ArticlePDF Available

EFL learners’ L2 achievement and its relationship with cognitive intelligence, emotional intelligence, learning styles, and language learning strategies

Taylor & Francis
Cogent Education
Authors:

Abstract

The purpose of this study is threefold: firstly, to explore the relationship between EFL learners’ cognitive intelligence, emotional intelligence, and language learning achievement, secondly, to find out the relationship between EFL learners’ language learning styles and strategies and their L2 achievement, and thirdly, to uncover the relationship between EFL learners’ emotional and cognitive intelligence and their use of learning styles and strategies. To this end, 188 Iranian EFL learners completed five different instruments, namely Raven’s Progressive Matrices, Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory, Kolb’s Learning Style Inventory, Strategy Inventory for Language Learning, and Final Test of English as a Foreign Language. The results of our study demonstrated that foreign language achievement was significantly correlated with IQ, three subdomains of EQ (interpersonal relationship, optimism, and problem-solving), and three learning strategies (cognitive, compensation, and social). Likewise, the findings manifested that emotional intelligence was significantly correlated with language learning strategies and learning styles. This study holds significant implications for curriculum developers, language policymakers, and educators to afford learners with critical learning opportunities to enhance their learning skills.
CURRICULUM & TEACHING STUDIES | RESEARCH ARTICLE
EFL learnersL2 achievement and its relationship
with cognitive intelligence, emotional
intelligence, learning styles, and language
learning strategies
Hamideh Taheri
1
, Firooz sadighi
1
*, Mohammad Sadegh Bagheri
1
and Mohammad Bavali
1
Abstract: The purpose of this study is threefold: firstly, to explore the relationship
between EFL learnerscognitive intelligence, emotional intelligence, and language
learning achievement, secondly, to find out the relationship between EFL learners
language learning styles and strategies and their L2 achievement, and thirdly, to
uncover the relationship between EFL learnersemotional and cognitive intelligence
and their use of learning styles and strategies. To this end, 188 Iranian EFL learners
completed five different instruments, namely Ravens Progressive Matrices, Bar-On
Emotional Quotient Inventory, Kolbs Learning Style Inventory, Strategy Inventory
for Language Learning, and Final Test of English as a Foreign Language. The results
of our study demonstrated that foreign language achievement was significantly
correlated with IQ, three subdomains of EQ (interpersonal relationship, optimism,
and problem-solving), and three learning strategies (cognitive, compensation, and
social). Likewise, the findings manifested that emotional intelligence was signifi-
cantly correlated with language learning strategies and learning styles. This study
holds significant implications for curriculum developers, language policymakers,
ABOUT THE AUTHORS
Hamideh Taheri is a Ph.D. candidate in TEFL at
the Islamic Azad University, Shiraz Branch. Her
main research interests include psycholinguis-
tics, foreign/second language teaching and
learning, and educational linguistics.
Firooz Sadighi is a professor of applied lin-
guistics. He has numerous publications in the
areas of educational linguistics, syntax, first/
second language acquisition, and second lan-
guage education.
Mohammad Sadegh Bagheri is an assistant
professor of TEFL at the Islamic Azad University,
Shiraz Branch. His research works mainly include
foreign/second language assessment, e-learn-
ing, and second language research methods.
Mohammad Bavali is an assistant professor of
TEFL at the Islamic Azad University, Shiraz
Branch. He has carried out research mainly in
the areas of foreign/second language
acquisition, second language education, and
dynamic assessment.
PUBLIC INTEREST STATEMENT
Language instructors are usually confronted with
situations wherein the same instruction might be
beneficial to some language learners, yet disad-
vantageous to others. This variation results from
learnersdifferent cognitive and emotional intelli-
gences on one hand and the use of learning styles
and language learning strategies on the other.
Having collected data through self-report ques-
tionnaires and an EFL final test, the authors in this
study investigated EFL learnersL2 achievement
regarding the above-mentioned variables. They
concluded that L2 achievement was more signif-
icantly correlated with IQ than EQ. Language
learning strategies also were observed to have
higher correlation with L2 achievement than
learning styles. Additionally, the findings mani-
fested that emotional intelligence, rather than
cognitive intelligence, enjoyed a significant rela-
tionship with learning styles and language learn-
ing strategies. The results acknowledged the need
of EFL learners to be afforded with ample learning
opportunities to hone their learning skills
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
© 2019 The Author(s). This open access article is distributed under a Creative Commons
Attribution (CC-BY) 4.0 license.
Received: 28 January 2019
Accepted: 05 August 2019
First Published: 17 August 2019
*Corresponding author: Firooz
Sadighi, Department of English lan-
guage, Shiraz Branch, Islamic Azad
University, Shiraz, IRAN (ISLAMIC
REPUBLIC OF)
E-mail: firoozsadighi@yahoo.com
Reviewing editor:
Yaser Khajavi, English, Salman Farsi
University of Kazerun, Iran, IRAN
(ISLAMIC REPUBLIC OF)
Additional information is available at
the end of the article
Page 1 of 21
and educators to afford learners with critical learning opportunities to enhance their
learning skills.
Subjects: Education - Social Sciences; Individual Differences/IQ; Language, Psychology of;
Learning; Cognition & Emotion; Educational Psychology; Language Teaching & Learning
Keywords: Cognitive intelligence; emotional intelligence; language learning strategies;
learning styles; L2 achievement
1. Introduction
Various processes e.g. observing, memorizing, and note-taking are involved in learning. These
processes do not account for how and why learning takes place (Brown, 2004). The innate
complication of the ideational processes of mans mind strangely fascinated scholars to reflect
on what goes in the brain (Hasanzadeh & Shahmohammadi, 2011).
Second/foreign language learning is noticeably more erratic than first language acquisition. For
this, scholars in the field of second/foreign language acquisition have recently touched upon EFL
learnersvariations. Closely associated with learnersacademic success, intelligence is among the
key factors that can optimize learning. Abstract intelligence has hitherto been regarded as playing
a significant role in problem-solving abilities. EFL learnerssocial and psychological difficulties and
more specifically their low degree of attainment have prompted scholars to assume that mental
intelligence is not the only requirement for a thriving life (Hasanzadeh & Shahmohammadi, 2011).
Owing to its primary significance in education, foreign language learning achievement requires
a keen appreciation of its defining attributes. EFL learners, as Mitchel and Myles (2004) point out,
progressively pursue a general path; yet the rate of their final achievement has conclusively been
proved to be different given their diverse characteristics. Learnersinternal aspects have hence
been high on the agenda in educational settings (Moafian & Ghanizadeh, 2009). As learners form
the focal point of learning performance, experiential approach to learning with humanism at its
core underline the fact that learners as human beings view internally and act externally at the
same time (Nunan, 2001). This indicates that both cognition and emotion are salient factors in
the second language acquisition process which should be attended to by foreign language teach-
ing authorities in their programs (Amer, 2003; Rastegar & Memarpour, 2009).
Learnersacademic achievement is dependent upon their capability and task completion. It
entails multiple aspects dealing primarily with the individualscognitive and emotional augmenta-
tion. Psychologists have long viewed cognitive intelligence as a great predictor of learnerseduca-
tional achievement. Along the same lines, researchers have recently shifted their attention from
cognitive intelligence (IQ) to emotional intelligence (EQ). The present study in the first place
attempts to probe into EFL learnerscognitive and emotional intelligence. It also aims to investi-
gate the relationship between EFL learnersachievement and their cognitive and emotional
intelligence. Furthermore, we examine whether learnersemotional intelligence and cognitive
intelligence are correlated with their language learning styles (LSs) and language learning strate-
gies (LLSs). More specifically, the study is guided by the following research questions:
(1) Is there any statistically significant relationship between EFL learnerscognitive intelligence
and their language achievement?
(2) Is there any statistically significant relationship between EFL learnersemotional intelligence
and their language achievement?
(3) Is there any statistically significant relationship between EFL learnerslanguage learning
styles and their language achievement?
(4) Is there any statistically significant relationship between EFL learnerslanguage learning
strategies and their language achievement?
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 2 of 21
(5) Are there any statistically significant relationships between EFL learnersemotional and
cognitive intelligence and their use of language learning styles and strategies?
2. Review of literature
2.1. IQ and EQ contribution to second/foreign language learning
As a combination of numerical, spatial, and verbal capabilities, cognitive intelligence involves
individualsabilities including imagination, memorization, perception, induction, and deduction
(Sternberg, 1996). Learning, as Brown (1994) has put forth, is interconnected with intelligence
and memory. He maintains that the most formidable obstacle on the way of learning a second
language pertains to the issue of recollection. Emotional intelligence is defined as the ability to
monitor ones own and othersfeelings and emotions, to discriminate among them, and to use this
information to guide ones thinking and actions(Salovey & Mayer, 1990). Emotional intelligence
emphasizes the interplay between emotion and cognition (Mayer, Salovey & Caruso, 2004).
A person who is emotionally intelligent is skillful enough to recognize, employ, understand, and
moderate their emotions (Mayer & Salovey, 1993).
The impact of IQ and EQ on educational achievement in general and language learning, in
particular, has long turned into a controversial issue among scholars. Academic achievement,
according to Neisser et al. (1996), Appelbaum and Tuma (1982), Ryan and Rosenberg (1983), and
Grossman and Johnson (1982), was acknowledged to be better predicted by IQ tests. Salehi and
Sadighi (2012) however, demonstrated a weak correlation between intelligence and reading
comprehension. In the same vein, Goleman (1995) and Salovey and Mayer (1990) demonstrated
that EQ more than IQ accounts for success in education. According to Goleman (1995, p. 34), EI
emerges more effective than IQ in distinguishing the individualstalent. Goleman (1998) mentions
that people can be sorted according to their IQ before they start a career; it determines which
fields or professions they can hold. Goleman (1998) has eventually asserted that EI maximizes the
effects of IQ and other technical activities.
Emotional components involved in L2 learning have recently been a matter of concern for many
instructors. Thus, a large number of studies have focused on the effect of emotions on learning
a foreign language (e.g., Arnold, 2011; Dewaele, 2015; Hogan et al., 2010; Méndez & Fabela,
2014; Méndez, Marin, & Hernandez, 2015; Soodmand Afshar & Rahimi, 2016). Despite the interac-
tion between emotion and learning, ELT studies have given more considerable attention to the
cognition and thought (Swain, 2013). Méndez (2011), however, believes that learning a foreign
language is emotionally directed. Building on this, Swain (2013) has highlighted the importance of
emotional processes in addition to cognitive processes in L2 acquisition and demonstrated that
cognition and emotion are indivisible.
To determine the significance of cognitive, affective, personality-related, and demographic
factors in anticipating learnerssecond language achievement, Onwuegbuzie, Slate, Paterson,
Watson, and Schwartz (2000) conducted a study on university students. They observed that both
cognitive and affective variables played pivotal roles in predicting foreign language learning
achievement. Emotional intelligence, as Chao (2003) points out, could universally denote academic
success. In the same vein, Fahim and Pishghadam (2007) purported that more than one aspect of
EI, namely stress management, intrapersonal, and general mood competencies could be effective
in escalating success in education. Their study indicated that academic achievement had a higher
correlation with verbal intelligence, a subpart of the IQ test, than the IQ itself. Although reading
comprehension ability was comparatively related to some aspects of EQ (intrapersonal, interper-
sonal, and stress management), Ghabanchi and Rastegar (2014) demonstrated that the global EQ
and its subsections could not significantly predict learnersreading comprehension proficiency.
They concluded that the correlation between IQ and reading comprehension was much stronger
than the correlation between universal EQ and reading comprehension skill. They suggested
developing learnersintelligence to boost reading comprehension. This can be accomplished by
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 3 of 21
exercising the active recollection of learners and employing some organizational performance
(Buschkuehl & Jaeggi, 2010). Experimental discoveries made concerning cognitive competence
remedy denote that educating learners to improve the skills of observing and self-managing,
handling troubles, and exchanging information can promote emotional intelligence (Carr, 2011)
Notwithstanding the fact that recent studies have centered on cognitive, emotional, and social
aspects of language learning, they manifest more inclination toward affection (e.g. Han & Hyland,
2015;Sato,2017; Swain, 2013). A great number of scholarly researchers revealed that emotions enjoy
a substantial function in SLA (e.g., Dewaele, 2011,2015;Imai,2010;Murphy&Dörnyei,2010;Swain,
2013). Thus, they have shifted their focus of attention fromnegativeemotionssuchaslanguagelearning
anxiety (e.g., Gkonou, Daubney, & Dewaele, 2017; Gregersen & MacIntyre, 2014)topositiveemotionsfor
better L2 achievement (e.g., Dewaele & MacIntyre, 2014;Murphy,2014;Oxford,2014). Cognition and
emotion were viewed by Swain (2013) as inseparable concepts conducive to L2 acquisition. In the same
vein, Poehner and Swain (2016) regard L2 development as a cognitive-emotive process.
2.2. Language learning strategies and academic achievement
Language learning strategies defined as specific actions, behaviors, steps, or techniquessuch as
seeking out conversation partners, or giving oneself encouragement to tackle a difficult language
task(Scarcella & Oxford, 1992, p. 63) are among the affective factors resulting in EFL learners
success. Among different taxonomies, Oxfords(1990) has gained the greatest consideration. She
has classified the strategies into memory, cognitive, and compensation as direct strategies and
affective, metacognitive, and social as indirect strategies. According to Jensen (2004), learning can
be made meaningful in case different lobes of the brain are provoked throughout the learning
practice. This is indicative of the significant function of neuropsychology in the learning process.
Thus, learners, as Muelasa and Navarroa (2015) put it, need to take on different types of cognitive
styles, capabilities, and skills while employing learning strategies.
Recently, one of the mounting concerns in the context of language teaching is how to assist low
achieving learners in mastering a second language successfully. During the last decades, LLSs have
been the main concern of many studies mostly investigating the relationship between the strategy
use and foreign language achievement. Studies carried out by Al-Qahtani (2013), Bromley (2013),
Charoento (2016), Fewell (2010), Habók and Magyar (2018), Loret (2011), Muelasa and Navarroa
(2015), Tejedor-Tejedor, González Salvador, and García SeñoráI (2008), Uslu, Sahin, and Odemis
(2016), and Wong and Nunan (2011) manifested a positive and meaningful relationship between
EFL learnersstrategy use and their academic achievement. According to Gharbavi and Mousavi
(2012) and Pei-Shi (2012), the more strategies learners employ, the higher level of achievement
they will possibly obtain. Gani, Fajrina, and Hanifa (2015) have proposed that speaking skill
development results from learnersconscious, intentional, and frequent utilization of appropriate
strategies. Language learning strategies make learning situations more active, enjoyable, and
learner-oriented that would lead to learners higher level of proficiency (Bromley, 2013).
The conformity of learning strategies with learning styles is still a debatable issue among
scholars (Tulbure, 2012). Further investigations are thus called for to provide insights into the
relationship between learning styles and strategies. Such insights can help students, teachers, and
even prospective researchers attempting to re-assess learning approaches to enhance learners
academic achievement.
2.3. Learning styles and academic achievement
Learning styles defined as a persons preferred approach to information processing, idea forma-
tion, and decision making(Kalsbeek, 1989, p. 32) are of paramount importance in education and
learner-centered pedagogy. According to Xu (2011), individuals diverge regarding their character,
culture, learning skills, and learning styles giving rise to learnersvarious levels of achievement.
Educationalists should keep learners abreast of their learning styles to provide an instructive
environment for them to choose the most appropriate teaching methods (Sprenger, 2003).
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 4 of 21
According to Xu (2011), more than seventy learning style models have been identified by scholars.
Some of these models are more frequently employed in second language learning. These learning
style models cannot entirely account for foreign language learning. Nilson (2003)delineatesdiverse
teaching models attending to the learners introduced in Kolbs Learning Styles Model (1984). At first,
divergers can employ their true experience while going through contemplative inspections.
Convergers try to find out how things work. They are interested in making small alterations in things
in order to make them more effective. Accommodators, on the other hand, prefer to act than to think.
They give priority to practical learning rather than lectures. Assimilators, as the fourth group, are
interested in learning materials which are structured and organized. They look for something to learn.
Thus, EFL learnersstyles identification and training, as Banner and Rayner (2000) put forth, lead
to more efficient, relevant, and meaningful learning. Wong and Nunan (2011) are of the opinion
that the adaptability of styles needs to be maintained by teachers in their educational activities. In
doing so, they should take on a variety of skills and practices well suited to different learners with
diverse learning preferences.
2.4. EI relationship with learning styles and learning strategies
There are abundant social and psychological factors showing variations among learners. Macaro
(2007) has proposed considering individual learnerspsychological and human dimensions to
acquire a foreign language. In this regard, various types of investigations were performed by
scholars leading to divergent findings. In this regard, Hasanzadeh and Shahmohammadi (2011)
and Aghasafari (2006) conducted a study on the relationship between emotional intelligence and
learning strategies. They found that a significant correlation existed between EI and the students
use of learning strategies. However, in a study conducted on 87 university students, Shakarami and
Khajehei (2015) discovered a low correlation between learnersemotional intelligence and lan-
guage learning strategies. Unlike the quantitative data, qualitative data gleaned from the study
indicated a strong relationship between the two variables in question. They concluded that in
planning instruction for a learner or groups of learners, a teacher may wish to target all the
intelligence of a specific domain to provide for the experiences that strengthen that particular
domain(p.236). The relationship between language learning strategies and trait emotional intelli-
gence was explored among post-graduate Iranian students studying overseas by Fouladi (2012).
She detected that no relationship existed between high and low EQ regarding language learning
strategies. Zafari and Biria (2014), on the other hand, demonstrated that emotional intelligence
and language learning strategy were significantly correlated and students with higher EI employed
more strategies than those with lower EI. They also discovered that the two groups of learners
significantly diverged regarding their type of strategy use.
With regard to the relationship between emotional intelligence and learning styles, Alavinia and
Ebrahimpour (2012) revealed that these two variables were positively and significantly correlated.
Their findings also indicated that an increase in one variable e.g. EI could cause an increase in
another, e.g. LS. The result of this study is in accord with the results obtained by such scholars as
Saklofske, Austin, and Minski (2003), and Saklofske, Austin, Galloway, and Davidson (2007).
Elizabeth and Chirayath (2013) did not observe a strong correlation between EI and LS. They,
however, stated that the impact of emotional intelligence on learning style was undeniable. In
other words, emotional intelligence was viewed as a major determinant of learning style efficacy
and consequence. According to Shahtalebi and Javadi (2014), the ability is associated with emo-
tional intelligence, whereas preferences deal with learning styles. Thus, it is common not to find
any relationship between learnersabilities and preferences.
Taken together, the foregoing discussion implies that the effect of EFL learnerscognitive and
emotional intelligence on such variables as language learning styles and strategies has provoked
controversy among EFL/ESL scholars. Consequently, it warrants further study to provide insights
into the variables in question and their relationship.
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 5 of 21
3. Methodology
3.1. Participants
A total of 188 university students majoring in different fields of study mainly engineering, law, art,
and accounting at Islamic Azad University, Bushehr Branch, took part in the study. Participants
were studying in their second semester of the academic year 20172018 mostly at intermediate
and low intermediate proficiency levels with their ages ranging from 19 to 35. They were selected
based on convenience sampling. As depicted in Table 1, 84 (44.7%) female and 104 (55.3%) male
participants took part in the study.
Table 1also illustrates that out of the 188 participants taking part in this study, 28 (14.9%) were
less than 20 years old while 154 (81.9%) were between 20 to 30 years old and merely 6 (3.2%)
participants were more than 30 years old. Hence, the majority of the participants were between 20
to 30 years old.
3.2. Research design
To obtain a deep insight into EFL learnerslanguage achievement and their cognitive intelligence,
emotional intelligence, learning styles, and language learning strategies, the researchers in this
study employed a quantitative research design.
3.3. Instrumentation
This study features five instruments to address the research questions. The instruments are
described at length.
3.3.1. Ravens progressive matrices
Ravens Progressive Matrices or Ravens Matrices is a nonverbal group test commonly applied in
educational settings to gauge learnerscognitive ability. In every item of the test, the test- taker is
supposed to find the missing component that completes a pattern of shapes. It is commonly made
up of 60 items primarily employed to estimate abstract reasoning. It also measures fluid intelli-
gence which shows the reasoning ability and problem-solving competency gained by making use
of current information without building on the skills and knowledge formerly achieved.
The internal consistency reliability estimate for APM total raw score was reported as .87 in the
standardization sample of 462 individuals. Test-retest reliability was reported as .91, and the
Cronbachs alpha reliability index turned out to be .82. The concurrent validity of the Ravens
Progressive Matrices as proved by correlations with Wechsler intelligence test was found to be .73.
3.3.2. Bar-On emotional quotient inventory
The second instrument employed in this study was the Bar-On EI test, otherwise known as the
Emotional Quotient Inventory (EQ-I). It is a self- report questionnaire assessing emotional and
Table 1. Distribution of gender and age
Frequency Percentage
Female 84 44.7
Gender Male 104 55.3
Total 188 100
<20 28 14.9
Age Group 2030 154 81.9
>30 6 3.2
Total 188 100
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 6 of 21
socially intelligent behavior, and emotional-social intelligence on a five-point Likert scale designed
by Bar-On in 1980 (Bar-On, 1997). Bar-Ons domains and sub-domains include the following:
(1) Intrapersonal skills (self-regard, emotional self-awareness, assertiveness, independence,
and self-actualization)
(2) Interpersonal skills (empathy, social responsibility, and interpersonal relationships)
(3) Adaptability (reality testing, flexibility, and problem-solving)
(4) Stress management (stress tolerance and impulse control)
(5) General mood (optimism and happiness) (pp. 4345)
In order to eliminate cross-cultural variations and avoid possible misunderstanding concern-
ing the content of the questionnaire, the translated Persian version of the inventory was
employed. The final version was declined into 90 items taking participants approximately
30 minutes to complete.
The Cronbachs alpha values obtained in this study for the subdomains of Bar-Ons emotional
intelligence questionnaire are presented in Table 2.
Following Table 2, the Cronbachs alpha values for the subdomains of emotional intelligence
were within the acceptable range of 0.700 to 0.921 confirming the reliability of the questionnaire.
3.3.3. Kolbs learning style inventory
The other instrument deployed in this study was Kolbs Learning Style Inventory (LSI) con-
structed in 1984. It was then modified in 2005, a broadly renowned measure to study
characteristics of various learning styles. The LSI is a 12-item self-report instrument aimed
to assess individualspriorities concerning specific learning styles, namely diverging, assim-
ilating, converging, and accommodating. Participants were required to rank order the state-
ments from 1 (the least you like) to 4 (the most you like) assessing how well they thought
each statement conformed to their learning processes (Smith & Kolb, 1996)invariouslearn-
ing conditions. Concrete experience (CE) and reflective observation (RO) are described as
diverging learning styles (Kolb, Boyatzis, & Mainemelis, 2000; Kolb & Kolb, 2005).
Assimilating learning styles consist of abstract conceptualization (AC) and reflective
Table 2. Reliability analysis of emotional intelligence subdomains
Emotional Intelligence
subdomains
Number of items Cronbachs alpha
Independence 6 0.762
Stress tolerance 6 0.784
Self-actualization 6 0.712
Emotional self-awareness 6 0.709
Reality testing 6 0.804
Interpersonal Relationship 6 0.921
Optimism 6 834
Self- reliance 6 0.784
Impulse Control 6 0.725
Flexibility 6 0.7
Social responsibility 6 0.835
Empathy 6 0.709
Assertiveness 6 0.81
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 7 of 21
observation (Kolb, 1984; Kolb et al., 2000). Abstract conceptualization and active experimen-
tation (AE) constitute converging learning styles (Kolb, 1984; Kolb et al., 2000). Concrete
experience and AE are identified as accommodating learning styles (Kolb, 1984; Kolb et al.,
2000). Participants had to take the reverse skills into account. They continuously chose
between these extreme points, that is, CE/RO (Diverging), AC/RO (Assimilating), AC/AE
(Converging), and CE/AE (Accommodating). The first statement of each row corresponds to
CE,thesecondtoRO,thethirdtoAC,andthefourthtoAE(Koob&Funk,2002). Their
responses indicated their learning preferences in different situations. Noteworthy to mention
is that the translated version of Kolbs learning style questionnaire was utilized in this study.
The Cronbachs alpha reliability indices are presented in Table 3. The Cronbachsalphavalues
for learnerslearning styles confirmed the reliability of the questionnaire.
3.3.4. Language learning strategy inventory
Strategy Inventory for Language Learning (SILL) was developed by Oxford in 1990 in order to
assess language EFL learners learning strategies. It is perhaps the most comprehensive classifi-
cation of learning strategies to date(Ellis, 1994, p. 539). Six strategies have been identified in the
SILL: (1) Memory (2) Cognitive (3) Compensation (4) Metacognitive (5) Affective, and (6) Social. This
50-item inventory is classified into two parts comprising of direct learning strategies with 29 items
and indirect learning strategies with 21 items. Direct strategies are classified into memory (9
items), cognitive (14 items), and compensation (6 items) strategies, while indirect strategies are
categorized into metacognitive (9 items), affective (6 items), and social (6 items) strategies. The
participants responded to the inventory on a five-point Likert scale representing what they often
did while learning a language.
As shown in Table 4, the Cronbachs alpha reliability indices of all the language learning
strategies were moderately beyond the cut-off point of 0.7 confirming the reliability of all six
types of strategies.
3.3.5. Final test of English as a Foreign language
In order to explore the learnersEnglish language achievement, a course-related test was
administered to the students at the end of the semester of the academic year 20172018 to
determine their final scores. The test included 60 multiple-choice items on vocabulary,
Table 3. Reliability analysis of different learning styles
Learning Styles Cronbachs alpha
Concrete Experience(CE) 0.85
Reflective Observation (RO) 0.82
Abstract Conceptualization(AC) 0.8
Active Experimentation (AE) 0.75
Table 4. Reliability analysis of different language learning strategies
Language Learning
Strategies
Number of Items Cronbachs alpha
Memory 9 .768
Cognitive 14 .707
Compensation 6 .768
Metacognitive 9 .825
Affective 6 .808
Social 6 .826
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 8 of 21
grammar, as well as reading comprehension passages selected from the studentstextbook.
It was developed and pilot-tested before its actual administration. The learnersachievement
scores were obtained right after completing all the questionnaires.
Thetestwasfoundtobebothreliableandvalid.Thecontentvalidityofthetestwaschecked
by two university professors teaching the same course. The reliability of the test obtained
through Cronbachs alpha turned out to be .924 which is a high index confirming the reliability
of the test.
3.3.5.1. Procedures. The Ravens APM was administered to learners in a separate session without
any time limit (up to 1 h). It was typically completed within 30 minutes. As the students were
promised to be informed about the results of the IQ test, they all appeared well- motivated.
The Bar-On EQ-I was administered to the students in another session. It took them about
30 minutes to accomplish. Kolbs learning style questionnaire was administered afterward. No
time limit was set for completing this inventory. It took them nearly 1015 minutes to complete.
The next inventory administered in the same session was Strategy Inventory for Language
Learning. To complete the inventory, the students were requested to read the statements carefully
and mark a tick by one of the options Always true of me,”“Usually true of me,”“Somewhat true of
me,”“usually not true of me,and never true of me.It took them about 1520 minutes to
complete the inventory. As the last instrument of this study, the final test of English as a foreign
language was administered to the students in a separate session at the end of the semester. It
took them about 45 minutes to complete. The scores obtained from the final test indicated their
level of English language achievement.
3.3.5.2. Data analysis. Statistical data analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics 24 to
address the research questions previously formulated. Descriptive statistics and inferential statis-
tics were computed to analyze the data collected.
3.3.5.3. Descriptive statistics. Descriptive data of the study as depicted in Table 5were used to
examine the variables under consideration in the study.
The results show the means (average of variablesscores), the standard deviation (the difference
of variablesscores and the mean), and the range of each variable describing minimum and
maximum values.
Table 5. Descriptive statistics of IQ, EQ, LS, LLS, and FS
Research variables N Minimum Maximum Mean Std.
Deviation
Cognitive Intelligence (IQ) 188 11 36 27.4 5.126
Emotional
Intelligence(EQ)
188 269 372 309.5914 20.15681
Learning Styles(LS) 188 1 4 2.46 1.096
Language learning
strategies(LLS)
188 105 213 151.5591 20.94908
L2 achievement (Final
Scores)(FS)
188 10 56 29.9043 11.3421
Valid N 188
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 9 of 21
3.3.5.4. Inferential statistics. In this section, each research question was examined either by
parametric or non-parametric statistical tests. The parametric tests are more precise, but they
require that the data be normally distributed. For this reason, the normality of the variables was
examined employing the Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test.
Table 6shows the results of the Kolmogorov-Smirnov Test. Since pvalues were greater than
0.05 (p > 0.05) for all variables (Final Scores, Emotional Intelligence, Cognitive Intelligence,
Language Learning Strategies, and Learning Styles) suggesting that the assumption of normality
was retained. Thus, parametric tests were used to analyze the research questions.
3.3.5.5. Research question 1. The scoring system used for cognitive intelligence questionnaire was
on the Likert scale. Thus, the participants could choose one of the options ranging from 1 to 7.
They were divided into two groups: a) those enjoying high cognitive intelligence, and b) those
possessing low cognitive intelligence. The Pearsons correlation test was employed to investigate
the first research question. The results are presented in Table 7.
As appeared in Table 7, the correlation between cognitive intelligence and language achieve-
ment was significant at the level of 0.01 (r = .25). This implies that learners with high cognitive
intelligence were considered high language achievers and vice versa.
3.3.5.6. Research question 2. The scoring system for emotional intelligence questionnaire was also
on a Likert scale. The Pearsons correlation test was utilized in order to answer the second research
question. Tables 8and 9demonstrate the results of descriptive statistics and the Pearson correla-
tion, respectively.
As displayed in Table 9, the correlation between EI and language achievement was not sig-
nificant at the level of 0.01 and 0.05. Nevertheless, the correlation between some of the sub-
variables of emotional intelligence (interpersonal relationship, optimism, and problem-solving) and
L2 achievement was significant at the level of 0.05. It indicates that learnerslanguage achieve-
ment enhances as the use of interpersonal relationship, optimism, and problem-solving increases.
3.3.5.7. Research question 3. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was applied to investigate the
relationship between EFL learnerslearning styles and their language achievement. Tables 10 and
11portray the results of descriptive statistics and the One-way ANOVA.
Following Table 11, the results of One-way ANOVA did not reflect any statistically significant
relationship between learnersL2 achievement and language learning styles (sig. = .654). In other
words, language achievement was not significant for different language learning styles.
3.3.5.8. Research question 4. The scoring system used for SILL questionnaire was on a Likert scale.
Pearsons correlation test was used to find out the relationship between learnerslanguage
achievement and their second language learning strategies. Tables 12 and 13illustrate the
descriptive analysis of six various language learning strategies and the Pearson correlation results,
respectively.
As presented in Table 13, the correlation between language learning strategies and language
achievement was not significant at the level of 0.01 and 0.05. Nonetheless, the correlation
between cognitive, compensation, and social strategies and language achievement was significant
at the level of 0.05. That is, EFL learnerslanguage achievement enhances as the use of cognitive,
compensation, and social strategies increases.
3.3.5.9. Research question 5. A one-way ANOVA was conducted to investigate if there were any
significant relationships between the studentslearning styles and their cognitive intelligence and
emotional intelligence. The results are summarized in Table 14.
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 10 of 21
Table 6. Kolmogorov-Smirnov test of normality for variables
Emotional
intelligence
Learning styles Language learning
strategies
Cognitive
intelligence
L2 achievement
N 188 188 188 188 188
330 2.46 151.56 27.4 29.9043
38.882 1.096 20.949 5.126 11.3421
Normal Parameters
a,b
Mean 0.19 0.237 0.145 0.215 0.174
Std. Deviation 0.061 0.077 0.052 0.063 0.054
Test Statistic
Asymp. Sig. (2-tailed)
a. Test distribution is Normal.
b. Calculated from data.
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 11 of 21
Table 14 depicts that the significance level of test for cognitive intelligence was higher than 0.05
(sig. = .681). Therefore, cognitive intelligence was not significant for different language learning
styles. In contrast, the significance level of test for emotional intelligence was less than 0.05
(sig. = .019) revealing that emotional intelligence is significant for different learning styles.
In order to examine whether any significant relationships existed between EFL learnersdifferent
language learning strategies and their cognitive vs. emotional intelligence, Pearsons correlation test
was performed. The pertaining results are presented in Table 15.
According to Table 15, the correlation between language learning strategies and cognitive intelli-
gence was not significant at the level of 0.01 and 0.05 (sig. = .934). However, the correlation between
language learning strategies andemotional intelligence was significant at the level of 0.01 (sig. = .009).
4. Discussion
The current research was undertaken to explore the relationship between cognitive and emotional
intelligence and foreign language achievement. It also aimed to find out the relationship between
EFL learnerslearning styles, language learning strategies, and their language achievement.
Eventually, the relationship between EFL learnersemotional intelligence and cognitive intelligence
and their use of learning styles and strategies was investigated. The results obtained from the
quantitative analysis of the instruments manifested that cognitive intelligence played a highly
influential role in the development of Iranian learnerslanguage achievement. This finding is in line
with those of Genesee (2006), Ghabanchi and Rastegar (2014), Ellis (2008), and Ghonchepour and
Mohaddam (2018) who demonstrated that intelligence is an instrumental factor in language
Table 7. Pearson correlation between cognitive intelligence and L2 achievement
cognitive intelligence
L2 achievement Pearson Correlation .251**
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.001
**. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
Table 8. Descriptive statistics of emotional intelligence subdomains
EQ Sub-domains N Minimum Maximum Std. Deviation
Problem-solving 188 17 30 2.80169
Happiness 188 11 24 2.15562
Independence 188 9 29 3.99791
Stress tolerance 188 14 25 2.67511
self-actualization 188 13 27 2.90824
Emotional self-awareness 188 14 26 2.94146
Reality testing 188 6 26 3.28543
InteInterpersonal
relationship
188 14 30 3.84138
Optimism 188 15 28 2.7064
Self-reliance 188 15 29 3.0247
Impulse control 188 6 30 5.78383
Flexibility 188 11 27 2.97494
Social responsibility 188 15 30 2.7386
Empathy 188 16 30 2.70592
Self-assertiveness 188 12 27 2.84471
Valid N (listwise) 188
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 12 of 21
Table 9. Pearson correlation between emotional intelligence subdomains and L2 achievement
Emotional Intelligence
Subdomains
L2 achievement
Pearson Correlation .145*
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.047
Happiness Pearson Correlation 0.021
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.774
Independence Pearson Correlation 0.043
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.557
Stress tolerance Pearson Correlation 0.014
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.851
Self-actualization Pearson Correlation 0.038
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.601
Emotional self-awareness Pearson Correlation 0.12
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.1
Reality testing Pearson Correlation 0.012
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.874
Interpersonal relationship Pearson Correlation .174*
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.017
Optimism Pearson Correlation .204**
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.005
Self-reliance Pearson Correlation 0.111
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.13
Impulse control Pearson Correlation 0.101
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.17
Flexibility Pearson Correlation 0.003
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.967
Social responsibility Pearson Correlation 0.137
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.061
Empathy Pearson Correlation 0.013
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.855
Assertiveness Pearson Correlation 0.047
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.52
Emotional Intelligence Pearson Correlation 0.089
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.225
*. Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
**. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
Table 10. Descriptive statistics of EFL learnerslearning styles
Learning styles Frequency Percent
Valid Converging (AC and AE) 52 27.7
Diverging (CE and RO) 32 17
Assimilating (AC and RO) 68 35.1
Accommodating (CE and AE) 36 19.1
Total 188 100
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 13 of 21
achievement. Our findings revealed that total emotional intelligence was not significantly corre-
lated with language achievement. This is consistent with the finding obtained by Berenji (2010)
and Woitaszewski and Aalsma (2004) who discovered that emotional intelligence did not con-
tribute significantly to the academic achievement of EFL learners. Notwithstanding the insignif-
icant relationship between the whole emotional intelligence and language achievement, some
sub-variables of emotional intelligence were proved to be correlated with language learning. This is
commensurate with the findings uncovered by Salovey and Grewal (2005), Pishghadam (2009),
Table 11. Results of One-way ANOVA for L2 achievement and language learning styles
Sum of
Squares
Df Mean Square F Sig.
Between Groups 201.515 3 70.172 0.541 0.654
Within Groups 23,845.762 184 129.597
Total 24,056.277 187
Table 12. Descriptive analysis of EFL learnerslanguage learning strategies
Language Learning
strategies
N Minimum Maximum Mean St. Deviation
188 15 41 26.9468 5.1799
Memory 188 23 67 42.2128 8.34404
Cognitive 188 9 30 18.4894 4.25081
Compensation 188 16 45 32.5319 6.74945
Metacognitive 188 7 21 14.0532 3.76976
Affective 188 7 30 17.7128 4.09675
Social
Valid N (listwise) 188
Table 13. Correlations between language learning strategies and L2 achievement
Language Learnin Strategies L2 achievement
Memory Pearson Correlation 0.055
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.457
Cognitive Pearson Correlation .247**
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.001
Compensation Pearson Correlation .256**
Metacognitive Sig. (2-tailed) 0
Pearson Correlation 0.045
Affective Sig. (2-tailed) 0.542
Pearson Correlation 0.076
Social Sig. (2-tailed) 0.302
Pearson Correlation .174*
Language Learning Strategies Sig. (2-tailed) 0.017
Pearson Correlation 0.101
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.169
**. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
*. Correlation is significant at the 0.05 level (2-tailed).
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 14 of 21
Berenji (2010), and Farahani and Shahbazi (2019) who manifested a number of EQ components as
positively affecting learnerssecond language achievement.
To justify the results of the study in terms of the significance of IQ and non-significance of the
whole EQ in predicting language achievement, it can be pointed out that owing to the teacher-
centeredness of educational system in Iran, learnersemotional intelligence are not taken into
much consideration by EFL educators during instruction. Further, the trainability of EQ (Lotfi
Kashani, Lotfi Azimi, & Vaziri, 2012), the inseparability of cognition and emotion (Swain, 2013),
and the impact of affection on cognitive processes (Heydarnejad & Ebrahimi, 2019) are neglected
and not attended to by most EFL teachers in Iranian contexts.
The results of the study also illustrated that all types of language learning strategies were not
significantly correlated with learnerssecond language achievement. The same was uncovered by
Bialystok (1981), and Mullins (1992). However, not all the sub-variables were found unrelated to
language success but among six strategies of language learning three strategies were considered
strong predictors of language achievement. These results are in conflict with the findings uncovered by
Soodmand Afshar, Tofighi, and Hamazavi (2016), Hong-Nam and Leavel (2006), and Ghonchepour and
Moghaddam (2018) who have established that strategy use plays a crucial role in learning a foreign
language and is also a strong predictor of language achievement. The finding of the study regarding
the significant relationship of merely three strategies among six components in learning a second
language can be justified on the ground that strategy use depends on such factors as learning
situation, learnerscapabilities, and language skill. Thus, not all the strategies could prove effective
in the same way. Further, learnerslack of awareness and sufficient background knowledge concerning
learning strategies may hinder their utilization of appropriate strategies.
In the same vein, the current study reflected no statistically significant correlation between EFL
learnerslearning styles and their L2 achievement relationship. This finding agrees with the results
obtained by a number of researchers such as Bailey, Onwuegbuzie, and Daley (2000), Bicer (2014),
Busato,Prins,Elshout,andHamaker(2000), Soodmand Afshar et al. (2016), and Yildirim, Cevat Acar,
Bull, and Sevinc (2008) who demonstrated no significant relationship between learning styles and L2
achievement. One line of explanation might justify the results regarding the insignificant correlation
Table 14. Results of one-way ANOVA for cognitive intelligence, emotional intelligence, and
language learning styles
Sum of squares Df Mean square F Sig.
Cognitive intelligence Between Groups 39.985 3 13.328 .503 .681
Within Groups 4873.292 184 26.485
Total 4913.277 187
Emotional intelligence Between Groups 3957.861 3 1317.620 3.400 .019
Within Groups 71,298.053 184 387.489
Total 75,250.915 187
Table 15. Correlations between cognitive intelligence, emotional intelligence, and language
learning strategies
language learning strategies
Cognitive intelligence Pearson Correlation 0.006
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.934
Emotional intelligence Pearson Correlation .189**
Sig. (2-tailed) 0.009
**. Correlation is significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 15 of 21
between LSs and language achievement. In point of fact, educatorslimited knowledge of learning styles
efficacy and up-to-date teaching methods have made them not to be mindful of the learning styles of
their learners during instruction. Additionally, learners lack of awareness and knowledge of their own
learning styles and preferences prevent them from perceiving the learning styles required for better
achievement.
Furthermore, the current study demonstrated a significant relationship between EQ and language
learning styles and strategies. These findings disagree with the results obtained by Shahtalebi and
Javadi (2014), Giadenaka (2008), and Suliman (2010) who demonstrated a weak relationship or even
no relationship between learning styles and language achievement. However, in line with the results
of the current study, Alavinia and Mollahossein (2012), Garcia-Fernandez et al. (2015), Hasanzadeh
and Shahmohamadi (2011), and Soodmand Afshar et al. (2016), and Zafari and Biria (2014) demon-
strated a significant relationship between LLSs and language achievement. They revealed that
learners with higher emotional intelligence applied more strategies than those with lower emotional
intelligence. The rationale behind the findings of the study in terms of the significant relationship
between learnersEQ and their use of LLSs and LSs lies in the fact that emotions not only generate
learnersinclination to perform but also impel them to act physically. Further, positive emotions lead
to the learnersproductivity, new potentiality, and opinions concerning social, mental, and physical
promotions. Negative emotions, in contrast, restrict learnersrepertoire of thinking and performing.
As far as the results of the study are concerned, EFL learnerscognitive intelligence was discovered
to be correlated neither with learning styles nor with language learning strategies. However, this does
not underestimate the significance and value of cognitive intelligence. Conversely, as the first finding
of the study demonstrated, cognitive intelligence plays a fundamental role in university learners
language achievement. The significance of cognitive intelligence in language achievement on the one
hand and the high correlation between emotional intelligence and language learning strategies and
learning styles, on the other hand, elucidates the fact that both types of intelligence are integral to
language acquisition. Along the same lines, Ghonchepour and Moghaddam (2018) stress the impact of
intelligence on language learning. Nonetheless, they hold the degree to which intelligence influences
language learning is not specified. In addition to cognition, a number of emotional factors such as
happiness, independence, self-awareness, assertiveness, empathy, etc. can also play a pivotal role in
learnersutilization of LLSs and LSs and thereby learning a foreign or second language.
The current study thus illuminates the fact that cognitive intelligence, despite being indispensable
in L2 achievement, is not sufficient. Developing emotional intelligence leads the learners to attain
their great goal of L2 learning more smoothly as it leads to productive thought as well as reasonable
performance (Nelson & Low, 2006). The skills associated with emotional intelligence can synchronize
cognitive intelligence with emotional intelligence leading to practical performance. Also, they assist
learners in coping with the challenges they encounter in their surroundings.
5. Conclusion and implications
The current study investigated the relationship between cognitive intelligence and emotional intelli-
gence and learnerslanguage achievement. The results of the study demonstrated that language
achievement possessed a significant correlation with cognitive intelligence, yet a nonsignificant
relationship with total emotional intelligence. Among 15 subdomains of emotional intelligence, only
three i.e. interpersonal relationship, optimism, and problem-solving manifested a significant relation-
ship with language achievement. Thus, teachers should take the EQ sub-variables most effective in
learning a foreign language into consideration to provide learners with the necessary assistance and
emotional training. This will raise their awareness of the importance of emotional intelligence in
learning a foreign language in addition to cognitive intelligence.
The findings of the study manifested no statistically significant relationship between learners
language achievement and their utilization of all types of language learning strategies and
learning styles. However, among the six types of language learning strategies, merely cognitive,
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 16 of 21
compensation, and social strategies were found highly correlated with language achievement. EFL
teachers, thus, should impart the knowledge of learning strategies to language learners. Language
learners require teachersguidance and instruction to learn how to learn. In so doing, educators
have to train and empower learners to plan for their own learning and react to what they have
achieved. This will prepare individuals for independent language learning.
In respect of the relationship between EFL learnersemotional and cognitive intelligence and their
use of language learning styles and language learning strategies, the findings manifested that
emotional intelligence, unlike cognitive intelligence, was significantly correlated with language learn-
ing strategies and learning styles. EFL teachers should thus refrain from using their comfort zone and
try a diversity of styles and strategies conducive to successful language learning. Moreover, they
should do their best to extend the learnersrepertoire of styles and strategies, identify their various
types, acquaint learners with various tasks related to each type, and instruct them to capitalize on the
necessary activities and skills properly.
Additionally, curriculum developers, language policymakers, and educators should afford learners
important learning opportunities to hone their learning skills. Affective strategies, according to Oxford
(1990), are instrumental in generating an emotional atmosphere which seems to be appropriate for the
classroom. By making use of many tasks and practices, learners could overcome their negative attitudes
towards cultural issues, shyness, and vagueness. Learners should also be motivated by teachers to take
risks in learning a second language. These skills and many others might assist students to enhance their
emotional intelligence. This mission, as Vieira (2003) points out, could be achieved by prevailing over the
curriculum constraints present in the learning environment. Language instructors could also make the
classroom environment as enjoyable and stimulating as possible by utilizing a variety of techniques such
as second language learning practices and stimulating tasks and games.
The results of the current study could carry important implications for curriculum developers and
syllabus designers to have an effective collaboration with English teachers. Through this coopera-
tion, they could dedicate a considerable amount of time in the curriculum to teachers so as to
familiarize learners with language learning strategies in EFL classrooms. This collaboration could
also lead to the development of textbooks and materials that conform to learnersemotional
intelligence, language learning strategies, and learning styles.
The results of the present study might be of great assistance to educators to opt for various sorts
of well-suited teaching materials to meet the requirements of language learners enjoying differing
capabilities. Thus, educationalists and program developers are required to carry out needs analysis
before specifying instructional materials or textbooks. This will allow instructors to base their
proposed tasks and activities upon learnerslearning requirements. Educationalists, thus, can
design and develop textbooks and materials which highlight language learning strategies and
learning styles that best suit EFL learnersemotional intelligence.
The findings of the study may also contribute to teachers to modify their instruction, design
appropriate syllabuses and materials, afford considerable learning opportunities, and specify
appropriate tasks and assignments with a view to promoting EFL learnersacademic achievement.
Funding
The authors received no direct funding for this research.
Author details
Hamideh Taheri
1
E-mail: taherih86@yahoo.com
ORCID ID: http://orcid.org/0000-0002-0323-9258
Firooz sadighi
1
E-mail: firoozsadighi@yahoo.com
Mohammad Sadegh Bagheri
1
E-mail: firoozsadighi@yahoo.com
Mohammad Bavali
1
E-mail: taherih86@yahoo.com
1
Department of English language, Shiraz Branch, Islamic
Azad University, Shiraz, Iran.
Citation information
Cite this article as: EFL learnersL2 achievement and its
relationship with cognitive intelligence, emotional intelli-
gence, learning styles, and language learning strategies,
Hamideh Taheri, Firooz sadighi, Mohammad Sadegh
Bagheri & Mohammad Bavali, Cogent Education (2019), 6:
1655882.
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 17 of 21
References
Aghasafari, M. (2006). On the relationship between emo-
tional intelligence and language learning strategies.
Unpublished masters thesis, Allameh Tabatabai
University, Iran.
Alavinia, P., & Ebrahimpour, S. (2012). On the correlation
between emotional intelligence and learning styles:
The case of Iranian academic EFL learners. Theory
and Practice in Language Studies,2(6), 12911299.
Alavinia, P., & Mollahossein, H. (2012). On the correlation
between Iranian EFL learnersuse of metacognitive
listening strategies and their emotional intelligence.
International Education Studies,5(6), 189203.
Al-Qahtani, M. F. (2013). Relationship between English
language, learning strategies, attitudes, motivation,
and studentsacademic achievement. Journal of
Medical Education,5,1929.
Amer, A. A. (2003). Teaching EFL/ESL literature. The
Reading Matrix,3(2), 6373.
Appelbaum,A.S.,&Tuma,J.M.(1982). The relationship of
theWISC-Rtoacademicachievementinaclinical
population. Journal of Clinical Psychology,38, 401405.
Arnold, J. (2011). Attention to affect in language learning.
International Journal of English Studies,22(1), 1122.
Bailey, P., Onwuegbuzie, A. J., & Daley, C. E. (2000). Using
learning style to predict foreign language achieve-
ment at the college level. System,28(1), 115133.
Banner, G., & Ryan, S. (2000). Learning language and
learning style: Principles, process and practice.
Language Learning Journal,21,2137.
Bar-On, R. (1997). Bar-On Emotional Quotient Inventory
(EQ-i): Technical manual. Toronto, Canada: Multi-
Health Systems.
Berenji, S. (2010). The relationship between emotional
intelligence and students` academic achievements in
general EFL classes. The Journal of Applied
Linguistics,3(2), 5066.
Bialystok, E. (1981). The role of conscious strategies
in second language proficiency. Modern Language
Journal,65(1), 2435.
Bicer, D. (2014). The effect of studentsand instructors
learning styles on achievement of foreign language
preparatory school students. Procedia - Social and
Behavioral Sciences,141, 382386. doi:10.1016/j.
sbspro.2014.05.067
Bromley, P. (2013). Active learning strategies for diverse
learning styles: Simulations are only one method.
Political Science & Politics,46(4), 818822.
doi:10.1017/S1049096513001145
Brown, G. (2004). How students learn: A supplement to the
Routledge Falmer key guides for effective teaching.
Retrieved from www.routledgeeducation.com
Brown, H. D. (1994). Teaching by principles: An interactive
approach to language pedagogy. NJ: Prentice Hall
Regents.
Busato, V. V., Prins, F. J., Elshout, J. J., & Hamaker, C.
(2000). Intellectual ability, learning style, personality,
achievement, motivation and academic success of
psychology students in higher education. Personality
and Individual Differences,29(6), 10571068.
doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00253-6
Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2010). Improving intelli-
gence: A literature review. Swiss Medical Weekly,140
(1920), 266272.
Carr, A. (2011). Positive psychology: The science of happi-
ness and human strengths. Sussex: Routledge.
Chao, C. (2003). Foreign language anxiety and emotional
intelligence: A study of EF students in Taiwan.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, A & M University,
College Station, Texas.
Charoento, M. (2016). Individual learner differences and
language learning strategies. Contemporary
Educational Researches Journal,7,5772.
Dewaele, J.-M. (2011). The differences in self-reported use
and perception of the L1 and L2 of maximally pro-
cient bi- and multilinguals: A quantitative and quali-
tative investigation. International Journal of
Sociology of Language,208,2551.
Dewaele, J.-M. (2015). On emotions in foreign language
learning and use. The Language Teacher,39(3),
1315.
Dewaele, J.-M., & MacIntyre, P. D. (2014). The two faces of
Janus? Anxiety and enjoyment in the foreign lan-
guage classroom. Studies in Second Language
Learning and Teaching,4(2), 237274. doi:10.14746/
ssllt.2014.4.2.5
Elizabeth, N., & Chirayath, S. (2013). Emotional intelli-
gence and learning style and its strategies. Journal of
Business Management & Social Sciences Research
(JBM&SSR),2(6), 9295.
Ellis, R. (1994). The study of second language acquisition.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Ellis, R. (2008). The study of second language acquisition
(2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Fahim, M., & Pishghadam, R. (2007). On the role of emo-
tional, psychometric, and verbal intelligences in the
academic achievement of university students
majoring in the English language. Asian EFL Journal,
9, 240253.
Farahani, A. A. K., & Shahbazi, H. G. (2019). The rela-
tionship between the emotional intelligence of
Iranian EFL learners and their performance on the
listening section of IELTS. Journal of Language
Teaching and Research,10(3), 469476.
doi:10.17507/jltr.1003.09
Fewell, N. (2010). Language learning tsrategies and
English language proficiency: An investigation of
Japanese EFL university students. TESOL Journal,2,
159174.
Fouladi, M. (2012). The effects of emotional intelligence
on the choice of language learning strategies. Indian
Journal of Education and Information Management,1
(11), 518526.
Gani, S. A., Fajrina, D., & Hanifa, R. (2015). Students
learning strategies for developing speaking ability.
Studies in English Language and Education,2(1),
1628. doi:10.24815/siele.v2i1.2232
Garcia-Fernandez, J. M., Ingles, C. J., Suria, R., Lagos-San
Martin, N., Gonzalvez-Macia, C., Aparisi, D., &
Martinez-Monteagudo, M. C. (2015). Profiles of emo-
tional intelligence and learning strategies in
a sample of Chilean students. European Journal of
Psychology of Education,30(4), 437455.
doi:10.1007/s10212-015-0254-9
Genesee, F. (2006). The role of intelligence in second
language learning. Language Learning,26(2),
267280. doi:10.1111/j.1467-1770.1976.tb00277.x
Ghabanchi, Z., & Rastegar, R. (2014). The correlation of IQ
and emotional intelligence with reading
comprehension. The Reading Matrix,14(2), 135144.
Gharbavi, A., & Mousavi, S. A. (2012). Do language profi-
ciency levels correspond to language learning strat-
egy adoption? English Language Teaching,5(7),
110122. doi:10.5539/elt.v5n7p110
Ghonchepour, M., & Mohaddam, M. P. (2018). The role of
intelligence in learning English as a foreign language.
Language Learning,26(2), 267280.
Giadenaka, K. J. (2008). Learning styles and emotional
intelligence of the adult learner. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, Auburn University, Alabama.
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 18 of 21
Gkonou, C., Daubney, M., & Dewaele, J.-M. (2017). New
insights into language anxiety: Theory, research, and
educational implications. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York:
Bantam Books.
Goleman, D. (1998). Working with emotional intelligence.
London: Bloomsbury.
Gregersen, T., & MacIntyre, P. D. (2014). Capitalizing on
individual differences: From premise to practice.
Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Grossman, F. M., & Johnson, K. M. (1982). WISC-R factor
scores as predictors of WRAT performance:
A multivariate analysis. Psychology in the Schools,19,
465468. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1520-6807
Habók, A., & Magyar, A. (2018). The effect of language
learning strategies on proficiency, attitudes and
school achievement. Frontiers in Psychology,8, 2358.
doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02358
Han, Y., & Hyland, F. (2015). Exploring learner engage-
ment with written corrective feedback in a Chinese
tertiary EFL classroom. Journal of Second Language
Writing,30,3144. doi:10.1016/j.jslw.2015.08.002
Hasanzadeh, R., & Shahmohammadi, F. (2011). Study of
emotional intelligence and strategies. International
conference on education and educational psychol-
ogy (ICEEPSY). Procedia - Social and Behavioral
Sciences,29, 18241829. doi:10.1016/j.
sbspro.2011.11.430
Heydarnejad, T., Ebrahimi, M. R., & Adel, S. M. R. (2019).
The influence of applying emotion-based language
instruction in teaching oral skills to EFL learners.
International Journal of Instruction,12(2), 275288.
doi:10.29333/iji.2019.12218a
Hogan, M. J., Parker, J. D., Wiener, J., Watters, C.,
Wood, L. M., & Oke, A. (2010). Academic success in
adolescence: Relationships among verbal IQ, social
support and emotional intelligence. Australian
Journal of Psychology,62(1), 3041. doi:10.1080/
00049530903312881
Hong-Nam, K., & Leavel, A. G. (2006). Language learning
strategy use of ESL students in an intensive English
learning context. System,34(3), 399415.
doi:10.1016/j.system.2006.02.002
Imai, Y. (2010). Emotions in SLA: New insights from col-
laborative learning for an EFL classroom. The Modern
Language Journal,94(2), 278292. doi:10.1111/
modl.2010.94.issue-2
Jensen, E. (2004). Cerebro y aprendizaje: competencias
e implicaciones educativas. Madrid: Narcea.
Kalsbeek, D. H. (1989). Linking learning style theory with
retention research: The TRAILS project. Association
for Institutional Research,32,17.
Kolb, A., & Kolb, D. (2005). Learning style and learning
space: Enhancing experiential learning in higher
education. Academy of Management Learning &
Education,4(2), 193212. doi:10.5465/
amle.2005.17268566
Kolb, D. (1984). Experiential learning: Experience as the
source of learning and development. Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Kolb, D. A., Boyatzis, R. E., & Mainemelis, C. (2000).
Experiential learning theory research and new direc-
tions. In R. J. Sternberg & F. Zhang (Eds.), Perspectives
on cognitive, learning, and thinking styles (pp.
193210). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Koob, J, & Funk, J. (2002). Kolb's learning style inventory:
issues of reliability and validity. Research on Social
Work Practice,12(2), 293-308.
Loret, J. E. (2011). Learning styles and strategies for
academic achievement of the students of the uni-
versity Peru. Rev Learn Style,8(8), 140.
LotKashani, F., Lotfi Azimi, A., & Vaziri, S. H. (2012).
Relationship between emotional intelligence and
educational achievement. Procedia - Social and
behavioral sciences (Vol. 69, pp. 12701275).
Macaro, E. (2007). Learning strategies in foreign
and second language classrooms. London:
Continuum.
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1993). The intelligence of
emotional intelligence. Intelligence,17, 443.
doi:10.1016/0160-2896(93)90010-3
Mayer, J. D, Salovey, P, & Caruso, D. R. (2004). emotional
intelligence: Theory, findings, and implications.
Psychological Inquiry,15(3), 197215.
Méndez, M. G. (2011). The motivational properties of
emotions in foreign language learning. Colombian
Applied Linguistics Journal,13,4359. doi:10.14483/
22487085.3764
Méndez, M. G., Marin, A., & Hernandez, I. (2015).
Emotional experiences revealed by foreign language
students at the UQROO. In M. Méndez (Ed.), Emotions
reported by English language teaching major students
in Mexico (pp. 147169). Chetumal, Q. Roo:
Universidad de Quintana Roo.
Méndez, M. G., & Fabela, M. (2014). Emotions and their
effects in a language learning Mexican context.
System,42, 298307. doi:10.1016/j.
system.2013.12.006
Mitchel, R., & Myles, F. (2004). Second language learning
theories. London: Arnold.
Moafian, F., & Ghanizadeh, A. (2009). The relationship
between Iranian EFL teachersemotional intelligence
and their self-efficacy in language institutes. System,
37(4), 708718. doi:10.1016/j.system.2009.09.014
Muelasa, A., & Navarroa, E. (2015). Learning strategies
and academic achievement. Procedia - Social and
Behavioral Sciences,165, 217221. doi:10.1016/j.
sbspro.2014.12.625
Mullins, P. (1992). Successful English language learning
strategies of students enrolled in the faculty of arts,
Chulalongkorn University, Bangkok, Thailand.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, United States
International University, San Diego, CA.
Murphy, R. S., & Dörnyei, Z. (2010). Where does psychol-
ogy and second language acquisition research con-
nect? An interview with Zoltán Dörnyei. The
Language Teacher,34(2), 1923.
Murphy, V. (2014). Second language learning in early
school years: Trends and contexts. Oxford: Oxford
University Press.
Neisser, U., Boodoo, G., Bouchard, T. I., Wade Boykin, A.,
Brody, N., Stephen, J., Urbina, S. (1996).
Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American
Psychologist,51,77101.
Nelson, D., & Low, G. (2006). Emotional intelligence and
college success: A research h-based assessment and
intervention model. In J. Cassidy, A. Martinez, &
C. Swift (Eds.), Supporting student success (pp.
236247). Corpus Christi, TX: Texas A & M University-
Corpus Christi.
Nilson, L. B. (2003). Teaching at its best: A research-based
resource for college instructors (2nd ed.). Bolton:
Heinle & Heinle.
Nunan, D. (2001). Second language acquisition. In
R. Carter & D. Nunan (Eds.), The Cambridge guide
to teaching English to speakers of other languages
(pp. 8792). Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Onwuegbuzie, A. J., Slate, J., Paterson, F., Watson, M., &
Schwartz, R. (2000). Factors associated with under-
achievement in educational research courses.
Research in the Schools,7,5365.
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 19 of 21
Oxford, R. L. (1990). Language learning strategies: What
every teacher should know. Boston, MA: Heinle and
Heinle.
Oxford, R. L. (2014). What we can learn about strategies,
language learning, and life from two extreme cases:
The role of well-being theory. Studies in Second
Language Learning and Teaching,4(4), 237274.
doi:10.14746/ssllt.2014.4.4.2
Pei-Shi, W. (2012). The effect of learning styles on learn-
ing strategy use by EFL learners. Journal of Social
Sciences,8((2)), 230234. doi:10.3844/
jssp.2012.230.234
Pishghadam, R. (2009). A quantitative analysis of the
relationship between emotional intelligence and for-
eign language learning. Electronic Journal of Foreign
Language,6(1), 3141.
Poehner, E., & Swain, M. (2016). L2 development as
cognitive-emotive process. Language and
Sociocultural Theory,3(2), 219241. doi:10.1558/lst.
v3i2.32922
Rastegar, M., & Memarpour, S. (2009). The relationship
between emotional intelligence and self-efficacy
among Iranian EFL teachers. International Journal
of Educational Technology and Applied Linguistics
(system),37,700707. doi:10.1016/j.
system.2009.09.013
Ryan, J. J., & Rosenberg, S. J. (1983). Relationship
between WAIS-R and a wide range achievement test
in a sample of mixed patients. Perceptual and Motor
Skills,56(2), 623626. doi:10.2466/
pms.1983.56.2.623
Saklofske, D. H., Austin, E. J., Galloway, J., & Davidson, K.
(2007). Individual differences correlate of
health-related behaviors: Preliminary evidence for
links between emotional intelligence and coping.
Personality and Individual Differences,42, 491502.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.08.006
Saklofske, D. H., Austin, E. J., & Minski, P. S. (2003). Factor
structure and validity of a trait emotional intelligence
measure. Personality and Individual Differences,34,
702721. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00056-9
Salehi, M., & Sadighi, F. (2012). The relationship between
intelligence and foreign language learning, and the
role of practice. Journal of Language, Culture, and
Translation (LCT),1(1), 3348.
Salovey, P., & Grewal, D. (2005). The science of emotional
intelligence. Current Directions in Psychological
Science,14(6), 281285. doi:10.1111/j.0963-
7214.2005.00381.x
Salovey, P., & Mayer, J. D. (1990). Emotional intelligence.
Imagination, Cognition, and Personality,9, 185211.
doi:10.2190/DUGG-P24E-52WK-6CDG
Sato, M. (2017). Interaction mindsets, interactional
behaviors, and L2 development: An affective-social-
cognitive model. Language Learning,67(2), 249283.
doi:10.1111/lang.12214
Scarcella, R. C., & Oxford, R. L. (1992). The tapestry of
language learning: The individual in the communica-
tion classroom. Boston: Heinle.
Shahtalebi, B., & Javadi, H. (2014). Relationship between
emotional intelligence and learning styles of
students. Journal of Applied Environmental and
Biological Sciences,4(2), 245251.
Shakarami, A., & Khajehei, H. (2015). How emotional
intelligence and language learning strategies inter-
act in an EFL setting. International Journal of Applied
Linguistics & English Literature,4(2), 229237.
Smith, D, & Kolb, D. (1996). User guide for the learning
style inventory: A manual for teachers and trainers.
Boston: McBer and Company.
Soodmand Afshar, H., & Rahimi, M. (2016). Reflective
thinking, emotional intelligence, and speaking ability
of EFL learners: Is there a relation? Thinking Skills and
Creativity,19,9
7111. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2015.10.005
Soodmand Afshar, H., Tofighi, S., & Hamazavi, R. (2016).
Iranian EFL learnersemotional intelligence, learning
styles, strategy use, and their L2 achievement. Issues
in Educational Research,26(4), 635.
Sprenger, M. (2003). Differentiation through learning styles
and memory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.
Sternberg, R. J. (1996). Cognitive Psychology. New York,
NY: Harcourt Brace.
Suliman, W. A. (2010). The relationship between learning
style, emotional intelligence, and academic success
of the undergraduate nursing student. Journal of
Nursing Research,18(2), 136143. doi:10.1097/
JNR.0b013e3181dda797
Swain, M. (2013). The inseparability of cognition and
emotion in second language learning. Language
Teaching,46, 195207. doi:10.1017/
S0261444811000486
Tejedor-Tejedor, F. J., González Salvador, G., & García
SeñoráI, M. M. (2008). Attentional strategies and
academic performance in secondary school students.
Latin American Journal of Psychology,40(1), 123132.
Tulbure, C. (2012). Learning styles, teaching strategies
and academic achievement in higher education: A
cross-sectional investigation. Procedia - Social and
Behavioral Sciences,33, 398402. doi:10.1016/j.
sbspro.2012.01.151
Uslu, M. E., Sahin, E., & Odemis, I. S. (2016). The effect of
language learning strategies on academic
achievement. Journal of Educational and
Instructional Studies in the World,6(3), 7378.
Vieira, F. (2003). Addressing constraints on autonomy in
school contexts: lessons from working with teachers.
In Palfreyman D. & Smith R.C. (Eds.), Learner auton-
omy across cultures: Language education perspec-
tives (pp. 220-239). Basinstoke: Palgrave, Mc Millan.
Woitaszewski, S. A., & Aalsma, M. C. (2004). The contri-
bution of emotional intelligence to the social and
academic success of gifted adolescents as measured
by the multifactor emotional intelligence scale -
adolescent version. Roeper Review,27(1), 2530.
doi:10.1080/02783190409554285
Wong,L.L.C.,&Nunan,D.(2011). The learning styles
and strategies of effective lan guage learners.
System,39(2), 144163. doi:10.1016/j.
system.2011.05.004
Xu, W. (2011). Learning styles and their implications in
learning and teaching. Theory and Practice in
Language Studies,1(4), 413416. doi:10.4304/
tpls.1.4.413-416
Yildirim, O., Cevat Acar, A., Bull, S., & Sevinc, L. (2008).
Relationships between teachersperceived leader-
ship style, studentslearning style, and academic
achievement: A study on high school students.
Educational Psychology,28(1), 7381. doi:10.1080/
01443410701417945
Zafari,M.,&Biria,R.(2014). The Relationship between
emotional intelligence and language learning
strategy Use. Procedia - Social and Behavioral
Sciences,98, 19661974. doi:10.1016/j.
sbspro.2014.03.630
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 20 of 21
© 2019 The Author(s). This open access article isdistributed under a CreativeCommons Attribution (CC-BY) 4.0 license.
You are free to:
Share copy and redistribute the material in any medium or format.
Adapt remix, transform, and build upon the material for any purpose, even commercially.
The licensor cannot revoke these freedoms as long as you follow the license terms.
Under the following terms:
Attribution You must give appropriate credit, provide a link to the license, and indicate if changes were made.
You may do so in any reasonable manner, but not in any way that suggests the licensor endorses you or your use.
No additional restrictions
You may not apply legal terms or technological measures that legally restrict others fromdoing anything the license permits.
Cogent Education (ISSN: 2331-186X) is published by Cogent OA, part of Taylor & Francis Group.
Publishing with Cogent OA ensures:
Immediate, universal access to your article on publication
High visibility and discoverability via the Cogent OA website as well as Taylor & Francis Online
Download and citation statistics for your article
Rapid online publication
Input from, and dialog with, expert editors and editorial boards
Retention of full copyright of your article
Guaranteed legacy preservation of your article
Discounts and waivers for authors in developing regions
Submit your manuscript to a Cogent OA journal at www.CogentOA.com
Taheri et al., Cogent Education (2019), 6: 1655882
https://doi.org/10.1080/2331186X.2019.1655882
Page 21 of 21
... Although previous research has advanced parts of the understanding of the role of personality and intelligence in L2 learning (Anglim et al., 2022;Wang & Liu, 2023), many gaps remain. First, studies have either focused on personality (e.g., Oz, 2015;Dewaele, 2019) or intelligence (e.g., Taheri et al., 2019;Slevc & Miyake, 2006), and very few have attempted to explore the terrain of both domains at once. Second, comprehensive frameworks for investigating multiple dimensions of intelligence (cognitive, emotional, cultural) and personality have not yet been explicitly integrated into L2 research, although researchers 1.1. ...
... For instance, Oz et al. (2015) found that emotional intelligence contributed significantly to foreign language learning quality, suggesting that intelligence cannot be reduced to a single dimension if we aim to understand the wide variance in L2 outcomes. In the same vein, Taheri et al. (2019) explored links between cognitive intelligence, emotional intelligence, and L2 achievement, showing that multiple facets of cognitive and noncognitive intelligence simultaneously shape L2 success. More straightforward relationships, such as the correlation between "traditional" intelligence measures and L2 skills, have been reported by Wilson and Lynn (1990) (in the context of Irish language learners) and by Slevc and Miyake (2006) (in investigating the role of music aptitude and general intelligence in L2 phonology). ...
... For example, greater emotional regulation capacities (Shafiee Rad & Hashemian, 2022) and greater well-being (Shafiee Rad & Hashemian, 2022;Reza, 2020) facilitate L2 performance by fostering more positive learner mindsets, reducing inhibition, and increasing motivation (Thao et al., 2023;Herrera et al., 2020;Yu et al., 2023). The advantage brought by personality traits, such as extraversion or openness (Furnham & Monsen, 2009;Stanek & Ones, 2023), can act in combination with domain-specific intelligence factors (Taheri et al., 2019;Sasaki, 1993) to contribute to greater inter-learner variability in L2 success. ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper discusses the interrelation between personality and intelligence in acquiring a second language (L2). From 13 studies and the available literature, it determines that extraversion, openness, and conscientiousness, as well as subdomains of emotional intelligence (well-being, empathy, and sociability) are important in predicting L2 motivation, perseverance, and achievement. The interpretation also outlines L2-specific trait emotional intelligence (TEI) and cultural intelligence as the key factors explaining why learning may be consistent with various measures other than general variables. Furthermore, it elaborates on how self-control, perseverance, and mindset assist in coping with language learning anxiety and promoting greater engagement with language learning. However, there are some limitations to this study, focusing on cross-sectional data and the homogeneity of the learner population. Therefore, follow-up work should take into account a longitudinal approach, examine the role of IPAs in learning L2, and move towards integrative perspectives in which the dispersive connections between traits of personality and intelligence and L2 proficiency would be adequately captured.
... Self-regulation involves setting goals, time management, and perseverance [32], while emotional intelligence assists in comprehending and managing emotions during language learning [31]. These two skills impact the selection and efficacy of language learning strategies, allowing learners to handle social situations, overcome challenges, and adjust to various learning environments [33,34]. ...
... Empathy is commonly defined as the ability to imagine oneself in someone else's situation or to understand and share one's emotions and perspective, which involves comprehending and relating to another person's mental or emotional state [37]. Empathy has often been associated with EI and language learning and communication [34,38,39]. The combination of empathy and emotional intelligence fosters an environment that supports language learners in developing their language skills and cultivating a deep understanding and respect for different cultures and experiences [36,38]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The significance of emotions in language education is increasingly acknowledged as a crucial element that impacts both teaching and learning. This article delves into the role of Emotional Intelligence (EI) in enhancing the language learning experience, emphasizing the importance of perceiving, using, understanding, and regulating emotions. Educators, as integral to the learning process, can cultivate more inclusive, supportive, and effective learning environments by integrating emotional awareness and regulation strategies into language teaching. This holistic approach to language education intertwines cognitive and emotional development, emphasizing the practical implications for educators in fostering EI in the language classroom to improve learner engagement and success.
... This perspective has led to what scholars have termed the "emotional turn" in language education, which emphasizes the central role of emotions in the learning process (White, 2018). In this context, EI has been found to play a facilitative role in enhancing various affective and behavioral aspects of language learning, including enjoyment, second language (L2) identity, learning strategies, learning styles, and learner beliefs (Li et al., 2021;Farsad and Modarresi, 2023;Taheri et al., 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
Emotional intelligence (EI) has garnered sustained theoretical and empirical attention over recent decades. Within the domain of linguistics, a growing body of research has investigated the relationship between EI and language achievement. Publication trends in this area reveal two distinct phases: a period of lukewarm attention (2009–2017), followed by a phase of rapid growth (2018–present). The present meta-analysis aims to determine whether EI significantly influences language achievement. Drawing on data from 47 independent studies, comprising 63 effect sizes and a total sample of 18,649 participants, this study found a small but significant correlation between EI and subjective language achievement (r = 0.24), and a moderate correlation with objective language achievement (r = 0.41). Moderator analyses revealed that the relationship between EI and objective language achievement varied significantly by educational level, target language, language skill assessed, and publication year. In contrast, no significant moderation effects were found for research type, learning context, students’ major, first language, or the measurement instruments employed. These findings underscore the important role of EI in language learning and highlight the need for emotionally responsive and supportive pedagogical environments that contribute to the sustainable development of foreign language education.
... The curriculum should be designed to not only rely on the material in the textbook, but also integrate more communicative and experiential learning methods. In preparing lesson plans, teachers need to adapt the learning approach to the level of students' cognitive development (Taheri et al., 2019), for example by adding project-based activities, role-plays, or group discussions to enrich the context of language use. Thus, the lesson plan is not only oriented towards completing the material in the textbook, but also ensures that students can understand and use the language functionally according to their communication needs. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose. Textbooks have an important role in learning foreign languages, especially in shaping students' linguistic competencies. This study analyzes elementary school English textbooks with psycholinguistics theory and Functional Systemic Linguistics (SFL) approaches to assess their suitability with students' cognitive development and language acquisition. Method. Using a qualitative content analysis method, this study examines the language structure, text type, and cognitive aspects in the selected textbooks. Results. The results showed that although textbooks use a simple and repetitive structure to facilitate understanding, some materials are still too complex for elementary school students. Visual illustrations help with understanding, but phonological exercises and opportunities for oral interaction are still limited. From the perspective of SFL, textbooks are dominated by existential processes, declarative sentences, and modal verbs, but lack the exploration of the variety of language modes as well as interactive elements. Conclusion. The study emphasizes the need for textbook design that is more balanced between cognitive and linguistic aspects, by adding phonological exercises, simplifying sentence structure, and more interactive content.
... Given that L2 learning is a long-term academic pursuit that requires goal-setting behavior (Han & Lu, 2018), it may be that Machiavellian traits are beneficial in L2 learning. However, conversely, previous research has also found Machiavellianism to be negatively linked to Emotional Intelligence (Michels & Schulze, 2021), academic self-efficacy (Saadat et al., 2017), and classroom prosocial behaviors (Berger & Palacios, 2014), with each of these variables positively associated with L2 learning (Taheri et al., 2019;Young Kyo, 2022;Olivero, 2021). Whereas in our findings, Machiavellianism was positively linked to peer engagement, social engagement, and self-perceptions. ...
Article
Full-text available
The duality of human nature, consisting of positive and negative personality traits, has intrigued scholars in different fields. Despite an overwhelming dominance of research on positive characteristics, particularly in the field of education, negative traits, such as those constituting the Dark Triad (DT; i.e., Psychopathy, Narcissism, and Machiavellianism), have been confirmed to be associated with both positive and detrimental outcomes. This paper aims to investigate the potential influence of these aversive traits on second language (L2) learning outcomes-L2 learning motivation, engagement, achievement, and willingness to communicate. L2 learners from a single country (n = 431) participated in this study. Multiple structural equation models (SEMs) were run to analyze the links and the directionality of significant effects. Overall, among the undesirable DT traits, Psychopathy and Narcissism were both positive and negative predictors of the L2 outcomes, while Machiavellianism unexpectedly emerged solely as a positive predictor. The intricacy of the results underscores the vague nature of the effects, pinpointing the need for more caution while examining negative personality traits in education and the L2 contexts. Based on the results of this study, implications and directions for future research on DT and language learning are suggested.
... La necesidad de retroalimentación positiva en este contexto es señalada por varias teorías pedagógicas que sostienen que la corrección debe ser acompañada por un enfoque constructivo, para reforzar la autoestima del estudiante y mantenerlo interesado en aprender. También, se examinarán las mejores prácticas para integrar las emociones en el aula de EFL creando un ambiente educativo seguro y alentador donde los estudiantes puedan expresarse e intentar diferentes formas del lenguaje sin miedo a juicio o error(Taheri et al., 2019).Al adoptar estas estrategias de enseñanza, se espera que los estudiantes de tercer año de bachillerato en la Unidad Educativa Fiscal Mixta "Plan Internacional" no solo mejoren sus habilidades lingüísticas, sino que también desarrollen una mayor autonomía en su aprendizaje y una actitud más positiva hacia el uso del inglés. Este enfoque integral, que considera tanto el desarrollo cognitivo como el afectivo de los estudiantes, tiene el potencial de transformar la enseñanza del inglés en una experiencia más significativa y exitosa, permitiendo que los estudiantes se conviertan en aprendices más activos, motivados y competentes(Alsaraireh, 2022).En estudio pretende proporcionar una base sólida para la implementación de estrategias pedagógicas innovadoras que no solo favorezcan el aprendizaje cognitivo del inglés, sino que también promuevan un entorno emocionalmente favorable para los estudiantes. ...
Article
Full-text available
El objetivo general de este estudio fue analizar las estrategias de enseñanza del inglés como lengua extranjera (EFL) para fomentar las habilidades cognitivas y afectivas en los estudiantes del tercer año de bachillerato en la Unidad Educativa Fiscal Mixta 'Plan Internacional'. La investigación empleó un enfoque mixto, combinando un diagnóstico cuantitativo con test estandarizados de habilidades cognitivas y afectivas, y un análisis cualitativo basado en observaciones de clases y entrevistas semiestructuradas con docentes del área de inglés. Los resultados obtenidos a partir de los test diagnósticos revelaron que los estudiantes presentan dificultades significativas en comprensión lectora, expresión oral, uso del vocabulario y gramática. Además, se observó una baja motivación intrínseca, falta de confianza al hablar en público y elevados niveles de ansiedad durante las conversaciones orales. Las entrevistas con los docentes indicaron que, aunque emplean actividades dinámicas como debates y juegos de rol, las limitaciones de tiempo, recursos y currículo inflexible dificultan una implementación efectiva de estas estrategias. En conclusión, se destacó la necesidad de fortalecer tanto las habilidades cognitivas como afectivas de los estudiantes, creando un entorno de aprendizaje que favorezca la interacción y reduzca la ansiedad. Las metodologías innovadoras, como la gamificación y el aprendizaje basado en proyectos, junto con el uso de recursos tecnológicos, fueron propuestas como alternativas clave para mejorar la motivación y el rendimiento de los estudiantes en el aprendizaje del inglés.
... The diversity of cognitive models leads to the heterogeneity of learning methods and the difference of information processing efficiency. For example, some students may rely on superior visual visualization to improve lexical representation, whereas some peers may rely on auditory loops to stabilize comprehension of syntactic structures [2]. The core focus of this paper is to systematically examine how differences in cognitive models shape the selection and practice of foreign language acquisition strategies. ...
Article
Full-text available
Finding out how different cognitive styles affect the strategies chosen for learning a foreign language was the driving force behind this research. The rapid pace of globalization has made fluency in more than one language an essential component of every well-rounded person’s toolbox. However, in the actual learning process, there are significant achievement differences between students, which is not only related to language talent, but also influenced by individual cognitive style. By reviewing the relevant literature, this paper gives a clear definition and classification of cognitive style, and gives an overview of foreign language learning strategies. On this basis, 150 foreign language learners from different schools were surveyed by a combination of questionnaire and interview. Students’ use of various tactics for learning a foreign language is influenced by their cognitive styles, according to the results. Metacognitive methods are more commonly used by students who are independent in their subject, whereas field-dependent students depend more on tactics that involve social and emotional support. Furthermore, the study concluded that in order to maximize the effectiveness of instruction, educators should use more individualized approaches that take into account individuals’ unique cognitive styles. In addition to offering concrete recommendations for enhancing the current state of foreign language instruction, this study offers a fresh viewpoint on the unique characteristics of language learners.
... Negative emotions can have unfavorable consequences in language learners such as poor performance in the target language or restrict learners' thinking among other (Cheng et. al., 2014;Taheri et al., 2019). Nevertheless, negative feelings of regret and remorse encouraged learners to look for ways to improve their target language performance. ...
Book
Full-text available
Estas investigaciones, ensayos, propuestas y sistematizaciones comprenden cuestiones tales como: activismo lingüístico a través de la traducción, enseñar español a estudiantes mayores, diseños tecnopedagógicos de cursos de lenguas, percepciones sobre lenguas originarias para determinar políticas lingüísticas, análisis de nuevas estrategias educativas, desarrollo de competencia textual, emociones en estudiantes de licenciatura y centros de idiomas, desigualdad de género en la academia mexicana, autoeficacia de los docentes y dimensiones causales de logros educativos. Como puede constatarse, el título que da nombre a este libro sobre nuevas formas de definir la enseñanza y el aprendizaje de lenguas se revela en los textos seleccionados para esta publicación.
Article
Full-text available
Coastal environments have in recent years become the focus of government development with the hope of becoming a center for ecological protection and an educational tourism destination due to the large number of mangrove plant species and migratory bird species that stop by from various parts of the world. A high level of environmental awareness and good English language skills are very urgent to support the sustainability and benefits of this ecosystem and to expose it internationally. However, the majority of English language learning in schools focuses more on general knowledge without any specifications and differentiation tailored to the environment around students. Therefore, this research aims to determine the level of effectiveness of digital-based English learning media and the coastal environment with the hope of increasing elementary school students' cognitive response to the sustainability and optimization of coastal sustainability, which is located on the north coast of East Java. The research method used is mixed-method with explanatory sequential strategy conducted in three elementary schools located around the coastal area. Then the data obtained will be processed using nested ANOVA. The results of this research are crucial stages needed to create an effective integration model for English language learning and the environment that meets regional needs to accommodate the forthcoming policy from the ministry of education that will make English as one of the integral subjects in elementary school.
Article
Full-text available
Emotions are at the heart of the foreign language learning process. Without emotion, boredom would reign and very little learning would take place. I report on some recent work that has investigated the role of emotion in the foreign language classroom, both positive (foreign language enjoyment) and negatives ones (foreign language anxiety). It seems that both learners and teachers play a crucial role in managing emotions in the classroom. I also report on the difficulties associated with the communication of emotions in a foreign language and on their relative absence in foreign language course books and during classes. This leaves learners ill-prepared to recognise and express emotions appropriately in a foreign language, which is an essential part of sociopragmatic competence. 外国語学習過程の中心には「感情」がある。感情がなければ飽きるのも早く、学びも限られてしまう。本論では、外国語の授業で感情が果たす肯定的な(例:外国語学習の楽しみ)および否定的な(例:外国語学習不安)役割について報告する。そして最近の研究を基に、いかに学習者と教員双方がクラスでの感情のコントロールに深くかかわっているかを考察する。また、外国語で感情を伝えることの難しさについても触れ、外国語の教科書や授業で感情表現が扱われることの少なさが、社会語用論的能力の主な要素である感情表現の理解不足につながっていることを指摘する。
Article
Full-text available
Learning could not be thought of as unrelated to cognitive processes and cognitive processes are not unrelated to affective ones; thus, the current study tried to empirically examine the influence of Emotion Based Language Instruction (EBLI) in fostering teaching oral skills to EFL learners. An IELTS test was utilized to measure the progress of both control and experimental groups at the beginning of the research and also at the end. The content of the material chosen to be taught for experimental group was selectively written based on the Metric of Emotioncy designed by Pishghadam (2016). The differences in scores of pre and posttest of IELTS were statistically calculated to observe the effect of EBLI. The results revealed a significant change in speaking skill in experimental group and a change in listening skill, though it was not statistically significant. The results of the current investigation can increase the knowledge on applying EBLI in teaching foreign/second languages.
Article
Full-text available
What is Psycholinguistics? How does it differ from Cognitive Linguistics? Why is this general area of linguistics in the limelight these days? Dr. Zoltan Dornyei is a professor at Nottingham University who is at the forefront of this research. He recently wrote a book titled, The Psychology of Second Language Acquisition (Dornyei, 2009). In it, Dornyei discusses the newer challenges SLA researchers in this century need to come to terms with. He details research of the Critical Period Hypothesis (the connection between emotion and learning), Individual Differences (the implicit/explicit dichotomy), and Dynamic Systems Theory. This interview provides an overview of such recent research and serves as an introduction to his book
Article
Full-text available
This study examines language learning strategy (LLS) use in connexion with foreign language attitude, proficiency and general school achievement among lower secondary students in Years 5 and 8 (n = 868) in Hungary. An adapted version of the Strategies Inventory for Language Learning questionnaire was used for data collection. The results showed that Hungarian students mainly engage in metacognitive strategies in both years. Differences between more and less proficient language learners’ strategy use have also been found. With regard to the effect of LLS on foreign language attitude, the foreign language mark and school achievement, path analysis indicated a good fit in both years. The metacognitive, social and memory strategies primarily influenced foreign language attitudes and marks in Year 5. The metacognitive strategies had a slight impact on school achievement as well as on foreign language marks. We demonstrated the dominant effect of metacognitive strategies and the low effect of memory strategies in Year 8. In addition, metacognitive strategies also influenced foreign language marks. The effect of foreign language marks on school achievement was also remarkable. There was a strong impact on the children’s attitudes through these variables.
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to investigate what language learning strategies were frequently used by English as a foreign language undergraduates at a public university in Bangkok, Thailand and the significant differences in the use of language learning strategies based on individual differences, including gender and self-rated English proficiency. The research was conducted with 392 Thai undergraduates through administering a quantitative method. Instruments for the study included a demographic questionnaire and Oxford’s Strategy Inventory for Language Learning. The findings revealed that research participants reported a low-to-medium use of language learning strategy. The strategies most used by learners were compensation strategies while the least were cognitive strategies. Regarding gender differences, female participants used the all six strategy categories more than did the male counterparts. The results also demonstrated that participants with different English proficiency employed learning strategies at different levels with a statistical significance at .05 in almost six strategies, except affective strategies. Research participants who were good at English most used metacognitive while social strategies were most used by participants with less English proficiency.
Article
The sociocultural educational environment determines the formation and self-improvement of the student’s personality. A modern university is an educational space within which knowledge is accumulated and improved and various competencies, including soft skills, appear. Among these skills, emotional intelligence is essential. Our research aimed to study the relationship between the emotional intelligence of teachers and the emotional intelligence of students. In our study, the existence of this relationship was confirmed.
Article
This study was an attempt to explore the role of emotional intelligence on the whole and its components particularly problem solving in EFL learners’ listening comprehension performance in the IELTS tests. For this purpose, a total of 39 advanced learners homogenized according to Oxford Placement Test took part in this study. Participants completed the IELTS listening tests and were asked to fill out the Bar-On emotional intelligence questionnaire. The obtained quantitative data were analyzed using Pearson correlation coefficients. Results indicated a non-significant correlation between the whole EI scores and listening performance of learners. There were, however, a significant relationship between problem solving and EI. The social responsibility component of EI was also found to have a significant relationship with listening. The findings are discussed with regard to the role of the IELTS test as a standardized test and the high level of learners. Implications and suggestions for further research are presented.
Article
This study investigates the relationship between intelligence and foreign language learning in general, and the learning of vocabulary and grammar in particular. It also investigates the effect of extra practice on foreign language learning on learners with equal intelligence. The participants were 182 high school students at grades two and three. They were given Raven’s Progressive Matrices, which is a test of general intelligence, and an achievement test of English based on their EFL textbook at school. The English test consisted of two subparts: vocabulary and grammar. Correlation coefficient was run on intelligence test scores and English test scores. The results showed a weak positive relationship between intelligence and foreign language learning, and learning of vocabulary and grammar. For investigating the effect of practice, some students who attended English classes and some of those who didn’t were chosen in a way that their mean intelligence score was equal. Then their mean English test score was calculated. The mean English test score of the students who attended English classes was significantly higher than those students who didn’t. The conclusion is that intelligence affects foreign language learning, but extra practice can offset the effect of intelligence.
Article
The aim of this paper is twofold. On a conceptual level, we argue that the cognitive and emotive exist in dialectic relation to one another and that as such both are ever present in psychological activity, including L2 development. Related to this first point, we seek to orient researchers and practitioners to ways in which interactions with learners may simultaneously attend to both aspects of this cognitive-emotive dialectic to optimally promote learner development. We begin with an example that demonstrates the intertwining of cognition and emotion, thus setting the scene for what follows: a discussion of the concept of perezhivanie as the most explicit and detailed discussion of the cognitive-emotive unity in Vygotsky's writings. We follow this with an overview of more recent work within SCT that has elaborated upon Vygotsky's ideas and that further specifies the genesis of the emotive as an inherent component of human psychology and one that is always in relation to the cognitive. With this as background, we turn to the research of Reuven Feuerstein, whose clinical work with learners with special needs aligns closely with SCT principles and includes the emotive as both an element of learner functioning that must be attended to during cognitive intervention as well as a legitimate focus of mediation in its own right. Data from a recent project involving Mediated Development, an interactional framework derived from Vygotsky's writings and influenced by the work of Feuerstein, are discussed as illustrative of what may be revealed when mediator focus in joint engagement with a learner is on the cognitive-emotive unity. We conclude that the concept of perezhivanie, understood as a cognitive-emotive dialectic, orients us to understanding how each shapes the other, and how either may gain prominence during particular moments in development. Our understanding of perezhivanie suggests that mediation of grammatical errors, lexical choice and pragmatic violations has been one-sided, and should include the mediation of, for example, sharing behavior, frustration and feelings of competence. We end by suggesting several ideas for future research.