ArticlePDF Available

Tree Climbing Methodology for Orangutan Conservation

Authors:
  • Dallas College

Abstract and Figures

Habitat loss, poaching and the pet trade have resulted in thousands of orangutans being rescued and taken to rehabilitation centers, many of them infant or juvenile orphans. Besides caring for them, the goal of these centers is to train them for release back into the wild, and critical to the success of post-release survival is their competence in moving through the forest canopy. Orphaned orangutans need regular practice at tree-climbing in order to prepare them for their future life in the wild. Training for this is lacking at rehabilitation centers, however, because caregivers engage with the orphaned orangutans largely on the ground. Orangutan rehabilitation can be greatly improved by training orangutan caregivers in a standardized and safe method of tree climbing so that they can encourage and accompany orangutans to spend more time in the trees. Caregivers trained in tree climbing can also help in wild orangutan rescues, in center maintenance and, through arboreal data collection, in scientific research. We discuss the practical solution of implementing a standardized tree climbing method to orangutan caregivers. Professional climbers have volunteered with the nonprofit Tree Monkey Project to teach orangutan caregivers in Bornean Indonesia and Malaysia. The tree-climbing training program has been successful in allowing caregivers to climb trees alongside orphaned orangutans, encouraging the orangutans to increase the time they spend in the trees and allowing them to hone their agility and skills in moving about in them.
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Primate Conservation 2019 (33): 6 pp.
Tree Climbing Methodology for Orangutan Conservation
Judah M. Epstein1 and James Reed2
1Department of Math and Science, Brookhaven College, Farmers Branch, TX, USA
2Tree Monkey Project, San Rafael, CA, USA
Abstract: Habitat loss, poaching and the pet trade have resulted in thousands of orangutans being rescued and taken to rehabili-
tation centers, many of them infant or juvenile orphans. Besides caring for them, the goal of these centers is to train them for
release back into the wild, and critical to the success of post-release survival is their competence in moving through the forest
canopy. Orphaned orangutans need regular practice at tree-climbing in order to prepare them for their future life in the wild.
Training for this is lacking at rehabilitation centers, however, because caregivers engage with the orphaned orangutans largely on
the ground. Orangutan rehabilitation can be greatly improved by training orangutan caregivers in a standardized and safe method
of tree climbing so that they can encourage and accompany orangutans to spend more time in the trees. Caregivers trained in
tree climbing can also help in wild orangutan rescues, in center maintenance and, through arboreal data collection, in scientic
research. We discuss the practical solution of implementing a standardized tree climbing method to orangutan caregivers. Pro-
fessional climbers have volunteered with the nonprot Tree Monkey Project to teach orangutan caregivers in Bornean Indonesia
and Malaysia. The tree-climbing training program has been successful in allowing caregivers to climb trees alongside orphaned
orangutans, encouraging the orangutans to increase the time they spend in the trees and allowing them to hone their agility and
skills in moving about in them.
Key words: Orangutan, tree climbing, rehabilitation, arboreal locomotion
Introduction
Orphaned orangutan post-release survival is substantially
dependent upon their receiving proper training at their rehabil-
itation centers. It is vital that they be prepared for an arboreal
life, yet orangutan training at rehabilitation centers is largely
at ground level or only limited heights above the ground. A
survey of survival rates from reintroduction showed that it
ranges from 20% to 80%, providing a realistic average of
40% successful survival (Russon 2009). One of the critical
skills determining survival is competence in travelling in the
tree canopy to explore their home range, to make nests to rest
and sleep in (Descovich et al. 2011) and, most particularly, to
nd food sources (Rosen and Byers 2002) and have the agility
and skill to reach and feed on them (Russon 2002; Descovich
et al. 2011). It has been shown that captive orangutans that
build complex nests for a more comfortable sleeping environ-
ment sleep better (Samson and Shumaker 2013). Enhanced
arboreal skills enable orangutans to build more complex nests,
and nests higher in the canopy where they are better protected
from tigers (Riedler et al. 2010).
Common practice at orangutan rehabilitation centers and
their forest schools is for the human caregivers to interact with
orangutans while at ground level where the humans are most
comfortable. Caregivers may also encourage the orphans to
explore constructed wooden jungle gyms of limited height.
Much better would be to have the caregivers climb the trees
and/or rest on arboreal platforms. This would encourage the
orangutans to follow their mother substitutes to higher up in
the trees, and thus make them exercise better their natural
climbing abilities. The climbing technique for human care-
givers we describe here is synthetic-rope-supported access to
the canopy. The aim is not to teach locomotion techniques
to orangutans but for the caregivers to get up into the canopy
and encourage and motivate the orangutans to follow them,
navigating the trunks, branches and lianas to acquire and
practice the necessary skills. Climbing towers and canopy
decks should be constructed if there are insufcient or no
trees in the training areas.
There are other advantages to tree climbing competency.
Staff could use the skill to improve the rehabilitation center’s
outside facilities through maintenance such as trimming trees
Epstein and Reed
2
around enclosures and pens to keep animals from escaping.
When forests are being clear-cut for oil palm plantations or
being degraded and fragmented by logging and mining opera-
tions, orangutans are often stranded and unable to escape, and
personnel from orangutan rehabilitation and rescue organiza-
tions are then asked to rescue them. Rescuers dart the orang-
utan and are generally able to catch it in a net as it falls, but
sometimes, the drugged orangutan becomes stuck in the tree,
and a rescuer then has to climb up, without safety equipment,
to release it (IAR 2015). Technical tree-climbing techniques
using ropes, harnesses and the appropriate equipment would
benet rescuers in safely retrieving and rescuing orangutans.
A safe method for climbing trees would enable expanded
and improved research on the ecology of orangutans. Moni-
toring orangutan numbers and distribution could be more
precise if there were better measures of nest decay. Current
research relies on ground and aerial surveys (Ancrenaz et al.
2004) and getting close up to the nest would yield more accu-
rate results. Access to canopy nests could allow for research
on long-term stress levels of orangutans. The Department of
Biopsychology at the Dresden Technical University in Ger-
many has a laboratory that analyzes hair cortisol, the primary
stress hormone and a crucial indicator of animal welfare and
survival. Hair taken from captive orangutans has yielded
cortisol data correlated to stress levels, showing stress his-
tory over several years. Yet the department has not collected
hair cortisol data from any wild or reintroduced orangutans
(Carlitz 2016). Technical tree climbing could enable access
to wild nests to directly retrieve hair samples for examining
stress history and for genetic analyses. Hair samples from
nests could identify reintroduced orangutans, contributing to
monitoring their ranging and survival and, as indicated by
Houle et al. (2004; p.237), “primate ecological studies can
benet from accessing the canopy to estimate intra-tree and
inter-tree variation in food availability and nutrient value,
patch and subpatch depletion, foraging efciency, as well as
nest structure and nesting behaviors, parasitic transmission
and predator detectability.”
A way to enhance orangutan rehabilitation, rescue, and
scientic research is to train orangutan caregivers and conser-
vation staff in safe, technical tree-climbing methods.
Tree-climbing Courses
Modern tree-climbing methodologies and reliable climb-
ing gear are already available. Using climbing methods
derived from those of arborists, rock climbers, and speleolo-
gists (Houle et al. 2004) allows for access to the tree canopy
for scientic research and data collection (Lowman and Whit-
tman 1996). We have given courses to orangutan caregiv-
ers during four trips to Indonesia and Malaysia—early 2016,
early 2018, late 2018, and early 2019 (Fig 1). The instructors
completed ofcial training from Tree Climbing International
(TCI) and/or had signicant eld experience in the arborist
industry. The methodology these instructors taught to the
orangutan caregivers is the standard method used by TCI,
which is intended for recreational tree-climbing purposes.
The recreational method provides climbers with a simple
and basic system to climb trees and maneuver within them.
More advanced techniques used by experienced professional
arborists for large limb removal, arboreal chainsaw work, and
for the use and maintenance of a much larger assortment of
equipment are not necessary for initial training and tasks for
orangutan conservation. The basic recreational method is
quicker to learn, simpler to remember, and has less equipment
involved. Members of the Tree Monkey Project have found it
to be fully suitable for the orangutan caregivers to accomplish
and safely succeed in the conservation tasks discussed in this
article. We emphasize, however, that this article is not an
instructional climbing manual. A multi-day, hands-on climb-
ing course supervised by experienced instructors is essential
to achieve safe climbing practices. Here, we simply provide
a basic overview of the methodology we have taught.
The climbing courses taught by the Tree Monkey Project
instructors to the orangutan caregivers consisted of about ve
full days of eld instruction. The rst day begins with an
introduction to the environment and the gear. Each orang-
utan conservation organization was given one or two, full
sets of climbing gear (donated by the Tree Monkey Proj-
ect), consisting of arborist climbing rope with a storage bag,
auto-locking carabiners, additional gear carabiners, an arbor-
ist climbing saddle (harness) with side D rings for ipline
attachment, ipline, lanyard for foot ascent, mechanical foot
ascender, helmet, throwline cube with throwline, throwline
bag with throwline, throw weights, slingshot on six foot pole,
gloves, and a whistle (Fig. 2). All gear provided was new,
certied climbing equipment. The students were told that if
replacement gear is required in the future, then it is manda-
tory for arborist climbing rope, carabiners, arborist climbing
saddle, and helmet to be purchased as new certied climbing
Figure 1. Judah Epstein teaching tree climbing to orangutan caregivers.
Photograph by Jim Cairnes.
Tree climbing to help orangutan orphans
3
Figure 2. Tree-climbing gear provided to orangutan rehabilitation centers.
Photograph by Judah Epstein.
equipment. Climbing equipment that is used or not certied
for climbing is not authorized for use. Students were also
instructed to wear boots for footwear. They were familiarized
with the gear and proper gear inspection such as checking for
cracks, fraying, worn materials, and cuts. All climbing gear
has to be inspected prior to each use to ensure that it is clean
and in suitable condition. After each use, all gear has to be
cleaned and dry before properly storing.
Next, the site environment is inspected for hazards such
as power lines, structures, and other hazards or fallen trees
near the tree. The ground below the tree is inspected for
indications of tree health, such as root are, soil mounding,
and standing water, and the tree is carefully checked for its
health and the possibility that it presents personal hazards—
the natural strength of the species, poisonous vines, insects,
animals, cracks and splits, bark, fungus, rotting, dead and
weak branches, and potentially falling branches. When the
gear, the environment, and the tree are approved for scaling,
the team then develops a climbing plan and begin setup for
tree entry. The students are instructed with details regard-
ing anchor (branch) selection, considering tree type, angles,
branch union placement, and minimum diameter.
The climbing method taught is the Double Rope Tech-
nique (DRT). The students learn and practice the series of
knots on the rope to setup the DRT system. Safety knots are
always employed. The same system is used for ascent and
descent. Such uniformity, with no modication to the system
to switch between ascending and descending, increases
safety and enables more ease and condence to novice, and
advanced, climbers. Upon sufcient repetitions to practice
the basic climbing techniques, the students’ progress to more
advanced climbing and maneuvering while in the canopy.
They employ the same skills learned on the ground, but now
implemented while in the tree. The climber learns to analyze
and set a new anchor high up in the tree while connected to the
initial anchor until the new anchor system has been veried,
fully weight tested, and with safety knots tied. Students also
learn to maneuver within the trees and among the branches,
while maintaining a safety connection to the main anchor and
possibly an additional line. Students learn to limb-walk along
the branches. They primarily climb the rope to summit the
canopy, but the branches can also be used for summiting and
exploring through the height and width of the tree. They can
even transfer to an adjacent tree. With the help and in-tree
supervision of the Tree Monkey Project instructors, students
climb various trees to experience and overcome the different
challenges presented by each.
Courses given—2016
The Tree Monkey Project completed four climbing train-
ing courses to staff of eight conservation centers. The courses
were not held in orangutan rehabilitation centers, and the Tree
Monkey Project instructors did not meet with orangutans. In
February and March of 2016, Tree Monkey Project personnel
traveled throughout Borneo instructing staff of the following
centers.
The Orangutan Project: Matang Wildlife Center,
Kuching, Malaysia
Bornean Sun Bear Conservation Center (BSCC):
Sepilok, Malaysia
Tabin Orangutan Rescue: Sepilok, Malaysia
Borneo Orangutan Survival (BOS), East
Kalimantan Orangutan Reintroduction Pro-
gram: Balikpapan, Indonesia
Kawasan Wisata Pendidikan Lingkungan Hidup
(KWPLH): Balikpapan, Indonesia
Pro Natura Foundation, Sungai Wain National
Forest: Balikpapan, Indonesia
Borneo Orangutan Survival (BOS) / Orangutan
Tropical Peatland Project (OUTrop): Nyaru Men
teng Rescue Center- Palanka raya, Indonesia
International Animal Rescue: Ketapang, Indonesia
Courses given—2018-2019
In February 2018, October 2018, and April 2019, we
returned to the area of Balikpapan, East Kalimantan, Indonesia,
specically to train orangutan caregiver staff of Jejak Pulang,
an Indonesian foundation supported by Four Paws Interna-
tional, Vienna, Austria (<https://www.four-paws.org/>). Ini-
tial training was provided to new students (February: ve;
October: ve). Returning students (same as in February, plus
one from KWPLH of 2016) received advanced training of
split tail and ip line, which are techniques for maneuvering
in the tree, along branches as well as up and down branches
to reach higher and lower positions, and spiking which is a
technique for ascending without using a high anchor. They
were also given demonstrations of pruning and limb removal.
The April 2019 course consisted of initial training to four new
students, while returning students received advanced training
of aerial rescue and the Single Rope Technique (SRT).
Epstein and Reed
4
Considerations Regarding the Courses and Methods
Taught
The two main methods of tree climbing with ropes are
Double Rope Technique (DRT) and Single Rope Technique
(SRT). The original plan of the Tree Monkey Project was
to instruct all students rst in DRT and then in SRT, within
one week. During the rst instructional week session in
2016, students successfully learned DRT and then progressed
to receive initial instruction in SRT. Students had dif-
culty learning and achieving condence with SRT because
of the additional complexities of the mechanical equipment
and varied method of ascent and descent, as compared to
the simpler knot method of DRT. To maximize safety, the
Tree Monkey Project leadership decided to discontinue SRT
instruction for the initial one-week training sessions. The
time that would have been allocated to SRT instruction tran-
sitioned to additional DRT practice, condence building and
maneuvering skills in the canopy. In April 2019, SRT was
successfully instructed to returning students, after they had
had months of DRT self-practice.
The expedition in 2016 trained dozens of climbers of
several rehabilitation centers. Each center was left with a
complete set of climbing gear. From most centers, either no
climbing feedback had been received, or feedback of no ongo-
ing climbing due to various reasons such as employee attri-
tion of the trained climber. Positive feedback was received
from KWPLH in Balikpapan, Indonesia, as one climber in
the 2016 training returned for advanced training to both of
the courses in 2018 with Jejak Pulang. This climber was very
enthusiastic and has climbed often since the initial training in
2016. KWPLH is a center for Sun Bears, so this climber has
used his abilities for center maintenance but not for orangutan
rehabilitation.
The 2016 courses introduced the methodology through-
out the island of Borneo as an innovative method to help
orangutans improve their skills in moving about in the trees,
and set the stage for success when reintroduced. While the
2016 courses may have had minimal direct impact towards
the training of orangutans, the 2018 and 2019 courses have
already made progress in this sense. The success of the 2018
and 2019 courses was in large part due to the Tree Monkey
Project closely interfacing with the onsite leadership of Jejak
Pulang, Dr. Signe Preuschoft, and the rehabilitation coor-
dinator Nur Aoliya. The onsite leadership and scientists
experienced the eld training to understand the process and
requirements. The leadership made the decisions regarding
implementing forest school methods and adapting to the new
procedures. Jejak Pulang is a relatively small rehabilitation
center, which began operating only in 2017, and the lead-
ership is more open and exible to innovative methods for
orangutan rehabilitation. The center currently houses only a
small number of orangutan orphans with a focus solely on
rehabilitation. These attributes have enabled Jejak Pulang’s
initial success in implementing tree climbing at its forest
school. The caregivers climb trees and the orangutans follow
alongside their surrogate mothers, ascending to the canopy
(Winter 2018). Caregiver climbing with orangutan orphans
has been limited to infants of about 2 years of age, with high
learning and high re-introduction potential (Figs. 3 and 4).
The 2016 training courses were marked by minimal inter-
action and rst-hand climbing experience with the leadership
of each organization. The instruction focused on the tech-
nique of climbing to the individual conservation staff. There
was at least initial buy-in from leadership since it was the
leadership that arranged for each tree-climbing training pro-
gram. But we suspect that climbing may have been discontin-
ued post-training, at least in part, due to leadership not being
directly involved during the training. At Jejak Pulang, on
the other hand, leadership was, and continues to be directly
involved throughout.
All of the students in the 2016 courses and all of the
students in the February 2018 course were men. Many of
them were not the actual orangutan caregivers, but instead
staff with other responsibilities at the centers. Most orang-
utan caregivers are women, acting as surrogate mothers,
Figure 3. The orphaned orangutan, Tegar, following her tree climbing surrogate
mother, Aoliya, into the canopy. Photograph © Four Paws | Nanang Sujana.
Figure 4. Tegar, an orphaned orangutan, encouraged by caregivers, Yani and
Aoliya, to climb into the canopy. Photograph© Four Paws | Nanang Sujana.
Tree climbing to help orangutan orphans
5
and women in Borneo, for cultural reasons, are less encour-
aged to participate in the tree-climbing program. The suc-
cessful February-2018 training at Jejak Pulang of only male
students (including actual caregivers) was followed by the
October-2018 and April-2019 training at Jejak Pulang that
included female orangutan caregivers. We were able to suc-
cessfully engage the main caregivers, by working closely
with the leadership in demonstrating the usefulness and worth
of the methodology.
The October-2018 course successfully instructed caregiv-
ers in climbing trees to encourage orangutans to be arboreal,
but the Tree Monkey Project also constructed two platforms
high up in the trees at the forest school that could be reached
with ladders. This allowed caregivers to be up in the trees
without the need or expertise of tree climbing. This method
improved on the caregiver interacting on the ground with the
orangutan in that it led to the orangutans being higher up and
spending more time in the trees, even though it lacked the
maneuverability in the arboreal environment for optimum
interaction between the caregiver and orangutan.
Conclusions
An orphaned and rescued orangutan’s acquisition of
competence in arboreal living skills is obviously critical to
its survival post-release. We have successfully taught orang-
utan conservation center staff throughout Borneo a standard-
ized tree-climbing methodology for working with young
orangutans in one important aspect of their rehabilitation. To
enhance effectiveness, the training should be well coordi-
nated with the onsite leadership and decision-makers of the
center. The leadership must be inclined towards innovation
and capable of, and committed to, adapting their existing pro-
grams. The tree-climbing training program for the conserva-
tion staff has had some initial success in so far as caregiv-
ers have climbed trees at the forest school and been able to
encourage the orphaned orangutans towards arboreal activ-
ity. Time will tell if this will be signicant in increasing the
success rate in reintroduction as a result of what we believe
will enhance their skills in moving about in their arboreal
world. Next steps may be to carry out comparative studies
of the well-being and skills of those orangutans that receive
care that extends to providing this extra arboreal activity with
those that perchance do not, and to subsequently accrue data
on rehabilitation success and the benets of tree climbing to
the maintenance, rescue, and research activities of the rescue
centers.
Acknowledgments
We thank the Tree Monkey Project and its donors for
nancially supporting these expeditions to teach tree climbing
to orangutan conservation staff, and the volunteers and lead-
ers of Tree Monkey Project, specically Jim Cairnes, Wouter
Baa, and Tamela Fish. We also thank all of the conservation
centers and staff that we trained throughout Borneo. Speci-
cally, we acknowledge the Matang Wildlife Center and the
leadership of Leo Biddle, Natasha Beckerson, and Dominic
Kelundek for supporting and coordinating the multiple train-
ings on the 2016 expedition, and we are grateful to Jejak
Pulang and Four Paws International and the leadership of
Signe Preuschoft for continued support and their partnership
in the 2018 and 2019 expeditions.
Literature Cited
Ancrenaz, M., O. Gimenez, L. Ambu, K. Ancrenaz, P. Andau,
B. Gossens, J. Payne, A. Sawang, A. Tuuga and I. Lack-
man-Ancrenz. 2004. Aerial surveys give new estimates
for orangutans in Sabah, Malaysia. PLoS Biol. 3(1): e3.
DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030003
Carlitz, E. H. 2016. Monitoring Long-term Stress through
Hair Cortisol Analysis for Conservation and Captive
Management in Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and
Orangutans (Pongo spp.). Doctoral dissertation. Univer-
sity of Zurich, Zurich.
Descovich, K. A., B. M. Galdikas. A. Tribe, A. Lisle and C. J.
Phillips, C. 2011. Fostering appropriate behavior in reha-
bilitant orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Int. J. Primatol.
32: 616–633.
Houle, A., C. Chapman and W. Vickery. 2004. Tree climbing
strategies for primate ecological studies. Int. J. Primatol.
25: 237-260.
IAR. 2015. Rescues Mother and Infant Orangutans
from Forest Fires. International Animal Rescue
(IAR), Uckeld, East Sussex, UK. URL: <https://
www.internationalanimalrescue.org/news/
iar-rescues-mother-and-infant-orangutans-forest-res>.
Lowman, M. D. and P. K. Whittman. 1996. Forest canopies:
methods, hypothesis, and future directions. Ann. Rev.
Ecol. Syst. 27: 55–81.
Riedler, B., E. Millesi and P. H. Pratje. 2010. Adaptation to
forest life during the reintroduction process of immature
Pongo abelii. Int. J. Primatol. 31: 647-663.
Rosen, N. and O. Byers (eds.). 2002. Orangutan Conservation
and Reintroduction Workshop: Final Report. IUCN/SSC
Conservation Breeding Specialist Group, Apple Valley,
MN. URL: <http://www.cpsg.org/content/orangutan-
conservation-and-reintroduction-workshop-2002>.
Russon, A. E. 2002. Return of the native: cognition and site-
specic expertise in orangutan rehabilitation. Int. J. Pri-
matol. 23: 461–478.
Russon, A. E. 2009. Orangutan rehabilitation and reintroduc-
tion. In: Orangutans: Geographic Variation in Behav-
ioral Ecology and Conservation, S. A. Wich, S. S. Utami
Atmoko, T. Mitra Setia and C. P. van Schaik (eds.), pp.
327–350). Oxford University Press, New York.
Samson, D. R. and R. W. Shumaker, R. W. 2013. Document-
ing orang-utan sleep architecture: sleeping platform com-
plexity increases sleep quality in captive Pongo. Behav-
iour 150: 845–861.
Epstein and Reed
6
Winter, S. 2018. King of the swingers: young orang-
utans being taught to climb rainforest trees by
humans. Express 31 October 2018. URL: <https://
www.express.co.uk/news/nature/1038916/
orangutans-taught-to-climb-by-humans>/
Authors' addresses:
Judah M. Epstein, Department of Math and Science,
Brookhaven College, 3939 Valley View Lane, Farmers
Branch, TX 75244, USA; and James Reed, Tree Monkey
Project, 2100 4th St., San Rafael, CA 94901, USA. E-mail of
rst author: <jepstein@dcccd.edu>.
Received for publication: 6 May 2019
Revised: 8 June 2019
... In periods when Tegar was bullied by Gonda he would eschew being in trees while Gonda was using the canopy and would climb up after Gonda had returned to the caregivers. We attempted to familiarise the infants with the canopy by using professional tree climbing skills to hoist caregivers into over 10 m height where they could act as a safe base while infants explored, fed, peered at experts, competed or played [57]. ...
... By the age of weaning, an orangutan juvenile's diet overlaps by nearly 90 percent with that of his or her mother [58]. Deprived of mother's modelling and tuition orphans have to rely on human surrogates with imperfect knowledge, different techniques for food extraction and lacking arboreal competences (but see [57]). The modelling of human surrogate mothers is complemented by trial-and-error learning and by social learning from other orphans (horizontal-oblique transmission). ...
Article
Full-text available
Orangutans depend on social learning for the acquisition of survival skills. The development of skills is not usually assessed in rescued orphans’ pre‐release. We collected data of seven orphans over an 18‐months‐period to monitor the progress of ontogenetic changes. The orphans,1.5–9 years old, were immersed in a natural forest environment with human surrogate mothers and other orphans. Social interactions deviated significantly from those of wild mother‐reared immatures. Infants spent more time playing socially with peers, at the expense of resting and solitary play. Infants were also more often and at an earlier age distant from their human surrogate mothers than wild immatures are from their biological mothers. We found important changes towards an orangutan‐typical lifestyle in 4‐ to 7‐year‐old orphans, corresponding to the weaning age in maternally reared immatures. The older orphans spent less time interacting with human surrogate mothers or peers, started to use the canopy more than lower forest strata and began to sleep in nests in the forest. Their time budgets resembled those of wild adults. In conclusion, juvenile orphans can develop capacities that qualify them as candidates for release back into natural habitat when protected from humanising influences and immersed in a species‐typical environment.
... In periods when Tegar was bullied by Gonda he would eschew being in trees while Gonda was using the canopy and would climb up after Gonda had returned to the caregivers. We attempted to familiarise the infants with the canopy by using professional tree climbing skills to hoist caregivers into over 10 m height where they could act as a safe base while infants explored, fed, peered at experts, competed or played [57]. ...
... By the age of weaning, an orangutan juvenile's diet overlaps by nearly 90 percent with that of his or her mother [58]. Deprived of mother's modelling and tuition orphans have to rely on human surrogates with imperfect knowledge, different techniques for food extraction and lacking arboreal competences (but see [57]). The modelling of human surrogate mothers is complemented by trial-and-error learning and by social learning from other orphans (horizontal-oblique transmission). ...
Article
Full-text available
Simple Summary Like humans, great apes have extended childhoods during which they depend on maternal pedagogy. To help rescued orphans recover from maternal loss our rehabilitation programme is modelled on the natural infant development of orangutans. Orphaned apes cannot be released back into freedom before they have learned the skills necessary to survive alone. To prevent suffering after release we documented the development of survival skills during the rehabilitation process. Seven orangutan orphans aged 1.5–9 years were observed over 18 months in their forest school, immersed in a natural forest environment with human surrogate mothers and other orphans. Social interactions deviated from wild mother-reared immatures: Infant orphans spent more time playing with peers, rested less, and were far from their human surrogate mothers earlier and often than wild immatures are from their biological mothers. Around weaning age, 4- to 7-year-old orphans took up a typical orangutan life-style: they interacted less with human surrogate mothers and peers, stayed high in the trees and slept in nests in the forest. Their time budgets resembled those of wild adults. We conclude that it is not only ethical but also possible to assess survival competences of rehabilitant orphans before release and choose release candidates accordingly. Abstract Orangutans depend on social learning for the acquisition of survival skills. The development of skills is not usually assessed in rescued orphans’ pre-release. We collected data of seven orphans over an 18-months-period to monitor the progress of ontogenetic changes. The orphans, 1.5–9 years old, were immersed in a natural forest environment with human surrogate mothers and other orphans. Social interactions deviated significantly from those of wild mother-reared immatures. Infants spent more time playing socially with peers, at the expense of resting and solitary play. Infants were also more often and at an earlier age distant from their human surrogate mothers than wild immatures are from their biological mothers. We found important changes towards an orangutan-typical lifestyle in 4- to 7-year-old orphans, corresponding to the weaning age in maternally reared immatures. The older orphans spent less time interacting with human surrogate mothers or peers, started to use the canopy more than lower forest strata and began to sleep in nests in the forest. Their time budgets resembled those of wild adults. In conclusion, juvenile orphans can develop capacities that qualify them as candidates for release back into natural habitat when protected from humanising influences and immersed in a species-typical environment.
Article
Full-text available
Wild orangutans ( Pongo spp.) rescued from human-wildlife conflict must be adequately rehabilitated before being returned to the wild. It is essential that released orangutans are able to cope with stressful challenges such as food scarcity, navigating unfamiliar environments, and regaining independence from human support. Although practical skills are taught to orangutans in rehabilitation centres, post-release survival rates are low. Psychological resilience, or the ability to ‘bounce back’ from stress, may be a key missing piece of the puzzle. However, there is very little knowledge about species-appropriate interventions which could help captive orangutans increase resilience to stress. This scoping review summarises and critically analyses existing human and non-human animal resilience literature and provides suggestions for the development of interventions for orangutans in rehabilitation. Three scientific databases were searched in 2021 and 2023, resulting in 63 human studies and 266 non-human animal studies. The first section brings together human resilience interventions, identifying common themes and assessing the applicability of human interventions to orangutans in rehabilitation. The second section groups animal interventions into categories of direct stress, separation stress, environmental conditions, social stress, and exercise. In each category, interventions are critically analysed to evaluate their potential for orangutans in rehabilitation. The results show that mild and manageable forms of intervention have the greatest potential benefit with the least amount of risk. The study concludes by emphasising the need for further investigation and experimentation, to develop appropriate interventions and measure their effect on the post-release survival rate of orangutans.
Article
Full-text available
Reintroduction of Sumatran orangutans can be an important tool to conserve the species in the face of the current decline of wild populations. Monitoring ex-captive orangutans during the reintroduction process provides insight into their adaptation to forest life and is important for evaluating the success of such programs. We investigated activity patterns, food choice, height use, and nest-building abilities of 8 immature orangutans at a reintroduction station in Sumatra. All focal individuals spent most of their time in the forest feeding, but at a lower proportion than wild conspecifics. Among the focal individuals, the behavior of orangutans that avoided human contact differed in several aspects from those that were human bonded. Their diet was composed mainly of fruits, similar to the food choice of wild orangutans, and their ground-avoidance and superior nest-building abilities indicated more effective predation-avoidance behavior vs. that of human-bonded animals. Finally, these immatures showed regular social contact with more experienced conspecifics, which ultimately may facilitate the social learning of local feral expertise. Human-bonded individuals, in contrast, preferentially stayed on the ground and at low heights and rarely built nests, thereby increasing predation risk. Their choice of leaves over fruits as foods indicated a less effective foraging strategy. Nevertheless, this group exhibited individual learning trends toward an adult-like foraging pattern and height use. We conclude that social interactions with conspecifics and humans, especially during an early developmental period, can affect adaptation to forest life and probably also influence the success of orangutan reintroduction processes. Keywordsforaging-height use-immature-orangutan-reintroduction
Article
Full-text available
Primate ecological studies can benefit from accessing the canopy to estimate intra-tree and inter-tree variation in food availability and nutrient value, patch and subpatch depletion, foraging efficiency, as well as nest structure and nesting behaviors, parasitic transmission and predator detectability. We compare several ways to access the canopy and examine their suitability for studies of primates. Two of them—the Single Rope Technique and the Climbing Spur Method—allow people to safely access almost all kinds of trees, regardless of their size, height or shape. Modern climbing gear and contemporaneous safety protocols, derived from rock climbers, speleologists, and industrial arborists, are reliable and appropriate for primate ecological studies. Climbing gear is specialized and still expensive for students, but tree climbing can be dangerous during specific maneuvres. Consequently, formal training and preliminary experience are essential before attempting to collect data. We discuss the physics of falling, risk assessment associated with a fall, knots, gear and safety precautions. Finally, we propose a Tree Climbing Safety Protocol adapted for 2 climbing methods and primate field ecology. Researchers should be aware that climbing safety depends on their own judgment, which must be based on competent instruction, experience, and a realistic assessment of climbing ability. Therefore, the information we provide should be used only to supplement competent personal instruction and training in situ. Although most primate observations have been and will mostly be done from the ground in the future, canopy information complements the observations. Canopy data will add a significant new dimension to our knowledge of primates by providing strategic information otherwise unavailable.
Article
Full-text available
Ex-captive orangutans that have returned to free forest life in Bornean forests offer exceptional opportunities to assess the cognitive challenges facing feral great apes. They also provide opportunities to track readaptation, which requires building expertise to survive in tropical rain forests and to integrate into orangutan communities. I outline some of the foraging problems that ex-captive orangutans encounter, the cognitive processes used in their solutions, and acquisition patterns. Evidence derives primarily from observational studies of feeding behavior in free-ranging ex-captives. Discussion focuses on implications for conservation initiatives.
Article
Of the extant primates, only 20 non-human species have been studied by sleep scientists. Notable sampling gaps exist, including large-bodied hominoids such as gorillas (Gorilla gorilla), orangutans (Pongo spp.) and bonobos (Pan paniscus), for which data have been characterized as high priority. Here, we report the sleep architecture of three female and two male orang-utans housed at the Indianapolis Zoo. Sleep states were identified by scoring correlated behavioural signatures (e.g., respiration, gross body movement, muscle atonia, random eye movement, etc.). The captive orang-utans were focal subjects for a total of 70 nights (1013 h) recorded. We found that orangutans slept an average of 9.11 h (range 5.85-11.2 h) nightly and were characterized by an average NREM of 8.03 h (range 5.47-10.2 h) and REM of 1.11 (range: 0.38-2.2 h) per night. In addition, using a sleeping platform complexity index (SPCI) we found that individuals that manufactured and slept in more complex beds were characterized by higher quality sleep. Sleep fragmentation (the number of brief awakenings greater than 2 mm per hour), arousability (number of motor activity bouts per hour), and total time awake per night were reduced by greater quality sleep environments. Therefore, comfortable sleeping environments reduced arousability and improve sleep quality in captive orang-utans. These results support Fruth & Hohmann's (1996) hypothesis, which purported that the tree-limb-to-sleeping platform transition in Miocene apes improved sleep quality.
Article
Rehabilitation centers in Indonesia and Malaysia accommodate displaced orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus and P. abelii) and aim to facilitate their release into the wild by developing in them the skills that are necessary for survival. Regular forest excursions are provided but their efficacy in improving learning of appropriate behaviors is unknown. We observed 40 rehabilitating orangutans from the Orangutan Care and Quarantine Centre during 3 forest excursions each to determine whether their behavior fostered the development of survival skills. In total, 38% of their time was spent in locomotion, particularly quadrupedal arboreal travel (13%), walking (8%), climbing (7%), and vine-swinging (4%). 26.5% of their time was spent ≥ 5 m from the ground, at heights up to 25 m. Arboreal activities were more common early in the excursions and interaction with caregivers more common later (h 1: 0.3% of time; h 5: 0.9% of time). Individuals of lower body mass were significantly more likely to engage in arboreal movement, locomotion in general, eating of bark and leaves, and social play, and less likely to eat insects. Those that had been at the Centre the longest were less likely to perform arboreal activities and significantly more likely to be found standing and at ground level than those that had been there for a shorter time. During this study, many forest food items were consumed, particularly leaves and fruit, but also invertebrates and bark. Little time was spent in sexual behavior, tool use, nest building, or socially mediated learning, but social play occupied almost 6% of their time. We conclude that regular excursions into the forest are likely to assist in the development of locomotion and feeding skills for survival in rehabilitating orangutans, but special attention is needed to encourage nest building, social activities, and arboreal activity. Heavy individuals and those that have been captive for a long time are least likely to benefit.
Article
Forest canopies contain a major proportion of the diversity of organisms on Earth and constitute the bulk of photosynthetically active foliage and biomass in forest ecosystems. For these reasons canopy research has become integral to the management of forest ecosystems, and to a better understanding of global change. Ecological research in forest canopies is relatively recent and has been primarily descriptive in scope. The development of new methods of canopy access has enabled scientists to conduct more quantified research in tree crowns. Studies of sessile organisms, mobile organisms, and canopy interactions and processes have emerged as subdisciplines of canopy biology, each requiring different methods for collecting data. Canopy biology is beginning to shift from a descriptive autecology of individuals to a more complex ecosystem approach, although some types of field work are still limited by access. Questions currently addressed in canopy research are extremely diverse but emphasize comparisons with respect to sapatial and temporal variation. Spatial scales range from leaves (e.g. quantifying the number of mites on individual phylloplanes) to trees (e.g. measuring photosynthesis between sun and shade leaves), to forest stands (e.g.measuring turbulence above the canopy), and entire landscapes (e.g. comparing mammals between different forest types). Temporal variation is of particular significance in tropical forest canopies, where populations of organisms and their resources have diurnal, seasonal, or even annual periodicity. As the methods for canopy access improve, more rigorous hypotheses-driven field studies remain a priority of this newly coalesced discipline.
Aerial surveys give new estimates for orangutans in Sabah
  • M Ancrenaz
  • O Gimenez
  • L Ambu
  • K Ancrenaz
  • P Andau
  • B Gossens
  • J Payne
  • A Sawang
Ancrenaz, M., O. Gimenez, L. Ambu, K. Ancrenaz, P. Andau, B. Gossens, J. Payne, A. Sawang, A. Tuuga and I. Lackman-Ancrenz. 2004. Aerial surveys give new estimates for orangutans in Sabah, Malaysia. PLoS Biol. 3(1): e3. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030003
King of the swingers: young orangutans being taught to climb rainforest trees by humans
  • S Winter
Winter, S. 2018. King of the swingers: young orangutans being taught to climb rainforest trees by humans. Express 31 October 2018. URL: <https:// w w w. e x p r e s s. c o. u k / n e w s / n a t u r e / 1 0 3 8 9 1 6 / orangutans-taught-to-climb-by-humans>/