Available via license: CC BY 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
children
Brief Report
Immediate Effect of a Yoga Breathing Practice on
Attention and Anxiety in Pre-Teen Children
Shirley Telles *, Ram Kumar Gupta , Kumar Gandharva, Babita Vishwakarma, Niranjan Kala
and Acharya Balkrishna
Patanjali Research Foundation, PatanjaliYogpeeth, Haridwar 249405, Uttarakhand, India
*Correspondence: shirleytelles@gmail.com; Tel.: +91-1334-240008
Received: 31 May 2019; Accepted: 18 July 2019; Published: 22 July 2019
Abstract:
Pre-teen children face stressors related to their transition from childhood to adolescence,
with a simultaneous increase in academic pressure. The present study compared the immediate
effects of 18 min of (i) high frequency yoga breathing with (ii) yoga-based breath awareness and (iii)
sitting quietly, on (a) attention and (b) anxiety, in 61 pre-teen children (aged between 11 and 12 years;
25 girls). Attention was assessed using a six letter cancellation task and Spielberger’s State Trait
Anxiety Inventory STAI-S was used to measure anxiety before and after the three practices, practiced
on separate days. Repeated measures ANOVA, followed by Bonferroni adjusted post-hoc analyses
showed an increase in total attempts and net scores after high frequency yoga breathing (p<0.05),
while wrong attempts increased after yoga based breath awareness (p<0.05). Anxiety decreased
comparably after all three interventions. The 25 girls in the group had the same trend of results as the
whole group with respect to the attention-based cancellation task, while boys showed no, how since
change. For both girls and boys, anxiety decreased after all three 18min interventions. The results
suggest that high frequency yoga breathing could be a short, useful school based practice to improve
attention and reduce anxiety.
Keywords:
pre-teen children; attention; anxiety; yoga breathing; yoga based breath awareness;
sitting quietly
1. Introduction
Pre-teen children are at a stage of both promise and challenges, as they transition from childhood
to being teenagers. Some of the challenges include an increase in academic pressure, a greater
involvement in family and social dynamics, adjusting to changing social and behavioral demands of
their peer group, and sometimes a growing difference in values and opinion with their parents and
guardians [
1
]. Along with these stressors, in India pre-teens face a definite increase in academic load
as they begin to prepare to answer an important, competitive, and career-determining school leaving
examination in the next two to three years [
2
]. Academic pressure is a particularly important factor in
those pre-teens the world over, who see education as a way to improve their own and their family’s
social and economic prospects [3].
Researchers from medicine and public health disciplines have assessed the usefulness and benefits
of yoga for children in school settings [
4
]. The benefits included improved psychological well-being,
reduced rates of being overweight or obese, children being calmer and more attentive, more at ease,
and completing assigned tasks successfully. In a separate study, yoga breathing techniques were
introduced in a suburban, private, English medium school in north India [
5
]. Two sections of the
third grade (children aged 7–8 years), with approximately 50 students in each section were selected.
One section received yoga breathing (i.e., deep breathing and alternate nostril yoga breathing) as a
5min intervention every school day for the academic year. Scores obtained in Math and languages
Children 2019,6, 84; doi:10.3390/children6070084 www.mdpi.com/journal/children
Children 2019,6, 84 2 of 13
(English and Hindi) were noted at the beginning and end of the year. In the group who practiced yoga
breathing there was a statistically significant increase in cumulative scores. Though the effect size was
small (eta squared =0.108) the teachers reported other benefits such as better attention, improved
classroom behavior, and active participation.
The neurocognitive effects of yoga breathing were further discussed in a narrative review, which
searched PubMed, PubMed Central, and IndMed for citations with ‘Pranayama’ and ‘Yogic breathing’
as the search words [
6
]. In the review, 68 studies reported the effects of yoga breathing alone and met
other inclusion criteria, and hence were included. The studies were categorized and one sub-category
was neurocognitive assessments. The yoga breathing techniques discussed included those which varied
the rate of breathing, the depth of breathing, the nostril(s) breathed through, and where exhalation
was associated with specific sounds. The details of these yoga breathing practices as well as others
mentioned in this study are provided in Table 1.
The neurocognitive assessments included electrophysiological variables (i.e., EEG, evoked
potentials, and event related potentials), spatial and verbal memory tasks, auditory and visual
reaction time, the Purdue pegboard task, and substitution and cancellation tasks. The authors
concluded that pranayama or yoga breathing influenced neurocognitive abilities positively and that
some practices were also useful in a clinical setting.
Among yoga breathing techniques the effects of those which influenced the rate of breathing were
compared in young adults aged 18–25 years [
7
]. Into three groups, 84 participants were randomized
as (i) fast yoga breathing (rate not specified), (ii) slow yoga breathing (rate not specified) and (iii) a
control group. Participants practiced the respective yoga breathing techniques for 35 min, thrice a
week for 12 weeks. Assessments included cancellation tasks, trail making tests, digit span forward
and backward, and reaction time (auditory and visual). With both fast and slow yoga breathing,
performance in cancellation tasks, trail making tests, digit span forward, and auditory and visual
reaction time improved, with a greater magnitude of improvement in the group who practiced fast
yoga breathing.
Most of the studies described above assessed the effects of yoga breathing practiced for a number
of days or weeks. However there are also acute or immediate effects of yoga breathing, which occur
directly after the practice. Examples of the immediate effects of yoga breathing studied in children
are discussed here. In 60 adolescents (mean age 14.6 years, 38 males), 45 min of yoga bumble bee
breathing reduced the heart rate and blood pressure [
9
]. Yoga breathing with an increased depth of
breathing (called bellows breath), when practiced as nine rounds, where one round was one breath
cycle, reduced the auditory and visual reaction time significantly in 22 healthy school boys (mean age
14.5 years) [10]. A shorter reaction time suggested that participants responded sooner.
Similarly, the immediate effects of short durations of yoga breathing have been assessed in adults.
Into two groups, 94 persons were assigned to(i) high frequency yoga breathing (mean age 39.3 years;
28 females; breath frequency 1.0 Hz) and (ii) breath awareness (mean age 39.8 years; 26 females) [
8
].
The O’Connor finger dexterity task and a shape and size discrimination task were administered to all
participants just before and after 10 min of high frequency yoga breathing or breath awareness. There
were improvements in the way participants performed both tasks, though the magnitude of change
was greater after high frequency yoga breathing. These tasks require tactile discrimination, fine motor
skills, and eye hand co-ordination; hence the results suggest these abilities improved to a greater extent
with 10 min of high frequency yoga breathing. A similar trend was seen with an assessment of the
degree of optical illusion perceived, which is a measure of visual discrimination, in 30 adult males
(mean age 26.9 years), where a greater degree of improvement followed 15 min of high frequency yoga
breathing at 1.0 Hz compared to breath awareness [11].
Children 2019,6, 84 3 of 13
Table 1. A brief description of the yoga breathing practices mentioned in this study.
No. Aspect of Breathing Which Was Modified Name of the Yoga Breathing Practice Method
Sanskrit English
1. Rate
Fast yoga breathing
Kapalabhati High frequency yoga breathing Rapid nasal breathing with forceful exhalation by contracting the anterior abdominal muscles, followed by
passive inhalation at a rate of 60 to 120 breaths/min [7,8].
Kukkuriya Dog pant like yoga breathing
After sitting in the diamond pose (vajrasana) with the palms on the ground in front; rapid breathing through
the mouth is practiced with the tongue kept fully out of the mouth [7].
Bhastrika Bellows type yoga breathing Deep inhalation followed by complete exhalation, with an increase in breath rate [7].
Slowyoga breathing
Nadishodhana Alternate nostril yoga breathing
with breath-holding
The practice begins with exhalation from the left nostril, followed by inhalation from the nostril (while the
right nostril is gently occluded with pressure from the fingers), then exhalation from the right nostril
(occluding the left), inhalation from the right, followed by exhalation from the left nostril completing the cycle.
This practice may include a period of breath-holding [7].
Pranav Om repetition with breathing
Practiced in four parts: (i)Lower chest breathing with the sound of AAA, (ii) mid-chest breathing with the
sound of UUU, (iii) upper chest breathing with the sound of MMM and (iv) union of the earlier three parts
with the sound of AAA, UUU and MMM. Exhalation lasts for duration of 2 to 3 times the duration of the
inhalation [7].
Savitri Breathing related to the sun
Slow, deep, and rhythmic breathing with inhalation: Inhalation breath holding: Exhalation: Exhalation breath
holding: In a ratio of 2:1:2:1 [7].
2. Nostril breathed through Anulom-Vilom Alternate nostril yoga breathing
The practice begins with exhalation from the left nostril, followed by inhalation from the nostril (while the
right nostril is gently occluded with pressure from the fingers), then exhalation from the right nostril
(occluding the left), inhalation from the right, followed by exhalation from the left nostril completing the cycle.
This practice does not include breath-holding [5].
3. Depth Bhastrika Bellows type yoga breathing Deep inhalation followed by complete exhalation, with an increase in breath rate [7].
4. Exhalation with a sound Bhramari Bumble bee yoga breathing Deep inhalation followed by prolonged exhalation along with a humming sound like a bumble bee [9].
Children 2019,6, 84 4 of 13
High frequency yoga breathing practiced at breath frequencies between 1.0 and 2.0 Hz improved
the performance scores in an attention-based cancellation task as an immediate effect of practice across
different age groups [
12
]. The cancellation task requires the ability to focus attention, shift attention,
as well as other abilities such as visual scanning and psychomotor speed [
13
]. There was a 56.1 percent
reduction in total errors in the cancellation task in medical students (n=46, age range 18–23 years);
a 32.5 percent improvement in the net scores of middle aged adults (n=48, age range 30–59 years) and
16.4 percent in older adults (n=16, over the age of 60 years) [
12
]. High frequency yoga breathing has
not been specifically tested as a classroom based intervention in pre-teen children.
Hence the present study was designed to assess performance in an attention based cancellation
task immediately before and after 18 min of high frequency yoga breathing in 61 pre-teen children,
compared to sessions of yoga based breath awareness or quiet rest of equal duration, in a random
sequence, on separate days.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
There were 61 pre-teen children with ages ranging from 11 to 12 years (group mean age (SD),
11.2 (0.5) years) recruited for the study from a residential school in India. They were students of grade 6
and 7 and there were 25 girls. The sample size was not calculated prior to the trial. However, the power
was calculated from STAI-S scores, which were significantly different between states in the repeated
measures ANOVA and in post-hoc analysis comparing post values with pre in the high frequency
yoga breathing (HFYB) session. With the sample size of 61, and the Cohen’s d of 0.49 (medium),
the power obtained was 0.99 [
14
]. The children were recruited based on the following inclusion criteria:
(i) Apparently normal health based on a routine case history and medical examination; (ii) aged between
11 and 12 years, both genders; (iii) staying in a residential school for at least 2 years, with bi-annual
home visits; (iv) at least one years’ experience (with a frequency of 26 days/month) of yoga practice
including high frequency yoga breathing (v) right hand dominant based on Edinburgh handedness
inventory [
15
] and (vi) willingness to take part in the study. The exclusion criteria were: (i) Girls who
attained menarche or boys who reached puberty, (ii) presence of any physical or mental illness and (iii)
taking any medication.
The school was located in north India. Admission was competitive and students answered an
exam and their parents were spoken to, to understand the family background before admission. Boys
and girls are housed in separate accommodation but have classes together. The parents are allowed to
visit the children 3 to 5 times a year and the children get holidays to go home in summer and winter
for a total of two months in a year. There are 4 house-persons, who stay with the children and one
class teacher per class. There are 40 to 50 students in a class. The students have activities starting
from 04.30 h to 21.30 h, which were supervised. These activities included a 50 min yoga class in the
morning, universal prayer, regular classes, games (this included team and individual games as well
as competitive and non-competitive games) for 75 min, self-study for 60 min, and time for meals.
The remaining time was for their individual activities.
The study was approved by the institution’s ethical committee (approval number: YRD-018/018-19).
Signed informed consent was taken from the principal of the school. The principal informed the
parents of the students about the study and their oral approval was also taken.
2.2. Study Design
The children were randomly assigned to 3 groups, i.e., group A, group B, and group C using an
online random sequence generator [
16
]. The procedure was as follows: Children were given serial
numbers from 1 to 61 independent of their name, order of enrolment, or any other factor. Using an
online random generator, 61 random numbers were generated [
16
]. These random numbers were
noted beside the serial number assigned to each child. The random numbers were then written on
Children 2019,6, 84 5 of 13
identical slips of paper and folded in a similar manner. The slips were mixed and a person who was
unaware of the study design put the slips one at a time into three boxes labelled A, B, and C. The letters
A, B, and C represented group A, group B and group C respectively.
After allocation of the children to three groups, the online randomizer [
16
] was used to assign the
sequence of the three interventions to the three groups for three days. As a result, group A practiced
HFYB, BAW, and QS on day 1, day 2, and day 3 respectively; group B practiced BAW, HFYB and QS
on day 1, day 2, and day 3 respectively and group C practiced QS, BAW, and HFYB on day 1, day 2,
and day 3, respectively. Assessments of SLCT and STAI were taken before and after each session. The
data were taken in November, 2018.
2.3. Assessments
2.3.1. Six Letter Cancellation Test (SLCT)
This is a standardized paper-and-pencil test, which is used to assess selective attention [
12
,
13
].
The SLCT consists of an A4 size sheet of paper in which letters of the English alphabet are randomly
arranged in a 22
×
14 matrix. At the top of the test sheet there were6 target letters; these 6 letters
were interspersed with non-target letters in the work sheet below. Participants were instructed to spot
the 6 target letters in the work sheet and to cancel them with a slash (/). Cancellation of non-target
letters was counted as errors and the total time given to complete the test was 90 s. The net score was
calculated by subtracting wrongly cancelled letters from the total letters cancelled.
2.3.2. Spielberger’s State Trait Anxiety Inventory – State (STAI-S)
The STAI-S is a standardized 20 item inventory, which assesses the anxiety, i.e., the anxiety at
the moment of testing [
17
]. The 20 items are sentences which describe the feelings of a person which
can be assessed on a 4-point (i.e., not at all, somewhat, moderately so, and very much so) Likert scale.
The participants had to choose one out of the 4 options provided for each item. This scale was not
culture sensitive and had been standardized for use in an Asian population (of which 6.5% was an
Indian population) with test-retest reliability (r =0.60–0.94) and Cronbach’s alpha score of 0.86 [17].
2.3.3. Visual Analog Scales (VAS)
Visual analog scales were used to self-rate the quality of practice after each session. Each scale
was a 10 cm horizontal line that allowed participants to self-rate the quality of practice on a scale of 0
(worst possible) to 10 (excellent). The participants were told to put a mark on the line to describe how
well they were able to follow the instructions during the practice of HFYB, BAW, or QS.
2.4. Interventions
There were 3 interventions which each group practiced in 3 days in random order. These were (i)
high frequency yoga breathing or HFYB, (ii) breath awareness (BAW), and (iii) quiet sitting (QS).
2.4.1. High Frequency Yoga Breathing (HFYB)
HFYB consisted of rapid breathing with forceful exhalation contracting the anterior abdominal
muscles, at a rate of 1.0 Hz (60 breaths per minute). Children were instructed to avoid using excessive
force. During the practice the volunteers checked that the children appeared relaxed, especially their
facial expression, shoulders, and trunk. The children were instructed to sit cross-legged with their
eyes closed and spine and neck erect and aligned during HFYB. During this practice children were
instructed to be aware of the passage of air through the nasal and respiratory passages, while also
being aware of all the movements during the practice.
There were two teachers of each gender appointed by the school to teach yoga. Each of them
had a minimum of 2 years training in yoga, both theory and practice. They were familiar with the
contraindications to practice high frequency yoga breathing, i.e., a history of febrile convulsions or
Children 2019,6, 84 6 of 13
seizures due to any cause, recent surgery or injury to the thorax or abdomen, and any breathing disorder
in the children, which would make it difficult to carry out the practice. The yoga teachers were also
aware of the precautions they needed to observe i.e., check that the children were not hyperventilating,
over-breathing, or fatigued.
2.4.2. Breath Awareness (BAW)
BAW consisted of being aware of the movement of air in the nasal and respiratory passages
without manipulating it. The children were asked to sit cross-legged with their eyes closed and neck
and spine erect and aligned, and attention directed to awareness of the breath. Breath awareness was
added as an interventional control because being aware of one’s breath is an essential part of yoga
breathing. Hence this is yoga-based breath awareness, which for the sake of convenience is referred to
as ‘breath awareness’.
2.4.3. Quiet Sitting (QS)
QS was the control session in which participants were instructed to sit quietly with closed eyes
and their neck and spine erect and aligned. They were instructed to allow their thoughts to wander
without attempting to regulate or direct them.
The total time for each intervention was 18 min, which consisted of 3.5 min epochs of the respective
practice followed by 1 min of rest after each epoch. The SLCT and STAI-S were assessed at the beginning
and end of the sessions and the VAS was assessed at the end of each session. Figure 1represents the
study design schematically.
Figure 1.
Schematic representation of the study design. This is a schematic representation of the study
design. The interventions are as follows: The hatched rectangle indicates high frequency yoga breathing
(HFYB), the stippled rectangle indicates breath awareness (BAW), the cross hatched rectangle indicates
quiet sitting (QS) and the smaller darkened rectangles indicate rest periods of one minute duration.
2.5. Data Analyses
Data analyses were done using SPSS (24.0). The data obtained before and after the 3 sessions
were compared using a repeated measures analysis of variance (RM-ANOVA) followed by Bonferroni
adjusted post-hoc analyses. The RM-ANOVA had 2 within-subjects’ factors i.e., states, with 2 levels (pre
Children 2019,6, 84 7 of 13
and post), and sessions, with 3 levels (HFYB, BAW and QS). The data were analyzed for the group as a
whole, as well as for boys and girls separately.
Parametric tests (i.e., repeated measures ANOVA followed by multiple post-hoc tests) were used
since with sample sizes greater than 30 whether the data were normally distributed (or not) does not
impact accuracy [18].
3. Results
The group mean values (SD) for SLCT, STAI-S, and VAS are given in Table 2. ANOVA values for
SLCT and STAI-S are given in Table 3.
3.1. Repeated Measures Analyses of Variance (RM-ANOVA)
In the whole group, the STAI-S scores differed between States (F =39.606, df =1,60, p<0.001).
A significant interaction between Sessions
×
States was noted in net scores of SLCT (F=3.766, df =2,120,
p<0.05) suggesting interdependence of the two factors [
19
]. The STAI-S scores differed between States
for boys (F =25.420, df =1,35, p<0.001) as well as for girls (F =14.595, df =1,24, p<0.01). There was a
significant interaction between Sessions
×
States for the total attempted scores (F =5.791, df =2, 48,
p<0.01) and net scores (F =6.817, df =2,48, p<0.01) in girls alone.
3.2. Post-Hoc Analyses
For the whole group, there was a significant increase in the total attempted scores (p<0.05; 95%
CI =
−
4.75,
−
0.34) and net scores (p<0.05; 95% CI =
−
4.47,
−
0.15) of SLCT after HFYB, while there
was a significant increase in wrong attempts after BAW (p<0.05; 95% CI =
−
0.97,
−
0.02). The Anxiety
decreased significantly after all three sessions i.e., HFYB (p<0.001; 95% CI =3.09, 7.17), BAW (p<0.001;
95% CI =2.59, 6.82), and QS (p<0.001; 95% CI =2.73, 6.58). The average (SD) visual analog scale
scores for HFYB, BAW, and QS were 8.4 (1.8), 8.3 (1.3) and 8.4 (1.6), respectively.
For boys alone, no changes were seen in the scores of SLCT after any of the three sessions; however
the anxiety was decreased after all three sessions i.e., HFYB (p<0.001; 95% CI =3.10, 884), BAW
(p<0.01; 95% CI =1.37, 6.90) and QS (p<0.01; 95% CI =1.39, 5.78). The average (SD) visual analog
scale scores were 8.8 (1.8), 8.6 (1.3), and 8.7 (1.3) for HFYB, BAW and QS respectively for boys.
For girls alone, the total attempted scores (p<0.05; 95% CI =
−
8.22,
−
0.58) and the net score
(p<0.05; 95% CI =
−
8.22,
−
0.58) of SLCT were significantly increased after HFYB. After BAW there was
an increase in wrong attempts (p<0.05; 95% CI =
−
1.83,
−
0.13) and a decrease in net scores (p<0.05;
95% CI =0.53, 5.71). The anxiety was decreased after all three sessions in girls also [(HFYB =p<0.05;
95% CI =0.96, 6.88), (BAW =p<0.01; 95% CI =2.01, 9.03), (QS =p<0.01; CI =2.60, 9.80)]. The average
(SD) visual analog scale scores for HFYB, BAW and QS were 7.8 (1.2), 7.8 (1.7), and 7.8 (1.6), respectively,
for girls.
Children 2019,6, 84 8 of 13
Table 2. Mean values (SD) of the SLCT and anxiety pre and post HFYB, BAW, and QS sessions.
Whole Group (n=61)
Variables
HFYB BAW QS
PreMean (SD) PostMean (SD) Cohen’s d p-Value PreMean (SD) PostMean (SD) Cohen’s d p-Value PreMean (SD) PostMean (SD) Cohen’s d p-Value
TA 27.15 (7.40) 29.69 (10.47) 0.280 0.024 28.14 (8.43) 27.38 (8.65) 0.089 0.351 27.91 (7.27) 28.72 (8.73) 0.101 0.394
WA 0.59 (1.20) 0.81 (1.91) 0.138 0.391 0.44 (0.89) 0.93 (1.93) 0.326 0.043 0.48 (1.02) 0.85 (2.50) 0.194 0.275
NA 26.55 (7.32) 28.86 (10.33) 0.258 0.036 27.70 (8.34) 26.44 (8.58) 0.148 0.134 27.49 (7.41) 27.87 (8.45) 0.048 0.623
STAI-S 37.59 (10.51) 32.46 (10.10) 0.498 <0.001 37.15 (10.53) 32.44 (9.93) 0.460 <0.001 36.63 (9.98) 31.98 (10.33) 0.458 <0.001
Girls (n=25)
TA 27.84 (8.10) 32.24 (12.20) 0.424 0.026 30.76 (8.23) 28.56 (8.16) 0.268 0.097 29.28 (7.09) 29.12 (7.18) 0.022 0.909
WA 0.76 (1.36) 1.28 (2.70) 0.24 0.362 0.4 (0.57) 1.32 (2.06) 0.608 0.047 0.6 (1.08) 0.92 (3.09) 0.138 0.619
NA 27.08 (8.39) 30.96 (12.06) 0.373 0.037 30.36 (8.14) 27.24 (8.32) 0.379 0.020 28.80 (7.40) 28.20 (7.04) 0.083 0.593
STAI-S 35.48 (8.86) 31.56 (8.80) 0.44 0.012 35.76 (9.68) 30.24 (7.48) 0.638 0.003 35.96 (10.64) 29.7 6(9.19) 0.62 0.002
Boys (n=36)
TA 26.67 (6.95) 27.92 (8.83) 0.157 0.355 26.33 (8.19) 26.56 (9.0) 0.026 0.835 26.97 (7.40) 28.44 (9.76) 0.169 0.251
WA 0.47 (1.08) 0.5 (1.03) 0.028 0.905 0.47 (1.06) 0.67 (1.82) 0.134 0.455 0.389 (0.99) 0.80 (2.12) 0.248 0.289
NA 26.19 (6.58) 27.42 (8.82) 0.158 0.375 25.86 (8.08) 25.89 (8.83) 0.003 0.979 26.58 (7.40) 27.63 (9.40) 0.124 0.316
STAI-S 39.06 (11.41) 33.08 (10.97) 0.534 <0.001 38.11 (11.20) 33.97 (11.18) 0.369 0.004 37.11 (9.61) 33.53 (10.91) 0.348 0.002
Abbreviations: SLCT: six letter cancellation test; TA: Total attempted in SLCT; WA: Wrongly attempted in SLCT; NA; net attempted in SLCT; STAI-S: Spielberger’s state trait anxiety
inventory; HFYB: High frequency yoga breathing; BAW: Breath awareness; QS: Quiet sitting.
Children 2019,6, 84 9 of 13
Table 3. Details of repeated measures analysis of variance.
Whole Group (n=61)
Sn. Variables Sources df MS F p-Value Partial Eta Squared
1STAI
Sessions 2,120 16.167 0.585 0.559 0.010
States 1,60 2135.126 39.606 <0.001 0.398
Sessions ×States
1893,113.580
2.203 0.103 0.892 0.002
2 TA
Sessions
2.000,120.000
15.257 0.415 0.661 0.007
States 1,60 67.347 2.462 0.122 0.039
Sessions ×States
2.000,120.000
83.638 2.940 0.057 0.047
3WA
Sessions
1.820,109.184
0.057 0.020 0.974 0.000
States 1,60 12.265 3.563 0.064 0.056
Sessions ×States
1.807,108.396
0.584 0.262 0.748 0.004
4NA
Sessions
1.991,119.486
15.882 0.414 0.661 0.007
States 1,60 20.680 0.914 0.343 0.015
Sessions ×States 2,120 97.607 3.766 0.026 0.059
Girls (n=25)
1
Sessions
1.828,43.877
6.615 0.199 0.801 0.008
STAI States 1,24 1019.207 14.595 0.001 0.378
Sessions ×States
1.883,45.201
18.187 1.137 0.327 0.045
2
Sessions
1.872,44.931
9.451 0.191 0.813 0.008
TA States 1,24 17.340 0.457 0.505 0.019
Sessions ×States 2,48 142.740 5.791 0.006 0.194
3
Sessions
1.802,43.252
0.954 0.255 0.753 0.011
WA States 1,24 12.907 2.282 0.144 0.087
Sessions ×States
1.699,40.044
1.398 0.409 0.630 0.017
4
Sessions
1.795,43.081
3.796 0.066 0.920 0.003
NA States 1,24 0.107 0.004 0.951 0.000
Sessions ×States 2,48 157.127 6.817 0.002 0.221
Boys (n=36)
1
Sessions 2,70 13.019 0.492 0.614 0.014
STAI States 1,35 1125.227 25.420 <0.001 0.421
Sessions ×States
1.848,64.671
30.447 1.220 0.300 0.034
2
Sessions 2,70 29.866 0.977 0.381 0.027
TA States 1,35 52.019 2.498 0.123 0.067
Sessions ×States
1.957,68.491
8.181 0.267 0.762 0.008
3
Sessions
1.925,67.378
0.250 0.108 0.891 0.003
WA States 1,35 2.449 1.266 0.268 0.035
Sessions ×States
1.885,65.978
0.727 0.470 0.616 0.013
4
Sessions
1.981,69.337
30.132 1.014 0.368 0.028
NA States 1,35 31.894 1.615 0.212 0.044
Sessions ×States 2,70 7.532 0.282 0.755 0.008
Abbreviations: TA: total attempted in SLCT; WA: wrongly attempted in SLCT; NA; net attempted in SLCT; STAI-S:
Spielberger’s state trait anxiety inventory.
4. Discussion
The study was intended to determine whether an 18 min school-based intervention would increase
performance in an attention task and decrease anxiety. In 61 pre-teen children the level of anxiety
decreased comparably immediately after 18 min of high frequency yoga breathing, breath awareness,
and sitting quietly at rest. The performance in an attention-based, cancellation task improved after
yoga breathing alone.
The decrease in anxiety after all three interventions may be due to some common as well as some
distinct factors. Yoga breathing has been associated with reduced anxiety, which may be due to multiple
factors [
20
–
22
]. Similarly breath awareness and mindfulness are widely known to reduce anxiety
though different mechanisms. During breath awareness and mindfulness the mental state is focused
on the breath and is less likely to be diverted to stressful thoughts. This benefit of mindful-awareness
has also been shown in school settings [
23
]. Sitting quietly at rest may have induced a quiet meditative
state in these pre-teen children who already had 1–2years’ experience of yoga, since an earlier study in
adult yoga practitioners showed that persons with experience in yoga have lower levels of arousal
demonstrated by lower basal metabolic rates at rest compared to non-yoga practitioners [
24
]. Sitting in
silence has been shown to be stress reducing as an in-school intervention, practiced as quiet time [
23
].
Breath awareness in the present study cannot be considered the same or as effective as mindfulness
meditation or mindful breath awareness. Breath awareness is a part of yoga breathing which may be
as effective to reduce anxiety as sitting quietly with random thinking. However yoga-based breath
Children 2019,6, 84 10 of 13
awareness and mindful breath awareness can be expected to differ, as mindful breath awareness
involvespaying attention to experiences as they occur along with maintaining a non-judgmental and
accepting mental state, whilestable attention and associated cognitive processes arehighlighted [
25
].
The children in the present trial were trained in yoga breathing and in breath awareness as a part of
yoga practice.
The cancellation task used in the present study, chiefly assesses the ability to focus and shift
attention but also assesses short term recall (of the test letters), as well as visual scanning and motor
performance [13]. In 107 adults (age range 18–30 years), active inhalation improved facial expression
recognition and object recall [
26
]. Since high frequency yoga breathing involves exhaling actively and
passively inhaling through the nose at an increased rate, this breathing practice may directly influence
recognition and recall. In earlier studies on adults the immediate effect of high yoga breathing was
an improvement in visual perception [
11
]. This ability may also have partially accounted for the
improvement in the letter cancellation task performance in the present study. The present results
suggest that yoga breath awareness, which does not involve breath manipulation is less efficient
compared to yoga breathing with respect to the attention-based cancellation task, since an increase in
wrong attempts in the group as a whole occurred after breath awareness.
The 25 girls in the study showed the same results as the whole group, i.e., better performance in
the attention task following high frequency yoga breathing, increased wrong attempts after breath
awareness and reduced anxiety after all three interventions. The boys showed a decrease in anxiety
after all three interventions, with no increase in the attention task performance after any of them.
This gender-difference may be explained on the basis of another study as follows. The differential
effect of gender on the degree of engagement and the degree of effectiveness of a mindfulness program
was compared in male and female adolescents [
27
]. Females were more engaged than males in the
class and also reported less stress post-intervention. Despite the small sample size (10 female and
5 male adolescents), the results of the study may also explain the present gender differences following
yoga breathing in which girls alone showed an increase in total attempts and net scores after yoga
breathing, with increased wrong attempts after breath awareness. Apart from this, normative data for
performance in the six letter cancellation task were obtained in India, in 819 school students between 9
and 16 years of age (mean age 12.1 years) [
28
]. The children had normal health and were proficient in
English. The authors reported that girls had higher scores than boys but did not provide a possible
explanation for the difference. However, given that girls score better than boys in the cancellation
task this advantage may be further enhanced by an intervention, in this case, high frequency yoga
breathing. Finally, the program for International Student Assessment, which is a triennial international
survey has observed that even though several countries have been able to close gender gaps in learning
outcomes—boys and girls differ in their attitudes towards learning—their behavior in school, how they
choose to spend their leisure time, and the confidence they have (or do not have) in their own abilities
as students [
29
,
30
]. These factors may have also been responsible for the gender-difference in the
present study.
Since the present study aimed at assessing a yoga breathing intervention for use in a school
setting, another study which attempted a similar exercise is discussed here. In this earlier study two
yoga breathing practices were introduced for 5 min each school day for a year [
5
]. This was in an
English medium school near New Delhi. Two sections of the third grade, where students are between
7 and 8 years of age were selected. One section (n=54) was assigned the yoga intervention while
the other section (n=51) acted as the control. The scores in Math and languages (English and Hindi)
were noted in both groups at the start and end of a year. The yoga group had a significantly higher
cumulative gain in academic scores compared to the control group. The teachers also reported other
benefits (e.g., an increase in attention span, more active participation in the class room) in the yoga
group. The findings are limited by the fact that the children were not randomly assigned to the two
interventions and the additional practice could have been a source of motivation to the yoga group,
while the control group may have been less motivated in the absence of this additional attention [31].
Children 2019,6, 84 11 of 13
Unlike the present study the teachers in the study cited above were trained for a week before
introducing the yoga program in the classroom (whereas in the present study yoga teachers had a
two year post graduate degree in yoga). The students also took a shorter time, as they were trained
in one month (which included many holidays, as mentioned by the authors) [
5
]. The main reason
for this could be the difference in yoga breathing practices. In the earlier study [
5
], children were
taught deep breathing (which involved deep nasal inhalation and exhalation) and alternate nostril
breathing (which involves inhalation and exhalation through left and right nostrils alternately). High
frequency yoga breathing is appropriate for pre-teens and has been described to take approximately
two weeks for beginners to reach a breath frequency of 22 breaths per minute if they started with 11
breaths per minute, after which participants are asked to increase the rate by 11 breaths per minute
every two weeks [
32
]. Hence for novices to reach a breath rate of 55–60 breaths per minute (which was
the breath rate practiced by the pre-teen participants in the present study), the time required would be
8–10 weeks.
In the present study the finding that 18 min of high frequency yoga breathing are beneficial,
is limited by factors such as (i) the study was conducted in a residential school, which helped regulate
experimental conditions but may not be entirely the same for pre-teens living at home, (ii) the children
had already been practicing yoga for one to two years (iii) there were no assessments which would
have allowed a better understanding of the mechanisms involved, and (iv) the 18 min yoga breathing
intervention was not tested in novices to yoga.
5. Conclusions
There were 61 pre-teen children who showed an immediate response to 18 min of high frequency
yoga breathing, breath awareness, and quiet sitting in terms of reduced anxiety. Following high
frequency yoga breathing they obtained better scores in an attention-based cancellation task. In contrast
on another day the same children showed an increase in errors after yoga-based breath awareness.
School curricula are very demanding. Fitting in a stress reduction practice is challenging.
The present results suggest that high frequency yoga breathing if correctly taught can improve the
ability to focus and shift attention as an immediate effect, while reducing anxiety. Given the immediate
effects of the 18 min practice the intervention could be introduced at the start of the school day. Sitting
quietly and yoga based breath awareness were also useful to reduce anxiety and could be considered
as add-on practices to yoga breathing.
Author Contributions:
Conceptualization, S.T. and A.B.; methodology, R.K.G., K.G., B.V. and N.K.; validation,
R.K.G. and N.K.; formal analysis, K.G. and B.V.; investigation, K.G.; writing—original draft preparation, S.T. and
R.K.G.; writing—review and editing, S.T., R.K.G. and N.K.; supervision, S.T.; project administration, A.B. All
authors read and approved the manuscript.
Funding: This research received no external funding.
Acknowledgments:
This research received no external funding. The authors acknowledge the Patanjali Research
Foundation (Trust), Haridwar, India for funding the study.
Conflicts of Interest:
Authors declare no competing interests with respect to the research, authorship, and/or
publication of this article.
References
1.
Sivagurunathan, C.; Umadevi, R.; Rama, R.; Gopalakrishnan, S. Adolescent health: Present status and its
related programmes in India. Are we in the right direction? J. Clin. Diagn. Res.
2015
,9, LE01. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
2.
Central Board of Secondary Education. Exam stress: Natural Feeling Learn to Deal with it. Available online:
http://www.cbse.nic.in/examstress.htm (accessed on 21 May 2019).
3.
Hagberg, S. Learning to Live or to Leave? Education and Identity in Burkina Faso. In Education—A Way
Out of Poverty? Poverty Conference, Stockholm, Sweden, 2001; Melin, M., Ed.; The Education Division at Sida,
Department for Democracy and Social Development: Stockholm, Sweden, 2002.
Children 2019,6, 84 12 of 13
4.
Eggleston, B. The benefits of yoga for children in schools. Int. J. Health Wellness Soc.
2015
,5, 1–7. [CrossRef]
5.
Gupta, A.; Sinha, S.; Pribesh, S.; Maira, S. A fresh breath into student achievement: Pranayama and
educational outcomes. Int. J. Humanit. Soc. Sci. Invent. 2014,3, 38–46.
6.
Saoji, A.A.; Raghavendra, B.R.; Manjunath, N.K. Effects of yogic breath regulation: A narrative review of
scientific evidence. J. Ayurveda Integr. Med. 2019,10, 50–58. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
7.
Sharma, V.K.; Rajajeyakumar, M.; Velkumary, S.; Subramanian, S.K.; Bhavanani, A.B.; Madanmohan, A.S.;
Thangavel, D. Effect of fast and slow pranayama practice on cognitive functions in healthy volunteers. J. Clin.
Diagn. Res. 2014,8, 10–13. [PubMed]
8.
Telles, S.; Singh, N.; Balkrishna, A. Finger dexterity and visual discrimination following two yoga breathing
practices. Int. J. 2012,5, 37–41. [CrossRef]
9.
Kuppusamy, M.K.; Kamaldeen, D.; Pitani, R.; Amaldas, J. Immediate effects of bhramari pranayama on
resting cardiovascular parameters in healthy adolescents. J. Clin. Diagn. Res. 2016,10, 7–9. [CrossRef]
10.
Bhavanani, A.B.; Udupa, K. Acute effect of Mukh bhastrika (a yogic bellows type breathing) on reaction time.
Indian. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 2003,47, 297–300.
11.
Telles, S.; Maharana, K.; Balrana, B.; Balkrishna, A. Effects of high-frequency yoga breathing called kapalabhati
compared with breath awareness on the degree of optical illusion perceived. Percept. Mot. Ski.
2011
,112,
981–990. [CrossRef]
12.
Telles, S.; Raghuraj, P.; Arankalle, D.; Naveen, K.V. Immediate effect of high-frequency yoga breathing on
attention. Indian J. Med. Sci. 2008,62, 20–22. [CrossRef]
13. Lezak, M.D. Neuropsychological Assessment; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995.
14.
Erdfelder, E.; Faul, F.; Buchner, A. Gpower: A general power analysis program. Behav. Res. Methods
Instrum. Comput. 1996,28, 1–11. [CrossRef]
15.
Veale, J.F. Edinburgh handedness inventory-short form: A revised version based on confirmatory factor
analysis. Laterality 2014,19, 164–177. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
16.
Research Randomizer. 2007. Available online: https://www.randomizer.org/(accessed on 1 November 2018).
17.
Seok, C.B.; Hamid, H.A.; Mutang, J.; Ismail, R. Psychometric properties of the state-trait anxiety inventory
(form Y) among Malaysian university students. Sustainability. 2018,10, 3311. [CrossRef]
18.
Ghasemi, A.; Zahediasl, S. Normality tests for statistical analysis: A guide for non-statisticians. Int. J.
Endocrinol. Metab. 2012,10, 486–489. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
19. Zar, J.H. Biostatistical Analysis; Pearson: London, UK, 2018.
20.
Cramer, H.; Lauche, R.; Anheyer, D.; Pilkington, K.; Manincor, M.D.; Dobos, D.; Ward, L. Yoga for anxiety:
A systematic review and meta analysis of randomized controlled trials. Depress. Anxiety
2018
,35, 830–843.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
21.
Sengupta, P. Health impacts of yoga and pranayama: A state-of-the-art review. Int. J. Prev. Med.
2012
,3,
444–458.
22.
Pilkington, K.; Kirkwood, G.; Rampes, H.; Richardson, J. Yoga for depression: The research evidence.
J. Affect. Disord. 2005,89, 13–24. [CrossRef]
23.
Sibinga, E.M.; Perry-Parrish, C.; Chung, S.E.; Johnson, S.B.; Smith, M.; Ellen, J.M. School-based mindfulness
instruction for urban male youth: A small randomized controlled trial. Prev. Med.
2013
,57, 799–801.
[CrossRef]
24.
Chaya, M.S.; Kurpad, A.V.; Nagendra, H.R.; Nagarathna, R. The effect of long term combined yoga practice
on the basal metabolic rate of healthy adults. BMC Complement. Altern. Med. 2006,6, 28. [CrossRef]
25.
Schöne, B.; Gruber, T.; Graetz, S.; Bernhof, M.; Malinowski, P. Mindful breath awareness meditation facilitates
efficiency gains in brain networks: A steady-state visually evoked potentials study. Sci. Rep.
2018
,8, 13687.
[CrossRef]
26.
Zelano, C.; Jiang, H.; Zhou, G. Nasal Respiration Entrains Human Limbic Oscillations and Modulates
Cognitive Function. J. Neurosci. 2016,36, 12448–12467. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
27.
Bluth, K.; Roberson, P.N.E.; Girdler, S.S. Adolescent Sex differences in response to a mindfulness intervention:
A call for research. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2017,26, 1900–1914. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
28.
Pradhan, B.; Nagendra, H.R. Normative data for the letter-cancellation task in school children. Int. J.
2008
,1,
72–75.
29.
OECD. The ABC of Gender Equality in Education: Aptitude, Behaviour, Confidence; PISA, OECD Publishing:
Paris, France, 2015.
Children 2019,6, 84 13 of 13
30.
Nayak, P. 3 Reasons Why Girls Outshine Boys in Board Exams. Available online: https://yourstory.com/2016/
06/girls-outshine-boys-exams (accessed on 10 July 2019).
31.
Delbanco, T. The Healing Roles of Doctor and Patient. In Healing and the Mind; Moyers, B., Ed.; David Grubin
Productions: New York, NY, USA, 1993; pp. 7–23.
32.
Sovik, R. Learn Kapalabhati (Skull Shining Breath). Available online: https://yogainternational.com/article/
view/learn-kapalabhati-skull-shining-breath (accessed on 10 July 2019).
©
2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).