & Fallon, 1988) have
found that men and women differ in their opinions as to the ideal body
figure preferred by the opposite gender. Women believe that men prefer a
thinner ideal female figure than the figures men actually prefer, reflecting
common beauty ideals that are difficult to attain (Bergstrom, Neighbors, &
Lewis, 2004). Likewise, men believe that women prefer males to have larger
bodies than those that women actually prefer (Fallon & Rozin, 1985). All in
all, both genders typically prefer opposite sex figures that more closely
represent the average healthy body shape.
Psychological distress is argued to play a key role in both the develop-
ment of mental health problems and the ability to cope with these condi-
tions (Deasy, Coughlan, Pironom, Jourdan, & Mannix-McNamara, 2014).
Further, there is an established relationship between body image, distress
(Blodorn et al., 2016) and eating disorders (Corstophine, Mountford,
Tomlinson, Waller, & Meyer, 2007; Hay & Williams, 2013). More distress
may indicate greater susceptibility to mass media (Corcoran, Crusius, &
Mussweiler, 2011) and accessing social media websites, such as Facebook,
which in turn has been associated with greater distress (Chen & Lee, 2013).
Given the role distress plays in mental health and well-being, as well as its
association with social media usage and eating disorders, distress is an
important aspect to investigate when researching behavior that may lead to
eating disorder onset.
Tiggemann and Zaccardo (2015) conducted an experiment involving a
large group of female participants who either viewed a set of fitspiration
Instagram images (oriented toward fit instead of thin appearance) or looked
at traveling images. Those exposed to fitspiration images reported greater
body dissatisfaction, lower mood and decreased self-esteem around their
appearance. Thus, given the widespread and frequent use of social media
websites, it is important to understand whether exposure to thinspirational
and bodybuilding material is contributing to eating disorder risk (Knauss
et al., 2007).
Study overview
The current study sought to replicate and extend Tiggemann and
Zaccardos(2015) research, but we also included males to investigate the
impact of bodybuilding Instagram posts. The present study aimed to
address the following research question does exposure of males and
females to the images of difficult-to-accomplish beauty ideals lead to body
THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY 3
image issues, namely a dissatisfaction with self-image, a preference for and
preoccupation with more idealistic shapes?
Negative body image may be perpetuated and reinforced by an atten-
tional bias, defined as the tendency to selectively attend to disorder-relevant
stimuli (Smeets, Roefs, van Furth, & Jansen, 2008). This is relevant as body
objectification is a common feature of eating disorders and typically
involves isolating and evaluating individual body parts (Ghaznavi & Taylor,
2015). Since elucidating the role of attentional bias could help to identify
risk and maintenance factors in body dissatisfaction (Rodgers & Dubois,
2016), the present study also monitored the attention and deliberation
processes while selecting the ideal body shape. Longer times deliberating
may either be due to greater susceptibility to mass media or due to a social
comparison process triggered by acute stress from viewing difficult-to-
attain beauty ideals (Corcoran et al., 2011). We hypothesized that exposure
to thin-ideal and muscular-ideal imagery would cause both female and
male participants to spend more time in deliberation and consequently
choose a body shape similar to the ideal images depicted.
Materials and methods
Participants
This study was advertised from New Zealand, but the online sample was
collected from an internationally available public online noticeboard
(Reddit). Participants were recreational Internet users who partook in the
online survey, which was advertised and presented in English language.
The total sample for the present study was 200 respondents (74 males and
126 females) with a mean age of 24.10 years (SD ¼7.48). No incentives
were offered for participation. The study was approved by Auckland
University of Technology Ethics Committee (AUTEC). With this sample
size, a sensitivity analysis using Gpower indicated an ability to detect effects
with 80% power at 0.05 level of significance.
Measures
Stimulus material
Three sets of stimulus materials were compiled for the study, each contain-
ing 19 images originally sourced from publicly available Instagram profiles.
The thinspiration image set was made up of 19 pictures of females with
thin-ideal bodies from the thinspirationhashtag. The bodybuilding image
set was made up of 19 pictures of males with muscular-ideal bodies from
the bodybuildinghashtag. The house image set was made up of 19 pic-
tures of various external shots of houses from the househashtag. Images
4 K. PRNJAK ET AL.
of houses without people on it were chosen as neutral stimuli for the con-
trol group as they would not provide ideal bodies for viewers to compare
themselves to. Initial sets of 40 images per category were rated by a panel
of two women and one man, all recreational internet users in their 20 s.
Quality and visual appeal of images were rated on a 5-point Likert scale
(from 1 very poor, to 5 excellent). This was done to ensure the final
sets of images did not vary in perceived quality or visual appeal. The final
sets of images were similar for perceived quality, (F(2,54)¼0.734, p¼.485)
(thinspiration M¼3.65, SD ¼0.63; bodybuilding M¼3.47, SD ¼0.46;
houses M¼3.47, SD ¼0.48), and similar for visual appeal (F(2,54)¼0.380,
p¼.686) (thinspiration M¼3.63, SD ¼0.69; bodybuilding M¼3.49,
SD ¼0.24; houses M¼3.54, SD ¼0.47).
To ensure that participants attended to the images and evaluated them,
participants were required to rate images using semantic differential scales
(Osgood, Suci, & Tannenbaum, 1957). Participants had to rate each image
on 5-point scales separately for strong-weak(potency), active-passive
(activity), and good-bad(evaluation). Participants that did not rate these
images were not included in the data set.
Psychological distress
The Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K10) was used to determine a
persons level of general distress (Kessler et al., 2003). This brief 10-item
scale effectively measures distress and the severity of general psychological
symptoms across the 28 days prior to the questionnaire (e.g. In the past
4 weeks, about how often did you feel nervous?). Answers are rated along
a15 scale, from none of the timeto all of the time. Higher scores
indicate greater distress. Andrews and Slade (2001) obtained a correlation
of 0.30 between the K10 and frequency of consultations for a mental health
problem. The K10 has been found to correlate .37 with an eating disorder
inventory (Hay & Williams, 2013) and patients with eating disorder symp-
toms have significantly higher scores on the K10 (Mitchison, Dawson,
Hand, Mond, & Hay, 2016). Andrews and Slade (2001) described cutoff
scores according to normative data from the Australian national survey.
They found scores under 20 were likely to be well, while scores 2024,
2529 and 30 or over indicate a mild, moderate or severe mental disorder
respectively. In addition, good psychometric measures of K10 in Australian
population were reported by Slade, Grove, and Burgess (2011). For the cur-
rent study, under 20 was used as the cutoff to indicate not distressed par-
ticipants (denoting well), while 20 or over indicated potential distress. In
the present sample, the K10 obtained a Cronbachs alpha of .91, which
indicates a high level of internal consistency.
THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY 5
Drive for thinness
Drive for Thinness subscale of the Eating Disorder Inventory (Garner,
Olmstead, & Polivy, 1983) was used to assess eating disorder pathology,
risk and desire to be thin (e.g. I am terrified of gaining weight). This sub-
scale consists of seven items, with answers ranging from always(1) to
never(6). Higher scores indicate a greater eating disorder risk
(Tiggemann & Pickering, 1996). This scale has been used as a robust meas-
ure in a range of eating disorder research (Tiggemann & Pickering, 1996;
Wiederman & Pryor, 2000). In the current sample, this scale obtained a
good overall Cronbachs alpha of .79.
Body image dissatisfaction
Female participants were directed to the Body Dissatisfaction subscale of
the Eating Disorder Inventory (Garner et al., 1983), a 9-item measure that
reliably assesses participant satisfaction with a range of body part shapes
and size (e.g. I think that my stomach is too big). Participants answered
on a scale from always(1) to never(6), and higher average scores indi-
cate greater body dissatisfaction. In this sample, the questionnaire obtained
a good Cronbachs alpha of .88.
The Male Body Attitudes Scale (Tylka, Bergeron, & Schwartz, 2005) was
used to assess body dissatisfaction in male participants (e.g. I wish my
arms were stronger). In 24 items, on a 16 scale from alwaysto never,
male participants provided their answers for dissatisfaction with muscular-
ity, height, and body fat. Higher average scores indicate greater body dissat-
isfaction. In the current study, this questionnaire obtained a good
Cronbachs alpha of .82.
Outline figure rating scale
The Stunkard outline figure rating scale (Mutale, Dunn, Stiller, & Larkin,
2016) consists of images depicting a row of line-drawn bodies, ranging
from very underweight (1) through to very overweight (9). It is a method
to evaluate body image, beauty ideals and body dissatisfaction in males and
females. Participants selected a figure that represented their preferred figure
and also selected the figure they preferred for the opposite sex.
Photorealistic figure rating scale
Photorealistic figure rating scales were sourced from Harris, Bradlyn,
Coffman, Gunel, and Cottrell (2008), who developed their scale to more
realistically depict a range of body weights for body image research. The
scale was constructed using pictures of adults with known BMI scores to
create male and female figure rating scales. There were 10 standardized
6 K. PRNJAK ET AL.
figures, each with a composite face that ranged from underweight to class
III obesity. By administering this to a large sample of adults, Harris et al.
(2008) found the scale had strong psychometric properties. In the present
sample, the Photorealistic figure rating scale correlated significantly
(r¼.769, n¼81, p<.001) with the Stunkard Figure Outline rating scale
(Mutale et al., 2016).
Procedure
Participants reported their age and gender in order to characterize the sam-
ple. Participants then answered the K10 to determine how distressed they
were, and indicated their preferred body shape on a Stunkard outline figure
rating scale. Participants were then randomly allocated to one of two
experimental conditions, either viewing house images (control group) or
ideal body images (experimental group). To randomly allocate participants
into a condition, a question asked whether the day of their date of birth
falls on an even numbered or odd numbered day. If the day they were
born was an even number, the questionnaire branched, and they viewed
the neutral stimuli (images of houses). If the day they were born was an
odd number, the questionnaire branched, and they viewed material
designed to influence body image: either thinspirational content if they
were female, or bodybuilding content if they were male.
Female participants then responded to the Drive for Thinness scale and
the Body Dissatisfaction Subscale. Male participants completed the Drive
for Thinness scale and the Male Body Attitudes Scale. Finally, participants
were presented with photorealistic figure rating scales and asked for their
ideal body type, and then, the body they would prefer for the opposite sex.
For photorealistic rating scales, Qualtrics recorded the location of the cur-
sor when the mouse was clicked, and the time spent on that web page
before clicking the mouse. Qualtrics produced a response density distribu-
tion (heat map) that indicates where more clicks had occurred. The data
collected from these measures were imported into SPSS version 24. All
measures, manipulations and exclusions in the study are reported.
Results
Semantic differential
Participants were asked to evaluate images along dimensions employed by
the Semantic Differential (Osgood et al., 1957). As a manipulation check, a
222 Gender by Idealistic Images by Distress between group measures
ANOVA was used to examine how the rated images were viewed by partic-
ipants. As the Semantic Differential has three orthogonal dimensions, an
THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY 7
analysis of Potency, Activity, and Evaluative ratings were conducted separ-
ately. As will be shown below, the semantic differential ratings indicated
that images of body builders were viewed as strong and active by males.
Females viewed thinspirational images as more active, but weaker than
images of houses. Females evaluated thinspirational images more negatively
than houses, as shown on Figure 1.
Potency
The assumption of the homogeneity of variance was not violated according
to the Levenes test (F(7,192) ¼1.644, p¼.125). There was a significant
effect of Idealistic Images on ratings of potency (F(1,192) ¼16.337,
p<.001, g
p
2
¼.08). Idealistic images (M¼3.63) were rated as being stron-
ger than images of houses (M¼3.22). There was a significant Gender by
Idealistic Images interaction (F(1,192) ¼55.063, p<.001, g
p2
¼.22). Simple
main effects indicated that images of body builders were rated as being
strongerthan images of houses by males (F(1,192) ¼59.248, p<.05), but
females viewed thinspirational images as being weaker than houses
(F(1,192) ¼8.235, p<.05).
Activity
For ratings of activity the assumption of the homogeneity of variance was
not violated according to the Levenes test (F(7,192) ¼.728, p¼.649). There
was a significant effect of Idealistic Images on ratings of activity
(F(1,192) ¼56.278, p<.001, g
p
2
¼.23). Idealistic images (M¼3.70) were
rated more active than images of houses (M¼2.83). There was a significant
Gender by Idealistic Images interaction (F(1,192) ¼24.598, p<.001,
g
p
2
¼.11). Simple main effects indicated that images of body builders were
rated as being more active than images of houses by males
(F(1,192) ¼69.930, p<.05). Females also saw thinspirational images as
being more active than images of houses (F(1,192) ¼4.648, p<.05).
Figure 1. Mean ±standard errors of ratings of body-ideal and control images by male and
female participants on Potency, Activity and Evaluation dimensions.
8 K. PRNJAK ET AL.
Evaluative
The assumption of the homogeneity of variance for evaluative ratings was
not violated according to the Levenes test (F(7,192) ¼.545, p¼.800). There
was a significant effect of Idealistic Images on evaluative ratings
(F(1,192) ¼5.083, p¼.025, g
p
2
¼.03). Idealistic body images (M¼3.46)
were rated less positively than houses (M¼3.73). The significant Gender
by Idealistic Images interaction (F(1192) ¼5.782, p¼.017, g
p
2
¼.03) was
decomposed using simple main effects. Females rated thinspirational
images less positively than houses (F(1,192) ¼4.251, p<.05), but this effect
was much less noticeable for males, who rated idealistic images of body
builders comparably to houses (F(1,192) ¼0.011, p>.05).
Distress and body dissatisfaction
As Drive for Thinness was not homogenous according to a Levenes test
(F(3,194) ¼14.688, p<.001) it was square root transformed, whereupon
the assumption of homogeneity was met (F(3,194) ¼1.925, p¼.127). But
given that the effects were the same for the transformed data as for the
untransformed data, we report the untransformed data for purposes of
interpretability.
A two-way analysis of variance was conducted upon the transformed
data to determine any differences in Drive for Thinness between genders
or levels of distress. Although the numbers of women and men in two lev-
els of distress differed (N¼57 of nondistressed women; N¼48 of nondis-
tressed men; N¼69 of distressed women; N¼26 of distressed men), the
Levens test of variance homogeneity was not statistically significant,
F(1,140) ¼2.164, p¼.144. The 2 2 between group ANOVA showed sig-
nificant main effects of gender, F(1,194) ¼16.394, p<.001, g
p
2
¼.08, and
distress, F(1,194) ¼23.231, p<.001, g
p
2
¼.11, but not an interaction,
F(1,194) ¼1.857, p¼.175, g
p
2
¼.01. Females (M¼7.30) and more distressed
participants (M¼7.39) had greater drives for thinness than males
(M¼3.60) and nondistressed participants (M¼3.27).
As body dissatisfaction was evaluated with gender specific instruments,
the effects of distress on body dissatisfaction were examined separately for
males and females. Independent t-tests considered the effect of distress level
on female body dissatisfaction. Females who were more distressed
(M¼36.54) had significantly higher scores on the Body Dissatisfaction
Scale than less-distressed (M¼30.05) female participants, t(123) ¼
3.867, p<.001.
Males completed the Male Body Attitude Scale. Boxs M indicated that
covariance matrices of distressed and non-distressed males did not differ
significantly (BoxsM¼4.311, F(6,15705.869) ¼0.680, p¼.666). For the
THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY 9
males, oneway multivariate analysis of variance showed that those who
were more distressed were significantly more concerned about their body
muscularity, height, and body fat, K¼.763, F(3,68) ¼7.052, p<.001,
g
p
2
¼.24. Univariate tests indicated that more distressed males specifically
wanted to be more muscular (F(1,70) ¼16.374, p<.001, g
p
2
¼.19), taller
(F(1,70) ¼7.506, p¼.008, g
p
2
¼.24), and have less body fat (F(1,70) ¼
14.634, p<.001, g
p
2
¼.17).
Photorealistic figure ratings
To determine the impact of distress level and exposure to idealistic body
images on preferred photorealistic figure for oneself, a 2 (gender) 2 (dis-
tress) 2 (image type) factorial ANOVA was conducted. For the preferred
photorealistic figure for oneself, the results of Levenes test did not meet
the assumption of the homogeneity of variances (F(7,192) ¼2.501,
p¼.018), hence this variable was subjected to a square root transform,
whereupon Levenes test indicated it was now homogenous
(F(7,192) ¼1.228, p¼.289). As the pattern of findings was the same for
transformed and untransformed data, for interpretability we report the
findings based on untransformed numbers.
There were significant main effects of gender, F(1,192) ¼95.691, p<.001,
g
p
2
¼.33, and distress, F(1,192) ¼8.680, p¼.004, g
p
2
¼.04, upon photoreal-
istic figure preferences. Females selected thinner images for themselves (i.e.
pixels closer to screen origin on the left of the heatmap) (M ¼105.4) than
males (M ¼196.1). Distressed individuals (M ¼137.1) wanted to be thinner
than the less distressed individuals (M ¼164.4).
The effect of image type approached significance, F(1,192) ¼3.804,
p¼.053, g
p
2
¼.02. Although only a trend, participants that were exposed to
idealistic body images tended to select a slimmer figure as ideal
(M ¼141.7), when compared to those who viewed images of houses
Figure 2. Heat map over the photorealistic figure rating scale depicting where the most mouse
clicks have occurred for females picking the ideal female body and males picking the ideal
male body (top control group; bottom experimental group).
10 K. PRNJAK ET AL.
(M ¼159.8). This was more evident in the heatmaps of female participants
than male participants, as shown on Figure 2. No significant interactions
were observed between exposure to idealistic body images and gender,
F(1,192) ¼1.455, p ¼.229, g
p
2
¼.01, distress and gender, F(1,192) ¼0.630,
p¼.428, g
p
2
¼.00, nor between distress and exposure to idealistic body
images, F(1,192) ¼1.878, p¼.172, g
p
2
¼.01.
To examine preoccupation, a 2 22 factorial ANOVA was conducted
to investigate the effects of gender, distress, and image type on the time
spent deliberating on the photorealistic figure rating scale. The time spent
deliberating was homogenous according to a Levenestest(F(7,192) ¼0.948,
p¼.471). The main effect of distress was not significant, F(1,192) ¼0.473,
p¼.492, g
p
2
¼.00. However, there were significant main effects of gender,
F(1,192) ¼5.977, p¼.015, g
p
2
¼.03, and image type, F(1,192) ¼4.698,
p¼.031, g
p
2
¼.02. Females and participants exposed to idealistic body
images spent more time contemplating their preferred shape on the photo-
realistic rating scales than males and those exposed to images of houses,
respectively. There was a significant gender by exposure interaction,
F(1,192) ¼7.043, p¼.009, g
p
2
¼.04. The interaction is shown in Figure 3
and decomposed using simple main effects. Females exposed to beauty
ideals took longer deliberating than females who viewed houses,
F(1,192) ¼16.800, p<.01, while male deliberation upon exposure to images
of body builders was not significantly different from male deliberation after
exposure to house images, F(1,192) ¼0.107, p>.05.
Female versus male figure preferences
To demonstrate that concerns about body shape can be unrealistic, inde-
pendent t-tests were used to compare femalespreferred body shape on the
Figure 3. Mean ± standard error of time spent deliberating on the photorealistic figure rating
scale according to gender and experimental manipulation.
THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY 11
photorealistic scale with the body shape preferred by males. Females pre-
ferred (M ¼99.3) a significantly slimmer figure for themselves than the
males preferred for women (M ¼152.5), t(198) ¼5.801, p<.001. And males
preferred a significantly larger (M ¼198.6) figure for themselves than the
figure that females preferred for men (M ¼167.2), t(198) ¼3.252, p¼.0013
(Table 1).
Discussion
This study considered reactions to images portraying thin-ideal and muscu-
lar-ideals, and sought to understand the effects of ideal images upon pre-
ferred shapes and any preoccupation with shape in men and women. Males
viewed images of body builders as strong and active according to Semantic
Differential ratings. By contrast females viewed thinspirational images as
active, but thinspirational images were rated as weaker and less positively
evaluated than the neutral images of houses. Moreover, psychologically dis-
tressed participants appeared more susceptible to idealistic physical stand-
ards and showed greater drive for thinness. In particular, distressed females
were more dissatisfied with their body and distressed males were dissatis-
fied with their muscularity, height, and body fat. Females had a higher
drive for thinness and spent more time contemplating their preferred shape
after being exposed to thinspiration images. In addition, exposure to ideal-
istic standards of appearance tended to cause a shift toward desiring slim-
mer figures and significantly increased the time females spent
contemplating body size. These self-imposed expectations of body image
are unrealistic because female participants chose a slimmer figure as an
ideal than the figure males actually preferred for women. And male partici-
pants selected larger ideal figures than the figures females actually preferred
for men.
Body shape and distress
As predicted, females in the present study had a greater drive for thinness
and reported more body dissatisfaction. This finding is in accord with
sociocultural claims that females experience greater pressure to meet beauty
Table 1. Means (SE) in Pixels for Preferred Figures in Female and Male Participants
(Photorealistic Outline Figures).
Females (n¼126) Males (n¼74)
Preferred figure oneself 99.35 (5.01) 198.64 (8.00)
Preferred figure opposite sex 167.23 (5.72) 152.47 (8.37)
Preferred figure after exposure to house images 114.09 (9.827)
(n¼47)
203.82 (11.013)
(n¼34)
Preferred figure after exposure to idealistic images 90.58 (5.264)
(n¼79)
194.23 (11.555)
(n¼40)
12 K. PRNJAK ET AL.
standards (Knauss et al., 2007; Ravary et al., 2019). More generally, while
some female distress may be accounted for by anticipated rejection
(Blodorn et al., 2016) and inability meeting ideal body standards, male dis-
tress is less likely to be explained by appearance expectations (Lawler &
Nixon, 2011; Strahan, Wilson, Cressman, & Buote, 2006). Taking sociocul-
tural perspectives into account, male distress can be associated with mascu-
linity constructs that require that men should restrict emotions and attain
wealth and social status (Wester, Christianson, Vogel, & Wei, 2007). This
may be further reinforced by the fact that for males, resources are benefi-
cial in attracting a long-term partner (Schmitt, 2014; Waynforth, 2001).
Nevertheless, in the current study, distressed males were more concerned
that they should be taller, more muscular, and have less body fat.
High-distressed participants preferred slimmer figures than low-dis-
tressed participants in the present study. A potential explanation of such
findings may be that distressed people make more social comparisons
(Corcoran et al., 2011), and those individuals more driven to make social
comparisons are more prone to exposing themselves to beauty ideals in
media, and subsequently experience body dissatisfaction and drive for thin-
ness (Cohen, 2006). This suggests an unfortunate cycle in which distressed
individuals may expose themselves to idealistic body presentations, causing
them to feel worse about themselves.
Indeed, the attainment of these ideal shapes is unrealistic. The current
study replicated reports that womens ideal female body was thinner than
mens ideal female body. These findings are in keeping with other studies
(Cohn & Adler, 1992; Fallon & Rozin, 1985; Knauss et al., 2007; Phelps
et al., 1993; Rozin & Fallon, 1988). This suggests that the opposite sex pre-
fers more attainable body shapes for potential mates than individuals have
for themselves. In addition, misperceptions of the ideal bodies seem to be
associated with unhealthy eating attitudes in women (Bergstrom,
Neighbors, & Lewis, 2004), which is why it is important to recognize that
pattern when examining ideal body preferences and eating dis-
order correlates.
Figure preference
The photorealistic figure rating scales indicated that female participants
preferred a slimmer figure than males, and there was a tendency for partic-
ipants that were exposed to idealistic images to prefer even slimmer figures.
This suggests that acute exposure to beauty ideals may have some impact
on participants. However, the effect of ideal body imagery was less discern-
ible for male participants, who spent comparable amounts of time selecting
preferred shape after viewing houses or bodybuilding imagery. Moreover,
THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY 13
as opposed to males, it is widely known that females are more susceptible
to exposure to media influences (Rodgers, McLean, & Paxton, 2015) and
recognized as experiencing greater pressure to meet beauty ideals (Cohen,
2006). Strahan et al. (2006) observed that, while females are usually pre-
sented with a narrow definition of what a beautiful woman looks like
young, tall, and thin males are presented with beauty-ideal cultural
norms that are more relaxed and malleable. Across mass media, many dif-
ferent kinds of males with varying physical attributes are depicted as
worthyand attractive to females. This means that, while the ideal body
for males is a mesomorphic build, they may still feel accepted and attractive
with a body that doesnt fit in that category.
Longitudinal research by Rodgers et al. (2015) found that beauty ideal
internalization occurs before social comparisons and that both factors con-
tribute to body dissatisfaction. People are more likely to make social com-
parisons within areas of importance or relevance to them (Rodgers et al.,
2015), and females have been experimentally shown to make more social
comparisons about appearance than males do (Strahan et al., 2006). This
may mean that males are less likely to make appearance-related social com-
parisons, and more likely to make social comparisons based on masculinity
constructs such as wealth and status, as this is more salient to their self-
esteem (Wester et al., 2007). Although the males in the present study
appeared unaffected by bodybuilding content, experimental research has
shown other samples of males to be influenced by ideal bodies in media,
and this effect is more pronounced with younger males (Blond, 2008;
Hargreaves & Tiggemann, 2004; Tylka, 2011). Additionally, many online
communities exist promoting pro-muscularity, with cognitively and physic-
ally demanding advice to obtain larger muscular bodies (Murray et al.,
2016). It may be that younger, distressed males seek out these websites as
they are more driven to make social comparisons, and they may subse-
quently internalize and idealize the mesomorphic shape. Nevertheless, fur-
ther research is needed to establish this relationship.
Preoccupation
Thinspirational images were potentially distressing to women, being eval-
uated less positively than the neutral images of houses. Viewing thinspira-
tion led females to spend more time deliberating over their preferred
shape. Therefore, thinspiration created a preoccupation in females as they
took a significantly longer time deliberating than those that viewed houses.
When this finding is taken together with that of females displaying a trend
toward desiring a slimmer figure after viewing thin body images, we can
infer that body dissatisfaction could have been experimentally induced.
14 K. PRNJAK ET AL.
Likewise, Cho and Lee (2013) showed that women and men who are less
satisfied with their bodies will pay more attention to figures they find
attractive. Also, Rodgers and DuBois (2016) conducted a recent systematic
review of biases in body dissatisfaction research and found that attentional
biases and body dissatisfaction can be manipulated. They further concluded
that the research shows strong support for attentional biases among indi-
viduals with greater body dissatisfaction toward appearance and body
image related stimuli. Results of the present study further support previ-
ously mentioned findings and suggest that acute exposure to thinspiration
can induce body dissatisfaction and increase the salience of body-image
stimuli in a way that encourages allocation of more attention to these stim-
uli. This only affected female participants, furthering theories that beauty
ideal exposure can lead to greater motivation towards social comparison in
those who are distressed and uncertain (Corcoran et al., 2011).
The present study also demonstrates the real potential to monitor online
activity and intervene (Engel et al., 2016). Given that preoccupation with
weight, shape, and appearance is a significant predictor of disorder onset
(Rodgers & DuBois, 2016), the present study demonstrates how the time
spent on pro-ana or thinspirational websites (Borzekowski et al., 2010)
could be monitored and targeted advertising (Boerman, Kruikemeier, &
Zuiderveen Borgesius, 2017) could deliver appropriately framed and
worded disclaimers (Smith & Rieger, 2006; Thaler & Sunstein, 2017)or
messages of affirmation (Logel, Kathmandu, & Cohen, 2019). Women
should be aware that imagery on Instagram might not be realistic (Webber,
2000) and that it is not unusual for social media users to post only the best
pictures of their appearance (Vogel, Rose, Roberts, & Eckles, 2014).
Additionally, since high-distressed participants showed a tendency toward
desiring a thinner body after viewing idealistic beauty imagery, those who
experience a state of psychological distress may need to limit their time on
social media (Krasnova, Widjaja, Buxmann, Wenninger, & Benbasat, 2015)
in order to prevent potential body dissatisfaction and drive for thinness.
Some limitations of the current study should be noted. Firstly, although
age and gender were recorded, further demographic information such as
educational attainment could have been useful. Secondly, the instruments
used to measure the desired body size might not have been sensitive
enough to detect the differences between experimental and control group,
which could explain the absence of significant effects of thin-ideal and
muscular-ideal exposure on preferred shape. Alongside this, the photoreal-
istic figure rating scale for men did not provide a shape that would repre-
sent the muscular-ideal, as the scale was based on BMI instead. In
addition, research have shown a limited evidence of the K10 validity in a
culturally diverse populations (Stolk, Kaplan, & Szwarc, 2014). Hence, due
THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY 15
to the advertisement and recruitment of participants via internationally
available website, our findings could potentially be limited by the fact that
culturally diverse individuals had access to online survey used for
data collection.
Conclusions
Exposure to idealistic body imagery had a significant effect on females.
Idealistic body images were evaluated less positively by women. Females
desired slimmer figures for themselves and spent longer contemplating
their preferred body size. Bodybuilding images did not influence male fig-
ure preference or time spent choosing preferred shape. However, partici-
pants (both male and female) that were more distressed also experienced
more concerns regarding physical appearance. It seems that exposure to
beauty ideals in media can lead to greater motivation towards social com-
parison in those who are distressed, and might consequently increase their
body dissatisfaction and drive for thinness.
Disclosure statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.
ORCID
Katarina Prnjak http://orcid.org/0000-0002-6152-5025
References
Andrews, G., & Slade, T. (2001). Interpreting scores on the Kessler psychological distress
scale (K10). Australian and New Zealand Journal of Public Health,25(6), 494497. doi:
10.1111/j.1467-842X.2001.tb00310.x
Bergstrom, R. L., Neighbors, C., & Lewis, M. A. (2004). Do men find bonywomen
attractive?: Consequences of misperceiving opposite sex perceptions of attractive body
image. Body Image,1(2), 183191. doi:10.1016/S1740-1445(03)00025-1
Blodorn, A., Major, B., Hunger, J., & Miller, C. (2016). Unpacking the psychological weight
of weight stigma: A rejection-expectation pathway. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology,63,6976. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2015.12.003
Blond, A. (2008). Impacts of exposure to images of ideal bodies on male body dissatisfac-
tion: A review. Body Image,5(3), 244250. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2008.02.003
Boerman, S. C., Kruikemeier, S., & Zuiderveen Borgesius, F. J. (2017). Online behavioral
advertising: A literature review and research agenda. Journal of Advertising,46(3),
363376. doi:10.1080/00913367.2017.1339368
Borzekowski, D. L., Schenk, S., Wilson, J. L., & Peebles, R. (2010). e-Ana and e-Mia: A con-
tent analysis of proeating disorder web sites. American Journal of Public Health,100(8),
15261534. doi:10.2105/AJPH.2009.172700
16 K. PRNJAK ET AL.
Campbell, A., & Hausenblas, H. A. (2009). Effects of exercise interventions on body image:
A meta-analysis. Journal of Health Psychology,14(6), 780793. doi:10.1177/
1359105309338977
Chen, W., & Lee, K. H. (2013). Sharing, liking, commenting, and distressed? The pathway
between Facebook interaction and psychological distress. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and
Social Networking,16(10), 728734. doi:10.1089/cyber.2012.0272
Cho, A., & Lee, J. H. (2013). Body dissatisfaction levels and gender differences in atten-
tional biases toward idealized bodies. Body Image,10(1), 95102. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.
2012.09.005
Cohen, S. B. (2006). Media exposure and the subsequent effects on body dissatisfaction,
disordered eating, and drive for thinness: A review of the current research. Mind
Matters: The Wesleyan Journal of Psychology,1,5771. Retrieved from https://pdfs.
semanticscholar.org/7fe6/1e24a8f9b69e548b054a9b2aded28a9f976f.pdf
Cohn, L. D., & Adler, N. E. (1992). Female and male perceptions of ideal body shapes:
Distorted views among Caucasian college students. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
16(1), 6979. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1992.tb00240.x
Corcoran, K., Crusius, J., & Mussweiler, T. (2011). Social comparison: Motives, standards,
and mechanisms. In D. Chadee (Ed.), Theories in social psychology (pp. 119139).
Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.
Corstophine, E., Mountford, V., Tomlinson, S., Waller, G., & Meyer, C. (2007). Distress tol-
erance in the eating disorders. Eating Behaviors,8,9197. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2006.02.
003
Deasy, C., Coughlan, B., Pironom, J., Jourdan, D., & Mannix-McNamara, P. (2014).
Psychological distress and coping amongst higher education students: A mixed method
enquiry. PLoS One,9(12), e115193. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0115193
Engel, S. G., Crosby, R. D., Thomas, G., Bond, D., Lavender, J. M., Mason, T.,
Wonderlich, S. A. (2016). Ecological momentary assessment in eating disorder and obes-
ity research: A review of the recent literature. Current Psychiatry Reports,18(4), 37. doi:
10.1007/s11920-016-0672-7
Fallon, A. E., & Rozin, P. (1985). Sex differences in perceptions of desirable body shape.
Journal of Abnormal Psychology,94(1), 102105. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.94.1.102
Fardouly, J., & Vartanian, L. R. (2016). Social media and body image concerns: Current
research and future directions. Current Opinion in Psychology,9,15. doi:10.1016/j.cop-
syc.2015.09.005
Ferguson, C. J., Mu~
noz, M. E., Garza, A., & Galindo, M. (2014). Concurrent and prospect-
ive analyses of peer, television and social media influences on body dissatisfaction, eating
disorder symptoms and life satisfaction in adolescent girls. Journal of Youth and
Adolescence,43(1), 114. doi:10.1007/s10964-012-9898-9
Garner, D. M., Olmstead, M. P., & Polivy, J. (1983). Development and validation of a
multidimensional eating disorder inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia.
International Journal of Eating Disorders,2(2), 1534. doi:10.1002/1098-108X(198321)
2:2<15::AID-EAT2260020203>3.0.CO;2-6
Ghaznavi, J., & Taylor, L. D. (2015). Bones, body parts, and sex appeal: An analysis of
#thinspiration images on popular social media. Body Image,14,5461. doi:10.1016/j.
bodyim.2015.03.006
Halliwell, E., & Dittmar, H. (2004). Does size matter? The impact of models body size on
womens body-focused anxiety and advertising effectiveness. Journal of Social and
Clinical Psychology,23(1), 104122. doi:10.1521/jscp.23.1.104.26989
THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY 17
Hargreaves, D. A., & Tiggemann, M. (2004). Idealized media images and adolescent body
image: Comparingboys and girls. Body Image,1(4), 351361. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.
2004.10.002
Harris, C. V., Bradlyn, A. S., Coffman, J., Gunel, E., & Cottrell, L. (2008). BMI-based body
size guides for women and men: Development and validation of a novel pictorial method
to assess weight-related concepts. International Journal of Obesity,32(2), 336342. doi:10.
1038/sj.ijo.0803704
Hay, P., & Williams, S. E. (2013). Exploring relationships over time between psychological
distress, perceived stress, life events and immature defense style on disordered eating
pathology. BMC Psychology,1(1), 2734. doi:10.1186/2050-7283-1-27
Holland, G., & Tiggemann, M. (2016). A systematic review of the impact of the use of
social networking sites on body image and disordered eating outcomes. Body Image,17,
100110. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.02.008
Kessler, R. C., Barker, P. R., Colpe, L. J., Epstein, J. F., Gfroerer, J. C., Hiripi, E.,
Zaslavsky, A. M. (2003). Screening for serious mental illness in the general population.
Archives of General Psychiatry,60(2), 184189. doi:10.1002/mpr.310
Knauss, C., Paxton, S. J., & Alsaker, F. D. (2007). Relationships amongst body dissatisfac-
tion, internalisation of the media body ideal and perceived pressure from media in ado-
lescent girls and boys. Body Image,4(4), 353360. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2007.06.007
Krasnova, H., Widjaja, T., Buxmann, P., Wenninger, H., & Benbasat, I. (2015). Research
notewhy following friends can hurt you: An exploratory investigation of the effects of
envy on social networking sites among college-age users. Information Systems Research,
26(3), 585605. doi:10.1287/isre.2015.0588
Lawler, M., & Nixon, E. (2011). Body dissatisfaction among adolescent boys and girls: The
effects of body mass, peer appearance culture and internalization of appearance ideals.
Journal of Youth and Adolescence,40(1), 5971. doi:10.1007/s10964-009-9500-2
Logel, C., Kathmandu, A., & Cohen, G. L. (2019). Affirmation prevents long-term weight
gain. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,81,7075. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2018.07.005
Mitchison, D., Dawson, L., Hand, L., Mond, J., & Hay, P. (2016). Quality of life as a vul-
nerability and recovery factor in eating disorders: A community-based study. BMC
Psychiatry,16(1), 328. doi:10.1186/s12888-016-1033-0
Murray, S. B., Griffiths, S., Hazery, L., Shen, T., Wooldridge, T., & Mond, J. M. (2016). Go
big or go home: A thematic content analysis of pro-muscularity websites. Body Image,
16,1720. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.10.002
Mutale, G. J., Dunn, A. K., Stiller, J., & Larkin, R. (2016). Development of a body dissatis-
faction scale assessment tool. The New School Psychology Bulletin,13(2), 4757.
Retrieved from http://irep.ntu.ac.uk/id/eprint/28326
Nayir, T., Uskun, E., Y
urekli, M. V., Devran, H., C¸elik, A., & Okyay, R. A. (2016). Does
body image affect quality of life? A population based study. PLoS One,11(9), e0163290.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0163290
Osgood, C., Suci, G., & Tannenbaum, P. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL:
University of Illinois.
Phelps, L., Johnston, L. S., Jimenez, D. P., Wilczenski, F. L., Andrea, R. K., & Healy, R. W.
(1993). Figure preference, body dissatisfaction, and body distortion in adolescence.
Journal of Adolescent Research,8(3), 297310. doi:10.1177/074355489383005
Piran, N. (2015). New possibilities in the prevention of eating disorders: The introduction
of positive body image measures. Body Image,14, 146157. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.
03.008
18 K. PRNJAK ET AL.
Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (2002). Causes of eating disorders. Annual Review of
Psychology,53(1), 187213. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135103
Ravary, A., Baldwin, M. W., & Bartz, J. A. (2019). Shaping the body politic: Mass media
fat-shaming affects implicit anti-fat attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
45(11), 1580. doi:10.1177/0146167219838550
Rodgers, R. F., & Dubois, R. H. (2016). Cognitive biases to appearance-related stimuli in
body dissatisfaction: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review,46,111.
Rodgers, R. F., Lowy, A. S., Halperin, D. M., & Franko, D. L. (2016). A meta-analysis
examining the influence of pro-eating disorder websites on body image and eating path-
ology. European Eating Disorders Review,24(1), 38. doi:10.1002/erv.2390
Rodgers, R. F., McLean, S. A., & Paxton, S. J. (2015). Longitudinal relationships among
internalization of the media ideal, peer social comparison, and body dissatisfaction:
Implications for the tripartite influence model. Developmental Psychology,51(5), 706. doi:
10.1037/dev0000013
Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. (1988). Body image, attitudes to weight, and misperceptions of fig-
ure preferences of the opposite sex: A comparison of men and women in two genera-
tions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,97(3), 342345. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.97.3.342
Schmitt, D. P. (2014). Evaluating evidence of mate preference adaptations: How do we
really know what homo sapiens sapiens really want? In V. Weekes-Shackelford & T.K.
Shackelford (Eds.), Evolutionary perspectives on human sexual psychology and behavior
(pp. 339). New York, NY: Springer.
Slade, T., Grove, R., & Burgess, P. (2011). Kessler psychological distress scale: Normative
data from the 2007 Australian National Survey of Mental Health and Wellbeing.
Australian & New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,45(4), 308316. doi:10.3109/00048674.
2010.543653
Smeets, E., Roefs, A., van Furth, E., & Jansen, A. (2008). Attentional bias for body and
food in eating disorders: Increased distraction, speeded detection, or both? Behaviour
Research and Therapy,46(2), 229238. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2007.12.003
Smith, E., & Rieger, E. (2006). The effect of attentional bias toward shape-and weight-
related information on body dissatisfaction. International Journal of Eating Disorders,
39(6), 509515. doi:10.1002/eat.20291
Stolk, Y., Kaplan, I., & Szwarc, J. (2014). Clinical use of the Kessler psychological distress
scales with culturally diverse groups. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric
Research,23(2), 161183. doi:10.1002/mpr.1426
Strahan, E. J., Wilson, A. E., Cressman, K. E., & Buote, V. M. (2006). Comparing to perfec-
tion: How cultural norms for appearance affect social comparisons and self- image. Body
Image,3(3), 211227. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.07.004
Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2017). Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth
and happiness. New York, NY: Penguin Putnam.
Tiggemann, M., & Pickering, A. S. (1996). Role of television in adolescent womens body
dissatisfaction and drive for thinness. International Journal of Eating Disorders,20(2),
199203. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199609)20:2<199::AID-EAT11>3.0.CO;2-Z
Tiggemann, M., & Slater, A. (2013). NetGirls: The Internet, Facebook, and body image con-
cern in adolescent girls. International Journal of Eating Disorders,46(6), 630633. doi:10.
1002/eat.22141
Tiggemann, M., & Zaccardo, M. (2015). Exercise to be fit, not skinny: The effect of fitspi-
ration imagery on womens body image. Body Image,15,6167. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.
2015.06.003
THE JOURNAL OF GENERAL PSYCHOLOGY 19
Tylka, T. L. (2011). Refinement of the tripartite influence model for men: Dual body image
pathways to body change behaviors. Body Image,8(3), 199207. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.
2011.04.008
Tylka, T. L., Bergeron, D., & Schwartz, J. P. (2005). Development and psychometric evalu-
ation of the Male Body Attitudes Scale (MBAS). Body Image,2(2), 161175. doi:10.1016/
j.bodyim.2005.03.001
Vogel, E. A., Rose, J. P., Roberts, L. R., & Eckles, K. (2014). Social comparison, social
media, and self-esteem. Psychology of Popular Media Culture,3(4), 206222. doi:10.1037/
ppm0000047
Waynforth, D. (2001). Mate choice trade-offs and womens preference for physically
attractive men. Human Nature,12(3), 207219. doi:10.1007/s12110-001-1007-9
Webber, J. (2000). The ethics/skills interface in image manipulation. Australasian Journal of
Information Systems,7(2), 8994. doi:10.3127/ajis.v7i2.265
Wester, S. R., Christianson, H. F., Vogel, D. L., & Wei, M. (2007). Gender role conflict and
psychological distress: The role of social support. Psychology of Men and Masculinity,
8(4), 215224. doi:10.1037/1524-9220.8.4.215
Wiederman, M. W., & Pryor, T. L. (2000). Body dissatisfaction, bulimia, and depression
among women: The mediating role of drive for thinness. International Journal of Eating
Disorders,27(1), 9095. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(200001)27:1<90::AID-EAT10>3.0.
CO;2-0
20 K. PRNJAK ET AL.
... Similarly, women with bulimia nervosa (BN) have been found to rate images of underweight women more positively than healthy control women, though no group differences in valence ratings were found for images of overweight bodies (Mai et al., 2015). In non-clinical samples, some studies have found that images of thin women are rated less positively than neutral images (Prnjak et al., 2020), while others have reported these images are rated more positively (Dodd et al., 2017). Images of thin women have also been rated as more pleasant but equally as arousing as images of overweight women (Dodd et al., 2017;Rieger et al., 2017). ...
... Greater eating pathology was also associated with more negative valence ratings to thin body stimuli, which is at odds with the theory that the valence of a stimulus is associated with motivational orientation (Lang, 1995;Elliot, 2006). This finding, however, is consistent with some past research (e.g., Prnjak et al., 2020) and may reflect the effects of social comparison. Individuals who perceive themselves negatively in comparison to the individual depicted in the image may be more likely to feel negatively in response to that image, despite having a desire to approach/attain this body type. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The present study examined the relative roles of approach and avoidance motivation in eating pathology using a multi-method approach combining self-report and physiological measures. The potential effect of internalized ideals and fears was also investigated. Methods Fifty-seven undergraduate women completed a picture-viewing task in which they viewed images of women’s bodies (thin and non-thin) and affective images. Self-report ratings of valence and arousal were measured along with physiological indicators of approach (postauricular reflex) and avoidance (startle blink reflex) motivation. Results Greater eating pathology was associated with more negative valence ratings of both thin and non-thin images. There was a significant interaction between valence ratings of non-thin bodies and fear of the unattractive self in relation to eating pathology, such that eating pathology was highest in those who rated non-thin images as more unpleasant and internalized fears of being/becoming unattractive. Thin-ideal internalization did not significantly interact with ratings of thin images to predict eating pathology. There were no significant findings when examining physiological data. Conclusions Results from self-report measures suggest that eating pathology is associated with avoidant reactions to both thin and non-thin bodies and highlight the importance of internalized appearance-related fears.
... Those three aspects then also invade matters of beauty. It happens especially when there is a certain standard define what beauty is, the indicators of being beautiful, and how to achieve that charming status (Blaivas et al., 2002;Prnjak et al., 2020). That is also the main cause that makes the girl in the song try to make herself as pretty as the beauty standards want her to be. ...
... Ideal beauty is presented in advertisements in order to be perceived as flawless and faultless (Greenfield, 2018;Prnjak et al., 2020). Advertisements portray a thin ideal of beauty, often featuring perfect proportions, light skin, and soft hair. ...
Article
Full-text available
Kecantikan feminin yang ideal adalah seperangkat standar kecantikan yang dibangun di atas gagasan bahwa daya tarik fisik adalah salah satu aset wanita yang paling penting dan sesuatu yang harus diperjuangkan dan dipertahankan oleh semua wanita. Scars to Your Beautiful adalah lagu yang ditulis dan dinyanyikan oleh Alessia Cara. Lagu ini bercerita tentang seorang gadis yang ingin menjadi cantik seperti gadis sampul. Dia ingin semua orang terpikat dengan kecantikannya. Namun, dalam proses memenuhi standar kecantikan itu, dia melupakan apa yang berharga di dalam dirinya. Dia rela menderita rasa sakit dan kelaparan untuk menjadikan dirinya secantik yang standar kecantikan inginkan. Lalu, bagaimana kecantikan perempuan yang diidealkan dan dampak negatifnya direpresentasikan dalam Scars to Your Beautiful karya Alessia Cara? Melalui metode kualitatif, artikel ini menjelaskan bagaimana standar kecantikan berdampak negatif pada gadis dalam lagu Alessia Cara. Didukung oleh teori dari Savannah Greenfield, kecantikan wanita yang diidealkan dan dampak negatifnya tercermin ketika gadis dalam lagu ini mendambakan dirinya seperti gadis sampul. Kesimpulannya, gadis itu melakukan segalanya untuk menjadi cantik namun dia akhirnya tersiksa dalam usahanya untuk memenuhi standar kecantikan yang dipenuhi oleh gadis-gadis sampul.
... The concept of body image is not fixed or static but is a dynamic construct that varies based on personal experiences and social influences [2,23], and its relationship with sociocultural standards present in certain times and cultures would affect the degree of satisfaction towards the body [26,27], making adolescent women especially vulnerable to the ideal of thinness [15,28,29]. This ideal of thinness would constitute a powerful risk factor for the generation of an unsatisfactory body image [30,31] and the development of eating disorders [32,33]. ...
... Specifically for sex, women tend to be more concerned about their appearance and weight and have a more negative evaluation based on weight, which is consistent with numerous studies that indicate that young women overestimate their weight, compared to men [9,73,74], as well as less satisfaction with specific areas of their body [31,74]. This is consistent with results in the Brazilian adolescent female population [15], in addition to the relationship between the ideal of thinness and unsatisfactory body image [30,31]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study is to analyze the factorial structure of the Multidimensional Body–Self Relations Questionnaire—Appearance Scales (MBSRQ-AS) to determine the adjustment of the study structure proposed in the Spanish short version in a young, non-clinical Chilean population and to evaluate the possible influence of sex, age, and BMI on body image measurement. The sample consisted of 614 Chilean youth (259 male and 355 female) between 15 and 28 years old (M = 18.81; SE = 2.46), from the Metropolitan Region, and four regions of the coast and south-central zone of the country. The average Body Mass Index (BMI) was 22.5 kg/m2 (SD = 3.16). The model fit was evaluated by confirmatory factor analysis (WLSMV) using the following: a model with a single general factor, a model with the five factors of the original version, a five-factor ESEM model, and a MIMIC model analyzed including sex, age, and BMI. The results show that the MIMIC sex, age, and BMI model presents an acceptable fit, observing that four factors, Appearance Orientation (AO), Body Areas Satisfaction (BAS), Overweight Preoccupation (OP), and Self-Classification Weight (SCW), are affected significantly for the sex variable, one for age, Appearance Evaluation (AE), and four for BMI (AE, BAS, OP, and SCW). In conclusion, MBSRQ-AS replicates the five-dimensional structure in a non-clinical sample of young Chileans; however, their scores are not invariant as they depend on sex, age, and BMI.
... The current study extends these works by including a measure of SPA following exposure to images of female athletes. Idyllic images of female beauty (sometimes referenced as "thinspirational" images) have been associated with greater preoccupation with one's size, and increased deliberation time when making self-ratings of body figures (Prnjak et al., 2019). Furthermore, exposure to "fitspirational" Instagram models have been shown to lower women's state appearance self-esteem (Tiggemann & Zaccardo, 2015). ...
... These considerations may involve both bodily and facial attractiveness. It is precisely this self-preoccupation that likely instigates the self-deliberation about the degree to which one's body and size adheres to others' standards of desirability when thinideal images are encountered (Prnjak et al., 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
Media images depicting idealized female physiques have been shown to heighten body dissatisfaction and body objectification. A potentially buffering factor in media exposure are depictions of female athletes performing their sports, which are associated with reduced objectification. These findings have not been extended to social physique anxiety (SPA), a heightened concern that one’s body does not meet comparative standards of physicality and beauty. Sixty-nine college-aged women reported levels of SPA following exposure to images of the same female professional athletes performing their sport, or in a sexualized pose. Visual attention to body parts on the images was measured via an eye tracker to explore whether fixations corresponded with the experience of SPA. Performance images lowered feelings of SPA relative to sexual images, and induced a lesser percentage of time visually fixating on the head/face, and more time fixating on arms and legs, relative to sexual images of the athletes. No differences emerged for fixations on the torso across conditions. Exploratory mediation models were also conducted to explore the influence of visual attention on the relationship between image type and SPA. These findings are considered in light of the nature of objectifying images of women and the importance of promoting empowering images to audiences.
... In both cases, dissatisfaction with height may lead to attention towards and behaviours targeting other more changeable dimensions of body image: body fat and muscularity. If one believes that their height, whether too short or too tall, creates a deficit in attractiveness, then they may be driven to engage with compensatory behaviours that reduce their level of body fat and increase their muscle tone or athleticism and thus move closer toward the commonly reported ideal female body (Bozsik et al., 2018;Prnjak et al., 2020). In extreme cases, this could lead to behaviours like food restriction, purging, and compulsive exercise. ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent research has evidenced the importance of height dissatisfaction in male body image, however the impact of height on body image in women remains relatively unexplored. Our study aimed to investigate the association between height, heightdissatisfaction, body dissatisfaction, and eating disorder symptoms in a sample of 139 women from the USA. Participants were recruited using Amazon's MTurk and reported their actual and ideal height, as well as completing measures of height dissatisfaction, and drive for thinness, drive for muscularity, and eating disorder symptoms. A paired sample t-test was utilised to examine differences in participants’ actual and ideal height. Additionally, linear hierarchical multiple regression was used to assess whether height, eating disorder symptoms, drive for thinness, and drive for muscularity uniquely predicted height dissatisfaction, and significant interactions were explored using a simple slope analysis complemented with a Johnson-Neyman analysis. Results showed that 48.92% of women reported identical actual and ideal height, 33.09% of women wanted to be taller, and 13.67% wanted to be shorter than their actual height. Additionally, shorter women tended to report greater height dissatisfaction, and higher levels of drive for thinness and drive for muscularity were associated with increased height dissatisfaction. However, eating disorder symptoms did not uniquely account for significant variance in height dissatisfaction once accounting for drive for thinness and muscularity. Our exploratory analysis also revealed that for taller than average women, height dissatisfaction was more strongly predicted by drive for muscularity, thus implicating the significance of height and muscle dissatisfaction for taller women. Overall, our study demonstrated that height and height dissatisfaction are important components to the theoretical construct of women’s body image, and therefore should be integrated into theoretical models of female body dissatisfaction and considered in assessment, formulation, and treatment of body image-related disorders. Further research with larger and more diverse samples, including clinical populations, is warranted to validate and extend our findings.
... Specifically, both sighted and sight-impaired groups stated to consider and desire an hourglass-shaped body as the most attractive but, at the same time, to not perceive so their own. This discrepancy between esthetic ideals and self-body perception might simply highlight the welldemonstrated vision-independent media influence on female beauty models (72,73). Among visually impaired subjects, blind women particularly appreciated this armochromy-inspired tool, first ever applied in such a target population, finding it useful to mentally visualize/imagine their corporeal shape. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Sport has the well-known power of improving body awareness, self-esteem, and social interaction, thus promoting quality of life and psychophysical wellbeing. Specifically referring to adapted disciplines, habitual practice often becomes an effective integration and self-efficacy booster. Among disabilities, visual impairment deeply alters body image perception, autonomy, and environmental/social interaction heavily reducing sport or leisure involvement opportunities. In particular, visually impaired women represent one of the most vulnerable categories to gender and disability discrimination. Moreover, even when congenitally sightless, they perceive social pressure of mainstream beauty ideals, mostly spread by media, comparable to their sighted peers. On these premises and the previously demonstrated psychophysical benefits of Italian blind baseball practice on this target population, the present study aimed to deepen the social and educative potentialities of such adapted sport applying a more sociological research approach. Methods The “red diamonds” event, namely, the first ever female blind baseball match, was the setting for the administration of our structured online survey. In detail, our survey comprised different evaluation tools such as the 18-item Psychological Well-Being Scale, the 12-item Short Form questionnaire, the Dresden Body Image questionnaire, the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale, and sociological model designed questions. Quality of life, psychological wellbeing, self-esteem, body image, and perceived female sport psychological violence were investigated in the whole women sample (n = 33) voluntarily adhering to the game. Results Survey results revealed no statistically significant differences between visually impaired players (n = 13; mean age: 32.84 ± 12.05 years) and sighted on-field subjects (i.e., coaches, assistants, and referees; n = 20; mean age: 47.15 ± 12.31 years) in almost all the inquired variables, thus remarking the social and functional benefits of adapted sport through the “dual embodiment” and empowerment phenomenon. Conclusions Given that the event was inspired by and performed on the World Day against women violence, our study deepened not only the topic of disability discrimination but also the currently alarming gender-related one. In such a context, the present research might provide interesting cues for further investigations on disability and gender disparities in sports, hence spreading interest in this under-investigated field. In perspective, the “red diamonds” experience could also contribute to inspiring and progressively developing educative tools against any kind of discrimination by promoting integration and social growth through regular sports practice.
... Kim et al. (2016), for example, found that nearly 80% of female characters in commercials were thin, even though only 5% of the general population has this body shape. Females are typically portrayed with a "thinideal" body type (Mastro and Figueroa-Caballero, 2018;Prnjak et al., 2019) and have historically been portrayed as emotional, affectionate, sensitive, domestic and frail (Aubrey and Harrison, 2004). As a result of these gender stereotypes, girls tend to value thinness (Huang et al., 2021). ...
Article
Purpose This study aims to examine the effects of character body size, subject body size and product type on female adolescents’ attitudes toward the character. Given prior research showing that adolescents identify more strongly with those whom they view as similar to themselves, it is possible that heavy and obese adolescents will react more favorably to plus-size ad characters. Design/methodology/approach Two studies were conducted, one with females aged 12–14 and a second with females aged 15–17. Based on their body mass index, subjects were classified as of small/average size or overweight/obese. Ads featured either a thin, average-size or plus-size character, and promoted either a healthful or an unhealthful snack item. Findings In general, small/average size female adolescents responded more favorably toward thin characters, whereas their overweight/obese counterparts responded more favorably toward plus-size characters. Moreover, subjects’ responses were not moderated by the nutritional value (healthful vs unhealthful) of the product being advertised. Research limitations/implications To effectively promote healthy foods to overweight/obese adolescents, it may be advantageous to incorporate plus-size characters. Additional research is needed, however, to determine whether this approach can effectively influence brand attitudes and consumption behaviors. Social implications As obesity rates continue to rise, it has become vitally important to encourage healthier food choices among youth. To develop effective communication strategies, marketers need to better understand how young consumers respond to various advertising cues. Originality/value To the best of the authors’ knowledge, this is the first study to examine the effect of character size and subject size on female adolescents’ attitudes toward the character.
... Overall, eye-tracking studies indicated that males demonstrated greater attentional biases towards muscular images (Cho & Lee, 2013;Jin et al., 2018;Porras-Garcia et al., 2019) compared to other body stimuli. While this may be similar to findings with females who view thinner images (e.g., Glauert et al., 2010;Prnjak et al., 2020), findings suggest that males and females demonstrate different viewing patterns (Hewig et al., 2008;Pazhoohi et al., 2019). These findings are indicative of not only different motivations for engaging with media ideal images, but also potentially different cognitive processes when engaged with them. ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent literature has discussed the role of attentional biases towards body-related stimuli. Specific foci have been on those with high levels of body image concerns and female samples. Unfortunately, there has been limited focus on male samples within existing literature. The aim of the current study was to provide a critical synthesis of the findings of existing studies exploring attentional biases in adult males towards body-related stimuli. Critical synthesis of the findings of 20 studies explored four key methodologies: eye-tracking, dot-probe, visual search, and other methodologies (e.g. ARDPEI task). The current review provides evidence of specific attentional biases towards body-related stimuli in adult males experiencing body image concerns. Similar patterns of attentional biases are also demonstrated in males with body image pathologies. However, there appears to be distinct patterns of attentional biases for male and female participants. It is recommended that future research considers these findings and utilises measures developed specifically for male samples. Furthermore, additional variables require further attention, i.e. reasons for engaging in social comparison and/or engaging in physical activity.
... Furthermore, SM influencers may post "thinspiration" and "fitspiration" content (i.e., content idealizing thin or fit bodies that is purported to "inspire" thinness or fitness; Boepple & Thompson, 2016), which tends to increase young women's body dissatisfaction (Robinson et al., 2017). Experimental work indicates even brief exposure to such content may have negative effects on young women's state body satisfaction and mood (e.g., Prnjak et al., 2020;Rounds & Stutts, 2021). In more extreme cases, SM content may be "pro-ana" or "pro-mia," developed specifically to encourage eating disorders such as anorexia nervosa or bulimia nervosa (Bardone-Cone & Cass, 2007). ...
Article
Full-text available
In this theoretical review paper, we provide a developmental-sociocultural framework for the role of social media (SM) in adolescent girls' body image concerns, and in turn, depressive symptoms and disordered eating. We propose that the features of SM (e.g., idealized images of peers, quantifiable feedback) intersect with adolescent developmental factors (e.g., salience of peer relationships) and sociocultural gender socialization processes (e.g., societal over-emphasis on girls' and women's physical appearance) to create the "perfect storm" for exacerbating girls' body image concerns. We argue that, ultimately, body image concerns may be a key mechanism underlying associations between adolescent girls' SM use and mental health. In the context of proposing this framework, we provide empirical evidence for how SM may increase adolescent girls' body image concerns through heightening their focus on (1) other people's physical appearance (e.g., through exposure to idealized images of peers, celebrities, and SM influencers; quantifiable indicators of approval); and (2) their own appearance (e.g., through appearance-related SM consciousness; exposure to idealized self-images; encouraging over-valuing of appearance; and peer approval of photos/videos). Our framework highlights new avenues for future research on adolescent girls' SM use and mental health, which recognize the central role of body image.
Article
People with larger body sizes often face underrepresentation in popular media. However, the rise of social media has led to increased visibility for this demographic group. Our research, encompassing five studies, investigated how audiences react to social media images featuring individuals with larger body sizes, focusing on affective responses and social transmission dynamics. We identified a gender-specific gap in sharing behavior such that male viewers show lower sharing intentions for such images (Study 1), mediated by stereotypic perceptions of low competence in larger-sized people and decreased positive affect (Study 2). This pattern holds for images with both female and male models (Study 3) and extends to contexts where body size is not a central factor (Study 4). To address this, we developed social norm-based interventions that effectively increase sharing intentions evoked by images of people with larger bodies, mediated by social perceptions and affective responses (Study 5). Overall, our findings shed light on the psychological factors that influence the spread of body-related imagery on social media, demonstrating how stereotypes can shape affective responses and subsequent actions like social media sharing. Beyond theoretical insights, our research offers practical strategies for amplifying societal focus on underrepresented groups and promoting more diverse and inclusive representations of body image in digital spaces.
Article
Full-text available
This study explores some possible causes of the recent increase in dieting and eating disorders among American women. Measures on body image, attitudes to eating and weight, and eating behaviors were collected from male (sons) and female (daughters) college students and their biological parents. All groups but the sons considered their current body shape to be heavier than their ideal. Mothers and daughters believed that men (of their own generation) prefer much thinner women than these men actually prefer. Mothers and daughters both showed great concern about weight and eating. Although fathers resembled mothers and daughters in their perception of being overweight, they were more similar to their sons in being relatively unconcerned about weight and eating. Hence, the major factor in concern about weight is sex rather than generation or discrepancy between perception of current and ideal body shape.
Book
Full-text available
As we progress as a species, questions and controversies continue to surround sexuality, monogamy, perceptions of attractiveness, and sexual coercion. Yet no matter how intricate the issues and concepts become, we are still able to find valuable clues in our ancestral legacy. Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Sexual Psychology and Behavior offers a wealth of current theories and findings on the complex psychological adaptations that drive our strategies for selecting and retaining a partner. Groundbreaking studies examine sex differences and similarities in sex-related human behavior while providing object lessons in how evolutionary psychology is practiced and where the field is heading. Contributors present intriguing evidence for mate selection influencing the evolution of men's and women's voices, female orgasm, and men's use of humor, and explore emerging areas of evolutionary interest such as same-sex attraction. This interdisciplinary coverage has wide-ranging implications for sexual well-being as well as mental and general health. Among the featured topics: • Evaluating evidence of mate preference adaptations: how do we really know what Homo sapiens sapiens really want? • Sexual adaptation and sexual offending. • (Mis)reading the signs: men’s perception of women’s sexual interest. • Female perceptions of male body movements. • Intrasexual competition and other theories of eating restriction. • Social selection and the evolution of competition among women. Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Sexual Psychology and Behavior will appeal to evolutionary scientists across different disciplines of the academy among faculty, graduate students, and undergraduate students interested in sexuality. This volume makes a useful supplementary text in various upper-level undergraduate courses and in graduate courses that address sexuality.
Article
Full-text available
Advertisers are increasingly monitoring people's online behavior and using the information collected to show people individually targeted advertisements. This phenomenon is called online behavioral advertising (OBA). Although advertisers can benefit from OBA, the practice also raises concerns about privacy. Therefore, OBA has received much attention from advertisers, consumers, policymakers, and scholars. Despite this attention, there is neither a strong definition of OBA nor a clear accumulation of empirical findings. This article defines OBA and provides an overview of the empirical findings by developing a framework that identifies and integrates all factors that can explain consumer responses toward OBA. The framework suggests that the outcomes of OBA are dependent on advertiser-controlled factors (e.g., the level of personalization) and consumer-controlled factors (e.g., knowledge and perceptions about OBA and individual characteristics). The article also overviews the theoretical positioning of OBA by placing the theories that are used to explain consumers’ responses to OBA in our framework. Finally, we develop a research agenda and discuss implications for policymakers and advertisers.
Article
Full-text available
Background Emerging evidence suggests that changes in quality of life (QoL) predicts later changes in eating disorder (ED) symptoms. The objective of this study was to explore individual sufferers’ perspectives on the influence of QoL on the onset, maintenance, and/or remission of ED symptoms. Method19 women from the community with a history of eating disorders (n = 13 currently symptomatic; n = 6 recovered) were interviewed about their observations on the relationship between QoL and ED symptoms over time in their own lives. Interviews were audio-taped and transcribed, and then thematically analysed. ResultsThematic analysis uncovered two major themes: 1. QoL as a Vulnerability Factor, and 2. QoL as a Recovery Factor. In relation to the first theme, onset of ED symptoms was discussed by women in this study as having been triggered by impairment in QoL, including a general sense of lacking control in life, stress, abusive intimate relationships, poor role modelling from family, physical impairment related to obesity, peer pressure, and weight-related teasing. On the other hand, and in relation to the second theme, subsequent improvement in QoL was nominated as central to symptom improvement and recovery. QoL improvement was described by participants differently, but included increased general satisfaction in life, emotional maturation, prioritising and improving physical health, the development of a supportive intimate relationship and social relationships, and having children. Conclusions Impairment in QoL may act as a trigger for the onset and maintenance of ED symptoms, whereas improvement in QoL may be central to eating disorder improvement and eventual recovery. Treatment should involve consideration of a core focus on QoL improvement as a potential ‘backdoor’ approach to improving ED symptoms.
Article
Full-text available
Body image (BI) can be described as the assessment of both positive and negative emotion for one’s own body parts and their characteristics by himself or herself. Current research has concentrated mostly on the status of negative BI as a risk factor for mental health problems rather than as a public health problem, thereby little is known about the effects of BI on quality of life. Thus, the purpose of this study was to assess the BI and Quality of Life (QoL) of individuals and to investigate the relationship between the two. Individuals over 15 living in Isparta city center constitute the universe of this cross-sectional analytical study, carried out in 2014. The BI of individuals was measured by the Body Image Scale and The QoL of individuals was measured using the World Health Organization (WHO) Quality of Life Scale Short Form. The mean age of the participants was 31.9 ± 13.0 and 56.0% were female, 36.8% were married and 81.7% had education above high school. 25.7% had at least one chronic disease and 17.7% received medication regularly. Having good-very good health perception, having higher income than expenses, making regular exercises were predictors in enhancing the quality of life in certain aspects, however having a good body image came out as a predictor enhancing the quality of life in all sub-domains. BI was found closely related with QoL in all sub-domains. Our findings suggest that greater attention should be to be given to BI as a strong predictor of QoL.
Article
Full-text available
The present research details the development of a new pictorial scale (Body Dissatisfaction Scale) to be used in the measurement of body dissatisfaction. The scale comprises nine female and nine male images of computer generated bodies that increase successively in body weight. Using a sample of 190 students (female = 130, male = 60) results showed that the new scale exhibits good validity, with participants being able to correctly identify body weight differences between all bodies in the scale. Evidence for construct validity was demonstrated by significant correlations between ratings of perceived actual body size using the current scale and participants’ BMI. Body dissatisfaction measured using the current scale was also negatively correlated with a measure of body appreciation. Test-retest reliability remained stable over a 5 week period. The scale improves on previous pictorial scales by offering both male and female versions while offering improved realism and consistency between images.
Article
The human psyche is profoundly shaped by its cultural milieu; however, few studies have examined the dynamics of cultural influence in everyday life, especially when it comes to shaping people’s automatic, implicit attitudes. In this quasi-experimental field study, we investigated the effect of transient, but salient, cultural messages—the pop-cultural phenomenon of celebrity “fat-shaming”—on implicit anti-fat attitudes in the population. Adopting the “copycat suicide” methodology, we identified 20 fat-shaming events in the media; next, we obtained data from Project Implicit of participants who had completed the Weight Implicit Association Test from 2004 to 2015. As predicted, fat-shaming led to a spike in women’s (N=93,239) implicit anti-fat attitudes, with events of greater notoriety producing greater spikes. We also observed a general increase in implicit anti-fat attitudes over time. Although these passing comments may appear harmless, we show that feedback at the cultural level can be registered by the “body politic.”
Article
Weight gain and loss arise from environmental, genetic, biological, and behavioral factors. Most North Americans desire weight loss or maintenance, but most gain weight throughout adulthood. In a society rife with thin ideals and anti-fat bias, especially for women, one under-recognized factor could contribute to this weight-gaining trajectory: the activation and persistence of psychological threat. We used multiple methods to contact participants from two previous studies in which weight-dissatisfied women were given either a values affirmation to buffer them against psychological threat, or a control task. An average of 2.32 years after the affirmation manipulation, higher body mass index (BMI) women who had been affirmed maintained their weight, whereas controls gained weight. There was no effect on lower BMI women. Additional analyses ruled out alternative explanations. Results underscore the role of psychological threat in long-term weight gain. Our findings suggest that a more holistic analysis of weight-gaining trajectory is essential for understanding processes behind weight maintenance and change.