ArticlePublisher preview available

Processing Goals, Task Interest, and the Mood–Performance Relationship: A Mediational Analysis

American Psychological Association
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract

The authors examined the role of intrinsic interest in mediating the relationship among mood, processing goals, and task performance. Participants in induced happy, neutral, or sad moods generated similarities and differences between TV shows using performance-based, enjoyment-based, or no stop rule (cf. L. L. Martin, D. W. Ward, J. W. Achee, & R. S. Wyer, 1993). Pretask interest and both quantitative (time spent, number generated) and qualitative (creativity) performance were assessed. Happy participants spent more time and generated more items than other participants when using an enjoyment-based stop rule but spent less time and generated fewer items when using a performance-based stop rule. Happy participants also expressed greater pretask interest and were more creative than other participants regardless of stop rule. Regression-based path analyses indicated that pretask interest partially mediated the effects of mood on quantitative performance but not on creativity.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
1996, Vol. 71, No. 2. 245-261Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.
0022-3514/96/S3.00
Processing Goals, Task Interest, and the Mood-Performance
Relationship: A Mediational Analysis
Edward R. Hirt
Indiana University BloomingtonR. Jeffrey Melton
Martin University
Hugh
E.
McDonald
Indiana University BloomingtonJudith M. Harackiewicz
University of Wisconsin—Madison
The authors examined the role of intrinsic interest in mediating the relationship among mood,
processing
goals,
and task performance. Participants in induced happy, neutral, or sad moods gen-
erated similarities and differences between TV shows using performance-based, enjoyment-based,
or no stop rule (cf. L. L. Martin, D.
W.
Ward,
J.
W.
Achee,
&
R.
S.
Wyer, 1993). Pretask interest and
both quantitative (time spent, number generated) and qualitative (creativity) performance were
assessed. Happy participants spent more time and generated more items than other participants
when using an enjoyment-based stop rule but spent
less
time and generated fewer items when using
a performance-based stop rule. Happy participants also expressed greater pretask interest and were
more creative than other participants regardless of
stop
rule. Regression-based path analyses indi-
cated that pretask interest partially mediated the effects of mood on quantitative performance but
not on creativity.
The notion that human cognitive processes are colored by
emotional experience has considerable intuitive appeal. Con-
ventional wisdom suggests that people who are in a positive
mood
see
the world through "rose-colored
glasses";
that
is,
peo-
ple interpret their experiences through the
filter
of the emotions
they happen to be feeling at the time, even if those emotions do
not actually result from the experiences they are interpreting.
The validity of this intuition is supported by a large body of
research suggesting that evaluative judgments, problem-solving
strategies, and task performance are all influenced by preexist-
ing affective states (see Clore, Schwarz, & Conway, 1994, for a
review). Findings such as these have fascinated scholars, per-
haps because they illustrate the role of emotion in human judg-
ment. The result of
this
fascination has been an abundance of
research on the relationship between affect and cognitive pro-
cesses in recent years.
Edward R. Hirt and Hugh
E.
McDonald, Department of Psychology,
Indiana University Bloomington; R. Jeffrey Melton, Department of
Psychology, Martin University; Judith M. Harackiewicz, Department
of Psychology, University of Wisconsin—Madison.
Portions of this research were presented at the Goals and Affect Con-
ference sponsored by the University of Georgia and the Institute for
Behavioral Research, Athens, Georgia, January 1994.
We thank the participants of that conference for their feedback. We
also thank Chick Judd for statistical advice and Russ Fazio for his in-
sightful comments on an earlier version of
this
article.
Finally,
we
thank
Beth Goldner, Cindy Hopper, Kevin Leadingham, Michelle Merz, Lisa
Sonderegger, and Mike Stone for their assistance in data collection and
data coding.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Ed-
ward R. Hirt, Department of Psychology, Indiana University, Bloomin-
gton, Indiana
47405-1301.
Electronic mail may be sent via the Internet
to ehirt@indiana.edu.
Effects of Induced Mood on Task Performance
In the present article, we focus in particular on the effects
of induced mood on task performance. Intuitively, one might
expect people in positive moods to consistently outperform
those in other
moods;
indeed, many company policies are pred-
icated on the notion that happy employees will be more pro-
ductive. However, it has become increasingly clear that the rela-
tionship between affect and task performance is more complex
than this intuitive notion might suggest. On one hand, the in-
duction of positive affective states has been shown to increase
decision-making efficiency (Isen & Means, 1983) and facilitate
creative problem-solving (Isen, Daubman, & Nowicki, 1987;
Isen, Johnson, Mertz,
&
Robinson,
1985;
Mitchell
&
Madigan,
1984;
Murray, Sujan, Hirt, & Sujan, 1990). For instance, Isen
et al. (1987) found that participants in positive moods per-
formed better than control participants on creative problem-
solving tasks such as Duncker's (1945) candle task and Med-
nick, Mednick, and Mednick's (1964) Remote Associates Test.
More recently, Murray et al. (1990) found that happy partici-
pants
generated
a
greater quantity of similarities and differences
between pairs of television
shows
than did neutral mood partic-
ipants; moreover, their responses were more creative (quali-
tatively better), as judged by independent raters.
On the other hand, induced positive affect has been found
to produce performance decrements on such tasks as physics
problems
(Isen,
Means, Patrick,
&
Nowicki,
1982), correlation
estimation (Sinclair & Mark, 1995), performance appraisal
(Sinclair, 1988), and syllogisms (Melton, 1995). In many of
these same studies, inducing a negative mood has been found
to improve task performance (Sinclair,
1988;
Sinclair
&
Mark,
1995).
Moreover, evidence
suggests
that participants in positive
moods do not distinguish between strong and weak messages
when evaluating the merits of persuasive communications, un-
245
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
... For instance, Abele's research indicates that individuals experiencing positive moods tend to demonstrate greater cognitive fluency compared to those in negative moods [12]. Conversely, other studies argue that negative emotional states can foster greater creativity than positive or neutral emotions [13,14]. Despite these contributions, the literature has yet to reach a consensus on the precise nature of the affectcreativity relationship, suggesting the need for a deeper exploration of contextual and individual factors that may influence these dynamics. ...
Article
Full-text available
Affect plays a pivotal role in shaping employees’ work performance and mental health, with growing recognition of its capacity to drive creativity. However, the differential impacts of positive and negative affect on creative performance remain a subject of debate. This study aims to compare the relationships between high- and low-arousal affect, as well as PANA dimensions of affect, and creative performance, emphasizing the mediating role of work engagement from an occupational mental health perspective. A survey was conducted involving 278 employees and their managers across 25 companies in China. The findings reveal significant associations between high-arousal positive affect, low-arousal positive affect, high-arousal negative affect, and low-arousal negative affect with both work engagement and creative performance. Moreover, the results indicate that work engagement partially mediates the relationship between high-arousal affect and creative performance while fully mediating the relationship between low-arousal affect and creativity. These findings underscore the importance of occupational mental health—particularly work engagement—in fostering employee creativity and highlight its critical role in organizational management strategies.
... In contrast, a negative affect is interpreted as a signal that invalidates this content. Among these theories, the mood-asinput model (Martin et al., 1993; see also Hirt et al., 1996;Hirt et al., 1997) proposes that individuals interpret their affect as a "yes" (if positive) or as a "no" (if negative) answer to the question they have in mind while doing the task. For instance, if participants ask themselves whether they enjoy the task, they should persist longer on that task if a positive affect has been induced, compared to a negative affect. ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this research was to test an informational explanation of the effects observed in the affect misattribution procedure (AMP). According to this explanation, participants performing the AMP would simplify the task by asking whether the target is pleasant (yes vs. no) and would use the affective information provided by the prime to answer the question (positive = yes, negative = no). In line with this proposition, we observed in three preregistered experiments that slightly modifying the response options proposed in the task moderated the effect, which can be canceled (Experiment 1) and even reversed (Experiments 2 and 3). These results are consistent with the informational explanation and seem difficult to explain by the operation of misattribution processes.
... Task motivation in the workplace is important as it drives employees to engage in their work, out of genuine interest and satisfaction, rather than external rewards or pressures [22]. That is, a task-focused individual who is intrinsically motivated will participate in an activity for enjoyment rather than for potential benefits [23]. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigates how individual predispositions toward Virtual Reality (VR) affect user experiences in collaborative VR environments, particularly in workplace settings. By adapting the Video Game Pursuit Scale to measure VR predisposition, we aim to establish the reliability and validity of this adapted measure in assessing how personal characteristics influence engagement and interaction in VR. Two studies, the first correlational and the second quasi-experimental, were conducted to examine the impact of environmental features, specifically the differences between static and mobile VR platforms, on participants’ perceptions of time, presence, and task motivation. The findings indicate that individual differences in VR predisposition significantly influence user experiences in virtual environments with important implications for enhancing VR applications in training and team collaboration. This research contributes to the understanding of human–computer interaction in VR and offers valuable insights for organizations aiming to implement VR technologies effectively. The results highlight the importance of considering psychological factors in the design and deployment of VR systems, paving the way for future research in this rapidly evolving field.
... Kramer, 2003;Hirt, Melton, McDonald, & Harackiewicz, 1996;Isen et al., 1987). Another study using self-report measures found negative relationship between negative emotions and creativity among college students in Norway (Vosburg, 1998). ...
Article
Full-text available
Research on the implications of negative emotions for creativity has yielded mixed findings. To clarify this relationship, we conducted a short-term prospective study with data collected across two time points. We also explored the moderating roles of family expressiveness and gender in the creative process. The sample included 392 Japanese adolescents (54.7% female; age range: 12-13 years old). Adolescents reported on their experiences of negative activating emotion (e.g., fear, anxiety) and the degree to which their family members expressed positive emotions (e.g., expressing gratitude) toward each other. A drawing task and an alternative uses task (AUT) were used to measure adolescents' creativity. For creativity expressed in the drawing task, negative activating emotion enhanced creativity in girls only when their families' emotional expressivity was of low or average levels. For creativity indexed by the AUT, a high level of family expres-siveness strengthened the relationship between negative activating emotion and creativity in boys but suppressed such a relationship in girls. Findings suggest that, at least among Japanese adolescents, the associations between negative activating emotions and creativity can depend on multiple factors, including family emotional expressivity, gender, and the nature of the creativity task.
... Fourth, well-being promotes better health, which makes employees more likely to be present at work in good shape (Futterman et al., 1994;Salovey et al., 2000). Finally, well-being has positive effects on creativity, which is a key component of innovative problem solving (Hirt et al., 1996). Going further, Demerouti and Cropanzano (2010) argue that these aspects can improve both in-role job performance and extra-role organizational citizenship behaviors. ...
Article
Full-text available
We propose and test a new conceptual model in which overall justice is an antecedent to personal flourishing. Flourishing, in turn, partially mediates the relationship of overall justice to job performance and organizational citizenship behaviors directed toward individuals (OCB-I). These hypotheses are confirmed. However, high materialism weakens the relationship between overall justice and flourishing. Consequently, the mediated effects of justice on performance and OCB-I are moderated by materialism. In short, materialism sets limits on overall justice as an antecedent of flourishing and effective work behaviors.
... The purpose of this study was to examine the relationships between creativity, psychological well-being, and climate change concerns based on the data of the European Social Survey, 10th round. It was hypothesized, aligning with previous research, that creative individuals are prone to be happy [138][139][140][141][142] and may possess heightened awareness and concern for environmental issues, including climate change [56]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Previous studies, to some extent, link creativity, well-being, and environmental concerns. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine the associations between self-reported creativity, psychological well-being, and climate change concerns. This study was based on data derived from the European Social Survey’s 10th round and applied several items from sections H, B, and C. The findings revealed that creativity was positively linked to climate change concerns and psychological well-being, while psychological well-being was, to some extent, negatively related to climate change concerns. The SEM model demonstrated an acceptable fit, χ² = 1627.862; Df = 7; CFI = 0.959; TLI = 0.876; NFI = 0.958; RMSEA = 0.078 [0.075–0.082]. Additionally, the findings indicated that more creative and happy individuals were also more concerned about climate change; they were more prone to believe that climate change is caused by human activity, they felt a higher personal responsibility to reduce climate change, and they were significantly more worried about climate change. This implies that as creative and happy individuals have a heightened sense of responsibility for climate change, these individuals may indeed play a key role in promoting climate change awareness and actions, including communication on sustainability, sustainability-related projects, and other activities; thus, tailored campaigns could be developed within the frames of citizen science. The findings also suggest implications for policy development: policymakers could consider the perspectives of creative and happy individuals when crafting climate policies, potentially leading to more effective and widely accepted solutions.
Article
Full-text available
Background The aim of the present study was to investigate the relationship between certainty, positive anticipatory states, and positive feelings by analyzing written narratives collected during the COVID-19 pandemic lockdown in April 2020. Methods A total of 1,090 Italian participants were asked to write two narratives (one about the present and one about their representation of the future). The corpus was analyzed with the Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count software package. Results Results showed that during the acute phase of COVID-19, people felt more certainty about the present than about the future. In particular, the appraisal dimension of certainty influenced the elicitation of positive feelings through the effect of positive anticipatory states. People with high levels of certainty about the future experienced positive feelings more frequently. The results also suggest that people find it easier to focus on the present moment and experience positive feelings rather than try to predict the future and generate positive feelings based on those predictions. Conclusions The study is significant, as it is the first to investigate whether certainty may be a strategy for regulating the specific stressor represented by the COVID-19 pandemic. The results highlighted the importance of certainty in coping with environmental threats and stressors.
Article
Full-text available
Two studies examined how contextual cues influence the impact of receiving instructions for improving performance on intrinsic motivation. The authors proposed that whether instruction enhances or decreases motivation depends on the salience of performance goals. Goal salience was proposed to be a function of how an individual defines the activity, which, in turn, may be influenced by contextual features. To test this hypothesis, the authors used a computer game that emphasized fantasy in addition to skill, and they varied the presence of contextual cues highlighting performance. In Study 1, the authors varied the presence of prior performance feedback, and found that instruction decreased interest only when no prior performance feedback (positive or negative) was received. In Study 2, the authors explicitly manipulated contextual salience by describing the activity's goals as either skill- or fantasy-related. Instruction decreased interest in the fantasy-emphasis context, but increased interest in the skill-emphasis context. Furthermore, when instruction matched perceived goals Ss experienced greater positive affect while performing the task. The implications for models of intrinsic motivation are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Motivational and cognitive mediators of the reduced processing of persuasive messages shown by recipients in a positive mood were tested. Ss in positive or neutral moods read strong or weak counterattitudinal advocacies for either a limited time or for as long as they wanted. Under limited exposure conditions, neutral mood Ss showed attitude change indicative of systematic processing, whereas positive mood Ss showed no differentiation of strong and weak versions of the message. When message exposure was unlimited, positive mood Ss viewed the message longer than did neutral mood Ss and systematically processed it rather than relying on persuasion heuristics. These findings replicated with 2 manipulations of mood and 2 different attitude issues. We interpret the results as providing evidence that reduced cognitive capacity to process the message contributes to the decrements shown by positive mood Ss.
Chapter
When individuals freely engage in an activity for its own sake, their behavior is considered intrinsically motivated. In reality, however, very few behaviors occur in a social vacuum. Most people eventually encounter external constraints on their behavior, and these constraints can interfere with intrinsic motivation (Lepper, Greene, & Nisbett, 1973). A common constraint is the external evaluation of performance at an activity—task performance is constantly evaluated in educational, athletic, and professional domains. When teachers, coaches, and supervisors evaluate performance on an interesting and involving activity, their evaluation represents an extrinsic intrusion into what had been an intrinsically motivated activity, and may undermine subsequent intrinsic motivation. The effects of performance evaluation on intrinsic motivation depend on the properties of the evaluative situation, and on characteristics of the individual whose performance is evaluated.