ArticlePDF Available

The Social Media Party: Fear of Missing Out (FoMO), Social Media Intensity, Connection, and Well-Being

Authors:

Abstract

The human need to belong is an innate drive that dictates much of our behavior. Informed by The Belongingness Hypothesis and Information Foraging Theory, the present study examines the relationship between FoMO and well-being. Study 1 (107 college students) investigates the relationship between FoMO, social media intensity and social connection. Results find that FoMO is positively associated with social media intensity, but negatively associated with social connection. The mediation tests, interestingly, reveal more positive results regarding FoMO. Specifically, FoMO has a positive indirect effect on social connection through social media intensity, suggesting that FoMO may, in some cases, be a good thing leading to enhanced social connection. Study 2 (458 college students) finds that FoMO impacts subjective well-being both directly (negatively) and indirectly (positively) through its impact on social media intensity and social connection. Results of the two studies reveal a nuanced model of FoMO and its relationships with social media intensity, connection, and well-being. FoMO can have a positive impact on well-being if it leads to social media use that fosters social connection. Study limitations and future research directions are discussed.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=hihc20
International Journal of Human–Computer Interaction
ISSN: 1044-7318 (Print) 1532-7590 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hihc20
The Social Media Party: Fear of Missing Out
(FoMO), Social Media Intensity, Connection, and
Well-Being
James A. Roberts & Meredith E. David
To cite this article: James A. Roberts & Meredith E. David (2019): The Social Media Party: Fear of
Missing Out (FoMO), Social Media Intensity, Connection, and Well-Being, International Journal of
Human–Computer Interaction, DOI: 10.1080/10447318.2019.1646517
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/10447318.2019.1646517
Published online: 26 Jul 2019.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 166
View related articles
View Crossmark data
The Social Media Party: Fear of Missing Out (FoMO), Social Media Intensity,
Connection, and Well-Being
James A. Roberts and Meredith E. David
Marketing Department, Hankamer School of Business, Baylor University, Waco, Texas, USA
ABSTRACT
The human need to belong is an innate drive that dictates much of our behavior. Informed by The
Belongingness Hypothesis and Information Foraging Theory, the present study examines the relation-
ship between FoMO and well-being. Study 1 (107 college students) investigates the relationship
between FoMO, social media intensity and social connection. Results find that FoMO is positively
associated with social media intensity, but negatively associated with social connection. The mediation
tests, interestingly, reveal more positive results regarding FoMO. Specifically, FoMO has a positive
indirect effect on social connection through social media intensity, suggesting that FoMO may, in
some cases, be a good thing leading to enhanced social connection. Study 2 (458 college students)
finds that FoMO impacts subjective well-being both directly (negatively) and indirectly (positively)
through its impact on social media intensity and social connection. Results of the two studies reveal
a nuanced model of FoMO and its relationships with social media intensity, connection, and well-being.
FoMO can have a positive impact on well-being if it leads to social media use that fosters social
connection. Study limitations and future research directions are discussed.
1. Introduction
Sarah is twenty-one years old and a classic texts major at
a small private university in upstate New York. She spends
a good portion of the day scanning her social media feeds for
news on friends, classmates, and even complete strangers. She
scrolls social media before she goes to bed, often awakes to
notifications during the night, and social media is the first
thing she checks after waking in the morning. Most of the
time she scrolls through her various social media accounts
without any clear objective, other than to fulfill a nagging
sense that she may be missing out on something important.
The above composite is typical of the important role social
media plays in the life of the archetypal college student. Social
media addresses/exploits the need to belong that is present (in
varying degrees) in individuals of all ages (Beyens, Frison, &
Eggermont, 2016; Nadkarni & Hofmann, 2012). In a generation
thatischaracterizedbyadesiretobeconstantlyconnected,social
media allows its users to constantly monitor their social networks
for any developments (David, Roberts, & Christenson, 2017;
Roberts, Petnji YaYa, & Manolis, 2014;Roberts&Pirog,2013).
As noted by Przybylski, Murayama, DeHaan, and Gladwell
(2013), social media has a dual nature itcanopenmultiple
avenues for interaction and/or it can expose users to a vast array of
opportunities for interaction that are too numerous to pursue.
The fear of missing out (FoMO) is best understood as a
pervasive apprehension that others might be having
a rewarding experience from which one is absent(Przybylski
et al., 2013, p. 1841). Three-quarters of young adults have self-
diagnosed as having experienced a fear of missing out on enjoy-
able activities experienced by others, and, importantly, sharedon
social media (Przybylski et al., 2013). Why has the FoMO
reached such epidemic proportions? And, why is social media
so popular among young adults?
First and foremost, humans are social animals. Our innate
need to belong is essential to both our physical and mental
well-being. A strong social network increases our likelihood of
living longer and happier lives (Holt-Lunstad, Smith, &
Layton, 2010). A real or imagined sense of social exclusion
can negatively impact both the quantity and quality of our
lives (Konrath, 2018). From an evolutionary perspective,
social exclusion was often life threatening; social groups
which ostracized or excluded individuals often became stron-
ger together, while the excluded individual(s) often died
(Gruter & Masters, 1986). Indeed, the potential threat of
social exclusion poses a significant threat to ones innate
need to belong (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Baumeister &
Tice, 1990; Williams, 2007).
Previous research on the relationship between social media
use and well-being has been equivocal. Heavy social media use
has been linked to a variety of negative psychological out-
comes including increased stress levels, anxiety, depression,
lower levels of self-esteem, reduced relationship quality, and
lower sleep quality, as well as increased suicidal ideation and
suicide events among adolescents (Adams & Kisler, 2013;
CONTACT James A. Roberts jim_roberts@baylor.edu Marketing Department, Hankamer School of Business, Baylor University, One Bear Place, # 98007,
Waco, Texas 76798-8007, USA.
Color versions of one or more of the figures in the article can be found online at www.tandfonline.com/hihc.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMANCOMPUTER INTERACTION
https://doi.org/10.1080/10447318.2019.1646517
© 2019 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
Kross et al., 2013; Tromholt, 2016; Twenge, Joiner, Rogers, &
Martin, 2018; Woods & Scott, 2016).
Social media use has also been linked to positive psycho-
logical outcomes, particularly, building social capital (Ellison,
Steinfeld, & Lampe, 2007; Green-Hamann, Eichhorn, &
Sherblom, 2011), self-esteem (Best, Manktelow, & Taylor,
2014), connection with others (Sheldon, Abad, & Hinsch,
2011), and lessened feelings of depression (Deters & Mehl,
2013). Recent research suggests a possible explanation for this
apparent anomaly (Shaw, Timpano, Tran, & Joorman, 2015;
Verduyn et al., 2015).
1.1. The present study
The role FoMO plays in social media use, a sense of connec-
tion with others, and ultimately, psychological well-being is
the primary focus of the present research. The present study
makes several important contributions to the current litera-
ture. First, and as shown in our conceptual model in Figure 1,
the role of FoMO in social media use is investigated. The
Belonging Hypothesis (Baumeister & Leary, 1995) and
Information Foraging Theory (Muntinga & Taylor, 2018;
Trepass, 2018) are used to explain why individuals turn to
social media to assuage the innate need of all humans to be in
relationship with others. This is an evolving area of research
and is particularly important given the rising use of social
media, yet paradoxically, an increasing sense of social isola-
tion among industrialized populations (Holt-Lunstad et al.,
2010).
A second contribution of the present study is that it
hypothesizes and tests a sequence by which FoMO drives
social media use, which in turn impacts social connection,
and ultimately affects psychological well-being. FoMO is
usually associated with lower connection and well-being, but
our data shows that if manifested in social media intensity,
FoMO can foster social connection and well-being. The
apparent contradiction in the larger culture between increas-
ing use of social media and attendant increasing sense of
social isolation underscores the importance of research that
investigates the impact of social media use on well-being.
2. Theoretical support for the proposed model
2.1. FoMO and social media intensity
In their pioneering research on the fear of missing out
(FoMO), Przybylski et al. (2013) defined the fear as a perva-
sive apprehension that others might be having rewarding
experiences from which one is absent(p. 1841). People
with a high FoMO want to be constantly connected with
others and stay abreast with what others are doing (Abeele
& Rooij, 2016; Beyens, Frison, & Eggemont, 2016; Przybylski
et al., 2013). The need to belong, to feel close and connected
to others, is a basic psychological need that drives our beha-
vior as humans (Deci & Ryan, 2000). As found by Przybylski
et al. (2013), a deficit in the psychological need for relatedness
is likely to increase ones FoMO. In turn, this heightened
FoMO finds a convenient outlet in social media where users
can stay continually in touch with their peer group.
Using a student sample of 296 Israeli undergraduate stu-
dents, Alt (2015) found that FoMO leads to increased social
media use in the classroom. FoMO was found to mediate the
impact of two academic motivations and social media use.
A later study by Alt (2017) corroborated her earlier finding
that FoMO is positively associated with social media engage-
ment, thus underscoring the importance of FoMO as an
explanatory variable in studies of social media use.
In studying smartphone use and FoMO among a sample of
296 college students, Wolniewicz, Tiamiyu, Weeks, and Elhai
(2018) found that FoMO was positively associated with pro-
blematic smartphone and social smartphone use. The authors
used a 4-item measure of social uses of ones smartphone such
as making voice calls or videos, texting, e-mailing or using
social media sites. Consistent with Uses and Gratifications
Theory, Wolniewicz et al. (2018) results explain how an
individual high in FoMO is more likely to use his/her smart-
phone for social purposes including social media to stay
connected with his/her social network. Similar research by
Makki, DeCook, Kadylak, and Lee (2018) focused on
Snapchat and found that use of the social media platform
was associated with individualsinnate desire to be accepted,
affiliated, and connected with others. Indeed, the
Belongingness Hypothesis explains that, “…human beings
have a pervasive drive to form and maintain at least
a minimum quantity of lasting, positive, and significant inter-
personal relationships(Baumeister & Leary, 1995, p. 497).
The need to belong, argue Baumeister and Leary, is an innate
human motivation that drives much of our interpersonal
behavior. Thus, it is likely that FoMO drives social media
use. Further support for this prediction is provided by
Information Foraging Theory.
Information Foraging Theory explains that, from an evolu-
tionary perspective, humans have an innate drive to seek
information (Gazzaley & Rosen, 2016). Like animals foraging
for food (Muntinga & Taylor, 2018), humans are constantly
seeking information particularly regarding their relation-
ships with others. Social media addresses this important
need by providing access to information about others that
Figure 1. Conceptual model.
2J. A. ROBERTS AND M. E. DAVID
feeds an instinctive need for information regarding our rela-
tionships. Hours spent on social media can be explained,
especially for teens and young adults, by an intense desire to
gather information that signals ones place in the social hier-
archy (Roberts et al., 2014; Whiting & Williams, 2013).
Trepass (2018) argues that information foraging can be con-
sidered rational and goal-driven activity at all levels of gran-
ularity.Indeed, the emergence of social media has provided
access to a wide range of activities and personal information
on others which has heretofore been largely unavailable,
Based on this review, we posit that FoMO is positively
associated with social media intensity. However, and as dis-
cussed next, a closer look at the Belongingness Hypothesis
may suggests that, although FoMO likely encourages informa-
tion foraging and drives social media use, it is unlikely that
FoMO is positively associated with well-being related out-
comes. Indeed, the research findings of Baker, Kreiger, and
Leroy (2016) corroborate the positive impact of FoMO on
social media use and provide evidence of a negative impact of
FoMO overall. Using a sample of 368 U.S. college students,
the researchers found that heightened FoMO was positively
associated with time spent on social media. Higher levels of
FoMO were also associated with a higher incidence of depres-
sive symptoms and more negative physical symptoms such as
headaches, shortness of breath, chest pain, and sore throats.
2.2. FoMO, social media intensity, social connection, and
well-being
The Belongingness Hypothesis explains that satisfying the
innate need to be in relationships with others requires two
criteria must be met (Baumeister & Leary, 1995):
(1) People need to have frequent and reasonably pleasant
interactions with several significant others.
(2) The interactions between relationship partners must
occur in a stable and persistent framework of concern
for each other.
The above criteria have interesting implications for the grow-
ing use of social media to establish and maintain social rela-
tionships. First, as noted by Baumeister and Leary (1990),
interactions with a constantly changing flux of relationship
partners (think 500 Facebook friends) will be less fulfilling
than interactions with a few, close relationship
partners. Second, irregular or inconsistent contact/interaction
with close confidants is also less satisfying. Thus, it seems
likely that FoMO is negatively associated with social connec-
tion and overall well-being.
Oberst, Wegmann, Stodt, Brand, & Chamarro (2017) argue
that the positive rewards associated with social media use may
foster compulsive checking behaviors and excessive use of
social media and may ultimately hinder psychological well-
being. FoMO, the authors argue, although not the exclusive
purview of social media users, might lead individuals to check
their social media feeds more frequently to stay in constant
contact with the activities of important others. Clayton,
Leshner, and Almond (2015) conducted an experiment in
which 40 iPhone users were randomly assigned to complete
word puzzles either with or without their phones present in
their possession. During part of the task, researchers called the
study participantsphones, as a means by which to assess the
impact of phone separation on well-being. The results
revealed how phone separation has harmful effects on psy-
chological outcomes (e.g., state anxiety), as well as on physio-
logical outcomes including increased heart rate and blood
pressure (Clayton et al., 2015).
Przybylski et al. (2013) investigated the emotional and
behavioral correlates of FoMO in a sample of young adults
and concluded that, although FoMO is positively associated
with higher use of Facebook, it is also associated with poorer
mood states and lower levels of life satisfaction. Similarly,
research with a nationally representative sample of 2,079
British adults assessed the correlation between FoMO and
the psychological needs variables of autonomy, competence,
and relatedness, as well as well-being and found that all three
of the psychological needs were inversely associated with
FoMO (Przybylski et al., 2013). As these needs went unmet,
FoMO correspondingly increased. These findings seem to
suggest that FoMO may well be associated with lower social
connection and overall well-being.
Based on the above, it can be posited that FoMO is nega-
tively associated with feelings of social connection and psy-
chological well-being. Importantly, however, additional
research and empirical findings seem to suggest that FoMO
could in some situations foster social connection, in which
case it may not be harmful to well-being. Specifically, and as
discussed next, social media intensity likely plays a key med-
iating role between FoMO and both social connection and
well-being.
A small body of research has found that social media use
can help build bridging, bonding, and maintained social capi-
tal (Burke & Kraut, 2014; Ellison et al., 2007; Riedl, Köbler,
Goswami, & Krcmar, 2013). Steinfield, Ellison, and Lampe
(2008) found that intensity of Facebook use among college
students was a robust predictor of bridging social capital
a year later. Students with lower self-esteem benefited more
from their Facebook use than those with higher levels of self-
esteem. A recent meta-analysis by Domahidi (2018) analyzed
342 effect sizes from 63 studies which included data from over
35,000 individuals; the results showed a small positive rela-
tionship (r = .15) between online media use and perceived
social resources.
Using an experimental design, Deters and Mehl (2013)
tested the effect of posting status updates on Facebook on
psychological well-being, and found that posting status
updates to Facebook reduced loneliness. The authors posited
that the decrease in reported loneliness was due to a sense of
better connection with ones friends. Interestingly, the impact
of posting status updates was independent of social feedback
from members of the subjects friend networks.
Similarly, a study by Seo, Kim, and Yang (2016) used
a combination of information gathered from the participants
Facebook page and self-reported data to examine whether
social interactions on Facebook lead to more positive well-
being. The results showed that more interactions with
Facebook friends lead to greater perceptions of social support,
which in turn reduced ones loneliness (Seo et al., 2016).
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMANCOMPUTER INTERACTION 3
These findings are consistent with research which has shown
that the simple act of sharing personal information lights up
reward areas in the brain; specifically, important neurochem-
ical releases occur when we share personal information
(which is greatly enhanced by the variety of social media
available) (Richtel, 2014). Based on this review, it is hypothe-
sized that the intensity of social media use derived from ones
FoMO may well foster social connection and ultimately
increase ones well-being.
Overall then, it is hypothesized that FoMO is positively
associated with social media intensity, but negatively asso-
ciated with feelings of social connection and psychological
well-being. Importantly, we expect to find that FoMO can
foster social connection, in which case it may not be harmful
to well-being. Specifically, our conceptual model (see Figure
1) includes mediation and predicts that social media intensity
derived from a fear of missing out fosters social connection
and ultimately enhances well-being. Two studies were con-
ducted to test these predictions.
3. Method
Two studies were conducted to test our conceptual model.
Study 1 examined the relationships between FoMO, social
media use, and social connection. Given a small, but evolving
body of literature which has found that social media use may
help build social capital, Study 2 tested whether increasing
social media use could have a positive impact on psychologi-
cal well-being through its salubrious impact on social connec-
tion. It is predicted that FoMO is negatively associated with
social connection and well-being unless it transpires through
effective use of social media.
3.1. Study 1
3.1.1. Sample, procedure, and measures
One hundred and seven undergraduate students (47% female)
at a large U.S. university participated in the study. Participants
were invited into a lab and seated at individual computers
where they would complete the study questionnaire online.
The study included measures of FoMO, social media intensity,
and social connection. FoMO (α= .88, M = 2.49, SD = .83)
was accessed using the 10-item measure by Przybylski et al.
(2013). Example items include I fear my friends have more
rewarding experiences than me,”“I get anxious when I dont
know what my friends are up to,, and It bothers me when
I miss an opportunity to meet up with friends.Social media
intensity (α= .86, M = 2.90, SD = .89) was measured using the
6-item Ellison et al. (2007) scale. The items were assessed on
a 5-point scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly
agree. Example items include Social media is part of my
everyday activity,”“I feel out of touch when I havent logged
onto Facebook for a while,and Social media sites have
become part of my daily routine.Social connection
(α= .90, M = 5.16, SD = 1.24) was assessed using a 9-item
measure by Lee and Robbins (1995). A 7-point Likert scale
was used to assess the items. Example items included I feel
distant from people,”“I catch myself losing all sense of
connectedness with society,and I have no sense of together-
ness with my peers.
3.1.2. Results
The Process Model 4 (Preacher & Hayes, 2008)was used to
test our conceptual model including predictions involving
mediation (Krieger & Sarge, 2013). To begin, the model tested
the relationship between FoMO and social media intensity
(F
(1, 105)
= 21.43, p< .01, R
2
= .17). As predicted, the results
indicated that FoMO is positively associated with social media
intensity (β= .44, p< .01). Next, the model tested whether
FoMO and social media intensity are directly associated with
social connection. The results (F
(2, 104)
= 7.22, p< .01,
R
2
= .12) indicated that FoMO is negatively associated with
social connection (β=.55, p< .01), and social media inten-
sity is positively associated with social connection (β= .35,
p< .05). Importantly, and in support of mediation, the indir-
ect effect of FoMO on social connection (through social
media intensity) is significant and positive (β= .16,
SE = .08, 95% CI: .01, .33).
Overall, these findings support our predictions.
Interestingly, although FoMO is negatively associated with
social connection, the mediation tests revealed more positive
results surrounding FoMO. Specifically, FoMO had an indir-
ect effect on social connection through social media intensity,
thus suggesting that FoMO may in some instances be a good
thing which can lead to greater feelings of social connection.
Study 2 sought to extend study 1 by examining the same
relationships but also including a measure of well-being and
testing it as an ultimate outcome of FoMO (through social
media intensity and social connection).
3.2. Study 2
3.2.1. Sample, procedure, and measures
The sample consisted of 458 undergraduate students from
alarge U.S. university (55% male, M
age
= 20.35, SD = .940).
The study 2 questionnaire included measures of FoMO
(α= .86, M = 2.50, SD = .79) (Przybylski et al., 2013), social
media intensity (α= .87, M = 2.86, SD = .99) (Ellison et al.,
2007), and social connection (α= .93, M = 5.15, SD = 1.22)
(Lee & Robbins, 1995), all of which were assessed using the
same scales as previously in study 1. In addition, study 2 also
included a measure of subjective well-being. Specifically, sub-
jective well-being (α= .89, M = 3.01, SD = .59) was assessed
using a 5-item measure by Diener, Emmons, Larsen, and
Griffiths (1985).
3.2.2. Results
The Process Model 6 (Preacher & Hayes, 2008)was used to
test our conceptual model including predictions involving
sequential mediation (Krieger & Sarge, 2013). Unlike the
Process Model 4, the Process Model 6 offers a test of sequen-
tial, or serial, mediation; since study 2 measured subjective
well-being and was designed to test our complete conceptual
model in which subjective well-being is an ultimate outcome
of FoMO (through its impact on social media intensity and
social connection), the Process Model 6 was the most appro-
priate to method to analyze the study data. To begin, the
4J. A. ROBERTS AND M. E. DAVID
model tested the relationship between FoMO and social
media intensity. As predicted, and consistent with the findings
in Study 1, the results (F
(1, 459)
= 93.65, p< .01, R
2
= .17)
indicated that FoMO is positively associated with social media
intensity (β= .52, p< .01).
Next, the model tested whether FoMO and social media
intensity are directly associated with social connection. The
results (F
(2, 458)
= 34.63, p< .01, R
2
= .13) indicated that
FoMO is negatively associated with social connection
(β=.61, p< .01), and social media intensity is positively
associated with social connection (β= .14, p< .05). The model
next tested the relationship that FoMO, social media intensity,
and social connection have with subjective well-being. The
results (F
(3, 457)
= 58.36, p< .01, R
2
= .28) showed a significant
relationship between social connection and subjective well-
being (β= .17, p< .01). In addition, FoMO is a significant
predictor of subjective well-being (β=.19, p< .01). Social
media intensity had a marginally significant effect on well-
being (β=.05, p= .05). Further, the mediation results
suggested that although FoMO has a negative indirect effect
on well-being through social media intensity (β=.026;
SE = .01, 95% CI: .056, .001) and through social connection
(β=.101; SE = .02, 95% CI: .141, .069) separately, the
results provided evidence suggesting that FoMO can have
a positive effect on well-being when acted upon by engaging
in social media so as to enhance social connection (β= .012;
SE = .01, 95% CI: .001, .026).
Importantly, the results showed support for sequential
mediation, suggesting that FoMO has an indirect positive
effect on well-being through social media intensity and social
connection (β= .012; SE = .01, 95% CI: .001, .026). Table 1
summarizes the results of studies 1 and 2. Overall, these
results suggest that FoMO can have a positive impact on well-
being if acted upon by engaging in social media in a manner
that fosters social connection. These findings seem to high-
light the potential importance of the way in which social
media is used.
4. General discussion, and conclusions
Despite the emergence of social media that allows one to be
continually connected to others, research suggests that both the
quantity and quality of social relationships in industrialized
societies has decreased. Individuals are perhaps more socially
isolated now than ever before. Most Americans report having
no confidant (Holt-Lunstad et al., 2010). The primary focus of
the present research was to investigate the Howof the rela-
tionship between FoMO and well-being. We found support for
the process (How) by which FoMO is linked to well-being.
As hypothesized, the present study found that FoMO is
positively associated with social media intensity. Although the
correlational nature of these studies precludes one from any
causal attributions, it is likely that FoMO encourages the use
of social media to stay connected. As informed by the
Belongingness Hypothesis and Information Foraging Theory,
humans have an innate drive to be in relationships with
others. Social media is simply an omni-present conduit for
attempting to make such connections. The dual nature of
social media, however, suggests that, faced with a nearly
unending number of opportunities to connect, such social
media use may increase ones FoMO. A bi-directional causal
flow between the two constructs is a distinct possibility which
merits further research scrutiny (Buglass, Binder, Bette, &
Underwood, 2017).
Study results suggest that, although FoMO is generally nega-
tively associated with social connection and well-being, this is
not always the case. Importantly, the results show a more
nuanced model of FoMO and its relationships with social
media use, connection, and well-being. FoMO drives social
media intensity and has an indirect positive effect on social
connection through social media intensity, thus suggesting
that FoMO may in some instances be a good thing which can
lead to greater feelings of social connection. Although FoMO
has a negative indirect effect on well-being through social media
intensity and through social connection separately, the results
provide evidence suggesting that FoMO can have a positive
effect on well-being if acted upon by engaging in social media
in a manner that fosters social connection.
4.1. Limitations and future research directions
The present research and the results provided herein must be
tempered by certain limitations. First, although a strong case
has been made theoretically and empirically that FoMO is
associated with social media intensity, experimental research
is needed to examine the direction of the causal flow between
FoMO and social media use.
Table 1. Study 1 and study 2 results.
Path
a
Coefficient
b
SE t
95%
Confidence
Interval
Study 1: FoMO Social Media
Intensity
Study 2: FoMO Social Media
Intensity
.44***
.52***
.095
.054
4.63
9.68
.25, .63
.41, .63
Study 1: FoMO Social
Connection
Study 2: FoMO Social
Connection
.55***
-.61***
.151
.074
3.64
8.24
.85, .25
-.75, .46
Study 1: Social Media Intensity
Connection
Study 2: Social Media Intensity
Connection
.35**
.14**
.141
.059
2.49
2.34
.07, .63
.02, .25
Study 2: FoMO Subjective Well-
being
.19** .035 5.57 .25, .13
Study 2: Social Media Intensity
Well-being
.05* .026 1.92 .10, .00
Study 2: Social Connection
Well-being
.17*** .021 8.07 .13, .21
Study 1 Mediation:
FoMO Social Media Intensity
Connection
Study 2 Mediation:
FoMO Social Media Intensity
Well-being
FoMO Connection Well-
being
FoMO Social Media Intensity
Connection
Well-being
.16**
.03**
.10**
.01**
.081
.014
.019
.006
.01, .33
.06, .00
.14, .07
.00, .03
a
Study 1 results (F
(2,104)
= 7.22, p< .01 R
2
= .12) based on the Preacher and Hayes
(2008) Model 4; Study 2 results (F
(3,457)
= 58.36, p< .01 R
2
= .28) based on the
Preacher and Hayes (2008) Model 6
b
* denotes p< .10, ** denotes p< .05, *** denotes p< .001
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMANCOMPUTER INTERACTION 5
It is likely that the causal flow between the two variables
may be bi-directional (Buglass et al., 2017). FoMO is likely an
innate drive to be in relationship with others that positively
impacts ones social media use. The greater the FoMO, the
more one turns to social media to regain a sense of belonging.
It is equally likely, however that being exposed to a myriad of
opportunities on social media will, in turn, increase ones
FoMO. This tendency to be overwhelmed by social opportu-
nities on social media has been referred to as the dual nature
of social media (Przybylski et al., 2013).
Additionally, as noted by Przybylski et al. (2013), the pre-
sent study treated FoMO as an individual difference variable.
It is possible that both time and context could impact ones
sense of FoMO. FoMO, assert the authors, may vary across
the span of months, weeks, or even within the course of
a single day. Longitudinal and experimental research will
help address such concerns.
Lastly, it may be an over-simplification to treat social media
use as a catch-all for time spent on social media. A small body of
emerging research suggests it is not the time spent on social
media that dictates its relationship with well-being but how that
time is spent. Passive use of social media (also referred to as
lurkingor creeping) has been found in several studies to be
negatively associated with well-being while active use of social
media (posting, commenting, and interacting) has been found
to be positively associated with several psychological well-being
measures (Deters & Mehl, 2013;Shawetal.,2015;Verduyn
et al., 2015). Additional research is needed to examine the
impact of how one uses social media and its relationship to
both physical and psychological well-being.
References
Abeele, M. V., & Rooij, T. V. (2016). Fear of missing out (FoMO) as
a predictor of problematic social media use. International Conference
on Behavioral Addictions, Geneva, Switzerland.
Adams, S. K., & Kisler, T. S. (2013). Sleep quality as a mediator between
technology-related sleep quality, depression, and anxiety.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking,16(1), 2531.
doi:10.1089/cyber.2012.0157
Alt, D. (2015). College studentsacademic motivation, media engagement
and fear of missing out. Computers in Human Behavior,49,11119.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.057
Alt, D. (2017). Studentssocial media engagement and fear of missing out
(FoMO) in a diverse classroom. Journal of Computers in Higher
Education,29, 388410. doi:10.1007/s12528-017-9149-x
Baker, Z. G., Krieger, H., & LeRoy, A. S. (2016). Fesar of missing out:
Relationships with depression, mindfulness, and physical symptoms.
Translational Issues in Psychological Science,2(3), 275282.
doi:10.1037/tps0000075
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for
interpersonal attachments as a fundamental drive. Psychological
Bulletin,117(3), 497529.
Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. (1990). Anxiety and social exclusion.
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,9(2), 165195. doi:10.1521/
jscp.1990.9.2.165
Best, P., Manktelow, R., & Taylor, B. (2014). Online communications, social
media and adolescent wellbeing: A systematic narrative review. Children
and Youth Review,41,2736. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2014.03.001
Beyens, I., Frison, E., & Eggermont, S. (2016). I dont want to miss
a thing: Adolescentsfear of missing out and its relationship to
adolescentssocial needs, facebook use, and facebook related stress.
Computers in Human Behavior,64,18. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.083
Buglass, S. L., Binder, J. F., Bette, L. R., & Underwood, D. M. (2017).
Motivators of online vulnerability: The impact of social network site
use and FoMO. Computers in Human Behavior,66, 248255.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2016.09.055
Burke, M., & Kraut, R. E. (2014). Growing closer on Facebook: Changes
in tie strength through social network site use. In Proceedings of the
32nd annual ACM conference on human factors in computing systems
(pp. 41874196). Toronto, ON: ACM.
Clayton, R. B., Leshner, G., & Almond, A. (2015). The extended iSelf:
The impact of iPhone separation on cognition, emotion, and
physiology. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication,20,
119135. doi:10.1111/jcc4.12109
David, M. E., Roberts, J. A., & Christenson, B. (2017). Too much of
a good thing: Investigating the association between actual smartphone
use and individual well-being. International Journal of Human-
computer Interaction. doi:10.1080/10447318.2017.1349250
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The whatand whyof goal pursuits:
Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological
Inquiry,11, 227268. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01
Deters, F. G., & Mehl, M. R. (2013). Does posting Facebook status
updates increase or decrease loneliness? An online social networking
experiment. Social Psychology and Personality Science,4(5), 579586.
doi:10.1177/1948550612469233
Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffiths, S. (1985). The
satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment,49(1),
7175. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13
Domahidi, E. (2018). The associations between online media use and
usersperceived social resources: A meta-analysis. Journal of Computer-
Mediated Communication,23, 181200. doi:10.1093/jcmc/zmy007
Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of
Facebook Friends: Social capital and college studentsuse of online
social network sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication,
12(4), 11431168. doi:10.1111/jcmc.2007.12.issue-4
Gazzaley, A., & Rosen, L. D. (2016). The distracted mind: Ancient brains
in a high-tech world. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Green-Hamann, S., Eichhorn, K. C., & Sherblom, J. C. (2011). An
exploration of why people participate in second life social support
groups. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication,16, 465491.
doi:10.1111/jcmc.2011.16.issue-4
Gruter, M., & Masters, R. D. (1986). Ostracism as a social and biological
phenomenon: An introduction. Ethology & Sociobiology,7(34),
149158. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(86)90043-9
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships
and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review. PLoS Medicine,7(7),
e1000316. doi:10.137/journal.pmed.1000316
Konrath, S. (2018, May 7). Americans are becoming more socially iso-
lated, but theyre not feeling lonelier. The Conversation.
Krieger, J. L., & Sarge, M. A. (2013). A serial mediation model of message
framing on intentions to receive the human papillomavirus (HPV)
vaccine: Revisiting the role of threat and efficacy perceptions. Health
Communication,28(1), 519. doi:10.1080/10410236.2012.734914
Kross, E., Verduyn, P., Park, J., Seungjae, D., Lin, N., Shablack, H.,
Ybarra, O. (2013). Facebook use predicts declines in subjective
well-being in young adults. PLoS One,8(8), e69841. doi:10.1371/jour-
nal.pone.0069841
Lee, R. M., & Robbins, S. B. (1995). Measuring belongingness: The social
connectedness and the social assurance scales. Journal of Counseling
Psychology,42, 232241. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.42.2.232
Makki, T. W., DeCook, J. R., Kadylak, T., & Lee, O. J. (2018). The social
value of snapchat: An exploration of affiliation motivation, the tech-
nology acceptance model, and relational maintenance in Snapchat use.
International Journal of HumanComputer Interaction,34(5),
410420. doi:10.1080/10447318.2017.1357903
Muntinga, T., & Taylor, G. (2018). Information-seeking strategies in med-
icine queries: A clinical eye tracking study with gaze-cued retrospective
think-aloud protocol. International Journal of Human-computer
Interaction,34(6), 506518. doi:10.1080/10447318.2017.1368949
Nadkarni, A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2012). Why do people use Facebook?
Personality and Individual Differences,52, 243249. doi:10.1016/j.
paid.2011.11.007
6J. A. ROBERTS AND M. E. DAVID
Oberst, U., Wegmann, E., Stodt, B., Brand, M., & Chamarro, A. (2017).
Negative consequences from heavy social networking in adolescents:
The mediating role of fear of missing out. Journal of Adolescents,55,
5160. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.12.008
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies
for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator
models. Behavior Research Methods,40, 879891.
Przybylski, A. K., Murayama, K., DeHaan, C. R., & Gladwell, V. (2013).
Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing
out. Computers in Human Behavior,29, 18411848. doi:10.1016/j.
chb.2013.02.014
Richtel, M. (2014). A deadly wandering: A tale of tragedy and redemption
in the age of attention. New York, NY: William Morrow.
Riedl, C., Köbler, F., Goswami, S., & Krcmar, H. (2013). Tweeting to feel
connected: A model for social connectedness in online social
networks. International Journal of Human-computer Interaction,29
(10), 670687. doi:10.1080/10447318.2013.768137
Roberts, J. A., Petnji YaYa, L. H., & Manolis, C. (2014). The invisible
addiction: Cell-phone activities and addiction among male and female
college students. Journal of Behavioral Addictions,3(4), 254265.
doi:10.1556/JBA.3.2014.015
Roberts, J. A., & Pirog, S. F., III. (2013). A preliminary investigation of
materialism and impulsiveness as predictors of technological addictions
among young adults. Journal of Behavioral Addictions,2(1), 5662.
Seo, M., Kim, J., & Yang, H. (2016). Frequent interaction and fast feedback
predict perceived social support: Using Crawled and self-reported data
of Facebook users. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication,21,
282297. doi:10.1111/jcc4.12160
Shaw, A. M., Timpano, K. R., Tran, T., & Joorman, J. (2015). Correlates
of Facebook usage patterns: The relationship between passive
Facebook use, social anxiety symptoms, and brooding. Computers in
Human Behavior,48, 575580. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.003
Sheldon, K. M., Abad, N., & Hinsch, C. (2011). A two-process view of
Facebook use and relatedness need-satisfaction: Disconnection drives
use, and connection rewards it. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology,100(94), 766775. doi:10.1037/a0022407
Steinfield, C., Ellison, N. B., & Lampe, C. (2008). Social capital, self-este
em, and use of online social network sites: A longitudinal analysis.
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,29(6), 434445.
doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2008.07.002
Trepass, D. 2018. Information foraging theory, the glossary of human
computer interaction. Retrived from www.interaction-design.org/lit
erature/book/the-glossary-of-human-computer-intera
Tromholt, M. (2016). The Facebook experiment: Quitting Facebook leads
to higher levels of well-being. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social
Networking,19(11), 661666. doi:10.1089/cyber.2016.0259
Twenge, J. M., Joiner, T. E., Rogers, M. L., & Martin, G. N. (2018).
Increases in depressive symptoms, suicide-related outcomes, and sui-
cide rates among U.S. adolescents after 2010 and links to increased
new media screen time. Clinical Psychological Science,6,317.
doi:10.1177/2167702617723376
Verduyn, P., Lee, D. S., Park, J., Shablack, H., Orvell, A., Bayer, J.,
Kross, E. (2015). Passive Facebook usage undermines affective
well-being: Experimental and longitudinal evidence. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General,144(2), 480488. doi:10.1037/
xge0000057
Whiting, A., & Williams, D. (2013). Why people use social media:
A uses and gratifications approach. Qualitative Market Research:
An International Journal,16(4), 362369. doi:10.1108/QMR-06-
2013-0041
Williams, K. D. (2007). Social ostracism: The kiss of death. Social and
Personality Psychology Compass,1/1(2007), 236247. doi:10.1111/
j.1751-9004.2007.00004.x
Wolniewicz, C. A., Tiamiyu, M. F., Weeks, J., & Elhai, J. D. (2018).
Problematic smartphone use and relations with negative affect, fear
of missing out, and fear of negative and positive evaluation. Psychiatry
Research,262, 618623. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.058
Woods, H. C., & Scott, H. (2016). #Sleepyteens: Social media use in
adolescence is associated with poor sleep quality, anxiety, depression
and low self- esteem. Journal of Adolescence,51,4149. doi:10.1016/j.
adolescence.2016.05.008
About the Authors
James A. Roberts is the Ben H. Williams Professor of Marketing at
Baylor University. He has approximately 80 articles published in numer-
ous journals including Computers in Human Behavior, the Journal of
Applied Psychology, Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Journal
of Consumer Psychology, Psychology & Marketing, and many others.
Meredith E. David (PhD, University of South Carolina) is an Assistant
Professor of Marketing in the Hankamer School of Business at Baylor
University. Her research focuses on consumer behavior and appears in
journals including the Journal of Business Research, Journal of
Advertising, European Journal of Marketing, and Psychology & Marketing.
INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF HUMANCOMPUTER INTERACTION 7
... First, as a developmental stage adolescence is typically a period in which young people are strongly engaged in identity formation and very conscious about their appearance and social roles. During this period, (emotional) changes abound and the impact of the peer group is large (Baumeister & Leary, 1995;Roberts & David, 2020). Adolescents and young adults are more sensitive to social information than adults (Lamblin et al., 2017;Riordan et al., 2018), and social connection with friends and acceptance from peers is extremely important during this phase of life (Brown & Larson, 2009;Rubin et al., 2011). ...
... Some research supports the idea that the negative influence of social media on mental health stems from the fear of missing out on experiences (Baker et al., 2016;Beyens et al., 2016), and FoMO has been shown to be related to the well-being of youths (Baker et al., 2016;Riordan et al., 2018). Moreover, several studies have found a strong relationship between FoMO and the intensity of social media use (e.g., Reer et al., 2019;Riordan et al., 2018;Roberts & David, 2020) and revealed FoMO to be influencing mediating factor of the relation between indicators of well-being and engagement with social media (Przybylski et al., 2013;Reer et al., 2019). A number of studies have demonstrated that the effect of frequency or intensity of social media use can be fully explained by FoMO (e.g., Baker et al., 2016), while others have observed that the frequency of use and FoMO demonstrate independent impacts on mental health and well-being. ...
... For example, Oberst and colleagues (2017) found that both FoMO and the intensity of social media use can trigger the relationship between symptoms of anxiety and depression and social media use, but by means of different mechanisms. Additional research on students showed that FoMO has a positive indirect effect on social connectedness through frequency of social media use, suggesting that subjective well-being may actually benefit from FoMO if it leads to social media use that promotes social connectedness (Roberts & David, 2020). ...
Article
Despite the sharp rise of studies on social media, there is still little consensus concerning the impact of social media use on adolescents’ well-being. We argue that this is due to the narrow focus on quantitative aspects of social media (e.g. frequency of use) and the use of very specific indicators of well-being. This research contributes to the literature by (1) looking at both quantitative indicators of social media use (frequency of use) and qualitative indicators (compulsive use and motives of use) and (2) covering diverse aspects of adolescents’ well-being (overall life satisfaction -SWLS, emotional well-being (SPANE-N and SPANE-P), satisfaction with mental health and with social contacts). Multiple regression analyses are carried out on cross-sectional data gathered in 2018 based on a representative sample among Flemish adolescents aged 14 to 25 (n = 1406; 52.6% girls; Mage = 18.96). Results show that more intensive use of social media coincides with more negative emotions, less overall life satisfaction, and less satisfaction with mental health. Especially for boys, the use of social media was associated with more negative emotions. In addition, we observed that compulsive social media use leads to more negative and less positive emotions and satisfaction with mental health. However, using social media for interpersonal contact was positively related to higher well-being (all indicators). The findings underscore the importance of a nuanced debate on the potential impact of social media use on adolescents’ well-being in which both its positive and negative consequences are considered.
... However, for those individuals in which craving might be a barrier to maintaining a significant detox period (and we report that SMAQ scores would appear to be a good predictor of such individual differences), researchers should look to develop ways to reduce these urges (see Houghton et al., 2021, for a recent review on the benefits of intranasal oxytocin in this respect). While we focused on examining whether craving might act as a primary driver to end periods of detox, our qualitative and free-text responses may have indicated a more significant factor in relapse to use-feelings of disconnection (see Roberts & David, 2020;Tandon et al., 2021). This is related to the extent to which users have come to rely on social media for essential information and social connectedness. ...
... The U&G framework can provide a possible reason why the exploration was associated with familiar sources. Social media users are likely to explore content from familiar sources, as much as they can, to constantly monitor their social networks for developments (Roberts & David, 2020). The particular design of social media platforms guides users to explore (rather than exploit) ephemeral content displayed on single feeds, where social connections' and publishers' content are mixed. ...
... Indeed, social media offers a variety of ways to connect with those close to you, such as family, friends, and relatives, as well as with strangers, coworkers, and acquaintances [16]. It also plays a significant role for people of all ages as they make the most of their sense of community [41]. Social media as a way for individuals to communicate with one another through the creation, sharing, and/or exchange of information and ideas through online groups and networks. ...
Article
Full-text available
This research study employed a linear regression analysis to examine the influence of social media usage and the level of motivation on students' academic performance. The research questions explored the levels of social media usage in terms of academic, socialization, entertainment, informativeness, and constraint; level of motivation in terms of intrinsic motivation, grade motivation, career motivation, and self-determination; and academic performance in quarters 1 to 3 among Junior High School and Senior High School students. In the sampling procedures the researchers utilized Slovin's formula. Based on Slovin's formula, a sample size of approximately 320.69 or 321 is required to achieve a margin of error of 5% for a total population size of 1614. The researchers utilized the Stratified Random Sampling which defines as a probability technique that involves segmenting the population into distinct subpopulations and then selecting samples from each subpopulation. The findings revealed that students exhibited a moderate level of social media usage (x=3.36), while demonstrating a high level of motivation (x=3.62). However, despite these moderate to high levels, students' academic performance remained at an average level (x=3.15). Furthermore, the research indicated that there was no significant relationship between social media usage and academic performance, as well as between the level of motivation and academic performance. Consequently, the study concluded that neither social media usage nor motivation significantly influenced students' academic performance. Lastly, further research is warranted to explore additional factors and potential interventions that could enhance academic outcomes in this population.
Article
Full-text available
The occurrence of covid-19 has a major effect on the smooth running of education, one of which is in higher education. So that lecturers and students need supporting media in the smooth learning process, such as the use of software. The use of software can make it easier for students to carry out the online learning process. This study aims to reveal the effect of using software on students in carrying out the distance learning process. In conducting this research, researchers used quantitative research methods with observations made in the form of distributing questionnaires to students to see the effect of software as a supporting medium in the learning process. The results of this study are that learning media using software is very helpful and has a good impact on students in the learning process during the covid-19 pandemic. The conclusion of this study is that fluency in the world of education during the co-19 pandemic is very limited, because students cannot integrate directly with lecturers in the learning process, therefore students and lecturers need media that can be used as supporting tools such as software for the smooth discussion and learning process, teaching in higher education. The limitation in this study is that researchers did not get accurate data in data collection. Therefore, it is hoped that future researchers will also conduct the same research in order to maintain relevant data results in analyzing the use of software in the learning process in higher education. This study also recommends that future researchers make this topic an ongoing discussion to obtain relevant and very effective research results so that this research can become a benchmark in the influence of using software as a learning medium during the covid-19 pandemic, so as to create a learning process based on supporting science and technology.
Article
Full-text available
Background Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) sufferers show problematic patterns of Internet use such as fear of missing out (FOMO) and sharing misinformation and fake news. This study aimed to investigate these associations in survivors of the 2008 earthquake in Wenchuan, China. Methods A self-reported survey was completed by 356 survivors of the Wenchuan earthquake. A mediated structural equation model was constructed to test a proposed pattern of associations with FOMO as a mediator of the relationship between PTSD symptoms and belief in fake news, as well as moderators of this pathway. Results PTSD was directly associated with believing fake news (β = 0.444, p < .001) and with FOMO (β = 0.347, p < .001). FOMO mediated the association between PTSD and fake news belief (β = 0.373, p < .001). Age moderated the direct (β = 0.148, t = 3.097, p = .002) and indirect (β = 0.145, t = 3.122, p = .002) pathways, with effects more pronounced with increasing age. Gender was also a moderator, with the indirect effect present in females but not in males (β = 0.281, t = 6.737, p < .001). Conclusion Those with higher PTSD symptoms are more likely to believe fake news and this is partly explained by FOMO. This effect is present in females and not males and is stronger in older people. Findings extend knowledge of the role of psychological variables in problematic Internet use among those with PTSD.
Article
Purpose Many believe that any social media harms kids because of frequent use. This study aims to examine these assumptions. It proposes and tests a model that considers two alternative pathways – one negative and one positive – through which social media affects teens’ self-reported subjective well-being. Design/methodology/approach This research used Preacher and Hayes process modeling to conduct path analysis on data collected on 585 teenagers from across the USA. Findings Results showed that along a negative pathway, frequent social media use leads to greater risky social media engagement that ultimately diminishes adolescent’s sense of well-being. Also, and perhaps simultaneously, frequent social media use leads to socially-connected social media use that enhances adolescent’s sense of well-being. Practical implications The research recommends ways parents, policymakers and platforms can encourage teens to use social media to connect with friends while guiding them away from pathways exposing them to risky behaviors. Originality/value Findings show more social media use is not necessarily harmful, but more of some types is bad, while more of others is good.
Article
Full-text available
Research suggests that people around the world spend anywhere from an hour to as much as ten hours daily on their smartphones. However, the vast majority of this research has used self-reported data, which is widely considered unreliable. The present research is among the first to capture actual smartphone use and to examine its relationship with individual and relational well-being. Results reveal that, although smartphone use is generally negatively associated with well-being, this is not always so. Deeper analyses show that certain categories of apps are positively associated with well-being, thus revealing a more nuanced relationship between smartphone use and individual well-being. Research which suggests a negative association between smartphone use and well-being may represent an oversimplified perspective of a complex relationship.
Article
Full-text available
With the growing attention paid to fear of missing out (FoMO) psychological phenomenon in explaining social media engagement (SME), this mixed-method research measured the relative impact of FoMO on students’ SME for personal reasons during lectures. The moderating effect of culture (minority vs. non-minority students) on the connection between FoMO and SME was also considered. Quantitative data were gathered from 279-undergraduate students. The structural equation modeling results showed a positive moderate connection between the FoMO and SME variables. The bootstrapping result showed a significant indirect effect between the minority group of students and SME through increased levels of FoMO. A sequential explanatory strategy was used to refine and interpret the quantitative results. Accordingly, qualitative data were gathered by using semi-structured interviews to assist in explaining the findings of the quantitative phase. The qualitative data suggested several explanations for students’ distractive behavior enabled by technology during class. The main recurrent theme was the frequently used instructional activities based on the teacher-centered pedagogical approach. This approach imposed greater challenges for minority students as they tend to grapple with a host of language barriers. These students reported using social media tools to seek help from friends during lectures and feared missing out a useful assistance. Another finding showed that mainly non-minority students who experienced FoMO admitted using social media during lessons regardless of the teaching method implemented.
Article
Full-text available
Social networking sites (SNS) are especially attractive for adolescents, but it has also been shown that these users can suffer from negative psychological consequences when using these sites excessively. We analyze the role of fear of missing out (FOMO) and intensity of SNS use for explaining the link between psychopathological symptoms and negative consequences of SNS use via mobile devices. In an online survey, 1468 Spanish-speaking Latin-American social media users between 16 and 18 years old completed the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS), the Social Networking Intensity scale (SNI), the FOMO scale (FOMOs), and a questionnaire on negative consequences of using SNS via mobile device (CERM). Using structural equation modeling, it was found that both FOMO and SNI mediate the link between psychopathology and CERM, but by different mechanisms. Additionally, for girls, feeling depressed seems to trigger higher SNS involvement. For boys, anxiety triggers higher SNS involvement.
Article
Conflicting findings have emerged from the large number of studies on the relationship of online media use (OMU) and users’ perceived social resources (PSR). In contrast to the numerous primary studies, a comprehensive meta-analysis on the relationship between the use of different online media and PSR has been lacking to date. The findings presented are based on 342 effect sizes from 63 studies and represent data from over 35,500 individuals. The results reveal a small and positive relationship between the two variables. Detailed analyses suggest that the use of different online media, as well as the measurement of OMU and PSR, might affect the relationships obtained. Implications and directions for theoretical development and empirical research are also discussed.
Article
In two nationally representative surveys of U.S. adolescents in grades 8 through 12 (N = 506,820) and national statistics on suicide deaths for those ages 13 to 18, adolescents’ depressive symptoms, suicide-related outcomes, and suicide rates increased between 2010 and 2015, especially among females. Adolescents who spent more time on new media (including social media and electronic devices such as smartphones) were more likely to report mental health issues, and adolescents who spent more time on nonscreen activities (in-person social interaction, sports/exercise, homework, print media, and attending religious services) were less likely. Since 2010, iGen adolescents have spent more time on new media screen activities and less time on nonscreen activities, which may account for the increases in depression and suicide. In contrast, cyclical economic factors such as unemployment and the Dow Jones Index were not linked to depressive symptoms or suicide rates when matched by year.
Article
For many individuals, excessive smartphone use interferes with everyday life. In the present study, we recruited a non-clinical sample of 296 participants for a cross-sectional survey of problematic smartphone use, social and non-social smartphone use, and psychopathology-related constructs including negative affect, fear of negative and positive evaluation, and fear of missing out (FoMO). Results demonstrated that FoMO was most strongly related to both problematic smartphone use and social smartphone use relative to negative affect and fears of negative and positive evaluation, and these relations held when controlling for age and gender. Furthermore, FoMO (cross-sectionally) mediated relations between both fear of negative and positive evaluation with both problematic and social smartphone use. Theoretical implications are considered with regard to developing problematic smartphone use.
Article
Background: Medicines are increasingly purchased online, yet little is known regarding the ocular information-seeking behavior with medicine queries in search engines. A share of pharmacies found via search engines operate unlicensed and sell prescription-only medicines without a prescription. This study aimed to investigate how search engine users distinguish between genuine and falsified sources of information in terms of unlicensed and licensed online pharmacies in the case of medicine queries. Methods: Eye-tracking of search tasks (transactional, navigational, informational and two limited results) in a Google search engine environment with retrospective gaze-cued think aloud protocol. Purposive sample of N = 50 across three hospitals and one general practitioner. Results: Discovery of a trichotomy of ocular search strategies based on the inclusion or exclusion of URLs in the information-seeking process. Finding of dissonance to existing studies related to fixation duration of search engine result page (SERP) elements. Discovery of an addition to information foraging theory (IFT): proximal cues are, in environments with non-credible information, used in both positive and negative ways.
Article
The present study proposes that affiliation motivation plays a key role in driving Snapchat use. Constructs from the technology acceptance and relational maintenance literature were incorporated to examine the normative and interpersonal goals associated with Snapchat use. It was hypothesized that these two models and their associated constructs illustrate behavioral and cognitive phenomena that are tied to and fueled by the innate human desire to affiliate with others. The study was conducted using an online survey at a large, Midwestern university with a final student sample of 236. Perceived playfulness, subjective norm, trust, and critical mass (TAM constructs), as well as assurances and positivity (RM behaviors), were all found to be significantly and positively correlated with Snapchat use. This study highlights the interpersonal value of Snapchat and adds to the existing literature examining various information and communication technologies (ICTs) as tools for relational maintenance.
Article
Most people use Facebook on a daily basis; few are aware of the consequences. Based on a 1-week experiment with 1,095 participants in late 2015 in Denmark, this study provides causal evidence that Facebook use affects our well-being negatively. By comparing the treatment group (participants who took a break from Facebook) with the control group (participants who kept using Facebook), it was demonstrated that taking a break from Facebook has positive effects on the two dimensions of well-being: our life satisfaction increases and our emotions become more positive. Furthermore, it was demonstrated that these effects were significantly greater for heavy Facebook users, passive Facebook users, and users who tend to envy others on Facebook.