ArticlePDF Available

Abstract

Breaking from the typical focus on the antecedents of creativity, we investigate the psychological and interpersonal consequences of being creative. Across five experiments, we find that generating creative ideas is revealing of the self and thus prompts the perception of self-disclosure. Individuals respond to the expectation to be creative with greater self-focus—adopting their own idiosyncratic perspective on the task and thinking about their own personal preferences and experiences in connection to the problem. Because creative ideas derived from self-focused attention are uniquely personal, the act of sharing a creative idea is, in turn, perceived to be revealing of the self. Finally, an interactive dyad study shows that sharing creative ideas makes partners more confident in the accuracy of judgments they made about each other’s personality. We discuss the implications of our findings for future research investigating the consequences of creativity.
1
Running head: CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Your Soul Spills Out:
The Creative Act Feels Self-Disclosing
Jack A. Goncalo & Joshua H. Katz
University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign
FORTHCOMING, PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY BULLETIN
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jack A. Goncalo,
goncalo@illinois.edu
2
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Abstract
Breaking from the typical focus on the antecedents of creativity, we investigate the psychological
and interpersonal consequences of being creative. Across five experiments, we find that
generating creative ideas is revealing of the self and thus prompts the perception of self-
disclosure. Individuals respond to the expectation to be creative with greater self-focus—
adopting their own idiosyncratic perspective on the task and thinking about their own personal
preferences and experiences in connection to the problem. Because creative ideas derived from
self-focused attention are uniquely personal, the act of sharing a creative idea is, in turn,
perceived to be revealing of the self. Finally, an interactive dyad study shows that sharing
creative ideas makes partners more confident in the accuracy of judgments they made about each
other’s personality. We discuss the implications of our findings for future research investigating
the consequences of creativity.
Keywords: Creativity; Idea Sharing; Self-Disclosure; Person Perception
3
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Your Soul Spills Out:
The Creative Act Feels Self-Disclosing
“Paintings have a life of their own that derives from the painter’s soul.” Vincent Van Gogh
Creativity, from the development of new technology to breakthroughs in science and
commerce, is a process that involves not only thinking of, but also openly expressing novel and
appropriate ideas (Paulus & Nijstad, 2003; Sawyer & DeZutter, 2009; Uzzi & Spiro, 2005).
Though individuals might privately hold highly creative solutions, those breakthrough ideas
might go unexpressed because people fear rejection and they are hesitant to share ideas that
might generate controversy or criticism (Camacho & Paulus, 1995; Diehl & Stroebe, 1987). For
this reason, a large and growing stream of research over the last three decades has addressed the
question of how we can encourage individuals to freely share their creative ideas (Paulus,
Dzindolet & Kohn, 2012; Reiter-Palmon, Wigert & de Vreede, 2012).
While creative output is clearly important, this singular focus on the variables that boost
creativity might come at the cost of exploring the psychological and interpersonal consequences
of sharing creative ideas (Goncalo, Vincent & Krause, 2015; Sassenberg & Moskowitz, 2005;
Wronska, Kolańczyk & Nijstad, 2018; Khessina, Goncalo & Krause, 2018). Rather than focusing
on the antecedents of creative ideation, we turn instead to consider an important but as yet
unexamined consequence of idea sharing. Specifically, we suggest the act of sharing creative
ideas is revealing of the self and thus leads to the perception of self-disclosure. This novel
connection between creativity and self-disclosure has numerous theoretical and practical
implications for research in clinical (Henry & Strupp, 1994), social (Collins & Miller, 1994; Reis
& Shaver, 1988) and organizational psychology (Phillips, Rothbard & Dumas, 2009; Polzer,
4
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Milton & Swann, 2002), because self-disclosure elicited in the process of creative ideation might
provide the foundation for bonding, intimacy, and social connectedness (Collins & Miller, 1994;
Laurenceau, Barrett & Pietromonaco, 1998; Reis & Shaver, 1988).
Risky Business: Creativity and Self-Disclosure
Self-disclosure is defined as personal information verbally communicated to another
individual (Chelune, 1979; Cozby, 1973; Wheeless & Grotz, 1976). In prior research, self-
disclosure has been limited to verbal as opposed to non-verbal interaction that can include both
oral and written communication (Omarzu, 2000). Individuals may choose to disclose a wide
range of personal information during an encounter such as descriptive information about
themselves (e.g. I was born in the U.S.), evaluative information about their attitudes, values or
beliefs (e.g. I favor gun control), or affective information about their moods and emotions (e.g.
I’m ashamed) (Morton, 1978).
This wide array of information can also vary along several important dimensions (Cozby,
1973). Breadth of self-disclosure refers to the number of different domains covered when an
individual shares personal information (Jones & Archer, 1976). Duration of self-disclosure is the
sheer amount or persistence and time spent sharing personal information (Chelune, 1979). And,
depth of self-disclosure describes the intimacy level of the personal information shared (Altman
& Taylor, 1973), where intimacy means that the information shared is emotionally intense or
potentially embarrassing (Howell & Conway, 1990).
Sharing personal information is interpersonally risky and might lead to rejection. Baxter
and Montgomery (1996) outlined several potential risks to self-disclosure. An individual who
discloses personal information could be rejected by the listener and rejection could cause hurt
and embarrassment. Sharing personal information can also limit one’s autonomy and personal
5
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
integrity. Once personal information is shared, it cannot be retracted, which might also contribute
to a loss of control over potentially sensitive information that might make a person feel
vulnerable. Finally, a failed attempt at self-disclosure stemming from the inability to articulate
one’s thoughts clearly could also lead to a distorted impression that could persist and impact
relationships over time (Kelly & McKillop, 1996). For these reasons, self-disclosure is more
likely to occur when individuals feel anonymous or liberated from social desirability concerns
(McKenna & Bargh, 2000; Walther, 1996).
Balanced against the risks of self-disclosure are potential rewards such as increased
intimacy and liking between partners (Collins & Miller, 1994; Laurenceau, Barrett &
Pietromonaco, 1998). The gradual disclosure of personal information can draw a desired partner
into a closer relationship (Reis & Shaver, 1988; Birnbaum et al, 2017). Taking this tradeoff into
account, self-disclosure has been modeled as a decision-making process in which people weigh
both the risks and potential rewards of disclosure before choosing whether and how much to
disclose and to whom (Omarzu, 2000).
Aside from the objective amount of type of information disclosed, individuals can vary in
their perceptions of having disclosed personal information. For example, partners can have
different perceptions of how intimate or revealing particular information is (Wheeless & Grotz,
1976). Prior research has also shown that men and women can have different perceptions of how
much each partner has disclosed. These subjective perceptions of self-disclosure are
consequential because, for example, when self-disclosure patterns uphold established gender
roles (e.g. women disclose more than men), both partners report greater relationship satisfaction
(Millar & Millar, 1988). Thus, it is not simply objective information about what has been
disclosed that matters, but also to what extent people feel as though they have disclosed.
6
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Like self-disclosure, the decision to share a creative idea is also perceived to be an
interpersonally risky act that exposes an individual to the possibility of rejection. Creative ideas
are novel, untested, and unproven, making them likely to be rejected, at least initially (Mueller,
Melwani & Goncalo, 2012). Because individuals fear rejection (Baumeister & Leary, 1995;
DeWall & Bushman, 2011), they tend to withhold rather than express their most creative ideas
and instead suggest only their most conservative and uncontroversial ideas to avoid criticism
(Camacho & Paulus, 1995; Diehl & Stroebe, 1987).
Why is evaluation apprehension in the creative process so deeply rooted? Existing
research on how to reduce evaluation apprehension, such as making people feel anonymous
(Connolly, Jessup, Valacich, 1990), hints at an intriguing possibility. Creative ideas may not
merely be potential solutions—abstract possibilities that may or may not survive an impersonal
selection process (Rietzschel, Nijstad & Stroebe, 2006). Rather, a creative idea might offer a
window into one’s own idiosyncratic point of view of the world, making the act of sharing a
creative idea one that lays bare the self. Though no direct evidence of this link exists, we
investigate the possibility that sharing creative ideas produces the perception of self-disclosure.
Creativity Ideation and Self-Focus: A Pathway to Perceived Self-Disclosure
The creative process can be quasi-random as available knowledge is dispassionately
combined to yield original combinations through blind variation and selective retention
(Campbell, 1960; Staw, 1990; Simonton, 1999; Simonton, 2003). There are a number of inputs
into the creative process, including domain relevant knowledge, expertise, and the ability to
combine seemingly disparate pieces of information in a novel and appropriate way (Amabile,
1996). Likewise, in the process of brainstorming, the goal is to build upon, combine, and
improve on the ideas suggested by others to reach a solution that is better than any one individual
7
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
could have come up with alone (Paulus & Yang, 2000). From this influential perspective, ideas
are merely inputs that are combined through an iterative process.
Yet, for human problem solvers, the creative process might be more personal. There is a
great deal of anecdotal evidence to suggest that creative people pour themselves into their work.
For example, when Jack Dorsey listened to the short messages dispatched by taxi drivers he was
inspired to create Twitter (Fast Company Staff, 2015). Hungarian entrepreneur, Adam Somlai-
Fischer wanted to share his own media art with the world so he developed presentation software
that became Prezi (Coleman, 2014). Both of these creative ideas resulted from a unique
combination of elements drawn from personal experience, consistent with Richard Branson’s
view that, “The ideas for the best businesses tend to come from personal experience” (Branson,
2015).
These anecdotes are suggestive and revealing because they imply creative work can be
personally meaningful (Amabile & Pratt, 2016). In the creative process, individuals have the
latitude to decide which problems to pursue and which ideas to share or withhold based on
whether they believe the problem is significant to them (Lepisto & Pratt, 2017). In order to
generate creative ideas, individuals draw on and integrate their unique knowledge and experience
to share solutions that reflect their own original point of view (Cheng, Sanchez-Burks & Lee,
2008; Leung, Maddux, Galinsky & Chiu, 2008). In turn, that uniquely personal perspective
differentiates one’s ideas from those suggested by others (Beersma & De Dreu, 2005; Goncalo &
Staw, 2006; Kim, Vincent & Goncalo, 2012; Zitek & Vincent, 2015). Rather than being
impersonal, creative expression is intertwined with the self. When prompted to be creative,
people may reach into the aspects of the self that make them unique (Dollinger, 2003). In doing
so, they might not only express creative ideas (the focus of much prior research), but the creative
8
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
ideas they share might feel personally revealing, thus prompting the perception of self-disclosure
(the focus of our research).
The Present Research: The Creative Self-Disclosure Effect
In investigating the link between creativity and self-disclosure, this paper makes several
important contributions. First, we break from the typical focus on the antecedents of creativity to
investigate the interpersonal consequences of being creative. Second, we show for the first time
that being creative feels self-disclosing—an insight that might provide a theoretical framework
guiding future investigation into the downstream consequences of creativity. Third, we show that
when people share creative ideas with a partner, they not only perceive they have personally
disclosed, but they also feel as though their partner has disclosed to them. Perceived self-
disclosure, in turn, can impact how interaction partners perceive each-other. Taken together, the
present studies represent a new direction in research integrating creativity, person perception,
and social cognition.
Overview of Studies
Across five experiments we test our hypothesis that generating creative ideas will prompt
the perception of self-disclosure. We first show that generating creative ideas causes individuals
to report stronger perceived self-disclosure compared to those who were asked to generate
uncreative ideas (Experiments 1-2); a process that is mediated by a focus on the self (Experiment
3). Next, we identified and tested an important boundary condition on creative self-disclosure—
restricting problem solvers to one particular idea category significantly attenuates the perception
of self-disclosure (Experiment 4). Finally, we replicate and extend these findings in the context
of a face-to-face interaction between partners who share their ideas with each other (Experiment
5). For each experiment, all measures, manipulations and exclusions in the experiment are
9
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
disclosed. No analyses were conducted until data collection was complete. All experiments were
run with IRB approval and conformed to APA ethical standards.
Experiment 1
Participants and procedure. Given there is no prior research linking creativity to self-
disclosure, we decided on a large sample size of 100 per cell, which would give 80% power to
detect any effect of .4 SD or above. We recruited 202 Participants (37% male, Mage = 21) from a
large public university who were randomly assigned to one of two idea generation conditions
(creative vs uncreative). Participants read the following prompt with the wording in parentheses
varied by condition (creative vs uncreative): “This next section is a task about product
development. We are looking for some ideas for (new/word omitted) flavors of potato chips that
are (creative, novel, and unique/ generic, conventional, and typical). We want these flavors to be
as (creative, novel, and unique/ generic, conventional, and typical) as possible. Please take the
next five minutes to write as many (creative, novel, and unique/generic, conventional, and
typical) ideas as possible for (new/word omitted) potato chip flavors.” We intentionally selected
a mundane and widely used household product as the brainstorming prompt to provide a
conservative test of our prediction. Discussing potato chip flavors should not be inherently
revealing, given Americans eat about 1.85 billion pounds of potato chips, or about 6.6 pounds
per person annually (Atwood, 2017). We also chose to compare the creative condition to an
uncreative condition, rather than a control condition with no instructions, because brainstorming
is so frequently used as a tool for creativity across so many different contexts, simply asking
participants to brainstorm, even without instructions, would likely cue the expectation to be
creative (Paulus & Yang, 2000). After participants finished brainstorming, they were asked to
complete a short survey.
10
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Measure
Self-reported creativity of ideas. As a manipulation check, participants responded to a four-
item (7-point Likert response) scale about the creativity of their ideas with items as follows: “My
ideas were creative”, “My ideas were novel”, “My ideas were unusual”, “I generated a wide
range of different ideas”. The scale was reliable (α = .87), so the items were averaged together.
Perceived self-disclosure. Existing measures of self-disclosure fall into one of three categories:
(1) self-report inventories and self ratings, (2) observer or recipient ratings and (3) objective
metrics (Chelune, 1979). Because our interest in these experiments was on the subjective
judgment of the idea sharer that he or she disclosed personal information, we opted to use a self-
report measure. Like other self-report measures of disclosure, we assessed self-disclosure from
the perspective of the discloser which necessarily involves self-evaluation using an internalized
standard (Chelune, 1979). We also needed a measure that was neutral with respect to topic so
that the same measure could be used to compare results across experiments. Accordingly, we
asked participants to rate their perception of self-disclosure on the following four-item scale (7
point Likert from strongly disagree to strongly agree): “My ideas reveal something about my
personality”, “My ideas demonstrate something about me”, “My ideas indicate some of what I
am like as a person”, and “My ideas do not reveal anything about me” (R). The scale was reliable
(α = .84), so the items were averaged together.
Independent coding of idea creativity: As an additional cross-check to confirm that feelings of
self-disclosure were based on actually generating creative ideas, two coders, who were blind to
the experimental conditions and hypotheses of the study, coded each idea for creativity on a one
to five scale (1 = Not at all creative; 5 = Highly creative). Because the two coders demonstrated
significant agreement in their ratings of the ideas (r = 0.73, p < .001), their assessments were
11
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
averaged together. All idea scores for each participant were then averaged to create an overall
creativity score for each participant.
Results
The manipulation check confirmed that participants in the creative idea generation
condition rated their own ideas as significantly more creative (M = 4.79, SD = 1.21) than
participants in the uncreative condition (M = 3.95, SD = 1.40), F (1, 201) = 20.54, p < .001, 95%
CI = [.47, 1.20]. We also analyzed the independent coding of idea creativity as an additional
cross-check of the effectiveness of the creativity instructions. As expected, participants randomly
assigned to the creativity condition, generated ideas rated by evaluators as significantly more
creative (M = 3.14, SD = .42) than participants assigned to the less creative condition (M = 2.32,
SD = .64; t(201) = 9.69, p < .001), 95% CI = [.64, .99]. Regression analysis revealed that coded
ratings of creativity were significantly correlated with self-report ratings of creativity, β = .92, p
< .001, R = .47. And, consistent with our prediction, a regression analysis also revealed that rated
creativity was significantly correlated with perceived self-disclosure β = .15, p < .001, R = .27.
Finally, confirming our hypothesis, a one-way ANOVA revealed participants who
generated creative ideas reported significantly higher perceptions of self-disclosure (M = 4.37,
SD = 1.19) compared to those who generated uncreative ideas (M = 3.85, SD = 1.38), F(1, 201)
= 8.03, p = .0051, 95% CI = [.16, .87].
Discussion
Experiment 1 provides the first evidence that the act of generating creative ideas prompts
the perception of self-disclosure. In experiment 2 our aim was to demonstrate the robustness and
12
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
replicability of the creative self-disclosure effect using a different brainstorming topic and a
different participant sample.
Experiment 2
Method
Participants and procedure. We recruited 202 participants through Amazon Mechanical Turk
(53% male, Mage = 36) who were randomly assigned to one of two conditions (Creative vs.
Uncreative). Participants read the following prompt with the wording in parentheses varied by
condition (creative vs uncreative): “This next section is a task about product development. We
are looking for some ideas for (new/word omitted) types of scented candles that are (creative,
novel, and unique/ generic, conventional, and typical). We want these scents to be as (creative,
novel, and unique/ generic, conventional, and typical) as possible. Please take the next five
minutes to write as many (creative, novel, and unique/generic, conventional, and typical) ideas as
possible for (new/word omitted) candle scents.”
Again, we deliberately selected a mundane object, the discussion of which would not be
inherently self-disclosing, in order to provide a conservative test of our hypothesis. Seven out of
ten households in the U.S. use scented candles and there are more than 10,000 candle scents on
the market (National Candle Association, 2018), so simply discussing candle scents should not,
in and of itself, be personally revealing. After participants finished brainstorming, they were
asked to complete a short survey.
Measures
13
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Creativity of ideas. Participants once again rated the creativity of their ideas using the same
items as in experiment 1. The scale was once again reliable (α = .90), so the items were averaged
together.
Perceived self-disclosure. Participants rated their perceptions of self-disclosure on a four-item
scale that was identical to the scale used in experiment 1. The scale was highly reliable (α = .94),
so the items were averaged together.
Independent coding of idea creativity: Following the same procedure used in Experiment 1,
two coders rated each idea for creativity on a one to five scale (1 = Not at all creative; 5 = Highly
creative). Because the two coders demonstrated significant agreement in their ratings of the ideas
(r = 0.71, p < .001), their assessments were averaged together. All idea scores for each
participant were then averaged to create an overall creativity score for each participant.
Results
The manipulation check confirmed that participants in the creative idea generation
condition did rate their own ideas as significantly more creative (M = 5.00, SD = 1.27) than
those in the uncreative condition (M = 3.43, SD = 1.47), F(1, 201) = 66.07, p < .001, 95% CI =
[1.19, 1.96]. Also, as expected, participants randomly assigned to the creativity condition,
generated ideas rated by evaluators as significantly more creative (M = 2.72, SD = .56) than
participants assigned to the less creative condition (M = 1.71, SD = .34; t (201) = 14.50, p <
.001), 95% CI = [.87, 1.14]. Regression analysis revealed that coded ratings of creativity were
significantly correlated with self-report ratings of creativity, β = 1.13, p < .001, R = .50. And,
consistent with our prediction, a regression analysis also revealed that rated creativity was
significantly correlated with perceived self-disclosure, β = .77, p < .001, R = .31.
14
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Finally, replicating the results of experiment 1 and confirming our main hypothesis, a
one-way ANOVA revealed that participants randomly assigned to generate creative ideas
reported significantly higher perceptions of self-disclosure (M = 4.72, SD = 1.48) than did
participants who generated uncreative ideas (M = 3.55, SD = 1.69), F(1, 201) = 27.59, p < .001,
95% CI = [.74, 1.62].
Discussion
The results of experiment 2 support our hypothesis that creativity feels self-disclosing
and they provide a direct replication of experiment 1 using a different idea generation prompt. A
limitation of the first two experiments is that we did not directly measure the underlying
psychological mechanism. Because creative ideas derived from self-focused attention are
uniquely personal, the act of sharing a creative idea should feel revealing of the self. Thus, in the
next experiment, we measured self-focus directly to trace the underlying mechanism behind the
creative self-disclosure effect.
Experiment 3
Method
Participants and procedure. We recruited 203 participants through Amazon Mechanical Turk
(56% male, Mage = 34) who were randomly assigned to one of two conditions (Creative vs.
Uncreative). Participants read the same prompt as in experiment 2. After participants finished
brainstorming, they were asked to complete a short survey. Seven participants were excluded for
writing nonsense answers in response to the idea generation prompt, leaving a final sample of
196.
Measures
15
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Perceived creativity of ideas. Participants rated the creativity of their ideas on the same scale
employed above. The scale was highly reliable (α = .92). Having established in experiments 1-2
that the instructions to be creative actually did elicit more creative ideas and that rated creativity
is significantly correlated with self-reported creativity, we did not code the ideas generated in
study 3.
Perceived self-disclosure. Participants rated their perceptions of self-disclosure on the same
self-disclosure scale as above. The scale was highly reliable (α = .91).
Self-Focus. Participants rated their perceptions of self-focus on the following six-item scale (7
point Likert from strongly disagree to strongly agree): “I was thinking about my own
preferences”, “I considered what I would want in a product”, I was thinking about my own past
experiences”, “I was focusing on the impression I was making with my ideas”, “I was focusing
on my feelings during the task”, and “I was thinking about what other people would want” (R).
The scale was reliable (α = .81) so the items were averaged together.
Results
The manipulation check confirmed that participants in the creativity condition rated their
ideas as significantly more creative (M = 4.28, SD = .92) than those in the uncreative condition
(M = 2.71, SD = 1.21), F(1, 195) = 102.6, p < .001, 95% CI = [1.26, 1.87]. Confirming our main
hypothesis, a one-way ANOVA revealed that participants randomly assigned to generate creative
ideas reported significantly higher perceived self-disclosure (M = 4.77, SD = 1.37) than did
participants who generated uncreative ideas (M = 3.64, SD = 1.66), F(1, 195) = 26.32, p < .001,
95% CI = [.69, 1.55]. Also, as expected, a one-way ANOVA revealed that participants randomly
assigned to generate creative ideas reported significantly higher self-focus (M = 4.75, SD = .87)
16
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
than did participants who generated uncreative ideas (M = 4.21, SD = 1.13), F (1, 195) = 13.56, p
< .001, 95% CI = [.25, .83].
We conducted a mediation analysis using the bootstrap method with 5000 samples to test
the hypothesis that the relationship between creativity and perceived self-disclosure is mediated
by self-focus. (Mediation Package in R, Tingley, Yamamoto, Hirose, Keele, and Imai 2013).
Group condition (creative: 1 or uncreative: 0) was the independent variable in the model, while
perceived self-disclosure was the dependent variable. Self-focus was the mediating variable.
Results showed significant mediation: indirect effect = .54, p < .001, 95% CI = [.24, .85]. Once
self-focus was controlled for in the model, there was still a significant direct effect = .54, p =
.002, 95% CI = [.22, .88]. This mediation is partial (proportion mediated = .50, p < .001, 95% CI
= [.27, .73]. Overall, generating creative ideas triggered a focus on the self, which, in turn,
boosted perceived self-disclosure.
Discussion
The results of experiments 1-3 demonstrate the creative self-disclosure effect is robust
and replicable across two different brainstorming topics and different participant samples. The
results of experiment 3 also shed light on the underlying psychological mechanism—perceived
self-disclosure is partially mediated by self-focus during the process of generating creative ideas.
Because creative ideas derived from self-focused attention are uniquely personal, the act of
sharing a creative idea is perceived to be revealing of the self. In the next study, we sought to
investigate a potential boundary condition of the creative self-disclosure effect—the ambiguity
and structure of the problem space.
17
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
A problem space can be highly structured and well defined, or it can be relatively broad
and ambiguous (Voss & Post, 1988; Vartanian, 2009). For instance, one might ask problem
solvers to generate ideas for new candle scents without indicating what kind of scent or scents
upon which to focus, thus leaving the objective open to interpretation. Conversely, one might
restrict problem solvers to one particular idea category (e.g. generate new ideas for fruit scented
candles), thus making the problem narrower and less ambiguous. It is possible that engaging in
the creative process can feel more personally revealing when individuals have the latitude to
freely decide from a wide range of possibilities which areas of the problem space are interesting
and meaningful and then choose to focus more attention on that subset of the problem space
(Lepisto & Pratt, 2017). A highly structured and well defined problem space might limit the
ability to make personally revealing choices in the process of generating ideas—a restriction that
should attenuate the creative self-disclosure effect. When a task is narrowly defined, the fact that
a problem solver generates ideas within one topic domain can be externally attributed to task
demand rather than to a revealing personal preference (Milgram, 1974; Moore & Gino, 2013). In
contrast, when generating ideas in response to a more ambiguous and unstructured problem, the
decision to focus disproportionate attention within one topic domain may reflect a choice that
might reveal one’s own personal interests and preferences and hence may feel more self-
disclosing (Lepisto & Pratt, 2017).
The forgoing logic leads to two predictions. First, we expect that the creative self-
disclosure effect should be significantly attenuated when individuals are restricted via specific
instructions to generate creative ideas within one idea category (e.g. Generate creative ideas for
fruit scented candles) compared to when they are not restricted by idea category (e.g. Generate
creative ideas for scented candles). Second, zeroing in on and becoming relatively more
18
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
absorbed with one particular idea category while working on a less restricted problem should
produce greater feelings of self-disclosure. The decision to focus disproportionate attention on
one or a few idea categories as opposed to other possibilities is potentially revealing of the self
because the decision to focus and where may disclose one’s own unique preferences. We test
these hypotheses in experiment 4.
Experiment 4
Participants and procedure. We recruited 402 participants through Amazon Mechanical Turk.
Of those 402 participants recruited initially, three were excluded for entering all blanks instead
of ideas, leaving a final participant count of 399 (49% male, Mage = 36) who were randomly
assigned to one of four conditions in a 2 (Instructions: creative vs uncreative) x 2 (Topic:
category restricted vs unrestricted) between subjects design. Within the category unrestricted
conditions, the creative and uncreative prompts were identical to experiment 1. The wording in
parentheses varied by condition (creative vs uncreative): “This next section is a task about
product development. We are looking for some ideas for (new/word omitted) types of scented
candles that are (creative, novel, and unique/ generic, conventional, and typical). We want these
scents to be as (creative, novel, and unique/ generic, conventional, and typical) as possible.
Please take the next five minutes to write as many (creative, novel, and unique/generic,
conventional, and typical) ideas as possible for (new/word omitted) candle scents.”
Within the category restricted conditions, the prompt was altered so that participants were
requested to only focus on candle scent ideas that were fruit scented. Thus, participants in the
category restricted conditions read the following prompt with the wording in parentheses varied
by condition (creative vs uncreative): “This next section is a task about product development.
We are looking for some ideas for (new/word omitted) types of fruit scented candles that are
19
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
(creative, novel, and unique/ generic, conventional, and typical). We want these fruit scents to be
as (creative, novel, and unique/ generic, conventional, and typical) as possible. Please take the
next five minutes to write as many (creative, novel, and unique/generic, conventional, and
typical) ideas as possible for (new/word omitted) fruit candle scents.”
Measures
Perceived self-disclosure: Participants responded to the same self-disclosure scale used in
experiments 1-2. The perceived self-disclosure scale was once again reliable (α = .94), so the
items were averaged together.
Within-category fluency: In order to test our hypothesis that greater focus in the unrestricted
condition should be correlated with greater perceived self-disclosure, we asked to coders to
independently categorize all of the ideas in the unrestricted category condition (1,400 ideas total)
based on how similar the ideas were to each other. Following procedures used in prior research
(e.g. Nijstad, Stroebe, & Lodewijkx, (2002), the ideas were sorted into categories of similar types
of candle scent ideas (e.g. all laundry related scents into one category, all wood related scents
into another category and so forth). Once the ideas were sorted into categories, we then divided
the total number of ideas by the total number of categories covered by each participant to reach a
final score of within-category fluency. Higher scores indicate greater focus—more ideas
generated within fewer categories.
Results
An ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of creativity condition on self-disclosure
such that participants in the creativity condition reported significantly more self-disclosure (M =
4.65, SD = 1.45) than did participants in the uncreative condition (M = 3.61, SD = 1.65), F(3,
20
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
396) = 44.5, p < .001, 95% CI = [.36, .67]. There was also a significant main effect of category
restriction such that participants in the unrestricted category condition reported higher self-
disclosure (M = 4.32, SD = 1.69), than did participants in the restricted condition (M = 3.95, SD
= 1.57), F (3, 396) = 5.09, p = .024, 95% CI = [.02, .33]. Finally, as predicted, there was a
significant interaction between the creativity and category restriction conditions F(3, 396) = 4.50,
p = .031, 95% CI = [.01, .32].
We conducted planned contrasts to reveal the form of the interaction (See Figure 1). First,
replicating the results of experiments 1-3, the results showed that, within the category
unrestricted condition, participants reported higher self-disclosure when generating creative ideas
(M = 4.99, SD = 1.36) compared to participants generating uncreative ideas (M = 3.63, SD =
1.72) F(1, 398) = 37.07, p < .001, 95% CI = [.46, .90]. Second, as predicted, the creative self-
disclosure effect emerged more strongly when participants were asked to generate creative ideas
that were unrestricted by category (M = 4.99, SD = 1.36) than when they were asked to generate
creative ideas that were restricted by category (M = 4.31, SD = 1.46), F(1, 398) = 8.93, p < .001,
95% CI = [.12, .57]. Finally, within the uncreative conditions, the results showed that simply
allowing participants to generate ideas that were unrestricted by category (M = 3.63, SD = 1.72)
did not produce higher perceived self-disclosure than participants who generated uncreative
ideas that were restricted by category (M = 3.61; SD = 1.60), F(1, 398) = .05, p = .94, 95% CI =
[-.25, .20].
INSERT FIGURE ONE HERE
We also predicted that focusing on and becoming relatively more absorbed with one
particular idea category while working on a less restricted problem should produce greater
feelings of self-disclosure. In support of this prediction, among participants assigned to the
21
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
unrestricted condition, within-category fluency was positively correlated with perceived self-
disclosure, R = .23, p = .023. In contrast, neither the sheer number of ideas generated, R = - .005,
p = .91, nor the sheer number of categories covered, R = .07, p = .49, were correlated with
perceived self-disclosure.
Discussion
Replicating and building on the results of experiments 1-3, we again confirmed our
prediction that creative ideation feels self-disclosing and we also identified the structure and
ambiguity of the problem space as a boundary condition that attenuates this effect. Interestingly,
we also found that within the unrestricted category condition, the more narrowly focused the
ideas were (within category fluency), the more self-disclosing the ideas were perceived to be. In
other words, it was not enough to merely share a lot of ideas to produce feelings of self-
disclosure, nor was it sufficient to generate ideas across a wide range of categories. Rather,
perceived self-disclosure was higher among participants who generate a larger number of ideas
within fewer idea categories. This result seems consistent with a growing stream of research
suggesting that creativity not only demands cognitive flexibility but also focused persistence
(Nijstad, De Dreu, Rietzschel & Bass, 2010). Our results suggest that focused persistence in the
creative process might feel personally revealing because it reflects an idiosyncratic decision to
pursue one subset of ideas from among a number of available options.
One limitation that might be addressed in future research is that we restricted participants
to a relatively common category (fruit scented candles) that might, in and of itself, be somewhat
uncreative. Would this pattern of results have emerged if we had restricted participants to a more
inherently revealing category such as, “scents that remind you of your childhood” or would the
22
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
fact that participants are instructed to focus on one category reduce the autonomy and choice that
might be necessary for the creative self-disclosure effect to emerge?
One important limitation of experiments 1-4 is that we asked people to report their
perceptions of self-disclosure without interacting with a partner. Testing our hypothesis in the
context of a face-to-face interaction is crucial for several reasons. First, we want to demonstrate
the robustness of creative self-disclosure in the context of a social interaction given that in the
real world, ideas are often shared in collaboration with others (Paulus & Yang, 2000). Second, a
dyad study will allow us to extend our focus from perceptions of self-disclosure (did I share
something revealing?) to partner self-disclosure (did my partner share something revealing with
me?).
Finally, we consider an important implication of creative self-disclosure—revealing and
listening to creative ideas might impact how partners view each other in two ways. First, creative
self-disclosure might boost liking between partners because there is evidence that we both like
others who disclose to us and we also like people as a result of disclosing to them (Collins &
Miller, 1994). If creative collaboration provides the opportunity for self-disclosure, then hearing
and sharing creative ideas should cause partners to like each other more. Second, if self-
disclosure is taking place during creative collaboration then sharing personal information might
allow partners to form judgments about their partner’s personality traits and be more confident,
based on that information, that their judgments about their partner are accurate (Swann & Gill,
1997). In other words, hearing a partner’s creative ideas, if they are personally disclosing, should
make us feel that we know our partner better. Consequently, in the next study, we test our
hypothesis that sharing creative ideas with a partner should elicit mutual perceived disclosure
23
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
and disclosure should, in turn, boost liking between partners and raise the confidence with which
partners’ hold judgments about each-other.
Experiment 5
Participants. We recruited 334 undergraduates (59% female, Mage = 20) to participate in the
experiment from a departmental subject pool. Students were compensated with extra credit in
exchange for their participation. Eight participants were excluded for failing to follow
instructions, thus rendering their data uninterpretable. After these exclusions, a total of 326
participants remained, divided into 163 pairs. Eighty-three pairs were randomly assigned to the
uncreative condition and 80 pairs were assigned to the creative condition.
Procedure. Upon entering the lab, all participants were asked to complete an idea generation
task that was identical to the one used in experiments 2 and 3. Once seated at a computer,
participants read the following prompt with the wording in parentheses varied by condition
(creative vs uncreative): “This next section is a task about product development. We are looking
for some ideas for (new/word omitted) types of scented candles that are (creative, novel, and
unique/ generic, conventional, and typical). We want these scents to be as (creative, novel, and
unique/ generic, conventional, and typical) as possible. Please take the next five minutes to write
as many (creative, novel, and unique/generic, conventional, and typical) ideas as possible for
(new/word omitted) candle scents.” Participants then shared their ideas with each other by
reading their ideas aloud. After sharing all of their ideas with each-other, partners were asked to
reveal which of their own ideas they thought were most creative or uncreative, depending on the
24
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
condition to which they were randomly assigned. Upon completing this discussion, participants
were then asked to complete a survey on their own.
Measures
Perceived self-disclosure. We used the same four-item self-report measure of self-disclosure
used in experiments 1-3. The items were as follows: “My ideas revealed something about my
personality,” “My ideas demonstrated something about me,” “My ideas indicated some of what I
am like as a person,” and “My ideas did not reveal anything about me (reverse scored).” The
scale was highly reliable (α = .90), so the items were averaged together.
Perceived partner self-disclosure. Participants then responded to a similar scale about their
partners, also rated on a seven-point Likert scale from 1, strongly disagree, to 7, strongly agree.
The items were as follows: “My partner's ideas revealed something about their personality,” “My
partner's ideas demonstrated something about them,” “My partner's ideas indicated some of what
they are like as a person,” and “My partner's ideas did not reveal anything about them.” The
scale was highly reliable (α = .91), so the items were averaged together.
Liking. Participants then responded to a similar scale about how much they liked their partner,
also rated on a seven-point Likert from strongly disagree to strongly agree. The items for that
were: “I would work with this person again if given the chance,” “I enjoyed participating with
this person,” “In general, I like the person I was paired with,” and “I would not want to interact
with this person again.” The scale was highly reliable, (α = .89), so the items were averaged
together.
Confidence in the accuracy of partner judgments. Participants were then asked to rate their
partner on a series of 10 Barnum statements (Forer, 1949), such as “Most of the time they are
25
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
positive and cheerful, but there has been a time in the past when they were very upset,” or “They
are a very kind and considerate person, but when somebody does something to break their trust,
they feel deep-seated anger.” The items were each rated on a five-point scale asking how
accurately the statements described their partner from “not accurately at all” to “extremely
accurately.” We had no hypothesis about the content of the judgments themselves, so we did not
analyze those data. Rather, we were interested in the confidence with which they held these
judgments. Therefore, after completing each statement participants responded to the question
“How confident are you in the answer above on a scale of 1 (not at all confident) to 100
(extremely confident)?” The 10 items measuring confidence were reliable (α = .96), so the
confidence ratings for each of the 10 statements were averaged together.
Results
Across all of our hypothesis tests, the unit of analysis was the dyad, so the scores of both
partners were averaged together to create a dyadic score. Replicating the results of experiments
1-3, dyads in the creative condition reporting significantly higher self-disclosure (M = 5.16, SD
= .79) than did dyads in the uncreative condition, (M = 4.58, SD = 1.01), F(1, 162) = 16.72, p <
.001, 95% CI = [.15, .43]. Second, as hypothesized, dyads reported their partner self-disclosed
significantly more in the creative (M = 5.11, SD = .70) compared to the uncreative condition (M
= 4.69, SD = 1.06), F(1, 162) = 8.83, p = .0037, 95% CI = [.07, .35]. Third, contrary to our
prediction, dyads in the creative condition did not report liking their partner significantly more
(M = 5.66, SD = .74), compared to dyads in the uncreative condition (M = 5.48, SD = .82), p =
.14, F(1, 162) = 2.107, 95% CI = [-.03, .21]. This null result could be due to a ceiling effect on
the liking measure as the mean reported liking across conditions was relatively high and the
standard deviation was somewhat low (M = 5.57, SD = .78). We speculate further in the
26
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
discussion section. Finally, turning to the measures of perceived confidence, an ANOVA
revealed dyads in the creative condition felt significantly more confident in the personality
judgments they made about their partner (M = 53.7, SD = 19.0) compared to dyads in the
uncreative condition, (M = 43.4, SD = 19.0), F(1, 146)1 = 10.14, p = .0018, 95% CI = [1.95,
8.35].
We conducted a mediation analysis using the bootstrap method with 5000 samples to test
the hypothesis that confidence in personality judgment about one’s partner is mediated by the
extent to which an individual perceived that their partner self-disclosed (Mediation Package in R,
Tingley, Yamamoto, Hirose, Keele, and Imai 2013). Group condition (creative: 1 or uncreative: -
-1) was the independent variable in the model, while confidence in the accuracy of judgments
made about the partner was the dependent variable. Ratings of dyads perceived partner self-
disclosure was the mediating variable. Results showed a significant mediated effect = 1.50, p =
.001, 95% CI = [.48, 2.88]. Once partner self-disclosure was controlled for in the model, there
was still a significant direct effect = 3.68, p = .024, 95% CI = [.50, 6.76]. This mediation is only
partial (proportion mediated = .28, p < .01, 95% CI = [.09, .75]. Overall, sharing creative ideas
triggered the perception that one’s partner self-disclosed which, in turn, boosted the confidence
with which partners held personality judgments about each other.
Discussion
Experiment 5 demonstrated the creative self-disclosure effect emerges even in the context
of a face-to-face interaction in which ideas were actually shared. Moreover, the results showed
that individuals not only believed they disclosed personal information after sharing creative
ideas, they also felt that their partner also revealed personal information. An important
implication of these results is that creative self-disclosure should also shape our perceptions of
27
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
the individual with whom we are collaborating. Indeed, the results also show that self-disclosure
also boosted confidence in the accuracy of judgments that partners made about each-other’s
personality. In other words, because creative ideas are personal revealing, they become a basis
upon which to make judgments about our interaction partners, making them seem more known to
us.
We also expected that sharing creative ideas should boost liking since mutual self-
disclosure can foster social bonds (Rook, 1984; Collins & Miller, 1994). This null result might
be due to a ceiling effect, given overall liking was high across conditions. Alternatively, it is
possible that, though creative expression is self-disclosing, the information shared might not
inevitably lead to positive impressions (Staw, 1995). Sharing creative ideas can cue a range of
perceptions that can be positive or negative depending on the context. For example, in an
educational setting, teachers like less creative students more than highly creative students
(Westby & Dawson, 1995). When evaluating leadership potential, creative people are viewed as
too risky and unpredictable to be effective leaders (Mueller, Goncalo & Kamdar, 2011). These
contradictory impressions of creative people suggest opportunities for future research on
potential moderators of a link between creative self-disclosure and liking (Collins & Miller,
1994). For example, creative self-disclosure might vary on appropriateness—ideas that are
overly revealing might diminish liking (Derlega and Grzelak, 1979).
Consider a few ideas from one participant’s list of candle scent ideas who was assigned
to the creativity condition, “Tears of the Dead, Vanquished Foes, Guile Guilt and Gore, Dog
Farts, Zombie Outbreak, Burning Cookies, Spoiled Milk in a Hot Car, etc.” A partner who hears
these ideas might feel they are personal revealing, but the impression might not necessarily be
positive. On the other hand, ideas like these are so distinctive that, if one finds a partner who
28
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
appreciates them, then liking should increase considerably. In most social contexts, ideas like
these might be withheld to avoid making a negative impression but in the context of
brainstorming, these filters are intentionally removed, making it more difficult to engage in
impression management and be creative at the same time. Future research on the interpersonal
consequences of creativity can test these possibilities by varying norms around whether
creativity is viewed as desirable or appropriate, how much creativity is too much or too soon and
whether a creative act is viewed as spontaneous or highly controlled by situational cues (Collins
& Miller, 1994).
General Discussion
Existing research on creative idea generation has been focused almost exclusively on the
question of how to encourage people to express a wide range of creative ideas. Given creative
ideas are highly prized across so many contexts, the antecedents of creativity are clearly
important. However, the present studies suggest that the act of sharing a creative idea may also
have considerable personal and interpersonal consequences. The creative process involves idea
expression, but our findings suggest it is not merely ideas that are being shared as people may
also feel personal information has spilled out in the process. When prompted to be creative,
individuals are not just responding in the abstract; rather they are reaching into themselves to
share ideas that reflect their unique point of view, an act that feels self-disclosing.
Finally, we found that people use creative ideas to make personality judgments about
their partners and these judgments are held more confidently. Sharing creative ideas prompted
the mutual perception of self-disclosure (I disclosed to my partner and my partner disclosed to
me) and that exchange made people feel they knew their partner better. People were more
confident in the judgments they made about their partner but did not necessarily like them better,
29
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
highlighting why the creative act is so risky. Ideas can convey personal information that can be
used to form judgments that can be either positive or negative. This pattern of results is
consistent with prior research on self-disclosure showing that a single act of self-disclosure does
tends to elicit self-disclosure from one’s partner. In other words, there may not have been a
merely subjective sense that one’s partner had disclosed more in the process of being creative,
but it also appears people are recognizing and possibly responding to the greater disclosure of
their partner—a process that could lead to escalating self-disclosure over time (Collins & Miller,
1994).
Implications and Future Directions
Our findings suggest numerous opportunities for future research that could proceed in
several directions.
Downstream Consequences of Creative Self-Disclosure
Given that self-disclosure has so many downstream consequences for interpersonal
relationships including, intimacy (Laurenceau, Barrett & Pietromonaco, 1998), liking (Collins &
Miller, 1994), loneliness (Wei, Russell & Zakalik, 2005), interdependence (Rusbult & Van
Lange, 2003) and so on, the implications for potential consequences of creativity are
considerable. Rather than viewing creative ideation from the narrow lens of boosting creative
output, we can begin to explore the role of creative collaboration in fostering social bonds. In
thinking about the potential interpersonal consequences of idea sharing, however, caution flags
should be raised around the assumption that the consequences will necessarily be positive.
Indeed, sharing creative ideas makes an individual vulnerable and that vulnerability can either be
30
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
an opportunity for bonding and subsequent relationship building or an interpersonal risk that
might have negative repercussions.
Potential moderators of the creative self-disclosure effect
We decided to stick with one topic across studies to provide multiple direct and indirect
replications of the creative self-disclosure effect. We also picked candle scents and potato chips
to provide a somewhat conservative test of our hypothesis—these products are extremely
common and not inherently highly revealing of the self. However, future research should
investigate whether this effect occurs in a wider range of tasks and whether the effect is
moderated by task type. For instance, tasks that allow for autonomy, agency or choice might all
give people that latitude to make decisions that might be personally revealing. Prior research has
shown that individuals may choose to self-disclose directly, but our results suggests that
disclosure may occur indirectly as people work on tasks that allow for personally revealing
choices.
Although our focus in this paper was on the effect of engaging in creative ideation, future
research might also look at the possible role of trait creativity in moderating creative self-
disclosure. Highly creative personalities are notoriously self-focused, to the point of being
described as highly competitive, individualistic, self-aggrandizing and self-absorbed (Gough,
1979). It is possible that highly creative people are generally more willing to self-disclose in
social situations, perhaps even when it is not necessarily appropriate or advantageous to do so,
perhaps contributing to the perception that creative people are quirky and unpredictable
(Mueller, Goncalo & Kamdar, 2011).
31
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Relatedly, perceived self-disclosure might be moderated by the extent to which creativity
is a core element of one’s personal identity. For example, Janssen’s (2003) work on job
involvement and innovative behavior shows that highly involved employees are more likely to
react negatively and engage in conflict with co-workers who resist their ideas, because being
innovative is part of their self-concept. In the context of the current results, this raises the
possibility that the self-disclosure effect found here should be particularly strong for people
whose self-concept is strongly tied to creativity because they take their own work much more
personally (Vincent & Kouchaki, 2016).
Finally, future research should investigate the possibility that the mere instruction to be
“creative” could also act as a prime that might trigger a host of associations that could prompt
feelings of self-disclosure without actually generating creative ideas. For instance, previous work
has shown that the concept of creativity can indeed be primed (Gino and Ariely, 2012) and that
priming the concept of creativity produces a mindset that carries over to subsequent, unrelated,
tasks (Vincent and Kouchaki, 2016). Moreover, simply invoking the word “creative” could
trigger associations like “standing out” and being “unique and different”—associations that
might produce feelings of self-disclosure even in the absence of actual creative ideation
(Sassenburg & Moskowitz, 2005). However, our results suggest invoking the word “creative”
alone might not be sufficient, given the results of study 4 showing that instructions to be creative
did not trigger perceived self-disclosure when participants were restricted to only one idea
category. In other words, actually engaging in the task and generating creative ideas might be
necessary as opposed to a simple instructional prime.
An Alternative Interpretation of Evaluation Apprehension
32
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
One of the biggest barriers to creative expression is evaluation apprehension—individuals
withhold creative ideas to avoid rejection. Why do people take rejection so personally? Our
results may offer an alternative explanation for evaluation apprehension. It might not result from
the rejection of the idea per-se, but from the implied rejection of one’s self. If in the process of
being creative, people rely heavily on their own idiosyncratic experiences and perspectives then
ideas are not merely abstract suggestions but deeply personal self-disclosures. This interpretation
might explain why, for example, anonymity has been shown to liberate both creative expression
and self-disclosure by reducing public self-awareness (Joinson, 2001). Similarly, prior research
has shown that people generate more creative solutions when they are asked to generate ideas for
other people compared to when they generate ideas for themselves (Polman & Emich, 2011).
Ideas generated for one’s self are directly revealing of one’s personal preferences and to the
extent that people might be uncomfortable disclosing information about themselves, they might
be reluctant to propose solutions that might reveal too much. In contrast, ideas generated for
other people might reflect what an individual thinks the other person might find appealing, thus
offering cover to suggest unusual ideas without the risk of self-disclosure. The self-disclosing
nature of creative work could contribute to lower psychological safety during collaborative
creativity which could, in turn, make people reluctant to risk sharing valuable ideas and
perspectives (Carmeli, Reiter-Palmon & Ziv, 2010; Kessel, Kratzer & Schultz, 2012).
Conclusion
Our research has shown that the creative act feels self-disclosing. Creative self-disclosure
can also lead partners to form judgments about each-other and to be more confident in those
judgments. Future research in this vein thus has the potential to investigate how decisions about
whether to disclose, to whom and how much might also impact the ideas we choose to share or
33
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
withhold from others. Given that self-disclosure is central to the formation and quality of
personal relationships, the present work suggests the many discoveries that await as we integrate
creativity with the literatures on person perception, social cognition, and human bonding.
34
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Figure 1
Figure 1: Mean self-disclosure by creativity and category restriction conditions (Experiment 4)
4.99
4.31
3.63 3.61
1
2
3
4
5
6
Unrestricted Restricted
Creative
Uncreative
35
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Endnotes
1 Consistent with IRB policies, we did not force respondents to answer each question. The lower degrees
of freedom in this analysis resulted from participants who chose not to respond to certain questions.
36
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Appendix
Materials and data are available at:
https://osf.io/2hzjm/?view_only=d8619134af4645478dec1b1ffc831d9f
37
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
References
Altman, I., & Taylor, D. A. (1973). Social penetration: The development of interpersonal
relationships. Holt, Rinehart & Winston. New York
Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context: Update to the social psychology of creativity.
Westview Press. Boulder, CO.
Amabile, T. M., & Pratt, M. G. (2016). The dynamic componential model of creativity and
innovation in organizations: Making progress, making meaning. Research in
Organizational Behavior, 36, 157-183.
Atwood, Liz. "Palate-Pleasing Chips." Daily Press, July 2, 2003. Accessed: November 24, 2017
http://candles.org/facts-figures-2/
Baas, M., Koch, S., Nijstad, B. A., & De Dreu, C. K. (2015). Conceiving creativity: The nature
and consequences of laypeople’s beliefs about the realization of creativity. Psychology of
Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9(3), 340-354.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497-529.
Baxter, L. A., & Montgomery, B. M. (1996). Relating: Dialogues and dialectics. Guilford Press.
New York.
Beersma, B., & De Dreu, C. K. (2005). Conflict's consequences: Effects of social motives on
postnegotiation creative and convergent group functioning and performance. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 89(3), 358-374.
Birnbaum, G. E., Mizrahi, M., Kaplan, A., Kadosh, D., Kariv, D., Tabib, D., Ziv, D., Sadeh, L.,
& Burban, D. (2017). Sex unleashes your tongue: Sexual priming motivates self-
disclosure to a new acquaintance and interest in future interactions. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(5), 706-715.
Branson, R (2015, June) A life of ideas. Trinidad and Tobago Guardian. Retrieved from
http://www.guardian.co.tt
Camacho, L. M., & Paulus, P. B. (1995). The role of social anxiousness in group brainstorming.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(6), 1071-1080.
Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retentions in creative thought as in other
knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67(6), 380.
Carmeli, A., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Ziv, E. (2010). Inclusive leadership and employee
involvement in creative tasks in the workplace: The mediating role of psychological
safety. Creativity Research Journal, 22(3), 250-260.
38
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Chelune, G. J. (1979). Measuring openness in interpersonal communication. Self-disclosure:
Origins, Patterns, and Implications of Openness in Interpersonal Relationships, 1-27.
Cheng, C. Y., Sanchez-Burks, J., & Lee, F. (2008). Connecting the dots within: Creative
performance and identity integration. Psychological Science, 19(11), 1178-1184.
Coleman, A. (2014, November). How Prezi helped turn budapest into Europe's newest startup
hub. Forbes. Retrieved from https://www.forbes.com/
Collins, N. L., & Miller, L. C. (1994). Self-disclosure and liking: a meta-analytic
review. Psychological Bulletin, 116(3), 457-475.
Connolly, T., Jessup, L. M., & Valacich, J. S. (1990). Effects of anonymity and evaluative tone
on idea generation in computer-mediated groups. Management Science, 36(6), 689-703.
Cozby, P. C. (1973). Self-disclosure: a literature review. Psychological Bulletin, 79(2), 73-91.
Derlega, V. J., & Grzelak, J. Appropriateness of self-disclosure. In G. Chelune (Ed.), Self-
disclosure: Origins, patterns, and implications of openness in interpersonal
relationships. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, 1979.
DeWall, C. N., & Bushman, B. J. (2011). Social acceptance and rejection: The sweet and the
bitter. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(4), 256-260.
Diehl, M., & Stroebe, W. (1987). Productivity loss in brainstorming groups: Toward the solution
of a riddle. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(3), 497-509.
Dollinger, S. J. (2003). Need for uniqueness, need for cognition, and creativity. The Journal of
Creative Behavior, 37(2), 99-116.
Fast Company Staff (2015, June) Most Creative People in Business. Fast Company. Retrieved
from www.fastcompany.com
Forer, B. R. (1949). The fallacy of personal validation: a classroom demonstration of
gullibility. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 44(1), 118-123.
Gino, F., & Ariely, D. (2012). The dark side of creativity: original thinkers can be more
dishonest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 445-459.
Goncalo, J. A., & Staw, B. M. (2006). Individualism–collectivism and group
creativity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 100(1), 96-109.
Goncalo, J. A., Vincent, L. C., & Krause, V. (2015). The liberating consequences of creative
39
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
work: How a creative outlet lifts the physical burden of secrecy. Journal of Experimental
Social Psychology, 59, 32-39.
Gough, H. G. (1979). A creative personality scale for the adjective check list. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 37(8), 1398-1405.
Henry, W. P., & Strupp, H. H. (1994). The therapeutic alliance as interpersonal process. The
working alliance: Theory, research, and practice, 173, 51-84.
Howell, A., & Conway, M. (1990). Perceived intimacy of expressed emotion. The Journal of
Social Psychology, 130(4), 467-476.
Janssen, O. (2003). Innovative behaviour and job involvement at the price of conflict and less
satisfactory relations with co‐workers. Journal of Occupational and Organizational
Psychology, 76(3), 347-364.
Joinson, A. N. (2001). Self‐disclosure in computer‐mediated communication: The role of self‐
awareness and visual anonymity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31(2), 177-
192.
Jones, E. E., & Archer, R. L. (1976). Are there special effects of personalistic self-disclosure?.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12(2), 180-193.
Kelly, A. E., & McKillop, K. J. (1996). Consequences of revealing personal secrets.
Psychological Bulletin, 120(3), 450-465.
Kessel, M., Kratzer, J., & Schultz, C. (2012). Psychological safety, knowledge sharing, and
creative performance in healthcare teams. Creativity and Innovation Management, 21(2),
147-157.
Khessina, O. M., Goncalo, J. A., & Krause, V. (2018). It’s time to sober up: The direct costs,
side effects and long-term consequences of creativity and innovation. Research in
Organizational Behavior.
Kim, S. H., Vincent, L. C., & Goncalo, J. A. (2013). Outside advantage: Can social rejection fuel
creative thought?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(3), 605-611.
Laurenceau, J.P., Barrett, L.F. & Pietromonaco, P.R. (1998). Intimacy as an interpersonal
process: The importance of self-disclosure, partner disclosure, and perceived partner
responsiveness in interpersonal exchanges. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 74(5), 1238-1251
Lepisto, D. A., & Pratt, M. G. (2017). Meaningful work as realization and justification: Toward a
dual conceptualization. Organizational Psychology Review, 7(2), 99-121.
Leung, A. K. Y., Maddux, W. W., Galinsky, A. D., & Chiu, C. Y. (2008). Multicultural
40
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
experience enhances creativity: The when and how. American Psychologist, 63(3), 169-
181.
McKenna, K. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2000). Plan 9 from cyberspace: The implications of the Internet
for personality and social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(1),
57-75.
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to Authority. An Experimental View. New York, NY: Harper and
Row.
Millar, K. U., & Millar, M. G. (1988). Sex differences in perceived self-and other-disclosure: A
case where inequity increases satisfaction. Social Behavior and Personality: an
international journal, 16(1), 59-64.
Moore, C., & Gino, F. (2013). Ethically adrift: How others pull our moral compass from true
North, and how we can fix it. Research in Organizational Behavior, 33, 53-77.
Morton, T. L. (1978). Intimacy and reciprocity of exchange: A comparison of spouses and
strangers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(1), 72-81.
Mueller, J. S., Goncalo, J. A., & Kamdar, D. (2011). Recognizing creative leadership: Can
creative idea expression negatively relate to perceptions of leadership potential?. Journal
of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(2), 494-498.
Mueller, J. S., Melwani, S., & Goncalo, J. A. (2012). The bias against creativity: Why people
desire but reject creative ideas. Psychological Science, 23(1), 13-17.
Nijstad, B. A., De Dreu, C. K., Rietzschel, E. F., & Baas, M. (2010). The dual pathway to
creativity model: Creative ideation as a function of flexibility and persistence. European
Review of Social Psychology, 21(1), 34-77.
Nijstad, B. A., & Stroebe, W. (2006). How the group affects the mind: A cognitive model of idea
generation in groups. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(3), 186-213.
Nijstad, B. A., Stroebe, W., & Lodewijkx, H. F. (2002). Cognitive stimulation and interference
in groups: Exposure effects in an idea generation task. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 38(6), 535-544.
Omarzu, J. (2000). A disclosure decision model: Determining how and when individuals will
self-disclose. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(2), 174-185.
Paulus, P. B., & Yang, H. C. (2000). Idea generation in groups: A basis for creativity in
organizations. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 82(1), 76-87.
41
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Paulus, P. B., & Nijstad, B. A. (Eds.). (2003). Group creativity: Innovation through
collaboration. Oxford University Press.
Paulus, P. B., Dzindolet, M., & Kohn, N. W. (2012). Collaborative creativity—Group creativity
and team innovation. In Mumford, M. D., Hester, K. S., & Robledo, I. C. (Eds.),
Handbook of organizational creativity (pp. 327-357).
Phillips, K. W., Rothbard, N. P., & Dumas, T. L. (2009). To disclose or not to disclose? Status
distance and self-disclosure in diverse environments. Academy of Management
Review, 34(4), 710-732.
Polman, E., & Emich, K. J. (2011). Decisions for others are more creative than decisions for the
self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(4), 492-501.
Polzer, J. T., Milton, L. P., & Swann Jr, W. B. (2002). Capitalizing on diversity: Interpersonal
congruence in small work groups. Administrative Science Quarterly, 47(2), 296-324.
Reiter-Palmon, R., Wigert, B., & de Vreede, T. (2012). Team creativity and innovation: The
effect of group composition, social processes, and cognition. InMumford, M. D., Hester,
K. S., & Robledo, I. C. (Eds.), Handbook of organizational creativity (pp. 295-326).
Reis, H. T., & Shaver, P. (1988). Intimacy as an interpersonal process. In Duck, S., Hay, D. F.,
Hobfoll, S. E., Ickes, W., & Montgomery, B. M. (Eds.), Handbook of personal
relationships, 24(3), (pp. 367-389).
Rietzschel, E. F., Nijstad, B. A., & Stroebe, W. (2006). Productivity is not enough: A
comparison of interactive and nominal brainstorming groups on idea generation and
selection. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(2), 244-251.
Rook, K. S. (1984). The negative side of social interaction: impact on psychological well-
being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(5), 1097-1108.
Rusbult, C. E., & Van Lange, P. A. (2003). Interdependence, interaction, and relationships.
Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 351-375.
Sassenberg, K., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2005). Don’t stereotype, think different! Overcoming
automatic stereotype activation by mindset priming. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 41(5), 506-514.
Sawyer, R. K., & DeZutter, S. (2009). Distributed creativity: How collective creations emerge
from collaboration. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3(2), 81-92.
Simonton, D. K. (1999). Creativity as blind variation and selective retention: Is the creative
process Darwinian?. Psychological Inquiry, 309-328.
42
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Simonton, D. K. (2003). Scientific creativity as constrained stochastic behavior: the integration
of product, person, and process perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 129(4), 475-494.
Staw, B. M. (1990). An evolutionary approach to creativity and innovation. In M. A. West & J.
L. Farr (Eds.), Innovation and creativity at work: Psychological and organizational
strategies (pp. 287-308). Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons.
Staw, B.M. (1995) Why no one really wants creativity. In C. M. Ford and D. A. Gioia, (Eds.),
Creative Action in Organizations: Ivory Tower Visions and Real World Voices (pp. 161-
166). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Swann Jr, W. B., & Gill, M. J. (1997). Confidence and accuracy in person perception: Do we
know what we think we know about our relationship partners?. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 73(4), 747-757.
Tingley, D., Yamamoto, T., Hirose, K., Keele, L., & Imai, K. (2013). mediation: R package for
causal mediation analysis, available at the Comprehensive R Archive Network (CRAN).
Tolstedt, B. E., & Stokes, J. P. (1984). Self-disclosure, intimacy, and the depenetration process.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(1), 84-90.
Uzzi, B., & Spiro, J. (2005). Collaboration and creativity: The small world problem. American
Journal of Sociology, 111(2), 447-504.
Vartanian, O. (2009). Variable attention facilitates creative problem solving. Psychology of
Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3(1), 57-59.
Vincent, L. C., & Kouchaki, M. (2016). Creative, rare, entitled, and dishonest: How
commonality of creativity in one’s group decreases an individual’s entitlement and
dishonesty. Academy of Management Journal, 59(4), 1451-1473.
Voss, J. F., & Post, T. A. (1988). On the solving of ill-structured problems. In M. T. H. Chi, R.
Glaser, & M. J. Farr (Eds.), The Nature of Expertise (pp. 261-285). Hillsdale, NJ, US:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Wei, M., Russell, D. W., & Zakalik, R. A. (2005). Adult attachment, social self-efficacy, self-
disclosure, loneliness, and subsequent depression for freshman college students: A
longitudinal study. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52(4), 602-614.
Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal, interpersonal, and
hyperpersonal interaction. Communication Research, 23(1), 3-43.
West, M. A. (2002). Sparkling fountains or stagnant ponds: An integrative model of creativity
and innovation implementation in work groups. Applied Psychology, 51(3), 355-387.
43
CREATIVITY AND SELF-DISCLOSURE
Westby, E. L., & Dawson, V. L. (1995). Creativity: Asset or burden in the classroom?. Creativity
Research Journal, 8(1), 1-10.
Wheeless, L. R., & Grotz, J. (1976). Conceptualization and measurement of reported self‐
disclosure. Human Communication Research, 2(4), 338-346.
Wronska, M. K., Kolańczyk, A., & Nijstad, B. A. (2018). Engaging in Creativity Broadens
Attentional Scope. Frontiers in Psychology, 9: 1772.
Zitek, E. M., & Vincent, L. C. (2015). Deserve and diverge: Feeling entitled makes people more
creative. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 242-248.
... Clothing can allow people to express aspects of their identity (Rafaeli & Pratt, 1993) and can change our actual performance at work (Kim, Holtz, & Vogel, 2022). Similarly, creativity can allow us to express and reveal aspects of ourselves (Goncalo & Katz, 2020). This research examines how wearing clothing that represents or discloses aspects of one's identity will activate disparate systems of information and allow people to access unique information and ideas, which, in turn, will allow them to be more creative. ...
... The individual can leverage and integrate those unique perspectives and sets of the information to generate creative ideas that other people could not do or have not done previously (Amabile & Pratt, 2016;Cheng, Sanchez-Burks, & Lee, 2008;Leung, Maddux, Galinsky, & Chiu, 2008). In this way, one's creative ideas are a unique representation of that person (Goncalo & Katz, 2020). Goncalo and Katz (2020) noted that, "When prompted to be creative, individuals are not just responding in the abstract; rather, they are reaching into themselves to share ideas that reflect their unique point of view, an act that feels self-disclosing." ...
... In this way, one's creative ideas are a unique representation of that person (Goncalo & Katz, 2020). Goncalo and Katz (2020) noted that, "When prompted to be creative, individuals are not just responding in the abstract; rather, they are reaching into themselves to share ideas that reflect their unique point of view, an act that feels self-disclosing." ...
... We argue that this occurs because the experience of being creative can, in and of itself, have positive psychological consequences (Sassenberg & Moskowitz, 2005;Goncalo, Vincent, & Krause, 2015;Khessina, Goncalo, & Krause, 2018). Engaging in the process of creative ideation entails the free and unfettered exploration of alternative ideas and perspectives (Goncalo & Katz, 2020)-a process of divergent thinking that can boost feelings of autonomy (Kim, Goncalo, & Rodas, 2023) and liberation from emotional and psychological constraints Kim, Zeppenfeld, & Cohen, 2013). In other words, for those with low power, initial constraints on the free expression of creative ideas might be overcome with initial engagement with the creative task. ...
... Following the power prime, participants were introduced to a creative task, which was to generate novel ideas for a new candle scent following procedures used in previous research (Goncalo & Katz, 2020). Specifically, the participants read the following: "This next section is a task about product development. ...
... Idea novelty. Two independent coders scored the novelty of participants' candle scent ideas, following Goncalo and Katz (2020). Specifically, two trained coders who were blind to the experimental condition and predictions of the study coded each idea for novelty, which was defined as "the extent to which the idea does not resemble the typical candle scent". ...
... We argue that this occurs because the experience of being creative can, in and of itself, have positive psychological consequences (Sassenberg & Moskowitz, 2005;Goncalo, Vincent, & Krause, 2015;Khessina, Goncalo, & Krause, 2018). Engaging in the process of creative ideation entails the free and unfettered exploration of alternative ideas and perspectives (Goncalo & Katz, 2020)-a process of divergent thinking that can boost feelings of autonomy (Kim, Goncalo, & Rodas, 2023) and liberation from emotional and psychological constraints Kim, Zeppenfeld, & Cohen, 2013). In other words, for those with low power, initial constraints on the free expression of creative ideas might be overcome with initial engagement with the creative task. ...
... Following the power prime, participants were introduced to a creative task, which was to generate novel ideas for a new candle scent following procedures used in previous research (Goncalo & Katz, 2020). Specifically, the participants read the following: "This next section is a task about product development. ...
... Idea novelty. Two independent coders scored the novelty of participants' candle scent ideas, following Goncalo and Katz (2020). Specifically, two trained coders who were blind to the experimental condition and predictions of the study coded each idea for novelty, which was defined as "the extent to which the idea does not resemble the typical candle scent". ...
Preprint
Prior research suggests that having power makes individuals more creative, because the powerful are more willing to break with convention. We investigate the possibility that lower power individuals can also be creative when given the opportunity to warm up by completing a creative task more than once. In Study 1 (N = 153), we divided a creative ideation session into two consecutive rounds and found that low (vs. high) power individuals were less creative in the first round (replicating prior research), but low power individuals improved in the second round, attenuating the low power disadvantage. We replicated this effect in Study 2 (N = 121; pre-registered), with a different creativity task (i.e., structured imagination task) and expanded timeframe (i.e., five rounds instead of two). In Study 3 (N = 179; pre-registered), we again replicated the warm-up effect using two different creativity tasks that allowed us to rule out an alternative explanation. We conclude by discussing the theoretical implications of our findings for research on the dynamic effects of power on creativity and the practical implications for creativity, social equality, and education.
... We argue that this occurs because the experience of being creative can, in and of itself, have positive psychological consequences (Sassenberg & Moskowitz, 2005;Goncalo, Vincent, & Krause, 2015;Khessina, Goncalo, & Krause, 2018). Engaging in the process of creative ideation entails the free and unfettered exploration of alternative ideas and perspectives (Goncalo & Katz, 2020)-a process of divergent thinking that can boost feelings of autonomy (Kim, Goncalo, & Rodas, 2023) and liberation from emotional and psychological constraints Kim, Zeppenfeld, & Cohen, 2013). In other words, for those with low power, initial constraints on the free expression of creative ideas might be overcome with initial engagement with the creative task. ...
... Following the power prime, participants were introduced to a creative task, which was to generate novel ideas for a new candle scent following procedures used in previous research (Goncalo & Katz, 2020). Specifically, the participants read the following: "This next section is a task about product development. ...
... Idea novelty. Two independent coders scored the novelty of participants' candle scent ideas, following Goncalo and Katz (2020). Specifically, two trained coders who were blind to the experimental condition and predictions of the study coded each idea for novelty, which was defined as "the extent to which the idea does not resemble the typical candle scent". ...
Preprint
Heeding growing calls to investigate the downstream consequences of being creative for psychological well-being, we propose that the consequences of creativity can be a double-edged sword—boosting feelings of autonomy while at the same time triggering a fear of judgement. In three pre-registered experiments (N = 740), participants were asked to generate either creative or non-creative ideas. Participants in the creative (vs. non-creative) condition reported feeling a higher sense of autonomy while completing the task (Study 1). This feeling of autonomy emerged because participants instructed to generate creative ideas were able to cross multiple idea domains and cross idea boundaries during the process (Studies 2 and 3). However, creative ideation also increased evaluation apprehension because the freedom to think divergently affords the opportunity for choice, which heightens the fear of judgement (Study 3). We discuss the implications of our findings for the promise and peril of creative ideation as a psychological intervention to improve well-being.
... When consumers engage in creative thinking, they become more flexible in their thinking, such that they can better imagine how seemingly unrelated things can go together in a new way (Goncalo & Katz, 2020;Sassenberg et al., 2017). As a result, creative thinking helps consumers to develop ideas that break away from convention and norms (Burroughs et al., 2008). ...
... To begin, participants indicated their gender (male/female). In line with previous research, participants were then asked to complete a creative (control) product development task that involved coming up with potato chip flavors that were creative, novel, and unique (generic, conventional, and typical) (Goncalo & Katz, 2020; see Appendix B). Next, participants viewed a product based on their identified gender. ...
Article
Full-text available
Consumer embarrassment occurs frequently and can negatively impact both consumers and marketers. The current work demonstrates that encouraging consumers to engage in creative thinking—whether generating new ideas (e.g., crowdsourcing) or through exposure to creativity-related words—is one way for marketers to address the challenges posed by consumer embarrassment. Three studies demonstrate that prompting creative thinking makes consumers feel less embarrassed in subsequent consumer contexts. Specifically, the findings suggest that when consumers are prompted to think creatively, they assess behaviors that violate social norms as more socially acceptable, lowering feelings of embarrassment across a variety of consumption contexts. This research contributes to our understanding of the many benefits of engaging consumers in creative thinking and the growing stream of work exploring tactics companies can employ to help mitigate consumer embarrassment. This research also offers practical implications for both marketers and consumers.
... Creativity and creative ideation are not activities in which a few people engage but, rather, a fundamental aspect of daily life (Kaufman & Beghetto, 2009;Richards, 2007). Thus, there are growing calls to broaden research on the downstream psychological consequences of creative ideation for the individual (Foulk et al., 2022;Goncalo et al., 2015;Goncalo & Katz, 2020;Haselhuhn et al., 2022;Khessina et al., 2018;S. Kim et al., 2023;Sassenberg & Moskowitz, 2005). ...
Article
Full-text available
Given that creative ideation has been widely characterized as involving disinhibition, we tested whether a brief creative ideation effort increased subsequent indulgence through the choice of real or imagined rewards. Across 10 experiments (and an additional four in the Supplemental Material) and 3,412 participants (including the ones in the Supplemental Material), we show that a short creative ideation (vs. control) task led to more indulgent eating, drinking, and exercise choices and behaviors. Participants who generated creative ideas subsequently assembled burgers with higher calorie content (Studies 1a, 3a, 3b, 4), proposed cocktails with higher alcohol content (Study 1b), planned workouts that burned fewer calories (Study 1c), chose candy more often (Study 1d), and ate more candy (Study 2) than participants who engaged in control tasks. In line with the perspective that creativity involves disinhibition, these effects were mediated by the behavioral activation system, which is thought to underlie the uninhibited pursuit of desires and rewards (Studies 2, 3a, 4). Furthermore, an experiment in which we manipulated the behavioral activation system showed a causal effect on indulgent eating choices (Study 5). We discuss the implications for future research on the consequences of creativity as well as the possibility that creative work might lead to indulgent, potentially unhealthy choices and behaviors.
... At least three items were used to reflect each construct (Sarstedt et al., 2016). Measures for self-focus and cognitive reappraisal were adapted from Goncalo and Katz (2020) and John and Gross (2004), respectively. To assess the moral emotions of guilt, shame, and embarrassment, we adapted the scale from Han et al. (2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent studies explain how to detect fake reviews and their impacts on consumers and businesses. Despite the prevalence and significance of fake reviews, prior research has yet to fully explore the motivations and psychological mechanisms behind voluntary participation in writing fake reviews, especially in incentivized campaigns. This paper explains why consumers engage in the immoral act of writing fake reviews in return for free products. We investigate the interactive role of individuals' self‐focused motivations and cognitive reappraisal ability in diminishing consumers' moral emotions of guilt, subsequently influencing their intention to write incentivized reviews. Built on three studies, the findings reveal that customers' self‐focused motivations coupled with their ability to regulate emotions dissuade their feelings of guilt. Consequently, customers who experience downregulated emotions of guilt show a higher intention to write incentivized fake reviews. This study contributes to the existing literature by illustrating the driving factors toward writing incentivized fake reviews, especially by depicting the negative role of cognitive reappraisal. The study also has managerial implications indicating that such customer behavior can be avoided by fostering a sense of consideration toward others by making them aware about the impact of their actions.
Article
Full-text available
Existing research has suggested a predominantly negative view of dependency-oriented help. In contrast, the current research aims to test the positive function of dependency-oriented help in intimate relationships where interpersonal dependency is valued. We hypothesized that dependency-oriented help-seeking could function in communicating liking and romantic interests and, therefore, can be instrumental in attracting mates. Our hypothesis was confirmed across nine studies (N = 2,535). For help-seekers, a mate-seeking motivation could positively predict (Study 1) and lead to (Studies 2A–4) dependency-oriented help-seeking behavior tendencies (Studies 1–2B) and actual behavior (Studies 3 and 4). For help-providers, after activating a mating goal, imagining (Studies 5A and 5B) and actually receiving (Study 6) dependency-oriented (vs. autonomy-oriented) help-seeking requests from a potential mate increased help-providers’ romantic interests in that mate. Study 7 further showed the function of dependency-oriented help from the perspective of romantic competitors. As such, people in romantic relationships were more likely to see a potential competitor as a mate poacher when this person asked for dependency-oriented help from their partner. Theoretical and practical implications have been discussed.
Article
Full-text available
The creative process has the potential to increase wellbeing and foster human flourishing (Dolan and Metcalfe, 2012; Forgeard and Eichner, 2014; O’Brien and Murray, 2015; Conner et al., 2018; Kaufman, 2018), yet has received little attention in the humanistic management literature. In this paper, we present three experiments showing that idea originators experience greater relationship conflict with counterparts who have committed perceived “idea theft”, i.e., proposed identical or related ideas. We test a model that identifies a mechanism—self-threat—that mediates the relationship between idea theft and relationship conflict and identifies an intervention to ameliorate that self-threat via self-affirmation. Study 1 demonstrates the idea originator’s attribution of malign attributions to, and negative moral emotions toward, the counterpart. Study 2 demonstrates that idea originators ostracize both those who propose identical ideas and those who propose merely related ideas. In Study 3, evidence for self-threat as the mechanism underlying the negative relational consequences is provided via both a direct test of mediation as well as via moderation by self-affirmation. Our research contributes to the nascent idea theft literature and suggests a way to reduce its negative consequences.
Article
Full-text available
Self-disclosure plays a central role in the development and maintenance of relationships. One way that researchers have explored these processes is by studying the links between self-disclosure and liking. Using meta-analytic procedures, the present work sought to clarify and review this literature by evaluating the evidence for 3 distinct disclosure–liking effects. Significant disclosure–liking relations were found for each effect: (a) People who engage in intimate disclosures tend to be liked more than people who disclose at lower levels, (b) people disclose more to those whom they initially like, and (c) people like others as a result of having disclosed to them. In addition, the relation between disclosure and liking was moderated by a number of variables, including study paradigm, type of disclosure, and gender of the discloser. Taken together, these results suggest that various disclosure–liking effects can be integrated and viewed as operating together within a dynamic interpersonal system. Implications for theory development are discussed, and avenues for future research are suggested.
Article
Full-text available
The literatures on creativity and innovation are each premised on the same important assumption that has gone largely unquestioned: Creativity and innovation are outcomes that are almost inherently positive. Decades of research on creativity in organizations have been motivated by the assumption that creative ideas can be implemented to realize innovations that will inevitably increase profit, strengthen competitive advantage and ensure firm survival. The assumption that creativity and innovation have positive downstream consequences has constrained existing research by forcing a myopic focus on creativity and innovation as dependent variables. Thus, in a significant departure from the existing literature, we turn the tables to conceptualize creativity and innovation as independent variables that can have a sweeping and frequently negative impact on a wide range of other important outcomes. We conclude by calling for a new stream of research to more soberly evaluate the direct costs, side effects and long-term consequences of creativity and innovation.
Article
Full-text available
Previous studies have shown that creativity is enhanced by a broad attentional scope, defined as an ability to utilize peripheral stimuli and process information globally. We propose that the reverse relationship also holds, and that breadth of attention also is a consequence of engaging in a creative activity. In Study 1, participants showed increased breadth of attention in a visual scanning task after performing a divergent thinking task as opposed to an analytic thinking task. In Study 2, participants recognized peripheral stimuli displayed during the task better after performing a divergent thinking task as compared to an analytic task, whereas recognition performance of participants performing a task that involves a mix of divergent and analytic thinking (the Remote Associates Test) fell in between. Additionally, in Study 2 (but not in Study 1), breadth of attention was positively correlated with performance in a divergent thinking task, but not with performance in an analytic thinking task. Our findings suggest that the adjustment of the cognitive system to task demands manifests at a very basic, perceptual level, through changes in the breadth of visual attention. This paper contributes a new, motivational perspective on attentional breadth and discusses it as a result of adjusting cognitive processing to the task requirements, which contributes to effective self-regulation.
Article
Full-text available
Research has demonstrated the contribution of sexual activity to the quality of ongoing relationships. Nevertheless, less attention has been given to how activation of the sexual system affects relationship-initiation processes. Three studies used complementary methodologies to examine the effect of sexual priming on self-disclosure, a relationship-promoting behavior. In Study 1, participants were subliminally exposed to sexual stimuli (vs. neutral stimuli), and then disclosed over Instant Messenger a personal event to an opposite-sex stranger. Results showed that merely thinking about sex, even without being aware of it, encouraged self-disclosure. Study 2 replicated these findings in relatively naturalistic conditions (live face-to-face interactions following supraliminal video priming). Study 3 extended these findings, indicating that sexual priming facilitated self-disclosure, which, in turn, increased interest in future interactions with the stranger. Together, these findings suggest that activation of the sexual system encourages the use of strategies that allow people to become closer to potential partners.
Article
Leveraging insights gained through a burgeoning research literature over the past 28 years, this paper presents a significant revision of the model of creativity and innovation in organizations published in Research in Organizational Behavior in 1988. This update focuses primarily on the individual-level psychological processes implicated in creativity that have been illuminated by recent research, and highlights organizational work environment influences on those processes. We revisit basic assumptions underlying the 1988 model, modify certain components and causal connections, and introduce four new constructs into the model: (1) a sense of progress in creative idea development; (2) the meaningfulness of the work to those carrying it out; (3) affect; and (4) synergistic extrinsic motivation. Throughout, we propose ways in which the components underlying individual and team creativity can both influence and be influenced by organizational factors crucial to innovation.
Article
We review and develop two alternative conceptualizations of meaningful work: a predominant perspective we label realization and an underdeveloped, yet critical, perspective we label justification. We develop each conceptualization by identifying the core problem meaningful work is thought to address and accompanying solutions. Next, we build from this distinction to propose a research agenda that advances scholarly understanding of meaningful work from a justification perspective. Through building this research agenda, we elevate scholarly understanding on meaningful work by illuminating new foci for research, highlighting the relevance of social-cultural mechanisms, and suggesting alternative outcomes.