Content uploaded by Elina Lampert-Shepel
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Elina Lampert-Shepel on Jun 22, 2019
Content may be subject to copyright.
Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology
Volume 17, Number 3, 2018
Learning to Reect: Teachers’ Mastery and
Development of Mediational Means and
Psychological Tools of Reective Practice
Elina Lampert-Shepel
Graduate School of Education, Touro College, New York, New York
Colette Murphy
School of Education, Trinity College, Dublin, Ireland
Authors
ID:p0070
conceptualize the notion of reection as a higher psychological function from
the perspective of cultural–historical psychology of Lev Vygotsky and discuss the
development of teachers’ reective practice in the process of mastering and creating
mediational means of reective practice. In the current research literature the terms
mediational means and psychological tools are often used interchangeably. On the
grounds of the ndings from the studies of teachers’ reection, conducted in Ireland,
United States, and Russia, the authors distinguish mediational means from psycho-
logical tools, discuss their heterogeneity, and explore how the choice of mediational
means transforms the process of reective practice. Authors argue that teachers need
to be educated to master meditational means of reection to build their reective prac-
tice and develop reection as a higher psychological function.
Keywords: reective
ID:p0075
action; higher psychological function; mediational means;
psychological tools; coteaching
Increasingly
ID:p0080
, teachers and the knowledge they glean from daily work with children are
seen as the proper focus of professional development and reform. Teacher education, both
in-service and preservice, now incorporates educating teachers in critical examination of
their practice (National Board for Professional Teaching Standards, 2016; National Council for
Accreditation of Teacher Education, 2010), resolution of inconsistencies in their school culture,
self-reection, and building their own understandings into theories.
“Teacher-proof” scripted curricula, however, and related reforms centered on testing, now
often focus teacher reection on ecient technical implementation rather than on inquiry
and meaning-making. Most often, reection is thus deemed an isolated technical skill (Giroux
ID:p0085
,
1985; Homan-Kipp, Artiles, & Lopez-Torres, 2003; Lindsay & Mason, 2000). More than ever,
the craft of teaching requires the acceptance of ambiguity, and engagement in active meaning-
making. In the context of growing diversity, moral uncertainty, conicting intellectual demandsPdf_Folio:278
278 © 2018 International Association for Cognitive Education and Psychology
http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/1945-8959.17.3.278
Development of Mediational Means and Psychological Tools of Reective Practice 279
and views on teaching and learning, teachers’ reection as a mere ability to implement the
prepackaged curriculum is insucient to support meaningful educational practice. Also, for
teachers engaged in reective practice as self-emancipation and inquiry (Freire, 1997) it sup-
ports their ability to facilitate students’ classroom inquiry.
e
ID:p0090
cultural–historical psychology of Vygotsky both underpins meaning-making by concep-
tualizing reective action and practice, and inquiry into teaching and learning leading the devel-
opment of teachers themselves, and their students. Conceptualization of reective practice as a
tool-mediated activity for teachers to develop reection as a higher psychological function, per-
mits designing teacher education programs and activities leading to teachers’ development as
learners and researchers of their practice.
In
ID:p0095
this article, we aim to conceptualize the notion of reection from the perspective of
the cultural–historical psychology of Lev Vygotsky, to explore the mastery and development of
mediational means of teachers’ reective practice based on the ndings of two research stud-
ies conducted in Dublin, Ireland and New York, United States/ Moscow, Russia; document the
heterogeneity of mediational means of reective practice, and explore their potential for their
transformation into teacher’s psychological tools of reection.
While
ID:p0100
the studies were conducted in dierent socio-cultural contexts and cultures, their
ndings show similar processes in the development of teachers’ reection as a higher psycholog-
ical function. In the rst study, reective practice was explored amid teachers’ actions. Although
teacher’s reection was in the core of school cultures, teachers received no professional develop-
ment on reective practice or using various mediational means of reection in either Manhattan
Country School (MCS, New York) or School of Self-Determination (SSD, Moscow, Russia). In
the second study, teachers were educated in various mediational means of reection that were
reported to signicantly improve the reective process, and transform spontaneous reection
into reection as a higher psychological function.
REFLECTION
ID:T
I0015
AS A HIGHER PSYCHOLOGICAL FUNCTION
e
ID:p0105
notion of reection employed in the studies discussed in this article stems from the cultural–
historical psychology of Lev Vygotsky and the followers of his scholarship. Cultural–historical
psychology provides a theoretical approach to conceptualize reection in the context of teachers’
development as agents of their own practice that is viewed as their professional learning activ-
ity. e studies discussed in this article consider reection as a higher psychological function
(Vygotsky, 1982). Such reection develops in the course of dialectical, socially constructed, and
culturally mediated actions of meaning-making, through the continuous exploration of experi-
ence by the agent of the action.
Reection
ID:p0110
as a higher psychological function (Vygotsky, 1998) is socially constructed in
the course of culturally mediated human activities (Vygotsky, 1982). Reection is socially con-
structed, because it is rst developed in the form of shared cognition among the community of
learners and then transformed through the process of internalization into individual conscious-
ness (Vygotsky, 1982; Wertsch, 1985, 1998). Reective practice, like any other human activity,
is mediated by cultural tools (Vygotsky, 1982; Zinchenko, 1985).
Reection
ID:p0115
is a human ability of the agent of the action to be self-conscious. It is the ability
to regard oneself or one’s own action as the other, as the subject of purposeful change. It is
metacognitive since it requires thinking about thinking. us, reection is manifest through
our cognition and practice, developed in the course of specic sociocultural interactions, and
inuenced by our attitudes and moral values.
Pdf_Folio:279
280 Lampert-Shepel and Murphy
is
ID:p0120
view of reection as a higher psychological function, helps teacher educators and
researchers to theorize reective practice as a learning activity leading toward teachers’ devel-
opment as agents of their own practice. Several foundational ideas of Lev Vygotsky’s theory of
development of higher psychological functions ground our conceptualizations of reection as a
higher psychological function.
Lower and Higher Psychological Functions
e
ID:p0125
most fundamental qualitative shift in human development, as Vygotsky identied it, is from
lower, elementary processes to higher, conscious ones. is shift is a transition from “direct,
innate, natural forms and methods of behavior to mediated, articial, mental (psychological –
ELS) functions that develop in the process of cultural development” (Vygotsky, 1998, p.168).
ere is a dierence not only in the nature, but also in the origin of higher and lower psycho-
logical functions. Lower, elementary psychological functions are biological, natural, immediate
responses to stimuli. By contrast, higher psychological functions are conscious, intentional, and
purposeful psychological processes that are developed socioculturally.
Development of Higher Psychological Functions as the Foundation for the
Development of Agency
Vygotsky
ID:p0130
(1998) considered the development of higher psychological functions to be a key pro-
cess for the development of agency.1ese are the processes in human consciousness that lead
to human freedom from contextual constraints. By acquiring higher psychological functions,
human behavior can transform from reaction to the environment to intentional action toward
it, so people act via their own will. In other words, higher psychological functions are voluntary
and volitional as the person manages, “establishes control,” not over the environment but over
internal psychological processes of memory, attention, thinking, and emotion.
e
ID:p0135
development of higher psychological functions is the foundation for the development
of agency. It is a dialectical relationship since agency develops as the result of the develop-
ment of higher psychological functions. at is because they ensure voluntary action, the free
action of a self-determining agent. Simultaneously, every act of volition leads to the restructur-
ing of the system of psychological functions, as the new level of mastery and voluntary behavior
of an agent.
Agency
ID:p0140
for Vygotsky is not a state, but a process, that is dynamic, dramatic, and spe-
cic unity of aect and intellect for each individual. It stems from personal sense, and the
unique combination of motivational, volitional, and emotional processes that dene the
dynamics of thought and action. erefore, the process of the development of higher psy-
chological functions is not universal, but specic for personal development. e historically
developing systems of psychological functions play out dierently in each individual con-
sciousness. “ . . . [I]f a person is thinking, let’s ask which person . . . with the same laws
of thinking . . . the process will be dierent depending on inside which person it occurs”
(Vygotsky, 1986, p. 59).
ere
ID:p0150
is a dialectical relationship between development of agency and development of higher
psychological functions. Agency develops as the result of the development of higher psychologi-
cal functions because the more developed the higher psychological functions are, the higher the
possibility of a voluntary action, as a free action of a self-determining subject.
Pdf_Folio:280
Development of Mediational Means and Psychological Tools of Reective Practice 281
Higher Psychological Functions as Psychological Systems
Much
ID:p0155
confusion may arise from the discussion of higher psychological functions as indepen-
dent, isolated processes in human consciousness, such as separating imagination out from its
connection to emotion, and separating reection from will and theoretical thinking. Considera-
tion of higher psychological functions as isolated processes out of the context of their develop-
ment within a system, leads to the reductionist view of them as elementary functions. Vygotsky
(1982) introduces the notion of a psychological function as a system of relationships that has
its unique history, stressing the importance of examining it as a whole:
e
ID:p0160
concept of a system and a function [italicized by ELS] is obviously completely dierent
from the concept of an arithmetic sum and mechanistic chain of reactions. It implies the
specic consistent pattern of building the system, the specic role of a system as it is, and,
nally, the history of development and creation of the system . . . It applies also to the
concept of a psychological function . . . , rst of all as the relationship to the whole, it is
the relationship in the in the context of which the individual function is being performed,
and secondly, as an understanding of the wholeness of that psychological process that is
called function. (Vygotsky, 1983, p. 110) [Translated by ELS]
Vygotsky
ID:p0165
(1998) stresses the importance of understanding the development of higher psy-
chological functions as a qualitative shift, a transformation rather than an accumulation:
Higher
ID:p0170
mental functions are not simply a continuation of elementary functions and are
not their mechanical combination, but a qualitatively new mental formation that devel-
ops according to completely special laws and is subject to completely dierent patterns.
(p. 34)
Vygotsky
ID:p0175
(1999) argued that in the process of human development not only are separate
functions reconstructed, but interfunctional relations and connections among those functions
change radically.
As
ID:p0180
a result, new psychological systems arise that unite in complex cooperation a number
of separate elementary functions. We arbitrarily call these psychological systems, these
units of a higher order that replace the homogeneous, single, elementary functions, the
higher mental functions.2(p. 61)
us
ID:p0185
, a higher psychological function is a psychological system for Vygotsky that is dened
by the unique conguration of interrelationships among elementary functions, “heterogeneous
in composition” and historically changing in the process of ontogenesis. Such a psychological
system is a complex combination of symbolic and practical activity.
Higher Psychological Functions As a Product of Historical Development
Vygotsky
ID:p0190
stressed in a number of his works the importance of history in building the theory
of development. To understand the development of higher psychological functions we need to
consider them in the course of their development, capturing the history of transformations
Pdf_Folio:281
282 Lampert-Shepel and Murphy
of lower, naturally unfolding psychological functions into higher, tool-mediated ones. us, to
study the development of reection as a higher psychological function, we need to focus on
the history of its development from the lower function to its more developed state as a higher
psychological function, from elementary and single function to a complex system of interrelated
functions.
Social Nature of Higher Psychological Functions
Vygotsky
ID:p0195
(1993) challenged the belief that human mind exists independently of our sociocul-
tural practices and the history of their development. Moreover, for him the human mind was
among people, rather than in their individual heads. He argued that the development of higher
psychological functions entails transformation of collective social forms of behavior into per-
sonal higher psychological functions that have a history of being socially constructed.
. . . the relation between higher mental functions was at one time a concrete relation
among people; collective social forms of behavior in the process of development become
a method of individual adaptations and forms of behavior and thinking of the agent.
(Vygotsky
ID:p0200
, 1998, p. 168)
Vygotsky
ID:p0205
theorized the transformation of interpsychological processes into intrapsycholog-
ical ones. His great discovery was the description of the dialectical transformation of collectively
distributed cognition3(Rogo, 1998) into individual consciousness. In other words, he basi-
cally described the psychological mechanism of qualitative transformation of the social into the
individual consciousness, and argued for their interdependence in the process of development.
Vygotsky called this transformation the process of internalization mediated by cultural tools.
He claimed that the social origins of individual higher psychological functions rst appear exter-
nally, in a situation of socially shared cognition, and then on an internal plane, as individual
psychological functioning. Vygotsky (1982) called this process a general genetic law of cultural
development:
Any
ID:p0210
function in the child’s cultural development appears twice, or on two planes. First
it appears on the social plane, and then on the psychological plane. First it appears
between people as an inter-psychological category, and then within the child as an intra-
psychological category. is is equally true with regard to voluntary attention, logical
memory, the formation of concepts, and the development of volition. We may consider
this position as a law in the full sense of this word, but it goes without saying that inter-
nalization transforms the process itself and changes its structure and functions. Social
relations or relations among people, genetically underlie all higher functions and their
relationships. (cited in Wertsch, 1985, p. 61)
Vygotsky
ID:p0215
’s understanding of internalization as a dialectical relationship between interpsycho-
logical and intrapsychological, external and internal planes of higher psychological functioning
is one of the central ideas of his psychological work. erefore, the development of an individ-
ual teacher’s reection will depend to a large extent on the character of the interpsychological
activity.
Pdf_Folio:282
Development of Mediational Means and Psychological Tools of Reective Practice 283
Mediational Means and Psychological Tools of Reection
Vygotsky
ID:p0220
(1982) argued that human action is always mediated. Every mediated action, however,
does not necessarily lead to the development of higher psychological function. Vygotsky distin-
guished mediational means (Wertsch, 1998) from psychological tools. Mediational means, being
external, can resemble actions of the human hand. Via mediational means we can alter exter-
nal objects or processes. Psychological tools, though, are internal “instruments.” ey support
psychological activity, just as manual labor uses tools. Psychological tools, Vygotsky claimed,
support a mastery of oneself—an internal mastery.
To
ID:p0225
study teachers’ reection as a higher psychological function, we need to explore the tool
kit of mediational means and psychological tools of reection. e authors of this article explore
the mastery of the presented mediational means of reection they use, and psychological tools
teachers develop in the process of reective practice.
is
ID:p0230
line of argument can apply to conceptualization of reection as a higher psychological
function and an important condition for the development of self-consciousness. Like any other
higher psychological function, reection is systemic, historically developing, culturally medi-
ated, and socially constructed. Reection may also serve as a metacognitive activity of meaning-
making, since it entails thinking about thinking through the agent of the action continuously
exploring of the experience.
●Cultural
ID:p0235
. Reective action is mediated by such cultural tools as narrative, metaphor,
schema, narrative, and role-play/performance (Lampert-Shepel, 2006). ese tools are
culturally specic and have their limitations and benets. Teachers need to be educated
to internalize and use them purposefully, depending on the type of reective action they
join.
●Systemic
ID:p0240
. Higher psychological functions are psychological systems. Reection as a higher
psychological function develops in the system of dynamically changing relationships with
other elementary and higher psychological functions. erefore, development of reec-
tion is tightly connected with the development of imagination, language and speech, anal-
ysis, theoretical thinking, and emotion.
●Historical
ID:p0245
. Higher psychological functions are a product of historical development.
●Social
ID:p0250
. Higher psychological functions are acquired in the process of social interaction.
For reection to develop as a higher psychological function, teachers must participate in
various interactions and collaborations enabling a move from the inter- to the intrapsy-
chological.
●Mediated
ID:p0255
. Higher psychological functions are mediated by psychological tools, tools of
the mind that mediate one’s own cognitive or emotional processes. e purposeful use of
the psychological tools leads to the development of higher psychological functions, and
thereby to the mastery of one’s own behavior.
REFLECTIVE
ID:T
I0050
ACTION AS A UNIT OF ANALYSIS
Within
ID:p0260
the Vygotskian theoretical framework and research methodology to study the develop-
ment of a higher psychological function, one must dene the unit of analysis. For both studies
discussed in this article, reective action is such a unit of analysis. It helped capture the process
Pdf_Folio:283
284 Lampert-Shepel and Murphy
of reective practice in American and Russian Dewey schools, while in the study of the develop-
ment of reective practice through coteaching, the model of reective action was introduced to
teachers as a mediational means of reection.
Vygotsky
ID:p0265
(1987) held that the study of higher psychological functions in human develop-
ment was “the study of the complex whole” (p. 47). He distinguished between element and unit
analysis, arguing that only unit of analysis allows the study of a complex phenomenon in its
development. In element analysis, it is key to know multiple elements and their specic features.
In unit analysis, by contrast, one must nd the basic concept, or so-called “germ cell,” and follow
its development within a system of concepts.
Zinchenko
ID:p0270
(1985) stresses the unit’s capacity for development. e unit must have inherent
properties that enable its transformation. Wertsch (1998) suggests that a mediated action or
“agent-acting-with-mediational-means” (Wertsch, Tulviste, & Hagstrom, 1993) is a suitable unit
of analysis to study the mind. us, we suggest that such unit of analysis of reection as a higher
psychological function is reective action.
Reection
ID:p0275
is inseparable part of human action and should be studied in its midst. Figure 1
is the graphic representation of the cycle of reective action as a unit of analysis. e model
includes two dimensions of reective action: the rst one (real) describes the sequence of actions
teachers perform in practice, the second one (ideal) presents the corresponding psychological
functions that accompany practical actions. As we learned from teachers, the cycle of reective
action start from the experience of diculty in practice. John Dewey called it a situation of
perplexity; in the theory of learning activity this situation is referred to as a developmental
gap. In such a situation, the agent of action can’t continue it, and experiences a need to either
try again, or stop and think why the action can’t continue. If the second option is chosen, it
characterizes the A1 action, that is„ the beginning of the reective action on the model. e cycle
of reective action may start from the teacher asking herself why she can’t perform the practical
FIGURE 1. Reective
ID:p0280
action as a unit of analysis.
Pdf_Folio:284
Development of Mediational Means and Psychological Tools of Reective Practice 285
action (A2). e next step (A3) involves analysis of the experienced diculty and choosing the
mediational means; analysis allows teachers to grasp the performed action as a whole in context.
e step of analysis is necessary to move to modeling of the future practical action (A4), planning
(A5) of how to implement the new model of action in practice, and testing the new model of
action in practice (A6). is unit of analysis of reective action represents the steps of only one
cycle of reective action. Each step described is not predetermined and multiple variations of
the model can be observed in practice. However, to capture the development of reection as a
higher psychological function, there was a need to develop a model that can allow to capture
reection in the midst of its development and complexity.
Teachers
ID:p0285
who participated in the study described various situations that can lead to the
beginning of reective action. Reective action may originate from unresolved classroom situ-
ation, student’s behavior or learning diculties, communication with parents, interaction with
colleagues, and so forth. e important aspect of each of these star ting points of reective action
is teacher’s puzzlement, experienced diculty that is strong enough for the agent of the action
to break trial and error and start reective action. e experience of the developmental gap [\/]
helps teachers to grasp the situation as a whole and question the grounds of their actions, and,
therefore, become the agents of their practice, build the continuity of learning experiences for
all, create their praxis.
e
ID:p0290
transition from the real to ideal4plane of action is necessary for the reection’s beginning;
the next steps are its process. In the ideal plane, specic cultural tools that mediate the reective
action. ese symbolic psychological tools are culturally specic and may be conceptual mod-
els, metaphors, images, drawings, narratives, or other symbolic systems. e cycle of reective
action leads to the development of reection as a higher psychological function as teachers mas-
ter the mediational means of reection as well as analysis, modeling, and planning of their pro-
fessional actions. e cycle of the reective action has a transformational potential as teachers
develop reection as a higher psychological function and, therefore, speaking Vygotskian lan-
guage, master their own behavior, that is, become agents of their own practice. Teachers from
both schools spoke about this transformation deriving from reective action:
Teacher
ID:p0295
3 (SSD): When the understanding of your teacher’s self is growing inside you, when
this awareness is being claried for you, the work in this school is eternal joy (Interview 1, p. 4).
Teacher
ID:p0300
2 (MCS): ere is no meaningful experience without reection on it. You suddenly see
the situation as a whole and then can look for ways to transform it (Interview 1, p. 16).
In
ID:p0305
the second part of this article, we will discuss the ndings of two studies that explored
various aspects of the development of teachers’ reection as a higher psychological function:
a qualitative case study of Russian and American teachers’ reective practice in Dewey schools
(New York, United States; Moscow, Russia) and a multimethod ethnographic study of in-service
coteachers’ reective practice (Dublin, Ireland). Although the studies include multiple ndings,
for the purpose of this article we will limit the discussion to the sign-mediated reective actions
that contributed to the development of teachers’ reection as a higher psychological function
and to the mediational means and psychological tools teachers used to reect on their practice.
A
ID:P0310
CROSS-CULTURAL STUDY OF TEACHERS’ REFLECTIVE ACTIONS IN TWO
RUSSIAN AND AMERICAN DEWEY SCHOOLS
Introduction and Background
is
ID:p0315
cross-cultural qualitative study was conducted in Russian and American Dewey (Tanner,
1997) schools. e purpose of the study was to examine, document, and compare the variety
of issues that constitute the content of teachers’ reective practice; the processes of teachers’
Pdf_Folio:285
286 Lampert-Shepel and Murphy
reective actions; and the mediational means employed in teachers’ reective actions. e study
was conducted in an American and a Russian schools that were identied through a survey as
Dewey schools. Teachers’ reective actions were studied in the midst of their practice and the
data captured the use of mediational means and their occasional transformation into psycholog-
ical tools in teachers’ classrooms. Besides the requirement in both schools for teachers to build
their classroom practice based on their ongoing reection, no additional professional develop-
ment was oered to teachers to learn how to reect.
Methodology
is
ID:p0320
study’s overall research design combined multiple-case-study design with cultural–
historical research methodology. e model of reective action (Lampert-Shepel, 2008) served
as unit of analysis of teachers’ processes of reection. Researcher thus captured the process of
reective action in its development and identied the mediational means of reection teachers
used individually, with their coteacher, and in small groups.
is
ID:p0325
multicase qualitative study was conducted in two schools: the MCS (New York) and
the SSD (Moscow). It was important to choose the schools that valued reective practice and
supported teachers’ daily reection. With the use of the survey constructed on the previous
research that determined the features of John Dewey schools, it was possible to identify both
schools sharing the same school culture of Dewey Schools that rely on emergent curriculum and
teacher’ decision making. As it was determined that schools have similar school culture made it
possible to compare teachers’ reective actions cross-culturally. Data collection included a com-
bination of participant observations, videotaping of sta meetings, interviewing, and review of
documents. Two foci of observations—group reection and classroom teaching—provided the
essential information necessary for the three semistructured interviews with each of the six
teachers (three teachers per school). ree semistructured interviews were conducted with each
of six teachers. Data analysis included triangulation of data collected from observations, docu-
ments, group reection, and three individual interviews for each teacher, three teachers in each
school, and then cross-culturally.
e
ID:p0330
six case studies were analyzed rst within the same culture and then cross-culturally. e
grounded theory qualitative analysis case comparison was conducted cross-culturally against
predened categories of content, process, and mediational means of reection. For the purpose
of this publication, we will mainly focus on the ndings related to mediational means of reec-
tive action. Although the purpose of this study did not include focus on distinguishing medita-
tional means and psychological tools of reection, the dierence was captured at the stage of
data analysis.
In
ID:p0335
both studies we refer to meditational means as those that mediate the reective action
and are used spontaneously and psychological tools as those that teachers use intentionally to
shape the process of reection.
Findings
Teachers
ID:p0340
’ reective practice in both schools was socially constructed in the course of social
interactions, and culturally mediated with an array of mediational means of reection.
e participating teachers employed the following mediational means: (a) reective dialog,
(b) text/narrative/story, (c) schema/plan-book/note-pad, (d) inquiry/observation/example,
(e) performance/creative activity, and (f) metaphor. Teachers were not aware of their choice of
mediational means of reection. ey mainly used one or two without awareness of how the
Pdf_Folio:286
Development of Mediational Means and Psychological Tools of Reective Practice 287
mediational means of reection can limit or benet the process of reection. During the inter-
views, teachers became aware of how the mediational means of reection can inuence the con-
tent and process of reective action. Below are the examples of teachers’ using metaphors and
text/narrative as mediational means, and transforming them into psychological tools of reec-
tion. e purposeful, intentional use of metaphor or text to shape the process of reection is the
evidence that teachers master their own process of reective thought and, therefore, develop
reection as a higher psychological function.
Metaphors as Mediational Means and Psychological Tools
e
ID:p0345
study participants used metaphors to explain their reective process. ey found metaphors
helpful to reect on the meaning of reection. Teachers believed that metaphor allows searching
for the meaning without strictly dening it and capturing the new idea for the future clarica-
tion. Below are some examples of how teachers’ making sense of their reection itself mediated
by metaphor or image:
Teacher
ID:p0350
1 (MCS, New York): . . . the image that formed in my mind when thinking of this
process [reection] is that of a number of people holding up a exible mirror in front of me,
twisting the reection so that my experience can be seen with a new potential for change . . .
Teacher
ID:p0355
2 (SSD, Moscow): Reection is a brush of the archeologist with the help of which
he manages to remove the dust of time from the discovered object to reveal the meanings con-
cealed by many occasional happenings. Reection is a thinking tool of discovering the essential
meanings of your teaching and meaningful connections with the other people and events. . . .It
may change over time, so it should be an ongoing process . . . .
Teacher
ID:p0360
3 (MCS, New York):Reection is similar to children’s carbon paper, . . . transfers . . .
when one has to gently remove the obvious to reveal the important ideas, objectify your practice.
If you are not skillful, the image is ruined and has no meaning, and then you are in crisis and
have to overcome yourself . . . and to start again . . . and again.
Teacher
ID:p0365
4 (SSD, Moscow): One image I always have on my mind is that of life as a river, then
reection is like looking at this river from the bank of the river, or jumping above. I see reection
. . . and I observe it with children too . . . I will jump from the river, make a picture, a quick shot,
and jump back (laughs).
Elina
ID:p0370
:And when you “jump back,” are you the same?
Teacher
ID:p0375
4 (SSD, Moscow): I do not know . . . Maybe I am the same, but the river is changing.
Well, you see that the complex nature of the metaphor, when it leads your thinking . . . you see,
in this metaphor it would be dicult for me to reect on the changes . . . what changes in the
river . . . It should be a dierent metaphor. is metaphor is helpful to realize that I leave the
practice to reect on it and that it is kind of “uneven,” “jumpy” process.
Metaphor
ID:p0380
as a mediational means arises out of interactions between the conceptual struc-
tures that lie beneath the level of words. In the work of Lako and Johnson (1980), metaphor
is described as a cognitive device that establishes perspectives that form the irreducible fabric
of thought and expression.
Metaphor
ID:p0385
is a condition for construction of meaning in communication that is oriented
toward understanding. e potential of metaphor as a mediational means is in its capability of
systematically extending, changing, exhausting, decontextualizing, and historicizing the pro-
cess of reective action. Metaphor leads the process of teachers’ reection, letting them discover
new meanings for the object of reective thought. In all the examples, when teachers created
Pdf_Folio:287
288 Lampert-Shepel and Murphy
metaphors of reection, the initial metaphor led to the exploration of other meanings included
in the image. Metaphor as a mediational means is a dynamic cognitive tool that leads reective
thought to the exploration of new meanings.
As
ID:p0390
with every mediational means, metaphor has its potential and limitations. Both Ameri-
can and Russian teachers used metaphor as a mediational means and psychological tool of reec-
tion. As a mediational means, teachers used metaphors spontaneously in a dialog with others
trying to communicate a new understanding or grasp the general meaning of the event in their
teaching. However, MCS teachers, when asked to reect on the video of a project-based teach-
ing and learning similar to their own practice and given no direction on how to structure the
discussion, chose metaphor purposefully as a starter of their conversation to reect on a most
important idea of the video.
Teacher
ID:p0395
3: . . . I have a suggestion . . . Before we start discussion of the project Post-Oce we
have just seen . . . And we do it in our school as well, right? . . .Can we start with image or
metaphor as we did when reected on the way we reect? It may help to get some big ideas we
may lose in the future discussion.
In
ID:p0400
this case image or metaphor was chosen deliberately, to capture multiple ideas as a psycho-
logical tool, as it managed the future individual and group reection and, therefore, has become
a purposefully used tool of the mind.
Metaphor
ID:p0405
as a mediational means can be quite eective in the process of conceptualization
of previously identied ideas and values, for example, for creating and questioning one’s teach-
ing philosophy. It is also eective when teachers want to question an initial conceptualization
of practice, or when emotional responses to the situation in practice prevail. It seems to be a
less eective mediational means when teachers need to construct initial meanings in practice,
or when a detailed objectication of the course of events in the classroom is important.
Text/Narrative as a Mediational Means and Psychological Tool of Reection
e
ID:p0410
act of writing text is often reective; Yuriy Lotman argued that text acts as a “thinking
device” and “a generator of meaning” (Wells, 2000, p. 77). Although all six teachers admitted that
reection on their practice through writing was challenging for them, they referred to various
forms of writing as important mediational means. ey identied dierent purposes for writ-
ing texts. For some of them, texts became the mediational means to become aware of important
aspects of their practice (Teacher 4, SSD; Teacher 3, MCS) and to construct meanings while gen-
eralizing tendencies (Teacher, 5, SSD; Teacher 1, MCS). In addition, as MCS and SSD teachers
created original and complex practices of schooling, texts were not only the mediational means
of reective action to conceptualize their practice, but also to communicate it to other teachers.
For SSD teachers writing texts, creating stories of their practice mediated their dialog about the
philosophy and design of their elementary school. In addition, a story about teaching practice
becomes a “safe” mediator to discuss individual practice among the colleagues. As SSD teachers
were challenged by school administration to nd ways of learning from each other by going to
each other’s classrooms, they admitted that reading teachers’ texts about their practice was a
helpful rst step before they came to the classroom.5
Teachers
ID:p0415
identied texts as helpful mediational means to conceptualize their teaching phi-
losophy and construct the general image of their practice; still texts might be questioned
as a means to mediate reection on an emerging diculty, not yet identied. For example,
Teacher 4, SSD, questioned text as a helpful mediational means to reect on an experienced
Pdf_Folio:288
Development of Mediational Means and Psychological Tools of Reective Practice 289
diculty in the classroom. She argued that instead of helping to capture the reality of the class-
room, texts created a new reality that could be very dierent from the problem experienced in
practice. However, she nds writing “psychological portraits of children” helpful mediational
means to reect on her practice. us, the purpose of reective action, as well as the genre of
the text, inuences the eectiveness of text as a mediational means of reection.
Text
ID:p0420
as a mediational means appeared dierently in the process of reective action of the
participating Russian and American teachers. For the American teachers, text as a mediational
means of reection could be a story of practice, a description of its critical moments, as well
as ideas, values, and choices. For the Russian teachers, to create such a text was to present the
general philosophical ideas their practice is based on, and to describe and question its rationale,
and theoretical framework, and provide examples from practice (the latter was optional). e
specic details and everyday observations were for reective dialog, not for the written text.
us, the meanings of mediational means were developed in the history of the larger educational
cultures; and a particular mediational means, for example, a text can be a viewed as working
dierently for Russian and American teachers.
Interestingly
ID:p0425
, in both schoolsteachers were encouraged to learn how to use writing as medi-
ational means of teachers’ reective actions. us, writing texts as mediational means did not
emerge naturally in the course of teacher’s experiences. Instead, it was introduced by their school
in the course of professional development. As teachers appropriated text as a mediational means
of their reective actions, it changed their visions of practice. e introduction of new medi-
ational means changes the process of reective action. Teachers used writing a story of their
practice as a cognitive tool for conceptualization and a cultural tool of communication. e intro-
duction of the new mediational means, text, changed also the way teachers used other medi-
ational means. For example, reective dialog with text involved unfolds quite dierently from
one without text, for text oers objectied ideas and meanings that can be interpreted indepen-
dently by the participants of a dialog. Situations in which reective action is mediated by several
mediational means require further research.
Each
ID:p0430
of the study participants had a favorite mediational means that they would use most
often when they need to reect on their practice. Some preferred to have a dialog with another
teacher or write a reective journal, trying to capture through writing the complexities of every-
day practice. Others would draw schematic representations of the event of practice to capture
the event of practice as a whole. However, none of the teachers were employing these cultural
tools they either learned in their college education or other life experiences purposefully with
full understanding of their limitations and opportunities. On a dierent note, it is possible
to argue that teachers who mastered a specic cultural tool and used it purposefully to reect
moved away from spontaneous reection as description or verication of their actions to reec-
tion as a higher psychological function, that is, they used the cultural tool to master their own
process of reective thought using the mediational means as a psychological tool to organize
their reection.
Although
ID:p0435
both MCS, New York, and SSD, Moscow teachers who participated in the rst
study had coteachers in their classroom, coteaching was not explored as a model contributing
to the development of reection as a higher psychological function. e second study explores
the development of reection as a higher psychological function in the context of coteaching.
Pdf_Folio:289
“05_JCEP-D-18-00022_proof” — 2019/5/25 — 11:46 — page 290 — #13
290 Lampert-Shepel and Murphy
COTEACHING
ID:T
I0080
AS A MEANS TO DEVELOP REFLECTION AS A HIGHER
PSYCHOLOGICAL FUNCTION
is
ID:p0440
study focused on the development of preservice primary teachers’ condence and ability to
teach primary science through reection and reective practice in an innovative program in which
the preservice teachers (PST) shared expertise as they coplanned, copracticed, and coevaluted
lessons with experienced cooperating teachers for one morning per week over a period of 7
weeks as part of their school experience placement (internship). e focus on coteaching as a
means to develop reection as a higher psychological function is based on its unique context
in which coteachers are constantly engaging in coreection as they prepare, teach, and evaluate
lessons which aim at “ideal” practice in relation to student learning. In addition, part of this ideal
practice was for coteachers to reect on their appropriation of the coteaching process in order to
improve their own and each other’s creative practice such that their conscious shared contribution
to design, implementation, and evaluation results in enhanced learning and teaching for all. We
introduced a set of mediational means, to enable PSTs to develop the psychological tools that
are required for eective development of the higher psychological function (HPF) of reection
(Figure 2):
e
ID:p0450
focus on developing reection as an HPF in coteaching was derived from our observations
over many years of coteaching that most reection by coteachers evidenced surface level only,
for example, below is a PST reection on her experience of coteaching:
I
ID:p0455
had a real opportunity to build condence in teaching science and this helped me pre-
pare for my teaching practice [internship]. I feel this was a totally worthwhile experience.
I built good relationships with all children and enjoyed teaching the lessons. I denitely
feel much more condent in teaching practical science and have a clearer understanding
of how children learn science. I think the children enjoyed it. ey loved getting involved
and I could tell they were excited every Tuesday morning. e teacher enjoyed it as well.
She let me know this when I was leaving. (Murphy & Beggs, 2010, p. 28)
ese
ID:p0460
comments provide no evidence of constructive critique or reference to adjusting her
own practice for improvement. e development of teacher reection, as a higher psychologi-
cal function mediated by psychological tools is considered within Vygotsky’s cultural–historical
theory. Reection is a systematic, rigorous process of inquiry-based thinking with action, which
requires collaboration with others. Its development is onerous, complex, and, at times, uncom-
fortable for both pre- and in-service teachers (IST). Coteaching, during school experience, can
provide a mediational means whereby both ISTs and PSTs can expand their opportunities for
learning and reection. In terms of the reection process, our research suggests reection for,
on, and in action as units of analysis, coteachers plan, teach, and reect on lessons together (see
Figure 3).
Mediational means
(external)
Psychological tools
(internal) HPF of reflection
FIGURE 2. Mediational
ID:p0445
means and psychological tools of HPF.
Note
ID:p0445
.HPF = higher psychological function.
Pdf_Folio:290
“05_JCEP-D-18-00022_print” — 2019/5/11 — 7:06 — page 291 — #14
Development of Mediational Means and Psychological Tools of Reective Practice 291
REFLECTION FOR ACTION
REFLECTION ON ACTION
REFLECTION IN ACTION
Coreflection
Coplanning
Copractice
FIGURE 3. Reection in coteaching.
Within the reection process, coteachers also reected on the development of their own
reection, using tools such as Larrivee’s (2008) levels of reection, an adaptation of Lampert-
Shepel’s (2008) model of reection based on the work of Dewey and Vygotsky, and cogenerative
dialogue (Tobin, 2006).
Our conceptual model for coteaching is based on Vygotsky’s Zone of Proximal Develop-
ment (ZPD), incorporating its essential elements described by Veresov (2010), regression and
recursion in the ZPD discussed by other researchers (e.g., arp & Gallimore, 1988; Zebroski,
1994) and critical reection, using Lampert-Shepel’s (2008) model. Our coteaching model also
embraces advice from Roth and Radford (2010) who introduced the dimension of power rela-
tions between individuals involved in interactions, like those experienced in coteaching. Roth
and Radford suggested that insucient attention is given to co-constitution of subjective and
collective consciousness in which interacting participants “become each other’s teachers and
students independent of their institutional positions” (p. 300). eir proposition to consider
the ZPD from a symmetrical perspective connects strongly with our position that coteaching is
based on sharing expertise. Our model also addresses the complexity of interactions that need to
occur between successive stages of development via Rogo, Mistry, Goncu, and Mosier (1993)
consideration of individual development within the ZPD as:
incomplete unless it also considers the societal basis of the shared problem . . . the nature
of the problem the partners seek to solve, the values involved in determining the appro-
priate goals and means, the intellectual tools available, the institutional structures of the
interactions . . . (Rogo et al., 1993, p. 232, cited in Daniels, 2001)
METHODOLOGY FOR COTEACHING
e coteaching project formed the context for an optional primary science education module
within the 3rd and 4th year of a Bachelor of Education program and involved 10 PSTs partnered
with 10 teachers from six local primary schools. ere were three distinct phases of activity:
planning and preparation; copractice; and solo practice. During the planning and preparation
phase, IST–PSTcoteaching pairs attended workshops on a range of innovative approaches to
science teaching, including using stories, puppets, thinking skills, digital resources, drama andPdf_Folio:291
292 Lampert-Shepel and Murphy
role-play. In addition, the participants explored coplanning, coteaching, and coreection and
designed their own methods for recording and data collection. e researchers adopted a con-
structivist stance with regard to the design and use of the research instruments and data collec-
tion methods. Coteachers designed formats for coplanning and corection templates and chose
to use video to record their experiences. During the copractice stage, a total of seven science
lessons were coplanned, cotaught, and coevaluated (via coreection) during semester one. Each
coteaching pair chose topics and content to coteach. A dissemination seminar was held early in
semester two for coteachers to share their coteaching experiences and exchange ideas, extend-
ing out PSTs’ and ISTs’ repertoires of engaging science lessons in preparation for the nal stage
of the project, their solo practice. During the course of the PSTs’ school-based solo teaching
placement, they put into practice the pedagogical approaches which they had developed during
the copractice phase. e project concluded with a celebration and dissemination event during
which the teachers and PSTs described how their practice had developed through the course of
the coteaching project.
Data
ID:p0490
was collected from several sources. Researchers and participants constructed all data
collection methods collaboratively. Questionnaires were used at the start of the project, to
identify a baseline regarding participants’ practice and perceptions of good practice, and then
repeated at the end of the pilot, a few months after the coteaching placement had taken place.
Semistructured interviews were also held at the end of the pilot. Documents, for example,
coplans, coreections, classroom observations, PSTs’ assignment essay on coteaching and reec-
tive practice, and video recordings, also provided a rich source of data.
METHODOLOGY
ID:T
I0090
FOR DEVELOPING REFLECTION AS AN HPF
Our
ID:p0495
emphasis on engaging in reective practice through the coteaching model was supported by
creating opportunities and contexts to encourage deep reection, following the advice of Spald-
ing and Wilson (2002) who suggested that “we must actively teach and model reective skills
in a variety of ways if we are to demystify reection” (p. 139). Alger (2006) found that model-
ing the various levels of reection made it accessible and a useful “tool for pre-service teachers
to do the organizing and reorganizing of their understanding” (p. 287). PSTs attended inten-
sive reection workshops in which they were provided with tools to support the development of
deep reection. We purposefully made reection explicit, rather than implicit, in this approach.
Our set of tools, and the workshop context together represented a reection pedagogy. e tools
used as mediational means included
●Study
ID:p0500
of eight key research papers on reection, selected and introduced to them by the
research team
●A
ID:p0505
summary of levels of reection (adapted from the work of Larivee, 2008)
Use
ID:p0510
the following guide in your coreection discussions to ensure that you address
each of the levels below:
Level
ID:p0515
1: Surface reection (e.g., using evidence and making adjustments based on
experience only)
Level
ID:p0520
2: Pedagogical reection (e.g., adjust methods and practices based on stu-
dents’ relative performance)Pdf_Folio:292
“05_JCEP-D-18-00022_print” — 2019/5/11 — 7:06 — page 293 — #16
Development of Mediational Means and Psychological Tools of Reective Practice 293
Level 3: Critical pedagogical reection (e.g., commitment to continuous learning
and improved practice; constructive criticism of own practice; sees teaching prac-
tices as remaining open to further investigation)
●A schematic representation of how they might develop from novice coteachers toward
experts (Figure 4)
●Development of a coreection sheet during the workshop which focused on reection
as consideration of their coteaching practice in terms of the Vygotskian ideal of pupil
learning and of their development as coteachers. Coteachers commented on the following
after lessons:
“Ideal” children’s science learning from this lesson—how did we or they do?
What limited their learning?
How good were the tools used and how could we improve them?
What changes should be made?
How far did coteaching enable both teachers to attain their planned learning/develop-
ment?
●Requirement for PSTs to complete an assessed essay on their experience of coteaching
and the development of their reection as an HPF.
e use of these mediational means was intended to promote the development of psycho-
logical tools to analyze problems and model the mediational means in the production of strate-
gies to improve aspects of their coteaching, and thus the children’s (and their own) learning.
e internal psychological tools developed can be used intentionally and applied in new situa-
tions. In the next section, the ndings illustrate that not all coteachers developed reection as
an HPF to the same extent. Some exhibited reection as lower levels than others. Experiment-
ing with dierent tools and monitoring dierent ways to use them could generate a way forward
to improve the levels of teacher reection, leading to teaching mastery and the mastery of their
own reective process.
Overall Findings: Reection on Coteaching
Evidence from PST reections on coteaching indicated that some essential elements of the ZPD
appeared to be more important than others for developing their condence through coteach-
ing. e data was collected at the end of the project, so PSTs reected on development during
coteaching and afterward, when they enacted only solo teaching in the block placement.
eir reections on coplanning highlighted the interaction between real and ideal form as their
most useful construct. ”Ideal” practice became a powerful tool in coplanning. Most commented
Participation
Shared
participation
Cooperation
Shared
cooperation
Contribution
Shared
contribution
Sure, I’ll be
involved!
What
should I
do?
Should I
look
after?
What if we...?
Why don’t
we..?
Wow, let’s
make…
FIGURE 4. Stepwise development of coteaching toward shared contribution.
Pdf_Folio:293
294 Lampert-Shepel and Murphy
on ideal practice as thinking more about children’s learning than about lesson resources. Indica-
tive quotes from two of the PSTs’ reective essays were:
When
ID:p0590
I am planning a lesson now I am constantly thinking is this the best way for my
pupils, can I improve on their learning?
Before
ID:p0595
I focused on resources and how they worked, whereas after coteaching I went:
“okay this group didn’t get this, and this is why I think they didn’t get it, so this is what I’ll
do instead next time.” It was much more detailed in terms of children’s learning instead
of the practical setup of the classroom.
Reection
ID:p0600
on copractice indicated that Vygotskian imitation was the most useful concept.
PSTs recalled examples in their interviews, mostly relating to aspects they had noticed during
coteaching and had integrated into their solo practice, for example,
Let
ID:p0605
them ramble on for longer bring up their ideas . . .
More
ID:p0610
discussion in my lessons; the teacher had the class organized in specic ways so
they weren’t just sitting at their desk—they engaged more in discussion. I do that now.
Vygotskian
ID:p0615
imitation was commented on in more depth in PSTs’ reective essays, for
example,
. . . as such I seemed to move from the surface level to the more pedagogical and critical
levels of reection quicker, as I asked the questions the [coteaching partner] teacher would
have asked, such as: “where is the progression in this lesson?” “is this particular aspect of
the lesson benecial to learning?” “how can you overcome the common misconceptions a
child will make in this lesson?” etc.
FINDINGS
ID:T
I0100
ON DEVELOPING REFLECTION AS AN HPF
e
ID:p0625
reections above indicated aspects of deeper reection indicating that PSTs were reecting
in relation to ideal practice and adjusting their own practice in search of the ideal. e begin-
ning of the comment just above indicates a PST’s metareection, in which she is monitoring her
progression from lower, surface level toward the deeper pedagogical and critical levels. e data
in Table 1 below evidences examples of PSTs reections at levels 1–3, (surface, pedagogical, and
critical), and a new level of metareection on the process of their reection, in which one of the
tools we gave them in the workshops could restrict their reective processes, which indicated
very deep and productive reection as a higher psychological function.
DISCUSSION
ID:T
I0105
Earlier
ID:p0670
coteaching studies (Murphy & Scantlebury, 2010) have revealed fairly low-level reec-
tion from coteachers, in that there is little attention paid to how lessons were enacted in relation
to ideal practice and more to surface-level description. In this study, PSTs were provided with
specic mediational means, such as eight core articles from the literature which explored reec-
tive practice, and support from the research team to use these eectively in writing a reective
essay on their coteaching experiences. Such structured reection, which included reference to
Larivee’s (2008) tool for reection which identied levels of PST reection, generated a much
deeper engagement by PSTs with coteaching practice in their essays than was evidenced in the
Pdf_Folio:294
Development of Mediational Means and Psychological Tools of Reective Practice 295
TABLE 1. Preservice
ID:p0630
Teacher Reection—Data From Reective Essays
Major
Category Finding Indicative Quotes
Coreection
ID:t0005
Identied huge
benets of
coteaching,
including working
with a critical
friend . . .
1. “I wouldn’t have touched investigative science side
before with a barge pole . . . Not on your life would
I have given them [6-7yrs] cups of water . . . too
nervous of what they would do. . . my [coteaching]
experience totally changed that.”
Level
ID:p0640
2 (surface reection) (e.g., using evidence
and making adjustments based on experience
only).
Individual
ID:t0020
reections
on
coteaching
Most PSTs
indicated that
they progressed
from evaluating
resources and
classroom
activities to
reecting on
children’s learning
2. “e content of reection changed. Before I focused
on resources . . . whereas after coteaching I went:
‘OK this group didn’t get this and this is why
I think they didn’t get it, so this is what I’ll do
instead next time.’ It was much more detailed in
terms of children’s learning . . .”
Level
ID:p0650
3 (pedagogical reection)(e.g., adjusts
methods and practices based on students’ relative
performance).
eory
ID:t0035
/Practice Direct and deep
reection on
theory into
practice and
sometimes
developing new
theory from
practice was more
evident in PSTs
than ISTs.
3. “rough coteaching I have developed my reective
practice through the levels of progression . . . It is
evident that whilst coteaching has developed my
RP the road to becoming a competent RPer will be
long. R is arguably a process, not a method . . . must
be developed throughout . . . career. is journey
. . . facilitating lessons which site pupils’ learning in
the forefront has begun and it will be interesting to
chart the progress & eectiveness of my reection
throughout . . . career.”
Level
ID:p0660
4 (critical pedagogical reection) (e.g.,
commitment to continuous learning and improved
practice; constructive criticism of own practice;
sees teaching practices as remaining open to
further investigation).
Metareection
ID:t0050
Some PSTs
critiqued the
process (using an
agreed template
for reection)
4. “roughout the coteaching experience, reection
arguably occurred through the provision of a
structure, in the coreection template, maximising
the thinking process. e structure gave reections
a premise, however, care must be taken in the use
of templates when reecting to ensure that the
process is not hampered by the completion of
sections on paper.”
Level
ID:p0665
4 (as above)
Pdf_Folio:295
296 Lampert-Shepel and Murphy
interviews. In addition, PSTs used a template for reection developed by the research team, and
one critiqued this instrument in her reective essay:
roughout
ID:p0675
the coteaching experience, reection arguably occurred through the provi-
sion of a structure, in the coreection template, maximizing the thinking process. is
structure gave reections a premise, however, care must be taken in the use of templates
when reecting to ensure that the process is not hampered by the completion of sections
on paper.
Another
ID:p0680
comment from a reective essay indicated how the PST was considering her
coteaching experience in relation to her teaching career.
rough
ID:p0685
coteaching I have developed my reective practice through the levels of progres-
sion and in a variety of ways through reection in action and reection on action . . . It is
evident that whilst coteaching has developed my refective practice, the road to becoming
a competent “Reective Practitioner”(GTCNI, 2007) will be long. Reection is arguably
a process, not a method, but a process which must be developed throughout a teaching
career. is journey of eective reection, facilitating lessons which site pupils’ learning
in the forefront has begun and it will be interesting to chart the progress and eective-
ness of my reections throughout my teaching career.
Use
ID:p0690
of these external mediational means enabled the PSTs to develop the required, internal
psychological tools in order to utilize the higher psychological funtion of reection more eec-
tively in the development of their current and future practice as creative teachers. Teachers who
used the mediational means as the imposed structure rst, could apply them next on their own
to facilitate the reection with the coteacher, their own individual reection or metareection.
is is the evidence that they developed the psychological tools to facilitate their own process
of reection and therefore develop reection as a higher psychological function.
Implications
ese
ID:p0695
studies’ ndings generated a number of implications for the practice of teachers and
teacher educators. First, it is argued that reection as a higher psychological function develops
in the course of teachers’ reective actions. At the core of the development of reection as a
higher psychological function is teachers’ mastery of an array of mediational means of reec-
tion, and the development of psychological tools of reection that help them master their own
process of reection and reective action. e education of teachers in the dierent mediational
means of reection can support their development of reection as a higher psychological func-
tion. In the rst study, teachers were using various mediational means spontaneously. Only in
the course of the study did it become clear for teachers that these were not universally help-
ful for every situation in practice. Only a few of the teachers used mediational means purpose-
fully (see the example with metaphor, in the previous section Metaphors as Mediational Means
and Psychological Tools), and therefore had limited opportunity to develop psychological tools of
reection. In the second study, teachers mastered various mediational means to structure their
reection (texts, graphic models, levels) and were engaged in the joint activity of coteaching that
provided the opportunity for dialogical reection on their practice. As the data analysis shows,
Pdf_Folio:296
Development of Mediational Means and Psychological Tools of Reective Practice 297
teachers in the second study had more opportunity to develop reection as a higher psycholog-
ical function, the more they used mediational means purposefully to direct their reection on
practice, and master their own process of reection. In addition, the continuous joint activity
with the coteacher creates an opportunity of a double-mediation when another teacher in the
pair becomes an additional meditational means in the process of reection. Such situation of
double-mediation helps teachers to reect not only on the situation, but also on the medita-
tional means they are using. In the context of the coteaching, teachers master the meditational
means and have a better opportunity to develop psychological tools of reection.
Continuous
ID:p0700
teachers’ reective practice is vital for ongoing inquiry and learning about
their practice and, therefore, their development as professionals capable of critical inquiry and
transformation of their own practice. Certain conditions are necessary for the development of
teachers’ reective practice. First, since professional teachers’ reection is part of the craft of
teaching, teachers must learn how to reect on practice, the array of mediational means of reec-
tion, and ways of developing psychological tools of reection. ey need to know what the pro-
cess of reective action entails, and possibilities and limitations of various ways of reection. To
meet the needs of diverse students, teachers need to be educated how to identify the problem
in practice and the matching mediational means to reect on it. Since teachers indicate in some
cases that dialogical reection is not as helpful as creating a graphic model of the situation in
practice, the use of individual narrative can help to capture some important details but not nec-
essarily pose critical questions necessary to advance the reective process. erefore, teachers
need to be educated in the library of the mediational means of reection and understand their
limitations to guide reective thought.
Since
ID:p0705
reection as a higher psychological function develops in the course of social interac-
tion, teachers should have an opportunity to belong to a community of inquiry that oers con-
tinuous reective dialog to construct emergent meanings, challenge existing understandings,
and conceptualize practice. ere is a dialectical relationship between the level of the devel-
opment of reection as a higher psychological function and teacher’s agency. e more devel-
oped the teacher’s reective practice is, the higher the ability to conceptualize your own practice
and transform it into praxis, the stronger is the ownership of one’s own approaches to teach-
ing and learning, the teacher’s agency in continuous meaning-making. A coteaching model that
includes educating teachers in mediational means of reection supports teachers’ inquiry into
their teaching, and develops their ability to reect.
As
ID:p0710
many educational leaders and teacher educators expect teachers to engage in reective
practice as a natural ability of the mind, teachers need to be educated in reective practice as
a professional learning activity. Like any human activity, it has its complex cultural system of
goals, structure, processes, and mediational means. Learning how to make meaning of everyday
practice using the lenses of various philosophical and theoretical frameworks, and learning how
to create, develop, and conceptualize their own practice is important for teachers to become the
agents of their professional learning activity through the mastery of the reection as a higher
psychological function. Teachers who are engaged in the continuous inquiry into their teaching
and learning have a better chance to create, develop, and sustain the inquiry in their classroom,
so vitally important for learning and development of all students in their classrooms.
NOTES
1. Although Vygotsky uses the word “personality,” the direct translation might not be helpful, as person-
ality is understood as a developmental transformational process characterized by agentive behavior.Pdf_Folio:297
298 Lampert-Shepel and Murphy
e concept of personality has been conceptualized as self (Stetsenko, A. & Arievitch, 2004) and/or
agency. In this publication, we refer to Vygotsky’s concept of personality as “agency.”
2. Vygotsky used the term higher psychological functions, that has been often translated as “mental func-
tions.” is choice of language caused inaccurate connotations, as mental functions can be considered as
purely cognitive processes, while Vygotskian understanding of higher psychological functions implies
the unity of aect and intellect.
3. Distributed cognition is a school of psychology developed in the 1990s by Edwin Hutchins. Using
insights from sociology, cognitive science, and Vygotskian cultural–historical psychology, it empha-
sizes the social aspects of cognition. Distributed cognition proponents argue that cognition is not to be
found within the head only; rather cognition is distributed among other people and tools.
4. e ideal plane of action represents here cognitive functions of reection, analysis, modeling and plan-
ning, that is, cognitive and emotional aspects of human action.
5. e issue of how to learn from going to other teachers’ classrooms emerged at SSD’s third group meeting
when they were discussing the videotape “e Lightning Post-Oce.” Teacher 1, SSD and Teacher 4,
SSD suggested that the lens for observing other teacher’s classroom should be their own problem area
in teaching. But all the teachers present at the meeting said that teachers’ texts describing their practice
are very helpful to learn about other teacher’s practice, especially their rationale for making decisions.
(Fieldnotes from SSD group meeting 3).
REFERENCES
ID:T
I0120
Alger, C. (2006). ‘What went well. What didn’t go so well’: Growth of reection in pre-service teachers.
Reective Practice,7(3), 287–301.
Daniels, H. (2001). Vygotsky and pedagog y. London, England: Routledge.
Freire, P. (1997). Pedagogy of the oppressed. New York, NY: Continuum.
General Teaching Council Northern Ireland. (2007). Teaching: e reective profession. Belfast, Ireland:
GTCNI.
Giroux, H. (1985). Teachers as transformative intellectuals. Social Education,49(5), 376–379.
Homan-Kipp, P., Artiles, A. J., & Lopez-Torres, L. (2003, Summer). Beyond reection: Teaching learning
as praxis. eory into practice. Retrieved from http://www.ndarticles.com/cf_dls/m0NQM/3_42/
108442653/p1/
Lako, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
Lampert-Shepel, E. (2006). Cultures of reection and reection in culture: cross-cultural study of
teachers’ reective actions in two Russian and American Dewey schools (Doctoral dissertation).
http://digitalcommons.libraries.columbia.edu/dissertations/AAI3225166.
Lampert-Shepel, E. (2008). Cultural–historical psychology and activity theory (CHAT) and case study
design. International Journal of Case Study Research and Application,XX(2), 1554–7752. Retrieved from
https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/3c74/f9a48c58ad060d91c43d9e5b4375975bd3f2.pdf
Larrivee, B. (2008). Development of a tool to assess teachers’ level of reective practice. In Reective Practice
9, 341–360. doi:10.1080/14623940802207451
Lindsay, A., & Mason, R. (2000). Focusing on reection with early childhood practitioners. Net-
works: An Online Journal for Teacher Research,3(2). Retrieved from https://dx.doi.org/10.4148/2470-
6353.1218
Murphy, C., & Beggs, J. (2010). A ve-year systematic study of coteaching science in 120 primary schools.
In C. Murphy & K. Scantlebury (Eds.), Coteaching in international contexts: Research and practice
(pp. 105–124). Netherlands: Springer.
Murphy, C., & Scantlebury, K. (2010). Coteaching in International Contexts. New York, NY: Springer Pub-
lishing.
National Board for Professional Teaching Standards. (2016). What teachers should know and be able to do.
Retrieved from http://accomplishedteacher.org/proposition-4/Pdf_Folio:298
Development of Mediational Means and Psychological Tools of Reective Practice 299
National Council for Accreditation of Teacher Education. (2010, November 25). Transforming teacher educa-
tion through clinical practice: A national strategy to prepare eective teachers. Retrieved from http://www.
ncate.org/LinkClick.aspx?leticket=zzeiB1OoqPk%3d&tabid=715
Rogo, B. (1998). Cognition as a collaborative process. In D.Kuhn, & R.S .Siegler (Eds.), Cognition, perception
and language (pp. 679–744). New York, NY: Wiley.
Rogo, B., Mistry, J., Göncü, A., Mosier, C., Chavajay, P., & Heath, S. B. (1993). Guided participation in
cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
i–179.
Roth, W. M., & Radford, L. (2010). Re/thinking the zone of proximal development (symmetrically). Mind,
Culture, and Activity,17(4), 299–307. doi:10.1080/10749031003775038
Stetsenko, A., & Arievitch, I. (2004). e self in cultural-historical activity theory: Reclaiming the unity
of social and individual dimensions of human development. eory and Psychology,14(4), 475–503.
doi:10.1177/0959354304044921
Spalding, E., & Wilson, A . (2002). Demystifying reection: A study of pedagogical strategies that encourage
reective journal writing. Teachers College Record, 104(7), 1393–1421.
Tanner, L. N. (1997). Dewey’s laboratory school: Lessons for today. New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
arp, R. G., & Gallimore, R. (1988). Rousing minds to life: Teaching, learning, and schooling in social context.
New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Tobin, K. (2006). Learning to teach through coteaching and cogenerative dialogue. Teaching Education,17,
133–142. doi:10.1080/10476210600680358
Veresov, N. (2010). Introducing cultural historical theory: Main concepts and principles of genetic research
methodology. Retrieved from http://nveresov.narod.ru/KIP.pdf
Vygotsky, L. S. (1982). Myshlenie i rech’. Collectedwork s of L. S.Vygotsky (Vol. 2). Moscow, Russia: Pedagogika.
Vygotsky, L.S.. (1986). Konkretnaya psihologia cheloveka. [Concrete Psychology of a Human Being.] Vest-
nik Moskovskogo Universiteta, 14(1), 53–64.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). ought and word.In R. W. Rieber & A. S. Carton (Eds.), e collected works of
L.S.Vygotsky, Vol. 1: Problems of general psychology (pp. 243–285). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Vygotsky, L. (1993). ought and language. Cambridge, MA: e Massachussetts Institute of Technology.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1998). Collected works (Vol. 5). New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1999) e collected works of L.S. Vygotsky, Volume 6, scientic legacy. (M. J. Hall, Trans.).
New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
Wertsch, J. V. (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, MA and London, England: Har-
vard University Press.
Wertsch, J. V. (1998). Mind as action. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Wertsch, J. V., Tulviste, P., & Hagstrom, F. (1993). A socio-cultural approach to agency. In E. Forman,
N. Minick, & C. A. Stone (Eds.), Contexts for learning: Socio-cultural dynamics and children’s development
(pp. 336–356). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Wells, G. (2000). Dialogic inquiry in education: Building on the legacy of Vygotsky. In C. Lee & P. Smagorin-
sky (Eds.), Vygotskian perspectives on literacy research: Constructing meaning through collaborative inquiry.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Zebroski, J.T. (1994). inking through theory: Vygotskian perspectives on the teaching of writing. Portsmouth,
NH: Boynton/Cook.
Zinchenko, V. P. (1985). Vygotsky’s ideas about units for the analysis of mind. In J. W. Wetsch (Ed.), Cul-
ture, communication and cognition. Vygotskian perspectives (pp. 94–119). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge
University Press.
Pdf_Folio:299
300 Lampert-Shepel and Murphy
Disclosure
ID:p0740
. e authors have no relevant nancial interest or aliations with any commercial
interests related to the subjects discussed within this article.
Correspondence regarding this article should be directed to Elina Lampert-Shepel, Gradu-
ate School of Education, Touro College, 320 W 31st St., Room 215C, New York, NY. E-mail:
elina.lampert-shepel@touro.edu
Pdf_Folio:300