ArticlePDF Available

Does suppressing negative emotion impair subsequent emotions? Two experience sampling studies

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Suppression is one of the most commonly studied emotion-regulation strategies and a variety of studies have shown that suppression of emotions is associated with adverse affective outcomes. Most of the evidence for this conclusion comes from laboratory manipulations in which people enact experimentally induced suppression or from survey-based recollections. In the present pair of studies (468 participants total), we used real-time experience sampling data to examine the effect of naturally occurring suppression of negative emotion at one moment on subsequent reports of both negative and positive emotion. Results demonstrated that suppression led to later increases in both high-activation and low-activation negative emotions, over and above the level of negative emotion being suppressed. These findings add ecologically valid support to the growing body of evidence showing that emotional suppression is not only an ineffective emotion-regulation strategy, but also a costly one.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Vol.:(0123456789)
1 3
Motivation and Emotion (2020) 44:427–435
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-019-09774-w
ORIGINAL PAPER
Does suppressing negative emotion impair subsequent emotions?
Two experience sampling studies
YanRuan1,3 · HarryT.Reis1· WojciechZareba1· RichardD.Lane2
Published online: 18 June 2019
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2019
Abstract
Suppression is one of the most commonly studied emotion-regulation strategies and a variety of studies have shown that
suppression of emotions is associated with adverse affective outcomes. Most of the evidence for this conclusion comes from
laboratory manipulations in which people enact experimentally induced suppression or from survey-based recollections.
In the present pair of studies (468 participants total), we used real-time experience sampling data to examine the effect of
naturally occurring suppression of negative emotion at one moment on subsequent reports of both negative and positive
emotion. Results demonstrated that suppression led to later increases in both high-activation and low-activation negative
emotions, over and above the level of negative emotion being suppressed. These findings add ecologically valid support to
the growing body of evidence showing that emotional suppression is not only an ineffective emotion-regulation strategy,
but also a costly one.
Keywords Emotion suppression· Experience sampling· Emotion regulation· Negative affect
Introduction
Suppression is one of the most commonly studied emotion-
regulation strategies. Most research associates suppression
of emotions with adverse affective outcomes. For example,
in laboratory studies, experimentally induced suppression
has been shown to lead to worse mood (Campbell-Sills etal.
2006; Gross 1998; Gross and Levenson 1995), increased
sympathetic activation (Demaree etal. 2006; Gross and
Levenson 1993; Roberts etal. 2008), and negative feelings
about social interactions (Butler etal. 2003). In correlational
studies, expressive suppression has been related to continued
or worsened negative emotions (Ehring etal. 2010; John and
Gross 2004; Moore etal. 2008; Naragon-Gainey etal. 2017;
Nezlek and Kuppens 2008) and diminished positive emo-
tions (Wood etal. 2003). In short, the weight of evidence
clearly indicates that suppression generally is a taxing, inef-
fective, and often maladaptive emotion-regulation strategy
(Gross 2002).
For the most part, existing research has documented these
effects either in the context of laboratory manipulations or in
people’s survey-based self-reports of their experiences with
emotion regulation and its affective correlates. Rare in this
literature is real-time evidence about the temporal effects of
naturally occurring suppression; that is, when people spon-
taneously choose to suppress their emotions at one moment,
are there deleterious effects on subsequent emotional expe-
rience? In this report we provide such evidence from two
experience-sampling studies.
Experience sampling represents a useful approach for
examining spontaneous emotion-regulation in everyday life
(Augustine and Larsen 2012). Experimental studies have
value for their ability to support causal inferences but they
may not tap the emotion-regulation strategies that people
actually use in daily living (Heiy and Cheavens 2014). Sur-
veys are limited by people’s ability to recall and accurately
describe their experiences regulating emotions. In contrast
to these two approaches, experience sampling collects real-
time reports of what people are doing or feeling at any given
moment, reflecting their personal choices and predilections
about how to behave in each context and circumstance,
* Yan Ruan
yan.ruan@rochester.edu
1 University ofRochester, Rochester, NY, USA
2 University ofArizona, Tucson, AZ, USA
3 Department ofClinical andSocial Sciences inPsychology,
The University ofRochester, River Campus Box270266,
Rochester, NY14627-0266, USA
428 Motivation and Emotion (2020) 44:427–435
1 3
without instructions from experimenters about what to do.
These real-time reports are useful for describing spontane-
ous emotional experience, while minimizing retrospection
bias and theory-based retrospections (that is, beliefs about
the effects of suppression, instead of their actual invivo
experience of suppression) (Reis etal. 2014; Robinson and
Clore 2002). This approach to research thereby provides an
informative complement to existing laboratory experiments
and retrospective surveys.
A few experience-sampling studies have investigated
emotional suppression. Heiy and Cheavens (2014) found that
participants rated suppression the least effective of 20 strate-
gies for reducing negative emotions. Catterson etal. (2017)
showed that suppression was associated with lower momen-
tary well-being (although this effect was attenuated when
people felt low in social power). Le and Impett (2013) found
that highly interdependent people reported better personal
and relational well-being if they suppressed negative emo-
tions associated with sacrificing for their partner’s benefit.
In end-of-day reports, people high in trait-level tendencies
to suppress emotion indicated that their experiences of fear
lasted longer (Verduyn etal. 2009). None of these studies
examined with real-time reports the effects of suppressing
emotion at one moment on subsequent reports of emotion.
The present research provides the first such report of which
we are aware.
A pair of experience-sampling studies by Brans and
colleagues did look at affective changes associated with
emotion suppression (Brans etal. 2013). They found that
reports of suppression at one moment were associated with
more negative and less positive affective states at that same
moment, relative to prior emotional states. Because these
reports of suppression and affective state were contempo-
raneous, these studies did not establish whether emotion
suppression at one time leads to later emotional afteref-
fects. To be sure, contemporaneous accounts offer valuable
information about the affects associated with suppression,
but they do not speak to temporally later effects, something
that most theorizing predicts (Gross 2015). Evidence of this
link would provide the most direct evidence yet available
about the deleterious effects of suppression on subsequent
emotions.
We note here two experience-sampling studies that
yielded seemingly contrary findings. In these studies, sup-
pression led to positive outcomes: specifically, positive emo-
tions for participants with borderline personality disorder
(Chapman etal. 2009) and less negative emotions among
older Chinese insurance workers (Yeung and Fung 2012).
However, in the former study, participants were instructed
to suppress their emotions on the third day of the study, and
therefore this study did not examine spontaneous (i.e., self-
chosen) suppression. The latter study looked at contempora-
neous reports of suppression and negative emotions and not
downstream effects. Nevertheless, these studies point to the
utility of examining suppression in different populations, as
well as potential moderators for the effects of suppression,
both of which the present research did.
The present pair of studies examined the effect of sup-
pressing negative emotions on subsequent emotional experi-
ence in natural contexts. Adult participants were randomly
signaled 10 times a day for 3days, and asked their emotional
state at that moment, as well as whether they had suppressed
any negative emotions since the prior signal. The design
of these studies allowed us to investigate whether reports
of having suppressed negative emotion at one signal were
systematically related to changes in negative emotion at the
following signal (usually, between 30min and 3h later).
The short sampling interval between signals has the advan-
tage of capturing suppression as close as is feasible to the
moment when participants voluntarily and spontaneously
choose to suppress. Moreover, because the exact time of
response was recorded, the use of relatively brief intervals
makes it possible to examine the duration of these effects;
that is, whether the affective consequences of suppression
tend to subside during an identifiable interval after the report
of suppression, if at all.
Method
Participants
Participants were 468 individuals drawn from two exist-
ing studies.1 Study 1 included 167 patients (121 women)
with the congenital long QT syndrome from the Interna-
tional Long QT Syndrome Registry (located at the Univer-
sity of Rochester Medical Center, Rochester, NY). These
patients ranged in age from 16 to 50years (M = 34.9). Study
2 included 301 participants, of whom 199 were patients (49
women) with coronary artery disease. Half of the patients
(n = 100) had no history of serious arrhythmias, and the rest
had implanted defibrillators for primary prevention of sud-
den cardiac death. These participants ranged in age from 28
to 86 (M = 62.6). In addition, 50 older healthy participants
(13 women) were randomly selected but matched by age and
gender (1:4 ratio) to patients, ranging in age from 33 to 85
(M = 59.8). Fifty-two healthy control subjects (28 women)
were also recruited, their age matching the patients with long
QT syndrome, their age range from 21 to 50 (M = 36.8).2
1 Data from these two studies were previously reported in Lane etal.
(2011) and (2018). These papers did not examine suppression.
2 The decision to include subsamples of individuals with long QT
syndrome and coronary artery disease in part reflects the possibility
that these groups might be particularly prone to demonstrate down-
stream effects of sympathetic activation (e.g., Zipes 1991), which
may occur when people suppress emotion. However, as we explain
429Motivation and Emotion (2020) 44:427–435
1 3
Participants in Study 1 resided throughout North Amer-
ica. All participants in Study 2 resided in the Tucson, Ari-
zona, and Rochester, New York, metropolitan areas. Details
about these samples are provided in Table1. Enrollment in
both studies was limited to individuals who were fluent in
English and who did not show signs of diminished cognitive
capacity or extensive psychopathology.
Procedure
Prior to data collection, participants were visited by a
research assistant in their homes for an intake session,
where they were familiarized with study procedures. They
also completed a preliminary survey, including the follow-
ing measures.
Trait anxiety
Trait anxiety was measured using the State-Trait Anxiety
Inventory (STAI; Spielberger etal. 1983), a 20-item self-
report assessing consistent predispositions for anxiety. A
sample item reads, “I worry too much over something that
really doesn’t matter.” Cronbach’s alpha = .92 and .91 in
Study 1 and Study 2, respectively.
Baseline positive andnegative aect
Participants’ baseline positive and negative affect was
assessed using the Positive and Negative Affect Scale
(PANAS; Watson etal. 1988). The baseline PANAS includes
20 adjectives, with ten each for positive affects (e.g. “enthu-
siastic”), and negative affects (e.g., “upset”). In Study 1,
Cronbach’s alpha was .86 for positive affect, and .86 for
negative affect. In Study 2, Cronbach’s alpha was .87 for
positive affects, and .88 for negative affects.
Neuroticism
In Study 1, the baseline neuroticism was measured using the
Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R; Costa and
McCrae 1992). Cronbach’s alpha = .94. In Study 2, base-
line neuroticism was measured using the Big Five Inven-
tory (Hendriks etal. 1999). Cronbach’s alpha = .85. In all
analyses with this variable, we used a standardized score.
The experience sampling method (ESM) was used to
assess momentary suppression and affect. Procedures in both
studies were similar, except where noted, and are described
simultaneously. Participants were paged ten times a day for
3 days during their usual waking hours (typically, between
8 AM and 10 PM), following a modified random schedule.
The minimum gap between signals was 60min in Study 1
and 30min in Study 2. For each day of the ESM protocol,
participants in Study 1 received a stipend of $50 per day; in
Study 2, this stipend was $100 per day.
Table 1 Demographic information and descriptive data for study participants
Tabled values are means and in parentheses, standard deviations. Means sharing a subscript do not differ from each other (p < .05) in post-hoc
Fisher’s least significant difference tests. Neuroticism was assessed using different measures in Study 1 and Study 2, and therefore descriptive
data should not be compared across studies
Study 1 Study 2
Long QT Low-risk CAD High-risk CAD Young healthy Older healthy
N 167 100 99 52 50
Age 36.8 (9.0) 63.7 (10.0) 61.5 (11.7) 34.9 (10.4) 59.5 (10.4)
% Female 72.5 33.0 16.1 53.8 26.0
Baseline measures
Anxiety 38.02b (9.87) 35.34c (8.48) 34.44c (8.43) 41.25a (11.26) 33.66c (7.96)
Neuroticism 129.08 (25.39) 2.36 (.78) 2.30 (.85) 2.68 (.91) 2.07 (.73)
Negative affect 17.66b (5.74) 15.92c (5.75) 15.77c (5.35) 19.88a (5.91) 15.35c (4.27)
Positive affect 35.02a,b (6.16) 35.32a,b (5.78) 34.44b (5.87) 34.10b (7.03) 36.38a (5.43)
ESM measures
Suppression 1.46a (1.48) 1.21b (.61) 1.23b (.73) 1.48a (.96) 1.30a,b (.68)
High-arousal negative affect 1.38a (.55) 1.16b (.34) 1.12b (.29) 1.44a (.86) 1.17b (.31)
Low-arousal negative affect 1.39a (.55) 1.21b (.37) 1.27b (.39) 1.54c (.73) 1.23b (.35)
High-arousal positive affect 3.32b (1.42) 3.69a (1.15) 3.54a (1.26) 3.36b (.84) 3.71a (1.12)
Low-arousal positive affect 3.50b (1.42) 4.10a (1.32) 4.23a (1.37) 3.62b (.99) 3.99a (1.28)
later, there were no differences between these groups and the normal
controls.
Footnote 2 (continued)
430 Motivation and Emotion (2020) 44:427–435
1 3
Participants in Study 1 were paged on Palm Pilot personal
digital assistants programmed to vibrate when a report was
scheduled, using the Experience Sampling Program (Barrett
and Feldman Barrett 2000). Participants in Study 2 were
paged on Motorola Droid X or X2 smartphones, using a
homemade program that provided an auditory signal at the
scheduled time and that recorded the exact time at which
answers were entered. Participants in both studies were
instructed to turn to their device as soon as possible after
the page, to begin responding immediately, and to complete
the protocol without interruption. Responses were permitted
up to 20min after signal delivery.
The ESM protocols in both studies included the follow-
ing questions.
Emotion suppression
At each signal, participants were asked “Since the last sig-
nal, have you felt negative emotions that you’ve kept bottled
up?” We used only a single item because of the necessity
to minimize participant burden in EMA studies. This item
was chosen to be representative of items used on multi-item
scales (e.g., “when I’m upset, I bottle it up;” Courtauld
Emotional Control Total Score; Watson and Greer 1983).
Responses used a 1 (not at all) to 7 (extreme) scale. Because
74.9% of the responses were reported as 1, we treated this
variable as a dichotomy,3 namely none (1, recoded as 0)
vs. some (2–7, recoded as 1), representing whether or not
suppression had occurred. Some level of suppression was
reported by 309 (66%) participants during the study period.
Aect
Participants were asked to rate the extent to which they had
experienced affects from the PANAS (Watson etal. 1988)
during the 5min preceding the signal, using a 1 (not at all)
to 7 (extreme) scale. Sixteen affect items were included
in the present analyses4: (1) High arousal positive affect:
interested, attentive, excited (in a positive way), enthusiastic,
and alert (α = .84); (2) high arousal negative affect: guilty,
anxious, angry, hostile, jittery, and afraid (α = .73); (3) low
arousal positive affect: calm, and relaxed (α = .74), and (4)
low arousal negative affect: sad, depressed (substituted for
the PANAS-X term, blue), and lonely (α = .61).These Cron-
bach’s alphas were calculated by first computing the reliabil-
ity separately for each signal, and then averaging across the
30 signals, so that reliability is not confounded with within-
person dependencies.
Results
Compliance with the ESM protocol was computed by com-
paring scheduled signal times to the device’s internal record
of when responding began. Participants responded to 93.0%
and 91.6% of the signals in Studies 1 and 2, respectively.
More than half the sample in both studies began all or all
but one of their reports within 10min, and only 19 partici-
pants in Study 1 and 2 participants in Study 2 began 4 or
more reports more than 15min after the page. On average,
participants in Studies 1 and 2 took 2.35 ± 1.36min and
4.00 ± 3.50min, respectively, to complete a report. Using the
VARCOMP procedure in SPSS, within-person effects were
estimated to account for the following variance percentages
in affect: high-arousal negative affect (55.9%), low-arousal
negative affect (45.8%), high-arousal positive affect (49.8%),
and low-arousal positive affect (42.4%).
Analyses were conducted using multilevel modeling
(MLM) within SPSS 24.0 MIXED. To maximize power,
the samples from Study 1 and 2 were combined in all analy-
ses.5 All predictors were entered as fixed effects, except that
the level-one variable, within-person suppression (Prior
suppressionwithin), and the intercept were modeled as ran-
dom effects. We followed Bolger and Laurenceau’s (2013)
recommendations strictly: level-one predictors were first
grand-mean centered to establish an interpretable zero point,
and then person-mean centered, so that coefficients reflect
momentary deviations from each person’s average. We also
partitioned each level-one predictor into two orthogonal
components: a between-subjects component, represented by
the person’s grand-mean-centered aggregated mean score
over the course of the full ESM period, and a within-subjects
component, represented by the person-mean-centered score
(i.e., the momentary deviation from the mean). Both within-
and between-subjects indicators of suppression were entered
the model simultaneously (marked by the subscript “within”
and “between”, respectively, in the tables).
High-arousal positive affect, low-arousal positive affect,
high-arousal negative affect, and low-arousal negative affect
served as outcome variables. First, concurrent models were
tested to examine the relationship between emotion suppres-
sion and affect within the same signal. Temporal artifacts
were controlled by including time of day, survey day (1–3),
and time elapsed since the prior survey (all centered) in
the model. We applied a first-order autoregressive [AR(1)]
3 All later analyses were run with suppression as a dichotomous vari-
able. The findings remain the same if suppression is treated as a con-
tinuous variable with log transformation.
4 Affect items in the two studies differed slightly; these 16 items were
selected because they were used with all participants in both studies.
5 Study was treated as a potential moderator in preliminary analyses.
Study group did not moderate any of the results reported here.
431Motivation and Emotion (2020) 44:427–435
1 3
model to correct for autocorrelation of residuals and an
unstructured covariance matrix for the random effects.
Consistent with Brans and colleagues’ findings (2013),
suppression was associated with significantly lower posi-
tive affect and higher negative affect, as shown in Table2.
Emotion suppression was significantly associated with
higher levels of high arousal negative affect at the same
signal [B = .26, t(257.08) = 12.36, p < .001] and low arousal
negative affect at the same signal [B = .15, t(232.60) = 8.45,
p < .001]. Similarly, suppression was significantly associ-
ated with lower levels of high arousal positive affect at the
same signal [B = − .14, t(953.02) = −3.78, p < .001] and
low arousal positive affect at the same signal [B = −.43,
t(236.42) = −10.57, p < .001].
Next, we tested our main question, about the effect of
suppression at one signal predicting mood at the subsequent
signal. Here, we computed lagged models where affect at
signal t was predicted by suppression at signal t-1, control-
ling for affect at signal t-1. The first signal of each day was
dropped to exclude overnight lags. Results are presented in
Table3. Reporting emotion suppression at one signal (Prior
Suppressionwithin) significantly predicted higher levels of
both high-arousal and low-arousal negative affect in the
next signal [B = .04, t(270.17) = 3.26, p = .001] and [B = .04,
t(250.50) = 2.58, p = .010], respectively.6 Similar analyses
were conducted for predicting positive affect, but were
not significant for high-arousal positive affect [B = −.04,
t(1009.85) = −1.21, p = .23] or low-arousal positive affect
[B = .01, t(226.37) = .29, p = .77]. In short, suppressing neg-
ative emotions predicted higher levels of negative affect at
Table 2 Results of concurrent
analysis predicting affect from
suppression
B(SE) t p95% CI
High arousal negative affect
Intercept .91(.02) t(2564.77) = 43.73 <.001 [.87, .95]
Suppressionwithin .26(.02) t(257.08) = 12.36 <.001 [.22, .30]
Suppressionbetween .50(.03) t(352.01) = 14.69 <.001 [.43, .56]
Prior high arousal negative affect .28(.01) t(7790.75) = 32.55 <.001 [.26, .30]
Time of day −.00(.00) t(3704.37) = −2.06 .039 [−.00, .00]
Day of the survey .01(.00) t(2158.96) = 3.38 .001 [.01, .02]
Time elapsed since last signal .00(.00) t(7242.88) = 5.47 <.001 [.00, .00]
Low arousal negative affect
Intercept .85(.02) t(2041.91) = 40.37 <.001 [.80, .89]
Suppressionwithin .15(.02) t(232.60) = 8.45 <.001 [.11, .18]
Suppressionbetween .47(.04) t(295.83) = 13.10 <.001 [.40, .54]
Prior low arousal negative affect .36(.01) t(5595.72) = 45.41 <.001 [.34, .37]
Time of day −.01(.00) t(2093.51) = −1.92 .055 [−.01, .00]
Day of the survey .00(.00) t(3707.57) = .75 .453 [.00, .00]
Time elapsed since last signal .00(.00) t(6763.21) = 2.43 .015 [.00, .00]
High arousal positive affect
Intercept 2.08(.05) t(2958.85) = 42.15 <.001 [1.98, 2.17]
Suppressionwithin −.14(.04) t(953.02) = −3.78 <.001 [−.21, −.07]
Suppressionbetween −.13(.08) t(366.90) = −1.59 .113 [−.29, .03]
Prior low arousal negative affect .47(.01) t(5693.66) = 55.95 <.001 [.45, .48]
Time of day −.00(.00) t(4810.77) = −6.21 <.001 [−.00, −.00]
Day of the survey −.01(.01) t(3169.11) = −1.63 .103 [−.03, .00]
Time elapsed since last signal .00(.00) t(8668.36) = 3.35 <.001 [.00, .00]
Low arousal positive affect
Intercept 2.44(.06) t(2302.43) = 38.87 <.001 [2.31, 2.56]
Suppressionwithin −.43(.04) t(236.42) = −10.57 <.001 [−.50, −.35]
Suppressionbetween −.58(.11) t(306.23) = −5.26 <.001 [−.80, −.36]
Prior low arousal negative affect .37(.01) t(6533.56) = 42.38 <.001 [.35, .38]
Time of day .00(.00) t(3954.22) = 3.83 <.001 [.00, .00]
Day of the survey −.08(.01) t(2326.70) = −7.51 <.001 [−.10, −.06]
Time elapsed since last signal .00(.00) t(7307.83) = .76 .450 [−.00, .00]
6 The findings remained significant after removing the three temporal
control variables from the models.
432 Motivation and Emotion (2020) 44:427–435
1 3
the next signal, controlling for levels of negative affect at the
time suppression was reported, but it did not predict change
in positive affect at the next signal.7
We then examined whether the time that elapsed between
two successive signals moderated the association between
prior suppression and negative affect. Following the afore-
mentioned plan, we tested models that included prior sup-
pression, time elapsed since the prior signal, and their
interaction, as well as time of day, survey day, negative
affect at the prior signal, and between-persons components
of suppression as covariates. As before, suppression was
first grand-mean centered, and then person-mean centered.
As Table4 shows, the prior suppression × time elapsed
interaction was significant for low-arousal negative affect
(p = .022) but not for high-arousal negative affect (p = .518).
For post hoc probing of this significant interaction, we com-
puted regions of significance, which indicate the specific
value of the moderator (time elapsed since the prior signal)
at which the effect of prior suppression on negative affect
becomes nonsignificant (i.e., p = .05; Preacher etal. 2006).
This analysis revealed that the effect of suppression on low-
arousal negative affect became nonsignificant at 1.37h (1h,
22min) after the previous signal.
Finally, we also explored several other potential mod-
erators, including gender, age, study (health status/age)
groups, as well as the trait measures mentioned above. Each
potential moderator (contrast codes were created for study
groups, trait measures were centered) and its interaction
with momentary suppression were added to the analyses
described earlier. None of these 28 analyses found significant
Table 3 Results of multilevel
models predicting affect from
suppression at the prior signal
B(SE) t p95% CI
High arousal negative affect
Intercept .81(.02) t(2608.23) = 39.25 <.001 [.77, .85]
Prior suppressionwithin .04(.01) t(270.18) = 3.26 .001 [.02, .07]
Suppressionbetween .45(.03) t(322.61) = 14.14 <.001 [.38, .51]
Prior high arousal negative affect .36(.01) t(6333.06) = 39.66 <.001 [.34, .38]
Time of day −.00(.00) t(3720.70) = −1.99 .046 [−.00, .00]
Day of the survey .01(.00) t(2221.19) = 3.19 .001 [.00, .02]
Time elapsed since prior signal .01(.00) t(7695.35) = 5.45 <.001 [.01, .02]
Low arousal negative affect
Intercept .82(.02) t(2035.27) = 39.30 <.001 [.78, .86]
Prior suppressionwithin .04(.01) t(250.50) = 2.58 .010 [.01, .06]
Suppressionbetween .47(.04) t(285.04) = 13.24 <.001 [.40, .54]
Prior low arousal negative affect .37(.01) t(5273.26) = 47.11 <.001 [.36, .39]
Time of day .00(.00) t(3716.12) = .64 .526 [.00, .00]
Day of the survey −.01(.00) t(2081.19) = −1.77 .077 [−.01, .00]
Time elapsed since prior signal .00(.00) t(6746.31) = 1.42 .156 [−.00, .01]
High arousal positive affect
Intercept 2.01(.05) t(3049.73) = 41.25 <.001 [1.92, 2.11]
Prior suppressionwithin −.04(.04) t(1009.85) = −1.21 .225 [−.11, .03]
Suppressionbetween −.13(.08) t(366.02) = −1.67 .095 [−.28, .02]
Prior low arousal negative affect .48(.01) t(5297.55) = 58.05 <.001 [.47, .50]
Time of day −.00(.00) t(4886.36) = −6.20 <.001 [.00, .00]
Day of the survey −.01(.01) t(3268.18) = −1.46 .144 [−.03, .00]
Time elapsed since prior signal .01(.00) t(8793.77) = 2.84 .005 [.00, .02]
Low arousal positive affect
Intercept 2.28(.06) t(2428.95) = 36.64 <.001 [2.15, 2.40]
Prior suppressionwithin .01(.03) t(226.36) = .29 .769 [−.06, .08]
Suppressionbetween −.57(.10) t(296.41) = −5.44 <.001 [−.78, −.36]
Prior low arousal negative affect .40(.01) t(4999.09) = 46.19 <.001 [.39, .42]
Time of day −.00(.00) t(4127.71) = 4.00 <.001 [−.00, −.00]
Day of the survey −.07(.01) t(2499.33) = −7.16 <.001 [−.09, −.05]
Time elapsed since prior signal .00(.01) t(7705.36) = .14 .887 [−.01, .01]
7 The findings remained significant after adding baseline positive
affect and baseline negative affect as control variables.
433Motivation and Emotion (2020) 44:427–435
1 3
moderation of the association between prior suppression and
negative affect.8
Discussion
Drawing on two experience-sampling studies, our findings
provide the most direct evidence yet available for the tem-
poral effect of naturally occurring emotion suppression.
We found that suppressing negative emotions at one signal
significantly predicted more negative emotions at the next
signal, for both high-arousal and low-arousal negative emo-
tion. These effects remained significant when controlling
for several potential moderators. Further, we found that the
effect of suppression on low-arousal negative affect faded
after 1h 22min, whereas time elapsed did not influence the
effect of suppression on high-arousal negative affect, sug-
gesting that, at least within the maximum 2–3h time frame
of our ESM protocol, time did not alleviate the high activa-
tion associated with negative affect brought by suppression.
The present research demonstrates with real-time reports
that naturally occurring suppression adversely affects sub-
sequent emotions, a finding that aligns with research exam-
ining the sequelae of suppression in laboratory sessions
(Campbell-Sills etal. 2006; Gross and Levenson 1993)
as well as research on individual differences in emotion
regulation tendencies (see review by Webb etal. 2012).
Taken together, our findings support the view that when
negative emotions arise, suppression is not only an ineffec-
tive emotion-regulation strategy, but also a costly one, bring-
ing with it lingering negative emotional experience. This
effect, although small in the moment, may accumulate over
multiple events and time, potentially undermining psycho-
logical well-being and relationship functioning in the long
run (Cameron and Overall 2018).
Note that in the current research, suppression was associ-
ated only with subsequent negative affect, but not positive
affect, which may at first glance seem to contradict prior
research that found suppression to be associated with both
higher negative affect and lower positive affect at the end
of the day (Brans etal. 2013). Although we did not find an
association between suppression and positive affect at the
next signal, we did replicate their finding that suppression
was associated with lower positive affect in the same signal.
This could indicate that while participants experience higher
negative affect and lower positive affect during suppression,
this dip in positive affect recovers soon afterwards while the
rise in negative affect persists. Additional studies are needed
to shed light on this possibility.
It is noteworthy that we did not find moderation of the
effects of suppression by neuroticism or baseline affect, traits
that have been linked to prolonged negative affect in prior
research (e.g., Gross etal. 1998). One potential explanation
is that the current studies examined voluntary, self-chosen
suppression—that is, these personality traits may influence
people’s choice of an emotion regulation strategy in daily
life (e.g., Heiy and Cheavens 2014), but not their reaction to
Table 4 Results of multilevel
models predicting negative
affect from suppression at the
prior signal as moderated by
time elapsed since prior signal
B(SE) t p95% CI
High arousal negative affect
Intercept .81(.02) t(2608.97) = 39.24 <.001 [.77, .85]
Prior suppressionwithin .04(.01) t(270.02) = 3.27 .001 [.02, .07]
Suppressionbetween .45(.03) t(322.77) = 14.14 <.001 [.39, .51]
Prior high arousal negative affect .36(.01) t(6337.80) = 39.65 <.001 [.34, .37]
Time of day .00(.00) t(3721.47) = −2.00 .046 [−.00, .00]
Day of the survey .01(.00) t(2221.35) = 3.18 .001 [.00, .02]
Time elapsed since prior signal .01(.00) t(7694.90) = 5.44 <.001 [.01, .02]
Prior suppressionwithin × time elapsed .00(.01) t(8068.52) = −0.65 .518 [−.02, .01]
Low arousal negative affect
Intercept .82(.02) t(2046.88) = 39.28 <.001 [.78, .86]
Prior suppressionwithin .04(.01) t(251.11) = 2.62 .009 [.01, .06]
Suppressionbetween .47(.04) t(285.95) = 13.23 <.001 [.40, .54]
Prior low arousal negative affect .37(.01) t(5279.71) = 47.24 <.001 [.36, .39]
Time of day .00(.00) t(3734.18) = .63 .527 [.00, .00]
Day of the survey −.01(.00) t(2096.97) = −1.77 .077 [−.01, .00]
Time elapsed since prior signal .00(.00) t(6783.04) = 1.41 .159 [−.00, .01]
Prior suppressionwithin × time elapsed −.02(.01) t(8403.73) = −2.28 .022 [−.03, −.00]
8 There also were no significant moderation effects for positive
affect. Full results can be found in the online supplemental material,
https ://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/PCVIW 6.
434 Motivation and Emotion (2020) 44:427–435
1 3
the strategy once it is self-chosen. If confirmed by additional
research, this finding would have important implications
for our understanding of individual differences in emotion
regulation. It is also possible that suppression of episodes of
more intense negative emotions, which were relatively rare
due to our emphasis on ordinary, everyday emotions, would
reveal moderation by individual differences.
The current study has several limitations that bear note.
First, the samples included individuals with specific medi-
cal conditions along with healthy control individuals, which
makes generalizability unclear. Our moderation analyses
help address this limitation, showing that study group did
not moderate the obtained results. However, additional
research is needed to see if similar results obtain in more
diverse samples. Another limitation lies in our measurement
of emotion expression. Because of the necessity to minimize
participant burden, emotion suppression was examined with
only a single item. In addition, we did not examine effects of
suppressing positive emotions, nor did we assess the motiva-
tion behind suppression, which has been shown to moderate
the association between suppression and well-being in the
specific case of sacrifice (Le and Impett 2013). Indeed, it
is reasonable to expect that the effects of emotional sup-
pression may be context-dependent to some extent, and it
will be useful in future research to identify those contexts
in which emotional suppression may be less detrimental.
Finally, our ESM protocol featured 10 signals per day, which
created relatively brief intervals designed to capture what-
ever short-lived effects emotional suppression might have.
Nevertheless, because the ESM reports asked about suppres-
sion “since the last signal,” it is possible that some diminish-
ment in negative affect had already taken place by the time
of the report of suppression. However, it seems reasonable
to expect that this would likely have lessened the changes
observed in the current analyses.
Conclusion
The current research utilized two experience-sampling stud-
ies and found that naturally occurring suppression of nega-
tive emotions at one signal led to higher negative affect in
the next signal. That emotion suppression may be ineffec-
tive and potentially costly is a robust idea in the existing
literature, as well as in popular ideas about emotion regula-
tion. These two studies provide the most direct evidence yet
available about this association: In everyday life, suppress-
ing one’s negative emotions is likely to yield a more negative
emotional experience that lingers afterwards.
Acknowledgement This research was funded by RO1 HL68764 and
RO1 HL103692 from the National Institutes of Health.
References
Augustine, A. A., & Larsen, R. J. (2012). Emotion research. In M.
R. Mehl & T. S. Conner (Eds.), Handbook of research methods
for studying daily life (pp. 497–510). New York: Guilford Press.
Barrett, D. J., & Feldman Barrett, L. (2000). The experience-sampling
program (ESP). http://www2.bc.edu/~barre tli/esp/.
Bolger, N., & Laurenceau, J. P. (2013). Intensive longitudinal methods:
An introduction to diary and experience sampling research. New
York: Guilford.
Brans, K., Koval, P., Verduyn, P., Lim, Y. L., & Kuppens, P. (2013).
The regulation of negative and positive affect in daily life. Emo-
tion, 13(5), 926–939.
Butler, E. A., Egloff, B., Wlhelm, F. H., Smith, N. C., Erickson, E.
A., & Gross, J. J. (2003). The social consequences of expressive
suppression. Emotion, 3(1), 48–67.
Cameron, L. D., & Overall, N. C. (2018). Suppression and expression
as distinct emotion-regulation processes in daily interactions: Lon-
gitudinal and meta-analyses. Emotion, 18(4), 465–480.
Campbell-Sills, L., Barlow, D. H., Brown, T. A., & Hofmann, S. G.
(2006). Effects of suppression and acceptance on emotional
responses of individuals with anxiety and mood disorders. Behav-
iour Research and Therapy, 44(9), 1251–1263.
Catterson, A. D., Eldesouky, L., & John, O. P. (2017). An experience
sampling approach to emotion regulation: Situational suppression
use and social hierarchy. Journal of Research in Personality, 69,
33–43.
Chapman, A. L., Rosenthal, M. Z., & Leung, D. W. (2009). Emotion
suppression in borderline personality disorder: An experience
sampling study. Journal of Personality Disorders, 23(1), 29–47.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Normal personality assessment
in clinical practice: The NEO personality inventory. Psychological
Assessment, 4(1), 5–13.
Demaree, H. A., Schmeichel, B. J., Robinson, J. L., Pu, J., Everhart, D.
E., & Berntson, G. G. (2006). Up- and down-regulating facial dis-
gust: Affective, vagal, sympathetic, and respiratory consequences.
Biological Psychology, 71(1), 90–99.
Ehring, T., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Schnülle, J., Fischer, S., & Gross, J.
J. (2010). Emotion regulation and vulnerability to depression:
Spontaneous versus instructed use of emotion suppression and
reappraisal. Emotion, 10(4), 563–572.
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An inte-
grative review. Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 271–299.
Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social
consequences. Psychophysiology, 39(3), 281–291.
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future pros-
pects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 1–26.
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physi-
ology, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 64(6), 970–986.
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1995). Emotion elicitation using films.
Cognition and Emotion, 9(1), 87–108.
Gross, J. J., Sutton, S. K., & Ketelaar, T. V. (1998). Relations between
affect and personality: Support for the affect-level and affective-
reactivity views. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24,
279–288.
Heiy, J. E., & Cheavens, J. S. (2014). Back to basics: A naturalistic
assessment of the experience and regulation of emotion. Emotion,
14(5), 878–891.
Hendriks, A. J., Hofstee, W. K., & De Raad, B. (1999). The five-factor
personality inventory (FFPI). Personality and Individual Differ-
ences, 27(2), 307–325.
John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regu-
lation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span
development. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 1301–1334.
435Motivation and Emotion (2020) 44:427–435
1 3
Lane, R. D., Reis, H. T., Hsu, C. H., Kern, K. B., Couderc, J. P., Moss,
A. J., etal. (2018). Abnormal repolarization duration during eve-
ryday emotional arousal in long QT syndrome and coronary artery
disease. The American Journal of Medicine, 131(5), 565–572.
Lane, R. D., Zareba, W., Reis, H. T., Peterson, D. R., & Moss, A.
J. (2011). Changes in ventricular repolarization duration during
typical daily emotion in patients with long QT syndrome. Psycho-
somatic Medicine, 73(1), 98–105.
Le, B. M., & Impett, E. A. (2013). When holding back helps: Sup-
pressing negative emotions during sacrifice feels authentic and
is beneficial for highly interdependent people. Psychological Sci-
ence, 24(9), 1809–1815.
Moore, S. A., Zoellner, L. A., & Mollenholt, N. (2008). Are expres-
sive suppression and cognitive reappraisal associated with stress-
related symptoms? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(9),
993–1000.
Naragon-Gainey, K., McMahon, T. P., & Chacko, T. P. (2017). The
structure of common emotion regulation strategies: A meta-ana-
lytic examination. Psychological Bulletin, 143(4), 384–427.
Nezlek, J. B., & Kuppens, P. (2008). Regulating positive and negative
emotions in daily life. Journal of Personality, 76(3), 561–580.
Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J., & Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational
tools for probing interaction effects in multiple linear regression,
multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educa-
tional and Behavioral Statistics, 31, 437–448.
Reis, H. T., Gable, S. L., & Maniaci, M. R. (2014). Methods for study-
ing everyday experience in its natural context. In H. T. Reis &
C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and
personality psychology (pp. 373–403). New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Roberts, N. A., Levenson, R. W., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Cardiovascu-
lar costs of emotion suppression cross ethnic lines. International
Journal of Psychophysiology, 70(1), 82–87.
Robinson, M. D., & Clore, G. L. (2002). Belief and feeling: Evidence
for an accessibility model of emotional self-report. Psychological
Bulletin, 128(6), 934–960.
Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., & Jacobs,
G. A. (1983). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo
Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Verduyn, P., Delvaux, E., Van Coillie, H., Tuerlinckx, F., & Van
Mechelen, I. (2009). Predicting the duration of emotional experi-
ence: Two experience sampling studies. Emotion, 9(1), 83–91.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and
validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The
PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
54(6), 1063–1070.
Watson, M., & Greer, S. (1983). Development of a questionnaire meas-
ure of emotional control. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,
27(4), 299–305.
Webb, T. L., Miles, E., & Sheeran, P. (2012). Dealing with feeling:
A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from
the process model of emotion regulation. Psychological Bulletin,
138(4), 775–808.
Wood, J. V., Heimpel, S. A., & Michela, J. L. (2003). Savoring versus
dampening: Self-esteem differences in regulating positive affect.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(3), 566–580.
Yeung, D. Y., & Fung, H. H. (2012). Impacts of suppression on emo-
tional responses and performance outcomes: An experience-
sampling study in younger and older workers. Journals of Ger-
ontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences,
67(6), 666–676.
Zipes, D. P. (1991). The long QT interval syndrome: A Rosetta stone
for sympathetic related ventricular tachyarrhythmias. Circulation,
84(3), 1414–1419.
Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
... However, this binary perspective does not always lead to enhanced well-being (e.g., Dalgleish et al., 2009;Desbordes et al., 2015;Gruber et al., 2011;Ruan et al., 2020). On one hand, not all positivity is good for well-being . ...
... On the other hand, the aversion to negativity poses risks. Suppressing or denying negative feelings can exacerbate these feelings and increase emotional dysregulation, leading to increased psychological distress and a range of mental health issues (Dalgleish et al., 2009;Ruan et al., 2020). This suppression can also result in a rebound effect, where the avoided emotions reoccur intensely and uncontrollably (Wegner, 1994;Wegner & Zanakos, 1994). ...
... A BALANCED MIND by the cultivation of equanimity (Chan et al., 2014;Dalgleish et al., 2009;Ruan et al., 2020;Wallace & Shapiro, 2006), which enables a balanced, unbiased mindset and enhances adaptability (Jijina & Biswas, 2021;Machado & Costa, 2015). Research has demonstrated associations between equanimity and enhanced psychological wellbeing as well as improved physical functioning Chan et al., 2014;Desbordes et al., 2015;Ekici et al., 2020;Farb et al., 2012;Jijina & Biswas, 2021;Lindsay et al., 2019;Maher & Cordova, 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
Awe has been shown to promote well-being through various mechanisms (see Monroy & Keltner, 2023). In this research, we propose a novel perspective for the well-being benefits of awe: Awe fosters equanimity—a balanced state of mind toward all experiences of any valence—and we document how this works, namely, through temporal distancing. Across seven studies, using a combination of experiments, big data analytics, and intervention methods, we provide support for our hypotheses. In Studies 1–3, induced awe increased equanimity, indexed by a self-report scale (Study 1), a decrease in emotional reactivity (Study 2), and an unbiased behavioral approach to positive and negative experiences (Study 3). In Studies 4–6, awe increased equanimity via temporal distancing. This effect persisted beyond self-diminishment (i.e., feeling small and insignificant) and proved to be cross-culturally robust (Study 4). An analysis of almost 200,000 posts on social media (Study 5) revealed that the proposed mediation model manifested in ecologically rich contexts. Study 6 provided causal evidence for the mediation model. Finally, in a 5-day awe intervention (Study 7), awe increased psychological and physical well-being, with equanimity accounting for these benefits. Taken together, these findings reveal that awe cultivates a balanced state of mind by shifting one’s temporal perspective on life events.
... Unlike positive emotions, stress and fear occur spontaneously without needing attentional control [70]. In addition, negative emotions have a long tail due to their cumulative effects in the frontal lobe [127], causing guilt, remorse, anxiety, rumination [128], and adverse mind-wandering [129][130][131]. ...
... Thus, negative emotions represent unprocessed information, which remains part of the cognitive space. The sense of permanence enhances negative emotions' stress-inducing power [70,127,136], causing erratic and attack behavior and criticism [128][129][130]. ...
Article
Full-text available
One of the most puzzling questions in neuroscience is the nature of emotions and their role in consciousness. The brain’s significant energy investment in maintaining the resting state indicates its essential role as the ground state of consciousness, the source of the sense of self. Emotions, the brain’s homeostatic master regulators, continuously measure and motivate the recovery of the psychological equilibrium. Moreover, perception’s information-energy exchange with the environment gives rise to a closed thermodynamic cycle, the reversible Carnot engine. The Carnot cycle forms an exothermic process; low entropy and reversible resting state turn the focus to the past, causing regret and remorse. The endothermic reversed Carnot cycle creates a high entropy resting state with irreversible activations generating novelty and intellect. We propose that the cycle’s direction represents psychological spin, where the endothermic cycle’s energy accumulation forms up-spin, and the energy-wasting exothermic cycle represents down-spin. Psychological spin corresponds to attitude, the determining factor in cognitive function and social life. By applying the Pauli exclusion principle for consciousness, we can explain the need for personal space and the formation of hierarchical social structures and animals’ territorial needs. Improving intuition about the brain’s intelligent computations may allow new treatments for mental diseases and novel applications in robotics and artificial intelligence.
... In contrast, in the case of suppression, one of the most commonly studied response-focused strategies, one might urge a partner to attempt to reduce or hold back on behavioral expressions of negative emotion. Suppression has often been linked to negative outcomes such as higher negative affect (Brans et al., 2013;Ruan et al., 2020), maladaptive physiological responses (Gross, 1998b;Gross & Levenson, 1997;Peters et al., 2014), and feelings of inauthenticity Impett et al., 2014). ...
... In studies of self-regulation, suppression is generally ineffective or even detrimental (e.g., Brans et al., 2013;Gross, 1998a;Ruan et al., 2020), so that this latter result is surprising. It is possible that when partners encourage suppression, their efforts are seen as caring and compassionate, a possibility that should be explored in future research. ...
Article
Full-text available
Most emotion regulation research investigates how individuals manage their own emotions but in everyday life, emotion regulation often takes place in an interpersonal context—that is, through the intervention of others, especially close relationship partners. In this manuscript, we describe a 2-week daily diary study of 197 couples, in which we examined the nature and effectiveness of partners’ attempts at interpersonal emotion regulation. Organized around the extended process model of emotion regulation (Gross, 2015), we examined the frequency and perceived effectiveness of six strategies for regulating negative emotions and two strategies for regulating positive emotions, as well as the impact of emotional regulation effectiveness on relationship satisfaction. Results indicated that situation modification was the most common approach to regulating negative emotions, whereas savoring was most common for positive emotions. Most strategies were perceived to be effective by both regulators and their targets, with the exception of suppression for negative emotion, for which evidence was mixed, and dampening for positive emotion, which was not viewed as effective. The influence of emotion regulation on relationship satisfaction depended on one’s perspective: Regulators felt that their effective efforts benefitted their relationships, but even when targets experienced emotion regulation as effective, they indicated no improvements in relationship satisfaction, perhaps because the emotion-eliciting events were still salient to them. Broadly speaking, this research highlights the value of examining emotion regulation in an interpersonal context, and in the context of partners’ ordinary, everyday social interaction.
... Multiple studies have associated suppression of negative emotions with increased negative affect and depressive symptoms [5,7,8]. For instance, in an experimental study, when participants were asked to suppress their negative emotions to an emotional clip or to allow their emotional response, those that suppressed reported higher distress and heart rate variability [9]. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ) assesses emotion regulation strategies, particularly expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal. However, the ERQ does not discern between regulating positive vs. negative emotions. Recent research suggests that suppression and reappraisal can impact mental health differently when targeting positive vs negative emotions. We developed and validated the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire - Positive/Negative (ERQ-PN), designed to measure positive and negative forms of suppression and reappraisal strategies. We recruited 963 participants (female = 478) through Prolific.com and administered the ERQ-PN. Participants had an average age of 45 years and were predominantly White (74%) and heterosexual (84%). Structural validity was assessed through confirmatory factor analyses. Model fit was estimated using the comparative fit index and the root-mean-square error of approximation. We also used the Bayesian information criterion to compare the fit of different models. Overall, participants used reappraisal more often to decrease negative emotions (vs. increasing positive) and leaned toward using suppression more for negative (vs. positive) emotions. These analyses revealed that the four-factor model (Model 2) delineating four latent variables (positive reappraisal, negative reappraisal, positive suppression, and negative suppression) had a good fit (RMSEA = 0.07, CFI = 0. 97, TLI = 0.96, χ ² (98) = 531.28, p < 0.001). An incremental validity assessment revealed that positive and negative reappraisal correlated similarly with related mental health constructs. By contrast, suppression of negative vs. positive emotions was differentially associated to the validators tested. The ERQ-PN represents a valid measure of emotion regulation that accounts for both positive and negative emotions.
Article
Sustaining the development of rural and pastoral communities’ hinges on livelihood resilience. Pastoralist household resilience relies on resource availability and decision-making abilities. Despite extensive studies on pastoralist livelihoods, a significant knowledge gap remains in understanding the nuanced adaptive capacities of diverse households, particularly amid grassland degradation. Thus, this study investigates the household-based livelihood resilience of pastoralists in China’s Three-River Headwater Region, offering policy recommendations for resilient livelihoods. Using stratified random sampling, 758 pastoralist household heads underwent semistructured interviews to collect data. Five household characteristics, encompassing age, gender, energy use, well-being perception, and multi-household grazing participation, were evaluated. Looking ot the nature of data, descriptive statistics and non-parametric tests were performed in this study to draw the valid inferences. The results revealed a positive correlation (p < 0.05) between household head age and livelihood resilience, with divergent resilience across age groups. Varied energy usage yielded distinct impacts; households employing solar or mixed energy sources exhibited heightened resilience (p < 0.05). Household well-being emerged as an invariant variable concerning resilience. Furthermore, engagement in multi-household grazing (an informal institution) significantly (p < 0.05) influenced pastoralist livelihood resilience. These insights advocate targeted support for young household heads and the adoption of clean energy. Exploring the deeper strategies and mechanisms of multi-household grazing can enhance understanding and policy integration, guiding eco-friendly progress within rustic landscapes for pastoral communities.
Article
Objective A positive association has previously been observed in healthy volunteers between emotional awareness (EA), the ability to identify and describe emotional experiences in oneself and others, and resting heart rate variability (HRV), which is dominated by vagus nerve activity. The current study aimed to investigate the EA-HRV association across multiple assessments in a “real-world” ambulatory context in patients with long QT syndrome (LQTS) who are at genetic risk for sudden cardiac death. Method Participants (157 LQTS patients; MeanAge = 35.1, SDAge = 10.4; 115 women) completed the levels of emotional awareness scale (LEAS) on one occasion, which served as our measure of EA. In an ecological momentary assessment study involving 10 assessments per day over three days, multiple 5-minute ECG assessments (Mean = 24.6, SD = 5.1) were obtained in each patient using a Holter monitor, from which high-frequency HRV (HF-HRV) was computed on each occasion. Results There was a significant positive association between LEAS scores and HF-HRV controlling for biobehavioral covariates. We also detected a similar inverse relation between EA and mean heart rate. Conclusion These findings suggest that, in patients with a well-defined genetic risk for ventricular arrhythmia and sudden death, the ability to experience emotions in a complex and differentiated way covaries with greater parasympathetic influences on the heart. These findings are consistent with the overlapping neural substrates of EA and HRV and their common contribution to adaptive emotional responding, consistent with the Neurovisceral Integration Model.
Book
Neuroscience has made significant progress in understanding the brain, but the nature of consciousness remains elusive. At the same time, recent spectacular advancements in artificial intelligence promise the prospect of machines attaining human-like cognitive abilities. At the center of both systems is a fundamental dance of stimuli and response, requiring a profound comprehension of the physical environment. Thus, quantum mechanics and general relativity can be applied to the mysteries of human behavior, such as the difficulty of predicting, controlling, or retracing our thoughts. This landmark book explores the nature of consciousness through the lens of physics rather than neuroscience. Physics has been an explanatory force in diverse phenomena, and it can offer an entirely new vision of consciousness as an irreducible entity, similar to particles, the fundamental units of energy or matter. The fermionic mind hypothesis emerges as a tour-de-force synthesis and framework for understanding consciousness, reimagined as the elemental unit of intellect. It highlights particle organization, a fundamental structure that cannot be understood as the sum of its parts, as the essential analogy between fermions and consciousness. The book presents an engaging scientific narrative that explores some of humanity's oldest and most challenging questions. What is consciousness? What are emotions? How can a physical brain create subjective experience? Do we have free will? Engaging and penetrating, Emotional Reasoning represents a groundbreaking perspective that will surprise you at every turn. It will enhance your confidence through understanding yourself and your place in the cosmic order. Beyond neuroscience, the book holds profound implications for artificial intelligence research. It reveals the intricate link between consciousness and the physical universe, echoing the philosophical insight of theoretical physicist John Wheeler: "The physical world is, in some deep sense, tied to the human being."
Article
Full-text available
Most past research on positive affect and emotion has focused exclusively on high-arousal positive affect (HAPA: e.g., excited), however, low-arousal positive affect (LAPA: e.g., calm) increasingly is included in emotion research. As such, there is a need to synthesize knowledge about the similarities and differences between LAPA and HAPA, the operationalization of LAPA and HAPA, and the distinct characteristics and importance of LAPA within emotional life. A systematic search identified 226 research papers comparing LAPA with HAPA from a broad spectrum of research topics; this review provides a narrative summary of their findings. Indications of differences between LAPA and HAPA were found in 89% of comparisons, with LAPA having a consistently distinguishable relationship to variables such as brain activity, cardiovascular health, decision-making, memory, mindfulness, personality, and solitude, among others. Other notable aspects of LAPA were found, including its role in stress, work, positive sociality, and well-being, as well as its importance in older adults and women. An analysis of items used to measure LAPA and HAPA revealed nuanced differences in conceptualizations, as well as emerging consensus around specific item usage. While considering item use in light of approach-avoidance motivation, we identified three possible LAPA subtypes: calm (a steady state of neither approach nor avoidance), satisfaction (having successfully approached), and relief (having successfully avoided). This review clarifies LAPA’s role in affective life, underscoring that LAPA’s differences from HAPA should be considered in research involving positive affect.
Chapter
Despite broad interest in how children and youth cope with stress and how others can support their coping, this is the first Handbook to consolidate the many theories and large bodies of research that contribute to the study of the development of coping. The Handbook's goal is field building - it brings together theory and research from across the spectrum of psychological, developmental, and related sciences to inform our understanding of coping and its development across the lifespan. Hence, it is of interest not only to psychologists, but also to neuroscientists, sociologists, and public health experts. Moreover, work on stress and coping touches many areas of applied social science, including prevention and intervention science, education, clinical practice, and youth development, making this Handbook a vital interdisciplinary resource for parents, teachers, clinical practitioners, social workers, and anyone interested in improving the lives of children.
Article
Full-text available
This review organizes a variety of phenomena related to emotional self-report. In doing so, the authors offer an accessibility model that specifies the types of factors that contribute to emotional self-reports under different reporting conditions. One important distinction is between emotion, which is episodic, experiential, and contextual, and beliefs about emotion, which are semantic, conceptual, and decontextualized. This distinction is important in understanding the discrepancies that often occur when people are asked to report on feelings they are currently experiencing versus those that they are not currently experiencing. The accessibility model provides an organizing framework for understanding self-reports of emotion and suggests some new directions for research.
Article
Full-text available
Emotional suppression and expression both occur frequently in daily social interactions, yet research examining these emotion regulation processes simultaneously in naturalistic contexts remains limited. Although theory and research tend to reflect an implicit assumption that suppression and expression represent opposite sides of the same construct, they are likely to occur independently and exert different influences on intrapersonal and interpersonal experiences. In 4 experience-sampling and longitudinal studies, we assessed the personal and interpersonal consequences of daily emotional suppression and expression within romantic and close relationships. Mixed-model analyses revealed that suppression and expression consistently predicted independent and distinct outcomes across the studies. When individuals suppressed their emotions, they experienced more intrapersonal costs such as greater depressed mood, greater fatigue, lower self-esteem, and lower life satisfaction. Interpersonally, they also felt less acceptance from others, more distancing by others, and less relationship satisfaction. Greater suppression in daily life also predicted increases in depressive symptoms and reductions in relationship satisfaction 3 months later. In contrast, when individuals were more emotionally expressive during daily interactions, they experienced interpersonal benefits such as greater acceptance from others, greater relatedness and relationship satisfaction, and less distancing by others. Greater emotional expression in daily life also predicted increases in self-esteem and relationship satisfaction across time. Meta-analyses of the 4 studies confirmed the reliability and significance of these relationships; |r’s| = .12–.33. These studies demonstrate that suppression and expression are distinct processes used to manage emotions within social relationships and operate differently in shaping personal well-being and relationship functioning.
Article
Full-text available
Emotion regulation has been examined extensively with regard to important outcomes, including psychological and physical health. However, the literature includes many different emotion regulation strategies but little examination of how they relate to one another, making it difficult to interpret and synthesize findings. The goal of this meta-analysis was to examine the underlying structure of common emotion regulation strategies (i.e., acceptance, behavioral avoidance, distraction, experiential avoidance, expressive suppression, mindfulness, problem solving, reappraisal, rumination, worry), and to evaluate this structure in light of theoretical models of emotion regulation. We also examined how distress tolerance—an important emotion regulation ability —relates to strategy use. We conducted meta-analyses estimating the correlations between emotion regulation strategies (based on 331 samples and 670 effect sizes), as well as between distress tolerance and strategies. The resulting meta-analytic correlation matrix was submitted to confirmatory and exploratory factor analyses. None of the confirmatory models, based on prior theory, was an acceptable fit to the data. Exploratory factor analysis suggested that 3 underlying factors best characterized these data. Two factors—labeled Disengagement and Aversive Cognitive Perseveration—emerged as strongly correlated but distinct factors, with the latter consisting of putatively maladaptive strategies. The third factor, Adaptive Engagement, was a less unified factor and weakly related to the other 2 factors. Distress tolerance was most closely associated with low levels of repetitive negative thought and experiential avoidance, and high levels of acceptance and mindfulness. We discuss the theoretical implications of these findings and applications to emotion regulation assessment.
Article
Background: Rare high arousal negative emotions are known triggers of sudden death in individuals with preexisting heart disease. Whether everyday fluctuations in emotional arousal influence arrhythmia risk is unknown. Methods: We studied 160 patients with the congenital Long QT Syndrome (LQTS), 199 patients with coronary artery disease, and two groups of matched healthy volunteers (n=53 and 50, respectively). Three-day home visits including a 12-hour Holter recording each day were completed. Subjects engaged in typical daily activities and were paged 10 times per day. On each occasion subjects rated the intensity of 16 different emotions during the 5 minutes preceding the page. Holter data over those 5 minute epochs were analyzed for heart rate and QT interval corrected for heart rate (QTc). Analyses focused on within-subject covariation of momentary emotion and QTc. Results: In LQTS patients activated positive affect (APA) and activated negative affect were associated with QTc shortening, whereas low arousal positive affect (LAPA) (calm and relaxed) was associated with QTc lengthening, which at times exceeded 500 msec. Findings were not affected by beta blocker status or observed in younger healthy subjects. Findings were 3-8 times stronger in the LQT2 genotype, known to be prone to emotion-induced events, relative to the LQT1 genotype. Findings in LQTS patients for APA and LAPA were replicated in coronary artery disease patients relative to older healthy subjects. Conclusion: These findings suggest that even subtle changes in emotional arousal may alter repolarization reserve and contribute to sudden death risk in vulnerable individuals.
Article
Whereas past research has examined the use of emotion regulation strategies in terms of individual differences or responses to experimental manipulations, this research takes a naturalistic and repeated-measures approach to examine suppression use in specific situations. Using an experience sampling design, we find evidence across two samples (total N = 215) that (1) there was substantial within-person variation in suppression use, (2) the situational use of suppression was explained by situational differences in extraversion and social hierarchy, and (3) when used in contexts in which people felt they were low in social hierarchy, the negative relationship between suppression and well-being was attenuated. These findings suggest there are contexts in which suppression use may not be maladaptive, and demonstrate the benefits of studying emotion processes in real-life.
Article
One of the fastest growing areas within psychology is the field of emotion regulation. However, enthusiasm for this topic continues to outstrip conceptual clarity, and there remains considerable uncertainty as to what is even meant by “emotion regulation.” The goal of this review is to examine the current status and future prospects of this rapidly growing field. In the first section, I define emotion and emotion regulation and distinguish both from related constructs. In the second section, I use the process model of emotion regulation to selectively review evidence that different regulation strategies have different consequences. In the third section, I introduce the extended process model of emotion regulation; this model considers emotion regulation to be one type of valuation, and distinguishes three emotion regulation stages (identification, selection, implementation). In the final section, I consider five key growth points for the field of emotion regulation.