ArticlePDF Available
Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology
Volume 17, Number 3, 2018
Tools of the Mind: e Vygotskian-Based
Early Childhood Program
Elena Bodrova, PhD
Deborah J. Leong, PhD
Tools of the Mind, Denver, Colorado
e article describes Tools of the Mind—an instructional program developed 25 years
ago and now implemented in a variety of early childhood settings across the United
States and in Canada. Based on the principles of cultural-historical psychology, this
program addresses developmental and learning needs of young children by oering a
comprehensive curriculum and by delivering professional development for early child-
hood educators. e article provides examples of how Vygotskian and post-Vygotskian
ideas get embodied in Tools of the Mind instructional strategies with a special empha-
sis on make-believe play as a leading activity for preschool- and kindergarten-aged chil-
dren. e authors discuss the results of several evaluation studies conducted on Tools
and how these results helped to shape the current state of the program.
Keywords: Vygotsky; post-Vygotskian; Tools of the Mind; early childhood; executive
functions; self-regulation
Tools of the Mind (Tools) is a unique example of an early childhood instructional pro-
gram based on the principles of cultural-historical psychology that is implemented in the
United States. While American educational landscape had seen examples of Vygotskian-
based curricula designed for older students (e.g., Campione & Brown, 1990; Newman, Grin,
& Cole, 1989; arp & Gallimore, 1989) most of the previous attempts to use Vygotskian-based
pedagogy with younger children were limited to individual instructional strategies such as the
use of “Elkonin boxes” to teach phonemic awareness (Clay, 1993) or the use of measurement to
introduce the concept of number (Sophian, 2007).
In contrast to these interventions aimed at promoting isolated areas of child development,
Tools applies cultural-historical approach consistently throughout the entire curriculum—from
the design of specic activities and materials to the organization of student’s daily experiences
in a classroom to the use of dynamic assessment in ongoing monitoring of student progress.
e hallmark of Tools’ activities is the emphasis on children’s development of self-regulation.
Self-regulation (often referred to as intentionality or deliberateness by Vygotskian scholars) is
a mental facility necessary for the development of higher mental functions (Vygotsky, 1987)
and one of critical prerequisites for school success (Elkonin, 1977, 1978). In addition to
Pdf_Folio:223
© 2018 International Association for Cognitive Education and Psychology 223
http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/1945-8959.17.3.223
224 Bodrova and Leong
Vygotskian educational philosophy, Tools approach to promoting self-regulation is also
informed by Alexander Luria’s work on the genesis of voluntary actions as well as by the recent
developments in cognitive neuropsychology, particularly in the area of self-regulation/executive
functions.
In Tools classrooms, children are given multiple opportunities to practice self-regulation
in specic (“focal”) activities where self-regulation is a focus as well as in the activities where
self-regulation strategies are embedded in the context of academic tasks. At the heart of Tools
curriculum are instructional strategies designed to promote the development of mature make-
believe play since such play is considered by Vygotskians both the leading activity for preschool-
and kindergarten-aged children and the “school for deliberateness” (Elkonin, 1978, p. 287).
Unlike small-scale Vygotsky-based interventions delivered primarily by researchers (e.g.,
Brofman, Rabinovitch, & Karpov, 2018), Tools was from its inception designed as a comprehen-
sive curriculum to be used by regular classroom teachers in early childhood classrooms. First
developed and pilot tested in Denver, Colorado in 1990’s, Tools has expanded to many other
states and has been implemented in a variety of early childhood settings including public and
private preschools, Head Start, Even Start, as well as half-day and full-day kindergarten class-
rooms. Outside of the United States, Tools is now being used in Canada.
Developing Vygotskian-based Early Childhood curriculum in the context of American edu-
cational system presented us with a set of challenges: some of these (like dealing with dierent
educational philosophies and classroom practices) we expected and the others we had to face as
they emerged. Among the latter ones was a tendency to “push down” content and pedagogy ini-
tially designed for older children to kindergartners and now even preschoolers. is required us
to maintain a ne balance between ever growing demands of state and national academic stan-
dards and the goal to help young children develop cognitive and social-emotional competencies
that cultural-historical scholars consider developmental accomplishments unique to preschool
and kindergarten age (Elkonin, 1977; Karpov, 2005). For Tools it meant that none of the mate-
rials or activities could be simply adapted from the curricula designed by post-Vygotskian edu-
cators in Russia or in Europe but had to be built “from the ground up” in response to the specic
demands of the US early childhood classrooms. It also meant that to ensure successful imple-
mentation of the curriculum across the United States we had to align Tools with preschool and
kindergarten learning standards and assessments in 18 states and 160 school districts and pro-
grams. Finally, while many of the Vygotskian-based instructional practices were developed in
the context of fairly uniform system of state-run Soviet preschools ECE settings in the United
States vary greatly in the quality of classroom environment, in the duration of time children
spend in the classroom, as well as in the professional background of the teachers. Making Tools
work in these diverse settings and with diverse children they serve added to the complexity of
out endeavor.
Since implementing Tools often means for teachers a considerable departure from famil-
iar instructional practices as well as a paradigm shift in thinking about teaching and learning,
the professional development component is as crucial for the success of Tools as the curricu-
lum itself. Tools trainers have been delivering professional development and technical assis-
tance to thousands of teachers, teacher assistants, administrators, and support sta with their
educational levels varying from high school to advanced degrees. e Tools professional devel-
opment program also follows the Vygotskian approach, scaolding teacher learning through a
Pdf_Folio:224
Tools of the Mind: e Vygotskian-Based Early Childhood Program 225
system of coaching, workshops, and self-reection activities designed to provide teachers with
an understanding of child development and learning and to help them master specic instruc-
tional strategies.
Applying the Principles of Cultural-Historical Approach in an Early Childhood Classroom
Tools is grounded in the Vygotskian theory of development and learning. e curriculum also
incorporates the contributions of other cultural-historical scholars including Piotr Galperin,
Daniel Elkonin, Alexander Zaporozhets, and Leonid Venger. e contributions made by post-
Vygotskians to the eld of early childhood education could be epitomized in the idea of ampli-
cation of child development. e term “amplication” was rst coined by Vygotsky’s colleague
and the founder of the All-Soviet Institute for Preschool Education, Alexander Zaporozhets.
Zaporozhets proposed the idea of amplication as the answer to the push down curricula on one
hand and as an alternative to the notion of “spontaneous development” of young children on
the other. Amplication underscores the role of education in child development, emphasizing
that properly designed educational interactions within developmentally appropriate activities
do not interfere with the development of young children but instead promote it. e idea of
amplication, therefore, is the application of Vygotsky’s principle of instruction leading devel-
opment to Early Childhood education.
In Tools classrooms, carefully planned activities and instructional strategies are designed to
“amplify” children’s social-emotional and cognitive skills and to support children’s internaliza-
tion of mental tools—“tools of the mind.” Among the rst tools children learn is self-talk or “pri-
vate speech.” According to Vygotsky, private speech in young children is both an outgrowth of
“public speech” and a precursor to verbal thinking. Private speech supports the ability to think
at the time when children’s higher mental functions are not fully developed (Vygotsky, 1987).
As it was later found by Alexander Luria, (Luria, 1969) and then conrmed by many studies
within and outside Vygotskian framework, private speech has another important function: it
helps children regulate their behaviors, both overt and mental (Berk, 1992; Winsler, de Leon,
Wallace, Carlton, & Willson-Quayle, 2003). To promote the use of private speech Tools teachers
model its use as they engage children in complex multi-step activities. In addition, teachers sup-
port children’s use of “public speech” as a precursor to the private speech by replacing calling on
individuals with encouraging children to discuss their answers with each other.
Another kind of “tool” used by young children is external mediators, that is, objects that
assist in carrying out intentional behaviors (Vygotsky, 1978). Tools teachers introduce children
to a variety of such tools—from simple external mediators such as counters and manipulatives
to more sophisticated ones such as a number line. roughout dierent Tools activities, chil-
dren use specically designed external mediators that help them better remember the directions
for an activity or pay attention to the role they play in a specic activity. An example is Buddy
Reading where children “read” books to each other, which exercises self-regulation in turn tak-
ing and requires children to develop the ability to remain in the role of “reader” or “listener” for
the entire activity. To help children remember their roles, Tools teachers use cards with the pic-
tures of “lips” and “ears” reminding children that “lips read and ears listen.” With some practice,
children internalize the rules and become able to stay in their roles without the support of the
picture cards. As children learn to use these tools they become encouraged to create their own
symbolic representations (in drawing, scribbles, or in writing) that can be used to support their
memory or assist in their social interactions with their peers.
Pdf_Folio:225
226 Bodrova and Leong
According to the “law of development of higher mental functions” (Vygotsky, 1978)
children’s self-regulatory abilities rst originate in social interactions and only later become
internalized and independently used by children. is means that to develop self-regulation,
children need to have an opportunity to engage in “other-regulation.” ey must act both as sub-
jects of another person’s regulatory behaviors and as actors regulating other person’s behaviors.
In Tools classrooms, the primary context where children engage in other-regulation is make-
believe play (described below). In addition, many of Tools activities are designed in such a way
that children take turns with one child performing a task (e.g., counting objects) and the other
checking the result, consequently sharing the activity. By engaging children in this shared activ-
ity, Toolsaccomplishes two goals. First, it maximizes children’s engagement as children do not
wait for the teachers’ help but help each other or work as much as they can on their own. Sec-
ond, by assuming strategically identied roles (such as “doer” and “checker”) children learn to
co-regulate by acting and then monitoring and evaluating the actions of their partner and thus
learning the criteria that they will apply to themselves. Eventually children internalize these
regulatory actions that they will apply to their own work. In respect to the development of self-
regulation and executive functions, such activities build cognitive and social competencies that
are hallmarks of self-regulated learning (Schunk, 1999). In addition, these activities provide
optimal contexts for children to practice perspective taking and develop insights into theory
of mind that are associated with the development of executive functions and social-emotional
competence (Carlson, Mandell, & Williams, 2004; Hughes & Ensor, 2007).
e Vygotskian principle of social mediation of learning is operationalized in Tools through
establishing adult-child and child-child interactions. Social mediation in Tools classroom takes
place in the form of adult- or peer-provided scaolding and in the form of mature make-believe
play children engage in with their peers.
Scaolding interactions are used in the course of formal or informal instruction to help a
child move from being assisted by an adult or by a more competent peer in performing a new
task to being able to perform this and similar tasks independently (Bodrova & Leong, 2007).
ese interactions must fall within each individual child’s Zone of Proximal Development so that
they would support the very skills and knowledge that are on the edge of emergence (Vygotsky,
1978). When providing scaolding, a teacher or a peer tutor does not make the task easier but
instead makes the learner’s job easier by giving the child maximum support in the beginning
stages and then gradually withdrawing this support as the child’s mastery of a new skill increases
(Bruner, 1985). us, eective scaolding provides only temporary support and is withdrawn
once these new mental processes and categories are fully developed and can be used by the child
without any outside assistance.
Not all scaolding is created equal: an appropriate scaold is the one that not only makes
it easier for a child to complete a current task or brings to the surface behaviors most mature
to date, but plays a role in the child’s “construction of mind,” inuencing the development of
mental categories and processes responsible for the child’s performance on a variety of tasks.
From Vygotskian perspective, scaolding may exist in dierent formats ranging from teacher-
child interactions when they work on a task jointly, to teacher introducing child to a strategy or
a “tool” the child will be later able to use on his/her own, to the teacher planning for a specic
context or environment where the child will be supported by other children (Bodrova & Leong,
2007; Campione & Brown, 1990; Wood, Bruner, & Ross, 1976). In Tools classrooms, teachers
provide scaolding in a variety of formats and across various contexts not limited to academic
tasks and introduce children to a variety of “tools” that promote their development.Pdf_Folio:226
Tools of the Mind: e Vygotskian-Based Early Childhood Program 227
As a comprehensive curriculum, Tools contains multiple activities targeting all major areas
of child development and intended to be used in large group, in small groups, as well as by pairs
of children and by individual children. One example of a large group activity is Story Lab—
the Tools version of read-aloud. Unlike a typical read-aloud activity, Story Lab is designed for
children to practice one comprehension strategy—from visualization to prediction to character
empathy—at a time with an picture card reminding children what to focus on as they are listen-
ing to a story.
Many of the small group activities are designed to be played by children as a game with min-
imal or no support by the teacher. In these games, children practice literacy and math concepts
while at the same time learning to follow increasingly complex sets of rules. All games included
in the Tools curriculum share some common characteristics that make these games compatible
each other but only with their own past results and children are not prohibited but are instead
encouraged to help each other when somebody does not know the answer. An example of such a
game is Number Line Hopscotch where children jump from one numbered carpet square to the
next following various instructions (starting from # 5, in reverse order, etc.).
e paired activities such as Buddy Reading and Numerals Game are designed in such a way
that children take turns acting out dierent roles (see the description of “doer and checker” and
“lips and ears” above). Many of these paired activities also have an imaginary component: for
example, in Market Farm activity, a child spells a word letter-by-letter as she orders items from
the farm to sell at the market while her partner tries to match each word with the picture of the
corresponding object.
All Tools activities are designed to be multi-level so that children of varying abilities can
engage in social interactions with each other over the same content. is design prevents ability
grouping and promotes the sense of community in all children. Tools teachers learn to support
children of dierent abilities within the same activity by providing dierentiated scaolding
to all of them. is makes Tools curriculum an ideal t for multi-age classrooms as well as the
inclusion classrooms. Another distinct feature of all Tools activities is that these are designed
to “grow with a child” from early preschool years into kindergarten constantly presenting chil-
dren with new levels of challenges. is makes activities and their variations too numerous to
describe in this article so we limited the description of Tools classroom to just two examples to
illustrate how Vygotskian principles are embedded in daily experiences of preschool and kinder-
garten children.
Pdf_Folio:227
Supporting Make-Believe Play as the Leading Activity of Preschool and
Kindergarten-Aged Children
For Vygotskians, certain children’s activities produce the greatest gains in their development.
ese “leading activities” are age-specic and provide the best conditions for the acquisition
of cognitive and social competencies most critical for child development at this age (Chaiklin,
Hedegaard, & Jensen, 1999; Karpov, 2005). For children of preschool- and kindergarten age,
make-believe play is such leading activity while the primary-grades students engage in the activ-
ity of intentional academic learning (frequently referred to as the “learning activity”) (Elkonin,
1977; Leont’ev, 1978). Vygotskians cite multiple benets of make-believe play focusing espe-
cially on play’s contributions to children’s developing symbolic thinking and self-regulation. In
Tools
with the goal of building classroom community: children and teams do not compete with
228 Bodrova and Leong
their writings, however Vygotsky and his colleagues limited their denition of play to the dra-
matic or make-believe play and did not include such activities as object manipulations and explo-
rations that are precursors to play in toddlers and younger preschoolers. Neither did they include
such activities as games and sports considering these to be an outgrowth of play in children of
school age.
Unlike many of Western scholars Vygotskians do not believe that play develops sponta-
neously in all children once they reach preschool age but rather associate the level of play sophis-
tication with the certain features of a child’s social situation of development, namely the adult
mediation of play. If the opportunity to learn how to play was absent in children’s prior expe-
rience these children would benet from scaolding, however teachers should not attempt to
turn child play into teacher-directed activity (Elkonin, 1978; Mikhailenko & Korotkova, 1990).
While Vygotsky himself did not specify what kind of play makes it a leading activity, some
of his students indicate that children need to reach a certain level of play in order for it to start
having its benecial impact on child development. In his book Psychology of Play, Daniel Elkonin
(1978, 2005) introduced the idea of “mature” (also called “advanced” or “fully developed”) play
emphasizing that only this kind of play can be a source of development in early childhood. When
playing at a mature level, children create an imaginary situation; use objects-substitutes as play
props, take on and sustain pretend roles, and follow rules determined by a pretend scenario and
by a chosen character in particular.
Unfortunately play that exists in many of today’s early childhood classrooms across the
world often does not reach this level of maturity: even 5- and 6-year-old children who accord-
ing to Vygotsky and Elkonin should be at the peak of their play performance play only with
realistic props, enact play scenarios that are stereotypical and primitive, and display a reper-
toire of themes and roles that is rather limited (Miller & Almon, 2009; Smirnova & Gudareva,
2004). Researchers attribute this apparent decline in play to changes in the culture of childhood
such as the disappearance of multi-age groups with older children serving as play mentors to
the younger ones, and the increase in time children spend in adult-led activities or playing with
gadgets.
With modern culture seldom giving young children an opportunity to develop mature play by
serving as “play apprentices” to older children, early childhood educators have to assume a new
position of play mentors. True to the Vygotskian approach, promoting mature play became of
on the main directions of Tools with a special focus on adult’s role in scaolding children’s play.
Scaolding is designed to support the main components of mature play such as using toys and
props in a symbolic way; developing consistent and extended play scenarios; taking on a pretend
role and staying in it for an extended play episode or a series of play episodes; and consistently
following the rules determining what each pretend character can or cannot do.
An essential part of scaolding is helping children to plan their play. Elkonin (1978) identi-
ed planning as one of the features of mature play, describing the developmental trajectory of
play as moving from abbreviated planning followed by extended acting out to extended plan-
ning followed by abbreviated acting out. As with other components of play, role and scenario
planning can benet from adult scaolding. In Tools preschool classrooms children engage in a
planning session preceding their playtime. e teacher starts by asking children what they want
to play or what they want to be, encouraging them to discuss the choice of the roles with their
peers. By making planning a necessary step in play, the Tools teachers direct children’s attention
to the specics of their roles and to the existence of rules associated with them. e planning
process rst takes place orally; later in the year, children are encouraged to represent their plansPdf_Folio:228
Tools of the Mind: e Vygotskian-Based Early Childhood Program 229
in drawing or pretend writing. As children’s play becomes more mature, teacher scaolding fades
away and children initiate discussions of their future play on their own leading to longer play
episodes with more elaborate pretend scenarios.
By kindergarten age, most children are expected to develop the level of make-believe play
that allows them to engage in a dierent kind of play—games with rules—where they abide
by decontextualized and often arbitrary rules (Leont’ev, 1981; Kravtsova, 1999). Unlike make-
believe play that is open-ended, games with rules make children focus on the outcome and plan
their actions accordingly. Experience with playing games with rules helps children develop plan-
ning and monitoring abilities essential for performing learning tasks. Unfortunately, many chil-
dren entering today’s kindergarten do not have higher levels of play that have been associated
with the development of self-regulation (Berk, Mann, & Ogan, 2006; Elkonin, 1978) and are
therefore unprepared to conform their actions to mandatory rules and norms of a classroom
(Miller & Almon, 2009; Ugaste, 2007). erefore, teachers in the Tools kindergarten classrooms
have to support play in two ways: (a) by continuing to promote mature and intentional make-
believe play as a part of daily activities and (b) by facilitating children’s transition from make-
believe play to playing games with rules.
To support make-believe play while at the same time building listening comprehension skills
in kindergarten children, Tools implements a series of dramatization activities where children
act out stories from chapter books read to them during the read-aloud time. After reading a
story, the teacher takes children through a series of exercises designed to help them see the
perspectives of dierent characters in the story, identify and summarize main events, think
about the beginning middle and end of stories, and to predict alternate endings. Later, during
center time, children act out the storyline, and are encouraged to make a prediction for the next
chapter. Children draw and write their predictions and act them out and after listening to a new
chapter compare their predictions with what actually happened in the book.
To facilitate kindergartner’s transition from make-believe play to playing games with rules,
Tools teachers engage children in learning games with increasingly complex rules. ese games
are designed to support learning of specic academic content along with other competencies
such as self-regulation or perspective taking. Similar to the games used in Tools preschool, many
of kindergarten games include external mediators (such as a magician’s hat in a counting game)
that help children sustain a specic role.
Pdf_Folio:229
Scaolded Writing—A Key Component in Tools of the Mind Literacy Program
Scaolded Writing is a Tools method to teach writing that exemplies the application of the
Vygotskian approach to a novel classroom situation that had not been previously addressed
within the cultural-historical framework. In addition to following Vygotsky’s (1997) views on
children’s mastery of written language, this method incorporates specic strategies used by
Vygotskians in designing curricula for preschool and primary grade students.
Discussing the pedagogy of teaching writing, Vygotsky (1997) emphasized that “teaching
must be set up so that reading and writing satisfy the child’s need” and that the goal of the
instruction should be “to teach a child written language and not writing the alphabet.” At the
same time, Vygotsky advocated early (at age 3-4) instruction in writing that emphasizes the
communicative and instrumental function of written language and not the mechanics of its pro-
duction. True to his own belief that good instruction should lead development and not follow
Pdf_Folio:7
230 Bodrova and Leong
it, Vygotsky explained the value of learning to write early not in the context of preparing chil-
dren for formal schooling, but in the broader context of using cultural tools for supporting the
development of higher mental functions.
is view of teaching writing stands in sharp contrast to the instructional practices com-
monly used in American classrooms where children are taught isolated sub-skills such as letter
formation and spelling in the contexts determined by a published curriculum and not by “the
child’s need.” As a result, many children continue to struggle with learning to write in spite of
many hours spent practicing often at the cost of play and other activities essential for their
development.
Unlike these practices, Tools rst introduces writing in the context of the activity that is
the most meaningful for young children: make-believe play. In preschool, 3- and 4-year old chil-
dren make their play plans by using scribbles and drawings. is also follows Vygotsky’s (1997)
insight about the genetic roots of writing being drawing and play. Tools teachers are encouraged
to help children elevate the level of their play and to make their drawings more representational
before focusing on writing letters.
While still supporting children’s oral message and drawing, teachers begin modeling writing
as they “record” children’s oral messages by drawing a line for each word in the message. After the
oral message gets “recorded” both the child and the teacher repeat the message pointing to each
line as he or she says the word. Once children develop consistent one-to-one correspondence
between the words in the oral message and the lines, teachers begin writing on these lines, ask-
ing a child to prompt each word in a sequence. Soon, children begin to plan their own messages
with the help of the lines, but at this time they still need teachers’ help in writing the words.
As children learn sound-symbol correspondences for the most frequent letters, they attempt to
write some of the words independently, placing one or more letters on each line. See Figure 1
for an example of the use of Scaolded Writing by a preschooler.
Tools kindergarten teachers also use Scaolded Writing with their students gradually
increasing the number of contexts for writing. Some of these contexts still involve play (e.g.,
when children write a chapter summary to act it out in a center). Children also write plans for
their center activities; write down riddles and tongue twisters (to later “torment” their parents
and siblings) and correct errors in the writing of a ctitious mouse, which they believe to live in
their classroom. All these activities solidify children’s understanding of writing as a tool of com-
municating to oneself and to the others. As a result, children in the Tools classrooms develop
and sustain interest in writing and surpass their peers in mastering literacy concepts and skills.
See Figure 2 for an example of the use of Scaolded Writing by a kindergarten student.
RESEARCH ON TOOLS OF THE MIND: RESULTS AND CHALLENGES
From the very beginning, Tools has been an evidence-based curriculum and all of its materials
and activities were not only research-based but also piloted and rened with the help of class-
room teachers. e early studies employed primarily microgenetic design focusing in-depth on
the changes in a single process such as writing as it had been systematically scaolded (Bodrova
& Leong, 1995, 1998). ese microgenetic studies were used to examine whether a specic
instructional procedure produced changes in the child’s behavior. ese were focused on improv-
ing procedures and the design of instruction.
e rst evaluation study was quasi-experimental in design and compared preschool and
kindergarten classrooms that implemented Tools strategies with non-Tools classrooms. AtPdf_Folio:230
Tools of the Mind: e Vygotskian-Based Early Childhood Program 231
FIGURE 1. An example of a preschooler’s use of Scaolded Writing on his play plan. e message
reads: I am going to y the helicopter.
that time, only few Tools strategies were developed, however the dierences between chil-
dren in treatment and comparison groups were signicant in several areas of development
which resulted in Tools being nominated as the rst innovative early childhood programs to be
included in the UNESCO’s international database INODATA (Bodrova & Leong, 2001).
Pdf_Folio:231
232 Bodrova and Leong
FIGURE 2. An example of a kindergartner’s use of Scaolded Writing in a fact book. It reads:
Trees in the rain forest make medicine. Coco beans grow in the rain forest.
e rst formal evaluation of the eectiveness of Tools was conducted by the National Insti-
tute for Early Education Research (NIEER) in a study where teachers and their students were
randomly assigned to either Tools or control classrooms (Barnett et al., 2008). e study was
conducted in a school district with a high level of poverty and a predominantly non-English-
speaking population. All of the classrooms were state-funded preschool classrooms with the
control group implementing an established, district-created model described as a “balanced lit-
eracy curriculum with themes.” Children (88 in Tools classrooms and 122 in control classrooms
ages 3 and 4) were compared on social behavior, language, and literacy growth. e Tools cur-
riculum was found to improve classroom quality and children’s self-regulation, as indicated by
lower scores on the Problem Behavior dimension of the Social Skills Rating Scale. In addition,
there were gains in language development, however these eects were smaller and did not reach
conventional levels of statistical signicance. Teachers trained in Tools scored higher on the
CLASS instrument in classroom management, use of classroom time, and appropriate engage-
ment interactions that challenged children to learn at the next level.
Tools’ impact on children’s self-regulation/executive functions was the focus of a sepa-
rate study that used a sub-sample of children participating in the Barnett et al. (2008) study
described above. In this study (Diamond, Barnett, omas, & Munro, 2007), children with
one year of Tools and two years of Tools were compared with children with similar demo-
graphic characteristics who had no Tools experience. All children were administered computer-
ized tests designed to measure their executive functions—the neuropsychological correlate ofPdf_Folio:232
Tools of the Mind: e Vygotskian-Based Early Childhood Program 233
self-regulation. At the time of the study all children attended kindergarten and were an average
of 5 years of age. To test executive functions, children were assessed on the Hearts and Flow-
ers (Dots) and Flanker tests. e results showed that on the test trials requiring minimal use of
executive functions(EF), children in the Tools and control conditions performed the same how-
ever in those conditions that taxed EF, children in Tools did signicantly better than controls.
Further analyses comparing the child’s scores on the two EF tests and the academic achieve-
ment measures collected on the Tools children found that the higher the level of EF, the higher
the achievement scores. In addition, children’s results on the EF measures correlated with the
teachers’ ratings of their behavior on the Social Skills Rating Scale. Another interesting outcome
of this study that was not formally measured but was however reported by the data collectors:
Tools graduates seemed to demonstrate higher task persistence, i.e. children kept asking the
testers to let them try one more time after the test was over and they felt they made errors.
In contrast, children from the control group got easily discouraged after they made a couple of
errors and did not want to keep trying.
In the years following the Barnett et al. study, there were several randomized controlled
studies (RCTs) where the entire Tools curriculum or some of the elements of the program were
compared with other early childhood curricula (Clements, Sarama, Unlu, & Layzer, 2012; Loni-
gan & Phillips, 2012; Morris et al., 2014; Wilson & Farran, 2012). In all of these studies children
in the Tools classrooms showed the same yearly progress on academic measures as children in
the control classrooms. In addition, children in the treatment and control conditions did not
dier on the measures of self-regulation, although self-regulation measures used in these stud-
ies were dierent from the ones used in the Diamond’s study of 2007.
e results from these four studies led to a redesign of the Tools program for both preschool
and kindergarten. e earlier (2001-2008) evaluations of Tools occurred on a much smaller scale
with the developers doing all of the training. e four studies described above overlapped with
the contracts Tools was executing simultaneously, so the sheer number of teachers being trained
at the same time (over 2,000) meant that the lack of formal structure to training as well as
resources for trainers led to diculties with scaling that were not anticipated. Consequently,
inconsistency in the quality of training. Issues associated with rapid scaling and with dening
the delity of implementation were not worked out in advance leading to problems in training
and in some instances with compromised delity. Resulting changes in the training and the
development of the iScaold (a technology based training platform) have led to more positive
recent results.
Changes in training led to an ecacy study showing stronger results. Researchers compared
academic achievement, behavioral self-regulation as well as the performance on EF tasks of
kindergarten students that were or were not enrolled in Tools (Blair & Raver, 2014) is time,
the results were consistent with earlier ndings in Diamond et al. (2007) and Barnett et al
(2008) studies: in addition to showing better results on a battery of EF tasks, children enrolled in
Tools outperformed their peers on literacy and math tests with strongest eects in high poverty
schools. Most compelling is the data showing that students carried these gains into rst grade,
with even higher results in reading and vocabulary. ese stronger outcomes may reect the
fact that the development of executive functions actually improves how children learn, mak-
ing learning more eective, even beyond kindergarten. e long-lasting eects of Tools train-
ing were also evident in the area of behavioral self-regulation, with children in Tools classrooms
demonstrated higher levels of social-emotional competence, lower rates of aggression, and lower
occurrence of behavior problems compared to their peers in control classrooms. Similar to thePdf_Folio:233
234 Bodrova and Leong
achievement gains, these behavioral outcomes were also carried into rst grade where Tools cur-
riculum was no longer implemented (Blair, McKinnon, & Daneri, 2018).
A second kindergarten study with matched controls was conducted in Canada (Diamond,
Lee, Senften, Lam, & Abbott, submitted). In this study, children in 9 Tools classrooms also sur-
passed their peers on the measures of pro-social behavior, achievement, and executive functions.
Relative to controls, children in Tools classrooms had signicantly higher reading, writing, their
ability to work independently and cooperatively, and their ability to get along with, and be kind
and helpful to, each other
Positive impact of Tools training on children’s EF was also demonstrated in a recent exper-
imental study conducted in Canada. In this study, preschool children attending Tools class-
rooms were compared to their peers participating in another play-based curriculum. Children in
Tools classrooms made greater gains on the behavioral measure of executive functions and the
eect was most signicant for children initially rated high in inattentiveness and hyperactivity
(Solomon et al., 2017). Qualitative study of teachers’ perceptions of Tools eects on children also
emphasizes the value of Tools in promoting cognitive and social competencies in preschool and
kindergarten students. Among most signicant outcomes of implementing Tools these teach-
ers listed fewer classroom behavior problems, more collaborative behaviors, and higher levels of
communication and verbalization (Imholz & Petrosino, 2012).
While many children in Tools studies were dual language learners (DLLs), the specic eects
of Tools training on DLLs has been studied only recently. One experimental study conducted
in two large urban Head Start program focused on the eects of Tools on academic growth in
children with varying degrees of mastery of English and Spanish. e ndings in this study
showed positive eects on children’s English math and literacy abilities as well as Spanish vocab-
ulary. In addition, for most outcomes, Spanish-dominant children demonstrated larger growth
in academic ability than their English-dominant peers (Daneri, Blair, Hammer, & Lopez, 2017).
A smaller nonexperimental study identied the best conditions for implementing Tools instruc-
tional strategies in a bilingual classroom. In this study, bilingual instruction during storybook
reading followed by literature-based dramatic play resulted in greater gains in English pro-
ciency in DLL preschoolers compared to English-only and nonplay conditions (Cohen, Kramer-
Vida, Frye, & Andreou, 2014).
High mobility of children typically enrolled in Tools classrooms makes it dicult to obtain
data on long-term eects of the program, although testimonials of teachers and administrators
indicate that these eects are positive. e only study that reports such data was conducted in
a high poverty school district that had been implementing Tools for many years. e purpose
of this study was to examine the sustained impact of participation in the Tools preschool pro-
gram on language arts, mathematics, reading, and writing achievement in middle school. Using
a nonexperimental, quantitative, longitudinal design, the achievement of the original cohort of
students who participated in the program was examined over two consecutive years. e results
of the study suggest that participation in the Tools program increases the overall achievement
of socioeconomically disadvantaged students and the writing performance of African American
students (Millaway, 2015).
Tools is a living program and as such it continues to evolve addressing new challenges and
attempting to solve new problems all the while staying true to the principles of the cultural-
historical approach. After successfully applying Vygotskian views of written speech to teaching
writing, we are now applying the same approach to teaching reading. Our rst experience of
using it with kindergarten children is encouraging: when reading “satises the child’s need” toPdf_Folio:234
Tools of the Mind: e Vygotskian-Based Early Childhood Program 235
learn new information and to share it with their friends children spend much more time reading
and read well above their grade level. With further renement of our program we hope to add
one more “tool of the mind” to young children’s toolboxes.
REFERENCES
Barnett, W. S., Jung, K., Yarosz, D., omas, J., Hornbeck, A., Stechuk, R., & Burns, S. M. (2008). Educa-
tional eects of the tools of the mind curriculum: A randomized trial. Early Childhood Research Quar-
terly,23, 299–313. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2008.03.001
Berk, L. E. (1992). Children’s private speech: An overview of theory and the status of research. In R. M.
Diaz & L. E. Berk (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp. 17–53). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Berk, L. E., Mann, T. D., & Ogan, A. T. (2006). Make-believe play: Wellspring for development of self-
regulation. In D. G. Singer, R. M. Golinko, & K . Hirsh-Pasek (Eds.), Play = learning: How play motivates
and enhances children’s cognitive and social-emotional growth (pp. 74–100). New York, NY: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Blair, C., McKinnon, R. D., & Daneri, M. P. (2018). Eect of the tools of the mind kindergarten pro-
gram on children’s social and emotional development. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,43, 52–61.
doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.01.002
Blair, C., & Raver, C. C. (2014). Closing the achievement gap through modication of neurocognitive and
neuroendocrine function: Results from a cluster randomized controlled trial of an innovative approach
to the education of children in kindergarten. PLOS ONE,9(11), e112393.
Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (1995). Scaolding the writing process: e Vygotskian approach. Colorado
Reading Council Journal,6, 27–29.
Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (1998). Scaolding emergent writing in the zone of proximal development.
Literacy Teaching and Learning,3(2), 1–18.
Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (2001). Tools of the mind: A case study of implementing the Vygotskian approach
in American early childhood and primary classrooms. Innodata Monographs, 7. Geneva, Switzerland:
UNESCO International Bureau of Education.
Bodrova, E., & Leong, D. J. (2007). Tools of the mind (2nd ed.). Columbus, OH: Merrill/Prentice Hall.
Brofman, V., Rabinovitch, I., & Karpov, Y. (2018). Vygotsky in Harlem: Amplication of preschoolers’
development in Vygotskian early childhood education program (VECEP). In N. Veraksa & S. Sheridan
(Eds.), Vygotsky’s theory in ea rly childhood education and research: Russian and western values (pp. 81–96).
New York, NY: Routledge.
Bruner, J. (1985). Vygotsky: A historical and conceptual perspective. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), Culture, com-
munication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives (pp. 21–34). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Campione, J. C., & Brown, A. L. (1990). Guided learning and transfer. In N. Fredricksen, R. Glaser, A.
Lesgold, & M. G. Shafto (Eds.), Diagnostic monitoring of skill and knowledge acquisition (pp. 141–172).
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Carlson, S. M., Mandell, D. J., & Williams, L. (2004). Executive function and theory of mind: Stabil-
ity and prediction from ages 2 to 3. Developmental Psychology,40, 1105–1122. doi:10.1037/0012-
1649.40.6.1105
Chaiklin, S., Hedegaard, M., & Jensen, U. J. (Eds.). (1999). Activity theory and social practice: Cultural-
historical approach. Aarhus, Denmark: Aarhus University Press.
Clay, M. (1993). Reading recovery. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Clements, D. H., Sarama, J., Unlu, F., & Layzer, C. (2012, March). e ecacy of an intervention synthesizing
scaolding designed to promote self-regulation with an early mathematics curriculum: Eects on executive
function. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Research on Educational Eective-
ness, Washington, DC.Pdf_Folio:13
236 Bodrova and Leong
Cohen, L. E., Kramer-Vida, L., Frye, N., & Andreou, M. (2014). e eect of bilingual instruc-
tion and play on preschoolers’ English prociency. International Journal of Play,3(1), 36–52.
doi:10.1080/21594937.2013.876573
Daneri, M. P., Blair, C., Hammer, C., & Lopez, L. (2017, April). Eect of the tools of the mind prekindergarten
program on the school readiness of English language learners in head start. Paper presented at the biennial
meeting for the Society for Research in Child Development, Austin, TX.
Diamond, A., Barnett, S., omas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves cognitive control.
Science,318, 1387–1388. doi:10.1126/science.1151148
Diamond, A., Lee, C., Senften, P., Lam, A., & Abbott, D. (submitted). Randomized control trial of Tools of
the Mind: Marked benets to kindergarten children and their teachers. PLOS ONE.
Elkonin, D. (1977). Toward the problem of stages in the mental development of the child. In M. Cole (Ed.),
Soviet developmental psychology (pp. 538–563). White Plains, NY: M. E. Sharpe.
Elkonin, D. (1978). Psychologija igry [e psychology of play]. Moscow, Russia: Pedagogika.
Elkonin, D. (2005). e psychology of play [Chapter 2: e subject of our research: e developed form of
play], (trans. Lydia Razran Stone). Journal of Russian and East European Psychology,43, 22–48.
Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2007). Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to
4. Developmental Psychology,43, 1447–1459. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1447
Imholz, S., & Petrosino, A. (2012). Teacher observations on the implementation of the tools of the mind
curriculum in the classroom: Analysis of interviews conducted over a one-year period. Creative Educa-
tion,3(02), 185. doi:10.4236/ce.2012.32029
Karpov, Y. V. (2005). e neo-Vygotskian approach to childd evelopment. New York, NY: Cambridge University
Press.
Kravtsova, E. E. (1999). Preconditions for developmental learning activity at pre-school age. In M. Hede-
gaard & J. Lompscher (Eds.), Learning activity and development (pp. 220–245). Aarhus, Denmark:
Aarhus University Press.
Leont’ev, A. (1978). Activity, consciousness, and personality. Englewood Clis, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Leont’ev, A. N. (1981). Problemy razvitiya psikhiki [Problems of psychic development]. Moscow: Moscow
State University Publishers.
Lonigan, C. J., & Phillips, B. M. (2012, March). Comparing skills-focused and self-regulation focused preschool
curricula: Impacts on academic and self-regulatory skills. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
Society for Research on Educational Eectiveness, Washington, DC.
Luria, A. R . (1969). Speech development and the formation of mental processes. In M. Cole & I. Maltzman
(Eds.), Handbook of contemporary Soviet psychology (pp. 121–162). New York, NY: Basic Books.
Mikhailenko, N. Ya., & Korotkova, N. A. (1990). Kak igrat s rebenkom (How to play with a child). Moscow:
Pedagogika.
Millaway, S. A. (2015). Impact of tools of the mind on middle school achievement. Doctoral dissertation.
Jersey City, NJ: Saint Peters University. Retrieved from http://librarydb.saintpeters.edu: 8080/han-
dle/123456789/193
Miller, E., & Almon, J. (2009). Crisis in the kindergarten: Why children need play in school. College Park, MD:
Alliance for Childhood.
Morris, P., Mattera, S., Castells, N., Bangser, M., Bierman, K., & R aver, C. C. (2014). Impact ndings from the
head start CARES demonstration: National evaluation of three approaches to improving preschoolers’ social
and emotional competence. OPRE Report 2014-44. Washington, DC: OPRE.
Newman, D., Grin, P.,& Cole, M. (1989). e construction zone: Working for cognitive change in school. Cam-
bridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
Schunk, D. (1999). Social self-interaction and achievement behavior. Educational Psychologist,34, 219–228.
doi:10.1207/s15326985ep3404_3
Smirnova, E. O., & Gudareva, O. V. (2004). Igra i proizvol’nost u sovremennykh doshkol’nikov [Play and
intentionality in modern preschoolers]. Voprosy Psychologii,1, 91–103.
Pdf_Folio:236
Tools of the Mind: e Vygotskian-Based Early Childhood Program 237
Solomon, T. L. S., Plamondon, A., O’Hara, A., Finch, H., Goco, G., Chaban, P., & Tannock, R. (2017). A C lus-
ter randomized-controlled trial of the impact of the tools of the mind curriculum on self-regulation in
Canadian preschoolers. Frontiers in Psychology,8, 2366. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02366
Sophian, C. (2007). e origins of mathematical knowledge in childhood. London: Routledge.
arp, R. G., & Gallimore, R. (1989). Rousing minds to life: Teaching, learning, and schooling in social context.
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
Ugaste, A. (2007). e cultural-historical approach to play in the kindergarten context. In T. Jambor &
J. Gils (Eds.), Several perspectives on children’s play: Scientic reections for practitioners (pp. 105–118).
Philadelphia, PA: Garrant.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: e development of higher mental processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). inking and speech (N. Minick, Trans.). (Vol. 1). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1997). e history of the development of higher mental functions (M. J. Hall, Trans.). (Vol. 4).
New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Wilson, S. J., & Farran, D. C. (2012, March). Experimental evaluation of the tools of the mind preschool curricu-
lum. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Research on Educational Eectiveness,
Washington, DC.
Winsler, A., de Leon, R. J. Wallace, B. A., Carlton, M. P., & Willson-Quayle, A. (2003). Private speech in
preschool children: Developmental stability and change, across-task consistency, and relations with
classroom behaviour. Journal of Child Language,30, 583–608. doi:10.1017/S0305000903005671
Wood, D., Bruner, J. C., & Ross, G. (1976). e role of tutoring in problem solving. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry,79, 89–100.
Disclosure. e authors have no relevant nancial interest or aliations with any commercial interests
related to the subjects discussed within this article.
Correspondence regarding this article should be directed to Elena Bodrova, Lakewood, Colorado. E-mail:
ebodrova@toolsofthemind.org
Pdf_Folio:237
CopyrightofJournalofCognitiveEducation&PsychologyisthepropertyofSpringer
PublishingCompany,Inc.anditscontentmaynotbecopiedoremailedtomultiplesitesor
postedtoalistservwithoutthecopyrightholder'sexpresswrittenpermission.However,users
mayprint,download,oremailarticlesforindividualuse.
... Thus, identifying school-based interventions that foster preschooler's EF skills has been an area of considerable focus. One of the more prominent early childhood interventions focused on the development of EF skills is Tools of the Mind (Tools; Bodrova and Leong, 2019). Based on Vygotsky's theory of development, Tools is a classroom curriculum enhancement designed to emphasize the role of teachers in facilitating children's play (Bodrova and Leong, 2019). ...
... One of the more prominent early childhood interventions focused on the development of EF skills is Tools of the Mind (Tools; Bodrova and Leong, 2019). Based on Vygotsky's theory of development, Tools is a classroom curriculum enhancement designed to emphasize the role of teachers in facilitating children's play (Bodrova and Leong, 2019). Through Tools training teachers learn how to help children develop a plan for play, as well as how to help them self-monitor, solve problems, and carry out their plan during play settings. ...
... Results from this phase revealed increased performance on measures of pre-literacy. In this initial study, Bodrova and Leong (2019) found that teachers with the strongest results were the ones who had higher degrees of fidelity; however, it is unclear how fidelity was measured. In the fourth phase, Bodrova and Leong (2019) continued the development of strategies and the application of strategies in diverse settings. ...
Article
Full-text available
One of the more prominent early childhood interventions focused on the development of executive function (EF) skills is Tools of the Mind ( Tools ; Bodrova and Leong, 2019 ). Intervention studies comparing Tools classrooms with control classrooms, however, reveal inconsistent findings for children’s EF outcomes. The current study utilizes Head Start CARES teachers assigned to the Tools of the Mind enhancement intervention (Tools ; N = 75) and the children in their classrooms (N = 738). Relations between teachers’ characteristics (i.e., teaching experience, psychological well-being, and educational background), training attendance and implementation (i.e., coach rated fidelity and observed scaffolding), and the interaction among these factors were examined as predictors of classroom-level gains in EF. Results revealed several significant moderation effects indicating that Tools implementation is related to classroom EF gains for some but not all teachers.
... getting the resources, encouraging discussions), or by proposing experiments to test out their ideas. Structure that is supportive of autonomy might also take the form of agreed rules for talk (Warwick et al., 2013), a highly ordered environment (Lillard, 2019), tools and procedures (Bodrova & Leong, 2018;Lillard, 2019) or scaffolding (Perry, 1998;Rojas-Drummond et al., 2013). ...
... Perry, 2013), which we observed on occasion. The adult would also be able to join in and sustain children's play, a practice which has been shown to be hugely beneficial to learning (Bodrova & Leong, 2018;Siraj-Blatchford & Sylva, 2004) but which we hardly ever observed. Boredom in a teacher-directed activity such as whole class work, however, may only be improved through external regulation (the teacher refocusing the child or extrinsic motivators), or by the teacher changing the task to make it more interesting or appealing (a strategy which would also apply to other settings). ...
Thesis
Children are born naturally curious and eager to learn, but as they go through school this inner motivation to learn diminishes. Yet children’s inner motivation to learn is essential to deep learning, positive attitudes to school and wellbeing. Self-Determination Theory suggests that supporting children’s need for autonomy – that is to say the feeling that actions stem from internal sources rather than being imposed externally – is essential to supporting inner motivational resources. This thesis is concerned with how teachers may be able to support children’s autonomy and inner motivation in the early Primary classroom in England and how we may be able to capture changes in children’s inner motivation in those settings. It is divided into two parts. In Part I, I used interpretive methods to understand teachers’ attempts to provide greater opportunities for children’s autonomy in Year 1 classrooms through a professional development programme. This programme was developed by a team of researchers at the PEDAL centre using a Community of Practice model and involved nine teachers in trying out strategies to support children’s autonomy. Through stories of change, I show that teachers’ use and interpretations of the strategies varied, and this was affected by the teachers’ school context and their own beliefs. Through thematic analysis, I show that the classrooms in the study functioned as ecosystems of teacher control, which was itself under pressure from top-down directions through governmental policies and institutions as well as senior leaders. This resulted in a teaching mindset focused on strict learning objectives which left little space for children to take ownership of their learning. Despite this, teachers were sometimes able to provide pockets of space for children’s autonomy, though these took diverse forms. The extent of these spaces for autonomy depended on individual school and classroom contexts. The proposed model – pockets of space within an ecosystem of teacher control – explains the tensions between teachers’ need for control in the classroom and opportunities for children’s autonomy, as well as areas where teachers’ attempts to increase children’s autonomy were successful. In particular, I show that teachers needed to provide support and stimulation as well as space in order to support both autonomy and inner motivation. Part II is concerned with measuring inner motivation for research purposes and in particular for future evaluations of the above professional development programme. This research focuses on the validity and reliability of an existing instrument, the Leuven Involvement Scale (LIS). This instrument aims to capture a form of engagement in learning activities that is related to inner motivation. The studies in Part II investigate the reliability and stability of the instrument, as well as factors associated with variation in engagement using multilevel modelling. I found that the LIS can be reliable as long as raters share a common understanding of different child behaviours in the classroom. In addition, I found that engagement varied hugely from one moment to the next, with very little variation between children. What little variation existed between children was explained by the association between engagement and aspects of children’s self-regulatory capacities, namely effortful control and negative emotions, measured through the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) and Child Behaviour Questionnaire (CBQ). However, overall this research suggests that it is the individual moment that matters, rather than characteristics of the children. To better understand the influence of contextual factors, I investigated the association of activity setting (whether children are in teacher-directed, independent or free choice situations) with engagement. Children were significantly more engaged in free choice settings compared to whole class teacher-directed settings. However, there was a large amount of remaining variation and I discuss the implications this has for the role of teachers in supporting children’s engagement. Overall, this thesis makes a contribution towards our understanding of children’s autonomy and inner motivation in the classroom and teaching practices that support it, as well as how we may be able to study it in classroom contexts.
... Dies tri auf die Montessoripädagogik, die ab 1907 zunächst in Kinderhäusern für die Altersgruppe der 3-bis 6-Jährigen und erst in weiterer Folge auch in Schulen Verbreitung fand (Meisterjahn-Knebel, 2018), genauso zu wie auf die in den 1940er Jahren entwickelte und auf Kleinkinder bis zum Alter von 3 Jahren fokussierende Piklerpädagogik (Gilles-Bacciu & Heuer, 2019) und das in den 1950er Jahren entwickelte Handlungskonzept des Waldkindergartens (Miklitz, 2011). Gleiches gilt für ‚neuere', mit Beginn der Bildungsreformphase Ende der 1960er Jahre bis in die 1990er Jahre etablierte nationale und internationale pädagogische Handlungskonzepte wie die Kinderladenbewegung/ Elterninitiativen (Bock, Göddertz, Heyden & Mauritz, 2020), den Situationsansatz (Zimmer, 1985), den Bewegungskindergarten (Schaff ner, 2004), den off enen Ansatz (Regel & Ahrens, 2016), das Pyramide-Konzept (Boontje et al., 2018), Tools of the Mind (Bodrova & Leong, 2018) und domänenspezifi sche Förderkonzepte (vgl. dazu Beiträge in Schmidt & Smidt, 2018). ...
... El primero es el Tools of the Mind: The Vygotskian Approach to Early Childhood Education (Bodrova y Leong, 2019) el cual tiene como fin la mejora de las funciones ejecutivas a través de actividades que consisten en la regulación del habla, el juego dramático y juegos de memoria y atención. Estudios que prueban la eficacia de este programa, demuestran que el alumnado que participa en él no solo mejora las funciones ejecutivas, sino que, estas mejoras se generalizan a otros ámbitos como comportamiento social o el rendimiento académico (Bodrova y Leong, 2019;Diamond et al., 2019). ...
Article
Resumen La intervención para la mejora de las funciones ejecutivas es crucial en educación preescolar puesto que el alumnado de esta etapa obtiene mayores beneficios de los programas de intervención. El objetivo de este estudio es presentar los resultados obtenidos de la aplicación del «Programa de entrenamiento en funciones ejecutivas en Educación Infantil», con el que se pretende la mejora del control inhibitorio, la memoria de trabajo y la flexibilidad cognitiva. Los participantes en este estudio son 100 niños y niñas de cinco y seis años, procedentes de dos colegios de Granada (España). Para evaluar el impacto del programa, se obtienen mediciones previas y posteriores a la intervención de los miembros del grupo experimental, y se comparan con las mediciones correspondientes a un grupo de control, utilizando la regresión jerárquica y el análisis mediante un modelo lineal mixto. Las funciones ejecutivas se evalúan mediante la Evaluación Conductual de la Función Ejecutiva-Versión Infantil (BRIEF-P). Los resultados del estudio muestran que el programa de intervención tiene un impacto significativo en todas las variables de función ejecutiva analizadas, con grandes tamaños de efecto (f de Cohen y g de Hedges). Las funciones ejecutivas son esenciales para muchas de las aptitudes necesarias en la vida adulta, como la memoria, la creatividad, la flexibilidad, el autocontrol y el cumplimiento de las reglas y normas. Por esta razón, existe una necesidad real de crear programas que promuevan el desarrollo de estas funciones en las primeras etapas de la vida. Los programas validados como el Programa de entrenamiento en funciones ejecutivas en Educación Infantil pueden ser llevados a cabo por el profesorado dentro del plan de estudios estándar, utilizando materiales que están fácilmente disponibles en las escuelas.
... Key Words: Turkish, Language, Preschool, Children GİRİŞ Dil, insanların karşılıklı iletişim kurmasına yarayan, düşüncelerini ve davranışlarını kontrol etmek amacıyla kullandıkları işaretlerden oluşan karmaşık ve dinamik bir sistemdir (Bodrova & Leong, 2017). Dil gelişimi ise, kelimelerin, sayıların, sembollerin kazanılması, saklanması ve dilin kurallarına uygun olarak kullanılmasıdır. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Language development is a process involving the use of sound, symbols and words within the gramer rules of spoken language. Language development begins with birth and continues through life. However, early childhood years have a critical role in language development. For this reason, early intervention of problems in children's language development is necessary for them to receive proper education and to prevent future negative situations. The aim of this study is to develop a measurement tool to determine the level of language development in early childhood years. For this purpose, data were collected by reaching 1000 children in the 4-6 age group. In the scoring of the scale items, it was evaluated as 1 if the participant performs the task and 0 if the participant cannot. In the development, validity and reliability studies of the scale, the coefficient of agreement between the experts (Kendall’s W) and the confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and compliance index values were used in the sub-dimensions. The results showed that Dumans TEÇDG Turkish Early Childhood Language Development Scale is a valid and reliable measurement tool.
Article
Full-text available
The kindergarten program, Tools of the Mind (Tools), has been shown to improve executive functions (as assessed by laboratory measures) and academic performance. The objective here was to see if Tools can improve executive functions in the real world (in the classroom), academic outcomes not previously investigated, reduce bullying and peer ostracism, and increase teachers' and students' joy in being in the classroom. This first randomized controlled trial of Tools in Canada included 351 kindergarten children (mean age 5.2 years at entry; 51% female) in 18 public schools. Stratified randomization resulted in teachers and students in both groups being closely matched. Teachers in both groups received the same number of training hours and same funds for new materials. Outcome measures were pre and post standardized academic skill assessments and teacher online survey responses. This study replicated that Tools improves reading and shows for the first time that it improves writing (far exceeding levels the school districts had seen before), self-control and attention-regulation in the real world (e.g., time on task without supervision), reduces teacher burnout and children being ostracized or excluded, and increases the joy students and teachers experience in school. By Spring, Tools teachers were still enthusiastic about teaching; control teachers were exhausted. These results were not only better than the control group but also better than Tools teachers experienced the year before Tools. Thus, children in a kindergarten curriculum that emphasized play, improving self-regulation, working together and helping one another, and hands-on learning performed better academically, showed less bullying and peer ostracism and more kindness and helping behavior than students in more traditional classes, and teacher enthusiasm for teaching soared. Tools reduced initial disparities separating children, schools, and teachers.
Technical Report
Full-text available
The experimental evaluation of the Tools of the Mind Pre-K Curriculum described in this report was designed to examine the effectiveness of the Tools of the Mind (Tools) curriculum for enhancing children’s self-regulation skills and their academic preparation for kindergarten when compared to the usual prekindergarten curricula in use in the school system. In order to assess the long-term impacts of Tools on student academic achievement and self-regulation outcomes, students were followed into kindergarten and first grade. Participating classrooms were also observed three times during the prekindergarten year using multiple measures designed to capture implementation fidelity as well as child and teacher behaviors in the classroom. The 5-year research study was funded by a grant from the US Department of Education Institute of Education Sciences (R305A090533) to Drs. Dale Farran, Mark Lipsey and Sandra Wilson of the Peabody Research Institute at Vanderbilt University. Training and coaching support for Tools of the Mind was funded through a sub-award to each of the developers, Dr. Deborah Leong at Metropolitan State College of Denver and Dr. Elena Bodrova of the McREL Institute of Denver Colorado.
Article
Full-text available
Early self-regulation predicts school readiness, academic success, and quality of life in adulthood. Its development in the preschool years is rapid and also malleable. Thus, preschool curricula that promote the development of self-regulation may help set children on a more positive developmental trajectory. We conducted a cluster-randomized controlled trial of the Tools of the Mind preschool curriculum, a program that targets self-regulation through imaginative play and self-regulatory language (Tools; clinical trials identifier NCT02462733). Previous research with Tools is limited, with mixed evidence of its effectiveness. Moreover, it is unclear whether it would benefit all preschoolers or primarily those with poorly developed cognitive capacities (e.g., language, executive function, attention). The study goals were to ascertain whether the Tools program leads to greater gains in self-regulation compared to Playing to Learn (YMCA PTL), another play based program that does not target self-regulation specifically, and whether the effects were moderated by children’s initial language and hyperactivity/inattention. Two hundred and sixty 3- to 4-year-olds attending 20 largely urban daycares were randomly assigned, at the site level, to receive either Tools or YMCA PTL (the business-as-usual curriculum) for 15 months. We assessed self-regulation at pre-, mid and post intervention, using two executive function tasks, and two questionnaires regarding behavior at home and at school, to capture development in cognitive as well as socio-emotional aspects of self-regulation. Fidelity data showed that only the teachers at the Tools sites implemented Tools, and did so with reasonable success. We found that children who received Tools made greater gains on a behavioral measure of executive function than their YMCA PTL peers, but the difference was significant only for those children whose parents rated them high in hyperactivity/inattention initially. The effect of Tools did not vary with children’s initial language skills. We suggest that, as both programs promote quality play and that the two groups fared similarly well overall, Tools and YMCA PTL may be effective curricula choices for a diverse preschool classroom. However, Tools may be advantageous in classrooms with children experiencing greater challenges with self-regulation, at no apparent cost to those less challenged in this regard.
Article
Full-text available
This study seeks to demonstrate the benefits of bilingual instruction, story drama, and play with low-income preschool children. Sixty-five percent of the children in the study were dual language learners (DLLs). The investigation took place in a state-funded, public, universal prekindergarten program implementing Tools of the Mind (Tools), a Vygotskian play-based curriculum. Teachers read aloud fairy tales twice a week. Children in three classrooms were exposed to one of three read-aloud conditions: English-only, bilingual and no play, or bilingual and play. The results indicated that the classroom that provided bilingual instruction and play had significantly higher English proficiency and story recall scores than either of the other two classrooms. Providing opportunities to play and reenact fairy tales seemed to have positive effects on English proficiency and story recall for low-income preschool children.
Article
Full-text available
Effective early education is essential for academic achievement and positive life outcomes, particularly for children in poverty. Advances in neuroscience suggest that a focus on self-regulation in education can enhance children's engagement in learning and establish beneficial academic trajectories in the early elementary grades. Here, we experimentally evaluate an innovative approach to the education of children in kindergarten that embeds support for self-regulation, particularly executive functions, into literacy, mathematics, and science learning activities. Results from a cluster randomized controlled trial involving 29 schools, 79 classrooms, and 759 children indicated positive effects on executive functions, reasoning ability, the control of attention, and levels of salivary cortisol and alpha amylase. Results also demonstrated improvements in reading, vocabulary, and mathematics at the end of kindergarten that increased into the first grade. A number of effects were specific to high-poverty schools, suggesting that a focus on executive functions and associated aspects of self-regulation in early elementary education holds promise for closing the achievement gap.
Article
Full-text available
The following pilot study reports on teacher observations and reflections of implementing the Tools of the Mind curriculum in pre-k and kindergarten classrooms in an east coast urban school district in the US. The study followed five teachers over the course of a school year. Structured interviews were conducted with each teacher individually shortly after Tools of the Mind teacher training sessions took place. The analysis reports on themes that emerged in these conversations. Findings address; challenges the teachers faced in implementing the program, training issues, and the effectiveness of the program in supporting children's intellectual and social skills.
Book
Russian followers of Vygotsky have expanded his ideas into a theory that integrates cognitive, motivational, and social aspects of child development--emphasizing the role of children's activity as mediated by adults in their development. This theory has become the basis for an innovative analysis of periods in child development and of the mechanism of children's transitions from one period to the next. The neo-Vygotskian approach to child development is thus introduced to English-speaking readers in this volume.