ArticlePDF Available

Mindfulness improves verbal learning and memory through enhanced encoding

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Recent research has begun to demonstrate the effectiveness of mindfulness in improving certain cognitive abilities, including verbal learning and memory. However, no research has investigated the potential mechanism by which mindfulness may improve verbal learning and memory. We examined encoding, consolidation, and retrieval as potential mechanisms by which learning and memory may be increased on a list learning test (Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task; RAVLT). After dividing participants into either a mindfulness or a control condition, in which they listened to a 10-min audio tape, results found that the mindfulness condition significantly outperformed the control condition on every RAVLT trial. Using the Item-Specific Deficit Approach, we discovered that this enhanced verbal learning and memory was specifically due to a significantly enhanced encoding process for the mindfulness group, which fully mediated the relationship between the mindfulness condition and performance on the RAVLT. There were no differences between the conditions on consolidation or retrieval. Furthermore, these improvements were not accompanied by improvements in verbal fluency or attention. In a second study, we presented a mindfulness or control audio before the first RAVLT delayed free-recall trial and another one before the second RAVLT delayed free-recall trial in order to better determine the effect of mindfulness on consolidation and retrieval. The results replicated Study 1, in that neither consolidation nor retrieval were significantly affected by mindfulness. This research indicates that mindfulness may primarily improve verbal learning and memory through improved encoding processes.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Mindfulness improves verbal learning and memory
through enhanced encoding
Adam Lueke
1
&Niloufar Lueke
1
#The Psychonomic Society, Inc. 2019
Abstract
Recent research has begun to demonstrate the effectiveness of mindfulness in improving certain cognitive abilities, including
verbal learning and memory. However, no research has investigated the potential mechanism by which mindfulness may improve
verbal learning and memory. We examined encoding, consolidation, and retrieval as potential mechanisms by which learning and
memory may be increased on a list learning test (Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task; RAVLT). After dividing participants into
either a mindfulness or a control condition, in which they listened to a 10-min audio tape, results found that the mindfulness
condition significantly outperformed the control condition on every RAVLT trial. Using the Item-Specific Deficit Approach, we
discovered that this enhanced verbal learning and memory was specifically due to a significantly enhanced encoding process for
the mindfulness group, which fully mediated the relationship between the mindfulness condition and performance on the RAVLT.
There were no differences between the conditions on consolidation or retrieval. Furthermore, these improvements were not
accompanied by improvements in verbal fluency or attention. In a second study, we presented a mindfulness or control audio
before the first RAVLT delayed free-recall trial and another one before the second RAVLT delayed free-recall trial in order to
better determine the effect of mindfulness on consolidation and retrieval. The results replicated Study 1, in that neither consol-
idation nor retrieval were significantly affected by mindfulness. This research indicates that mindfulness may primarily improve
verbal learning and memory through improved encoding processes.
Keywords Mindfulness .Learning .Memory .Encoding .Ver ba l
Introduction
Mindfulness is a state of being characterized by present-
moment awareness of the unfolding of experience in a nonre-
active way. This simple practice has been shown to have a
myriad of benefits for its practitioners, despite the science of
mindfulness being a relatively new field. Much of the early
work on mindfulness focused on its ability to improve indi-
vidual well-being through its ameliorative effects on stress,
anxiety, and depression (Baer, Carmody, & Hunsinger,
2012; Ciesla, Reilly, Dickson, Emanuel, & Updegraff, 2012;
Davidson et al., 2003; Desrosiers, Vine, Klemanski, & Nolan-
Hoeksema, 2013; Lagor, Williams, Lerner, & McClure,
2013). Furthermore, mindfulness has also been shown to be
effective in improving the healing process (Davidson et al.
2003; Kabat-Zinn et al., 1998) and helping clinical popula-
tions cope with painful medical conditions (Brown & Jones,
2013). In addition to personal well-being, mindfulness has
also been demonstrated to be an effective tool in improving
interpersonal relationships (Hopthrow, Hooper, Mahmood,
Meier, & Weger, 2017; Langer, Bashner, & Chanowitz,
1985; Lueke & Gibson, 2015,2016;Parks,Birtel,&Crisp,
2014; Pratscher, Rose, Markovitz, & Bettencourt, 2017;
Ramsey & Jones, 2015). Among all of these positive benefits
from the practice of mindfulness, there is also a burgeoning
literature on the ability of this practice to enhance cognitive
ability. The focus of the current article is to indicate a partic-
ular cognitive faculty that can be improved through brief
mindfulness training verbal learning and memory, while also
demonstrating the mechanism by which verbal learning and
memory is improved. Concurrently, we look to rule out other
potential explanations for improved learning and memory
namely attentional processes.
*Adam Lueke
aklueke@bsu.edu
Niloufar Lueke
nassar@bsu.edu
1
Department of Psychological Science, Ball State University,
Muncie, IN 47306, USA
Memory & Cognition
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-019-00947-z
Author's personal copy
Recent work with mindfulness indicates that verbal learn-
ing and memory can be positively affected by mindfulness
practice. Participants assigned to a 2-week mindfulness class
demonstrated improved ability on a reading comprehension
measure, whereas control participants did not. This effect
was particularly pronounced in individuals whose mind-
wandering significantly decreased due to the mindfulness
class (Mrazek, Franklin, Phillips, Baird, & Schooler, 2013).
Other research has also indicated that mindfulness can help
reduce mind-wandering (Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010;Rahl,
Lindsay, Pacilio, Brown, & Creswell, 2017). While it is pos-
sible that mindfulness may help to free up cognitive resources
necessary for peak verbal learning and memory performance
that would otherwise be engaged in task-unrelated thoughts,
the avenue by which mindfulness may positively impact ver-
bal learning and memory is currently uncertain.
One potential explanation for improvements observed in
verbal learning and memory could be due to increased atten-
tion, but currently the research is mixed as to whether mind-
fulness increases attentional capabilities or not (Anderson,
Lau, Segal, & Bishop, 2007; Chambers, Lo, & Allen, 2008;
Jensen, Vangkilde, Frokjaer, & Hasselbalch, 2012; Josefsson
& Broberg, 2011;Kerretal.,2011). Indeed, recent research
has found that long-term meditators were significantly better
on a measure of verbal learning and memory compared to
non-meditators, even though they found no significant differ-
ences between the two groups in terms ofattention or attention
switching (Lykins, Baer, & Gottlob, 2012). We propose that
rather than altering attention, mindfulness may improve verbal
learning and memory via its effect on the encoding process of
learning, which allows for enhanced memory for new infor-
mation over time.
List-learning and parsing out memory process
capabilities
List-learning is a common neuropsychological device that can
help measure general verbal learning and memory capabili-
ties. Typically, these tests provide participants with several
learning trials to practice a list of words, followed by an inter-
ference trial before a free-recall trial. Another longer delay is
then presented, followed by another free-recall trial, and then a
recognition trial. For example, the Rey Auditory Verbal
Learning Test (RAVLT) has participants listen to and recall
the same list of words over five consecutive trials. With rep-
etition, it is expected that participants improve across trials.
This is then followed by a single presentation and recall test of
a new list of words, which is used as interference to the pre-
vious word list. A free-recall test of the first list that the par-
ticipants had learned is then administered immediately after
the free-recall test of the interference list, and thenagain after a
delay period of approximately 20 min. Finally, a recognition
test of the word list is administered.
List-learning measures such as the RAVLT also attempt to
make distinctions in ability regarding encoding, consolidation,
and retrieval. Previous research has shown that the RAVLT
can measure encoding, consolidation, and retrieval (Vakil &
Blachstein, 1993), and there is evidence from similar list-
learning tests that immediate recall during the five learning
trials is associated with encoding, while the two delayed-
recall trials are associated with consolidation and retrieval
(Delis et al., 1991). However, the way in which these memory
processes are distinguished with these list-learning measures
may lack precision and potentially could be confounded by
other factors, and therefore may not accurately reflect the
measurement of these processes. For instance, encoding is
generally measured as the improvement in recall across learn-
ing trials in a list-learning task (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, &
Ober, 2000). However, this improvement may simply be due
to a lack of attention during early trials that is mitigated
through repeated practice. Thus, any improvements in recall
across trials may not accurately reflect encoding capabilities.
Likewise, consolidation is usually measured by comparing
performance on the final learning trial to performance on a
delayed free-recall trial (Delis, et al., 2000). Consolidation
difficulties are said to occur when words that were recalled
on the final learning trial are not recalled on the delayed free-
recall trial, as this indicates rapid loss of previously learned
information. However, it is certainly possible that poor perfor-
mance on the delayed free-recall trial may indicate a retrieval
rather than consolidation deficit. Similarly, retrieval is typical-
ly measured bycomparing a recognition trial to a delayed free-
recall trial, with retrieval deficits said to occur when recogni-
tion is better than delayed free-recall (Delis et al., 2000).
However, recall and recognition represent different processes
(Aggleton & Brown, 2006), which may not be ideal to com-
pare in order to adequately measure retrieval capabilities.
A relatively new approach has been developed to more
accurately distinguish and isolate encoding, consolidation,
and retrieval processes with regard to list-learning measures
such as the RAVLT the Item-Specific Deficit Approach
(ISDA). The ISDA is a psychometrically valid means of pars-
ing out these processes that otherwise would be unable to be
measured adequately. Additionally, the ISDA was developed
in order to eliminate, or at least minimize, the confounding
effects of inattention on encoding, consolidation, and retrieval,
thus allowing a process-specific measure that is not influenced
by attentional capabilities. This feature of the ISDA can help
us identify any improvement in encoding due to mindfulness
regardless of whether or not attention is also improved.
Additionally, the ISDA measures consolidation and retrieval
in a manner that better accounts for their potential overlap and
eliminates the use of recognition trials, which may include
confounding processes (Wright et al., 2009).
The ISDA must be applied to a list-learning measure that
utilizes multiple learning trials and at least two delayed free-
Mem Cogn
Author's personal copy
recall trials. Recognition trials are not utilized with the ISDA.
The encoding index is determined by identifying the number
of items that are not correctly recited on more than half of all
learning trials. The RAVLT has five learning trials, meaning
that an encoding error would be the inability to recall a spe-
cific word on atleast three of the learning trials. By calculating
encoding as the tendency to neglect certain words across trials,
rather than by examining the improvement of performance on
successive trials, the ISDA protects against the confounding
effects of inattention on any one trial (Wright et al., 2009).
The consolidation index is calculated by identifying the
words recalled during list learning that were not recalled on
either delayed free-recall trial. This value is then divided by
the total number of words that were recalled during list learn-
ing. For example, if someone recalled 13 out of 15 words in
total over the course of the list learning trials, but two of those
13 words were not recalled on either delayed free-recall trial,
then the consolidation index would be calculated by dividing
2 by 13. This helps control for encoding differences during list
learning. The inability to recall previously learned items
across both delayed free-recall trials indicates a deficit in con-
solidation more likely than with retrieval (Wright et al., 2009).
The retrieval index is calculated by identifying the
words recalled during list learning that were inconsistent-
ly recalled across multiple delayed free-recall trials. This
value is then divided by the total number of words that
were recalled during list learning. For example, if some-
one recalled 11 of 15 words over the course of the list
learning trials, but three of those 11 words were recalled
on only one of the two delayed free-recall trials, then the
retrieval index would be calculated by dividing 3 by 11.
This helps control for encoding differences during list
learning. Since the retrieval index identifies the inconsis-
tency of recall across multiple trials, it indicates a problem
with retrieval more likely than with consolidation (Wright
et al., 2009).
Research using the RAVLT and ISDA has shown that pa-
tients with a traumatic brain injury demonstrate deficits in
encoding and consolidation in comparison with controls
(Wright & Schmitter-Edgecombe, 2011). Likewise, research
with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis patients using these same
measures indicates impairment within the encoding and con-
solidation but not retrieval stages in comparison with controls
(Christidi, Zalonis, Smyrnis, & Evdokimidis, 2012).
Additionally, the ISDA has also revealed that HIV-positive
patients indicate impaired encoding processes in comparison
with controls, whereas HIV-positive patients who did not ad-
here well to treatment also indicated deficits in retrieval pro-
cesses (Wright et al., 2011).
If verbal learning and memory performance on the RAVLT
was better for a mindfulness group in comparison with a con-
trol group, the ISDA could determine the memory process(es)
responsible for this improvement, while also controlling for
potential differences in attention. There is reason to suspect
that the encoding process is particularly affected by a mindful
state. Recent research has indicated that mindfulness reduces
inattentional blindness (Schofield, Creswell, & Denson,
2015), which implies that people who are mindful are better
able to encode information that others may simply not see or
be aware of.
Additionally, research has shown that a primary brain re-
gion responsible for the encoding of verbal information, the
left parahippocampal gyrus (Dolan & Fletcher, 1999;
Fernandez et al., 1998; Strange et al., 2002), is more active
within people with higher dispositional mindfulness (Kong,
Wang, Song, & Liu, 2016). This overlap may indicate that the
higher activity in this region among mindful individuals con-
currently facilitates the encoding of verbal information.
Relatedly, mindfulness training has been shown to increase
volume in the left hippocampus, which reduced proactive in-
terference and thus improved memory (Greenberg et al.,
2018). Proactive interference is a major impediment to
encoding, but factors such as higher working memory capac-
ity can minimize the effects of proactive interference (Kliegl,
Pastotter, & Bauml, 2015), and thus improve encoding pro-
cesses. Mindfulness (even brief experience) has been shown
to be related to greater working memory capacity (Dubert
et al., 2016; Jha et al., 2010; Mrazek et al., 2013; Quach,
Mano, & Alexander, 2016), which in turn would minimize
proactive interference and increase encoding processes.
Other research has demonstrated that interference often takes
place specifically during the encoding process (i.e., encoding
interference; Gordon, Hendrick, & Johnson, 2001,2004;
Villata, Tabor, & Franck, 2018), which implies that large
memory gains can be achieved through an intervention that
reduces encoding interference.
Other research has reported that mindfulness improves ver-
bal memory through increased Bencoding^(Bonamo,
Legerski, & Thomas, 2015); however, the results only illus-
trated improved recall of learned verbal information, not an
actual improvement in encoding. There was no measure of the
separate memory processes, including the encoding process,
so there was no evidence to suggest that encoding specifically
was responsible for the improved verbal memory. Similarly,
research with trait mindfulness has indicated that people
higher on trait mindfulness tend to have an external
Bencoding^process (Herndon, 2008), but the measure used
(Lewickis Internal/External encoding styles; Lewicki, 2005)
merely measured the tendency to attend to information in the
internal or external world. It was not a measure of encoding as
a learning and memory process. Our study is the first to di-
rectly measure memory processes that are potentially en-
hanced by mindfulness and that subsequently improve verbal
learning and memory.
Our central hypothesis is that participants who engage in
mindfulness will perform significantly better on a measure of
Mem Cogn
Author's personal copy
verbal learning and memory (the RAVLT) than control partic-
ipants, and that this improvement will be due to an enhanced
encoding memory process. This encoding process should me-
diate the relationship between the mindfulness condition and
performance on the RAVLT. This relationship would desig-
nate mindfulness as a useful tool for learning new verbal in-
formation, which would aid in future recall, and not necessar-
ily in remembering previously learned verbal information.
As mindfulness is expected to work primarily on improv-
ing encoding, we would not expect increased performance on
a measure that relied on previously learned verbal informa-
tion. For this reason, we hypothesize that mindfulness partic-
ipants will not perform better than others on a measure of
verbal fluency that relies on recall of words that begin with
specified letters (thus word-knowledge stored in long-term
memory) within a limited timeframe.
Finally, we expect improvement in verbal learning and
memory without improvements in selective attention or atten-
tion switching, which would mirror the results of Lykins,
Baer, and Gottlob (2012). This would indicate that brief mind-
fulness works to improve verbal learning and memory by
specifically enhancing encoding processes, and not due to
increases in general attention abilities. To test this hypothesis,
we included the Color-Word Interference Test (CWIT; a
Stroop-like task that measures selective attention) and the
Trail Making Test (a measure of attention switching). All tasks
used in this study are commonly used and well validated neu-
ropsychological measures.
Method
Participants
The total number of participants was 94, but four participants
were excluded due to a technical error (i.e., no condition num-
ber), and another five were excluded due to having recently
completed the neuropsychological tests used in this experi-
ment. This was necessary because naïvety is crucial for mea-
sures like the RAVLT, in which participants are supposed to be
unaware of the final delayed free-recall trial. With previous
experience taking the RAVLT, participants know to rehearse
before the final delayed free-recall trial in order to increase
performance. Additionally, since the 15 target words on the
RAVLT never change, these participants had recently memo-
rized the words that they were given in this study, thus making
them easier to relearn. Therefore, our final sample was com-
prised of 85 undergraduate university students (50 females
and 35 males; 46 mindfulness condition and 39 control con-
dition) of traditional college age who were recruited from a
large Midwestern university. The final sample size was deter-
mined by a power analysis for independent samples t-tests,
with the desire to achieve a 95% probability of detecting a
large effect (d= .80), which required approximately 35 partic-
ipants per condition.
Measures
Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS; Brown &
Ryan, 2003)
The MAAS is a measure of trait mindfulness that consists of
15 items measured on a 6-point scale (1 Almost always, to 6
Almost never) that asks participants about how they normal-
ly go through activities in their daily lives (e.g., BIrush
through activities without being really attentive to them^).
State Mindfulness Scale (SMS; Tanay & Bernstein, 2013)
The SMS is a measure of state mindfulness that consists of 15
items on a 5-point Likert scale (0 Not at all, to 4 Ve ry
much) that asks participants to rate their experiences during an
audio recording immediately preceding the scale (e.g., BItried
to pay attention to pleasant and unpleasant sensations^).
Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT; Strauss, Sherman,
&Spreen,2006)
The RAVLT measures verbal learning and memory. A 15-
noun word list (List A) was read aloud to the participant, in
a fixed order and with a 1-s interval between words, for five
consecutive trials, with recall instructions repeated at the start
of each trial. Each of the five trials was followed by a free-
recall test, for which the participant was asked to repeat as
many of the words as possible, in any order. Responses were
tracked, and feedback was not provided. Upon completion of
Trial 5 free-recall, a single presentation and free-recall test of a
second (new) list of 15 words (List B; interference list; Trial 6)
was administered. Immediately after the free-recall test of List
B, and without an additional presentation of List A, a short-
delayed recall of the first list (List A; Trial 7) was assessed.
After a 20-min delay period (with no other verbal memory
tests administered during this time interval), the long-
delayed free-recall of List A (Trial 8) was assessed, again
without an additional presentation of List A. Immediatelyafter
Trial 8, a recognition test (Trial 9) was administered by pro-
viding the participant with a matrix array of 50 words contain-
ing both List A and List B words, in addition to 20 words that
were phonemically or semantically similar to those in both
lists, and requesting the identification of List A words only.
The RAVLT is a sensitive test of verbal learning and mem-
ory (see Schoenberg et al., 2006, for a review), and provides
scores for examination of immediate memory span, new ver-
bal learning, susceptibility to interference, retention of infor-
mation after a delay, and memory recognition (Magalhães &
Hamdan, 2010; Strauss et al., 2006).
Mem Cogn
Author's personal copy
Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT; FAS version:
phonemic verbal fluency; Benton, Hamsher, & Sivan, 1994)
The FAS measures verbal fluency with the spontaneous
oral generation of words that begin with a specified letter
during a prescribed timeframe, while adhering to the fol-
lowing restrictions: no proper nouns (no names of people
or places), no numbers, and no variants of the same word
(saying the same word using a different ending).
Following the abovementioned guidelines, participants
were asked to provide as many words as possible that
begin with the letters F, A, and S, for 60 s each (thus a
total of three trials were administered, one per letter).
Performance was measured by calculating the sum of all
admissible words for the three letters, minus the errors
(e.g., proper nouns, variations, repetitions, wrong words).
A recent factor-analytic study found that the FAS loaded
exclusively onto the language factor, and not the executive
functioning factor, implicating verbal fluency as primarily a
language-processing measure (Whiteside et al., 2016). Word-
knowledge (Ruff et al., 1997; Whiteside et al., 2016)aswellas
auditory attention and long-term memory, but not short-term
memory, have also been suggested to be the most important
determinants for FAS performance (Ruff et al., 1997).
Trail Making Test (TMT; Reitan & Wolfson, 1993)
The TMT is an attention-switching measure and consists of
two parts: Part A and Part B. Part A measures the speed at
which participants sequentially connect, by making pencil
lines, 25 encircled numbers that are variously spread across
a page. Part B measures the speed at which participants con-
nect 25 encircled, and variously spread, numbers and letters in
sequential, but alternating order (i.e., 1-A-2-B-3-C and so on).
For each part, participants were instructed to draw the line as
fast as they can. If errors occurred, they would be pointed out
as per standard procedure, and error-correction would subse-
quently influence the total time taken to complete each part. A
practice trial was provided prior to the administration of each
part. While the primary variables of interest were the total time
in seconds taken to complete each of the two parts, a ratio
score (Part B/Part A) was also calculated in order to control
for general processing speed when interpreting Part B
performance.
While visual search and scanning abilities as well as speed-
ed performance are required by both parts, Part B also requires
divided attention (alteration of operations), and it is sensitive
to cognitive flexibility (Strauss et al., 2006). The derived ratio
score (Part B/Part A) is thought to serve as an index of com-
plex divided attention in other words, the B/A ratio score is
thought to be the best indicator of attentional control processes
required for rapid alternation between two tasks (e.g.,
Arbuthnott & Frank, 2000).
Color-Word Interference Test (CWIT) from the Delis-Kaplan
Executive Function System (D-KEFS; Delis, Kaplan, & Kramer,
2001)
The CWIT is a subtest from D-KEFS, and an extension of the
classic Stroop procedure (Stroop, 1935). It is comprised of
four conditions: (1) color naming, consisting of rows of col-
ored patches (red, blue, and green) on a white surface. The
participant was asked to name the colors aloud as fast and as
accurately as possible; (2) word reading, consisting of rows of
the words Bred,^Bgreen,^and Bblue^printed in black ink on a
white surface. The participant was asked to read the words
aloud as fast and as accurately as possible; (3) inhibition,
consisting of rows of the same three words as before, but this
time the words were written in an incongruent ink color. The
participant was asked to not read the words (thus inhibit the
automaticresponseofwordreading),andinsteadsayaloud
the color of the ink in which each word was printed in (e.g.,
say Bred^when the word Bgreen^is printed in red ink) as fast
and as accurately as possible; (4) inhibition/switching,
consisting of rows of the same three words, with half of the
words enclosed within boxes. The participant was asked to
switch between naming aloud the color of the ink in which
the word was printed (same as the inhibition condition), and
actually reading the word (and not naming the ink color) if the
word was enclosed within a box, as fast and as accurately as
possible. The aim of this last condition is to measure both
inhibition and switching. Performance on this test is primarily
assessed via the total completion time in seconds for each of
the four conditions. Optional measurements include error
analysis: the total number of uncorrected and self-corrected
errors per condition.
Performance on the CWIT measures selective attention
with the integration of processing speed, inhibitory control,
and cognitive set-switching/attention shifting. Color naming
(condition 1) and word reading (condition 2) are thought to
reflect baseline low-level cognitive measures that are required
for the subsequent high-level cognitive functions (e.g., inhibi-
tion and cognitive flexibility) that are assessed in inhibition
(condition 3) and inhibition/switching (condition 4). In the
inhibition condition, the participant is presented with interfer-
ence in the form of competing responses, and their ability to
inhibit a dominant and automatic response (word reading) in
order to provide an intentional one (naming the ink color) is
assessed. In the inhibition/switching condition, the participant
is required to switch back and forth between the automatic
response (reading the word, when the word is enclosed in a
box), and the intentional response (not reading the word, but
instead naming the ink color that the word is printed in); thus it
not only measures inhibition, but also cognitive flexibility or
the ability of set switching (Delis et al., 2001).
In order to obtain pure measures of inhibition and cognitive
flexibility (Delis et al., 2001; Yu et al., 2018), lower level
Mem Cogn
Author's personal copy
factors such as basic processing speed (as captured by condi-
tions 1 and 2) were controlled for by calculating contrast
scores: inhibition (inhibition minus color-naming completion
time), inhibition/cognitive flexibility (inhibit/switching minus
color-naming completion time), and cognitive flexibility (in-
hibit/switching minus inhibit completion time).
Procedure
All participants entered the lab and first completed the MAAS
in order to assess trait mindfulness. They were then instructed
to listen to a 10-min audiotape, which was either a mindful-
ness audio tape that directed the participant to concentrate on
their breath and bodily sensations while remaining nonjudg-
mental about their experience, or a control audio tape that
described an English countryside. These audios have been
used in previous research a number of times, and the mindful-
ness audio has been shown to reliably increase mindfulness
over the control condition (Cropley, Ussher, & Charitou,
2007;Lueke&Gibson,2016). Participants alerted the exper-
imenter when the audio had finished, and then completed the
SMS.
Participants then completed the neuropsychological tests in
the same order regardless of condition. First, they completed
the CWIT, followed by the FAS, and then the RAVLT (except
for the second-delayed recall trial and the recognition test).
Participants then completed the TMTand a filler task in order
to have enough time elapse (about 20-min) before the second
part of the RAVLT was administered (the second delayed free-
recall, followed by the recognition test). The filler task was the
Wisconsin Card Sorting task, which was chosen for its length
and relianceon nonverbal processes. The order of presentation
was determined by the need to present the audio manipulation
before all subsequent neuropsychological tests. The neuropsy-
chological tests were ordered with the primary goal of achiev-
ing approximately a 20-min delay using tasks that were not
reliant on verbal activity between the first and second delayed
free-recall RAVLT trials. The TMT was administered during
the RAVLTs delay interval since it was a nonverbal task, as
was our filler task. The CWIT and FAS were presented before
the RAVLT so as not to bury them at the end of the experi-
ment. After all tasks were completed, participants were
debriefed and left the experiment.
Results
Preliminary results
We first investigated whether our two conditions were similar
in terms of trait mindfulness before any intervention had taken
place. An independent t-test analysis on the MAAS indicated
that the mindfulness and control conditions were not signifi-
cantly different in trait mindfulness, (t<1).
Additionally, as a manipulation check we measured
state mindfulness after the intervention to ensure that our
audio intervention did increase self-reported feelings of
mindfulness. An independent t-test analysis on the SMS
indicated that the mindfulness condition (M= 49.54, SD
= 14.34) did report significantly greater state mindfulness
than the control condition (M= 41.18, SD = 10.95), t(83) =
2.98, p= .004, d= .66.
RAVLT analyses for verbal learning and memory
We first examined our hypothesis that verbal learning and
memory would be better for individuals in the mindful-
ness condition than the control condition. In order to do
this, we performed two independent samples t-tests one
for the combined performance on the five learning trials
and one for the combined performance on the two delayed
free-recall trials. One participant did not complete the
RAVLT so was not included. A second participant had
to leave before the second delayed free-recall could be
administered so was not included in that specific analysis
or subsequent ISDA analyses. Overall, results revealed
significant support for the hypothesis that mindfulness
improves verbal learning and memory, as the mindfulness
condition (M=53.31,SD = 7.33) performed significantly
better than the control condition (M=48.51,SD =7.20)
on the learning trials, t(82) = 3.02, p< .002, d= .66, and
delayed free-recall trials, t(81) = 2.19, p< .02, d=.48
(Mindfulness: M= 22.38, SD =4.25;Control:M= 20.26,
SD = 4.54). All individual trial comparisons were also
significant, including a recognition trial (Fig. 1).
ISDA for memory processes
In order to examine what aspect of memory may have been
enhanced in the mindfulness condition, we utilized the ISDA
to analyze encoding, consolidation, and retrieval processes.
Following the instructions of Wright et al. (2009), we used
the five learning trials to calculate encoding errors, and the
two delayed free-recall trials to calculate consolidation and
retrieval. Independent t-tests were used to determine any dif-
ferences between mindfulness and control conditions for all
three memory processes. Results indicated significantly more
encoding errors for the control group (M=4.47,SD =2.30)
than for the mindfulness group (M=3.11,SD =2.18),t(81) =
2.77, p=.004,d= .61. The consolidation index was not quite
significantly different between the groups, t(81) = 1.53, p=
.07, d= .34. The retrieval process was also not significantly
different between the groups, t(81) = 1.24, p=.11,d=.27.
Both indices trended toward being better in the mindfulness
condition.
Mem Cogn
Author's personal copy
We then conducted a mediational analysis to determine
if the improvements in verbal learning and memory in the
mindfulness condition were due to increased encoding
capabilities. Using PROCESS macro, model 4 (Hayes,
2017), we used the audio condition as our X variable,
encoding as our M variable, and the combined delayed
free-recall trials as our Y variable. We found that the
overall model for X predicting Y was significant, F(1,
81) = 4.79, p= .032, R
2
= .06, as the mindfulness condi-
tion remembered more words on the delayed free-recall
trials, b=2.11,t(81) = 2.19, p= .032. Next, we found
a significant effect of X on M, as the overall model was
significant, F(1, 81) = 7.67, p= .007, R
2
=.09,withthe
mindfulness group showing greater encoding capabilities,
b=1.36,t(81) = 2.77, p= .007. Finally, we looked at X
and M together predicting Y. The overall model was sig-
nificant, F(2, 80) = 19.83, p< .0001, R
2
= .33. The effect
of M on Y was also significant, b= 1.06, t(80) = 5.74, p<
.0001, and the effect of X on Y was no longer significant,
b=.67,t(80) = .78, p= .44. A Sobel test confirmed that
there was a significant difference between the prediction of X
on Y based on the involvement of M, Z=2.49,p=.01.
We were unable to run the consolidation and retrieval indi-
ces as part of a multi-mediational analysis along with
encoding to determine their combined effects on the delayed
free-recall trials due to the nature of how these indices are
calculated. Both the consolidation and retrieval indices exclu-
sively utilize performance on the delayed free-recall trials to
calculate ability within these processes. Thus, it would be
inappropriate and meaningless to use these indices as a
potential mediator for performance on the delayed free-recall
trials. On the other hand, the encoding index only uses perfor-
mance on the learning trials to calculate ability on this process,
thus making it appropriate to use as a mediator for perfor-
mance on the delayed free-recall trials.
General verbal fluency
We further expected that mindfulness would not improve gen-
eral verbal ability even though it improved verbal learning and
memory. The ability of mindfulness to improve encoding
should help people learn verbal information, but this should
not affect verbal information that had already been learned
previous to the intervention, which would simply need to be
retrieved. We tested this by examining any differences be-
tween the mindfulness and control conditions in terms of per-
formance on the FAS, which measures verbal fluency through
the timed recall of words thatbegin with the letters F, A, and S.
An independent samples t-test confirmed that there was no
difference between the mindfulness and control conditions in
the number of words recalled that began with those letters,
t(83) = 1.20, p= .12 (Mindfulness: M=37.89,SD =10.27;
Control: M=35.10,SD = 11.17).
Attention
We then examined the possibility that mindfulness may im-
prove attentional capabilities. It is possible that any improve-
ments in encoding were largely derived from improvements of
attention. If there are no differences, it provides support for
Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 Trial 5 First
Delay
Second
Delay
Recognion
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
Mindfulness Control
RAVLT Performance
RAVLT Trial
Words Remembered
Fig. 1 Verbal learning and memory performance for both conditions on each Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task (RAVLT) trial. All comparisons are
significant
Mem Cogn
Author's personal copy
our hypothesis that mindfulness improves learning and mem-
ory specifically through enhanced encoding and not necessar-
ily through increased attention.
Independent samples t-tests were used in order to ex-
amine any differences with regard to two different mea-
sures of attention: the CWIT (selective attention measure)
and the TMT (attention switching measure). We examined
several different measures available within the CWIT, in-
cluding the time it took to complete each trial, and the
number of errors that were either self-corrected or uncor-
rected by the participant. Results indicated that there were
no differences between the mindfulness and control
groups in terms of the time taken to complete any of the
four CWIT trials, or the two critical trials combined (trials
3 and 4), or the combined time among all four trials (all ts
< 1). Likewise, there were no significant differences in the
number of errors of either type (self-corrected or
uncorrected) for either critical trial or the total number
of errors from both trials (all ts < 1.17). The total number
of uncaught errors from both critical trials combined was
also not significant, t(83) = 1.40, p= .08. The total num-
ber of caught errors from both critical trials combined was
not significantly different (t< 1). Additionally, we isolat-
ed more pure assessments of inhibition and cognitive flex-
ibility on the CWIT as outlined in previous research (see
Yu et al., 2018), and found no significant differences
among the three independent samples t-tests (all ts<1).
Three independent samples t-tests were performed on the
length of time to complete each of two parts on the TMT, as
well as their combined time. We also conducted a fourth in-
dependent samples t-test for the ratio between Part B over Part
A, which is thought to be a purer assessment of attention
switching (Arbuthnott & Frank, 2000). Results indicated that
none of these four comparisons indicated a significant differ-
ence (all ts < 1.21). Taken together, our results indicated that
the mindfulness condition did not affect selective attention or
attention switching. This provides support for the hypothesis
that encoding is likely enhanced without necessarily increas-
ing attention (see Table 1for all attention comparisons).
Discussion
Our results strongly supported our central hypothesis that
mindfulness improves verbal learning and memory through
the enhancement of the encoding process, which mediated
the relationship between mindfulness and recall at both de-
layed free-recall trials. Furthermore, this improved encoding
capability was not due to increases in attention, as there were
no differences in attention between the mindfulness and con-
trol groups. This means that encoding is enhanced by mind-
fulness without necessarily affecting attentional processes.
Additionally, general verbal ability was left unaffected by
brief mindfulness training, providing evidence that mindful-
ness is particularly effective for learning new verbal informa-
tion through improved encoding processes, rather than help-
ing to retrieve previously learned verbal information, or im-
proving verbal ability more generally. However, questions
remain regarding whether the mindfulness induced by the ma-
nipulation lasted until the final delayed free-recall, or even the
measure of attention switching (i.e., TMT). It is possible that
certain measures were not significant due to mindfulness hav-
ing worn off over the course of the experiment. In order to
address these questions, we conducted a second study to en-
sure that some of our null results were not due to dissipated
mindfulness.
Study 2
In Study 1, the mindfulness audio was administered before the
RAVLT learning trials. The ISDA calculates its encoding in-
dex using these learning trials, whereas the consolidation and
retrieval indices are calculated using the two delayed free-
recall trials. It is possible that the mindfulness produced by
the initial mindfulness induction improved encoding during
these learning trials but then dissipated over time and was no
longer present during one or both of the delayed free-recall
trials. Indeed, in Study 1 the consolidation and retrieval indi-
ces were not significant, but they did trend toward significance
in favor of the mindfulness condition. If this is the case, then a
mindfulness induction before the delayed free-recall trials but
after the learning trials could improve consolidation and re-
trieval while leaving encoding untouched. We would then
expect the delayed free-recall trials to be better for the mind-
fulness condition over the control, while the learning trials
would indicate no difference.
On the other hand, if mindfulness truly improves verbal
learning and memory through enhanced encoding, then with-
out a mindfulness induction before the learning trials, we
would expect no differences in verbal learning and memory
with regard to the learning trials or delayed free-recall trials,
even if participants were exposed to a mindfulness induction
before the two delayed free-recall trials. Furthermore, we
would expect no differences between conditions in terms of
their consolidation and retrieval indices, even with the audio
manipulations placed immediately before the delayed free-
recall trials. In Study 2 we made these adjustments to test for
these possibilities.
Finally, it is also possible that mindfulness wore off before
participants reached our measure of attention switching (i.e.,
TMT) in Study 1, thus leading to null results. Had mindfulness
been active during this measure, we may have found a differ-
ence. To test this, we moved the TMT closer to the mindful-
ness induction in Study 2.
Mem Cogn
Author's personal copy
Method
Participants
Participants were 57 college students (35 females and 22
males; 31 mindfulness and 26 control) of traditional college
age who were recruited from a large Midwestern university.
The final sample size was determined by a power analysis for
independent samples t-tests, with the desire to achieve a 90%
probability of detecting a large effect (d= .80), which required
approximately 27 participants per condition.
Measures
All measures were identical to Study 1 and were administered
in the same manner except for the SMS. Instead of the SMS,
we used three questions to measure state mindfulness. Two
questions came from the SMS and were presented on a 5-point
scale (0 Not at all, to 4 Very much; e.g., BDuring the audio,
I felt closely connected to the present moment^), while the
other was the one-question State MAAS (Ostafin & Kassman,
2012), presented on a 12-point scale (0 Strongly disagree, to
11 Strongly agree; BAt this moment I feel like I will rush
through activities without being really attentive to them^).
Procedure
The procedure had a few key differences from Study 1. First,
the mindfulness and control audios were administered before
the RAVLT in Study 1, whereas Study 2 placed these audios
after the RAVLT learning trials and interference trial but be-
fore the first delayed free-recall trial. Second, in Study 2 we
added a novel second audio (which varied by condition and
were similar to the audios used in Study 1) before the second
RAVLT delayed free-recall trial, given that there were approx-
imately 20 min in between delayed free-recall trials, and there
still existed the possibility of mindfulness dissipating before
the second delayed free-recall trial. Third, to account for the
addition of the second 10-min audio and the reordering of the
RAVLT in relation to the audio trials, we eliminated the filler
task and placed the remaining tests in an order designed to
provide approximately a 20-min gap between the two RAVLT
delayed free-recall trials.
All manipulations and measures were presented in the fol-
lowing order: MAAS, RAVLT learning trials and interference
trial, first 10-min audio tape (mindfulness/control), state mind-
fulness questions, first RAVLT delayed-recall trial, CWIT,
TMT, second 10-min audio tape (mindfulness/control), state
mindfulness questions, second RAVLT delayed free-recall tri-
al and recognition trial, FAS, and demographic questions.
Results
Preliminary results
An independent samples t-test revealed no differences be-
tween the mindfulness and control groups in trait mind-
fulness before any audio manipulation, (t< 1). However,
state mindfulness was significantly higher for the mind-
fulness condition after the first audio, t(55) = 4.06, p<
.001, d= 1.06 (Mindfulness: M= 12.48, SD = 2.82;
Control: M=8.81,SD = 4.00) and the second audio
t(55) = 2.38, p= .01, d= .63 (Mindfulness: M= 12.39,
SD = 3.30; Control: M= 10.27, SD = 3.39).
RAVLT and ISDA analyses for verbal learning
and memory
We then examined whether the mindfulness and control
conditions were different on the combined performance
for all five learning trials (which were completed before
any audio manipulation), as well as the combined
Table 1 Descriptive statistics for all attention comparisons
Test Mindfulness Control
CWIT
Time (in seconds)
Time 1 26.17 (4.74) 26.92 (4.74)
Time 2 19.96 (3.78) 20.69 (4.35)
Time 3 45.58 (12.67) 45.15 (8.49)
Time 4 55.09 (11.45) 54.36 (10.75)
Total Time 146.69 (27.88) 147.13 (22.88)
Times 3 and 4 100.40 (21.21) 99.51 (16.69)
Errors
Total 4.04 (3.26) 4.77 (3.39)
Trial 3 Uncorrected .37 (1.04) .67 (1.30)
Trial 3 Corrected 1.04 (1.30) .90 (1.10)
Trial 4 Uncorrected 1.11 (1.58) 1.54 (1.93)
Trial 4 Corrected 1.52 (1.81) 1.67 (1.56)
Trials 3 and 4 Uncorrected 1.48 (1.93) 2.21 (2.82)
Trials 3 and 4 Corrected 2.57 (2.59) 2.56 (1.83)
Pure processes
Inhibition 19.33 (9.49) 18.23 (6.79)
Cognitive Flexibility 9.24 (11.51) 9.21 (9.85)
Inhibition/Cog. Flexibility 28.91 (9.52) 27.44 (9.86)
Trail-Making Test (in seconds)
Trail A 19.72 (5.60) 20.26 (8.79)
Trail B 44.26 (16.60) 48.62 (21.15)
Trails A and B 63.98 (19.25) 68.87 (27.10)
Ratio B/A 2.33 (.86) 2.56 (.88)
Note. Data are given as means and standard deviations (in parentheses)
for all non-significant tests examined with regard to selective attention
(CWIT) and attention switching (Trail-Making Test)
Mem Cogn
Author's personal copy
performance on the two delayed free-recall trials (which
were all completed after an audio manipulation). There
were no differences with regard to the learning trials
(higher numbers means better performance), t(55) = 1.24,
p= .11 (Mindfulness: M= 47.32, SD = 7.46; Control: M=
49.77, SD = 7.44), or the delayed free-recall trials, t(55) =
1.34, p= .09 (Mindfulness: M= 19.03, SD =5.01;Control:
M= 20.69, SD = 4.22) Fig. 2. As this study predicted that
the null hypothesis would be supported for both of these
analyses, we calculated a Bayes factor for each using the
mean differences from Study 1 as an estimate of the effect
we should see if the mindfulness and control conditions are
different from each other. This mean difference was used
as the SD of a half normal (Dienes, 2014). As per Jeffrey
(1939), we interpret a Bayes factor above 3 as providing
substantial evidence for the alternative hypothesis, whereas
a Bayes factor below 1/3 providing substantial evidence
for the null hypothesis. The learning trials provided sup-
port for the null hypothesis, B
H(0, 4.80)
= .19, as did the
delayed recall trials, B
H(0, 2.12)
=.25.
To follow up with the ISDA, we examined the potential
difference between mindfulness and control conditions in
terms of their encoding, consolidation, and retrieval. The
encoding index was calculated using the five learning trials,
which were completed before any manipulation and therefore
were expected to be similar. This was the case, as there was no
significant difference between conditions, (t=1.00).Again,
we used the mean difference between conditions regarding
encoding from Study 1 as an estimate of the effect in order
to calculate a Bayes factor to determine whether or not the null
hypothesis was supported. Results indicated support for the
null hypothesis, B
H(0, 1.36)
=.23.
We then examined any potential consolidation or retrieval
benefits of mindfulness, as these indices were calculated using
the two delayed free-recall trials, each of which followed an
audio manipulation. Higher numbers on each index indicates a
greater deficit. There were no significant differences between
the mindfulness and control conditions for the consolidation
index, t(55) = 1.49, p= .07 (Mindfulness: M=.34,SD =.25;
Control: M=.25,SD = .18), or retrieval index, t(55) = 1.31, p
= .10 (Mindfulness: M=.14,SD = .11; Control: M=.17,SD =
.09). Once again, we calculated a Bayes factor with the results
from Study 1 as an estimate for the effect regarding consoli-
dation and retrieval. Results indicated some support for the
null hypothesis regarding consolidation, B
H(0, .0456)
=.43,
but no support for the null hypothesis regarding retrieval,
B
H(0, .0278)
=1.80.
General verbal fluency and attention
Similar to Study 1, we found no significant differences be-
tween the mindfulness and control conditions on our verbal
fluency measure (i.e., FAS), t< 1. Furthermore, our measure
of attention switching (i.e., TMT) also indicated no significant
differences among the four possible comparisons, (all ts < 1).
Our selective attention measure (CWIT) also found no signif-
icant differences among its comparisons, (all ts < 1). Tab. 2
Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 Trial 5 First
Delay
Second
Delay
Recognion
5
6
7
8
9
10
11
12
13
14
Mindfulness Control
RAVLT Performance
Words Remembered
RAVLT Trial
Fig. 2 Verbal learning and memory performance for both conditions on each Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Task (RAVLT) trial in Study 2. None of the
comparisons are significant at p= .05 except Recognition
Mem Cogn
Author's personal copy
Discussion
Study 2 found further support for the idea that mindfulness
improves verbal learning and memory through enhancing the
encoding process of memory rather than the consolidation and
retrieval processes. Without mindfulness during the learning
trials, the mindfulness condition did not perform any better on
the learning trials or delayed free-recall trials, even when giv-
en a mindfulness induction before both delayed free-recall
trials. This indicates that mindfulness must be present during
the learning process in order to enhance the encoding process
and subsequently recall the new verbal information at a later
time. Alternatively, it appears as though mindfulness is not
particularly effective at improving consolidation and retrieval
processes ifinstituted after a learning phase has already ended.
Additionally, we again found no effect of mindfulness on
general verbal fluency, indicating further that mindfulness is
particularly effective at helping to learn new verbal informa-
tion, rather than giving greater access to and retrieval of pre-
viously learned verbal material. Finally, the results of Study 2
reflected those of Study 1 with regard to attention. The mind-
fulness condition did not enhance selective attention or
attention-switching capabilities. As the attention-switching
task was placed much closer to the mindfulness induction in
Study 2, this indicates the null result found in Study 1 was not
due to mindfulness wearing off by the time participants
reached that task. Instead,we found evidence that mindfulness
did not alter selective attention or attention switching in either
study.
General discussion
Participants who engaged in mindfulness for a mere 10 min
exhibited improved verbal learning and memory through en-
hanced encoding. This demonstrates that even brief periods of
mindfulness can help people improve important areas of learn-
ing with relatively little investment before the learning process
begins, such as learning vocabulary, reading comprehension,
listening comprehension, etc. Furthermore, it seems likely that
experienced meditators who generally maintain a mindful
state throughout their day would exhibit higher encoding ca-
pabilities without the need to have recently engaged in medi-
tation. In this way, long-term meditators may indicate a greater
general capacity to learn, understand, and remember verbal
information within their environment throughout the course
of the day. Indeed, long-term meditators have shown better
verbal learning and memory performance on a task similar
to the RAVLT without having recently meditated (Lykins,
Baer, & Gottlob, 2012). It is important to understand if these
long-term meditators achieve these enhanced results due to
increased encoding capabilities as well, which would be a
fruitful area for future research. If so, it would elevate mind-
fulness as an extremely important tool to actively improve
verbal learning and memory processes through enhancing
the ability to encode new information.
This elevated learning through mindfulness should be able
to improve almost anyones verbal learning and memory ca-
pability without the need for invasive or difficult learning
techniques that may be off-putting or time-consuming for peo-
ple to incorporate into their lives. This seems particularly use-
ful for children, as learning progresses rapidly through early
school years. A number of school subjects may benefit from
enhanced verbal learning, such as learning vocabulary, learn-
ing a new language, understanding reading assignments, re-
membering important historical events, and many more.
Recent research with children has shown some promising ear-
ly signs regarding the benefits that mindfulness can have on
academic success (Ksendzov, 2017; Lu, Huang, & Rios,
2017). Research with children and the potential for increased
learning through mindfulness should be an area of primary
importance for future research.
Table 2 Descriptive statistics for all attention comparisons in Study 2
Test Mindfulness Control
CWIT
Time (in seconds)
Time 1 26.11 (3.72) 25.89 (2.25)
Time 2 19.69 (3.19) 19.30 (3.69)
Time 3 45.56 (11.46) 46.63 (8.20)
Time 4 53.50 (12.68) 54.19 (10.06)
Total Time 144.94 (26.12) 146.36 (15.56)
Times 3 and 4 99.08 (21.62) 100.82 (15.10)
Errors
Total 3.77 (3.45) 4.20 (4.19)
Trial 3 Uncorrected .77 (2.01) .92 (1.85)
Trial 3 Corrected 1.00 (1.13) 1.16 (1.40)
Trial 4 Uncorrected 1.23 (1.91) .84 (1.21)
Trial 4 Corrected 1.03 (1.22) 1.28 (1.43)
Trials 3 and 4 Uncorrected 1.70 (2.58) 1.76 (2.60)
Trials 3 and 4 Corrected 2.07 (1.82) 2.44 (2.62)
Pure processes
Inhibition 19.45 (9.41) 20.55 (8.01)
Cognitive Flexibility 7.91 (11.23) 7.56 (10.44)
Inhibition/Cog. Flexibility 27.46 (11.32) 28.11 (10.12)
Trail-Making Test (in seconds)
Trail A 21.51 (8.78) 20.57 (5.05)
Trail B 56.81 (17.38) 59.52 (19.74)
Trails A and B 78.32 (21.20) 80.20 (22.64)
Ratio B/A 2.89 (1.18) 2.91 (.85)
Note. Data are given as means and standard deviations (in parentheses)
for all non-significant tests examined with regards to selective attention
(CWIT) and attention switching (Trail-Making Test)
Mem Cogn
Author's personal copy
Additionally, our research demonstrated that mindfulness
increases encoding capabilities despite not improving atten-
tional capabilities, which implies that mindfulness may be a
particularly effective learning tool for people regardless of
their attentional capacity. While attention is important for
learning, and improving its capacity is valuable for the learn-
ing process, the ability of mindfulness to strengthen encoding
processes by bypassing attentional constraints can be incred-
ibly valuable in its own right. This may especially be the case
for those who struggle to pay attention, as this research implies
that mindfulness can improve verbal learning and memory
despite these difficulties. In this way, children or adults who
are vulnerable to distraction may still be able to improve their
ability to learn and recall new verbal information through
enhancing their encoding skills through mindfulness.
Perhaps this may even be true of clinical populations, such
as in people with Alzheimers disease whom show certain
impairments in encoding (Millet, Le Goff, Bouisson,
Dartigues, & Amieva, 2010). On the other hand, individuals
whose attentional capabilities are excellent, without much
room for improvement, may still be able to enhance their
verbal learning and memory capabilities by further improving
their encoding capacity through mindfulness. Theoretically,
this may work regardless of whether individuals show learn-
ing difficulties or not, which could potentially be another very
fertile area for future research.
While we found evidence that encoding enhancement was
not due to increases in attention, it is possible that other mea-
sures of attention may have been more likely to have been
affected by mindfulness, and therefore could have helped me-
diate the relationship between mindfulness and RAVLT per-
formance. We utilized measures of attention switching and
selective attention, but other attention measures (such as fo-
cused attention measures) could havebeen more likely to have
been enhanced by mindfulness. Future research should look at
these other attention measures as possible mediators between
mindfulness and improved verbal learning and memory.
Similarly, while we found that a brief mindfulness induc-
tion does not improve consolidation or retrieval processes, it is
possible that long-term practice may benefit these memory
processes. Taken together with our results, this would mean
that even a short mindfulness practice can help increase
encoding processes, which increases verbal learning and
memory, but long-term mindfulness practice could also
strengthen consolidation and retrieval, which would then en-
hance verbal learning and memory even more greatly. Future
research should examine whether consolidation and retrieval
processes are better in long-term meditators than controls.
Additionally, the current study demonstrated that mindful-
ness improved learning and memory in the verbal domain, but
it is possible that nonverbal learning capabilities may also
benefit from mindfulness. Future work will need to assess
whether visuo-spatial learning and memory is also enhanced
by a brief mindfulness practice. If it is the case that mindful-
ness can improve visuo-spatial learning and memory, this
would provide evidence that mindfulness can enhance general
learning capabilities, regardless of the modality in which
learning occurs. Preliminary evidence in this domain has
shown that mindfulness can enhance the recognition of visual
objects (Brown, Goodman, Ryan, & Analayo, 2016) and im-
prove visuo-spatial processing (Zeidan et al., 2010)
On the other hand, it could be that mindfulness is specifi-
cally good for verbal learning and memory, as perhaps the
enhanced encoding in this domain is produced by removing
cognitive load from the phonological loop an essential as-
pect of working memory that holds and manipulates auditory
information (Baddeley & Hitch, 1994). Activities that engage
the phonological loop do not necessarily impede with perfor-
mance in non-verbal tasks, such as spatial tasks, while spatial
tasks also do not necessarily impede with verbal tasks. For
instance, Baddeley (1992) reported an attempt to have partic-
ipants memorize chess-piece locations, a visuo-spatial task,
while loading either the phonological loop or the visuospatial
sketchpad. Compared to a control group, memory was im-
paired when the visuo-spatial sketchpad was loaded but not
when the phonological loop was loaded (also see Burnham,
Sabia, & Langan, 2014). When people allow their thoughts to
slip away from the current task into thoughts that are unrelated
to the task, this internal verbalization (or inner speech) likely
interferes with the task that currently requires the phonological
loop, which would negatively affect cognitive performance.
Alternatively, this internal verbalization may not have any
impact on other learning capabilities as long as the task does
not heavily involve the phonological loop. It is for this reason
that mindfulness may be particularly suited to improve verbal
learning and memory but not general learning capabilities.
This hypothesis is derived from the ability of mindful-
ness to help people disengage from their thoughts, includ-
ing decreasing rumination (Kumar, Felman, & C.H.S,
2008), even after a stressful event (Key, 2010).
Similarly, low levels of trait mindfulness are related to
elevated levels of rumination (Ciesla, Reilly, Dickson,
Emanuel, & Updegraff, 2012), and higher trait and state
mindfulness have also been shown to be associated with
lower rumination (Eisenlohr-Moul, Peters, Pond, &
DeWall, 2016). Similarly, mind-wandering is a natural
phenomenon (Killingsworth & Gilbert, 2010) that can be
alleviated through mindfulness (Mrazek et al., 2013;R
ahl
et al., 2017). Mind-wandering can be thought of as a form
of cognitive load, as it negatively impacts working mem-
ory through its apparent recruitment of working memory
resources (Kam & Handy, 2014). By allowing the mind to
let go and disengage from thoughts, it perhaps reduces
cognitive load by clearing the phonological loop, thereby
providing more resources for verbal learning and memory
tasks, but not necessarily for other non-verbal learning
Mem Cogn
Author's personal copy
tasks. If this is indeed the case, then mindfulness may not
lead to any improvements in visuo-spatial learning tasks.
Future research should examine this possibility.
Tremendous resources have been exhausted in the attempt
to improve learning capabilities and enhance the ability to
remember important information. The present research dem-
onstrates strong support for mindfulness as an important, ef-
fective, and easily utilized tool for improved verbal learning
and memory through its specific enhancements of the
encoding process. These improvements likely transcend age
groups, establishing mindfulness as a valuable learning tool
that can be started during childhood and maintained and fos-
tered throughout adulthood. Furthermore, the ease of imple-
mentation and limited requirements for practicing mindful-
ness make it a method of learning enhancement that is ap-
proachable and easily able to be incorporated into daily life.
While there still needs to be much investigation within this
field, early indications are promising that the more you prac-
tice mindfulness, the more you know.
Author Notes Both authors were equally involved in the de-
velopment of this research. As such, both authors share first
authorship, and both are corresponding authors. Data are
available upon email request.
Data Availability Data are available through email request. Neither study
was pre-registered.
References
Aggleton, J. P., & Brown, M. W. (2006). Interleaving brain systems for
episodic and recognition memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
10(10), 455-463. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1016/j.
tics.2006.08.003
Anderson, N. D., Lau, M. A., Segal, Z. V., & Bishop, S. R. (2007).
Mindfulness-based stress reduction and attentional control.
Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 14(6), 449-463. http://dx.
doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1002/cpp.544
Arbuthnott, K. & Frank, J. (2000). Trail making test, part B as a measure
of executive control: validation using a set-switching paradigm.
Journal of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology, 22,518528.
Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Is working memory working? The fifteenth
Bartlett lecture. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental
Psychology, 44A,131.
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1994). Developments in the concept of
working memory. Neuropsychology, 8(4), 485-493. http://dx.doi.
org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.485
Baer, R. A., Carmody, J., & Hunsinger, M. (2012). Weekly change in
mindfulness and perceived stress in a mindfulness-based stress re-
duction program. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68,755765. doi:
https://doi.org/10.1002/jclp.21865
Benton, A. L., de Hamsher, K. S., & Sivan, A. B. (1994). Multilingual
Aphasia Examination-Third Edition. Iowa City, IA: AJA
Associates.
Bonamo, K. K., Legerski, J., & Thomas, K. B. (2015). The influence of a
brief mindfulness exercise on encoding of novel words in female
college students. Mindfulness, 6(3), 535-544. http://dx.doi.org.
cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1007/s12671-014-0285-3 Retrieved from
http://cmich.idm.oclc.org/login?url=https://search-proquest-com.
cmich.idm.oclc.org/docview/1508428013?accountid=10181
Brown, K. W., Goodman, R. J., Ryan, R. M., & Anālayo, B. (2016).
Mindfulness enhances episodic memory performance: Evidence
from a multimethod investigation. PLoS ONE, 11(4), 19. Retrieved
from http://cmich.idm.oclc.org/login?url=https://search-proquest-
com.cmich.idm.oclc.org/docview/1898077554?accountid=10181
Brown, C. A., & Jones, A. K. P. (2013). Psychobiological correlates of
improved mental health in patients with musculoskeletal pain after a
mindfulness-based pain managemen t program. The Clinical Journal
of Pain, 29,233-244.doi:https://doi.org/10.1097/AJP.
0b013e31824c5d9f
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present:
Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 84,822848.
Burnham, B. R., Sabia, M., & Langan, C. (2014). Components of work-
ing memory and visual selective attention. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(1), 391-403.
http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1037/a0033753
Chambers, R., Lo, B. C. Y., & Allen, N. B. (2008). The impact of inten-
sive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and
affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32(3), 303-322. http://dx.
doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1007/s10608-007-9119-0
Christidi, F., Zalonis, I., Smyrnis, N., & Evdokimidis, I. (2012). Selective
attention and the three-process memory model for the interpretation
of verbal free recall in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society, 18(5), 809-18. http://dx.
doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1017/S1355617712000562
Ciesla, J., Reilly, L., Dickson, K., Emanuel, A., & Updegraff, J. (2012).
Dispositional mindfulness moderates the effects of stress among
adolescents: Rumination as a mediator. Journal of Clinical Child
and Adolescent Psychology, 41,760-770. doi:https://doi.org/10.
1080/15374416.2012.698724
Cropley, M., Ussher, M.,& Charitou, E. (2007). Acute effects of a guided
relaxation routine (body scan) on tobacco withdrawal symptoms and
cravings in abstinent smokers. Addiction, 102, 989993. doi:https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01832.x
Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller,
D., Santorelli, S. F., Sheridan, J. F. (2003). Alterations in brain
and immune function produced by mindfulness meditation.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 564-570. doi:https://doi.org/10.1097/
01.PSY.0000077505.67574.E3
Delis D. C., Kaplan E., Kramer J. H.. DelisKaplan Executive Function
System (D-KEFS): Examiners manual, 2001, San Antonio, TX.
The Psychological Corporation
Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E., & Ober, E. (2000). California
Verbal Learning TestII. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
Corporation.
Delis, D. C., Massman, P. J., Butters, N., Salmon, D. P., Cermak, L. S., &
Kramer, J. H. (1991). Profiles of demented and amnesic patients on
the california verbal learning test: Implications for the assessment of
memory disorders. Psychological Assessment, 3(1), 19-26. http://dx.
doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1037/1040-3590.3.1.19
Desrosiers, A., Vine, V., Klemanski, D. H., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S.
(2013). Mindfulness and emotion regulation in depression and anx-
iety: Common and distinct mechanisms of action. Depression and
Anxiety, 30(7), 654-61. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.
1002/da.22124
Dienes, Z. (2014). Using Bayes to get the most out of non-significant
results. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement, 5, 781. doi:
https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00781.
Dolan, R. J., & Fletcher, P. F. (1999). Encoding and retrieval in human
medial temporal lobes: An empirical investigation using functional
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). Hippocampus, 9(1), 25-34.
doi:3.0.CO;2-4" TARGET="_blank">http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.
Mem Cogn
Author's personal copy
oclc.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1999)9:1<25::AID-HIPO3>3.
0.CO;2-4
Dubert, C. J., Schumacher, A. M., Locker, L., Jr., Gutierrez, A. P., &
Barnes, V. A. (2016). Mindfulness and emotion regulation among
nursing students: Investigating the mediation effect of working
memory capacity. Mindfulness, 7(5), 1061-1070. http://dx.doi.org.
cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1007/s12671-016-0544-6
Eisenlohr-Moul, T., Peters, J. R., Pond, R. S., Jr., & DeWall, C. N. (2016).
Both trait and state mindfulness predict lower aggressiveness via
anger rumination: A multilevel mediation analysis. Mindfulness,
7(3), 713-726. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1007/
s12671-016-0508-x
Fernández, G., Weyerts, H., Schrader-Bölsche, M., Tendolkar, I., Smid,
H. G. O. M., Tempelmann, C., Heinze, H. (1998). Successful
verbal encoding into episodic memory engages the posterior hippo-
campus: A parametrically analyzed functional magnetic resonance
imaging study. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18(5), 1841-1847.
Retrieved from http://cmich.idm.oclc.org/login?url=https://search-
proquest-com.cmich.idm.oclc.org/docview/619293424?accountid=
10181
Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., and Johnson, M. (2001). Memory interfer-
ence during language processing. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 27, 14111423.
doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.27.6.1411
Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., and Johnson, M. (2004). Effects of noun
phrase type on sentence complexity. Journal of Memory and
Language, 51,97114. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.02.003
Greenberg, J., Romero, V. L., Elkin-Frankston, S., Bezdek, M. A.,
Schumacher, E. H., & Lazar, S. W. (2018). Reduced interference
in working memory following mindfulness training is associated
with increases in hippocampal volume. Brain Imaging and
Behavior, http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1007/s11682-
018-9858-4
Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and condi-
tional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
Herndon, F. (2008). Testing mindfulness with perceptual and cognitive
factors: External vs. internal encoding, and the cognitive failures ques-
tionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(1), 32-41. http://
dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.07.002
Hopthrow, T., Hooper, N., Mahmood, L., Meier, B. P., & Weger, U.
(2017). Mindfulness reduces the correspondence bias. The
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(3), 351-360.
http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.
1149498
Jeffreys,H. (1939/1961). The Theory of Probability, 1st/3rd Edn. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Jensen, C. G., Vangkilde, S., Frokjaer, V., & Hasselbalch, S. G. (2012).
Mindfulness training affects attentionOr is it attentional effort?
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(1), 106-123.
http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1037/a0024931
Jha, A. P., Stanley, E. A., Kiyonaga, A., Wong, L., & Gelfand, L. (2010).
Examining the protective effects of mindfulness training on working
memory capacity and affective experience. Emotion, 10(1), 54-64.
http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1037/a0018438
Josefsson, T., & Broberg, A. (2011). Meditators and non-meditators on
sustained and executive attentional performance. Mental Health,
Religion & Culture, 14(3), 291-309. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.
oclc.org/10.1080/13674670903578621
Kabat-Zinn, J., Wheeler, E., Light, T., Skillings, A., Scharf, M. J.,
Cropley, T. G., Bernhard, J. D. (1998). Influence of a mindfulness
meditation-based stress reduction intervention on rates of skin clear-
ing in patients with moderate to severe psoriasis undergoing photo-
therapy (UVB) and photochemotherapy (PUVA). Psychosomatic
Medicine, 60(5), 625-632. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.
1097/00006842-199809000-00020
Kam, J. W. Y., & Handy, T. C. (2014). Differential recruitment of exec-
utive resources during mind wandering. Consciousness and
Cognition, 26, 51-63. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.
1016/j.concog.2014.03.002
Kerr, C. E., Jones, S. R., Wan, Q., Pritchett, D. L., Wasserman, R. H.,
Wex l e r, A . , Moore, C. I. (2011). Effects of mindfulness medita-
tion training on anticipatory alpha modulation in primary somato-
sensory cortex. Brain Research Bulletin, 85(3-4), 96-103. http://dx.
doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.03.026
Key, B. L. (2010). The influence of rumination, distraction and mindful-
ness on cardiovascular recovery from stress (Order No. NR69488).
Available from ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global.
(848965766). Retrieved from http://cmich.idm.oclc.org/login?url=
https://search-proquest-com.cmich.idm.oclc.org/docview/
848965766?accountid=10181
Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an
unhappy mind. Science, 330,932.
Kliegl, O., Pastötter, B., & Bäuml, K. T. (2015). The contribution of
encoding and retrieval processes to proactive interference. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition,
41(6), 1778-1789. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1037/
xlm0000096
Kong, F., Wang, X., Song, Y., & Liu, J. (2016). Brain regions involved in
dispositional mindfulness during resting state and their relation with
well-being. Social Neuroscience, 11(4), 331-343. http://dx.doi.org.
cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1080/17470919.2015.1092469
Ksendzov, E. (2017). Associative relationship among mindfulness, aca-
demic grades, and affective outcomes in adolescence (Order No.
AAI10164953). Available from PsycINFO. (1910931448; 2017-
01058-262). Retrieved from http://cmich.idm.oclc.org/login?url=
https://search-proquest-com.cmich.idm.oclc.org/docview/
1910931448?accountid=10181
Kumar, S. M., Feldman, G. C., & Hayes, S. C. (2008). Changes in mind-
fulness end emotion regulation in an exposure based cognitive ther-
apy or depression. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 32,734744.
Lagor, A. F., Williams, D. J., Lerner, J. B., & McClure, K. S. (2013).
Lessons learned from a mindfulness-based intervention with chron-
ically ill youth. Clinical Practice in Pediatric Psychology, 1,146-
158. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/cpp0000015
Langer, E. J., Bashner, R. S., & Chanowitz, B. (1985). Decreasing prej-
udice by increasing discrimination. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 49, 113-120. doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
3514.49.1.113
Lewicki, P. (2005). Internal and external encoding style and social moti-
vation. In J. P. Forgas, K. D. Williams & S. M. Laham (Eds.), Social
motivation: Conscious and unconscious processes; social motiva-
tion: Conscious and unconscious processes (pp. 194-209,
Chapter xx, 386 Pages) Cambridge University Press, New York,
NY. Retrieved from http://cmich.idm.oclc.org/login?url=https://
search-proquest-com.cmich.idm.oclc.org/docview/620554589?
accountid=10181
Lu, S., Huang, C., & Rios, J. (2017). Mindfulness and academic perfor-
mance: An example of migrant children in China. Children and
Youth Services Review, 82, 53-59. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.
org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2017.09.008 Retrieved from http://cmich.
idm.oclc.org/login?url=https://search-proquest-com.cmich.idm.
oclc.org/docview/1976695701?accountid=10181
Lueke, A., & Gibson, B. (2015). Mindfulness meditation reduces implicit
age and race bias: The role of reduced automaticity of responding.
Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(3), 284-291. http://
dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1177/1948550614559651
Lueke, A., & Gibson, B. (2016). Brief mindfulness meditation reduces
discrimination. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research,
and Practice, 3(1), 34-44. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.
1037/cns0000081
Mem Cogn
Author's personal copy
Lykins, E. L. B., Baer, R. A., & Gottlob,L. R. (2012). Performance-based
tests of attention and memory in long-term mindfulness meditators
and demographically matched nonmeditators. Cognitive Therapy
and Research, 36(1), 103-114. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.
org/10.1007/s10608-010-9318-y Retrieved from http://cmich.idm.
oclc.org/login?url=https://search-proquest-com.cmich.idm.oclc.org/
docview/964200583?accountid=10181
Magalhaes, S., & Hamdan, A. C. (2010). The Rey Auditory Verbal
Learning Test: Normative data for the Brazilian population and anal-
ysis of the influence of demographic variables. Psychology &
Neuroscience,3, 85-91.
Millet, X., Le Goff, M., Bouisson, J., Dartigues, J., & Amieva, H. (2010).
Encoding processes influence word-stem completion priming in
alzheimer's disease: A meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(5), 494-504. http://dx.doi.org.
cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1080/13803390903224936
Mrazek, M. D., Franklin, M. S., Phillips, D. T., Baird, B., & Schooler, J.
W. (2013). Mindfulness training improves working memory capac-
ity and GRE performance while reducing mind wandering.
Psychological Science, 24(5), 776-781. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.
idm.oclc.org/10.1177/0956797612459659
Ostafin, B. D., & Kassman, K. T. (2012). Stepping out of history:
Mindfulness improves insight problem solving. Consciousness and
Cognition, 21,10311036.
Parks, S., Birtel, M. D., & Crisp, R. J. (2014). Evidence that a brief
meditation exercise can reduce prejudice toward homeless people.
Social Psychology, 45(6), 458-465. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.
oclc.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000212
Pratscher, S. D., Rose, A. J., Markovitz, L., & Bettencourt, A. (2017).
Interpersonal mindfulness: Investigating mindfulnessin interperson-
al interactions, co-rumination, and friendship quality. Mindfulness,
http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1007/s12671-017-0859-y
Quach, D., Mano, K. E. J., & Alexander, K. (2016). A randomized con-
trolled trial examining the effect of mindfulness meditation on work-
ing memory capacity in adolescents. JournalofAdolescentHealth,
58(5), 489-496. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1016/j.
jadohealth.2015.09.024
Rahl, H. A., Lindsay, E. K., Pacilio, L. E., Brown, K. W., & Creswell, J.
D. (2017). Brief mindfulness meditation training reduces mind wan-
dering: The critical role of acceptance. Emotion, 17(2), 224-230.
http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/10.1037/emo0000250
Ramsey, A. T., & Jones, E. E. (2015). Minding the interpersonal gap:
Mindfulness-based interventions in the prevention of ostracism.
Consciousness and Cognition, 31, 24-34. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.
idm.oclc.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.10.003
Reitan, R., & Wolfson, D. (1993). The halstead-reitan neuropsychologi-
cal test battery: Theory and clinical applications. Tucson, AZ:
Neuropsychology Press.
Ruff, R. M., Light, R. H., Parker, S. B., & Levin, H. S. (1997). The
psychological construct of word fluency. Brain and Language, 57,
394405. doi:https://doi.org/10.1006/brin.1997.1755
Schoenberg, M.R., Dawson, K.A., Duff, K., Patton, D., Scott, J.G., &
Adams, R.L. (2006). Test performance and classification statistics
for the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test in selected clinical sam-
ples. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21, 693-703.
Schofield, T. P., Creswell, J. D., & Denson, T. F. (2015). Brief mindfulness
induction reduces inattentional blindness. Consciousness and Cognition:
An International Journal, 37, 63-70. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.org/
10.1016/j.concog.2015.08.007 Retrieved from http://cmich.idm.oclc.org/
login?url=https://search-proquest-com.cmich.idm.oclc.org/docview/
1803818840?accountid=10181
Strange, B. A., Otten, L. J., Josephs, O., Rugg, M. D., & Dolan, R. J.
(2002). Dissociable human perirhinal, hippocampal, and
parahippocampal roles during verbal encoding. The Journal of
Neuroscience, 22(2), 523-528. Retrieved from http://cmich.idm.
oclc.org/login?url=https://search-proquest-com.cmich.idm.oclc.org/
docview/619830312?accountid=10181
Strauss E.H., Sherman E.M., & Spreen O. (2006). A compendium of
neuropsychological tests: administration, norms, and commentary.
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Stroop J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions,
Journal of Experimental Psychology,18, 643-666.
Tanay, G., & Bernstein, A. (2013). State Mindfulness Scale (SMS):
Development and initial validation. Psychological Assessment, 25,
12861299. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034044
Vakil, E., & Blachstein, H. (1993). Rey auditory-verbal learning test:
Structure analysis. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49(6), 883-890.
doi:3.0.CO;2-6" TARGET="_blank">http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.
oclc.org/10.1002/1097-4679(199311)49:6<883::AID-
JCLP2270490616>3.0.CO;2-6
Villata, S., Tabor, W., & Franck, J. (2018). Encoding and retrieval inter-
ference in sentence comprehension: Evidence from agreement.
Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 16. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.oclc.
org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00002
Whiteside, D. M., Kealey, T., Semla, M., Luu, H., Rice, L., Basso, M. R.
and Roper, B., (2016). Verbal fluency: language or executive func-
tion measure? Applied Neuropsychology, 23,2934.
Wright, M. J., & Schmitter-Edgecombe, M. (2011). The impact of verbal
memory encoding and consolidation deficits during recovery from
moderate-to-severe traumatic brain injury. The Journal of Head
Trauma Rehabilitation, 26(3), 182-191. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.
idm.oclc.org/10.1097/HTR.0b013e318218dcf9
Wright, M. J., Woo, E., Foley, J., Ettenhofer, M. L., Cottingham, M. E.,
Gooding, A. L., Hinkin, C. H. (2011). Antiretroviral adherence
and the nature of HIV-associated verbal memory impairment. The
Journal ofNeuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 23(3), 324-
31. Retrieved from http://cmich.idm.oclc.org/login?url=https://
search-proquest-com.cmich.idm.oclc.org/docview/916301253?
accountid=10181
Wright, M.J., Woo, E., Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., Hinkin, C.H., Miller,
E.N., & Gooding, A.L. (2009). The item-specific deficit approach to
evaluating verbal memory dysfunction: Rationale, psychometrics,
and application. Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Neuropsychology, 31,790802.
Yu, K., Seal, M. L., Reyes, J., Godfrey, C., Anderson, V., Adamson, C., ...
Catroppa, C. (2018). Brain volumetric correlates of inhibition and
cognitive flexibility 16 years following childhood traumatic brain
injury. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 96,642651.
Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z., & Goolkasian, P.
(2010). Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: Evidence of
brief mental training. Consciousness and Cognition: An
International Journal, 19(2), 597-605. http://dx.doi.org.cmich.idm.
oclc.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.03.014
PublishersnoteSpringer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Mem Cogn
Author's personal copy
... Although no between-group effects emerged, within-group analyses revealed notable prepost improvements in VF in MEMI but not SM. Findings might be due to evidence that brief MBIs could enhance verbal learning and memory via refinements in the ability to register information (Lueke & Lueke, 2019). Relatedly, since VF is associated with aptitude to efficiently recall words linked to emotions (Hegefeld, Satpute, Ochsner, Davidow, & Nook, 2023), MEMI might have strengthened VF of emotion and non-emotion words more than SM (Edwards, Shivaji, & Wupperman, 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
BACKGROUND: Theory and research indicated that executive functioning (EF) correlated with, preceded, and stemmed from worry in generalized anxiety disorder (GAD). The present secondary analysis (Zainal & Newman, 2023b) thus determined whether EF domains mediated the effect of a 14-day (5 prompts/day) mindfulness ecological momentary intervention (MEMI) against a self-monitoring control (SM) for GAD. METHOD: Participants (N=110) diagnosed with GAD completed self-reported (Attentional Control Scale, GAD Questionnaire, Perseverative Cognitions Questionnaire) and performance-based tests (Letter-Number Sequencing, Stroop, Trail Making Test-B, Verbal Fluency) at baseline, post-treatment, and one-month follow-up (1MFU). Causal mediation analyses determined if pre-post changes in EF domains preceded and mediated the effect of MEMI against SM on pre-1MFU changes in GAD severity and trait repetitive negative thinking (RNT). RESULTS: MEMI was more efficacious than SM in improving pre-post inhibition (β=-2.075, 95% [-3.388, -0.762], p=.002), WM (β=0.512, 95% [0.012, 1.011], p=.045), and set-shifting (β=-2.916, 95% [-5.142, -0.691], p=.010) but not verbal fluency and attentional control. Within groups, MEMI but not SM produced improvements in all examined pre-post EF outcomes except attentional control. Only pre-post improvements in inhibition mediated the effect of MEMI against SM on pre-1MFU reductions in GAD severity (β=-0.605, 95% [-1.357, -0.044], p=.030; proportion mediated=7.1%) and trait RNT (β=-0.024, 95% [-0.054, -0.001], p=.040; proportion mediated=7.4%). These patterns remained after conducting sensitivity analyses with non-linear mediator-outcome relations. CONCLUSIONS: Optimizing MEMI for GAD might entail specifically boosting inhibition plausibly by augmenting it with dialectical behavioral therapy, encouraging high-intensity physical exercises, and targeting negative emotional contrast avoidance.
... In particular, clients can be encouraged to approach experiences thinking about the sensory details of these experiences as they occur while thinking about how these events are unfolding and the steps that we are taking that lead to this unfolding. Fostering mindful, present moment awareness through brief direction to be attentionally focused, or through mindfulness-boosting techniques either in or between sessions, may be one strategy with which to improve the encoding of new information for memory storage (Lueke & Lueke, 2019). Clients may then be directed to use meta-cognitive awareness to focus on concrete detail while engaging in other therapeutic tasks, such as enacting pleasant activities, externally-focusing on social situations to reduce self-consciousness, or undertaking exposure exercises. ...
... Consistent with our hypothesis, parents with greater mindful attention at baseline used Questions and Instructions less during the follow-up phase (post through 6-month followup) and had lower rates of Questions and Instructions at 6-month follow-up than parents with lower mindful attention. Research indicates that verbal learning and memory can be improved by mindfulness practice (Lueke & Lueke, 2019), suggesting that parents with greater mindful attention may have more fully encoded the rationale for reducing Questions and Instructions during Child's Game. This possible improved encoding, paired with potentially reduced reactive or automatic responding, may have aided parents with greater mindful attention in maintaining their reduction of these discouraged behaviors at follow-up. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives Behavioral parent training (BPT) is considered the standard of care for early-onset behavior disorders which disproportionately affect children from low-income households. However, BPT does not work equally well for all families, highlighting the importance of better understanding predictors of treatment progress and outcomes. Parent mindfulness has been discussed as one such construct that may improve treatment response, and researchers have adapted BPT to target mindfulness with mixed results. Past research has not examined parent dispositional mindfulness as a predictor of BPT skill mastery. Method This study explored the link between parents’ baseline self-report of mindful attention, an aspect of dispositional mindfulness, and observed parent skill mastery at post-treatment and follow-up in a sample of 101 families with low income during a mastery-based BPT program validated for young children, Helping the Noncompliant Child. Results Results of a spline growth model suggest that parents with greater mindful attention achieved higher levels of skill mastery than those with lower mindful attention. However, this varied by aspect of skill mastery and assessment timepoint. Conclusions Parent mindful attention was associated with parental use of BPT skills. Findings support continued examination of the potential benefits of mindfulness-adapted BPT and have the potential to inform tailored BPT programs consistent with a precision medicine approach. Clinical implications and future directions are discussed. Preregistration This study is not preregistered.
... This novel observation enriches our understanding of mindfulness's potential to enhance memory functions in this demographic. Plausible mechanisms underlying mindfulness's positive effects on episodic memory encompass increased attention, decreased cognitive interference [70] or improved encoding capabilities [71]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background The development of effective strategies to maintain good mental health of older adults is a public health priority. Mindfulness-based interventions have the potential to improve psychological well-being and cognitive functions of older adults, but little is known about the effect of such interventions when delivered through internet. During the COVID-19 pandemic we evaluated short- and long-term cognitive, psychological, and physiological effects of a mindfulness-based intervention (MBI) delivered via web-based videoconference in healthy older adults. Methods Fifty older adults participated in an 8-week MBI, which comprised structured 2-h weekly group sessions. A comprehensive evaluation encompassing cognitive (verbal memory, attention and processing speed, executive functions) and psychological assessments (depression and anxiety symptoms, mindfulness, worries, emotion regulation strategies, well-being, interoceptive awareness and sleep) was conducted. Additionally, electroencephalography (EEG) data were recorded before and after the MBI and at the 6-month follow-up (T6). Data were analyzed using an intention-to-treat approach, using linear mixed models adjusted for age. The effect size for time was computed as omega squared. Results We observed significant improvements from pre-MBI to post-MBI and at the T6 across several measures. These improvements were notable in the areas of verbal memory (California Verbal Learning Test, p ≤ .007), attention and executive functions (Trail Making Test A and BA, p < .050), interoceptive awareness (Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness, p = .0002 for self-regulation and p < .05 for noticing, body listening, and trusting dimensions), and rumination (Heidelberg Form for Emotion Regulation Strategies, p = .018). These changes were associated with low to medium effect size. Moreover, we observed significant changes in EEG patterns, with a decrease in alpha1 (p = .004) and an increase in alpha2 (p < .0001) from pre-MBI to T6. Notably, improvements in TMTBA and rumination were correlated with the decrease in alpha1 (p < .050), while improvements in TMTA were linked to the increase in alpha2 (p = .025). Conclusions The results of our study show that a web-based MBI in older adults leads to improvements in cognitive and psychological measures, with associated modulations in specific brain rhythms. While these findings are promising, further controlled studies are required to validate these preliminary results. Trial registration The trial has been registered with the United States National Library of Medicine at the National Institutes of Health Registry of Clinical Trials under the code NCT05941143 on July 12, 2023.
... For example, they might encourage engagement in low-impact exercise with a cognitive component (eg, Tai Chi) and mindfulness meditation which appear to be effective in improving cognitive functioning. 36,37 Similarly, social engagement provides opportunities for social support and is associated with improved cognitive functioning. 36 It is important to emphasize, however, that intervention needs to be individualized given the cause and presentation of CD will vary across patients. ...
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive dysfunction (CD) is a common, impairing symptom experienced by persons with fibromyalgia (FM). This study explored how individuals with FM describe their experience of CD in an online peer support environment. Posts referencing cognitive symptoms were extracted from two Facebook FM peer support groups at two timepoints. Using inductive Thematic Analysis, key discussion themes were identified and compared across groups and time. Four themes represented the way members described their cognitive experiences in FM: understanding and describing CD experiences, distrust of cognitive abilities, choosing between pain and medication side effects, and misunderstanding and judgement from others. Two themes represented the impact of CD on members’ lives: impaired social interaction and loss of identity. Group members described broad impairments in daily functioning that significantly and negatively impacted their quality of life, indicating CD symptoms may play a more prominent role in the FM experience than previously understood. An infographic is included to aid patient education and help facilitate patient-provider discussions of CD symptoms.
... For instance, the research of Mrazek et al proves that mindfulness training can possibly help in the management of second language anxiety and eventually improve the language test score among university students learning English [66]. Similarly, Lueke and Lueke unveil that mindfulness can possibly improve learners' working memory by speeding up their encoding process [67]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Foreign language anxiety, also known as language learner apprehension or second language nervousness, has been extensively investigated in academia as it poses particular challenges to language learners. The paper provides a comprehensive review of foreign language anxiety, exploring its definitions, classifications, potential causes, and strategies to reduce such anxiety. Three classifications of anxiety are listed in section 2, while internal and external causes have been carefully examined in section 3 based on previous works. In the end, possible strategies for reducing this kind of anxiety are proposed in Section 4 based on previous analysis of the types and motivations. By analyzing various aspects of anxious emotion in language acquisition process, this review aims to shed light on the importance of addressing this phenomenon to enhance language acquisition and communication skills.
Article
Full-text available
Several recent studies have explored the relationships between mindfulness and time perception, an area of research that has become increasingly popular in the last 10–15 years. In this article, we present a systematic integrative review of the evidence on this subject. We also integrate the field’s findings into a conceptual framework which considers the multifaceted nature of both mindfulness, and time perception research. To identify the relevant literature, we searched the following databases using relevant keywords: PsycINFO; Medline; EBSCO Host Psychology and Behavioral Sciences Collection; and Web of Science. These searches were last performed on the 4th of May 2022, and additional hand searches were also conducted. To be included, articles had to be in English and contain original data about the potential relationship(s) between mindfulness and time perception. Articles which did not present usable data about the relationship(s) between the variables of interest were excluded. In total, 47 research articles were included in the review (combined sample size of ∼5800 participants). Risks of bias in the selected studies were evaluated using two separate assessment tools designed for this purpose. Through an integrative narrative synthesis, this article reviews how mindfulness may relate to time perception for various reference frames, and for various time perception measures and methods. It also provides new insights by exploring how a wide range of findings can be integrated into a coherent whole, in light of some relevant time perception models and mindfulness theories. Altogether, the reviewed data suggest the existence of complex and multifaceted relationships between mindfulness and time perception, highlighting the importance of considering many factors when planning research or interpreting data in this field. Limitations of the current review include the scarceness of data for certain categories of findings, and the relatively low prevalence of studies with a randomized controlled design in the source literature. This research was partly funded by a grant from the Natural Science and Engineering Research Council of Canada.
Article
Full-text available
Proactive interference occurs when previously relevant information interferes with retaining newer material. Overcoming proactive interference has been linked to the hippocampus and deemed critical for cognitive functioning. However, little is known about whether and how this ability can be improved or about the neural correlates of such improvement. Mindfulness training emphasizes focusing on the present moment and minimizing distraction from competing thoughts and memories. It improves working memory and increases hippocampal density. The current study examined whether mindfulness training reduces proactive interference in working memory and whether such improvements are associated with changes in hippocampal volume. 79 participants were randomized to a 4-week web-based mindfulness training program or a similarly structured creative writing active control program. The mindfulness group exhibited lower proactive interference error rates compared to the active control group following training. No group differences were found in hippocampal volume, yet proactive interference improvements following mindfulness training were significantly associated with volume increases in the left hippocampus. These results provide the first evidence to suggest that (1) mindfulness training can protect against proactive interference, and (2) that these benefits are related to hippocampal volumetric increases. Clinical implications regarding the application of mindfulness training in conditions characterized by impairments to working memory and reduced hippocampal volume such as aging, depression, PTSD, and childhood adversity are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Long-distance verb-argument dependencies generally require the integration of a fronted argument when the verb is encountered for sentence interpretation. Under a parsing model that handles long-distance dependencies through a cue-based retrieval mechanism, retrieval is hampered when retrieval cues also resonate with non-target elements (retrieval interference). However, similarity-based interference may also stem from interference arising during the encoding of elements in memory (encoding interference), an effect that is not directly accountable for by a cue-based retrieval mechanism. Although encoding and retrieval interference are clearly distinct at the theoretical level, it is difficult to disentangle the two on empirical grounds, since encoding interference may also manifest at the retrieval region. We report two self-paced reading experiments aimed at teasing apart the role of each component in gender and number subject-verb agreement in Italian and English object relative clauses. In Italian, the verb does not agree in gender with the subject, thus providing no cue for retrieval. In English, although present tense verbs agree in number with the subject, past tense verbs do not, allowing us to test the role of number as a retrieval cue within the same language. Results from both experiments converge, showing similarity-based interference at encoding, and some evidence for an effect at retrieval. After having pointed out the non-negligible role of encoding in sentence comprehension, and noting that Lewis and Vasishth’s (2005) ACT-R model of sentence processing, the most fully developed cue-based retrieval approach to sentence processing does not predict encoding effects, we propose an augmentation of this model that predicts these effects. We then also propose a self-organizing sentence processing model (SOSP), which has the advantage of accounting for retrieval and encoding interference with a single mechanism.
Article
Full-text available
There is growing interest in understanding the ways in which mindfulness influences interpersonal relationships. Two studies investigated the relationship between a newly proposed construct, interpersonal mindfulness, and the quality of a best or close friend. Interpersonal mindfulness is conceptualized as mindfulness during interpersonal interactions and includes awareness of self and others, accompanied with the qualities of nonjudgmental and nonreactive presence. Study 1 showed that interpersonal mindfulness was correlated with friendship quality, while accounting for trait mindfulness. Study 2 replicated and extended these findings by identifying and testing three possible mediators (i.e., perspective taking, basic psychological need satisfaction, and empathy) of the association between interpersonal mindfulness and friendship quality. Interrelations with co-rumination, or excessive talk about problems, were examined as well. Investigation of the psychometrics of the interpersonal mindfulness scale provided initial support for the reliability and validity. Results suggest that (1) interpersonal mindfulness is uniquely associated with the interpersonal outcome of friendship quality (controlling for trait mindfulness), whereas trait mindfulness is uniquely associated with the intrapersonal outcomes of depression and anxiety (while controlling for interpersonal mindfulness), (2) interpersonal mindfulness moderates the association of co-rumination and friendship quality, and (3) the association of interpersonal mindfulness and friendship quality is mediated by perspective taking and psychological need satisfaction. Limitations and future directions, as well as potential applied applications, are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Mindfulness meditation programs, which train individuals to monitor their present-moment experience in an open or accepting way, have been shown to reduce mind wandering on standardized tasks in several studies. Here we test 2 competing accounts for how mindfulness training reduces mind wandering, evaluating whether the attention-monitoring component of mindfulness training alone reduces mind wandering or whether the acceptance training component is necessary for reducing mind wandering. Healthy young adults (N = 147) were randomized to either a 3-day brief mindfulness training condition incorporating instruction in both attention monitoring and acceptance, a mindfulness training condition incorporating attention monitoring instruction only, a relaxation training condition, or an active reading-control condition. Participants completed measures of dispositional mindfulness and treatment expectancies before the training session on Day 1 and then completed a 6-min Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART) measuring mind wandering after the training session on Day 3. Acceptance training was important for reducing mind wandering, such that the attention-monitoring plus acceptance mindfulness training condition had the lowest mind wandering relative to the other conditions, including significantly lower mind wandering than the attention-monitoring only mindfulness training condition. In one of the first experimental mindfulness training dismantling studies to-date, we show that training in acceptance is a critical driver of mindfulness-training reductions in mind wandering. This effect suggests that acceptance skills may facilitate emotion regulation on boring and frustrating sustained attention tasks that foster mind wandering, such as the SART.
Article
Full-text available
This study examined the relationship between mindfulness and emotion regulation in nursing students as well as the potential mediator role of working memory capacity in this relationship. The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS), Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ), and an Automated Operation Span Task (AOSPAN) were administered to 80 undergraduate pre-licensure baccalaureate nursing students. Pearson correlation, structural equation path modeling, and one-way ANOVA tests were conducted. MAAS scores were significantly correlated with ERQ-Reappraisal scores (r = 0.19, p = 0.045) and AOSPAN scores (r = 0.30, p = 0.004). A structural equation path model indicated that there was a direct effect of dispositional mindfulness on emotion regulation (γ11 = 0.29, p = 0.034) and working memory capacity (γ21 = 4.98, p = 0.004). However, working memory capacity did not directly mediate the effect of mindfulness on emotion regulation (b weight = −0.03, p = 0.236). Also, MAAS scores were significantly different (p < 0.05) between the student cohort levels, with the first semester students having the highest MAAS scores and the fourth semester students have the lowest MAAS scores. These findings suggest that mindfulness may influence prospective nursing professionals’ working memory capacity and ability to regulate emotions. However, working memory capacity did not explain how mindfulness influenced the use of reappraisal as an emotion regulation strategy. Nurse educators should consider other potential mechanisms of how mindfulness influences emotion regulation in nursing students. Furthermore, educators should consider that potential differences in mindfulness exist between nursing students when they implement mindfulness-based interventions.
Article
Mindfulness emphasizes the awareness of the “here-and-now.” Studies in the recent decade have found that mindfulness can affect positive change in child and adolescent school performance. As an emerging field, however, the mechanisms and effects of mindfulness practice on academic performance for children in devel- oping countries have not been elaborated sufficiently in previous studies. Using China as an example context, this paper examines the relationship between mindfulness practice and academic performance of migrant children, and explores the role of executive function in this relationship. Through a survey with 219 fifth-graders in two migrant schools in Beijing, this study validates the positive association between mindfulness, executive function, and better school grades in Chinese, math, and English tests. Additionally, this study adds empirical evidence to the roles of mindfulness in child development, and also sheds light on the pathway through which mindfulness positively correlates with academic performance. These findings provide implications for addressing child academic challenges and conducting further research among migrant children in China and beyond.
Article
Executive functions (EFs), such as inhibition and cognitive flexibility, are essential for everyday functioning, including regulation of socially appropriate emotional responses. These skills develop during childhood and continue maturing into early adulthood. The current study aimed to investigate the very long-term impact of childhood traumatic brain injury (TBI) on inhibition and cognitive flexibility, and to examine whether global white matter is associated with these abilities. Twenty-eight young adult survivors of childhood TBI (mean age at 16-year follow-up = 21.67 years, SD = 2.70) and 16 typically developing controls (TDCs), group-matched for age, sex, and socioeconomic status, completed tests of inhibition and cognitive flexibility and underwent structural MRI. Survivors of childhood TBI did not significantly differ from TDCs on EF or white matter volume. However, the relationship between EF and white matter volume differed between survivors of TBI and TDCs. Survivors of TBI did not mimic the brain behavior relationship that characterized EF in TDCs. The inverse brain behavior relationship, exhibited by childhood TBI survivors, suggests disruptions in the whole brain underpinning EF following childhood TBI.