Conference PaperPDF Available

EMBODIED MATHEMATICAL IMAGINATION AND COGNITION (EMIC) WORKING GROUP

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Embodied cognition is growing in theoretical importance and as a driving set of design principles for curriculum activities and technology innovations for mathematics education. The central aim of the EMIC (Embodied Mathematical Imagination and Cognition) Working Group is to attract engaged and inspired colleagues into a growing community of discourse around theoretical, technological, and methodological developments for advancing the study of embodied cognition for mathematics education. A thriving, informed, and interconnected community of scholars organized around embodied mathematical cognition will broaden the range of activities, practices, and emerging technologies that count as mathematical. EMIC builds upon our prior working groups with a specific focus on how we can leverage emerging technologies to study embodied cognition and mathematics learning. In particular, we aim to develop new theories and extend existing frameworks and perspectives from which EMIC collaboration and activities can emerge.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Working Groups
Hodges, T.E., Roy, G. J., & Tyminski, A. M. (Eds.). (2018). Proceedings of the 40th annual meeting of
the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education. Greenville, SC: University of South Carolina & Clemson University.
1449
EMBODIED MATHEMATICAL IMAGINATION AND COGNITION (EMIC)
WORKING GROUP
Erin R. Ottmar Edward Melcer Dor Abrahamson
Worcester Polytechnic Institute Univ. of California, Santa Cruz Univ. of California, Berkeley
erottmar@wpi.edu eddie.melcer@ucsc.edu dor@berkeley.edu
Mitchell J. Nathan Emily Fyfe Carmen Smith
Univ. of Wisconsin-Madison Indiana University University of Vermont
mnathan@wisc.edu efyfe@indiana.edu carmen.smith@uvm.edu
Embodied cognition is growing in theoretical importance and as a driving set of design
principles for curriculum activities and technology innovations for mathematics education. The
central aim of the EMIC (Embodied Mathematical Imagination and Cognition) Working Group
is to attract engaged and inspired colleagues into a growing community of discourse around
theoretical, technological, and methodological developments for advancing the study of
embodied cognition for mathematics education. A thriving, informed, and interconnected
community of scholars organized around embodied mathematical cognition will broaden the
range of activities, practices, and emerging technologies that count as mathematical. EMIC
builds upon our prior working groups with a specific focus on how we can leverage emerging
technologies to study embodied cognition and mathematics learning. In particular, we aim to
develop new theories and extend existing frameworks and perspectives from which EMIC
collaboration and activities can emerge.
Keywords: Technology; Cognition; Informal Education; Learning Theory
Motivations for This Working Group
Recent empirical, theoretical and methodological developments in embodied cognition and
gesture studies provide a solid and generative foundation for the establishment of a regularly
held Embodied Mathematical Imagination and Cognition (EMIC) Working Group for
PME-NA. The central aim of EMIC is to attract engaged and inspired colleagues into a growing
community of discourse around theoretical, technological, and methodological developments for
advancing the study of embodied cognition for mathematics education, including, but not limited
to, studies of mathematical reasoning, instruction, the design and use of technological
innovations, learning in and outside of formal educational settings, and across the lifespan.
The interplay of multiple perspectives and intellectual trajectories is vital for the study of
embodied mathematical cognition to flourish. While there is significant convergence of
theoretical, technological, and methodological developments in embodied cognition, there is also
a trove of technological, methodological, and theoretical questions that must be addressed before
we can formulate and implement effective design principles. As a group, we aim to address basic
theoretical questions such as, What is grounding? And practical ones such as, How can we
reliably engineer the grounding of specific mathematical ideas? We need to understand how
variations in actions and perceptions influence mathematical reasoning, including self-initiated
vs. prescribed actions, and actions that take place in intrapersonal versus interpersonal
interactions; how gestural point-of-view when enacting phenomena from a first- versus third-
person perspective, including how gestures move through space, influences reasoning and
communication; how actions enacted by oneself, observed in others, or imagined influence
Working Groups
Hodges, T.E., Roy, G. J., & Tyminski, A. M. (Eds.). (2018). Proceedings of the 40th annual meeting of
the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education. Greenville, SC: University of South Carolina & Clemson University.
1450
cognition; and how gestures connect with external visual representations (Alibali & Nathan,
2012; in press).
A thriving, informed, and interconnected community of scholars organized around embodied
mathematical cognition will broaden the range of activities and emerging technologies that count
as mathematical, and envision alternative forms of engagement with mathematical ideas and
practices (e.g., De Freitas & Sinclair, 2014). This broadening is particularly important at a time
when schools and communities in North America face persistent achievement gaps between
groups of students from many ethnic backgrounds, geographic regions, and socioeconomic
circumstances (Ladson-Billings, 1995; Moses & Cobb, 2001; Rosebery, Warren, Ballenger &
Ogonowski, 2005). There also is a need to articulate evidence-based findings and principles of
embodied cognition to the research and development communities that are looking to generate
and disseminate innovative programs for promoting mathematics learning through movement
(e.g., Ottmar & Landy, 2016; Smith, King, & Hoyte, 2014). Generating, evaluating, and curating
empirically validated and reliable methods for promoting mathematical development and
effective instruction through embodied activities that are engaging and curricularly relevant is an
urgent societal goal. As embodied cognition gains prominence in education, so, too, does new
ways of using technology to support teaching and learning (Lee, 2015). These new uses of
technology, in turn, offer novel opportunities for students and scientists to engage in math
visualization, symbolization, intuition, and reasoning. In order for these designs to successfully
scale up, they must be informed by research that demonstrates both ecological and internal
validity.
Past Meetings and Achievements of the EMIC Working Group
“Mathematics Learning and Embodied Cognition,” the first PME-NA meeting of the EMIC
working group, took place in East Lansing, MI in 2015. Our group has been growing ever since.
In addition to the PME-NA meeting each year, there are a number of ongoing activities that our
members engage in. We have built an active website which provides updates on projects, and
hosts resources. On this website, we have a list of members with their emails and bios,
information about our PME-NA presence, and short personal introduction videos. We’ve also
created a space for members to share information about their research activities – particularly for
videos of the complex gesture and action-based interactions that are difficult to express in text
format. In addition, we have a common publications repository to share files or links (including
to ResearchGate or Academia.edu publication profiles, so members don’t have to upload their
files in multiple places). Our members collaborate on ongoing projects, and have presented at
other pre-conference workshop events such as the Computer Supported Collaborative Learning
conference (Williams-Pierce et al., 2017), and the APA Technology, Mind, and Society
conference (Harrison et. al, 2018). Several research programs have formed to investigate the
embodied nature of mathematics (e.g., Abrahamson 2014; Alibali & Nathan, 2012; Arzarello et
al., 2009; De Freitas & Sinclair, 2014; Edwards, Ferrara, & Moore-Russo, 2014; Lakoff &
Núñez, 2000; Melcer & Isbister, 2016; Ottmar & Landy, 2016; Radford 2009; Nathan,
Walkington, Boncoddo, Pier, Williams, & Alibali, 2014; Soto-Johnson & Troup, 2014; Soto-
Johnson, Hancock, & Oehrtman, 2016), demonstrating a “critical mass” of projects, findings,
senior and junior investigators, and conceptual frameworks to support an ongoing community of
like minded scholars within the mathematics education research community.
Some of our collaborative accomplishments since 2015 include:
Working Groups
Hodges, T.E., Roy, G. J., & Tyminski, A. M. (Eds.). (2018). Proceedings of the 40th annual meeting of
the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education. Greenville, SC: University of South Carolina & Clemson University.
1451
1. Developing a group website using the Google Sites platform to connect scholars, support
ongoing interactions throughout the year, and regularly adding additional
resources/activities https://sites.google.com/site/emicpmena/home
2. Joint submission of several NSF grants by members who first met during the 2015 EMIC
sessions
3. Some senior members joining junior members’ NSF grant proposals as Co-PIs and
advisors
4. Submission to the IES CASL program to study the role of action in pre-college proof
performance in geometry (Funded 2016-2020 for Nathan & Walkington), as well of the
use of perceptual learning on algebra learning (Ottmar & Landy, 2018)
5. Submission of 2 NSF Proposals to host Workshops on Embodied Cognition (one for
researchers and one for K-16 math educators)
6. A joint symposium on Embodied Cognition with 6 members at the 2018 APA
Technology, Mind & Society Conference
7. Examining the potential for an NSF Research Coordination Network (RCN)
8. Application for a grant from Association for Psychological Science (APS) to develop a
better website and offer stipends for contributors
9. Presenting a pre-conference workshop to CSCL 2017 on the embodied tools to promote
STEM education (Williams-Pierce et al., 2017)
10. A Conference at Berkeley’s Graduate School of Education to examine the relevance of
coordination dynamics -- the non-linear perspective on kinesiological development -- to
individuals’ sensorimotor construction of perceptual structures underlying mathematical
concepts. https://edrl.berkeley.edu/cdme2018
Current Working Group Organizers
As the Working Group has matured and expanded, we have a broadening set of organizers
that represent a range of institutions and theoretical perspectives (and is beyond the limit of six
authors in the submission system). This, we believe, enriches the Working Group experience and
the long-term viability of the scholarly community. The current organizers for 2018 are
(alphabetical by first name):
Candace Walkington, Southern Methodist University
Carmen J. Petrick Smith, University of Vermont
Caro Williams-Pierce, University at Albany, SUNY
David Landy, Indiana University
Dor Abrahamson, University of California, Berkeley
Edward Melcer, University of California, Santa Cruz
Emily Fyfe, Indiana University
Erin Ottmar, Worcester Polytechnic Institute
Hortensia Soto–Johnson, University of Northern Colorado
Ivon Arroyo, Worcester Polytechnic Institute
Martha W. Alibali, University of Wisconsin-Madison
Mitchell J. Nathan, University of Wisconsin-Madison
Focal Issues in the Psychology of Mathematics Education
Emerging, yet influential, views of thinking and learning as embodied experiences have
grown from several major intellectual developments in philosophy, psychology, anthropology,
Working Groups
Hodges, T.E., Roy, G. J., & Tyminski, A. M. (Eds.). (2018). Proceedings of the 40th annual meeting of
the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education. Greenville, SC: University of South Carolina & Clemson University.
1452
education, and the learning sciences that frame human communication as multimodal interaction,
and human thinking as multi-modal simulation of sensory-motor activity (Clark, 2008; Hostetter
& Alibali, 2008; Lave, 1988; Nathan, 2014; Varela et al., 1992; Wilson, 2002). As Stevens
(2012, p. 346) argues in his introduction to the JLS special issue on embodiment of mathematical
reasoning, “it will be hard to consign the body to the sidelines of mathematical cognition ever
again if our goal is to make sense of how people make sense and take action with mathematical
ideas, tools, and forms”.
Four major ideas exemplify the plurality of ways that embodied cognition perspectives are
relevant for the study of mathematical understanding: (1) Grounding of abstraction in perceptuo-
motor activity as one alternative to representing concepts as purely amodal, abstract, arbitrary,
and self-referential symbol systems. This conception shifts the locus of “thinking” from a central
processor to a distributed web of perceptuo-motor activity situated within a physical and social
setting. (2) Cognition emerges from perceptually guided action (Varela, Thompson, & Rosch,
1991). This tenet implies that things, including mathematical symbols and representations, are
understood by the actions and practices we can perform with them, and by mentally simulating
and imagining the actions and practices that underlie or constitute them. (3) Mathematics
learning is always affective: There are no purely procedural or “neutral” forms of reasoning
detached from the circulation of bodily-based feelings and interpretations surrounding our
encounters with them. (4) Mathematical ideas are conveyed using rich, multimodal forms of
communication, including gestures and tangible objects in the world.
In addition to theoretical and empirical advances, new technical advances in multi-modal and
spatial analysis have allowed scholars to collect new sources of evidence and subject them to
powerful analytic procedures, from which they may propose new theories of embodied
mathematical cognition and learning. Growth of interest in multi-modal aspects of
communication have been enabled by high quality video recording of human activity (e.g.,
Alibali et al., 2014; Levine & Scollon, 2004), motion capture technology (Hall, Ma, &
Nemirovsky, 2014; Sinclair, 2014), developments in brain imaging (e.g., Barsalou, 2008; Gallese
& Lakoff, 2005), multimodal learning analytics (Worsley & Blikstein, 2014), and data logs
generated from embodied math learning technologies that interacts with touch and mouse-based
interfaces (Manzo, Ottmar, & Landy, 2016).
Theme: Looking Back, Looking Ahead: Celebrating 40 Years of PME-NA
Inspired by the PME-NA 2018 theme, we will specifically focus on the ways in which the
field of embodied cognition has developed and how new emerging technologies and innovative
pedagogies can influence mathematics teaching and learning. This effort will be crafted to align
with recent developments in the embodiment literature, and new theoretical frameworks tying
various perspectives on embodiment to different forms of physicality in educational technology
(Melcer & Isbister, 2016; see Figure 1 below).
Working Groups
Hodges, T.E., Roy, G. J., & Tyminski, A. M. (Eds.). (2018). Proceedings of the 40th annual meeting of
the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education. Greenville, SC: University of South Carolina & Clemson University.
1453
Figure 1. Five distinct approaches to facilitating embodiment through bodily action, objects,
and the surrounding environment in educational technology.
Examples include: coding videos of pre-service teachers’ distributed gestures to explore a
mathematical conjecture (Walkington et al., 2018); exploring mathematical transformations
while using a dynamic technology tool (Ottmar & Landy, 2016), having students and teachers
play and create embodied technology games to teach mathematics and computational thinking
(Arroyo et al., 2017; Melcer & Isbister, 2018); usng dual eye tracking (Shvarts & Abrahamson,
2018), and a teacher guiding the movements of a learner exploring ratios (Abrahamson &
Sánchez-García, 2016). Through these examples, we will explore questions such as: what role
does technology play on supporting the connections between the mind, body, and action? During
the conference, participants in our EMIC workshop will engage in dedicated activities and
guided reflections as a basis for exploring the role of technology, action, and embodiment in the
emergence of mathematics learning.
Plan for Active Engagement of Participants
Our formula from prior PME-NA working groups proved to be effective: By inviting
participants into open ended math activities at the beginning of each session, we were rapidly
drawn into those very aspects of mathematics that we find most rewarding. We plan to facilitate
collaborative EMIC activities, followed by group discussions (and we now have many activities
and members who can trade off in these roles!) that will help us all to “pull back” to the
theoretical and methodological issues that are central to advancing math education research.
Within this structure of beginning with mathematical activities and facilitated discussions, on
Day 1 we plan to begin with four different group activities that highlight the interplay of
mathematics content, cognition, physicality, and action. These activities will serve as the
foundation for a broad group discussion about the varied roles of technology in EMIC. See
Figure 2 below for examples of collaborative activities from PME-NA 2017.
Figure 2. Collaborative activities. Participants explore geometric rotation and reflection(left);
two groups act out and prove mathematical conjectures (middle and right).
Working Groups
Hodges, T.E., Roy, G. J., & Tyminski, A. M. (Eds.). (2018). Proceedings of the 40th annual meeting of
the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education. Greenville, SC: University of South Carolina & Clemson University.
1454
The full first session will generally be taken up by introductions and a round of open-ended
activities followed by discussion. On Day 2, we will begin the session with technology-based
collaborative activities, with four stations that pairs of participants rotate through. Examples of
three of those stations are in Figure 3. Continuing with the routine established in Day 1, a full
group discussion will follow, with a particular focus on designing EMIC digital contexts to
support ongoing collaboration.
Figure 3. Technology activities. The Hidden Village (left); Graspable Math (middle); Bots &
(Main)Frames (right).
After the discussion, we will discuss different EMIC activity ideas, with the goal of developing
additional collaborative activities that can be used in various research and learning contexts. The
final activities will be shared on the EMIC website.
Day 3 is agenda-setting day, where we all discuss how we will keep the momentum going,
such as developing an NSF Research Coordination Network (RCN) to build the networked
community of international scholars from which many fruitful lines of inquiry can emerge. A
second group may draft a proposal for a special issue of the Journal of Research in Mathematics
Education that focuses on creating an integrated theoretical framework or sharing the different
theoretical perspectives, research activities, and operationalization of EMIC by the working
group members.
In order to find common ground for the RCN submission and the JRME special issue, we
will perform a live concept mapping activity that is displayed for all participants to explore the
range of EMIC topics and identify common conceptual structure. We will discuss different
general foci, such as teacher professional development with EMIC, designing EMIC games or
museum exhibits, etc. Building on the four major ideas that we developed earlier, possible topics
for organizing this activity will be explored, such as:
1. Grounding Abstractions
a. Conceptual blending (Tunner & Fauconnier, 1995) & metaphor (Lakoff & Núñez,
2000)
b. Perceptuo-motor grounding of abstractions (Barsalou, 2008; Glenberg, 1997;
Ottmar & Landy, 2016; Landy, Allen, & Zednik, 2014)
c. Progressive formalization (Nathan, 2012; Romberg, 2001) & concreteness fading
(Fyfe, McNeil, Son, & Goldstone, 2014)
d. Use of manipulatives (Martin & Schwartz, 2005)
2. Cognition emerges from perceptually guided action: Designing interactive learning
environments for EMIC
Working Groups
Hodges, T.E., Roy, G. J., & Tyminski, A. M. (Eds.). (2018). Proceedings of the 40th annual meeting of
the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education. Greenville, SC: University of South Carolina & Clemson University.
1455
a. Development of spatial reasoning (Uttal et al., 2009)
b. Math cognition through action (Abrahamson, 2014; Nathan et al., 2014)
c. Perceptual boundedness (Bieda & Nathan, 2009)
d. Perceptuomotor integration (Ottmar, Landy, Goldstone, & Weitnauer, 2015;
Nemirovsky, Kelton, & Rhodehamel, 2013)
e. Attentional anchors and the emergence of mathematical objects (Abrahamson &
Bakker, 2016; Abrahamson & Sánchez–García, 2016; Abrahamson et al., 2016;
Duijzer et al., 2017)
f. Mathematical imagination (Nemirovsky, Kelton, & Rhodehamel, 2012)
g. Students’ integer arithmetic learning depends on their actions (Nurnberger-Haag,
2015).
3. Affective Mathematics
a. Modal engagements (Hall & Nemirovsky, 2012; Nathan et al., 2013)
b. Sensuous cognition (Radford, 2009)
4. Gesture and Multimodality
a. Gesture & multimodal instruction (Alibali & Nathan 2012; Cook et al., 2008;
Edwards, 2009)
b. Bodily activity of professional mathematicians (Nemirovsky & Smith, 2013;
Soto-Johnson, Hancock, & Oehrtman, 2016)
c. Simulation of sensory-motor activity (Hostetter & Alibali, 2008; Nemirovsky &
Ferrara, 2009)
We will also discuss the implications of this work and the different areas of the concept map for
teaching, and discuss ideas for bridging the gap between research and practice.
Follow-up Activities
We envision an emergent process for the specific follow-up activities based on participant
input and our multi-day discussions. At a minimum, we will continue to develop a list of
interested participants and grant them all access to our common discussion forum and literature
compilation. Those that are interested in the NSF RCN plan will work to form the international
set of collaborations and articulate the intellectual topics that will knit the network together; and
those that are interested in the JRME special issue proposal will outline a specific timeline for
progressing. One additional set of activities we hope to explore is to create a series of
instructional activities that can be used to introduce educational practitioners at all levels of
administration and across the lifespan to the power and utility of the EMIC perspective.
In the past several years, we have seen a great deal of progress. This is perhaps best
exemplified by coming together of the EMIC website, the ongoing collaborations between
members, and the annual workshops, which each draws across multiple institutions. We thus will
strive to explore ways to reach farther outside of our young group to continually make our work
relevant, while also seeking to bolster and refine the theoretical underpinnings of an embodied
view of mathematical thinking and teaching.
References
Abrahamson, D. (2014). The monster in the machine, or why educational technology needs embodied design. In V.
Lee (Ed.), Learning technologies and the body: Integration and implementation (pp. 21-38). New York:
Routledge (Taylor & Francis Group).
Abrahamson, D., & Bakker, A. (2016). Making sense of movement in embodied design for mathematics learning.
Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 1(1), article 33. doi:10.1186/s41235-016-0034-3
Working Groups
Hodges, T.E., Roy, G. J., & Tyminski, A. M. (Eds.). (2018). Proceedings of the 40th annual meeting of
the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education. Greenville, SC: University of South Carolina & Clemson University.
1456
Abrahamson, D., & Sánchez-García, R. (2016). Learning is moving in new ways: The ecological dynamics of
mathematics education. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 25, 203–239.
Abrahamson, D., Shayan, S., Bakker, A., & Van der Schaaf, M. F. (2016). Eye-tracking Piaget: Capturing the
emergence of attentional anchors in the coordination of proportional motor action. Human Development, 58(4-
5), 218-244.
Alibali, M. W. & Nathan, M. J. (2012). Embodiment in mathematics teaching and learning: Evidence from learners'
and teachers' gestures. Journal of the Learning Sciences. (Special Issue on Embodiment in Mathematics.),
21(2), 247-286. DOI: 10.1080/10508406.2011.611446.
Alibali, M. W., Nathan, M. J., Wolfgram, M. S., Church, R. B., Jacobs, S. A., Martinez, C. J., & Knuth, E. J. (2014).
How teachers link ideas in mathematics instruction using speech and gesture: A corpus analysis. Cognition and
Instruction. 32(1), 65-100, doi: 10.1080/07370008.2013.858161.
Arroyo, I, & Ottmar, E. (2016). Developing Computational Thinking Through Embodied Math Games. National
Science Foundation.
Arzarello, F., Paola, D., Robutti, O., & Sabena, C. (2009). Gestures as semiotic resources in the mathematics
classroom. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 97-109.
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617-645.
Bieda, K. N. & Nathan, M. J. (2009). Representational disfluency in algebra: Evidence from student gestures and
speech. ZDM - The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 41(5), 637- 650. [DOI 10.1007/S11858-
009-0198-0.]
Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension: Embodiment, Action, and
Cognitive Extension. Oxford University Press, 2008.
Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Gesturing makes learning last. Cognition, 106(2), 1047-
1058.
De Freitas, E., & Sinclair, N. (2014). Mathematics and the body: Material entanglements in the classroom.
Cambridge University Press.
Duijzer, A. C. G., Shayan, S., Bakker, A., Van der Schaaf, M. F., & Abrahamson, D. (2017). Touchscreen tablets:
Coordinating action and perception for mathematical cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 8(144).
doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00144
Edwards, L. D. (2009). Gestures and conceptual integration in mathematical talk. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 70(2), 127-141.
Edwards, L., Ferrara, F., & Moore-Russo, D. (Eds). (2014). Emerging perspectives on gesture and embodiment in
mathematics. Charlotte, N.C.: Information Age.
ESM. Educational Studies of Mathematics (2009): PME Special Issue: Bodily Activity and Imagination in
Mathematics Learning.
Fyfe, E. R., McNeil, N. M., Son, J. Y., & Goldstone, R. L. (2014). Concreteness fading in mathematics and science
instruction: a systematic review. Educational Psychology Review, 26(1), 9-25.
Gallese & Lakoff, 2005)
Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for: Creating meaning in the service of action. Behavioral and brain
sciences, 20(01), 41-50.
Glenberg, A. M. (2010). Embodiment as a unifying perspective for psychology. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews:
Cognitive Science, 1(4), 586-596.
Goldstone, R. L., & Son, J. Y. (2005). The transfer of scientific principles using concrete and idealized simulations.
The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 14(1), 69-110.
Hall, R., & Nemirovsky, R. (2012). Introduction to the special issue: Modalities of body engagement in
mathematical activity and learning. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21(2), 207-215.
Hall, R., Ma, J. Y., & Nemirovsky, R. (2014). Rescaling Bodies in/as Representational Instruments in GPS
Drawing. Learning Technologies and the Body: Integration and Implementation In Formal and Informal
Learning Environments, 112.
Harrison A., Ottmar, E., Arroyo, I., Rosenbaum, L., Bakker, A., Abrahamson, D., Hulse, T., Manzo, D., & Landy,
D. (2018, April). Embodiment and action in mathematics games. Symposium conducted at the American
Psychological Association’s conference on Technology, Mind & Society, April 5-7, 2018, Washington, DC.
Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2008). Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin
& Review, 15(3), 495-514.
Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. E. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics
into being. Basic books.
Working Groups
Hodges, T.E., Roy, G. J., & Tyminski, A. M. (Eds.). (2018). Proceedings of the 40th annual meeting of
the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education. Greenville, SC: University of South Carolina & Clemson University.
1457
Landy, D., Allen, C., & Zednik, C. (2014). A perceptual account of symbolic reasoning. Frontiers in psychology, 5,
275.
Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics and culture in everyday life. Cambridge University Press.
LeVine, P., & Scollon, R. (Eds.). (2004). Discourse and technology: Multimodal discourse analysis. Georgetown
University Press.
Ma, J. Y., Hall, R., & Leander, K. M. (2010, June). Shifting between person, structure and settlement scales in
anthropological field work. In Proceedings of the 9th International Conference of the Learning Sciences-
Volume 2 (pp. 158-159). International Society of the Learning Sciences.
Martin, T., & Schwartz, D. L. (2005). Physically distributed learning: Adapting and reinterpreting physical
environments in the development of fraction concepts. Cognitive science, 29(4), 587-625.
Melcer, E. F., & Isbister, K. (2016). Bridging the Physical Learning Divides: A Design Framework for Embodied
Learning Games and Simulations. In Proceedings of the 1st International Joint Conference of DiGRA and
FDG.
Melcer, E. F., & Isbister, K. (2018). Bots & (Main)Frames: Exploring the Impact of Tangible Blocks and
Collaborative Play in an Educational Programming Game. In Proceedings of the 2018 CHI Conference on
Human Factors in Computing Systems. DOI: 10.1145/3173574.3173840
Nathan, M. J. (2014). Grounded Mathematical Reasoning. In L. Shapiro (Ed.). The Routledge Handbook of
Embodied Cognition (pp. 171-183). Routledge: New York.
Nathan, M. J., Srisurchan, R., Walkington, C., Wolfgram, M., Williams, C., & Alibali, M. (2013). Cohesion as a
mechanism of STEM integration. Journal of Engineering Education, 102(1), 77-116.
Nathan, M. J., Walkington, C., Boncoddo, R., Pier, E. L., Williams, C. C., & Alibali, M. W. (2014). Actions speak
louder with words: The roles of action and pedagogical language for grounding mathematical proof. Learning
and Instruction, 33, 182-193. DOI: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.07.001
Nemirovsky, R., (2007). AERA Symposium, “Mathematics Learning and Embodied Cognition” presented to the
American Educational Research Association annual meeting. Chicago, IL.
Nemirovsky, R., & Smith, M. (2013). Diagram-Use and the Emergence of Mathematical Objects. In B. M. Brizuela
& B. E. Gravel (Eds.), ‘Show me what you know' Exploring representations across STEM disciplines (pp. 143-
162). New York, NY. Teachers College Press.
Nemirovsky, R., & Ferrara, F. (2009). Mathematical imagination and embodied cognition. Educational Studies in
Mathematics, 70(2), 159-174.
Nemirovsky, R., Kelton, M. L., & Rhodehamel, B. (2012). Gesture and imagination: On the constitution and uses of
phantasms. Gesture, 12(2), 130-165.
Nemirovsky, R., Kelton, M. L., & Rhodehamel, B. (2013). Playing mathematical instruments: Emerging
perceptuomotor integration with an interactive mathematics exhibit. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education, 44(2), 372-415.
Núñez, R. (2004). Do real numbers really move? Language, thought, and gesture: The embodied cognitive
foundations of mathematics. In Embodied artificial intelligence (pp. 54-73). Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
Nurnberger-Haag, J. (2015). How students’ integer arithmetic learning depends on whether they walk a path or
collect chips. In Bartell, T. G., Bieda, K. N., Putnam, R.T., Bradfield, K.,& Dominguez, H.(Eds.).(pp. 165-172).
Proceedings of the 37th annual meeting of the North American Chapter of the International Group for the
Psychology of Mathematics Education. East Lansing, MI: Michigan State University.
Ottmar, E.R. & Landy. D. (2016). Concreteness fading of algebraic instruction: Effects on mathematics learning.
Journal of the Learning Sciences. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10508406.2016.1250212
Ottmar, E. R., Landy, D., Goldstone, R. L., & Weitnauer, E. (2015). Getting from here to there: Testing the
effectiveness of an interactive mathematics intervention embedding perceptual learning. Proceedings of the
Thirty-Seventh Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. (pp. 1793-1798). Pasadena, CA: Cognitive
Science Society.
Petrick Smith, Carmen, King, Barbara, & Hoyte, Jennifer (2014). Learning angles through movement: Critical
actions for developing understanding in an embodied activity. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 2014; 36:
95 DOI: 10.1016/j.jmathb.2014.09.001
Radford, L. (2009). Why do gestures matter? Sensuous cognition and the palpability of mathematical meanings.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 111-126.
Romberg, T. A., (2001). Designing Middle School Mathematics Materials Using Problems Created to Help Students
Progress from Informal to Formal Mathematical Reasoning. In L.P. Leutzinger, & S.P. Smith (Eds.)
Mathematics in the Middle (pp. 107-119). Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Working Groups
Hodges, T.E., Roy, G. J., & Tyminski, A. M. (Eds.). (2018). Proceedings of the 40th annual meeting of
the North American Chapter of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics
Education. Greenville, SC: University of South Carolina & Clemson University.
1458
Shvarts, A., & Abrahamson, D. (2018, April). Towards a complex systems model of enculturation: A dual eye-
tracking study. Paper presented at the annual conference of the American Educational Research Association
(Special Interest Group: Learning Sciences), NYC, April 13-17.
Sinclair, N. (2014). Generations of research on new technologies in mathematics education. Teaching Mathematics
and its Applications, 33(3), 166-178.
Soto-Johnson, H., Hancock, B., & Oehrtman, M. (2016). The Interplay Between Mathematicians’ Conceptual and
Ideational Mathematics about Continuity of Complex-Valued Functions. International Journal of Research in
Undergraduate Mathematics Education, 2(3), 362-389.
Smith, C., King, B., González, D., Garon, T., Barr, K., Craft, J., & Scrivani, S. (April, 2016). Patterns of
collaborative actions in a body-based mathematics task. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American
Educational Research Association, Washington, DC.
Stevens, R. (2012). The missing bodies of mathematical thinking and learning have been found. Journal of the
Learning Sciences, 21(2), 337-346.
Tunner, M., & Fauconnier, G. (1995). Conceptual integration and formal expression. Metaphor and Symbol, 10(3),
183-204.
Uttal, D. H., Meadow, N. G., Hand, L. L., Lewis, A. R., Warren, C., & Newcombe, N. S. (2009). Training spatial
skills: What works, for whom, and for how long. Psychological Bulletin.
Varela, F. J., Rosch, E., & Thompson, E. (1992). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience.
MIT press.
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience.
Cambridge, MA: M.I.T. Press.
Walkington, C., Nathan, M., Wolfgram, M., Alibali, M., & Srisurichan, R. (2014). Bridges and barriers to
constructing conceptual cohesion across modalities and temporalities: Challenges of STEM integration in the
precollege engineering classroom. In S. Purzer, J. Strobel, & M. Cardella (Eds.). Engineering in pre-college
Settings: Research into Practice (pp.183-210). West Lafayette, Indiana: Purdue University Press.
Williams-Pierce, C., Walkington, C., Landy, D., Lindgren, R., Levy, S., Nathan, M.J., & Abrahamson, D.
(accepted). Enabling and understanding embodied STEM learning. Pre-conference workshop submitted to the
2017 International Conference on Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning.
Williams-Pierce, C. (2016). Provoking mathematical play through hidden deep structures. In Looi, C. K., Polman,
J. L., Cress, U., and Reimann, P. (Eds.), Transforming Learning, Empowering Learners: The International
Conference of the Learning Sciences, Vol. 2 (pp. 1241-1242). Singapore: National Institute of Education,
Nanyang Technical University.
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 9(4), 625-636.
Worsley, M., & Blikstein, P. (2014). Using multimodal learning analytics to study learning mechanisms. In J.
Stamper, Z. Pardos, M. Mavrikis, & B. M. McLaren (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on
Educational Data Mining (pp. 431-432). London, UK: Institute of Education.
... Châtelet (1993) re-read key ideas of mathematics and physics considering abstract gestures, focusing on the "inventive" role of gesture to drive understanding as well as advancements in mathematics and physics. The research group by Ottmar et al. (2018) addresses embodiment of mathematics jointly with cognition. Starting from Châtelet's work, Freitas and Sinclair (2012) adopt the idea of gesture and diagrams to enhance mathematical teaching through embodiment. ...
Article
Full-text available
The comprehension of mathematical notions can profit from the personal background of a population of students, drawing upon their former knowledge in another field. In particular, the embodiment of mathematics provides us with a bridge to transfer competencies from music to intuitive understanding of non-trivial geometries and topologies. In this study, we analyse an experiment in which music composition students are introduced to the Klein bottle for the first time. Through the Ontological and Semiotic Approach (OSA), a conceptual framework in mathematics education, we evaluate the evolution from an initial intuition of the Klein bottle to a formal understanding of the object via musical renditions and verbal presentations to both neophyte and specialist audiences in mathematics.
... macrospace-scale objects). Researchers have investigated how interactions at different scales of space create opportunities for exploring mathematical concepts (Dimmel and Milewski 2019;Herbst and Boileau 2018;Kelton and Ma 2018;Ma 2016Ma , 2017Nemirovsky et al. 2012;Ottmar et al. 2018). Experiences with various scales of space are typically situated in the real, physical world. ...
Article
Full-text available
In this article, we examine spatial inscriptions marked in real or rendered spaces, rather than on two-dimensional surfaces, conceptualize spatial inscriptions from an inclusive materialist perspective and consider realizations of spatial inscriptions that are possible with emerging technologies (e.g. 3D pens, immersive virtual reality). We then describe two cases of immersive environments that allowed learners to make and interact with spatial inscriptions. Next, we analyze how movements of participant–environment–inscription assemblages realized diagrams. Our analysis highlights how varying scale and changing perspective can become resources for doing mathematical work with spatial inscriptions.
Article
Full-text available
In this article, we examine spatial inscriptions marked in real or rendered spaces, rather than on two-dimensional surfaces, conceptualize spatial inscriptions from an inclusive materialist perspective and consider realizations of spatial inscriptions that are possible with emerging technologies (e.g. 3D pens, immersive virtual reality). We then describe two cases of immersive environments that allowed learners to make and interact with spatial inscriptions. Next, we analyze how movements of participant–environment–inscription assemblages realized diagrams. Our analysis highlights how varying scale and changing perspective can become resources for doing mathematical work with spatial inscriptions.
Article
Full-text available
A rising epistemological paradigm in the cognitive sciences—embodied cognition—has been stimulating innovative approaches, among educational researchers, to the design and analysis of STEM teaching and learning. The paradigm promotes theorizations of cognitive activity as grounded, or even constituted, in goal-oriented multimodal sensorimotor phenomenology. Conceptual learning, per these theories, could emanate from, or be triggered by, experiences of enacting or witnessing particular movement forms, even before these movements are explicitly signified as illustrating target content. Putting these theories to practice, new types of learning environments are being explored that utilize interactive technologies to initially foster student enactment of conceptually oriented movement forms and only then formalize these gestures and actions in disciplinary formats and language. In turn, new research instruments, such as multimodal learning analytics, now enable researchers to aggregate, integrate, model, and represent students’ physical movements, eye-gaze paths, and verbal–gestural utterance so as to track and evaluate emerging conceptual capacity. We—a cohort of cognitive scientists and design-based researchers of embodied mathematics—survey a set of empirically validated frameworks and principles for enhancing mathematics teaching and learning as dialogic multimodal activity, and we synthetize a set of principles for educational practice.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
While recent work has begun to evaluate the efficacy of educational programming games, many common design decisions in these games (e.g., single player gameplay using touchpad or mouse) have not been explored for learning outcomes. For instance, alternative design approaches such as collaborative play and embodied interaction with tangibles may also provide important benefits to learners. To better understand how these design decisions impact learning and related factors, we created an educational programming game that allows for systematically varying input method and mode of play. In this paper, we describe design rationale for mouse and tangible versions of our game, and report a 2x2 factorial experiment comparing efficacy of mouse and tangible input methods with individual and collaborative modes of play. Results indicate tangibles have a greater positive impact on learning, situational interest, enjoyment, and programming self-beliefs. We also found collaborative play helps further reduce programming anxiety over individual play.
Article
Full-text available
Proportional reasoning is important and yet difficult for many students, who often use additive strategies, where multiplicative strategies are better suited. In our research we explore the potential of an interactive touchscreen tablet application to promote proportional reasoning by creating conditions that steer students toward multiplicative strategies. The design of this application (Mathematical Imagery Trainer) was inspired by arguments from embodied-cognition theory that mathematical understanding is grounded in sensorimotor schemes. This study draws on a corpus of previously treated data of 9–11 year-old students, who participated individually in semi-structured clinical interviews, in which they solved a manipulation task that required moving two vertical bars at a constant ratio of heights (1:2). Qualitative analyses revealed the frequent emergence of visual attention to the screen location halfway along the bar that was twice as high as the short bar. The hypothesis arose that students used so-called “attentional anchors” (AAs)—psychological constructions of new perceptual structures in the environment that people invent spontaneously as their heuristic means of guiding effective manual actions for managing an otherwise overwhelming task, in this case keeping vertical bars at the same proportion while moving them. We assumed that students’ AAs on the mathematically relevant points were crucial in progressing from additive to multiplicative strategies. Here we seek farther to promote this line of research by reanalyzing data from 38 students (aged 9–11). We ask: (1) What quantitative evidence is there for the emergence of AAs?; and (2) How does the transition from additive to multiplicative reasoning take place when solving embodied proportions tasks in interaction with the touchscreen tablet app? We found that: (a) AAs appeared for all students; (b) the AA-types were few across the students; (c) the AAs were mathematically relevant (top of the bars and halfway along the tall bar); (d) interacting with the tablet was crucial for the AAs’ emergence; and (e) the vast majority of students progressed from additive to multiplicative strategies (as corroborated with oral utterances). We conclude that touchscreen applications have the potential to create interaction conditions for coordinating action and perception into mathematical cognition.
Article
Full-text available
Abstract: Embodiment perspectives from the cognitive sciences offer a rethinking of the role of sensorimotor activity in human learning, knowing, and reasoning. Educational researchers have been evaluating whether and how these perspectives might inform the theory and practice of STEM instruction. Some of these researchers have created technological systems, where students solve sensorimotor interaction problems as cognitive entry into curricular content. However, the field has yet to agree on a conceptually coherent and empirically validated design framework, inspired by embodiment perspectives, for developing these instructional resources. A stumbling block toward such consensus, we propose, is an implicit disagreement among educational researchers on the relation between physical movement and conceptual learning. This hypothesized disagreement could explain the contrasting choices we witness among current designs for learning with respect to instructional methodology for cultivating new physical actions: Whereas some researchers use an approach of direct instruction, such as explicit teaching of gestures, others use an indirect approach, where students must discover effective movements to solve a task. Prior to comparing these approaches, it may help first to clarify key constructs. In this theoretical essay we draw on embodiment and systems literature as well as findings from our design research so as to offer the following taxonomy that may facilitate discourse about movement in STEM learning: (a) distal movement is the technologically extended effect of physical movement on the environment; (b) proximal movement is the physical movements themselves; and (c) sensorimotor schemes are the routinized patterns of cognitive activity that becomes enacted through proximal movement by orienting on so-called attentional anchors. Attentional anchors are goal-oriented phenomenological objects or enactive perceptions (“sensori-”) that organize proximal movement to effect distal movement (“-motor”). All three facets of movement must be considered in analyzing embodied learning processes. We demonstrate that indirect movement instruction enables students to develop new sensorimotor schemes including attentional anchors as idiosyncratic solutions to physical interaction problems. These schemes are by necessity grounded in students’ own agentive relation to the world while also grounding target content, such as mathematical notions. Significance: Engineering developments in computational technology have created unprecedented opportunities for industry to build and disseminate mathematics-education applications (“apps”). Thousands of these applications are now literally at the fingertips of any child who can access a tablet, smartphone, or personal computer with responsive touchscreen. Educational researchers could contribute to the quality of these ubiquitous consumer products by offering design frameworks informed by theories of learning. However, existing frameworks are derived from interaction theories drawing on epistemological assumptions that are no longer tenable, given the embodiment turn in the cognitive sciences. A proposed systemic reconceptualization of mathematical objects as grounded in sensorimotor schemes for material interaction offers educational designers heuristics for creating activities in which students learn by discovering motion patterns.
Article
Full-text available
Adopting Schiralli and Sinclair’s notions of conceptual mathematics (CM) and ideational mathematics (IM), we investigated mathematicians’ reasoning about continuity of complex-valued functions. While CM centers on formal mathematics as a discipline, IM focuses on how an individual perceives formal mathematics. There were four IM notions that the mathematicians used to convey the idea of continuity for complex-valued functions: control, topological features, preservation of closeness, and paths. The mathematicians’ IM tended to be grounded in their embodied experiences and espoused for pedagogical reasons, in preparation for other actions, or to assist their own reasoning. Some of the mathematicians’ IM metaphors conveyed a domain-first quality, which accounted for the domain of the function before mentioning any objects from the codomain. Given such metaphors did not capture the full structure of the epsilon-delta definition of continuity, the mathematicians transitioned to CM language in an effort to make their IM statements more rigorous. Our research suggests that while IM metaphors stemming from embodied experiences can serve as helpful tools for reasoning about continuity of complex-valued functions, one must be cognizant of ways in which the informal IM must be altered or extended to fully capture the CM. Given the pedagogical intent of many of the participants’ domain-first IM examples, we recommend that care be taken during instruction to deliberately elucidate where the IM is incomplete or fails to encapsulate the intricacies of the CM at hand.
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Due to a broad conceptual usage of the term embodiment across a diverse variety of research domains, existing embodied learning games and simulations utilize a large breadth of design approaches that often result in seemingly unrelated systems. This becomes problematic when trying to critically evaluate the usage and effectiveness of embodiment within existing designs, as well as when trying to utilize embodiment in the design of new games and simulations. In this paper, we present our work on combining differing conceptual and design approaches for embodied learning systems into a unified design framework. We describe the creation process for the framework, explain its dimensions, and provide examples of its use. Our design framework will benefit educational game researchers by providing a unifying foundation for the description, categorization, and evaluation of designs for embodied learning games and simulations.
Article
Full-text available
The combination of two methodological resources-natural user interface and multimodal learning analytics-is creating opportunities for educational researchers to empirically evaluate theoretical models accounting for the emergence of concepts from situated sensorimotor activity. Seventy-six participants (9-14 years old) solved tablet-based presymbolic manipulation tasks designed to foster grounded meanings for the mathematical concept of proportional equivalence. Data gathered in task-based semi-structured clinical interviews included action logging, eye-gaze tracking, and videography. Analysis of these data indicates that successful task performance coincided with spontaneous emergence of stable dynamical gaze path patterns soon followed by multimodal articulation of strategy. Significantly, gaze patterns included unmanipulated, non-salient screen locations. We present cumulative evidence that these gaze patterns served as "attentional anchors" mediating participants' problem solving. By way of further contextualizing our claim, we also present case studies from the various experimental conditions. We interpret the findings as enabling us to revisit, support, refine, and perhaps elaborate on seminal claims from Piaget's theory of genetic epistemology and in particular his insistence on the role of situated motor-action coordination in the process of reflective abstraction.
Article
Full-text available
Whereas emerging technologies, such as touchscreen tablets, are bringing sensorimotor interaction back into mathematics learning activities, existing educational theory is not geared to inform or analyze such learning. In particular, educational researchers investigating instructional interactions still need intellectual and methodological frameworks for conceptualizing, designing, facilitating, and analyzing how students’ immersive hands-on dynamical experiences become formulated within semiotic registers typical of mathematical discourse. We present case studies of tutor–student behaviors in a technologically enabled embodied-interaction learning environment, the Mathematical Imagery Trainer for Proportion. Drawing on ecological dynamics—a blend of dynamical-systems theory and ecological psychology—we examine how students develop effective sensorimotor schemes as solutions to interaction problems. We argue that in attempting to enact a new complex skill, learners may improve their performance by spontaneously constructing an attentional anchor—a real or imagined object, area, or other aspect or behavior of the perceptual manifold that facilitates motor-action coordination. We further argue that symbolic artifacts introduced into the arena may both mediate new affordances for students’ motor-action control and shift their discourse into explicit mathematical re-visualization of the environment. We thus implicate symbolic artifacts as ontological hybrids evolving from things you act with to things you think with. Students engaged in embodied-interaction learning activities are first attracted to symbolic artifacts as prehensible environmental features optimizing their grip on the world, yet in the course of enacting the improved control routines, the artifacts become frames of reference for establishing and articulating quantitative systems known as mathematical reasoning.
Book
The book establishes a common language for, and understanding of, embodiment as it applies to mathematical thinking, and to link mathematics education research to recent work in gesture studies, cognitive linguistics and the theory of embodied cognition. The purpose is to acknowledge the multimodal nature of mathematical knowing, and to contribute to the creation of a model of the interactions and mutual influences of bodily motion, spatial thinking, gesture, speech and external inscriptions on mathematical thinking, communication and learning.
Book
A new edition of a classic work that originated the “embodied cognition” movement and was one of the first to link science and Buddhist practices. This classic book, first published in 1991, was one of the first to propose the “embodied cognition” approach in cognitive science. It pioneered the connections between phenomenology and science and between Buddhist practices and science—claims that have since become highly influential. Through this cross-fertilization of disparate fields of study, The Embodied Mind introduced a new form of cognitive science called “enaction,” in which both the environment and first person experience are aspects of embodiment. However, enactive embodiment is not the grasping of an independent, outside world by a brain, a mind, or a self; rather it is the bringing forth of an interdependent world in and through embodied action. Although enacted cognition lacks an absolute foundation, the book shows how that does not lead to either experiential or philosophical nihilism. Above all, the book's arguments were powered by the conviction that the sciences of mind must encompass lived human experience and the possibilities for transformation inherent in human experience. This revised edition includes substantive introductions by Evan Thompson and Eleanor Rosch that clarify central arguments of the work and discuss and evaluate subsequent research that has expanded on the themes of the book, including the renewed theoretical and practical interest in Buddhism and mindfulness. A preface by Jon Kabat-Zinn, the originator of the mindfulness-based stress reduction program, contextualizes the book and describes its influence on his life and work.
Article
Learning algebra is difficult for many students, in part due to an emphasis on the memorization of abstract rules. Algebraic reasoners across expertise levels often rely on perceptual-motor strategies to make these rules meaningful and memorable. However, in many cases, rules are provided as patterns to be memorized verbally, with little overt perceptual support. Although most work on concreteness focuses on conceptual support through examples or analogies, we here consider notational concreteness—perceptual/motor supports that provide access into the dynamic structure of a representation itself. We hypothesize that perceptual support may be maximally beneficial as an initial scaffold to learning, so that later static symbol use may be interpreted using a dynamic perspective. This hypothesis meshes with other findings using concrete analogies or examples, which often find that fading these supports over time leads to stronger learning outcomes. In an experiment exploring this hypothesis, we compare gains from the fading out of dynamic concrete physical motion of symbols during instruction with the introduction of motion over the course of instruction. In line with our theoretical perspective, concreteness fading led to significantly higher achievement than concreteness introduction after Day 2 of the intervention.