ArticlePDF Available

The Impact of Message Source on the Effectiveness of Communications About Climate Change

Authors:

Abstract

We conducted a survey experiment in which we presented 1,850 respondents with one of two versions of an appeal emphasizing either the threats to the environment or threats to national security of the United States as a result of climate change. The messages were attributed to one of four sources: Republican Party leaders, Democratic Party leaders, military officials, or climate scientists. The results reveal that messages attributed to military leaders, or to Republican Party leaders, can enhance the impact of the appeal. This finding underscores the importance that the source of any communication can have on its overall effectiveness.
The Impact of Message Source on the Effectiveness of Communications about
Climate Change
Toby Bolsen
Risa Palm
Justin T. Kingsland
Abstract
We conducted a survey experiment in which we presented 1,850 respondents with one of
two versions of an appeal emphasizing either the threats to the environment or threats to
national security of the United States as a result of climate change. The messages were
attributed to one of four sources: Republican or Democratic party leaders, military
officials, or climate scientists. The results reveal that messages attributed to military
leaders, or to Republican party leaders, can enhance the impact of the appeal. This
finding underscores the importance that the source of any communication can have on its
overall effectiveness.
Keywords: Climate change, framing, message source, public opinion
Citation: Bolsen, T., Palm, R., & Kingsland, J. T. (2019). The Impact of Message Source
on the Effectiveness of Communications About Climate Change. Science
Communication, 41(4), 464-487.
https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547019863154
Americans report deep divisions along partisan lines over fundamental beliefs
about whether human-caused climate change is real, the level of threat it poses, and
whether there is a need for action to reduce greenhouse gases. Despite an overwhelming
scientific consensus that climate change is human-caused and presents a major threat to
human societies and ecosystems (Cook et al., 2016), a significant percentage of
Americans do not believe that climate change is occurring (30%) or that it is human-
caused (42%) (Leiserowitz et al., 2018; also see, Egan & Mullins, 2017). Public
discourse on climate change continues to be characterized by partisan and ideological
divisions in the U.S. (Dunlap, McCright, & Yarosh, 2016; Feldman et al., 2012;
Hamilton, 2016; Hart et al., 2015). For example, one recent survey reported that while
95% of liberal Democrats believe that human-caused climate change is real, only 40% of
conservative Republicans say the same (Goldberg et al., 2019; Leiserowitz et al., 2018).
An important challenge for science communicators is how to effectively engage
individuals who reject information about climate change.
Varying the way that climate change messages are framed – that is, varying the
words, phrases or images that highlight specific aspects of climate change – has been
shown to affect the response of various audiences (Feldman & Hart, 2018;
Lewandowsky, Gignac, & Vaughan, 2013; McCright et al, 2016; Myers, Nisbet, Maibach,
& Leiserowitz, 2012; Nisbet & Mooney, 2007; van der Linden, Leiserowitz, & Maibach,
2019). For example, a communicator might bring attention to the environmental,
economic, public health, or national security implications of climate change, providing an
“interpretive storyline that set(s) a specific train of thought in motion, communicating
why an issue might be a problem, who or what might be responsible for it, and what
should be done about it” (Nisbet, 2009, p.15).
A growing body of research has examined how exposure to issue, or emphasis,
frames shape climate change beliefs (for a review, see Bolsen & Shapiro, 2017). Yet little
work explicitly manipulates the source of an appeal focused on shifting public opinion on
climate change.1 Are framed messages more impactful when they are associated with an
ostensibly credible source? Does the presence of a partisan source manipulation (e.g., in-
group versus out-group) condition the impact of the message?
We extend research on issue framing and social identity theory to generate
predictions about how the presence of a source cue (climate scientists, military leaders,
Democratic Party leaders, or Republican party leaders) in an appeal highlighting either
the environmental or national security effects of climate change conditions the
effectiveness of an appeal. We implemented a survey experiment to test how the
presence of different sources conditioned a frame’s impact on individuals’ climate-related
beliefs. The results suggest that the presence of specific sources linked with frames
highlighting different effects of climate change can increase or decrease the message’s
impact on individuals’ related beliefs. This underscores the importance that the source of
any strategic climate communication effort can have on its overall effectiveness, as well
as the need for credible and trusted opinion leaders to convey messages who have the
capacity to persuade skeptical audiences.
1 We discuss several exceptions below, including Benegal & Scruggs, 2018; Ehret, Van
Boven, & Sherman, 2018a; Van Boven, Ehret, & Sherman, 2018b; Kousser & Tranter,
2018.
Issue frames and climate change beliefs
A growing body of research has explored how exposing people to frames that
emphasize different aspects of climate change shape citizens’ related beliefs and
willingness to take actions that would reduce greenhouse gas emissions (Hart, 2011; Hart
& Feldman, 2016; Hart & Nisbet, 2012; Maibach, Nisbet, Baldwin, Akerlof, & Diao,
2010; Leiserowitz, 2007; Myers, Nisbet, Maibach, & Leiserowitz, 2012; Nisbet, 2009;
Villar & Krosnick, 2011). An issue or emphasis framing effect occurs when exposure to a
communication causes an individual to place relatively greater “weight” on the
emphasized consideration in the opinion formation process (Chong & Druckman, 2007).2
Exposure to framed messages may increase the availability, accessibility, and/or
perceived applicability of specific aspects of the complicated issue of climate change.
Myers et al. (2012), for instance, tested how exposure to one of three distinct frames
associated with the effects of climate change – national security, environment, and public
health – affected self-reported emotions among respondents with different levels of pre-
existing skepticism about climate change. They found that exposure to the public health
frame increased feelings of hope whereas the national security frame unexpectedly
increased anger among “doubtful” and “dismissive” audience segments. Myers et al.
(2012) stated that one factor that might have accounted for this “boomerang effect”3 was
the lack of the congruence between the messenger and the frame. They posited that
2 We focus exclusively on issue framing effects and not equivalency, or valence, framing
effects that occur when positive or negative information unconsciously influences
preferences as a result of a negativity bias in the encoding of stimulus information
(Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; for a typology of framing effects, see Levin, Schneider, &
Gaeth, 1998).
participants may have experienced anger at the experimenters for making claims about
the national security implications of climate change, and called for future work to explore
the role that the presence of sources linked with specific frames might have on different
audiences.
Frames that accentuate scientific consensus among climate scientists regarding
the existence of human-caused climate change have been found to increase belief in
human-caused climate change and support among Americans for policy action to address
the problem (Lewandowsky et al., 2012; van der Linden, 2015; van der Linden et al.,
2019). Other studies have demonstrated that highlighting its local impacts, for instance
through visual images showing flooding that would occur as a result of sea level rise in
coastal communities, can increase public concern and support for actions to address the
problem (Bolsen, Kingsland, & Palm, 2018). However, response to otherwise credible
and persuasive information can be undermined when presented in competition with
rhetoric that politicizes climate science, for instance by actors seeking to cast doubt on
the existence of a scientific consensus. (Bolsen & Druckman, 2018; Druckman, 2017;
McCright & Dunlap, 2011; McCright et al., 2016; van der Linden et al., 2017).
Source credibility, trust, and persuasion
While a number of experiments have documented the impact of exposure to
strategic frames on Americans’ climate beliefs, few studies have varied the presence of
3 Hart and Nisbet (2012, p. 704) state, “A boomerang effect occurs when a message is
strategically constructed with a specific intent but produces a result that is the opposite of
that intent”; (for other examples of such effects in climate communication studies, see
Cook & Lewandowsky, 2016; Myers et al., 2012; Zhou, 2016).
sources linked with a pro-climate action message to determine if such information
moderates a frame’s impact on individuals’ climate beliefs. Research on environmental
risk communication and issue-framing makes clear that the effectiveness of any message
depends crucially on the audience’s trust in and perception of credibility toward its source
(Brewer & Ley, 2013; Druckman, 2001a; 2001b; Liu & Priest, 2009; Priest, 2001; Renn
& Levine, 1991). Perceptions about the competence, expertise, objectivity, impartiality,
and fairness of a message’s source determine its persuasive impact, as well as other more
peripheral factors such as the attractiveness of the source, sympathy or empathy toward
the source, and social status of the source (Renn & Levine, 1991).
The “elaboration-likelihood model of persuasion” (ELM) has been applied by
social scientists across disciplines to understand the mechanisms by which risk
communication can generate persuasion (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). The ELM
distinguishes between central and peripheral routes of persuasion. The peripheral route
involves the use of heuristics – such as the perceived credibility of a messenger – in
determining whether or not to accept a particular message; the central route is
characterized by more effortful information processing triggered by the motivation of an
individual to actively process the information (Brewer & Ley, 2013; Eagly & Chaiken,
1993; Renn & Levine, 1991).
Finally, empirical research has demonstrated the credibility of “scientists” as
generally credible sources. Surveys conducted in both the U.S. and among British
citizens has demonstrated that “scientists” tend to be among the most trusted sources for
providing accurate information about environmental issues in general and on climate
change in particular (Bickerstaff et al., 2008; Malka, Krosnick, & Langer, 2009; Nisbet &
Myers, 2007).4
Based on this literature, we hypothesize:
The presence of a credible source linked with a frame congruent with the
expertise attributed to this source will increase the frame’s impact on individuals’
beliefs. Specifically, climate scientists linked with a frame highlighting climate
change’s environmental effects will increase the frame’s impact on individuals’
related beliefs. (Hypothesis 1)
Similarly, military leaders linked with a frame that emphasizes climate change’s
effects on U.S. national security will increase the frame’s impact on individuals’
related beliefs. (Hypothesis 2)
Social identity theory and climate change beliefs
People derive their self-concept, in part, from the social groups and categories to
which they belong (Hogg & Reid, 2006; Huddy, 2001; Huddy, Mason, & Aaroe, 2015;
Kahan, 2016). These group attachments play a powerful role in shaping determinations
about the credibility and trust of any information source (Lenz, 2012; McCright,
Dentzman, Charters, & Dietz, 2013; Renn & Levine, 1991). Identity-based motivated
reasoning can lead audiences to interpret information about climate change in a biased
4 Partisanship, however, increasingly plays a role in perceptions about the credibility of
scientific information (Benegal & Scruggs, 2018; Gauchat, 2012; Motta, 2017), so it may
be that on a polarized issue such climate change some groups (e.g., Republicans) may not
find scientists to be the most effective source of any pro-climate appeal.
manner as a way to protect their existing beliefs or group attachments (Hart & Nisbet,
2012; Kahan et al., 2011).
One form of protection of social identity is the use of partisan motivated
reasoning (for a review, see Druckman, Leeper & Slothuus, 2018; Taber & Lodge, 2013;
Kahan, 2015). This process can occur when people possess strong opinions that guide
their reasoning strategies, or because of partisan cue-taking that simplifies and reduces
the amount of information and effort necessary to form an opinion in a given context
(Bartels, 2002; Bolsen, Druckman, & Cook, 2014; Cohen, 2003; Goren et al., 2009;
Guber, 2017; Kunda, 1990; Lelkes, Malka, & Bakker, 2019; Malka & Lelkes, 2010).
Partisans in pursuit of value-affirming information may therefore turn to sources who
share their group identity or cultural worldviews in seeking out or interpreting any new
information about climate change (Benegal & Scruggs, 2018; Feinberg & Willer, 2013;
Hmielowski et al., 2014; Kahan, Jenkins-Smith, & Braman, 2011)
Perception of consensus, particularly within ones’ close social network, also
influences people to align their beliefs with what they perceive to be the majority point-
of-view (Goldberg et al., 2019; Jost, 2018; van der Linden 2015). However, Americans
tend to underestimate the actual level of social consensus about human-induced climate
change (Mildenberger & Tingley, 2017). Empirical research has identified the power of
social consensus messages particularly among conservatives and Republicans. These
groups show a stronger drive for conformity to in-group norms and greater willingness to
adopt and share like-minded views with other Republicans (Benegal & Scruggs, 2018;
Goldberg et al., 2019; Jost, van der Linden, Panagopoulos & Hardin, 2018). In a recent
experiment that varied sources associated with a ballot proposal, Republicans were more
likely to support a climate proposal when it was endorsed by Republican party elites, and
perceptions that other Republicans also supported the initiative was a key mediating
belief (Ehret, Van Boven, & Sherman, 2018). In another recent study, a message
sponsored by Republican senators led Republican respondents to reject misinformation
about climate change, increase their belief in the existence of a scientific consensus, and
increase their belief that it is human-caused (Benegal & Scruggs, 2018). In these cases,
the partisan membership of the person delivering the message plays a powerful role in the
degree to which an appeal persuades any audience (Benegal & Scruggs, 2018; Ehret, Van
Boven, & Sherman, 2018; Kousser & Tranter, 2018; Li et al., 2016; Maibach et al., 2015;
Van Boven, Ehret, & Sherman, 2018).
Based on the findings of these studies, we hypothesize that:
The presence of an in-group (i.e., in-partisan) source regardless of the frame with
which it is associated will increase the frame’s impact on in-partisans’ related
beliefs.
(Hypothesis 3)
Data and methodology
We conducted a survey of 1,850 unique respondents recruited using Amazon’s
Mechanical Turk (MTurk) service in July 2018.5 Each participant was randomly
assigned to one of ten conditions that varied the content of an appeal accentuating either
5 MTurk samples offer greater diversity than student or other convenience samples and
have been used to replicate numerous studies across disciplines (Berinsky et al., 2012;
Mullinix et al., 2015). Descriptive statistics for the sample are available in a
supplementary file available upon request.
the environmental or national security risks of climate change (i.e., environmental or
national security frame). Two treatments were defined: one based on the threat that
climate change poses to the national security of the United States, and the second that
climate change poses for environmental sustainability. The national security treatment
emphasized the effects of climate change on human migration patterns and potential
“conflict over land ownership or water use that could result in war,” while the
environmental treatment emphasized coastal flooding due to sea-level rise and an
increased frequency of droughts and wildfires. In addition to highlighting these effects,
both treatments advocated action “to promote energy efficiency and renewable energy to
substantially decrease our greenhouse gas emissions.”6 We selected the environmental
frame given its dominance in climate discourses and the national security frame given the
and the call to link it with a credible source (e.g., Myers et al., 2012). The frames also
matched the domain of expertise for two of the sources we manipulated in the study.
To test the impact of the source, we assigned respondents to one of five groups.
For instance, the headline above the article in the no source (baseline) condition stated,
“Call for Action on Climate” and did not explicitly mention a source, whereas the other
conditions linked the appeal to a specific source – e.g., “Climate Scientists, Republican
Party Leaders, Democratic Party Leaders, or Military Leaders.” The appeal itself stated
that “Democratic Party leaders,” “Republican Party leaders,” “military leaders,” or
“climate scientists” were the ones highlighting these effects of climate change and calling
for policy action to reduce greenhouse gas emissions.
6 The complete wording of the stimuli for each condition and the survey instrument is
available upon request.
We measured how exposure to the treatments affected individuals’ beliefs about:
(a) the level of threat that climate change presents to national security (1-5 scale, none at
all / a great deal); (b) the level of threat that climate change presents to a sustainable
environment (1-5 scale, none at all / a great deal); and, (c) support for laws that promote
energy efficiency and renewable energy as a way to combat climate change (1–7 strongly
oppose / strongly support). The information that was employed across experimental
conditions highlighted one of these threats and called for a greater reliance on renewable
energy. As such, these measures serve as our primary dependent variables to evaluate
how message sources condition an appeal’s impact.
We also included several additional post-treatment measures. First, we measured
fundamental beliefs about climate change: (d) whether or not they believe climate change
is happening (1-7 Definitely not happening / definitely happening) and (e) the extent to
which they agreed with a statement that a scientific consensus exists regarding climate
change happening primarily as a result of human activity (1-7 strongly disagree / strongly
agree). Because our experiment explicitly manipulated the source of the message
highlighting the threats of climate change and need for policy action, we measured
respondents’ perceptions about (f) the degree to which political motives are a driver of
scientific research on climate change (1-7 strongly disagree / strongly agree). We
anticipate that audiences predisposed toward climate skepticism (e.g., Republicans) may
view messages from in-group sources about climate change as “less politicized”
compared to the same message without a source (or when attributed to an out-party
leader). Last, we measured respondents’ agreement with the statement (g) that the idea
climate change is primarily due to human activity is a hoax or a conspiracy (1-7 strongly
disagree / strongly agree). We included this item given the literature showing that
motivated reasoning processes appear to drive the expression of this relatively
widespread belief held by many Americans (i.e., estimated between 20%–40%) regarding
climate change (e.g., see Miller, Saunders, & Farhart, 2016; Uscinski, Klofstad, &
Atkinson, 2016; Uscinski & Parent, 2014). We anticipate similar dynamics resulting
from the source manipulation on climate hoax belief as with the other key measures – i.e.,
credible sources, or in-group/out-group sources, linked to an appeal highlighting the
threats of climate change will condition the impact of each message. Although belief in
conspiracies is a general predisposition (Lewandowsky, Oberauer, & Gignac, 2013;
Oliver & Wood, 2014), political ideology is strongly associated with which conspiracies
an individual will endorse (Uscinski & Parent, 2014). We expect that in-group sources
linked with a message highlighting climate change’s threats will decrease climate hoax
beliefs, whereas out-group sources may increase such beliefs among skeptical audiences
(i.e., Republicans who receive a message sponsored by Democratic Party leaders) as a
result of partisan motivated reasoning. We measured party identification on a 7-point
Likert scale, and then created a series of dummy variables for respondents who identified
as Democrat, Independent, or Republican.
Before turning to the results of our analyses, we include a condensed description
of our research design. Table 1 presents the framework of our experimental design, a
description of the treatments provided to respondents in each condition, and a restatement
of our hypothesized expectations.
[Insert Table 1 here]
Results
We evaluate the impact of the experimental treatments by regressing each
dependent variable on the experimental conditions, omitting the no-source appeal as the
baseline for each distinct appeal.7 Our design allows us to evaluate the effects of the
presence of two ostensibly credible, “expert” sources associated with each frame as well
as the impact of Republican and Democratic Party leaders.
The first column in Figure 1 reports the impact of the experimental conditions on
belief that climate change presents a threat to U.S. national security. First, in support of
Hypothesis 1, military leaders as the source of the message emphasizing climate change’s
threats to national security significantly increase the belief that climate change is a
national security threat across all respondents in the sample relative to the no-source
baseline (p = .04). We did not anticipate any effect of military leaders as the source of the
environmental message (Column 2, Figure 1) on beliefs that climate change presents a
threat to national security; however, as Figure 1 reports, military leaders linked with this
frame also significantly increased beliefs that climate change is a threat to national
security relative the no-source baseline (p = .01). Military leaders did not have an effect
on perceptions about the degree to which climate change presents an environmental threat
relative to the no-source baseline for that message. This suggests that the impact of a
source may depend on the degree to which the belief measured is in a domain in which
the source is perceived as an expert. Interestingly, in the full sample, military leaders
linked with a national security frame had no effect on support for a greater reliance on
renewable energy to reduce greenhouse gas emissions (Support Laws), as well as other
7 We report the means and standard deviations for each condition across all of the
dependent variables in a supplementary file available upon request from the
corresponding author.
beliefs we measured such as the belief climate change is happening, that a scientific
consensus exists, or that climate change is a hoax.
[Insert Figure 1 here]
The right side of Figure 1 reports the effects of the experimental conditions on
respondents’ belief that climate change presents a threat to a sustainable environment.
Hypothesis 2 predicted that climate scientists linked with an environmental message
would increase this message’s impact relative to the no source baseline. We find no
support for this hypothesis. We also find that when climate scientists are the source of a
message about the threat that climate change presents to national security and linked with
a call for policy action, respondents significantly reduced the strength of their beliefs
about the environmental threat climate change poses (p = .03), support for laws to
promote renewable energy (p = .04), belief that climate change is happening (p = .01),
and belief that a scientific consensus exists (p = .01).
The effects are even more dramatic when partisan information sources are
compared with the no-source baseline on the full sample. First, when Republican Party
leaders are the source of the national security climate message, respondents increase
their support for polices that would promote renewable energy to reduce greenhouse
gases (Support Laws) (p = .08). Republican sources linked with a national security
message reduced the perception that climate change research is driven primarily by
political motivations (p = .01). Further, Republican sources linked with this frame and
call for action also significantly decreased respondents’ perception that human induced
climate change is a hoax by nearly a one-half point shift on the five-point response scale
(p = .02). Source effects were more prominent in the national security frame conditions
compared to the environmental frame conditions. Nonetheless, when the environmental
message was attributed to Republican sources, respondents’ perceptions that a scientific
consensus exists on human-caused climate change increases (p = .09). On the other hand,
when the source of the national security message and call for action is attributed to
Democratic leaders, respondents became significantly less likely to see climate change as
a security threat (p = .10) and significantly less likely to say that climate change is
occurring (p = .03) or that a scientific consensus exists (p = .06).
To evaluate Hypothesis 3 regarding how in-group versus out-group partisan
sources may condition the impact of the experimental conditions on our dependent
measures, we replicate the analyses separately for Republican and Democrats in our
sample.8 First, we found very few significant treatment effects resulting from the source
manipulations when comparing Democrats in the no-source baseline condition to
Democrats across all other conditions (see Table 2 and Figure 2). There are a few
exceptions, such as military leaders having a significant positive effect on Democrats’
perceptions climate change is a national security threat, but the overall picture is that
Democrats in the baseline condition (and all source conditions) report highly skewed
beliefs that resulted in ceiling effects with little room for additional movement on many
of the response scales.
8 We chose this modeling approach rather than testing models with interaction effects
because we are interested (theoretically) in testing for source-effects associated with the
frames we introduce on Democrats (Republicans) in each condition relative to Democrats
(Republicans) in the No-Source baseline condition to examine within-subgroup effects.
We lack clear theoretical expectations for other estimation approaches that would rely on
Independents in the No-Source condition as a baseline for our hypothesis tests.
[Insert Table 2 and Figure 2 here]
Republicans tend to be more skeptical about climate change and less supportive of
laws to reduce greenhouse gases. However, we find that when Republican Party leaders
are the source of the environmental threat message, Republican respondents significantly
increased their belief that climate change is an environmental threat (Table 3, Model 2, p
= .03), that a scientific consensus exists (Table 3, Model 6, p = .04), and support for
polices that would increase a reliance on renewable energy to decrease greenhouse gas
emissions (Table 3, Model 4, p = .09). When Republican Party leaders are linked with
the national security message it significantly reduced Republicans’ perceptions that
climate change research is politically motivated by nearly a full point on the response
scale (Table 3, Model 1, p = .01) and decreased perceptions among Republicans that
evidence regarding climate change is a conspiracy or hoax (Table 3, Model 1, p = .09).
[Insert Table 3 here]
On the other hand, we find that when Democratic Party leaders are linked with the
environmental threat appeal, Republican respondents report significantly greater
perceptions that climate change research is politically motivated (Table 3, Model 1, p = .
05), and become significantly more convinced that climate change a hoax (Table 3,
Model 1, p = .05). In contrast, when Republican Party leaders are associated with the
national security message, Republican respondents were significantly less likely to
perceive climate science research as driven by political motivations (Table 3, Model 1, p
= .01).
Discussion and Conclusions
The U.S. public remains polarized along partisan and ideological lines on the
issue of human-induced climate change. Science communicators and others who seek to
build greater consensus thus face challenges both in securing the public’s attention to
important information (Lupia, 2013), as well as motivating skeptical audiences to
evaluate information in an “even-handed” manner with the goal of arriving at an
“accurate,” or correct, conclusion (Druckman, 2013; Druckman & Lupia, 2017).
We investigated an approach to communicating information about climate change
that involved manipulating the source of a message, while holding the content of the
message constant. We find that the presence of military leaders as a source of a pro-
climate appeal can significantly strengthen its persuasive impact, especially in the case of
an appeal emphasizing the effects of climate change on U.S. national security. Republican
Party leaders also enhanced the effectiveness of a frame both on average and among in-
group respondents, perhaps due to the surprising effect that unconventional or unexpected
sources of information can have in terms of its persuasive impact. Partisans making
statements that do not align with their perceived group’s position may draw greater
attention to the frame’s content, may be seen as a “costly” signal thereby enhancing its
perceived honesty and credibility, or may reduce identity protective forms of motivated
reasoning that would otherwise lead to the rejection of arguments related to a polarized
and highly salient issue such as climate change (Bengal & Scruggs, 2018; Druckman &
McGrath, 2019; Kahan et al., 2011; Kahan, 2015).
On the other hand, we found a perhaps surprising impact of one information
source: climate scientists. When climate scientists were linked with the national security
message it significantly reduced respondents’ perceptions about the threat of climate
change to national security, support for policy action, and perceptions of a scientific
consensus. Indeed when messages were attributed to climate scientists, they either had
no effect on most of the variables we tested, or their effect was a negative one. The call
by the American Meteorological Society for more engagement in policy communication
by climate scientists (https://amspolicyforum.org/index.cfm/amspolicyforum/), suggests
the need for more interdisciplinary communication between climate scientists and those
who study the efficacy of message communication. Dissonant science messages may
threaten in-group identity (Dixon, Hmielowski, & Ma, 2017), leading to resistance and a
greater distrust and negative affect toward scientists, and less support for action to
address climate change (Dixon & Huber, 2018; Nisbet et al., 2015). Targeted frames,
however, that emphasize free-market solutions have been found to increase acceptance of
climate science among conservatives (Campbell & Kay, 2014; Dixon et al., 2017).
We also find that there are circumstances under which skeptical audiences will be
more open to climate change policy information. Republicans who received an
environmental threat message sponsored by Republican Party leaders increased the
strength of their belief that climate change poses a threat to sustainable environment, their
support for laws to reduce greenhouse gas emissions, and their beliefs that a scientific
consensus exists regarding human activities being the primary cause of climate change.
Similarly, Republicans who received a national security threat message sponsored by
Republican Party leaders were less likely to state that climate change research is driven
by political considerations, and reported significantly lower perceptions that climate
change is a conspiracy or hoax. This clearly demonstrates the power that trusted in-group
(i.e., in-party leaders) sources could play in overcoming hurdles posed by partisan
polarization on climate change. On the other hand, we find that when the source of the
environmental threat message is Democratic Party leaders, Republicans become
significantly more likely to report that climate change research is driven by political
motivations and more likely to say that climate change is a hoax. The source of an
identical appeal thus has a powerful impact on how the information affects individuals’
beliefs not only about the threats that climate change presents and willingness to support
policy action, but also on broader perceptions about the motivations of climate change
research, and even beliefs that climate change is a hoax. This is a finding that should
prompt further exploration.
Future research should also explore the generalizability of our findings and the
impact of varied sources associated with climate change information across different
populations and using different content messages (e.g., highlighting the source of
information on scientific consensus reports or public health considerations). The results
we report are encouraging insofar as we show that sources perceived to be credible and
trusted and that deliver climate change messages to skeptical audiences can break through
the barriers that impede communication efforts.
This paper highlights the role that credible sources can play in shaping skeptical
audiences views about climate change and overcoming partisan barriers that impede
efforts to generate greater consensus among the U.S. public. The finding that Republican
Party leaders, and military leaders, had the strongest effect, on average, at enhancing the
framed messages suggests that source credibility may increase when an “unconventional”
position is taken by group leaders (Prior, Partridge, & Plant, 2014). The finding that
climate scientists had little effect, or at worst a negative effect, also needs further
attention. More exploration is necessary to understand the efficacy of source credibility
linked with distinct arguments on climate change as well as the duration and persistence
of efforts to overcome polarization and foster consensus and climate engagement.
References
Bartels, L. M. (2002). Beyond the running tally: Partisan bias in political perceptions.
Political Behavior, 24(2), 117-150.
Benegal, S. D., & Scruggs, L. A. (2018). Correcting misinformation about climate
change: the impact of partisanship in an experimental setting. Climatic
Change, 148(1-2), 61-80.
Berinsky, A. J., Huber, G. A., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Evaluating online labor markets for
experimental research: Amazon. com's Mechanical Turk. Political Analysis,
20(3), 351-368.
Bickerstaff, K., Lorenzoni, I., Pidgeon, N. F., Poortinga, W., & Simmons, P. (2008).
Reframing nuclear power in the UK energy debate: nuclear power, climate change
mitigation and radioactive waste. Public Understanding of Science, 17(2), 145-
169.
Bolsen, T., & Druckman, J. N. (2018). Do partisanship and politicization undermine the
impact of a scientific consensus message about climate change? Group Processes
& Intergroup Relations, 21(3), 389-402.
Bolsen, T., Druckman, J. N., & Cook, F. L. (2014). The influence of partisan motivated
reasoning on public opinion. Political Behavior, 36(2), 235-262.
Bolsen, T., Kingsland, J., & Palm, R. (2018). The impact of frames highlighting coastal
flooding in the USA on climate change beliefs. Climatic Change, 147(1-2), 359-
368.
Bolsen, T., & Shapiro, M.A. (2017). Strategic framing and persuasive messaging to
influence climate change perceptions and decisions. Oxford Research
Encyclopedia of Climate Science. DOI:10.1093/acrefore/9780190228620.013.435
Brewer, P. R., & Ley, B. L. (2013). Whose science do you believe? Explaining trust in
sources of scientific information about the environment. Science
Communication, 35(1), 115-137.
Campbell, T. H., & Kay, A. C. (2014). Solution aversion: On the relation between
ideology and motivated disbelief. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 107(5), 809-824.
Chong, D., & Druckman, J. N. (2007). Framing theory. Annual Review of Political
Science, 10,103-126.
Cohen, G. L. (2003). Party over policy: The dominating impact of group influence on
political beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(5), 808-822.
Cook, J., & Lewandowsky, S. (2016). Rational irrationality: Modeling climate change
belief polarization using Bayesian networks. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8(1),
160-179.
Cook, J., Oreskes, N., Doran, P. T., Anderegg, W. R., Verheggen, B., Maibach, E. W., ...
& Nuccitelli, D. (2016). Consensus on consensus: a synthesis of consensus
estimates on human-caused global warming. Environmental Research
Letters, 11(4), 048002.
Dixon, G., Hmielowski, J., & Ma, Y. (2017). Improving climate change acceptance
among US conservatives through value-based message targeting. Science
Communication, 39(4), 520–534.
Dixon, G., & Hubner, A. (2018). Neutralizing the effect of political worldviews by
communicating scientific agreement: A thought-listing study. Science
Communication, 40(3), 393-415.
Druckman, J. N. (2001a). On the limits of framing effects: Who can frame? Journal of
Politics, 63(4), 1041-1066.
Druckman, J. N. (2001b). Using credible advice to overcome framing effects. Journal of
Law, Economics, and Organization, 17(1), 62-82.
Druckman, J. N. (2013). Public opinion: Stunted policy support. Nature Climate
Change, 3(7), 617.
Druckman, J. N., Leeper, T. J., & Slothuus, R. (2018). Motivated responses to political
communications: framing, party cues, and science information. In Lavine, H.G.,
Taber, C.S. (eds.) The Feeling, Thinking Citizen: Essays in Honor of Milton
Lodge. New York: Routledge.
Druckman, J. N. (2017). The crisis of politicization within and beyond science. Nature
Human Behaviour, 1(9), 615.
Druckman, J. N., & Lupia, A. (2017). Using frames to make scientific communication
effective. In D. Scheufele, D.M. Kahan, K.H. Jamieson, (Eds.), Handbook of the
Science of Science Communication (pp. 13–31). New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Druckman, J. N., & McGrath, M. C. (2019). The evidence for motivated reasoning in
climate change preference formation. Nature Climate Change, 9(2), 111-119.
Dunlap, R. E., McCright, A. M., & Yarosh, J. H. (2016). The political divide on climate
change: Partisan polarization widens in the US. Environment: Science and Policy
for Sustainable Development, 58(5), 4-23.
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Harcourt Brace
Jovanovich College Publishers.
Egan, P. J., & Mullins, M. (2017). Climate change: US public opinion. Annual Review of
Political Science, 20, 209-227.
Ehret, P. J., Van Boven, L., & Sherman, D. K. (2018). Partisan Barriers to Bipartisanship:
Understanding Climate Policy Polarization. Social Psychological and Personality
Science, 9(3), 308–318.
Feinberg, M., & Willer, R. (2013). The moral roots of environmental
attitudes. Psychological Science, 24(1), 56–62.
Feldman, L., & Hart, P.S. (2018). Is there any hope? How climate change news imagery
and text influence audience emotions and support for climate mitigation policies.
Risk Analysis, 38(3), 585–602.
Feldman, L., Maibach, E. W., Roser-Renouf, C., & Leiserowitz, A. (2012). Climate on
cable: The nature and impact of global warming coverage on Fox News, CNN,
and MSNBC. The International Journal of Press/Politics, 17(1), 3-31.
Gauchat, G. (2012). Politicization of science in the public sphere: A study of public trust
in the United States, 1974 to 2010. American Sociological Review, 77(2), 167-
187.
Goldberg, M., van der Linden, S., Leiserowitz, A., & Maibach, E. (2019). Perceived
Social Consensus Can Reduce Ideological Biases on Climate Change.
Environment and Behavior https://doi.org/10.17863/CAM.37992.
Goren, P., Federico, C. M., & Kittilson, M. C. (2009). Source cues, partisan identities,
and political value expression. American Journal of Political Science, 53(4), 805-
820.
Guber, Deborah Lynn. (2017). Partisan cueing and polarization in public opinion about
climate change. Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Climate Science. doi:
https://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228620.013.306.
Hamilton, L. C. (2016). Public awareness of scientific consensus on climate. Sage Open,
doi: 10.1177/2158244016676296.
Hamilton, L. C. (2011). Education, politics and opinions about climate change evidence
for interaction effects. Climatic Change, 104(2), 231-242.
Hart, P. S., & Nisbet, E. C. (2012). Boomerang effects in science communication: How
motivated reasoning and identity cues amplify opinion polarization about climate
mitigation policies. Communication Research, 39(6), 701-723.
Hart, P. S. (2011). One or many? The influence of episodic and thematic climate change
frames on policy preferences and individual behavior change. Science
Communication, 33(1), 28-51.
Hart, P. S., & Feldman, L. (2016). The impact of climate change–related imagery and
text on public opinion and behavior change. Science Communication, 38(4), 415-
441.
Hart, P. S., & Feldman, L. (2014). Threat without efficacy? Climate change on US
network news. Science Communication, 36(3), 325-351.
Hart, P. S., Feldman, L., Leiserowitz, A., & Maibach, E. (2015). Extending the impacts
of hostile media perceptions: influences on discussion and opinion polarization in
context of climate change. Science Communication, 37(4), 506–532.
Hmielowski, J. D., Feldman, L., Myers, T. A., Leiserowitz, A., & Maibach, E. (2014). An
attack on science? Media use, trust in scientists, and perceptions of global
warming. Public Understanding of Science, 23(7), 866-883.
Hogg, M. A., & Reid, S. A. (2006). Social identity, self-categorization, and the
communication of group norms. Communication Theory, 16(1), 7-30.
Huddy, L. (2001). From social to political identity: A critical examination of social
identity theory. Political Psychology, 22(1), 127-156.
Huddy, L., Mason, L., & Aarøe, L. (2015). Expressive partisanship: Campaign
involvement, political emotion, and partisan identity. American Political Science
Review, 109(1), 1-17.
Jost, J. T. (2018). Underestimating belief in climate change. Nature Climate
Change, 8(3), 189.
Jost, J. T., van der Linden, S., Panagopoulos, C., & Hardin, C. D. (2018). Ideological
asymmetries in conformity, desire for shared reality, and the spread of
misinformation. Current Opinion in Psychology, 23, 77-83.
Kahan, D. M. (2015). Climatescience communication and the measurement
problem. Political Psychology 36, 1-43.
Kahan, D. M. (2016). The Politically Motivated Reasoning Paradigm, Part 1: What
Politically Motivated Reasoning Is and How to Measure It. In Emerging Trends in
the Social and Behavioral Sciences (eds R. A. Scott and S. M. Kosslyn).
doi:10.1002/9781118900772.etrds0417
Kahan, D. M., JenkinsSmith, H., & Braman, D. (2011). Cultural cognition of scientific
consensus. Journal of Risk Research, 14(2), 147-174.
Kahan, D. M., Peters, E., Wittlin, M., Slovic, P., Ouellette, L. L., Braman, D., & Mandel,
G. (2012). The polarizing impact of science literacy and numeracy on perceived
climate change risks. Nature Climate Change, 2(10), 732.
Kousser, T., & Tranter, B. (2018). The influence of political leaders on climate change
attitudes. Global Environmental Change, 50, 100-109,
doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2018.03.005.
Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 480.
Leiserowitz, A. (2007). Communicating the risks of global warming: American risk
perceptions, affective images, and interpretive communities. In S.C. Moser, L.
Dilling (Eds.), Creating a climate for change: Communicating Climate Change
and Facilitating Social Change, (pp. 44-63). Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Leiserowitz, A., Maibach, E., Roser-Renouf, C., Rosenthal, S., Cutler, M., & Kotcher, J.
(2018). Climate change in the American mind: march 2018. Yale University and
George Mason University. New Haven (CT): Yale Program on Climate Change
Communication.
Lelkes, Y., Malka, A., & Bakker, B. N. (2018). An expressive utility account of partisan
cue receptivity: Cognitive resources in the service of identity expression.
Unpublished manuscript.
Lenz, G. S. (2012). Follow the leader?: how voters respond to politicians' policies and
performance. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: A
typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organizational Behavior and
Human Decision Processes, 76(2), 149-188.
Lewandowsky, S., Oberauer, K., & Gignac, G. E. (2013). NASA faked the moon landing
—therefore,(climate) science is a hoax: An anatomy of the motivated rejection of
science. Psychological Science, 24(5), 622-633.
Lewandowsky, S., Gignac, G. E., & Vaughan, S. (2013). The pivotal role of perceived
scientific consensus in acceptance of science. Nature Climate Change, 3(4), 399.
Li, N., Hilgard, J., Scheufele, D. A., Winneg, K. M., & Jamieson, K. H. (2016). Cross-
pressuring conservative Catholics? Effects of Pope Francis’ encyclical on the US
public opinion on climate change. Climatic Change, 139(3-4), 367-380.
Liu, H., & Priest, S. (2009). Understanding public support for stem cell research: media
communication, interpersonal communication and trust in key actors. Public
Understanding of science, 18(6), 704-718.
Lupia, A. (2013). Communicating science in politicized environments. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences 110 (Supplement 3): 14048-14054.
Maibach, E., Leiserowitz, A., Roser-Renouf, C., Myers, T., Rosenthal, S., & Feinberg, G.
(2015). The Francis effect: how Pope Francis changed the conversation about
global warming. Yale University and George Mason University. New Haven,
CT: Yale Program on Climate Change Communication.
Malka, A., Krosnick, J. A., & Langer, G. (2009). The association of knowledge with
concern about global warming: Trusted information sources shape public
thinking. Risk Analysis: An International Journal, 29(5), 633-647.
Malka, A., & Lelkes, Y. (2010). More than ideology: Conservative–liberal identity and
receptivity to political cues. Social Justice Research, 23(2-3), 156-188.
McCright, A. M., Dentzman, K., Charters, M., & Dietz, T. (2013). The influence of
political ideology on trust in science. Environmental Research Letters, 8(4),
044029.
McCright, A. M., & Dunlap, R. E. (2011). The politicization of climate change and
polarization in the American public's views of global warming, 2001–2010. The
Sociological Quarterly, 52(2), 155-194.
McCright, A. M., Charters, M., Dentzman, K., & Dietz, T. (2016). Examining the
effectiveness of climate change frames in the face of a climate change denial
counter‐frame. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8(1), 76-97.
Mildenberger, M., & Tingley, D. (2017). Beliefs about climate beliefs: the importance of
second-order opinions for climate politics. British Journal of Political Science, 1-
29. doi:10.1017/S0007123417000321
Miller, J. M., Saunders, K. L., & Farhart, C. E. (2016). Conspiracy endorsement as
motivated reasoning: The moderating roles of political knowledge and
trust. American Journal of Political Science, 60(4), 824-844.
Motta, M. (2018). The Polarizing Effect of the March for Science on Attitudes toward
Scientists. PS: Political Science & Politics, 51(4), 782-788.
Mullinix, K. J., Leeper, T. J., Druckman, J. N., & Freese, J. (2015). The generalizability
of survey experiments. Journal of Experimental Political Science, 2(2): 109-138.
Myers, T. A., Nisbet, M. C., Maibach, E. W., & Leiserowitz, A. A. (2012). A public
health frame arouses hopeful emotions about climate change. Climatic
Change, 113(3-4), 1105-1112.
Nisbet, E. C., Cooper, K. E., & Garrett, R. K. (2015). The partisan brain: How dissonant
science messages lead conservatives and liberals to (dis) trust science. The
ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 658(1), 36-66.
Nisbet, M. C. (2009). Communicating climate change: Why frames matter for public
engagement. Environment: Science and Policy for Sustainable Development,
51(2): 12-23.
Nisbet, M. C., & Myers, T. (2007). The polls—trends: Twenty years of public opinion
about global warming. Public Opinion Quarterly, 71(3), 444-470.
O’Keefe, D.J. (1990). Persuasion: Theory and Research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Oliver, J. E., & Wood, T. J. (2014). Conspiracy theories and the paranoid style (s) of
mass opinion. American Journal of Political Science, 58(4), 952-966.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion.
In Communication and persuasion (pp. 1-24). Springer, New York, NY.
Priest, S. H. (2001). Misplaced faith: Communication variables as predictors of
encouragement for biotechnology development. Science Communication, 23(2),
97-110.
Renn, O., & Levine, D. (1991). Credibility and trust in risk communication.
In Communicating risks to the public (pp. 175-217). Springer, Dordrecht.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of
choice. Science, 211(4481), 453-458.
Uscinski, J. E., Klofstad, C., & Atkinson, M. D. (2016). What drives conspiratorial
beliefs? The role of informational cues and predispositions. Political Research
Quarterly, 69(1), 57-71.
Uscinski, J. E., & Parent, J. M. (2014). American conspiracy theories. Oxford University
Press.
Van Boven, L., Ehret, P. J., & Sherman, D. K. (2018). Psychological barriers to
bipartisan public support for climate policy. Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 13(4), 492-507.
van der Linden, S. (2015). The social-psychological determinants of climate change risk
perceptions: Towards a comprehensive model. Journal of Environmental
Psychology, 41, 112-124.
van der Linden, S., Leiserowitz, A., & Maibach, E. (2019). The gateway belief model: A
large-scale replication. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 62 (April 2019),
49-58 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.01.009.
Villar, A., & Krosnick, J. A. (2011). Global warming vs. climate change, taxes vs. prices:
Does word choice matter?. Climatic change, 105(1-2), 1-12.
Zhou, J. (2016). Boomerangs versus javelins: how polarization constrains communication
on climate change. Environmental Politics, 25(5), 788-811.
Table 1. Experimental Design and Predictions
Condition Treatment Predicted outcome
National
Security
(No source)
(N=184)
Some groups have recently argued that droughts and
flooding caused by climate change are a threat to the
security of the United States…
National Security
Baseline
Military
Leaders
(N=184)
A group of military leaders including generals and
homeland security officers have recently argued that
droughts and flooding caused by climate change are a
threat to the security of the United States…
H1: increase the
frame’s impact.
Democratic
Party Leaders
(N=178)
A group of Democratic leaders in the House and Senate
have recently argued that droughts and flooding caused
by climate change are a threat to the security of the
United States….
H3: increase
impact for Dems,
no effect on
Repubs.
Republican
Party Leaders
(N=183)
A group of Republican leaders in the House and Senate
have recently argued that droughts and flooding caused
by climate change are a threat to the security of the
United States…
H3: increase
impact for
Repubs., no effect
on Dems.
Climate
Scientists
(N=184)
A group of climate scientists have recently argued that
droughts and flooding caused by climate change are a
threat to the security of the United States…
No prediction
relative to
baseline.
Environment
(No source)
(N=192)
Some groups have recently argued that climate change
is a threat to the environment of the United States…
Environmental
Baseline
Military
Leaders
(N=187)
A group of military leaders including generals and
homeland security officers have recently argued that
climate change is a threat to the environment of the
United States…
No prediction
relative to
baseline.
Democratic
Party Leaders
(N=189)
A group of Democratic leaders in the House and Senate
have recently argued that climate change is a threat to
the environment of the United States.
H3: increase
impact for Dems,
no effect on
Repubs.
Republican
Party Leaders
(N=183)
A group of Republican leaders in the House and Senate
have recently argued that climate change is a threat to
the environment of the United States.
H3: increase
impact for
Repubs., no effect
on Dems.
Climate
Scientists
(N=184)
A group of climate scientists have recently argued that
climate change is a threat to the environment of the
United States.
H2: increase the
frame’s impact.
Figure 1. Message Source Effects Relative to No Source Appeal’s Baseline
Table 2: Main Effects (Democrats only)
National Security Frame
Threat Nat.
Sec.
(Model 1)
Threat
Env.
(Model 2)
Politically
Motivated
(Model 3)
Support
Laws
(Model 4)
Happening
(Model 5)
Scientific
Consensus
(Model 6)
Hoax
(Model 7)
Military Leaders 0.28*0.17 0.06 0.15 0.09 0.04 0.19
(0.18) (0.13) (0.25) (0.17) (0.15) (0.17) (0.24)
Democrat Leaders -0.21 0.18*-0.22 -0.07 0.02 -0.01 0.03
(0.19) (0.13) (0.25) (0.17) (0.15) (0.17) (0.24)
Republican Leaders -0.15 0.11 -0.36*0.14 0.16 0.06 -0.25
(0.18) (0.13) (0.25) (0.17) (0.15) (0.17) (0.24)
Climate Scientists 0.19 0.00 0.01 -0.18 -0.17 -0.24*0.13
(0.19) (0.14) (0.25) (0.17) (0.15) (0.17) (0.24)
Constant (No-Source Nat. Sec.) 3.33*** 4.38*** 2.41*** 6.43*** 6.47*** 6.31*** 1.84***
(0.12) (0.09) (0.17) (0.11) (0.10) (0.12) (0.16)
N392 392 392 392 392 392 391
Environmental Frame
Military Leaders 0.57*** 0.10 -0.49** 0.04 0.05 0.10 -0.04
(0.19) (0.12) (0.25) (0.16) (0.15) (0.16) (0.22)
Democrat Leaders 0.19 0.22** -0.19 -0.05 0.12 0.02 -0.11
(0.20) (0.13) (0.26) (0.17) (0.16) (0.17) (0.23)
Republican Leaders 0.21 0.09 -0.09 -0.13 -0.22*-0.02 0.23
(0.20) (0.12) (0.26) (0.16) (0.15) (0.17) (0.22)
Climate Scientists 0.06 0.16*0.01 -0.04 0.05 -0.23*0.06
(0.20) (0.12) (0.26) (0.16) (0.15) (0.17) (0.22)
Constant (No-Source Env.) 3.22*** 4.42*** 2.55*** 6.45*** 6.44*** 6.26*** 1.76***
(0.14) (0.09) (0.18) (0.11) (0.11) (0.12) (0.16)
N422 422 421 422 421 422 422
Note: Cell entries are OLS coefficient estimates, with standard errors in parentheses. Conditions receiving the national security frame are compared to the No-
Source National Security baseline; conditions receiving the environmental frame are compared to the No-Source Environmental condition. Asterisks represent
one-tailed p-values. * p < 0.10 ** p < 0.05 *** p < 0.01
Table 3: Main Effects (Republicans only)
National Security Frame
Threat Nat.
Sec.
(Model 1)
Threat
Env.
(Model 2)
Politically
Motivated
(Model 3)
Support
Laws
(Model 4)
Happening
(Model 5)
Scientific
Consensus
(Model 6)
Hoax
(Model 7)
Military Leaders 0.47** 0.01 -0.61** 0.13 -0.27 0.09 -0.61*
(0.23) (0.25) (0.37) (0.36) (0.35) (0.34) (0.38)
Democrat Leaders 0.19 -0.11 -0.50*0.20 -0.44*-0.06 -0.29
(0.22) (0.24) (0.35) (0.35) (0.34) (0.33) (0.37)
Republican Leaders 0.22 -0.01 -0.96*** 0.18 0.09 -0.01 -0.51*
(0.23) (0.25) (0.37) (0.37) (0.35) (0.35) (0.39)
Climate Scientists 0.12 -0.26 -0.23 -0.22 -0.32 0.05 -0.06
(0.21) (0.23) (0.34) (0.34) (0.33) (0.32) (0.36)
Constant (No-Source Nat. Sec.) 2.09*** 3.16*** 4.82*** 4.55*** 4.91*** 4.85*** 4.07***
(0.16) (0.17) (0.25) (0.25) (0.25) (0.24) (0.27)
N287 287 288 288 288 287 287
Environmental Frame
Military Leaders 0.13 0.17 0.38 0.16 0.02 0.02 0.54*
(0.26) (0.26) (0.38) (0.36) (0.35) (0.32) (0.38)
Democrat Leaders -0.21 -0.10 0.59*0.07 0.12 0.04 0.60**
(0.25) (0.25) (0.36) (0.34) (0.33) (0.30) (0.36)
Republican Leaders 0.18 0.50** -0.12 0.51*0.43 0.60** -0.16
(0.26) (0.26) (0.38) (0.36) (0.35) (0.31) (0.37)
Climate Scientists 0.09 0.05 0.44 0.11 0.06 0.54** 0.43
(0.25) (0.26) (0.37) (0.35) (0.34) (0.31) (0.37)
Constant (No-Source Env.) 2.48*** 3.08*** 4.24*** 4.76*** 4.80*** 4.82*** 3.52***
(0.18) (0.19) (0.27) (0.26) (0.25) (0.22) (0.27)
N274 274 271 273 273 274 273
Note: Cell entries are OLS coefficient estimates, with standard errors in parentheses. Conditions receiving the national security frame are compared to the No-
Source National Security baseline; conditions receiving the environmental frame are compared to the No-Source Environmental condition. Asterisks represent
one-tailed p-values. * p < 0.10 ** p < 0.05 *** p < 01.
... Political elites influence climate change attitudes and behaviors (Cole, Ehret, et al., in prep;Ehret et al., 2018;Tesler, 2018;Van Boven et al., 2018;. Bolsen et al. (2019) compared the influence of messages from Democratic party leaders, Republican party leaders, military leaders, and climate scientists about climate change. They found that Republican party leaders and military leaders were most effective for participants from both parties (Bolsen et al., 2019). ...
... Bolsen et al. (2019) compared the influence of messages from Democratic party leaders, Republican party leaders, military leaders, and climate scientists about climate change. They found that Republican party leaders and military leaders were most effective for participants from both parties (Bolsen et al., 2019). Nonpolitical elites can also be influential. ...
Preprint
Political polarization is a barrier to enacting policy solutions to global issues. Social psychology has a rich history of studying polarization, and there is an important opportunity to refine and define its contributions to the present political realities. We do so in the context of one of the most pressing modern issues: climate change. We synthesize the literature on political polarization and its applications to climate change, and we propose lines of further research and intervention design. We focus on polarization in the United States, examining other countries when literature is available. The polarization literature emphasizes two types of mechanisms: individual-level psychological processes related to political ideology and group-level psychological processes related to partisan identification. We highlight the potential intervention strategies of circumventing solution aversion, leveraging superordinate identities, correcting misperceived norms, and having trusted experts and politicians communicate about climate change. Interventions that address group-level processes can be more effective than those that address individual-level processes. These areas of research and intervention development are particularly important given that behavioral interventions grounded in scientific research are one of our most promising tools to achieve the behavioral wedge we need to address climate change and to make progress on other policy issues.
... Many studies consider normative claims such as acceptance of climate policy or with a mix of factual and normative claims (see, e.g.,Hart and Nisbet 2012;Constantino et al., 2021;Dietz 2013;Zhou 2016;Bolsen et al., 2019;Kahan 2010Kahan , 2016aKahan , 2016bKahan , 2017. See Hornsey and Lewandowsky 2022 for an overview). ...
Article
Full-text available
On the assumption that science communication is a part of the scientific practice and enterprise, I critically assess a highly influential science communication strategy, Value-Based Reporting, from the perspective of philosophy of science. On the basis of the critical assessment, I motivate an alternative positive approach, Justification Reporting which aligns better with the norms and values of science. In conclusion, I consider integrations between the two approaches.
... More specifically, part of rightleaning individuals' scepticism about the issue of climate change and related policies might derive from the fact that advocacy of these policies typically comes from sources such as left wing, progressive and liberal parties (Merkley and Stecula 2021), interest groups and organised movements (Mede and Schroeder 2024;Sorce 2022). If this is the case, climate policy proposals might be more attractive to right-wing recipients when they originate from sources perceived as politically right wing, such as conservative politicians, opinion leaders and interest groups (Bolsen, Palm, and Kingsland 2019), or from non-partisan sources, such as scientists, experts (Hmielowski et al. 2014(Hmielowski et al. , 2022 or even non-experts and ordinary people (see Mede, Schäfer, and Füchslin 2021 for a discussion of the phenomenon of scientific populism). In our Study 2, we varied the source of the message to which participants from the left and right were exposed, to assess whether the source of the message would influence their evaluation of the climate change policy addressed in the message. ...
Article
Full-text available
Political polarisation on the issue of climate change is an obstacle to an effective public debate on the measures needed to tackle the problem. Communication that focuses on the economic dimension could be one way to increase the limited support of right‐wing audiences for climate policy. In two experimental studies, we examined how a message advocating a switch from fossil fuels to renewable energy sources affects participants' support for this policy. The message focused on the environmental or economic consequences of the policy (Studies 1 and 2), was framed in terms of gain or loss (Study 1) and was attributed to different sources (Study 2). Participants' political orientation and their beliefs and concerns about climate change were also assessed. The results showed that attitudes towards climate change and support for the policy were strongly related to political orientation. Right‐leaning participants were significantly less concerned about climate change and less supportive of the proposed policy. This effect was moderated by the focus of the message, as right‐leaning participants agreed more with the economic focus message than the environmental focus message, while left‐leaning participants did the opposite. The discussion focuses on the role of communication and message framing in integrating climate action into the political agenda and bridging the existing political divide.
... For example, in cognitive response models of persuasion (Cacioppo et al., 1986;Chaiken & Maheswaran, 1994), attitudes are influenced by heuristic cues in the peripheral route (Cummings, 2014;Metzger et al., 2010). This includes source credibility (Alcalá et al., 2017;Bochner & Insko, 1966;Bolsen et al., 2019). Also known as epistemic authority (Kruglanski et al., 2009), credibility comprises both perceived expertise and trustworthiness, or the perceived intent not to harm a message. ...
Article
Full-text available
As COVID-19 vaccination hesitancy remains a major public health issue, understanding the factors influencing attitudes and COVID-19 vaccination intentions is a public health priority. Applying the theory of planned behavior (TPB), this study examined the role of two forms of social trust—namely, particularized trust toward relationally close others and generalized trust toward people in general—in moderating the relationship between social norms (injunctive and descriptive) and COVID-19 vaccination attitudes and intent. In two studies (n = 235 for Study 1, n = 273 for Study 2, total N = 508), we found some support for the TPB in the context of COVID-19 vaccination, with attitudes and injunctive norms significantly predicting vaccination intention. However, perceived behavioral control was not a significant predictor of COVID-19 vaccination intention. Extending the TPB, we found that trust in others had an amplifying effect on the relationship between descriptive norms and COVID-19 vaccination attitudes. However, trust attenuated the link between injunctive norms and attitudes toward COVID-19 vaccination. The implications of these findings are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Study purpose Social media influencers in China have significantly influenced public political and social attitudes and behaviors. Based on the origin of fame theory, this study explores which type of influencers – originals, celebrities, or journalists – are most effective in driving changes in their followers’ attitudes and behaviors. Methodology An online experiment used a 3 (type of influencer: original, celebrity, journalist) × 2 (message context: policy-based, social modeling) design. A total of 627 responses were collected to assess how participants evaluate social media influencers and to examine their attitudes and behaviors toward the issues supported by these influencers. Main findings The findings reveal that the effects of social media influencers depend on both their identity and the type of message they promote. Journalist influencers have the greatest effects on participants’ attitudes toward policy-based messages compared to original and celebrity influencers. By contrast, original influencers are more effective at shaping attitudes when sharing social modeling messages. Additionally, the study shows that journalist influencers are perceived to have greater expertise than other influencers, regardless of the message context. Social implications This study shows that identity and message context jointly determine the influence of social media influencers on promoting nonprofit campaigns. Practical implications Communicators can apply the findings of this study to select appropriate social media influencers by considering both the origin of influencers’ fame and the context of the message they are promoting. Originality/value This study emphasizes the power of social media influencers to promote nonprofit campaigns and identifies the conditions that can enhance their effectiveness.
Chapter
Full-text available
Public opinion plays a central role in determining the feasibility of efforts to transform energy systems in the coming years, yet scholarship on communication effects and public opinion about clean energy and energy efficiency seems to have expanded only relatively recently. There is a growing body of work that explores how targeted and strategically framed messages affect individuals' beliefs and motivations to act on matters affecting household energy choices as well as energy policies. One must attend particularly to the principal communication-based factors that shape the public's understanding of clean energy sources and promote efficiencies in energy use. To better understand the communication vehicles for improving both household energy efficiency and conservation, two research foci are most relevant: (1) field experiments that primarily assess how household energy consumption shifts after receiving energy consumption reports and (2) surveys/laboratory experiments that focus on the nuances of energy-related communications, paying particular attention to the role of politics and ideology. This bimodal classification of clean energy and efficiency communication research genres is not exhaustive but can be synthesized into two major contributions. First, providing households with information about specific benefits that would result from a greater reliance on clean energy may increase support for its development and move individuals toward energy efficiency outcomes; however, exposure to counter-messages that emphasize costs associated with clean energy and the associated policies can negate the effects of pro-clean energy messages. Second, there is still no reprieve from the politicization of energy, and thus the role of partisanship and motivated reasoning must be accounted for when assessing how individuals modify their decision-making processes regarding energy efficiency.
Chapter
Full-text available
Despite an accumulation of scientific evidence on both the causes and consequences of climate change, U.S. public opinion on the subject has splintered sharply along party lines. While a vast majority of Democrats now believe that global warming is real, that its effects will happen within their lifetime, and that human activity is the dominant cause, Republicans have grown increasingly skeptical, creating a yawning gap that complicates efforts to communicate the urgency of the problem and the need for aggressive action. When attitudes harden and diverge, it is often driven by the behavior of political elites, who shape the frames and mental models that people use to interpret events. Scholars have long observed that people resort instinctively to heuristics to ease the burden of making decisions, especially on issues like climate, where there is an obvious disconnect between scientific understanding and mass competence. Those cues, however, are often unreliable and prone to cognitive bias. When voters act upon signals provided by their preferred political party and by selective exposure to preferred media outlets, they may do so mechanically, with little regard for the accuracy of the evidence that they receive, or they may ignore and distort information in a way that reinforces preexisting assumptions. In the end, beliefs about climate change are as complex as the issue itself, which suggests that awareness of the problem and an understanding of its effects will not translate automatically-or even easily-into increased concern, issue salience, or policy preferences. The "pictures in our heads," to borrow Walter Lippmann's famous phrase, are shaped less by factual knowledge than by a variety of other factors more difficult to control-by personal experience and assorted real-world cues (such as the weather), but also by opinion leaders, media narratives, and political rhetoric, each of which provides a competing frame of reference with the power to filter and mislead. Because climate change has become so heavily laden with values and so absorbed into partisan identity, it will be nearly impossible to build social consensus through conventional means. Once a "hard" issue for all, which seemed to demand sophisticated calculation or technical expertise, it has now become an "easy" one for many, where the reactions that it prompts are familiar, stable, and symbolic, increasingly polarized, immune to rational argument, and vulnerable to manipulation by elites.
Article
Full-text available
Psychological scientists have the expertise—and arguably an obligation—to help understand the political polarization that impedes enactment of climate policy. Many explanations emphasize Republican skepticism about climate change. Yet results from national panel studies in 2014 and 2016 indicate that most Republicans believe in climate change, if not as strongly as Democrats. Political polarization over climate policy does not simply reflect that Democrats and Republicans disagree about climate change but that Democrats and Republicans disagree with each other. The results of a national panel experiment and of in-depth interviews with four former members of Congress suggest that Democrats and Republicans—both ordinary citizens and policymakers—support policies from their own party and reactively devalue policies from the opposing party. These partisan evaluations occur both for policies historically associated with liberal principles and politicians (cap-and-trade) and for policies associated with conservative principles and politicians (revenue-neutral carbon tax). People also exaggerate how much other Democrats and Republicans are swayed by partisanship. This foments false norms of partisan opposition that, in turn, influence people’s personal policy support. Correcting misperceived norms of opposition and decoupling policy evaluation from identity concerns would help overcome these seemingly insurmountable barriers to bipartisan support for climate policy.
Chapter
The need for effective communication, public outreach and education to increase support for policy, collective action and behaviour change is ever present, and is perhaps most pressing in the context of anthropogenic climate change. This book is the first to take a comprehensive look at communication and social change specifically targeted to climate change. It is a unique collection of ideas examining the challenges associated with communicating climate change in order to facilitate societal response. It offers well-founded, practical suggestions on how to communicate climate change and how to approach related social change more effectively. The contributors of this book come from a diverse range of backgrounds, from government and academia to non-governmental and civic sectors of society. The book is accessibly written, and any specialized terminology is explained. It will be of great interest to academic researchers and professionals in climate change, environmental policy, science communication, psychology, sociology and geography.
Article
The Gateway Belief Model describes a process of attitudinal change where a shift in people's perception of the scientific consensus on an issue leads to subsequent changes in their attitudes which in turn predict changes in support for public action. In the current study, we present the first large-scale confirmatory replication of the GBM. Specifically, we conducted a consensus message experiment on a national quota sample of the US population (N=6,301). Results support the mediational hypotheses of the GBM: an experimentally induced change in perceived scientific consensus causes subsequent changes in cognitive (belief) and affective (worry) judgments about climate change, which in turn are associated with changes in support for public action. The scientific consensus message also had a direct effect on support for public action. We further found an interaction with both political ideology and prior attitudes such that conservatives and climate change disbelievers were more likely to update their beliefs toward the consensus. We discuss the model's theoretical and practical implications, including why conveying scientific consensus can help reduce politically motivated reasoning
Chapter
Science can serve as a valuable foundation for the making of public policy. For science to have this effect, it must be effectively communicated to individuals, organizations, and institutions. Effective science communication often involves frames that highlight particular aspects of a scientific finding or issue. This chapter discusses ways in which frames can be used to facilitate effective scientific communication- particularly we explore the impact of frames with regard to attention limitations, political polarization, and the politicization of science. We also highlight unanswered questions and challenges. The main lesson of this chapter is that there are certain conditions under which choosing particular frames yields more effective communication. While understanding these conditions does not guarantee success, it can help science communicators avoid common mistakes.
Article
Despite a scientific consensus, citizens are divided when it comes to climate change — often along political lines. Democrats or liberals tend to believe that human activity is a primary cause of climate change, whereas Republicans or conservatives are much less likely to hold this belief. A prominent explanation for this divide is that it stems from directional motivated reasoning: individuals reject new information that contradicts their standing beliefs. In this Review, we suggest that the empirical evidence is not so clear, and is equally consistent with a theory in which citizens strive to form accurate beliefs but vary in what they consider to be credible evidence. This suggests a new research agenda on climate change preference formation, and has implications for effective communication.
Article
Americans’ attitudes toward scientists have become more negative in recent years. Although researchers have considered several individual-level factors that might explain this change, little attention has been given to the political actions of scientists themselves. This article considers how March for Science rallies that took place across the United States in late April 2017 influenced Americans’ attitudes toward scientists and the research they produce. An online panel study surveying respondents three days before and two days after the March found that liberals’ and conservatives’ attitudes toward scientists polarized following the March. Liberals’ attitudes toward scientists became more positive whereas conservatives’ attitudes became more negative. However, the March appears to have had little effect on the public’s attitudes about scientific research. In addition to answering questions about the March’s political impact, this research calls attention to the possibility that the political actions of scientists can shape public opinion about them.