ArticlePDF Available

Gender Stereotypes Have Changed: A Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of U.S. Public Opinion Polls From 1946 to 2018

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This meta-analysis integrated 16 nationally representative U.S. public opinion polls on gender stereotypes (N = 30,093 adults), extending from 1946 to 2018, a span of seven decades that brought considerable change in gender relations, especially in women's roles. In polls inquiring about communion (e.g., affectionate, emotional), agency (e.g., ambitious, courageous), and competence (e.g., intelligent, creative), respondents indicated whether each trait is more true of women or men, or equally true of both. Women's relative advantage in communion increased over time, but men's relative advantage in agency showed no change. Belief in competence equality increased over time, along with belief in female superiority among those who indicated a sex difference in competence. Contemporary gender stereotypes thus convey substantial female advantage in communion and a smaller male advantage in agency but also gender equality in competence along with some female advantage. Interpretation emphasizes the origins of gender stereotypes in the social roles of women and men. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
Content may be subject to copyright.
Gender Stereotypes Have Changed: A Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of
U.S. Public Opinion Polls From 1946 to 2018
Alice H. Eagly
Northwestern University Christa Nater
University of Bern
David I. Miller
American Institutes for Research, Washington, D.C. Michèle Kaufmann and Sabine Sczesny
University of Bern
This meta-analysis integrated 16 nationally representative U.S. public opinion polls on gender
stereotypes (N30,093 adults), extending from 1946 to 2018, a span of seven decades that
brought considerable change in gender relations, especially in women’s roles. In polls
inquiring about communion (e.g., affectionate, emotional), agency (e.g., ambitious, coura-
geous), and competence (e.g., intelligent, creative), respondents indicated whether each trait
is more true of women or men, or equally true of both. Women’s relative advantage in
communion increased over time, but men’s relative advantage in agency showed no change.
Belief in competence equality increased over time, along with belief in female superiority
among those who indicated a sex difference in competence. Contemporary gender stereotypes
thus convey substantial female advantage in communion and a smaller male advantage in
agency but also gender equality in competence along with some female advantage. Interpre-
tation emphasizes the origins of gender stereotypes in the social roles of women and men.
Keywords: gender stereotypes, public opinion polls, communion, agency, competence
Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/amp0000494.supp
Since the mid-20th century, dramatic change has taken
place in gender relations in the United States, as illustrated
by women’s labor force participation rising from 32% in
1950 to 57% in 2018 and men’s falling from 82% to 69%
(U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2017, 2018b). Women also
now earn more bachelor’s, master’s, and doctoral degrees
than do men, unlike decades ago (Okahana & Zhou, 2018).
Given such shifts, consensual beliefs about the attributes of
women and men—that is, gender stereotypes—should have
changed. Testing this proposition required assembling a
unique data set that consists of assessments of stereotypes in
nationally representative public opinion polls.
Gender stereotypes are ubiquitous because the social cat-
egory sex, which divides most humans into two groups
based on their reproductive functions, is fundamental to
human cognition and social organization. Even young chil-
dren recognize this grouping (Martin & Ruble, 2010). They
then begin to understand the meaning of these categories
through observation of the behaviors and events linked with
each sex. Throughout their lives, individuals receive exten-
sive information about women and men from direct obser-
vation as well as indirect observation through social sharing
and cultural representations. As a result, most people ac-
quire some version of their culture’s gender stereotypes.
The importance of stereotyping in theories of gender
(e.g., Bem, 1993; Deaux & Major, 1987; Eagly & Wood,
2012; Eccles, 1994; Ridgeway, 2011; Spence, 1993) has
inspired much research. However, with few exceptions, this
research has consisted of small-scale studies of college
undergraduates (e.g., Lueptow, Garovich-Szabo, & Luep-
tow, 2001) or other nonrepresentative samples (e.g., Haines,
Deaux, & Lofaro, 2016; Prentice & Carranza, 2002). These
sampling limitations have compromised external validity,
XAlice H. Eagly, Department of Psychology, Northwestern Uni-
versity; XChrista Nater, Department of Psychology, University of Bern;
XDavid I. Miller, American Institutes for Research, Washington, D.C.;
Michèle Kaufmann and XSabine Sczesny, Department of Psychology,
University of Bern.
The research was supported in part by a grant (P0BEP1_162210) from
the Swiss National Science Foundation awarded to Christa Nater.
Michèle Kaufmann is now at Growth from Knowledge (GfK), Nurem-
berg, Germany.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Alice H.
Eagly, Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, 2029 Sheri-
dan Road, Evanston, IL 60208. E-mail: eagly@northwestern.edu
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
American Psychologist
© 2019 American Psychological Association 2019, Vol. 1, No. 999, 000
0003-066X/19/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/amp0000494
1
especially given the overrepresentation of college students
from introductory psychology classes (Henry, 2008).
This project instead relied on public opinion polls that
surveyed large, nationally representative samples. Specifi-
cally, between 1946 and 2018, several respected polling
organizations surveyed U.S. adults’ beliefs about the attri-
butes of women and men. These data offer a valuable
opportunity to study gender stereotypes across a span of
decades that brought fundamental changes in relationships
between women and men.
Gender Stereotypes: Their Content and Origins
Most research on the content of these stereotypes has
found two themes, which, following Bakan (1966), are
typically labeled communion and agency (e.g., Diekman &
Eagly, 2000; Rucker, Galinsky, & Magee, 2018; Sczesny,
Nater, & Eagly, 2019; Williams & Best, 1990). Communion
orients people to others and their well-being (e.g., compas-
sionate, warm, expressive), whereas agency orients people
to the self and one’s own mastery and goal attainment (e.g.,
ambitious, assertive, competitive). Communion prevails in
the female stereotype, and agency in the male stereotype.
Notably, some researchers have emphasized competence
(e.g., intelligent) rather than agency as fundamental to ste-
reotyping, and these two qualities tend to be correlated
(Cuddy, Fiske, & Glick, 2008). Nonetheless, agency and
competence should show different trends, given that agency
is a much stronger theme than competence in the male
stereotype (Sczesny et al., 2019).
Like other stereotypes, gender stereotypes reflect essen-
tialism, or the tendency to infer essences, often taking the
form of traits underlying individuals’ behaviors (Prentice &
Miller, 2006). Although some people ascribe such trait
essences to biology, others instead ascribe them to social-
ization and social position in society (Rangel & Keller,
2011). For example, in U.S. public opinion poll data (Pew
Research Center, 2017), among the 87% of respondents
who indicated that men and women are different rather than
similar on “how they express their feelings,” 58% ascribed
these differences mainly to “society,” and 42% to “biol-
ogy.”
Origins of Gender Stereotype Content in
Social Roles
According to social role theory (Eagly & Wood, 2012;
Koenig & Eagly, 2014), gender stereotypes stem from peo-
ple’s direct and indirect observations of women and men in
their social roles. Role-constrained behavior provides cru-
cial information because most behavior enacts roles. More-
over, people spontaneously infer individuals’ social roles
(e.g., student) from their behaviors (e.g., studied in the
library), with downstream consequences of ascribing role-
consistent traits to them (e.g., hardworking; Chen, Banerji,
Moons, & Sherman, 2014). When people observe members
of a group (e.g., gender, race) occupying certain roles more
often than members of other groups do, the behaviors usu-
ally enacted within these roles influence the traits believed
to be typical of the group. To the extent that people in the
same society have similar observations, these beliefs be-
come shared cultural expectations.
Relevant to possible shifts in gender stereotypes, the
social roles of women and men have changed since the
mid-20th century (for causes, see Blau & Winkler, 2018).
Female and male labor force participation has converged
considerably in the United States, as in many other nations
(Ortiz-Ospina & Tzvetkova, 2017). Nevertheless, a com-
mon arrangement is a neotraditional division of labor (Ger-
son, 2017), whereby women perform the majority of the
domestic work and men have more continuous employment
with longer hours and higher wages (e.g., Bianchi, Lesnard,
Nazio, & Raley, 2014; U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics,
2018a).
As women entered the labor force in large numbers,
occupational sex segregation declined (Blau, Brummund, &
Liu, 2013). In particular, women entered many male-
dominated occupations that require higher education and
offer relatively high prestige (Lippa, Preston, & Penner,
2014). Nevertheless, at least half of U.S. female and male
employees would have to exchange jobs to produce a fully
integrated labor force (Hegewisch & Hartmann, 2014). This
segregation is patterned vertically and horizontally (e.g.,
Lippa et al., 2014). Vertical segregation places more men
than women in positions with higher pay and authority,
whereas horizontal segregation concentrates women and
Alice H. Eagly
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
2EAGLY, NATER, MILLER, KAUFMANN, AND SCZESNY
men in occupations requiring different skills and facilitating
different personal goals.
Demonstrating horizontal segregation, women and men
are concentrated in occupations with different attributes.
According to two U.S. studies based on the extensive oc-
cupational data available from the Occupational Informa-
tion Network (O
NET: www.onetonline.org), women’s rep-
resentation was predicted by occupations’ requirements for
social skills and opportunities for social contribution (i.e.,
helping others) and workplace flexibility, whereas men’s
representation was predicted by occupations’ requirements
for physical strength; competition; interaction with things;
and analytical, mathematical, and technical skills (Cortes &
Pan, 2018; Levanon & Grusky, 2016; see also Baker &
Cornelson, 2018, for analysis of occupations’ demands for
sensory, motor, and spatial aptitudes).
Occupational sex segregation based on these predictors is
intact, despite the desegregation that has followed mainly
from educated women entering male-dominated profes-
sional and managerial occupations. Yet, many of these
occupations have resegregated internally by developing
female-dominated subfields (Levanon & Grusky, 2016).
Replicating the familiar macrolevel themes, such female
specializations include pediatrics and gynecology in medi-
cine and human resources and public relations in manage-
ment.
Implications of Role Changes for Stereotype Content
Women’s increased labor force participation should have
boosted their perceived competence because employment
ordinarily requires complex task coordination and adher-
ence to bureaucratic constraints such as performance eval-
uations. Moreover, women’s educational gains have fos-
tered their entry into occupations with cognitive demands
and prestige similar to men’s occupations (Cortes & Pan,
2018; Lippa et al., 2014).
Despite these substantial changes in employment and
education and the egalitarian attitudinal shifts that have
accompanied them (e.g., Donnelly et al., 2016), gender
stereotypes would continue to follow from persisting occu-
pational segregation as well as the uneven division of wage
labor and domestic work between men and women (Gerson,
2017). Specifically, vertical segregation would further the
stereotype of men’s agency because of the agency ascribed
to leadership and authority roles (Koenig, Eagly, Mitchell,
& Ristikari, 2011). Following from horizontal segregation,
(a) men’s agency would also be conveyed by their presence
as families’ main provider and in occupations requiring
competitiveness, physical prowess, and robustness, and (b)
women’s communion would be conveyed by their presence
as families’ main homemaker and in occupations requiring
social skills and yielding social contribution (Cortes & Pan,
2018; Levanon & Grusky, 2016).
In summary, for communion, there is little reason to
expect that women’s advantage over men has lessened,
given their continuing concentration in communal roles. In
fact, Lueptow et al. (2001) reported a strengthening of the
female communal stereotype between 1974 and 1997, based
on his surveys of U.S. sociology students at the University
of Akron. For agency, it might seem that men’s advantage
would decrease, given increases of women in leadership
roles and in occupations such as lawyer and manager (Carli
& Eagly, 2017). However, any such influence on agency
would be attenuated by the fine-grained internal resegrega-
tion of such occupations. For competence, women should
gain relative to men because of their increased education
and employment, especially in higher prestige jobs. There-
fore, public opinion data on gender stereotypes should re-
veal continuing advantage for women in communion and
men in agency but gains for women relative to men in
competence.
Method
Search for Polls and Inclusion Criteria
Searches. Between 2010 and 2018, we searched for
public opinion polls in multiple databases, including Roper
Center for Public Opinion Research (inclusive of iPOLL),
Polling the Nations, PollingReport.com, Gallup Analytics
and World Poll, Pew Research Center, National Opinion
Research Center, General Social Survey, American Na-
tional Election Studies, Interuniversity Consortium for Po-
litical and Social Research, World Values Survey, Google,
and Google Scholar. Keywords included (a) stereotype
Christa Nater
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
3
GENDER STEREOTYPES
paired with gender, sex, male, female, men, or women; (b)
gender or sex paired with attitudes, beliefs,oropinions; (c)
women or men; and (d) gender, sex, men,orwomen paired
with traits such as intelligent, aggressive, and romantic,
which were frequent in the polls initially identified.
Inclusion criteria. The included polls were nationally
representative for the United States, which is the normative
practice for polling organizations (see Gallup Organization,
2019). Searching in archives found 24 national U.S. polls
that included one or more items assessing descriptive gen-
der stereotypes, at least one of which pertained to commu-
nion, agency, or competence. The 15 included polls asked
about the distribution of each trait between the sexes (e.g.,
“In general, do you think each of the following character-
istics is more true of women or men, or equally true of
both?”). The nine excluded polls asked about only one sex
(k5), asked about women and men in separate items (k
1), or used a different answer format (k3). To supplement
the 15 included polls with current data, we contracted the
polling organization GfK to collect a nationally representa-
tive U.S. sample using their Government & Academic Om-
nibus panel in April 2018. In total, the final set of 16 polls
included 30,093 adults sampled from 1946 to 2018 (see
Table 1).
Polling organizations disseminate their survey results at
their websites and in written reports and press releases.
Most of these polls are subsequently archived, predomi-
nantly at the Roper Center. Although private polls about
consumer and political issues may not be made public, these
are unlikely to have surveyed gender stereotypes. Given the
practice of archiving surveys regardless of their results,
publication bias is not relevant to this meta-analysis.
Classification of stereotypical traits. Guided by earlier
analyses (e.g., Diekman & Eagly, 2000; Koenig & Eagly,
2014, Study 4), three of the authors independently classified
the polls’ traits into the categories communal, agentic, and
competent. The overall interrater reliability (Fleiss kappa) was
␬⫽.81, with ␬⫽.93 for communion, ␬⫽.77 for agency, and
␬⫽.92 for competence. Based on at least two raters’ classi-
fying an item into the same category, the result was (a) 13
communal items: ability to handle people well, affectionate,
compassionate, emotional, generous, honest, nurturing, outgo-
ing, patient, polite and well-mannered, romantic, sensitive, and
unselfish; (b) 17 agentic items: ability to make decisions,
aggressive, ambitious, arrogant, calm in emergencies, confi-
dent, courageous, critical, decisive, demanding, hardworking,
independent, possessive, proud, selfish, strong, and stubborn;
and (c) 10 competent items: ability to create or invent new
things, creative, innovative, intelligent, level-headed, logical,
organized, smart, thorough in handling details, and willing to
accept new ideas. Because intelligent was the item most re-
peated across the polls, its data also appear in a separate
analysis, along with the similar item smart from Gallup (1989).
Items that did not fit the categories (e.g., cautious, happy) were
discarded.
Meta-Analytic Procedures
Effect size calculations. The main outcome variable
was the percentage of respondents who ascribed a trait more
to women than men (excluding equal responses). For each
trait, for example, if 300 of 1,000 respondents indicated that
ambitious is truer of women and 500 that it is truer of men,
that percentage was 37.5% (i.e., 300 of the 800 who chose
men or women, among the 1,000 respondents). An addi-
tional effect size was the percentage of respondents indicat-
ing that women and men are equal on a trait, as opposed to
more true of one sex. In this example, in which 200 of the
1,000 respondents answered equally true, that percentage
was 20% (i.e., 200 among the 1,000). Aggregated across
traits within each dimension, these percentages were con-
verted to log odds for statistical analysis and converted back
to the more intuitive percentage metric for descriptive sta-
tistics (see Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009,
p. 312). Most polls (12 of 16) provided survey weights to
adjust for unequal sampling probabilities and nonresponse
bias. To ensure national representativeness, effect size cal-
culations incorporated these weights, using the survey pack-
age in R (Lumley, 2017). In the polls lacking such weights,
all respondents were weighted equally. Design-based stan-
dard errors adjusted for correlated responses and survey
weights (McNeish, Stapleton, & Silverman, 2017).
Analyses conducted separately for each stereotype dimen-
sion averaged effect sizes within polls for each of the three
stereotype domains (e.g., for competence, averaging cre-
ativity and intelligence). Alternative analyses accounted for
David I. Miller
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
4EAGLY, NATER, MILLER, KAUFMANN, AND SCZESNY
such dependent effect sizes by using robust variance esti-
mation where it was appropriate (Tanner-Smith, Tipton, &
Polanin, 2016; Appendix S2 in the online supplemental
materials details the preregistered analytic strategy for han-
dling effect size dependencies).
Statistical models. Focal analyses modeled the log
odds of ascribing traits to women versus men for each
stereotype dimension, while ignoring equal responses. Ad-
ditional analyses modeled another effect size, the log odds
of equal responding. Also, multinomial logistic regression
analyses examined how all response categories changed
relative to each other (Begg & Gray, 1984; see Appendix S3
in the online supplemental materials; Figure S2 and Appen-
dix S3 also describe an analysis that assigned numeric
values to the response options). Mixed-effects meta-
regression models assumed that the observed effect size
variation was due to fixed effects of moderators (e.g., sur-
vey year), random effects of residual between-poll hetero-
geneity, and within-poll sampling error (Borenstein et al.,
2009). Between-poll heterogeneity was quantified by 90%
prediction intervals (a measure of the estimated dispersion of
true underlying effects) and I
2
statistics (percentage of total
variability in effect sizes due to true between-poll heterogene-
ity rather than chance). The metafor R package calculated
these models using restricted maximum likelihood estimation
and the Knapp–Hartung adjustment to account for uncertainty
in estimating between-poll heterogeneity (Viechtbauer, 2010).
The online supplemental materials and Open Science Frame-
work site (https://osf.io/g98c6/) contain the data files and R
code needed to reproduce all analyses including figures and
tables.
Tests of robustness. Four robustness checks examined
the trends over time while either controlling for covariates
or removing the polls from the 1940s and 1950. The first
robustness check controlled for the polling method, which
was a particularly stringent test given that polling switched
over the years from face-to-face to phone polling and then
to online panels (see Table 1). The second robustness check
controlled for the presence of the equal option of same/
equally true of both in the poll. Although an explicit equal
option was missing from 10 of the face-to-face and phone
surveys, respondents were free to volunteer this answer,
which interviewers recorded. The third robustness check
controlled for the log odds of respondents indicating same/
equally true of both, either by choosing this provided alter-
native or spontaneously giving this answer when it was not
provided. This control for equal responses, which are pre-
sumably more politically correct than is choosing one sex,
should account for confounds due to potential changes in
social desirability over time (e.g., Krupnikov, Piston, &
Bauer, 2016). The fourth robustness check excluded the two
1946 polls and the 1950 poll, which might have dispropor-
tionately influenced regression models because all other
polls were from 1974 or later. Furthermore, results of these
earliest polls might reflect the sharp rise in women’s labor
force participation during World War II and its subsequent
partial fall (Goldin & Olivetti, 2013). Therefore, an analysis
omitted these polls to examine whether historical trends
were due to these possibly anomalous early data points.
Evaluative content of stereotypical traits. Another
potential confound follows from traits’ evaluative content,
which differed across the items (e.g., courageous is more
positive than arrogant) and might have changed over time.
Such a confound could have contributed to the historical
trends if increasing pressures for political correctness en-
couraged ascribing more desirable traits to women. For each
dimension, these analyses tested whether the items’ posi-
tivity predicted ascribing traits more to women than men.
Assessment of the items’ evaluative content relied on
prior research on the likability of person-descriptive words.
Beginning with Anderson (1968), researchers repeatedly
obtained likability ratings of many such words. The latest
study (Chandler, 2018) did the following: (a) extended this
effort to ratings (on 7-point scales) of 1,048 words by
college students and Mechanical Turk workers and (b)
determined whether traits’ likability had changed over time
(the only change was on aggressive, which became more
negative). Our procedure matched each poll attribute to its
likability value in Chandler’s study or, for aggressive, its
average value across the surveys. For 26 attributes, an exact
word match was possible; for 12 attributes, the match was to
a close synonym; and for one attribute (calm in emergen-
cies), no match was possible. See Table S1 in the online
supplemental materials for items and means.
Michèle
Kaufmann
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
5
GENDER STEREOTYPES
Subgroup Analyses
The raw data were available for 15 of the 16 polls (Fox
News, 2006, was missing), allowing for analyses of the
demographic variables of respondent sex (k15 polls), age
(k15), college education (k12), race–ethnicity (k
14), marital status (k13), and employment status (k
13); Table S2 in the online supplemental materials lists the
polls included in these analyses. We preregistered several
hypotheses for these analyses on the Open Science Frame-
work website (https://osf.io/kmwg8). For instance, one such
prediction was that female respondents would ascribe
greater competence to women more often than would male
respondents, due to ingroup preference. Analyses also ex-
plored whether historical trends varied in magnitude across
demographic subgroups.
Results
The analyses addressed two main questions: (a) How
have gender stereotypes changed across a span of decades
(1940s to 2010s) that brought considerable change in wom-
en’s roles? and (b) Did respondent demographic variables
moderate the changes over time?
Analyses for Responding That Traits Are More
True of One Sex
Meta-analytic means. Table 2 presents the mean per-
centages of respondents ascribing stereotypical qualities to
women rather than men, along with associated distributional
statistics. A percentage of 50% means that the numbers of
“more true of women” and “more true of men” responses
were equal. These percentages varied considerably across
the dimensions, in the female direction for communion
(85%), competence (64%), and the item intelligent (66%)
and in the male direction for agency (32%). Hence, the
female communion stereotype showed the largest consensus
among respondents. Nearly all variability in observed effect
sizes could be attributed to between-poll heterogeneity
rather than chance (I
2
97.85 for all stereotype domains),
suggesting that moderators should help explain differences
across polls.
Simple regression analyses over time. These models
used the poll year to predict the log odds of respondents
indicating that women, rather than men, have more of the
relevant attribute (see Figure 1). Communion showed a
significant increase over time (b.037, SE .008, p
.001). For instance, among respondents stating a sex differ-
ence, 54% indicated that women are more communal in
Roper’s (1946) poll, but 83% did so in the 1989 poll and
97% in the 2018 poll. In contrast, agency showed no sig-
nificant trend (b⫽⫺.008, SE .005, p.140). Compe-
tence (b.025, SE .005, p.001) and its intelligence
component (b.024, SE .006, p.009) also showed a
significant increase. The direction of the competence ste-
reotype reversed over time; for instance, 34% of respon-
dents indicated that women are more intelligent than men in
Gallup’s (1946) poll, but 65% did so in the 2018 poll.
Tests of robustness. Multivariable meta-regression
models tested whether the predicted effects of historical
time remained after controlling separately for each of the
following three covariates: (a) polling method (two
dummy codes comparing online panels and face-to-face
vs. phone polling), (b) equal alternative (dummy code
comparing the presence vs. absence of the equal response
alternative), and (c) log odds of respondents responding
equal versus different. The fourth robustness check tested
whether the trends over time remained after removing the
three polls from the 1940s and 1950s. Figure 2 displays
the regression coefficients for poll year using each of
these four robustness checks.
These analyses showed that the historical trends were
sensitive to the robustness check of polling method for
communion (p.147) and the item intelligent (p.229),
perhaps due to the statistical difficulties in partitioning
variance due to polling method versus polling year (consis-
tent with the wide 95% confidence intervals [CIs] for the
blue bars [the second bar in each group] in Figure 2).
Nevertheless, the trend over time remained significant for
competence (p.001). No conclusions changed when
controlling for whether an equal alternative option was
provided (green bars [the third bar in each group]) or for the
proportion of equal answers (purple bars [the fourth bar in
Sabine Sczesny
Photo by © Luca
Christen, 2019
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
6EAGLY, NATER, MILLER, KAUFMANN, AND SCZESNY
each group]). The only conclusion that changed when re-
moving the 1940s and 1950s polls was the historical trend
for the single item intelligent (p.629; orange bars [the
fifth bar in each group] in Figure 2). This item was present
in 1946 and subsequently only from 1989 to 2018, a period
in which women were stably favored over men (see also
Figure 1). In summary, trends for competence were robust
to all four checks, whereas trends for communion and
intelligence showed some sensitivity. Trends for agency
remained nonsignificant.
Evaluative content of stereotypical traits. To exam-
ine whether the items’ evaluative content was a confound
(consistent with a political correctness interpretation of ste-
reotype change over time), secondary analyses tested
whether the traits’ likability ratings or their interaction
with poll year predicted the effect sizes (see the Method
section). Items’ likability did not significantly predict
effect sizes for agency (p.624) or competence (p
.169) but did for communion (b⫽⫺.428, SE .051, p
.001); unexpectedly, respondents overall ascribed com-
munal traits more often to women than to men for less
positive items. It is important to note, however, that the
historical trends for communion and competence re-
mained significant when items’ likability was included as
a covariate (communion, p.016; competence, p
.017), and the trend for agency remained nonsignificant
Table 1
Descriptive Information for U.S. Polls of Communion, Agency, and Competence Stereotypes
Source
Data
collection
year Sample size Method Equal
alternative
a
Dimension and items
Communion Agency Competence
Roper (1946) 1946 5,027 Face-to-face Present Ability to handle people well,
polite and well-mannered,
unselfish
Ability to make decisions Ability to create or
invent new
things, willing to
accept new
ideas,
thoroughness in
handling details
Gallup (1946) 1946 3,226 Face-to-face Absent Common sense,
intelligent
Gallup (1950) 1950 1,351 Phone Absent Courageous Level-headed
Virginia Slims
(1974) 1974 3,880 Face-to-face Present Romantic
Roper (1977) 1977 2,006 Face-to-face Absent Romantic
New York
Times
(1983) 1983 1,309 Phone Absent Honest Logical
Roper (1985) 1985 1,997 Face-to-face Present Calm in emergencies
U.S. News
(1987) 1987 1,005 Phone Present Romantic
Gallup (1989) 1989 1,234 Phone Absent Affectionate, emotional,
honest, patient, romantic,
sensitive
Aggressive, ambitious,
confident, courageous,
critical, demanding,
independent,
possessive, proud,
selfish, strong
Creative, level-
headed, logical,
organized, smart
Gallup (1995) 1995 1,020 Phone Absent Affectionate, emotional,
patient Aggressive, ambitious,
courageous Creative, intelligent
Gallup (2000) 2000 1,026 Phone Absent Affectionate, emotional,
patient Aggressive, ambitious,
courageous Creative, intelligent
Fox News
(2006) 2006 900 Phone Absent — Intelligent
Pew (2008) 2008 2,250 Phone Absent Compassionate, emotional,
outgoing Ambitious, arrogant,
decisive, hardworking,
stubborn
Creative, intelligent
Pew (2015) 2014 1,835 Online panel Present Compassionate, honest Ambitious, decisive Intelligent,
innovative,
organized
CBS News
(2015) 2015 1,018 Phone Absent Romantic
Eagly et al.
(2018) 2018 1,009 Online panel Present Affectionate, compassionate,
sensitive, emotional Ambitious, aggressive,
courageous, decisive Creative,
intelligent,
innovative,
organized
a
Same/equally true response alternative was present or absent in the question format.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
7
GENDER STEREOTYPES
(p.317). In addition, items’ likability did not interact with
poll year to predict effect sizes for any stereotype dimension
(all ps.082), meaning that historical trends did not signifi-
cantly differ for more versus less positive items.
Analyses for Responding That Traits Are the
Same or Equally True of Women and Men
The analyses presented so far predicted responding
more true of women versus more true of men, excluding
responses that the sexes are equal or the same. However,
the proportion of this equal responding could also have
changed over time. Therefore, all prior analyses were
repeated using a secondary effect size metric: the odds of
responding equal versus different.
Meta-analytic means. Communion produced the smallest
percentage of respondents responding equal (24%), agency
(33%) and competence (39%) were moderate, and intelligence
produced the largest percentage (59%). Hence, most partici-
pants differentiated women and men (i.e., expressed gender
stereotypes), although not on intelligence. Nearly all variability
in observed effect sizes could be attributed to between-poll
heterogeneity rather than chance (all I
2
99.59%; see Table
S3 in the online supplemental materials for details).
Simple regression analyses over time. These models
revealed that only competence (b.021, SE .007, p
.016) increased significantly over the years in respondents
indicating that women and men are equal (vs. different). For
instance, 25% indicated competence equality in the Roper
Communion
Percentage Choosing Women
0 255075100
b = 0.037
p < .001
aAgency
b = −0.008
p = .140
b
Competence
Poll Year
Percentage Choosing Women
1940 1960 1980 2000 2020
0 255075100
b = 0.025
p < .001
cIntelligence
Poll Year
1940 1960 1980 2000 2020
b = 0.024
p = .009
d
Figure 1. Change over historical time in the mean percentage and 95% confidence intervals of respondents
choosing women as more communal (Panel a), agentic (Panel b), competent (Panel c), and intelligent (Panel d)
than are men in U.S. polls. The regression coefficients (bs) and pvalues are from the simple meta-regression
models. The blue (thick dark lines) represent average model predictions converted from log odds to percentages.
The shaded blue (gray) regions indicate 90% prediction intervals based on average model predictions and
residual between-poll heterogeneity; on average, 90% of true poll averages will fall within these regions. A value
of 50% indicates that the number of respondents selecting the women more option equaled the number selecting
the men more option. See the online article for the color version of this figure.
Table 2
Mean Effect Sizes for Percentage of Respondents Choosing Women
in U.S. Polls of Communion, Agency, and Competence Stereotypes
Stereotype
measure kMean
a
90% prediction
interval t
2
I
2
Communion 12 85% [56, 96] 6.62
ⴱⴱⴱ
.84 99.52
Agency 9 32% [20, 47] 5.76
ⴱⴱⴱ
.15 98.53
Competence 11 64% [33, 87] 2.40
.63 99.58
Intelligence 8 66% [41, 85] 2.96
.41 97.85
Note.knumber of polls; mean random-effects weighted mean of the
percentage of respondents choosing women as possessing more of the
attribute; prediction interval the middle 90% of the true underlying
effects; ttest statistic for the mean being different from 50%;
2
tau-squared, the estimated between-poll variance of effect sizes on a log
odds scale; I
2
percentage of total variability in effect sizes due to true
between-poll heterogeneity rather than chance. Heterogeneity (Q, not dis-
played) was significant for all stereotype measures (all ps.001).
a
A mean of 50% signifies that the percentage of respondents indicating that
women have more of the attribute was equal to the percentage indicating that
men have more. Higher numbers indicate more ascription of the attribute to
women, and lower numbers indicate more ascription to men.
p.05.
ⴱⴱ
p.01.
ⴱⴱⴱ
p.001.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
8EAGLY, NATER, MILLER, KAUFMANN, AND SCZESNY
(1946) poll, whereas 70% provided this answer in the 2018
poll. See Figure S1 in the online supplemental materials for
robustness checks.
Multinomial Analyses
Figure 3 displays analyses that simultaneously took into
account three responses: women more, men more, and
equal. Of most interest is the analysis on competence, which
showed a significant increase over time in the percentage
answering equal. The responses indicating that either sex is
more competent than the other declined, but choosing men
declined more sharply than did choosing women. Thus,
women’s competence advantage over men increased, even
though paradoxically both types of sex-differentiated re-
sponses declined in absolute terms. The likelihood of re-
sponding same versus different did not change significantly
for agency or communion. For details, see Appendix S3 and
Figure S1 in the online supplemental materials.
Subgroup Analyses for Respondent Demographic
Characteristics
Subgroup analyses of the focal effect sizes comparing
women versus men (excluding equal responses) examined
variation in stereotypes across respondent sex, college ed-
ucation, employment status, marital status, raceethnicity,
and birth cohort. These analyses served as both further
robustness checks of the main findings and tests of specific
preregistered hypotheses.
Historical trends and current stereotypes. Historical
trends were similar across demographic subgroups. In total,
28 tests determined whether respondent demographics mod-
erated historical trends in gender stereotyping. Although
four of these 28 exploratory tests were significant, even in
these cases, the significant trends persisted for each sub-
group. In other words, across demographic subgroups, the
increase in ascribing communion and competence to women
versus men was robust.
In addition, the subgroups generally agreed on the direc-
tion of the stereotypes in recent years. Figure 4 shows the
predictions of simple regression models for the stereotypes
in the year 2018 (see also Figure S3 in the online supple-
mental materials for the stereotype means averaged across
poll years). In general, gender stereotypes for communion,
agency, and competence were widely shared, given that
none of the confidence intervals crossed 50% for any sub-
group. The percentages for intelligence ratings were less
precise (note the wider CIs) and sometimes did not differ
significantly from 50%, although point estimates always
exceeded 50%. Notable are the consistently extreme per-
centages of respondents in all subgroups indicating that
women are the more communal sex.
Confirmatory tests of preregistered hypotheses. Consis-
tent with preregistered hypotheses, results showed an in-
group preference for competence and intelligence. Specifi-
cally, the odds of respondents ascribing competence and
intelligence to women versus men were higher among fe-
male than male respondents, when analyzing both overall
means (competence, mean OR 2.50, p.001; intelli-
−0.05
0.00
0.05
0.10
Communion Agency Competence Intelligence
Stereotype Domain
Increase in Log Odds Per Year
(Regression Coefficient)
Baseline
Controlling polling method
Controlling equal alternative
Controlling answering same
Removing 1940s/50s data
Figure 2. Regression coefficients and 95% confidence intervals for the change over historical time for each
stereotype dimension and the item intelligent. Positive values indicate that a dimension was ascribed to women
(vs. men) more often over time. The bars (from left to right) indicate regression coefficients without controls,
with controls for each of three potential confounds, and with the removal of data from the 1940s and 1950s. See
the online article for the color version of this figure.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
9
GENDER STEREOTYPES
gence, mean OR 2.37, p.007) and the predictions for
the year 2018 shown in Figure 4 (competence, mean OR
2.46, p.002; intelligence, mean OR 2.56, p.039).
Nevertheless, in 2018, even male respondents ascribed com-
petence more often to women than men, despite ingroup
preferences. Predictions that were not confirmed are that the
odds of ascribing competence and intelligence to women
(vs. men) would be higher among college-educated than less
educated respondents (ps.25) or higher among younger
than older respondents within the same poll (ps.21).
Instead, respondents now ascribe competence in general and
intelligence more often to women than men, regardless of
college education and birth cohort.
Exploratory analyses of other demographic differen-
ces. These analyses found that the overall odds of ascrib-
ing traits to women versus men were higher for female than
male respondents for agency (mean OR 1.67, p.001)
and communion (mean OR 1.49, p.002); the same
differences were also found for the predicted means for year
2018 (p.049 and p.008, respectively). Hence, respon-
dent sex differences (i.e., higher odds of women assigning
traits to women vs. men) were similar for all stereotype
domains although largest for competence in general and
intelligence. Results also suggested racial differences. For
instance, the odds of ascribing agency and intelligence to
women versus men were higher among Black than White
respondents (agency, mean OR 1.72, p.005; intelli-
gence, mean OR 1.85, p.001); the same differences
were also found for the predicted means for year 2018 (p
.026 and p.004, respectively).
Discussion
Challenging traditional claims that stereotypes of women
and men are fixed or rigid, our study joins others in finding
stereotypes to be flexibly responsive to changes in group
members’ social roles (e.g., Koenig & Eagly, 2014). As the
roles of women and men have changed since the mid-20th
century, so have consensual beliefs about their attributes.
These conclusions derive from national public opinion
polls conducted in the United States with over 30,000 adult
respondents. Although such polls are limited in number,
they are sufficient to demonstrate clear increases in the
ascription of communion to women relative to men but a
lack of change in agency. Although women also gained in
competence relative to men, belief in competence equality
has increased over time as well.
As demonstrated by Diekman and Eagly’s (2000) re-
search on dynamic stereotypes, people generally think that
gender stereotypes on agency have converged and will
continue to do so because of the growing similarity in the
employment and domestic commitments of women and
men. Contrary to this propositional reasoning, our meta-
analysis showed that these socioeconomic changes are as-
Communion
a
Percentage of Respondents
0 255075100
women
men
equal
Agency
b
women
men
equal
Competence
c
Poll Year
Percentage of Respondents
1940 1960 1980 2000 2020
0 255075100
women
men
equal
Intelligence
d
Poll Year
1940 1960 1980 2000 2020
women
men
equal
Figure 3. Stereotypes displaying change over historical time in the mean percentage of respondents indicating
more true of women, more true of men, or equal/same on communal (Panel a), agentic (Panel b), competent
(Panel c), and intelligent (Panel d). The percentage of missing responses is not shown but was always less than
10%. See the online article for the color version of this figure.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
10 EAGLY, NATER, MILLER, KAUFMANN, AND SCZESNY
sociated with stereotypic increases in women’s communion
and competence but not in their agency. The flaw in peo-
ple’s reasoning likely flows from their lack of conscious
awareness of the extent of sex segregation of the labor force,
as demonstrated by Beyer’s (2018) findings of systematic
underestimation of occupational segregation. Thus, wom-
en’s increasing employment has crowded them mainly into
jobs emphasizing social skills and social contribution (Cor-
tes & Pan, 2018), often in the expanding service, education,
and health care sectors of the economy. Because people
have increasingly observed women in such jobs, the asso-
ciative processes of stereotype formation following from
observations of women’s paid work have more strongly
linked them with communion, supplementing observations
of women’s enduring, albeit diminished, domestic special-
ization (see Gawronski & Bodenhausen, 2006, for discus-
sion of propositional and associative processing).
Two other psychological processes may also have fos-
tered women’s communal stereotype. One is that, whereas
women’s earlier domestic role may have seemed obligatory,
people likely believe that women now have substantial
choice about the occupations they pursue, thus increasing
the strength of their inference to corresponding traits (Jones
& Davis, 1965). Yet another process contributing to the
escalation of women’s communion may follow from its
being the strongest gender stereotype. Its salience may have
fostered its accentuation as it became the most conspicuous
quality differentiating women and men (see Eyal & Epley,
2017).
Even though women are overrepresented in communal
roles, occupational sex segregation did decrease, mainly in
the late 20th century (Hegewisch & Hartmann, 2014). This
shift drew women into many male-dominated occupations;
however, many of these are not particularly agentically
demanding (e.g., veterinarian, dentist; Roos & Stevens,
2018). Moreover, even within the more agentic roles that
women entered, such as lawyer and manager, internal re-
segregation tended to put women into the more communal
variants of these roles (Levanon & Grusky, 2016). In some
instances, new female ghettos emerged through the redesign
of jobs to have lower authority and reward social skills (e.g.,
bank branch manager; Skuratowicz & Hunter, 2004). For all
of these reasons, women’s increasing employment has
likely driven the stereotype of women toward gains in
communion but not agency even while yielding gains in
competence.
Our findings reveal gender stereotypes’ remarkable con-
sensuality across respondent sex, education, employment,
marital status, raceethnicity, and generation. Sex of re-
spondent did reveal some ingroup favoritism, with women,
relative to men, rating women more favorably (cf. Rudman
& Goodwin, 2004). Nevertheless, women and men gener-
92
95
96
93
94
93
94
92
95
89
92
93
94
96
91
Millennials
Gen X
Baby Boomers
Silent Gen
Hispanic
Black
White
Not Married
Married
Not Working
Working
Not College Grad
College Grad
Women
Men
0 25 50 75 100
Communion
24
31
21
30
26
30
24
31
24
39
27
30
26
25
23
Millennials
Gen X
Baby Boomers
Silent Gen
Hispanic
Black
White
Not Married
Married
Not Working
Working
Not College Grad
College Grad
Women
Men
0255075100
Agency
70
85
80
77
77
75
78
77
78
79
75
77
82
79
78
Millennials
Gen X
Baby Boomers
Silent Gen
Hispanic
Black
White
Not Married
Married
Not Working
Working
Not College Grad
College Grad
Women
Men
0 25 50 75 100
Percent Choosing Women
Competence
65
83
79
73
76
69
70
71
73
82
68
70
76
72
68
Millennials
Gen X
Baby Boomers
Silent Gen
Hispanic
Black
White
Not Married
Married
Not Working
Working
Not College Grad
College Grad
Women
Men
0255075100
Percent Choosing Women
Intelligence
Figure 4. Subgroup analysis for demographic variables. Values displayed are predictions of the simple
regression models (see Figure 1) for the year 2018 for each demographic subgroup. Values indicate the
percentages of women more responses among the women more and men more responses. The dots indicate that
participants ascribed traits more to women (red [top left, bottom left, and most of bottom right panels]) or men
(blue [top right panel]) or that their responses did not significantly favor either sex (gray [1st, 11th, 13th, and
14th dots in bottom right panel]). Error bars denote 95% confidence intervals. Coll. college. See the online
article for the color version of this figure.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
11
GENDER STEREOTYPES
ally agreed with the overall patterns on communion, agency,
and competence, as did the subgroups based on other de-
mographics. For instance, in recent polls, among those
noting a sex difference in competence, even male respon-
dents shared the belief that women are the more competent
sex.
The competence findings challenge the assumption that
women, as a lower status social group, are accorded less
competence than are men (e.g., Ridgeway, 2014). This
assumption neglects women’s educational attainments and
entry into high-prestige occupations (e.g., physician, educa-
tion administrator), which diminished men’s once-strong
advantage in occupational prestige (Lippa et al., 2014; see
also Kleinjans, Krassel, & Dukes, 2017). Occupational pres-
tige may have a greater influence on competence beliefs
than do other status indicators (income and hierarchical
power), on which women remain more disadvantaged.
Interpretations of our findings should consider that social
movements organized to lessen group disadvantage usually
challenge cultural stereotypes about the group. With femi-
nist activism, gender stereotypes thus tended to become
politically incorrect, and belief in gender equality became
more correct (Eagly, 2018). To address this potential source
of bias, the analysis of equality responses to the poll items
found a significant increase only in competence. Moreover,
the increase in female advantage in both communion and
competence remained intact when controlled for equality
responding. A related consideration is that increasing pres-
sures for political correctness may have fostered the ascrip-
tion of positive traits to women. However, the analysis of
traits’ evaluative content did not support this supposition.
Another limitation is that our project could not explore
stereotypes resulting from sex intersecting with other cate-
gories (e.g., stereotypes about women and men differing in
race and ethnicity; Ghavami & Peplau, 2013). Yet, given
that people’s observations should be somewhat weighted
toward their own social group, decomposition of the poll
data by respondents’ demographic groups yielded findings
suggestive of intersectionality (e.g., Whites, more than
Blacks, found agency more true of men than women).
Measurement limitations included the categorical form of
the items (i.e., more true of women or men, or equal), which
did not allow expression of the magnitude of perceived sex
differences. Yet, Lueptow et al.’s (2001) report of trends
from 1974 to 1997, based on respondents’ separate ratings
of men and women on 7-point scales, revealed findings very
similar to those in our data: an increase in the communion
of women relative to men and little change in agency.
Another limitation is that the polls did not include implicit
measures of stereotypes, thus limiting understanding of
whether change over time might differ between the two
types of measures (Lemm & Banaji, 1999; Rudman &
Goodwin, 2004).
Yet another limitation is that the polls did not administer
consistent sets of items. In 10 instances, a dimension was
represented by only a single item, a practice that tends to
lower measure reliability but does not necessarily compro-
mise validity (Gardner, Cummings, Dunham, & Pierce,
1998). Variation in the items representing a dimension is
tolerable given that research has demonstrated that traits are
correlated within clusters of communal, agentic, and com-
petent items (e.g., Koenig & Eagly, 2014), lending legiti-
macy to treating similar items as assessing these dimen-
sions. Nevertheless, item responses did vary within
dimensions, as illustrated by the 2018 poll, which revealed
some item variability in responses to the four competence
traits (see Figure S4 in the online supplemental materials).
To address this item inconsistency issue for competence, we
also analyzed the trend over time in the intelligence item.
The similarity of its findings to those for competence in
general suggests that the gain in women’s stereotypical
competence is robust. Moreover, this gain persisted even
after controlling for items’ evaluative content. Nevertheless,
the direction of gender stereotyping no doubt varies across
domains of competence such as verbal, quantitative, and
technical skills.
Despite the project’s large, nationally representative sam-
ples extending over seven decades, another limitation is the
unfortunate lack of poll data between 1950 and 1974, a
period of limited public interest in gender issues. However,
women’s gains relative to men’s in competence and com-
munion were not solely dependent on the possibly anoma-
lous post-World War II data but remained significant even
after the elimination of these data points. All in all, despite
some limitations, this project is the first to recognize the
profound social trend of increasing belief in the compe-
tence equality of women and men, combined with a
decided edge of women over men among those who
perceive competence inequality.
Why haven’t women’s competence gains propelled them
to the top of hierarchies? The reasons probably reside in the
lesser agency ascribed to women, given that leadership is
imbued with this quality (Koenig et al., 2011). Consider also
that the female stereotype of high competence and commu-
nion combined with low agency likely makes women suit-
able for workplace tasks that Babcock, Recalde, Vesterlund,
and Weingart (2017) have labeled low promotability. Such
tasks demand competence, but ambitious employees avoid
them (e.g., routine committee service) because they seldom
further advancement. Indeed, Babcock et al. showed that
women are preferentially chosen for such tasks and are
more likely to accept them.
Despite these concerns about promotability, women’s ad-
vantage over men in communion is likely beneficial for
employment in general. The reason is that analyses of the
labor market over time revealed that jobs increasingly re-
quire high levels of social skills (Deming, 2017). Given also
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
12 EAGLY, NATER, MILLER, KAUFMANN, AND SCZESNY
that in general jobs’ cognitive demands have increased as
their physical strength demands have decreased, women’s
gains in perceived competence should favor increased ac-
cess to employment (Rendall, 2017).
In sum, U.S. poll data show that it is only in competence
that gender equality has come to dominate people’s thinking
about women and men. For qualities of personality, the past
73 years have produced an accentuated stereotype of
women as the more communal sex, with men retaining their
agency advantage. These personality findings challenge
people’s everyday assumptions about changes in gender
stereotypes (see Diekman & Eagly, 2000). These results
also call for correction of many social scientists’ claims that
gender stereotypes are unchanging (e.g., Haines et al., 2016)
and that a group’s lower status necessarily implies a stereo-
type of lesser competence (e.g., Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu,
2002; Ridgeway, 2014). Instead, despite—or because of—
the massive flow of women into paid employment, people’s
beliefs about the sexes are predominantly of personality
difference but competence equality.
References
References marked with an asterisk indicate public opinion polls in-
cluded in the meta-analysis.
Anderson, N. H. (1968). Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9, 272–279. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/h0025907
Babcock, L., Recalde, M. P., Vesterlund, L., & Weingart, L. (2017).
Gender differences in accepting and receiving requests for tasks with
low promotability. American Economic Review, 107, 714–747. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1257/aer.20141734
Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. Reading, PA: Addison
Wesley.
Baker, M., & Cornelson, K. (2018). Gender-based occupational segregation
and sex differences in sensory, motor, and spatial aptitudes. Demography,
55, 1749–1775. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s13524-018-0706-3
Begg, C. B., & Gray, R. (1984). Calculation of polychotomous logistic
regression parameters using individualized regressions. Biometrika, 71,
11–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2336391
Bem, S. L. (1993). The lenses of gender. New Haven, CT: Yale University
Press.
Beyer, S. (2018). Low awareness of occupational segregation and the
gender pay gap: No changes over a 16-year span. Current Psychology,
37, 373–389. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s12144-016-9521-4
Bianchi, S., Lesnard, L., Nazio, T., & Raley, S. (2014). Gender and time
allocation of cohabiting and married women and men in France, Italy,
and the United States. Demographic Research, 31, 183–216. http://dx
.doi.org/10.4054/DemRes.2014.31.8
Blau, F. D., Brummund, P., & Liu, A. Y.-H. (2013). Trends in occupational
segregation by gender 1970–2009: Adjusting for the impact of changes
in the occupational coding system. Demography, 50, 471–492. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s13524-012-0151-7
Blau, F. D., & Winkler, A. E. (2018). Women, work, and family. In S. L.
Averett, L. M. Argys, & S. D. Hoffman (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of
women and the economy (pp. 395–424). New York, NY: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P., & Rothstein, R. (2009). Intro-
duction to meta-analysis. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470743386
Carli, L. L., & Eagly, A. H. (2017). Leadership and gender. In J. Antonakis
& D. Day (Eds.), The nature of leadership (3rd ed., pp. 244–271).
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
CBS News. (2015). CBS News poll [USCBS. 021515.R12]. Retrieved
from https://ropercenter.cornell.edu/CFIDE/cf/action/ipoll
Chandler, J. (2018). Likeableness and meaningfulness ratings of 555 (487)
person-descriptive words. Journal of Research in Personality, 72, 50–57.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.07.005
Chen, J. M., Banerji, I., Moons, W. G., & Sherman, J. W. (2014). Spon-
taneous social role inferences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
ogy, 55, 146–153. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.07.003
Cortes, P., & Pan, J. (2018). Occupation and gender. In S. L. Averett, L. M.
Argys, & S. D. Hoffman (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of women and the
economy (pp. 425–452). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence
as universal dimensions of social perception: The stereotype content
model and the BIAS map. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,
40, 61–149. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0065
260107000020
Deaux, K., & Major, B. (1987). Putting gender into context: An interactive
model of gender-related behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 369–389.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.94.3.369
Deming, D. J. (2017). The growing importance of social skills in the labor
market. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 132, 1593–1640. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1093/qje/qjx022
Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Stereotypes as dynamic constructs:
Women and men of the past, present, and future. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1171–1188. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
0146167200262001
Donnelly, K., Twenge, J. M., Clark, M. A., Shaikh, S. K., Beiler-May, A.,
& Carter, N. T. (2016). Attitudes toward women’s work and family roles
in the United States, 1976–2013. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 40,
41–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0361684315590774
Dumas, J. E., Johnson, M., & Lynch, A. M. (2002). Likableness, famil-
iarity, and frequency of 844 person-descriptive words. Personality and
Individual Differences, 32, 523–531. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-
8869(01)00054-X
Eagly, A. H. (2018). The shaping of science by ideology: How feminism
inspired, led, and constrained scientific understanding of sex and gender.
Journal of Social Issues, 74, 871–888. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/josi.12291
Eagly, A. H., Nater, C., Miller, D. I., & Sczesny, S. (2018). New poll on
gender stereotypes conducted by GfK: Government and academic om-
nibus panel. Open Science Framework website: https://osf.io/g98c6/
Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (2012). Social role theory. In P. van Lange, A.
Kruglanski, & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories in social
psychology (pp. 458–476). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781446249222.n49
Eccles, J. S. (1994). Understanding women’s educational and occupational
choices: Applying the Eccles et al. model of achievement-related choic-
es. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 585–609. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1111/j.1471-6402.1994.tb01049.x
Eyal, T., & Epley, N. (2017). Exaggerating accessible differences: When
gender stereotypes overestimate actual group differences. Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43, 1323–1336. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1177/0146167217713190
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often
mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow
from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 82, 878–902. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514
.82.6.878
Fox News. (2006). Opinion dynamics poll [USODFOX. 092806.R39].
Retrieved from https://ropercenter.cornell.edu/CFIDE/cf/action/ipoll
Gallup Organization. (1946). Gallup Poll #370 [USAIPO1946-0370].
Retrieved from https://ropercenter.cornell.edu/CFIDE/cf/action/ipoll
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
13
GENDER STEREOTYPES
Gallup Organization. (1950). Gallup Poll #452 [USAIPO1950-0452].
Retrieved from https://ropercenter.cornell.edu/CFIDE/cf/action/ipoll
Gallup Organization. (1989). Gallup News Service poll: The women’s
movement [USAIPOGNS1989-89141-W2]. Retrieved from https://
ropercenter.cornell.edu/CFIDE/cf/action/ipoll
Gallup Organization. (1995). Gallup/CNN/USA Today poll: Drugs
[USAIPOCNUS1995-9508011]. Retrieved from https://ropercenter
.cornell.edu/CFIDE/cf/action/ipoll
Gallup Organization. (2000). December Wave 1 [USAIPOGNS2000-53].
Retrieved from https://ropercenter.cornell.edu/CFIDE/cf/action/ipoll
Gallup Organization. (2019). How does the Gallup U.S. poll work? Re-
trieved from https://www.gallup.com/224855/gallup-poll-work
.aspx?g_sourcelink_wwwv9&g_campaignitem_178685&g_medium
copy
Gardner, D. G., Cummings, L. L., Dunham, R. B., & Pierce, J. L. (1998).
Single-item versus multiple-item measurement scales: An empirical
comparison. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 58, 898
915. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0013164498058006003
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and proposi-
tional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and
explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692–731. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692
Gerson, K. (2017). The logics of work, care and gender change in the new
economy: A view from the U.S. In B. Brandth, S. Halrynjo, & E. Kvande
(Eds.), Work–family dynamics: Competing logics of regulation, econ-
omy and morals (pp. 19–35). New York, NY: Routledge. http://dx.doi
.org/10.4324/9781315716794-2
Ghavami, N., & Peplau, L. A. (2013). An intersectional analysis of gender
and ethnic stereotypes: Testing three hypotheses. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 37, 113–127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0361684312464203
Goldin, C., & Olivetti, C. (2013). Shocking labor supply: A reassessment of
the role of World War II on women’s labor supply. American Economic
Review, 103, 257–262. http://dx.doi.org/10.1257/aer.103.3.257
Haines, E. L., Deaux, K., & Lofaro, N. (2016). The times they are
a-changing...orarethey not? A comparison of gender stereotypes,
1983–2014. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 40, 353–363. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1177/0361684316634081
Hegewisch, A., & Hartmann, H. (2014). Occupational segregation and the
gender wage gap: A job half done. Washington, DC: Institute for
Women’s Policy Research. Retrieved from https://iwpr.org/publications/
occupational-segregation-and-the-gender-wage-gap-a-job-half-done/
Henry, P. (2008). College sophomores in the laboratory redux: Influences
of a narrow data base on social psychology’s view of the nature of
prejudice. Psychological Inquiry, 19, 49–71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
10478400802049936
Jones, E. E., & Davis, K. E. (1965). From acts to dispositions: The
attribution process in person perception. Advances in Experimental
Social Psychology, 2, 219–266. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601
(08)60107-0
Kleinjans, K. J., Krassel, K. F., & Dukes, A. (2017). Occupational prestige and
the gender wage gap. Kyklos, 70, 565–593. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/kykl
.12149
Koenig, A. M., & Eagly, A. H. (2014). Evidence for the social role theory
of stereotype content: Observations of groups’ roles shape stereotypes.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 371–392. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/a0037215
Koenig, A. M., Eagly, A. H., Mitchell, A. A., & Ristikari, T. (2011). Are
leader stereotypes masculine? A meta-analysis of three research para-
digms. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 616–642. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
a0023557
Krupnikov, Y., Piston, S., & Bauer, N. M. (2016). Saving face: Identifying
voter responses to Black candidates and female candidates. Political
Psychology, 37, 253–273. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/pops.12261
Lemm, K., & Banaji, M. R. (1999). Unconscious attitudes and beliefs about
women and men. In U. Pasero & F. Braun (Eds.), Wahrnehmung und
Herstellung von Geschlecht [Perceiving and performing gender] (pp.
215–235). Wiesbaden, Germany: VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-322-89014-6_18
Levanon, A., & Grusky, D. B. (2016). The persistence of extreme gender
segregation in the twenty-first century. American Journal of Sociology,
122, 573–619. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/688628
Lippa, R. A., Preston, K., & Penner, J. (2014). Women’s representation in
60 occupations from 1972 to 2010: More women in high-status jobs, few
women in things-oriented jobs. PLoS ONE, 9(5), e95960. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0095960
Lueptow, L. B., Garovich-Szabo, L., & Lueptow, M. B. (2001). Social
change and the persistence of sex typing: 1974–1997. Social Forces, 80,
1–36. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/sof.2001.0077
Lumley, T. (2017). Survey: Analysis of complex survey samples (Version
3.32) [Computer software]. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/
web/packages/survey/index.html
Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2010). Patterns of gender development.
Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 353–381. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/
annurev.psych.093008.100511
McNeish, D., Stapleton, L. M., & Silverman, R. D. (2017). On the
unnecessary ubiquity of hierarchical linear modeling. Psychological
Methods, 22, 114–140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/met0000078
New York Times. (1983). New York Times poll: Women’s survey
[USNYT1983-WOMEN]. Retrieved from https://ropercenter.cornell
.edu/CFIDE/cf/action/ipoll
Okahana, H., & Zhou, E. (2018). Graduate enrollment and degrees: 2007
to 2017. Washington, DC: Council of Graduate Schools.
Ortiz-Ospina, E., & Tzvetkova, S. (2017). Working women: Key facts and
trends in female labor force participation. Retrieved from https://
ourworldindata.org/female-labor-force-participation-key-facts
Pew Research Center. (2008). Pew social trends: Gender. Retrieved from
www.pewsocialtrends.org/2009/10/23/gender-data
Pew Research Center. (2015). Gender and leadership omnibus. Retrieved from
www.pewsocialtrends.org/2016/05/25/gender-and-leadership-omnibus
Pew Research Center. (2017). On gender differences, no consensus on
naturevs. nurture. Retrieved from http://www.pewsocialtrends.org/2017/
12/05/on-gender-differences-no-consensus-on-nature-vs-nurture/
Prentice, D. A., & Carranza, E. (2002). What women and men should be,
shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: The contents of
prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26,
269–281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1471-6402.t01-1-00066
Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (2006). Essentializing differences between
women and men. Psychological Science, 17, 129–135. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01675.x
Rangel, U., & Keller, J. (2011). Essentialism goes social: Belief in social
determinism as a component of psychological essentialism. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 1056–1078. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1037/a0022401
Rendall, M. (2017). Brain versus brawn: The realization of women’s
comparative advantage (SSRN Scholarly Paper No. ID 1635251). Re-
trieved from https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract1635251
Ridgeway, C. L. (2011). Framed by gender: How gender inequality per-
sists in the modern world. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199755776.001.0001
Ridgeway, C. L. (2014). Why status matters for inequality. American
Sociological Review, 79, 1–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/000312241
3515997
Roos, P., & Stevens, L. (2018). Integrating occupations: Changing occu-
pational sex segregation in the U.S. from 2000 to 2014. Demographic
Research, 38, 127–154. http://dx.doi.org/10.4054/DemRes.2018.38.5
Roper Organization. (1946). Men and women [USRFOR1946-054]. Re-
trieved from https://ropercenter.cornell.edu/CFIDE/cf/action/ipoll
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
14 EAGLY, NATER, MILLER, KAUFMANN, AND SCZESNY
Roper Organization. (1977). Roper Reports #77-2 [USRPRR1977-02].
Retrieved from https://ropercenter.cornell.edu/CFIDE/cf/action/ipoll
Roper Organization. (1985). Roper Reports #85-9 [USRPRR1985-09].
Retrieved from https://ropercenter.cornell.edu/CFIDE/cf/action/ipoll
Rucker, D. D., Galinsky, A. D., & Magee, J. C. (2018). The agentic–
communal model of advantage and disadvantage: How inequality pro-
duces similarities in the psychology of power, social class, gender, and
race. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 58, 71–125. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2018.04.001
Rudman, L. A., & Goodwin, S. A. (2004). Gender differences in automatic
in-group bias: Why do women like women more than men like men?
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 494–509. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.4.494
Sczesny, S., Nater, C., & Eagly, A. H. (2019). Agency and communion:
Their implications for gender stereotypes and gender identities. In A.
Abele & B. Wojciszke (Eds.), Agency and communion in social psy-
chology (pp. 103–116). New York, NY: Routledge.
Skuratowicz, E., & Hunter, L. W. (2004). Where do women’s jobs come
from? Job resegregation in an American bank. Work and Occupations,
31, 73–110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0730888403259779
Spence, J. T. (1993). Gender-related traits and gender ideology: Evidence
for a multifactorial theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 64, 624–635. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.4.624
Tanner-Smith, E. E., Tipton, E., & Polanin, J. R. (2016). Handling complex
meta-analytic data structures using robust variance estimates: A tutorial
in R. Journal of Developmental and Life-Course Criminology, 2, 85–
112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s40865-016-0026-5
Tipton, E. (2015). Small sample adjustments for robust variance estimation
with meta-regression. Psychological Methods, 20, 375–393. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1037/met0000011
U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2017). Women in the labor force: A
databook. Retrieved from https://www.bls.gov
U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2018a). American time use survey—2017
results. Retrieved from https://www.bls.gov/news.release/pdf/atus.pdf
U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2018b). TED: The economics daily:
Labor force participation rates by age and sex, seasonally adjusted.
Retrieved from https://www.bls.gov
U.S. News and World Report. (1987). Attitudes poll [USRSPUSN1987-
744-063]. Retrieved from https://ropercenter.cornell.edu/CFIDE/cf/ac
tion/ipoll
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor
package. Journal of Statistical Software, 36, 1–48. http://dx.doi.org/10
.18637/jss.v036.i03
Virginia Slims. (1974). The 1974 Virginia Slims American Women’s Opinion
Poll (Virginia Slims Poll #1974-0547) [USRSPVASLIMS1974-0547]. Re-
trieved from https://ropercenter.cornell.edu/CFIDE/cf/action/ipoll
Williams, J. E., & Best, D. L. (1990). Measuring sex stereotypes: A
multination study (Rev. ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Received October 18, 2018
Revision received April 26, 2019
Accepted May 2, 2019
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
15
GENDER STEREOTYPES
... Similarly, the mere presence of female in the leadership role helps in anticipation of the team members of the fairer treatment in the organization and greater projected salary and status. In the same way, Eagly et al. (2020)093 adults in a meta-analysis collected opinions including 16 different representative polls consisting more than 30,000 adults from 1946 to 2018. The findings showed that the competence stereotypes went through sea change over time. ...
Article
Though female surpass men both in quantity and quality in academic world as a whole, many of them evade themselves from workforce participation because of various causes. Further, those who are in workforce are very rare in top roles and are massively in subsidiary roles. In addition, women have to overcome a variety of hurdles when in leadership roles and being an aspirant for it. Based on the above background, this review was started aiming to access the challenges women face ascending the leadership positions. The gap in leadership roles and the future prospects of women to occupy the roles made this study a necessity. Two major academic and scientific journals as ResearchGate and Google Scholar have been selected. Twenty different review articles were selected for analysis and thematized into five heads namely female in politics, female in education, need of female leaders in education, barriers and biases to female leadership and strengthening female leadership through inclusion. Even though women are doing well in education and politics, developing networking and leadership skills, they are still blocked to achieve leadership roles. Persistent barriers exist for women to achieve the position which ranges from personal, organizational, and cultural as well. These insights have implications for the future personnel management especially in leadership positions in the educational institutions in the scenario of teaching being the job of less attraction for males.
... Gender biases and structural barriers can impact female teachers' participation in professional development (Opfer & Pedder, 2011;Ruales et al., 2021). These barriers might include time constraints, lack of institutional support, or competing responsibilities, which can limit their opportunities to engage deeply with transformative learning practices (Eagly et al., 2020). Moreover, the data indicates that while some female teachers report high familiarity with transformative learning, this is not as prevalent as among their male counterparts. ...
Article
Full-text available
The lack of teachers' understanding of transformative learning principles poses a major challenge in fostering innovative, student-centered learning. This study aims to compare teachers' levels of understanding of transformative learning principles across specific categories and examine the correlation between their understanding and teaching practices. The study employs a quantitative approach with exploratory descriptive and correlational methods. The sample consisted of 103 junior and senior high school teachers, randomly selected, comprising 50 females and 53 males. Data were collected through an online survey using a structured questionnaire with 46 questions. Data analysis was conducted using descriptive and inferential statistical techniques. The results revealed that the majority of teachers demonstrated low to moderate levels of understanding of transformative learning principles. Teachers with greater understanding tended to implement transformative approaches more frequently in their teaching practices. However, no significant differences were found based on gender, age group, or teaching experience. The study concludes that intensive and continuous professional development programs are needed to enhance teachers' understanding and skills in applying transformative learning. These programs should include conceptual training, reflective practices, and collaborative support to facilitate classroom implementation.
... Empirical evidence on dynamic stereotypes seems to con rm both that stereotypes about women and men match the traits perceived as necessary for male-and female-dominated occupations (e.g., Cejka & Eagly, 1999) and that stereotypes evolve in response to a change in gender di erences in occupational roles (e.g., Diekman & Eagly, 2000;Twenge, 2001). A recent meta-analysis of 16 U.S. public opinion polls conducted over 73 years (Eagly et al., 2020) found an accentuation in the stereotype of women as being more communal. is shi is a ributed to an increased observation of women in roles emphasizing social skills and contribution. ...
Article
Full-text available
Exposing students to professionals working in male- and female-dominated sectors: Effects of an online experiment on Italian high-school students
Article
Conventional wisdom holds that women’s political underrepresentation partly stems from gendered stereotypes, with women candidates perceived as lower in ability and assertiveness, and as less competent to handle key issues like the economy and national security. However, recent research uncovers how societal leadership stereotypes have become less advantageous for men. Two conjoint experiments show that Americans’ stereotypes about political candidates follow similar trends: although women candidates (following conventional expectations) are perceived as friendlier and more moral than men, they are also seen as higher in ability and as equally assertive. Similarly, men and women candidates are perceived as equally competent to handle the economy, crime, and national security. Further analyses reveal that liberals and individuals low in hostile sexism hold stereotypes most favorable to women. These findings suggest that gendered candidate stereotypes likely constitute less of a hindrance to women seeking political nominations than in the past.
Article
Informal job search—the use of personal and professional networks for job search—is a popular job search method. Yet, answers to the questions of whether informal job search is effective (relative to formal job search) and why have not been clearly articulated, hindering research progress and limiting practical recommendations for job seekers and institutions. We endeavored to address these questions via an integrative, interdisciplinary review of how job search methods (i.e., formal vs. informal) and forms of social capital (i.e., types of contacts and tie strength) relate to job search outcomes (i.e., finding a job vs. finding quality employment), and we summarize evidence for the role of job seeker characteristics as key contingencies on this process. In doing so, we uncover “a tale of two job searches,” wherein informal job search effectiveness is dependent upon job seeker characteristics that impart status within the labor market and/or society. Collectively, our review provides much‐needed clarity regarding whether an informal job search is better than a formal job search and why , revealing that answers to these questions depend on who is searching for a job. Considering these insights, we outline an agenda for future research focused on enhancing job seekers' access to social networks and integrating job search and social network perspectives to extend knowledge of how different types of job seekers can more effectively utilize their networks for job search.
Article
Full-text available
Feminisme telah menjadi fenomena yang dikenal dalam masyarakat luas sejak lama. Fenomena ini tidak hanya terbatas pada masyarakat umum, akan tetapi isu feminisme juga hadir dalam industri perfilman Indonesia. Penelitian ini bertujuan untuk mengetahui bagaimana perempuan Jawa perdesaan direpresentasikan dalam film pendek yang berjudul “Tilik” (2020). Film pendek “Tilik” (2020) mengangkat feminisme yang berkembang di Indonesia, khususnya di daerah Jawa perdesaan. Penelitian ini menggunakan pendekatan kualitatif deskriptif dengan menerapkan metode semiotika televisi yang dikembangkan oleh John Fiske, yang terdiri dari tiga level analisis, yaitu level realitas, level representasi, dan level ideologi. Urgensi penelitian ini berkaitan dengan pemahaman mengenai bentuk feminisme yang dapat ditemui pada perempuan Jawa perdesaan. Adapun novelty dalam penelitian ini terletak pada titik fokus data yang dianalisis secara menyeluruh pada tiap tokoh perempuan. Hasil penelitian yang diperoleh penulis yakni berjumlah tujuh temuan data dan diklasifikasikan dalam empat kategori feminisme, yaitu feminisme mendukung perempuan aktif berkampanye, feminisme mendukung perempuan sebagai pemimpin, feminisme mendukung kebebasan berekspresi perempuan, dan feminisme mendorong solidaritas antara perempuan.
Article
Job advertisements for jobs in male-dominated fields tend to contain more masculine language, and a commonly proposed intervention to increase gender diversity in applicant pools is to remove this language. In our research, we offer predictions about the broader impact of such interventions on individuals who may not “fit” with traditional masculine identity. Across four multimethod studies ( N = 37,920) spanning both field and lab settings, we demonstrate that a gender debiasing intervention that replaces masculine language in job postings with synonymous gender-neutral language increases application rates among women and men whose self-identities are less aligned with masculinity. Our research highlights that conceptualizing identity incongruence along a continuum can help explain when and why gender diversity initiatives foster the inclusion of a broader group of individuals than initially anticipated.
Article
People often come to work sick – a prevalent behaviour even during public health crises. Integrating trait activation and social role theories, we examine how organisational sickness preventative practices affect workplace anxiety, approach and avoidance motivation for presenteeism, in turn, contributing to male–female differences in presenteeism. We conducted a time-separated study. The results ( N = 86 working adults who completed all four waves of the data) supported our moderated mediation model in that organisational sickness preventative practices were beneficial for reducing avoidance motivation to engage in presenteeism for women, but less so for men. These practices were similarly beneficial for reducing workplace anxiety for women, which explained their lower presenteeism compared to men. We performed a cross-sectional replication using only employees who worked in the traditional office at Time 3 ( N = 56) and obtained similar findings. Our results offer steps on how to manage presenteeism. JEL Classification: M12, M14
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter shows that agency and communion are essential to understanding the psychology of gender. Gender stereotypes, in their descriptive and prescriptive forms, follow from a societal division of labor whereby women tend to be concentrated in communally demanding roles and men in agentically demanding roles. People’s inferences of communal and agentic traits underlying the typical role behaviors of women and men yield gender stereotypes. These stereotypes in turn can enhance or compromise the ability of women and men to occupy and succeed in social roles that demand agentic or communal qualities. To the extent that people internalize stereotypes pertaining to their gender, they gain gender identities by which women regard themselves as especially communal and men as especially agentic. In addition, these identities and related personal goals regulate the attraction of each sex to social roles that afford opportunities to meet communal or agentic goals.
Article
Full-text available
Stereotype research emphasizes systematic processes over seemingly arbitrary contents, but content also may prove systematic. On the basis of stereotypes' intergroup functions, the stereotype content model hypothesizes that (a) 2 primary dimensions are competence and warmth, (b) frequent mixed clusters combine high warmth with low competence (paternalistic) or high competence with low warmth (envious), and (c) distinct emotions (pity, envy, admiration, contempt) differentiate the 4 competence-warmth combinations. Stereotypically, (d) status predicts high competence, and competition predicts low warmth. Nine varied samples rated gender, ethnicity, race, class, age, and disability out-groups. Contrary to antipathy models, 2 dimensions mattered, and many stereotypes were mixed, either pitying (low competence, high warmth subordinates) or envying (high competence, low warmth competitors). Stereotypically, status predicted competence, and competition predicted low warmth.
Article
Full-text available
BACKGROUND Declining occupational sex segregation in the late 20th century helped to usher in unprecedented occupational and economic advancement for women. As the 21st century dawned, that advancement stalled. OBJECTIVE We examine how occupational integration occurred in the early decades of the 21st century by focusing on (1) the extent of occupational feminization and masculinization and (2) occupational succession. More broadly we examine how the representation of women in detailed occupational categories changed between 2000 and 2014, regardless of whether they were historically 'male' or 'female,' and how sociodemographic characteristics contributed to uneven shifts in occupational integration. METHODS We use Integrated Public Use Microdata Series data to estimate the percentage point female at the detailed occupation level, specifically the 5% census microdata sample for 2000, and two 1% American Community Survey (ACS) samples for 2013 and 2014. RESULTS Despite a stall in overall integration, there was much fluctuation within detailed occupations. Moreover, occupational inroads have been uneven in the post-2000 period. Women gained entry into the same types of professional and managerial occupations they entered between 1970 and 2000, especially in the health professions. Men increased their representation in lower-level, nonprofessional occupations.
Article
When the Second Wave of feminism emerged in the 1960s and 1970s, feminist psychologists vigorously attacked earlier scholarship on gender for assuming that women's intrinsic nature accounts for their deficits of power and status. Consistent with the liberal, progressive ideology that has prevailed among feminist psychologists, most of their research on the psychology of gender instead found the causes of women's disadvantage in the social context of women's lives. In particular, social psychologists championed this perspective by providing evidence of the influence of gender stereotypes, social norms, organizational barriers, and sexist prejudices on the behavior of women and men. This approach de‐emphasized other causes of women's disadvantage, notably the self‐construals and personal goals that guide individual choices. In addition, many feminist psychologists rejected biological causation as sexist and reductionist, thereby participating minimally in research on interactive processes by which nature and nurture work together in producing sex differences and similarities. The challenge now facing feminist psychologists is to reach beyond ideological constraints to discover how social, self, and biological causes interact to produce the phenomena of gender.
Article
Research on sex differences in humans documents gender differences in sensory, motor, and spatial aptitudes. These aptitudes, as captured by Dictionary of Occupational Titles (DOT) codes, predict the occupational choices of men and women in the directions indicated by this research. We simulate that eliminating selection on these skills reduces the Duncan index of gender-based occupational segregation by 20 % to 23 % in 1970 and 2012, respectively. Eliminating selection on DOT variables capturing other accounts of this segregation has a smaller impact.
Chapter
This integrative review presents the Agentic–Communal Model of Advantage and Disadvantage to offer insight into the psychology of inequality. This model examines the relation between individuals’ position of advantage or disadvantage in a social hierarchy and their propensity toward agency and communion. We begin by identifying and reviewing four inequalities—Resources, Opportunities, Appraisals, and Deference, or the ROAD of inequality—that are fundamental to social advantage and disadvantage. We explain how these inequalities can instill a sense of advantage and disadvantage in individuals. Next, we discuss two core drivers of human behavior: agency and communion. We integrate these literatures to introduce the model's central propositions: a sense of advantage orients individuals toward agency and a sense of disadvantage orients individuals toward communion. We review evidence for this model across four distinct social hierarchies: power, social class, gender, and race. A number of findings suggest that higher-power individuals, higher-class individuals, men, and Whites express greater agency, whereas lower-power individuals, lower-class individuals, women, and minorities express greater communion. We also consider results in the literature that appear inconsistent with our propositions (i.e., when the advantaged are communal and the disadvantaged are agentic) and offer theoretical integrations to resolve these apparent contradictions. In particular, we highlight how the orthogonal nature of agency and communion can produce behavior that results from the combination of high agency and communion. To help motivate a future research agenda, we note the importance of both hierarchy salience and cultural considerations in determining individuals’ orientations toward agency and communion. Finally, we consider the implications of this model for the study of social hierarchy and inequality, as well as the consequences of rising inequality levels.
Article
The labor market increasingly rewards social skills. Between 1980 and 2012, jobs requiring high levels of social interaction grew by nearly 12 percentage points as a share of the U.S. labor force. Math-intensive but less social jobs-including many STEM occupations-shrank by 3.3 percentage points over the same period. Employment and wage growth were particularly strong for jobs requiring high levels of both math skill and social skills. To understand these patterns, I develop a model of team production where workers "trade tasks" to exploit their comparative advantage. In the model, social skills reduce coordination costs, allowing workers to specialize and work together more efficiently. The model generates predictions about sorting and the relative returns to skill across occupations, which I investigate using data from the NLSY79 and the NLSY97. Using a comparable set of skill measures and covariates across survey waves, I find that the labor market return to social skills was much greater in the 2000s than in the mid-1980s and 1990s. © The Author(s) 2017. Published by Oxford University Press, on behalf of President and Fellows of Harvard College. All rights reserved.
Technical Report
The Council of Graduate Schools (CGS) reported continued growth in total graduate enrollment, first-time enrollments, number of applications, and degrees conferred at U.S. universities in its report, CGS/GRE Graduate Enrollment & Degrees: 2006-2016.