ArticlePDF Available

Is It Really Just a Joke? Gender Differences in Perceptions of Sexist Humor

Authors:

Abstract

Benign violation theory suggests humor mocking normative values is funny if the humor is non-threatening. Research suggests sexism toward women (versus men) is particularly threatening due to inequalities in social power. In Study 1, we examined whether men and women differ in how amused and offended they were by sexist humor. We predicted men would perceive sexist humor as more funny and less offensive than women would. In Study 2, we examined whether perceptions of threat were related to perceptions of sexist jokes. We predicted women would perceive more threat than men from sexist humor, which would lead to lower amusement and greater perceived offensiveness. Across both studies, jokes targeting women were perceived to be less funny, more offensive, and more sexist than jokes targeting men. Additionally, greater perceptions of threat were related to greater perceptions of jokes as offensive and sexist. However, women were not more threatened than men by sexist jokes. While these findings were not entirely consistent with our hypotheses, our findings suggest disparagement humor targeting lower-status groups is perceived more negatively than disparagement humor targeting higher-status groups and these perceptions may be inextricably rooted in threat posed to lower-status groups.
Running head: GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR 1
Is It Really Just a Joke? Gender Differences in Perceptions of Sexist Humor
Tiffany J. Lawless
Conor J. O’Dea
Stuart S. Miller
Donald A. Saucier
Kansas State University
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
Abstract
Benign violation theory suggests humor mocking normative values is funny if the humor is non-
threatening. Research suggests sexism toward women (versus men) is particularly threatening
due to inequalities in social power. In Study 1, we examined whether men and women differ in
how amused and offended they were by sexist humor. We predicted men would perceive sexist
humor as more funny and less offensive than women would. In Study 2, we examined whether
perceptions of threat were related to perceptions of sexist jokes. We predicted women would
perceive more threat than men from sexist humor, which would lead to lower amusement and
greater perceived offensiveness. Across both studies, jokes targeting women were perceived to
be less funny, more offensive, and more sexist than jokes targeting men. Additionally, greater
perceptions of threat were related to greater perceptions of jokes as offensive and sexist.
However, women were not more threatened than men by sexist jokes. While these findings were
not entirely consistent with our hypotheses, our findings suggest disparagement humor targeting
lower-status groups is perceived more negatively than disparagement humor targeting higher-
status groups and these perceptions may be inextricably rooted in threat posed to lower-status
groups.
2
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
Is It Really Just a Joke? Gender Differences in Perceptions of Sexist Humor
1 Literature Review
Disparaging humor is typically meant to denigrate, belittle, or vilify a social group (e.g.,
women, Black people; Janes and Olsen 2000), and has the potential to reinforce status
hierarchies between groups (Rappoport 2005; Saucier et al. 2015). While disparagement humor
may be used toward individuals belonging to any social group, disparagement has been shown to
be perceived as more threatening to people belonging to historically-oppressed groups (e.g.,
women) than toward people of historically-privileged groups (e.g., men; Schneider et al. 2000).
This additional threat experienced by targets of humor that “punches down” by disparaging
historically-oppressed groups may cause those groups to interpret the humor as more offensive
and less funny, while targets of humor that “punches up” by disparaging historically-privileged
groups might interpret humor that disparages them less severely (i.e., more funny and less
offensive) because those privileged groups might experience less threat in the social hierarchy.
Our goal in the current studies is to examine how men (a historically-privileged group) and
women (a historically-oppressed group) interpret gender-based disparaging humor differently,
and how threat experienced in response to disparaging humor might affect those interpretations.
1.1 Potential Harmful Effects of Disparaging Humor
The harm disparaging humor potentially poses to historically-oppressed groups is well
documented (e.g., Ford et al. 2013; Saucier et al. 2016). According to prejudiced norm theory,
disparaging humor contributes to a normative culture of prejudice, which then creates a tolerance
for discriminatory acts (Ford and Ferguson 2004; Ford et al. 2008). For example, sexist humor
has been shown to foster the release of explicit micro-assaults against women (Ford and
Ferguson 2004; Ford et al. 2008; Romero-Sanchez et al 2010; Ryan and Kanjorski 1998).
3
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
Disparaging humor can also increase individuals’ endorsement of expressions of prejudice. For
example, when individuals interpret a racial joke as disparaging, they are more likely to endorse
stereotypes about Black individuals (Saucier et al. 2018). Therefore, through disparaging humor,
stereotypes about stigmatized groups can be reinforced through the normative nature of humor.
Through disparaging gender-based humor, language can justify sexist attitudes by disguising
them as socially acceptable “jokes.” Research has shown when sexism is justified through
humor, men higher in hostile sexism will express beliefs justifying the gender status-quo to a
greater degree than if they had been exposed to non-humorous sexist material (Ford et al. 2013).
Moreover, Ford (1997) found that comedy skits disparaging Black people elicited more
stereotypical judgments than did neutral comedy skits among White participants. Ford (2000)
revealed those higher in hostile sexism were significantly more tolerant of sexist events upon
exposure to sexist humor. Taken together, this evidence suggests that disparaging humor can
activate a norm of discrimination tolerance due to perceptions that disparaging humor is more
socially acceptable than more direct forms of disparagement.
The potential for disparaging humor to be harmful and threatening to women has been
shown to be quite extreme. For example, exposure to sexist humor can influence the amount of
money men would cut from hypothetical women’s organizations (Ford et al. 2007). When men
are exposed to sexist jokes, they subsequently rate sexist workplace remarks as less offensive
(Ford 2000; Ford et al. 2001). While past research has demonstrated that women find sexist jokes
targeting men funnier than jokes targeting women (e.g., Abrams, Bippus, and McGaughey,
2015), it has also been shown that sexist jokes harm women's feelings of gender-group positive
distinctiveness (Tajfel and Turner 1986), with negative consequences for domain identification,
motivation, and performance. In each of these domains, sexist humor can instigate self-
4
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
objectification in women and cause them to engage in more body surveillance than neutral
humor (Ford et al. 2015). Over time and repetition, this self-objectification can create a stable,
chronic third-person perspective for perceiving the self, and sexist humor has been shown to
induce state self-objectification (Bemiller and Schneider 2010; Greenwood and Isbell 2002; Ford
et al. 2015). Additionally, research has shown, after exposure to a sexist joke, women could
exhibit discriminatory behaviors against other women while men would not exhibit
discriminatory behavior against other men under the same conditions (Abrams and Bippus,
2014). At an extreme level, exposure to sexist humor has been shown to increase acceptance of
rape myths, victim-blaming, violence against women, and even men’s self-reported rape
proclivity (Ryan and Kanjorski 1998; Romero-Sanchex et al. 2010; Strain et al. 2016; Thomae
and Viki 2013; Thomae and Pina 2015). Taken together, this evidence demonstrates that sexist
humor can threaten women’s financial and social status, self-image, credibility, and physical
safety. However, there is, to our knowledge, little consensus on the relationship between a joke’s
power to threaten and enjoyment of the joke. Some research suggests humor targeting
historically oppressed groups is seen as less funny and more offensive by third-party observers
(e.g., Strain et al., 2015) while other research suggests third-parties enjoy humor targeting lower-
status groups more than that targeting higher-status groups (e.g., Cantor, 1976; McGhee and
Duffey, 1983; Losco and Epstein, 1975). It is possible these conflicting findings are due to
differing natures of the jokes used. Therefore, in Study 1, we sought to determine perceptions of
sexist humor targeting men and women using either gender-specific stereotypes or
interchangeable stereotypes.
While sexist humor may threaten women, it might not be socially acceptable to confront
sexism presented as humor. For example, though similar in nature, it has been shown to be more
5
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
socially acceptable to label racist humor as “racist” than it is to label sexist humor as “sexist”
(Woodzicka et al. 2015). Further, people confronting racism are rated as more likeable than
people confronting sexism (Woodzicka et al. 2015). This lack of acceptable avenues for
confronting sexist humor may cause women to feel more threatened because they feel they are
not allowed to fight the prejudiced behavior. Because the American social hierarchy has
historically favored men over women, and because sexism against women is still rampant in
various forms, women may experience more threat from humor that disparages women than men
experience from humor that disparages men. This might affect interpretations of gender-based
disparaging humor such that women will find such humor more offensive and sexist and less
funny than will men.
1.2 Perceptions of Humor Disparaging Historically-Oppressed Groups
Benign violation theory suggests people may find humor that mocks normative values
funny as long as the humor is non-threatening (McGraw and Warren 2010). Some evidence
suggests that disparaging humor targeting historically-oppressed groups is less socially
acceptable than disparaging humor targeting historically-privileged groups (e.g., Schneider et al.
2016). For example, men who make anti-women jokes on social media are rated less positively
than both men who make anti-men jokes and women who make any kind of gender-based jokes
(Strain et al. 2015). Henry and colleagues (2014) found that slurs directed toward members of
lower-status groups were perceived as more offensive than slurs directed at higher-status groups,
and it has been shown that memes featuring stereotypes of historically-oppressed groups (e.g.,
Black people, women) were perceived as less funny and more offensive than memes featuring
stereotypes of historically-privileged groups (e.g., White people, men; Meza et al. in
preparation). This evidence suggests disparaging humor that “punches down” targeting
6
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
historically-oppressed groups is perceived more negatively than humor that “punches up”
targeting historically-privileged groups. We posit this may be because privileged groups
experience less threat in the current social hierarchy when targeted by disparaging humor than do
historically-oppressed groups. Taking together benign violation theory and past research (e.g.,
Schneider et al. 2016), it is possible the reason humor that “punches down” at more historically
oppressed groups is perceived as more offensive and less funny is that it is more threatening to
those groups. In the current studies, we explore this possibility.
2 The Current Studies
We designed the current studies to test our hypotheses about how men and women
perceive gender-based disparaging humor differently and how threat experienced in response to
disparaging humor affects those perceptions. We were particularly interested in perceptions of
threat in response to disparaging humor because this type of humor has been shown to foster
prejudiced behaviors and beliefs, including endorsements of violence. In two studies, we
measured reactions to disparaging gender-based humor that targeted both men and women. In
Study 1, we measured perceptions of jokes that contained stereotypes unique to each gender
(e.g., women are attracted to money, men are aggressive) and stereotypes that are
interchangeable between genders (e.g., men/women are stupid), and in Study 2, we measured
potential threat posed to target group (i.e., men or women) by the jokes that contained
interchangeable stereotypes in order to empirically examine the possibility the disparities in
perceptions of jokes targeting men and those targeting women were due to perceived threat to
those respective groups. We predicted that disparaging humor targeting women would be
perceived as less funny, more offensive, and more sexist than humor targeting men (Studies 1
and 2). Further, we predicted the relationship between participant gender and perceptions of the
7
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
joke would be mediated by threat, such that women would perceive more threat from jokes
targeting them than would men, which would lead to lower amusement and greater perceived
offensiveness of the joke (Study 2). Our present studies build upon previous research on group-
based disparaging humor (Ford and Ferguson 2004), and have the potential to contribute to
understanding of why men and women differ in perceptions of gender-based humor. Both men
and women may understand this type of humor is sexist, but they may differ in perceptions of
such humor as either harmless or threatening.
3 Study 1
3.1 Method
3.1.1 Participants
Our participants were 98 college students (68.7% identified as White/Caucasian, 10.1%
African American, 2% Asian, 11.1% Latinx, 1% Native American, and 7.1% Other; 55.6%
identified as female; ages 17 to 25, M = 19.28, SD = 1.18) enrolled in introductory psychology
courses at a large Midwestern university who completed the study for research credit.
3.1.2 Materials
3.1.2.1 Disparaging Jokes
We identified jokes that targeted men or women based on stereotypes unique to that
gender, and jokes that targeted men or women based on stereotypes that were interchangeable
across genders. The research team, consisting of content-matter experts who have published on
various forms of group humor, selected these jokes from a pool gathered and created by their
undergraduate research collaborators. The stereotypical jokes were selected because the
researchers agreed the primary intention of the joke was to disparage either men or women with
negative stereotypes. The interchangeable jokes were selected because the researchers agreed the
8
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
primary intention of the joke was to target a gender in a way that could also fit the other gender.
Jokes in each stereotypical set were selected to represent a broad range of well-known negative
stereotypes about each gender and care was taken to make sure the jokes were similar in text
length and had the potential to be humorous, even if the content was offensive. These jokes are
presented in Table 1. All participants read each joke based on stereotypes unique to each gender
in a within-subjects design. Participants also read each interchangeable joke once (for either men
or women) in a between-groups design. Therefore, each participant read a total of 30 jokes. For
each joke, participants responded to the items below.
Table 1
Jokes used in Study 1.
Targeting both genders interchangeably
1. What’s the difference between a knife and an argumentative man(woman)? A knife has
a point.
2. Why does a man(woman) have a clear conscience? Because it’s never used.
3. I have received hundreds of replies to my ad for a husband(wife) and they all say the
same thing – “Take mine.”
4. What should you do if your man(woman) walks out? Shut the door and celebrate.
5. What do you call a man(woman) with half a brain? Gifted.
6. Why do only 10% of men(women) make it to heaven? Because if they all went, it
would be called hell.
7. What do you call a man(woman) with an opinion? Wrong.
8. What’s the smartest thing a man(woman) can say? Anything that starts with, “My
wife(husband) says…”
9. How many men(women) does it take to screw in a light bulb? One – he(she) just holds
it up there and waits for the world to revolve around him(her).
10. What do you call a man(woman) who’s lost 95% of his(her) intelligence? Divorced.
Targeting men based on stereotypes unique to men
1. Why shouldn’t you let a man’s mind wander? Because it’s way too little to be out all
alone.
2. Why did God make Adam before Eve? Everyone needs a rough draft before they make
the final copy.
3. You might as well go for a younger guy. Why? They never mature anyway.
4. Why do little boys whine? Because they’re practicing to be men.
5. How come it’s so hard to make a fool out of a man? Because most of them are the DIY
type in that way.
6. Why can’t men get mad cow disease? Because they are pigs.
7. Why can men read maps better than women? Cause only the male mind could conceive
of one inch equaling a hundred miles.
9
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
8. Why are all dumb blonde jokes one-liners? So men can understand them.
9. How can you know if a man is faithful? If his options are.
10. Why do men play on artificial turf in sports? To keep them from grazing.
Targeting women based on stereotypes unique to women
1. I’m not sexist, because being sexist is wrong and being wrong is for women.
2. Why do men die before their wives? They want to.
3. What’s the difference between a woman and a rain puddle? A puddle goes away after
the sun comes up.
4. If your dog is barding at the back door and your wife is yelling at the front door, who
do you let in first? The dog of course…at least hel’ll shut up after you let him in.
5. What is the difference between a battery and a woman? A battery has a positive side
6. What do elephants have over women? Intelligence, loyalty, and genuine kindness.
7. Why is it a bad idea to ask Siri “What do women want?” She has been talking nonstop
for the last two days.
8. If I had a dollar for every girl that found me unattractive, what would happen? They
would eventually find me attractive.
9. How are the office of the President and Tolkien’s book The Hobbit alike? Neither had a
girl in them and neither should.
10. What is the difference between a wife and a mistress? One is cheating on you, the other
is cheating with you.
3.1.2.2 Perceptions of humor
We created items to measure perceptions of each joke as funny (“This joke is funny”),
sexist (“This joke is sexist”), and offensive (“This joke is offensive”). Participants responded on 1
(not at all) to 9 (very much) Likert scales for each item for each joke. Composite scores were
calculated for how funny, sexist, and offensive participants rated each type of joke by averaging
across participants’ assessments of each of the jokes such that higher scores indicated greater
perceptions of the jokes as funny, sexist, and offensive respectively (alphas available in Table 2).
3.1.3 Procedure
Participants were recruited via the Sona Systems Software at a large Midwestern state
university. Participants first provided informed consent and demographic information, then read
each joke provided in Table 1, and responded to three one-item measures assessing perceptions
of the joke as funny, sexist, and offensive. Participants were then debriefed and thanked.
10
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
4 Results and Discussion
Correlations between measures of perceptions of the jokes are presented in Table 2.
Predictably, perceptions of the jokes as offensive and sexist were strongly, positively correlated,
and these evaluations were weakly to moderately negatively correlated with how funny the jokes
were perceived to be. There are several possibilities as to why these perceptions are correlated,
but one of the more intriguing possibilities is that it is not socially acceptable to disparage
historically-oppressed groups via sexist humor, and therefore people report finding such humor
less funny, and more offensive and sexist.
4.1 Gender-unique stereotype-based jokes
To test our main hypotheses, we conducted planned contrasts to test whether female
participants perceived humor targeting women to be less funny, and more offensive and sexist
than male participants perceived humor targeting men to be, because the main effects and simple
effects of a factorial analysis would not allow us to make these direct comparisons. Consistent
with our hypotheses, female participants perceived the jokes targeting women to be less funny,
t(88)1 = 2.43, p = .02, d = 0.50, more offensive, t(86) = -5.49, p < .001, d = 1.17, and more sexist,
t(85) = -5.55, p < .001, d = 1.20, than male participants perceived the jokes targeting men (see
Figure 1 for the cell means).
To assess whether the above results were due to the effects of participant gender, joke
target, or the interaction between the two, we conducted 2 (joke target: men/women) x 2
(participant gender: men/women) mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) models with repeated
measures on the joke target for each of our three dependent measures of perceptions of the
gender-unique stereotype-based jokes (see Table 3 and Figure 1). We found a main effect of
1 We used pairwise deletion to handle missing data from participants in both Study 1 and Study 2. Thus, our degrees
of freedom are variable.
11
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
participant gender, such that female participants perceived the jokes to be less funny, more
offensive, and more sexist than did male participants. We found no main effects of joke target
and no gender x target interactions. In sum, female participants perceived the gender-unique
stereotype-based jokes to be less funny, and more offensive and sexist than did male participants.
Additionally, perceptions of gender-unique stereotype-based humor did not significantly differ
depending on whether the humor targeted men or women. These additional analyses suggest that
female participants perceived gender-unique stereotype-based jokes targeting women as less
funny, and more offensive and sexist than male participants perceived the jokes targeting men
because female participants perceive humor making fun of either gender as less funny, and more
offensive and sexist than do male participants. This could be because women are more socially
threatened by sexism than are men because of their traditionally lower social status.
4.2 Gender-interchangeable jokes
For the gender-interchangeable jokes, we conducted planned contrasts comparing male
participants’ perceptions of gender-interchangeable jokes targeting men to female participants’
perceptions of gender-interchangeable jokes targeting women. Consistent with our hypotheses,
female participants perceived the jokes targeting women to be less funny, t(39) = 4.27, p < .001,
d = 1.29 (Mwomen = 2.12, SDwomen = 1.32; Mmen = 4.44, SDmen = 2.18), more offensive, t(40) = -8.22,
p < .001, d = 2.63 (Mwomen = 7.07, SDwomen = 1.67; Mmen = 2.55, SDmen = 1.77), and more sexist,
t(40) = -6.41, p < .001, d = 1.94 (Mwomen = 7.71, SDwomen = 1.18; Mmen = 4.73, SDmen = 1.83), than
male participants perceived the jokes targeting men.
As with the gender-unique stereotype-based jokes, we conducted 2 (target of humor:
men/women) x 2 (participant gender: male/female) between-groups ANOVAs for each of our
three dependent measures of perceptions of the gender-interchangeable jokes (see Table 3 and
12
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
Figure 2). The significant main effects of participant gender paralleled our findings above, such
that female participants perceived the gender-interchangeable jokes as less funny, and more
offensive and sexist than did male participants. We also found main effects for gender targeted by
the gender-interchangeable jokes, such that jokes targeting women were perceived to be less
funny, and more offensive and sexist than jokes targeting men. Only for perceived offensiveness
did we find a significant gender x target interaction. Probing this interaction revealed that male
participants perceived a greater difference in offensiveness between jokes targeting men and
jokes targeting women (p < .001, partial η2 = .26) than did female participants (p = .01, partial η2
= .08). Interestingly, the effects are stronger for interchangeable jokes than for stereotype-
specific jokes. This may be because the interchangeable jokes tend to target core traits such as
competence (e.g., What do you call a man/woman with half a brain? Gifted.) that may be
perceived as especially threatening, particularly in a workplace. Many of the stereotype specific
jokes target more antiquated stereotypic traits, such as talking a lot for women (e.g., Why is it a
bad idea to ask Siri “What do women want?” She has been talking nonstop for the last two
days.). Therefore, it is possible that the interchangeable jokes as a whole were seen as more
threatening than the stereotype-specific jokes. However, the interchangeable results are
consistent with our analyses of the gender-unique stereotyped jokes and, taken together, they
could demonstrate that women are more threatened than are men socially by sexism itself rather
than by specific stereotypes associated with being a woman. This is possibly a result of women’s
historically lower place in the status hierarchy compared to men, which may condition modern
women to be more wary of sexism for fear of being pushed down socially. Study 2 examines the
possibility that the disparities in perceptions evidenced in Study 1 were due to differences in
perceived threat from sexist humor.
13
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
5 Study 2
5.1 Method
5.1.1 Participants
We recruited participants using Amazon Mechanical Turk (N = 259, 56.1% identified as
White/Caucasian, 5.4% African American, 26.1% Asian, 3.9% Latinx, 0.4% Native American,
and 8.1% Other; 45.8% identified as female; ages 19 to 69, M = 35.40, SD = 11.91).
5.1.2 Materials
5.1.2.1 Jokes
Participants read the 10 jokes that could target either men or women interchangeably used
in Study 1 (e.g., What’s the difference between a knife and an argumentative man(woman)? A
knife has a point).
5.1.2.2 Perceptions of the jokes as funny, sexist, and offensive
Participants completed the same three one-item measures assessing their perceptions of
each of the jokes as funny, sexist, and offensive used in Study 1. Again, higher scores indicated
greater perceptions of the jokes as funny (alphas provided in Table 5).
5.1.2.3 Perceptions of jokes as threatening to men and women
We also created items to examine how threatening male and female participants perceive
sexist humor targeting men and sexist humor targeting women to be toward the targeted gender.
This scale included 7 items (i.e., This joke harms a man’s[woman’s] sense of self. This joke
demeans men[women]. This joke devalues men[women]. This joke creates a negative perception
of men[women]. This joke attacks manhood[womanhood]. This joke threatens the social
standing of men[women]. This joke threatens the way men[women] are viewed.). to which
participants responded using 1 (not at all) to 9 (very much) Likert scales. Participants’ ratings on
these items were averaged for each of the jokes targeting men and each of the jokes targeting
14
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
women to form composites in which higher scores indicated greater threat to men (α = .99) and
women (α = .99) generally.
5.1.3 Procedure
Participants were recruited via Amazon’s TurkPrime software and accessed our study on
Qualtrics survey software. After providing informed consent and demographic information,
participants read the same 10 jokes that targeted men and women interchangeably in a
counterbalanced order. That is, each participant saw 5 jokes targeting men and 5 targeting
women, with no joke being repeated for a particular participant. Each joke was presented as
targeting men half the time and targeting women half the time. Participants indicated their
perceptions of each joke as it was presented as funny, sexist, offensive, and threatening to men or
women generally depending on the target of the joke. Participants were then debriefed and
thanked for their participation.
6 Results and Discussion
6.1 Perceptions of the jokes
We conducted planned contrasts comparing male participants’ perceptions of gender-
interchangeable jokes targeting men to female participants’ perceptions of gender-
interchangeable jokes targeting women (see Figure 2 for the cell means). Consistent with our
hypotheses and our results from Study 1, female participants perceived the jokes targeting
women to be less funny, t(254) = 5.59, p < .001, d = 0.69. However, inconsistent with our
hypotheses, there were no significant differences in perceived offensiveness, t(255) = -0.89, p = .
38, d = 0.10, perceived sexism, t(255) = -1.62, p = .11, d = 0.21, or perceived threat to targeted
gender t(255) = 0.02, p = .98, d = 0.004. While this may mean that women do not generally
perceive greater threat from sexist jokes targeting women than men perceive from jokes targeting
15
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
men, it is possible that women perceive greater threat across the board than men, or that men
agree that sexist jokes targeting women are more threatening to women than similar jokes are to
men.
As in Study 1, we conducted 2 (target of humor: men/women) x 2 (participant gender:
male/female) mixed ANOVAs with repeated measures based on the target of the humor (i.e., men
or women) for each of our three dependent measures of perceptions of the gender-
interchangeable jokes (see Table 4 and Figure 3). For all of our measures of perceptions of the
jokes, we found main effects of target gender, such that jokes targeting women were perceived as
less funny, and more offensive and sexist than jokes targeting men. Additionally, jokes targeting
women were perceived as more threatening to women than jokes targeting men were perceived
as threatening to men. This demonstrates that, though women do not perceive greater threat from
jokes targeting women than men perceive from jokes targeting men, people generally agree that
jokes targeting women are more threatening than jokes targeting men. This may imply that jokes
“punching down” at women are less acceptable than jokes “punching up” at men. For
perceptions of how funny the jokes were, we found a main effect of participant gender, such that
male participants perceived the jokes as funnier than did female participants, but also a
participant gender by target gender interaction. Probing this interaction revealed that female
participants perceived a greater difference in funniness between jokes targeting men and jokes
targeting women (p < .001, partial η2 = .11) than did male participants (p = .52, partial η2 = .002).
We also found a main effect of participant gender in perceptions of threat to the targeted gender
such that male participants perceived more threat than did female participants. This could be
because men, in their place of privilege, perceive there is more to lose when one’s gender is
threatened than do women, who are already lower on the status hierarchy.
16
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
6.2 Role of perceived threat
We initially wanted to test whether perceived threat mediated gender differences in
perceptions of how funny, offensive, and sexist the jokes were. Due to a lack of significant
gender differences (aside from perceptions of funniness), it was not appropriate to test these
models. However, the correlations between our measures of participants’ perceptions of the jokes
(Table 5) revealed threat to the targeted gender was strongly and positively correlated with
perceived sexism and offensiveness. This is partially consistent with our prediction threat would
be related to perceptions of offensiveness and sexism and implies that jokes posing a social threat
may be deemed too offensive to be appropriate. Given people generally perceived jokes targeting
women as more threatening, this may explain why targeting historically-oppressed groups is less
acceptable than targeting historically-privileged groups. Humor disparaging historically-
oppressed groups, such as women, is seen as more threatening and therefore less acceptable.
7 General Discussion
Across two studies, we examined men’s and women’s reactions to sexist humor which
included both gender-unique stereotypic and gender-interchangeable jokes. We hypothesized that
women would generally perceive disparaging sexist humor as less funny, more sexist, more
offensive, and more threatening than would men. Additionally, we predicted these effects would
be stronger for sexist humor targeting women, regardless of whether it was stereotype-specific or
interchangeable. In Study 1 we showed that women perceived sexist humor targeting both men
and women as less funny, more offensive, and more sexist than men did. These effects held for
both gender-unique stereotypically-based sexist humor and gender-interchangeable sexist humor.
More importantly, our results showed that women find sexist humor targeting women to be less
funny, more offensive, and more sexist than men find sexist humor targeting men. This could be
17
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
because women are more socially threatened by sexism than are men because of their
traditionally lower social status.
In Study 2 we attempted to replicate the previous findings by examining whether
participant gender, gender targeted by the sexist joke, and their interaction predicted differences
in perceptions of sexist jokes as funny, offensive, and sexist. We then extended these findings by
examining participants’ perceptions of the jokes as threatening to the gender targeted. Study 2
results, surprisingly, showed that women generally perceived sexist humor to be less threatening
than did men. However, sexist jokes targeting women were generally perceived to be more
threatening than sexist jokes targeting men. Higher perceptions of threat were associated with
significantly greater perceptions of jokes as offensive and sexist. Given that people generally
perceived jokes targeting women to be more threatening, this may explain why targeting
historically-oppressed groups is less acceptable than targeting historically-privileged groups.
Humor disparaging historically-oppressed groups, such as women, is seen as more threatening
and therefore potentially less acceptable.
Interestingly, our findings were not fully consistent between Studies 1 and 2. In Study 1,
women consistently evaluated the humor as less funny, more offensive, and more sexist than men
did. However, in Study 2, this relationship emerged only for how funny participants perceived
the jokes. Instead, men found the jokes to be more threatening than did women and men and
women did not differ on how sexist or offensive they perceived the jokes to be. This finding is
interesting and we wonder if these differences might be attributable to the age of the sample
being tested. Study 1 examined a sample of undergraduate students with an average age of 19,
while Study 2 examined a community sample of participants with an average age of 35. It is
possible that, due to generational differences, older men are more sensitive to threats to their
18
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
privileged place in the status hierarchy than younger men are because older men may have had
more experience being privileged and may feel that recent progress in gender equality is
jeopardizing that privilege. Admittedly, this is speculation based on the critical evaluation of two
studies, but future research should examine the possibility for generational differences in men’s
and women’s responses to sexism, with older generations potentially embracing traditional
gender roles more than younger generations and being more sensitive to threats to their position
in the status hierarchy.
7.1 Limitations and Future Direction
The current study findings have some notable limitations, but these inspire future avenues
of research which are important to better understand the role of threat in men’s and women’s
responses to sexist jokes. These were conducted using students from a general psychology
subject pool as well as Amazon’s TurkPrime software, and may not be representative of the
general population. Additionally, it is possible that simply asking participants to rate how sexist a
joke is may trigger an emotional or psychological response because the participant may feel
primed to think of the joke as sexist. In the future, more covert measures of perceived sexism
should be used, and the possibility of simple asking participants about perceived threat,
offensiveness, and humorousness without mentioning sexism should be explored.
Furthermore, it is possible that some of the jokes used in the stereotype-based sets may
not have been based on stereotypes per se, but were instead simply jokes that were not
interchangeable for another reason. For example, in the group of jokes targeting men, the joke
Why did God make Adam before Eve? Everyone needs a rough draft before they make the final
copy” is not based on a stereotype about men, but cannot be interchangeable because the Biblical
creation story involves Adam (the “rough draft”) being made first. In the future, research should
explore the effects of jokes more clearly linked to gender stereotypes rather than jokes that are
not interchangeable for other reasons.
19
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
Because the studies used written jokes with no social context, there was a lack of ability
to show rapport, facial expressions, tone of voice, and setting that may be present when someone
tells a sexist joke. Although sexist jokes are increasingly perceived as inappropriate in modern
society, especially with the recent #metoo movement, these forms of attempted humor continue
to be used toward both men and women (e.g., Ford et al. 2013; Ford et al. 2015). Some may
claim these forms of humor are less offensive and sexist and more funny when used by a close
friend in private, compared to being used by a boss in the workplace, and/or these forms of
humor may be reappropriated by the groups they were originally meant to target as a means of
affiliation and subversion rather than derogation (see Bianchi 2014; Rappoport 2005; Saucier,
O’Dea, and Strain 2016; Saucier, Strain, Miller, O’Dea, and Till 2018; Spotorno and Bianchi
2015). Instead, men and women may adopt the use of sexist humor targeting their own group as a
way to inoculate themselves or their group from the sting of the jokes when used by outgroup
members (see theoretical discussion by Rappoport 2005).
Therefore, future research should provide context when presenting participants with
sexist jokes. While it is likely the current findings would not generalize to situations in which
ingroup members use sexist jokes typically targeting their own group affiliatively and as a means
of inoculation from prejudice or discrimination, providing context that enhances the derogative
nature of the sexist jokes may enhance the findings of the current studies. For example, if
participants are given a news story or other form of short story that describes a male manager at a
company using a sexist joke toward a woman who has previously filed a sexual misconduct
claim against him, individuals would likely attribute greater threat to the situation and perceive
the joke as less funny, more offensive, and more sexist than when humor is used in
reappropriated ways. Thus, providing extra context, facial expressions, tones of voice, or details
about setting may extend the findings of the current studies. It is reasonable to expect the results
20
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
of the current study may not generalize situations of reappropriation, especially if there is no
perceived threat in such a situation. That said, the results may be stronger if individuals are
presented with context in which the sexist jokes are used in disparaging ways.
The cross-sectional nature of the current studies also limits our abilities to test the role of
threat in predicting participants’ responses to the sexist humor. Future studies on this topic should
manipulate threat and examine whether this manipulation impacts individuals’ perceptions of,
and responses to, sexist jokes. Building on previous research (e.g., Tajfel and Turner 1986; Ford
et al. 2015), it is likely that men and women may experience different types of threat when
confronted by sexist humor based on their position in the existing status hierarchy. Recent
research has begun to examine the difference in perceptions of harm and offensiveness of hate
speech directed at higher status versus lower status group members (e.g., O’Dea and Saucier,
under review b). It is likely that, while men’s and women’s responses to sexist humor are
inextricably rooted in their experiences of threat, they would have different responses to the
threat. Each of these responses theoretically lead to similar outcomes.
On one hand, men who experience threat from sexist humor may seek to engage in
behaviors that bolster their position in the status hierarchy (e.g., Ford et al. 2008; Romero-
Sanchez et al. 2010). These behaviors may include men becoming more polarized in their
attitudes toward women and perceiving enhanced justification for prejudice toward women.
Thus, sexist humor targeting men may ironically reinforce prejudice and discrimination toward
women. On the other hand, women may perceive sexist humor as a sword being wielded against
them to keep them from advancing in the status hierarchy. Discrimination against lower status
groups is perceived as more harmful and offensive and has longer-lasting impacts (Schneider,
Hitlan, and Radhakrishnan 2000), and because of this women may experience heightened
negative emotions following being targeted by sexist humor (Brandt and Henry 2012; Kremin
21
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
2017; Mendoza-Denton, Downey, Purdie, Davis, and Pietrzak 2002; Schneider, Hitlan, and
Radhakrishnan 2002). This may call to mind negative stereotypes about their group (Blakemore
2015; Jeshion 2013; Merskin 2010). Literature on stereotype threat suggests that when negative
stereotypes about groups are made salient, such as those made by sexist jokes, individuals are
more likely to perform poorly on complex tasks due to greater experiences of stress (e.g., Steele
and Aronson 1995). Thus, if women experience sexist humor in the workplace, this could inhibit
their performance on complex work-related tasks, reinforcing negative perceptions from others
in the workplace. This is consistent with findings from Greenberg and Pyszcynski (1985)
showing that overheard derogatory remarks can have lasting negative perceptions for targets of
discrimination in the workplace from bystanders. Admittedly, at the present time, these
hypotheses are based on the findings of two studies and recent research on intergroup relations.
Thus, future research should further examine the effect of threat on men’s and women’s
responses to sexist humor.
8 Conclusion
Across two studies we examined whether men and women differ in the extent to which
they perceive disparaging humor targeting someone based on their gender as funny, offensive,
sexist, and threatening. We predicted that women would perceive disparaging humor as more
negative than would men generally, and that women would perceive disparaging humor targeting
women to be more negative than men would perceive disparaging humor targeting men. Our
results generally supported these hypotheses. Specifically, across both studies, humor targeting
women was rated more negatively than was humor targeting men. Interestingly, in Study 2, men
generally rated sexist humor as more threatening than did women. Furthermore, sexist humor
targeting women was rated by both men and women as being more threatening to women than
sexist humor targeting men was rated as being threatening to men. This may explain why
22
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
disparaging historically-oppressed groups is generally less acceptable than targeting historically-
privileged groups. Humor disparaging historically-oppressed groups, such as women, is seen as
more socially threatening and therefore potentially less acceptable. Therefore, humor that
“punches up” at historically-privileged groups, such as men or White people, is more socially
acceptable and may be found funnier than humor that “punches down” at historically-oppressed
groups, such as women or people of color. Jokes that target genders may have unequal
consequences that mirror the traditional inequalities reinforced by the traditional gender
hierarchy.
References
Abrams, J. R., & Bippus, A. M. (2014). Gendering jokes: Intergroup bias in reactions to same-
versus opposite-gender humor. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 33(6), 692-
702.
Abrams, J. R., Bippus, A. M., & McGaughey, K. J. (2015). Gender disparaging jokes: An
investigation of sexist-nonstereotypical jokes on funniness, typicality, and the moderating
role of ingroup identification. Humor, 28(2), 311-326.
Bemiller, Michelle L., & Schneider, Rachel Z. (2010). It's not just a joke. Sociological Spectrum,
30(4), 459-479.
23
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
Bianchi, Claudia (2014). Slurs and appropriation: An echoic account. Journal of Pragmatics, 66,
35-44.
Blakemore, Diane (2014). Slurs and expletives: A case against a general account of expressive
meaning. Language Sciences, 52, 22-35.
Brandt, Mark J., & Henry, P. J. (2012). Psychological defensiveness as a mechanism explaining
the relationship between low socioeconomic status and religiosity. The International
Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 23, 321-332.
Cantor, J. R. (1976). Humor on television: A content analysis. Journal of Broadcasting &
Electronic Media, 20(4), 501-510.
Ford, Thomas E. (1997). Effects of stereotypical television portrayals of african-americans on
person perception. Social Psychology Quarterly, 60, 266-278.
Ford, Thomas E. (2000). Effects of sexist humor on tolerance of sexist events. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1094–1107.
Ford, Thomas E., Boxer, Christie F., Armstrong, Jacob, & Edel, Jessica R. (2008). More than
"just a joke": The prejudice-releasing function of sexist humor. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 34, 159-170.
Ford, Thomas E., & Ferguson, Mark A. (2004). Social consequences of disparagement humor: A
prejudiced norm theory. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 79-94.
Ford, Thomas E., Wentzel, Erin R., & Lorion, Joli (2001). Effects of exposure to sexist humor on
perceptions of normative tolerance of sexism. European Journal of Social Psychology,
31, 677–691.
Ford, Thomas E., Woodzicka, Julie A., Petit, Whitney E., Richardson, Kyle, & Lappi, Shaun K.
(2015). Sexist humor as a trigger of state self-objectification in women. Humor, 28(2),
253-269.
Ford, Thomas E., Woodzicka, Julie A., Triplett, Shane R., & Kochersberger, Annie O. (2013).
Sexist humor and beliefs that justify societal sexism. Current Research in Social
Psychology, 64-81.
24
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
Greenberg, Jeff J. & Pyszczynski, Tom (1985). The effect of an overheard ethnic slur on
evaluations of the target: How to spread a social disease. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 21(1), 61-72.
Greenwood, Dara, & Isbell, Linda M. (2002). Ambivalent sexism and the dumb blonde: Men's
and women's reactions to sexist jokes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26(4), 341-350.
Henry, P. J., Butler, Sarah E., & Brandt, Mark J. (2014). The influence of target group status on
the perception of the offensiveness of group-based slurs. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 53, 185-192.
Janes, Leslie M., & Olson, James M. (2000). Jeer Pressure: The Behavioral Effects of Observing
Ridicule of Others. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 264(4), 474-485.
Jeshion, Robin (2013). Slurs and stereotypes. Analytic Philosophy, 54(3), 314-329.
Kremin, Lena V. L. (2017). Sexist swearing and slurs. LingUU, 1(1), 18-25.
Losco, J., & Epstein, S. (1975). Humor preference as a subtle measure of attitudes toward the
same and the opposite sex. Journal of Personality.
McGhee, P. E., & Duffey, N. S. (1983). Children's appreciation of humor victimizing different
racial-ethnic groups: Racial-ethnic differences. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 14(1), 29-40.
McGraw, A. P. & Warren, Caleb (2010). Benign violations: Making immoral behavior funny.
Psychological Science, 21(8), 1141-1149.
Mendoza-Denton, Rodolfo, Downey, Geraldine, Purdie, Valerie J., Davis, Angelina, & Pietrzak,
Janina (2002). Sensitivity to status-based rejection: Implications for African American
students’ college experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(4), 896-
918.
Merskin, Debra (2010). The s-word: Discourse, stereotypes, and the American Indian woman.
The Howard Journal of Communications, 21, 345-366.
Meza, Emilia F., Lawless, Tiffany J., & Saucier, Donald A. (in preparation). Stereotyped
starterpacks: Intersectionality of gender and race in internet humor.
25
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
O'Dea, Conor J., Miller, Stuart S., Andres, Emma B., Ray, Madelyn H., Till, Derek F., & Saucier,
Donald A. (2015). Out of bounds: Factors affecting the perceived offensiveness of racial
slurs. Language Sciences, 52, 155-164.
O’Dea, Conor J., & Saucier, Donald A. (under review a). Perceptions of racial slurs used by
Blacks toward Whites: Derogation or affiliation? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology.
O’Dea, Conor J., & Saucier, Donald A. (Under review b). Sticks and stones: Racial prejudice and
perceptions of racial bigotry versus physical aggression. Group Processes and Intergroup
Relations.
O’Dea, Conor J., & Saucier, Donald A. (In preparation). Swords and shields: A theoretical
review on the functions, perceptions, and multidimensionality of racial slurs
Rappoport, Leon (2005). Punchlines: The Case for Racial, Ethnic, and Gender Humor.
Connecticut: Praeger Publishers.
Romero-Sánchez, Monica, Durán, Mercedes, Carretero-Dios, Hugo, Megías, Jesús L., & Moya,
Miguel (2010). Exposure to sexist humor and rape proclivity: the moderator effect of
aversiveness ratings. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 25, 2339-2050.
Ryan, Kathryn M. & Kanjorski, Jeanne (1998). The enjoyment of sexist humor, rape attitudes,
and relationship aggression in college students. Sex Roles, 38, 743-756.
Saucier, Donald A., O’Dea, Conor J., & Strain, Megan L. (2016). The bad, the good, the
misunderstood: The social effects of racial humor. Translational Issues in Psychological
Sciences. 2(1), 75-85.
Saucier, Donald A., Strain, Megan L., Miller, Stuart S. O’Dea, Conor J., & Till, Derek F. (2018).
“What do you call a Black guy who flies a plane?” Disparagement, confrontation, and
failed subversion in the context of racial humor. Humor, 31(1), 105-128.
Schneider, Kimberly T., Hitlan, Robert T., & Radhakrishnan, Phanikiran (2002). An examination
of the nature and correlates of ethnic harassment experiences in multiple contexts.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(1), 3-12.
26
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
Spotorno, Nicola, & Bianchi, Claudia (2015). A plea for an experimental approach to slurs.
Language Sciences, 52, 241-250.
Strain, Megan L., Martens, Amanda L., & Saucier, Donald A. (2016) “Rape is the new black:”
Humor’s potential for reinforcing and subverting rape culture. Translational Issues in
Psychological Science, 2(1), 86-95.
Strain, Megan L., Saucier, Donald A., & Martens, Amanda L. (2015). Sexist humor in Facebook
profiles: Perceptions of humor targeting women and men. Humor, 28(1), 119-141.
Tajfel, Henri, & Turner, John C. (1986) The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In W.
G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), Psychology of Intergroup Relations (pp. 7-24). Chicago,
Nelson-Hall.
Thomae, Manuela, & Pina, Afroditi (2015). Sexist humor and social identity: The role of sexist
humor in men’s in-group cohesion, sexual harassment, rape proclivity, and victim blame.
Humor – International Journal of Humor Research, 28(2). 187-204.
Thomae, Manuela, & Viki, G. T. (2013). Why did the woman cross the road? The effect of sexist
humor on men's rape proclivity. Journal of Social, Evolutionary and Cultural
Psychology, 7(3), 250.
Woodzicka, Julie A., Mallett, Robyn K, Hendricks, Shelbi, & Pruitt, Astrid V. (2015). It’s just a
(sexist) joke: Comparing reactions to sexist versus racist communications. Humor, 28(2),
289-309.
27
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
Table 2. Bivariate Correlations Between Perceptions of Jokes (Study 1).
Stereotype-Based
Target: Men 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8. 9. 10. 11. 12.
1. Funny (.92)
2. Offensive -.33*(.97)
3. Sexist -.28*.88*** (.96)
Stereotype-Based
Target: Women
4. Funny .56*** -.32** -.36** (.93)
5. Offensive -.28** .60*** .66*** -.38*** (.96)
6. Sexist -.17 .55*** .66*** -.26*.83*** (.96)
Interchangeable
Target: Men
7. Funny -.02 -.12 -.22 .81*** -.17 -.18 (.94)
8. Offensive -.03 .35*.32 -.32 .89*** .71*** -.32 (.96)
9. Sexist .11 .20 .26 -.06 .66*** .91*** -.20 .73*** (.95)
Interchangeable
Target: Men
10. Funny .67*** -.56*** -.58*** .66*** -.68*** -.62*** -- -- -- (.91)
11. Offensive -.38** .78*** .72*** -.43*** .80*** .70*** -- -- -- -.48*** (.93)
12. Sexist -.25 .63*** .75*** -.27 .71*** .80*** -- -- -- -.46*** .79*** (.94)
Note. Cronbach’s alphas appear in parentheses in the diagonal.
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
Table 3. The Effects of Participant and Target Gender in Perceptions of Sexist Humor (Study 1).
Dependent Measure Participant Gender Target Gender Participant X Target
Note. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
Table 4. The Effects of Participant and Target Gender in Perceptions of Sexist Humor (Study 2).
Dependent
Note. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
Table 5. Bivariate Correlations Between Perceptions of Jokes (Study 2).
Note. Cronbach’s alphas appear in parentheses in the diagonal.
*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
Target: Men Target: Women
Figure 1. Perceptions of Stereotype-Based Jokes (Study 1)
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
Target: Men Target: Women
Figure 2. Perceptions of Gender-Interchangeable Jokes (Study 1)
GENDER DIFFERENCES AND SEXIST HUMOR
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
9
Target: Men Target: Women
Figure 3. Perceptions of Gender-Interchangeable Jokes (Study 2)
... Subtle discrimination, in turn, has substantial negative impacts on individuals, organisations, and society; and evidence suggests that subtle discrimination can be even more detrimental than overt discrimination (Jones et al., 2016). This is the case because subtle discrimination is comparably harder to recognise and confront (Mallett et al., 2016), which ultimately reinforces a discriminatory climate in organisations as it makes discrimination more tolerable (Ford & Ferguson, 2004;Lawless et al., 2020). ...
... It is even more detrimental when sexist humour is expressed by men high in hostile sexism because they articulate their sexist beliefs more . This, in turn, fosters a climate in which discrimination is accepted (Lawless et al., 2020). Although often disguised as a "joke", sexist humour differs from other forms of workplace humour. ...
... Only a few empirical studies (e.g., Huo et al., 2012;Pundt & Herrmann, 2015), examined negative or negatively perceived workplace humour; sexist humour in the workplace is particularly under-researched. Hence, despite some recent scholarly attention on sexist humour in everyday life (e.g., Lawless et al., 2020;Woodzicka et al., 2020), the understanding of its impact in the workplace remains limited. Specifically, there is a lack of knowledge regarding the interpersonal work relationship outcomes of sexist workplace humour, the respective underlying processes, and the conditions under which these effects occur. ...
Article
Full-text available
(Workplace) humour can have positive effects on interpersonal (workplace) relationships. However, sexist humour – a form of subtle discrimination that communicates disparagement or degradation at the expense of (mainly) women in a humorous manner – might be highly detrimental in various ways. Despite sexist humour being pervasive in the workplace, little is known about when, how, and why sexist humour impacts the recipients and their work relationship with the initiator. With two pre-registered studies, theoretically based on affective events theory and social identity theory, this research advances knowledge on the interpersonal effects and gender dynamics of sexist humour at work. In the experimental Study 1, 255 participants rated an actor telling a (non-)sexist joke. In the field Study 2, 170 participants recalled a (non-)sexist humour event they encountered at work. Both studies produced converging results: Recipients of sexist humour were less willing to collaborate with the humour initiator and perceived them as less competent; this was psychologically explained by elevated negative affect. These detrimental effects particularly emerged when a man (vs. woman) initiated sexist humour toward a woman (vs. man).
... According to recent research, encountering disparagement humor directed at groups with an ambiguous social standing can release prejudices against them, whereas this effect was not observed when groups were targeted for whom the audience considers prejudice, or at least criticism, justified (Ford et al., 2014;Mendiburo-Seguel and Ford, 2019). Furthermore, it appears that audiences perceive it more negatively and particularly offensive when lowstatus groups are targeted rather than high-status groups (Lawless et al., 2020), as well as when they themselves belong to the target group and have previously encountered comparable aggressions (Williams et al., 2016). Similarly, perceptions of hate speech and its hostility differ among individuals and are influenced by factors such as content and context (Schmid et al., 2022). ...
... First, it was anticipated that participants would be less likely to classify sexist memes containing humorous hate speech as being hostile than ones containing non-humorous sexist hate speech (H1a) and that they need more time for deciding on humorous compared to non-humorous hate speech (H3a). Evidence from studies on the perception of sexists jokes (Lawless et al., 2020) and the belonging to the group targeted (Williams et al., 2016) suggested that women would classify sexist memes containing either humorous or non-humorous hate speech as being hostile more frequently than men would (H1b) and be faster in doing so (H3b). Beyond that, it was examined whether gender moderates the classification of memes as being hostile (RQ1) and the response time required (RQ2). ...
... The initial corpus was collected and coded by two women and one man using a predefined codebook (included in Study 2's pre-registration) with the same criteria as in Study 1. In addition, jokes that had been employed in previous studies (Drakett et al., 2018;Lawless et al., 2020) were included in the selection process. The final 48 memes were coded identically by at least two coders and had a comparable word count. ...
Article
Humor that denigrates social groups can be just as harmful as hate speech. Despite research indicating the prevalence of humorous hate speech, how audiences perceive and process the combination of humor (e.g. irony as a humor cue) and hate speech (e.g. dehumanization as a hate cue) remains unclear. Using a sequential mixed-methods approach combining a qualitative think-aloud study (Study 1, N = 41) with an experiment involving implicit measurements of response times (Study 2, N = 65), it was examined how individuals perceive memes that contain both humor and hate cues. While think-aloud interviews indicated that processing humorous hate speech may require multiple steps, the relative time spent by participants in Study 2 to rate humorous and non-humorous hate speech as being hostile or not did not entirely support that conclusion. However, findings imply that hostile views may become more commonplace when hate speech is masked by humor.
... Furthermore, how aggressive humor is perceived may also depend on the characteristics (e.g., gender, social status) of the victims. Prior research has shown that both men and women perceive female-targeted hostile jokes as more offensive than male-targeted hostile jokes (Abel & Flick, 2012), and disparagement humor targeting high-status groups is viewed more favorably than disparagement humor targeting low-status groups (Lawless et al., 2020). Future research is recommended to further investigate how the characteristics of aggressors and victims may interact to influence social relationship outcomes. ...
... Fourth, the current study relied on a self-reported measure of aggressive humor without examining the participants' interactions with the victims. The consequences of using aggressive humor may be contingent on the characteristics (e.g., gender, social status) of the victims (Abel & Flick, 2012;Lawless et al., 2020). For instance, aggressive humor targeting women is generally considered more offensive than aggressive humor targeting men (Abel & Flick, 2012). ...
... Consequently, factors that could affect the perception of humour here are one's familiarity with a patriarchal society and culture (as it can be imagined that such assumptions about male arrogance are not shared by members of a matriarchal culture). Additionally, due to the joke targeting men, there is the possibility for some men to be offended at the assumptions that it is making, and it is known that gender generally affects the perception of certain types of jokes such as these (Lawless et al., 2020). Furthermore, a large body of work has investigated the different tendencies of particular genders towards different patterns of language use, which in some cases may also affect the perception of different language types (Coates, 2016;Rabinovich et al., 2020). ...
... al., 2019). Overall, research indicates that women feel more socially threatened by sexist humor than men, even when comparing with jokes in which both men and women were the targets (Lawless et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has stated a relationship between sexist ideologies and humour appreciation. However, most research has been done in North America and Europe. In the present study, we aimed to approach in an exploratory way to the social perceptions of sexist humour in Costa Rica. Data was gathered through an online survey, participants (N = 323; 220 females) completed measures to characterize expressions of sexist humour. The main measures included perceived funniness, frequency and means of exposure, and perceived offensiveness, as well as completing the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory (ASI). Overall, previous exposure to this kind of humour was remarkably high. No significant differences were observed between women's and men's perceptions of the frequency, means and places of exposure, but emotional and behavioral reactions to sexist humour were gender-based. Women reported higher displeasure, more confrontation and felt angrier and more offended than men when exposed to this type of humour. Data support existing evidence of the relationship with sexist ideology. Specifically, hostile sexism was found to be a significant predictor of perceived funniness and offensiveness of the sexist jokes. An interaction effect between hostile sexism and the sex of participants was found, showing that as hostile sexism increased, perceived offensiveness decreased, but more rapidly for men. Results are discussed considering the practical implications and limitations, as well as the need for more research in the Latin American context.
Article
Full-text available
The article presents the results of a socio-philosophical study of the Ukrainian women‘s humor, which has embodied by the jokes of Telegram channels. A review of the theoretical heritage of researchers of women's humor and humor in conditions of war. It‘s substantiated that the study of female humor during the russian full-scale invasion is relevant for Ukraine. In particular, a query in the GPT 4.0 chat has been used to justify the relevance. It‘s also noted that the study of humor during the war goes beyond the analysis of theoretical provisions to the solution of applied problems. The humorous Telegram channels were selected for the research according to a specified number of criteria. For that selection, the ratings of Telegram channels were used, taking into account the number of subscribers and coverage. To relate the channel to the presentation of Ukrainian humor, it had to be Ukrainian-language and had checked for the absence of calque/fakes, which indicated by the Center for Countering Disinformation under the National Security and Defense Council of Ukraine. Russian-language humorous Telegram channels were viewed and used for comparison, but their names are not indicated in the present article. An intersectional socio-philosophical analysis was also conducted, suggesting the intersection of the following vectors: gender, national identity, civil position and social status. The scientific investigation confirmed the gendered humorous practices in the four areas previously highlighted by H. Kotthof. At the same time, a number of features of modern Ukrainian female humor are also noted. It has been demonstrated in the present article, that Ukrainian-speaking female humorous channels could be considered as prevention and counteraction to sexism and male domination. It has been also noted that the comic misandry of Ukrainian women cannot be considered as sexism, with the exception of hatred of men from among the Russian invaders. The vast majority of jokes in the female humorous Telegram channels correspond in their characteristics to the signs of feminist humor outlined in T. Hraban's study. There are also a number of jokes based on the traditional paradigm in intergender interaction. It's also has been shown that viewing Ukrainian humorous Telegram channels may improve the adaptation processes and facilitate women's response to a stressful situation. It is noted that humorous channels, returning women to peaceful topics, create an environment of peace and overcoming anxiety and can become a platform for fraudsters, information warfare and psychological operations.
Article
Full-text available
Two preregistered studies (N = 679) tested the links between women’s gender identity and their amusement with sexist jokes targeting men. We hypothesized that the interaction between the strength (i.e., level of in-group identification) and contents (traditional/progressive) of women’s gender identity would be associated with their amusement with jokes targeting men and that this relationship would be mediated by sexism toward men. Although Study 1 provided no support for this hypothesis, we found positive associations between women’s traditional identity contents and their amusement with benevolent and hostile jokes about men, explained by the higher endorsement of benevolent (but not hostile) sexism toward men. Study 2 replicated this effect, suggesting that women’s enjoyment of jokes targeting men might stem from their beliefs that justify the existing power (im)balance between sexes and not their hostility toward men.
Article
Disparagement humor may harm perceptions of joke-tellers’ core traits and trustworthiness differently when it “punches down” rather than “up” at its target. This was experimentally tested with a sexist joke in Study 1 ( n = 161) and a boss/employee joke in Study 2 ( n = 331). Consistently, joke-tellers who punched down seemed less competent, lower in status, and less trustworthy (assessed via a hypothetical monetary trust game). Jokes that punched “down” (versus “up”) seemed less affiliative in general, and particularly aggressive only in a sexist humor context, perhaps due to more normative expectations of aggression in gender-based contexts. Implications for curbing disparagement humor, particularly from high-status people, are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Racial humor has long been a part of American culture, but its potential for varied interpretation leads to a wide range of possible effects, which have only recently become an area of investigation in psychological research. The literature on racial and disparaging humor, and particularly its conceptualization according to the “sword and shield metaphor” (Rappoport, 2005), points to 3 possibilities that vary in terms of intention and perception and have the potential to affect the outcomes associated with racial humor. When racial humor is antisocial in intention (i.e., used as a sword) and perceived as such, it may reinforce the social hierarchy and stereotypes about the individuals it targets, potentially loosening societal norms that discourage expressions of prejudice. When racial humor is prosocial in intention (i.e., used as a shield) and perceived as such, it may serve to challenge and protect against prejudice and create affiliation between members of groups who may be affected by social inequality. Finally, some prosocially motivated ethnic humor may be misperceived as antisocial, presenting the possibility of unintentionally (and ironically) reinforcing the status quo rather than subverting it. Despite this wide range of potential outcomes, we maintain that humor’s inherently social and ambiguous nature presents a hopeful opportunity for the discussion and possible reduction of prejudice—as long as the humor itself is embedded in a discussion that raises awareness of the issues it addresses. Equally important is the need for those who use such humor to understand its potential to be bad, good, or misunderstood.
Article
Full-text available
In recent years there has been an increase in the use of rape humor in American popular culture (e.g., in stand-up comedy, on TV sitcoms). We consider the potential for such humor that references rape to reinforce or subvert rape culture. Reinforcing rape humor serves to normalize rape, thereby reinforcing rape culture, while subversive humor serves to challenge rape culture. We present evidence referencing the humor literature, as well as historic and current events, to support our argument that rape humor may serve either or both of these functions. Examining these 2 types of humor provides insight into an issue that has become increasingly important in the United States, and may also inform about the potential role of humor in contributing to the national discussion on the prevalence of rape. We conclude by discussing the challenges associated with using rape humor as well as demonstrating applications of subversive humor as a weapon against rape culture.
Article
Full-text available
An experiment was conducted to test the hypothesis that stereotypical television portrayals of African-Americans increase the likelihood that whites will make negative social perception judgments of an African-American (but not a white) target person. Forty white subjects were exposed to comedy skits featuring stereotypical or netural portrayals of African-American characters. Subjects then read a vignette describing an incident in which a college student was allegedly assaulted by his rommate. In half of the conditions, the alleged offender was assumed to be white; in the other half he was assumed to be African-American. Subjects rated the likelihood that the alleged offender was guilty of the assault. Guilt ratings of the white target did not differ significantly between the stereotypical and the neutral comedy skit conditions. In contrast, guilt ratings of the African-American target were higher in the stereotypical comedy skit condition than in the neutral comedy skit condition.
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has shown that exposure to sexist (vs. non-sexist) humor results in more tolerance of sexist discrimination (Ford & Fergusson, 2004). In the current research, three studies investigated the effects of exposure to sexist humor on men’s rape proclivity. In Study 1, male students were exposed to either sexist or non-sexist jokes. Males exposed to sexist jokes reported higher levels of rape proclivity in comparison to males exposed to non-sexist jokes. Study 2 was an online study in which we replicated Study 1, but also measured male participants’ levels of hostile and benevolent sexism. Study 3 was a replication of Study 2, in which we controlled for the sexual content of the jokes. Overall, the results of Study 2 and Study 3 indicated that men who scored high (vs. low) on hostile sexism reported higher levels of rape proclivity after exposure to sexist versus non-sexist jokes. No such effects were obtained for benevolent sexism. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2014 APA, all rights reserved)
Book
The concept of ethnic, racial, and gender humor is as sensitive a subject today as it has ever been; yet at no time in the past have we had such a quantity of this humor circulating throughout society. We can see the power of such content manifested continually in our culture's films and stand-up comedy routines, as well as on popular TV sitcoms, where Jewish, black, Asian, Hispanic, and gay characters and topics have seemingly become essential to comic scenarios. Though such humor is often cruel, it can be a source of pride and play among minorities, women, and gays. Leon Rappoport's incisive account takes an in-depth look at ethnic, racial and gender humor. Despite the polarization that is often apparent in the debates such humor evokes, the most important melting pot in this country may be the one that we enter when we share a laugh at ourselves.
Article
We conducted three studies to test our overarching hypothesis that racial humor may increase or decrease subsequent expressions of prejudice by setting social norms that indicate prejudice is either more or less acceptable, respectively. We selected riddles that were disparaging, confrontational, or neutral, and examined their effects on subsequent prejudiced expressions. We predicted humor that disparaged Blacks would convey that prejudiced expressions are more socially acceptable, resulting in increased expressions of prejudice toward Blacks. Conversely, we predicted humor that confronted prejudiced expressions would convey that prejudiced expressions are less socially acceptable, resulting instead in reduced expressions of prejudice toward Blacks. Our studies demonstrated that, consistent with prejudiced norm theory, disparagement humor, and confrontational humor perceived as disparaging, has the potential to disinhibit expressions of prejudice when used, even in brief social interactions. Our studies also showed that individuals often misinterpreted the subversive nature of confrontational humor, frequently perceiving the confrontation intended to challenge expressions of prejudice as instead intending to disparage Blacks. Thus, while it is possible racial humor may have the potential to tighten norms inhibiting prejudice, the perceptions of confrontational jokes as disparaging may result in jokes (created to subvert and inhibit prejudice) ironically reinforcing prejudiced responding.
Article
The aim of our paper is to provide the reader with a sort of vademecum on the possibilities and the limits of an experimental approach to the study of slurs and derogatory language. We distinguish between off-line and on-line studies and underline the advantages and constraints of both methodologies. Empirical studies have already contributed to the investigation of slurs, at least as far as off-line experiments are concerned: we argue that on-line techniques might also provide interesting insights, but only to the extent to which one can derive predictions about the processing of slurs from the theories under investigation. We provide the example of two theoretical debates in which an on-line approach may prove useful in assessing various hypotheses – namely the content-based/non content-based dispute and the echoic approach to slurs. In closing we suggest an alternative domain in which experimental research and theoretical investigation on slurs might fruitfully interact: cognitive and affective neuroscience, and more particularly the investigation of how our cognitive system handles negative stimuli. Slurs may be seen as a prototype of aggressive behavior concentrated in a few words: therefore they are well suited for testing the reactions of our brain and peripheral nervous system to verbal aggression.
Article
Based on social identity theory, this study tests whether individuals demonstrate out-group rejection by rating jokes targeting the opposite gender funnier and more gender typical than jokes targeting their gender. Prior research is extended by examining individuals’ level of gender identification and propensity to discriminate against a hypothetical man or woman after exposure to sexist jokes. Results indicated that women exhibited in-group rejection, whereas men showed bias toward women only in the 1-joke (vs. 5- and 10-joke) condition. Men and women with low gender identification rated jokes about their gender funnier and more gender typical than those about the opposite gender, and highly identified women also rated jokes about women as gender typical. The findings did not support the basic assumptions of social identity theory; however, the theory remained useful in interpreting the results, which undoubtedly reflect the intergroup dynamics inherent in gender relations.
Article
This paper argues against the case for treating slurs alongside expletives in a general account of expressive meaning (cf Hedger, 2102, 13; Kaplan, 1997; Richard, 2008). Working within a relevance theoretic account of communication (Sperber and Wilson, 1986/95), it argues that expletives (e.g. damn) have no descriptive content and pattern with smiles, gestures and tone of voice which are used to trigger procedures for the identification of emotional states. In contrast, slurs have descriptive content – content which provides a means of identifying the group of individuals they are used to target. However, (contra Hom, 2008) the offensive attitude a slur communicates is not part of its encoded content, but is derived from the meta-linguistic knowledge that the word is an offensive means of predicating and referring. This knowledge raises an expectation of relevance which is satisfied only if the hearer attributes the hearer with an indeterminate range of assumptions from the cultural stereotype which his use of the word evokes.