ArticlePDF Available

Expanding Models Testing Media Contributions to Self-Sexualization

SAGE Publications Inc
SAGE Open
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Although everyday exposure to mainstream media is theorized to be associated with women’s self-objectification, empirical tests of this association have yielded mixed results. There are several possible explanations for these mixed results. For example, it may be that mainstream media relate to women’s broader tendency to self-sexualize rather than to self-objectify, that newer forms of media (i.e., social media) should be incorporated into these models, or that these associations may operate indirectly. Accordingly, the current study examines how traditional media and Facebook investment are associated with women’s tendency to self-sexualize and tests whether these associations are mediated by the view of women as sexual objects, internalization of the thin ideal, and perceived pressure to conform to this ideal. Structural equation modeling demonstrated that magazine and Facebook investment predicted only internalization of the thin ideal, whereas reality television and sitcom exposure predicted all three mediators. Media use was indirectly related to self-sexualization through internalization of the thin ideal and the view of women as sexual objects. These results support the value of incorporating broader definitions of self-sexualization, diverse forms of media, and potential mediators into future models.
This content is subject to copyright.
https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244019848905
SAGE Open
April-June 2019: 1 –14
© The Author(s) 2019
DOI: 10.1177/2158244019848905
journals.sagepub.com/home/sgo
Creative Commons CC BY: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License
(http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) which permits any use, reproduction and distribution of
the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages
(https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).
Original Research
Objectification theorists (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997;
McKinley & Hyde, 1996) propose that women in Western
society must navigate a culture that views them not as active
agents but rather as sexual objects, a collection of body parts
whose value is determined by their physical appearance and
sexual attractiveness to others. Over time, women come to
internalize this third-person perspective and engage in self-
objectification, valuing the external, observable characteris-
tics of their body more than the internal, unobservable
characteristics (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998). As part of this
process, women are believed to engage in body surveillance
or body monitoring, whereby they continually assess the
extent to which their external appearance aligns with cultur-
ally valued ideals. The maintenance of this so-called objecti-
fied body consciousness (McKinley & Hyde, 1996) has been
theoretically and empirically linked to reduced awareness of
internal bodily states, reduced flow (i.e., mental focus or
concentration), disordered eating, and sexual dysfunction
(Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997; Moradi & Huang, 2008).
One of the many socialization agents believed to contrib-
ute to women’s development of an objectified body con-
sciousness are the mainstream media, the content of which
frequently equates women’s value as people to their sexual
attractiveness. The power of these portrayals in triggering
self-objectification may come both from their prevalence
and their accessibility. Indeed, sexually objectifying
portrayals have been observed at high rates in primetime and
reality television, in music videos, and in advertising (Aubrey
& Frisby, 2011; Flynn, Park, Morin, & Stana, 2015;
Messineo, 2008; Smith, Choueiti, Prescott, & Pieper, 2012;
Turner, 2011). Moreover, adolescents and emerging adults,
respectively, spend on average 8 and 12 hr consuming media
each day, creating circumstances in which regular exposure
to sexually objectifying images of women is likely chronic
and virtually unavoidable (Rideout, Foehr, & Roberts, 2010;
Ward, Seabrook, Giaccardi, & Zuo, 2015).
However, few studies have actually examined the direct
link between everyday media use and self-objectification
among youth (see Ward, 2016, for a review), and the pattern
of results that has emerged is inconsistent. Within this small
body of literature, self-objectification is typically operation-
alized as either the tendency to surveil one’s own body
(McKinley & Hyde, 1996) or as the tendency to value
observable, appearance-related aspects of the body more
than unobservable, competence-related aspects of the body
848905SGOXXX10.1177/2158244019848905SAGE OpenGrower et al.
research-article20192019
1University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, USA
2University of Leuven, Belgium
Corresponding Author:
Petal Grower, University of Michigan, 530 Church Street, Ann Arbor, MI
48109-1043, USA.
Email: pgrower@umich.edu
Expanding Models Testing Media
Contributions to Self-Sexualization
Petal Grower1, L. Monique Ward1, and Jolien Trekels2
Abstract
Although everyday exposure to mainstream media is theorized to be associated with women’s self-objectification, empirical
tests of this association have yielded mixed results. There are several possible explanations for these mixed results. For
example, it may be that mainstream media relate to women’s broader tendency to self-sexualize rather than to self-objectify,
that newer forms of media (i.e., social media) should be incorporated into these models, or that these associations may
operate indirectly. Accordingly, the current study examines how traditional media and Facebook investment are associated
with women’s tendency to self-sexualize and tests whether these associations are mediated by the view of women as sexual
objects, internalization of the thin ideal, and perceived pressure to conform to this ideal. Structural equation modeling
demonstrated that magazine and Facebook investment predicted only internalization of the thin ideal, whereas reality
television and sitcom exposure predicted all three mediators. Media use was indirectly related to self-sexualization through
internalization of the thin ideal and the view of women as sexual objects. These results support the value of incorporating
broader definitions of self-sexualization, diverse forms of media, and potential mediators into future models.
Keywords
sexualization, objectification, traditional media, social media, mediators
2 SAGE Open
(Noll & Fredrickson, 1998). A few studies have successfully
linked composite measures of media consumption to valuing
body appearance over body competence (Aubrey, 2006b;
Nowatzki & Morry, 2009). Other studies have found associa-
tions between sexually objectifying television content and
valuing body appearance over body competence (Aubrey,
2006a; Vandenbosch, Muise, Eggermont, & Impett, 2015).
Finally, several studies have linked consumption of sexually
objectifying magazines (particularly beauty/fashion maga-
zines or other appearance-oriented magazines) both to valu-
ing body appearance over body competence and to body
surveillance (Aubrey, 2007; Fardouly, Diedrichs, Vartanian,
& Halliwell, 2015; Morry & Staska, 2001; Vandenbosch &
Eggermont, 2012, 2015).
However, there are also studies that have demonstrated
null results. For example, some studies have failed to find a
link between sexually objectifying television and self-
objectification (Fardouly et al., 2015; Slater & Tiggemann,
2015; Vandenbosch & Eggermont, 2012; Zurbriggen,
Ramsey, & Jaworski, 2011) or have linked media exposure to
one component of sexual objectification but not others (i.e.,
valuing body appearance over body competence but not
body surveillance; Aubrey, 2006a, 2007). Other studies have
found inconsistent results when examining fitness magazines
specifically or sexually objectifying magazines more gener-
ally (Morry & Staska, 2001).
One Potential Culprit:
Self-Objectification Versus
Self-Sexualization
There are several possible explanations for the inconsistent
results observed in this literature. One potential explanation
is the fact that these studies emphasize self-objectification
alone as the outcome of interest. Although these studies
build upon prior literature by measuring body surveillance
and self-objectification (operationalized as valuing body
appearance over body competence), it may be that self-
objectification alone does not best capture how women
respond when exposed to sexually objectified women in the
media. Instead, it may be the case that chronic exposure to
sexually objectified portrayals in mainstream media relates
to women’s tendency not only to self-objectify, but also to
their tendency to self-sexualize, which includes elements
that extend above and beyond self-objectification. The
American Psychological Association [APA] Task Force on
the Sexualization of Girls (APA, 2007) defines sexualiza-
tion as occurring when
a person’s value comes only from his or her sexual appeal or
behavior, to the exclusion of other characteristics; OR a person
is held to a standard that equates physical attractiveness
(narrowly defined) with being sexy; OR a person is sexually
objectified—that is, made into a thing for others’ sexual use; OR
sexuality is inappropriately imposed upon a person. (p. 1)
Under this definition, self-objectification is just one compo-
nent of self-sexualization, which may more accurately repre-
sent how women respond to chronic exposure to sexual
objectification in the media. Women may not only learn to
self-objectify from the media but may also internalize mes-
sages about how their sex appeal contributes to their sense of
self-worth. For this reason, it is possible that studies examin-
ing self-sexualization may find more consistent links than
those examining self-objectification alone.
Two measures commonly employed in the examination
of this broader construct of self-sexualization are the
Enjoyment of Sexualization Scale (ESS; Liss, Erchull, &
Ramsey, 2011) and the Sexual Appeal subscale of the Self-
Worth Scale (SASW; Gordon & Ward, 2000). Following
the definition of sexualization outlined by the APA Task
Force on the Sexualization of Girls (APA, 2007), these
measures work well to capture both women’s potential
enjoyment in being valued for their sexiness and women’s
valuing of themselves primarily for their sexual appeal to
others. The ESS was designed to assess the extent to which
women seek to and enjoy emphasizing their sexiness, two
messages that are commonly purveyed through main-
stream media. In developing the ESS, the authors found
significant correlations between enjoyment of sexualiza-
tion, body surveillance, self-objectification, and engage-
ment in self-sexualizing behaviors, though these significant
correlations do not exceed .5. Together, these data suggest
that these items reflect similar, but not completely overlap-
ping, constructs.
Similarly, the SASW has been used successfully as part of
a latent variable capturing self-sexualization in three studies
(Manago, Ward, Lemm, Reed, & Seabrook, 2015; Ward,
Seabrook, Grower, Giaccardi, & Lippman, 2018; Ward,
Seabrook, Manago, & Reed, 2016). Items on this scale reflect
the extent to which an individual’s self-worth is grounded in
her or his romantic or sexual appeal to others. In each study
in which it was used, correlations between SASW, ESS, and
Body Surveillance were significant and ranged from .41 to
.50. Again, the fact that correlations between these variables
did not exceed .5 suggests that this scale may be capturing
some unique component of self-sexualization.
Accordingly, we argue that the existing literature provides
support for using these scales together to assess self-
sexualization, providing a means to better understand how
women may internalize different media messages and how
these media messages may influence women’s beliefs about
themselves and their bodies. These multiple measures serve
as a way of capturing the multi-dimensional construct of
self-sexualization. If models assessing self-sexualization
rather than self-objectification were to find more consistent
results, this outcome would lend credence to the notion that
including aspects of sexualization is an important next step
in clarifying and understanding the link between everyday
mainstream media consumption and women’s feelings about
their bodies.
Grower et al. 3
A Second Potential Culprit: Traditional
Media Versus Social Media
The second potential explanation for the inconsistent results
found in the existing literature is the conceptualization and
measurement of the media involved. Specifically, it may be
important to move beyond measures of activity on social net-
working sites (SNS) and instead consider the extent to which
a person is invested or involved with social media. Social
media are increasingly vital components of individuals’
media diets. At the time of data collection, Facebook was the
most popular SNS among teens and young adults; estimates
indicate that among 18- to 29-year-old women, 87% used
Facebook in 2014 (Duggan, Ellison, Lampe, Lenhart, &
Madden, 2015; Lenhart, 2015). More recent data show that
43% of teens use Facebook, with 15% reporting that they use
it the most compared with several other SNS (Common
Sense Media, 2018). Not only is social media use prevalent,
but these media differ fundamentally from traditional media
in their interactivity. Users not only consume but also con-
struct their social media environments, suggesting that these
media may have a particularly potent impact on women’s
tendency to self-sexualize. Moreover, the “characters” on
social media consist of an individual’s peers, perhaps serving
to reinforce the notion that the ideals presented therein are
particularly relevant for the self to obtain (Perloff, 2014). For
these reasons, users of SNS may be particularly affected by
the content.
Early research in this domain examined whether the amount
of time spent using social media affected women’s feelings
about their body and their tendencies to self-objectify. Here,
findings demonstrated that women who spent more time on
SNS such as Facebook and Myspace reported greater endorse-
ment of the thin ideal, a greater tendency to engage in social
comparison, more body dissatisfaction, and higher levels of
body surveillance when compared with women who spent less
time on SNS (Fardouly & Vartanian, 2015; Holland &
Tiggemann, 2016; Tiggemann & Slater, 2014, 2015, 2017). Of
particular note, some of this research implicated SNS as being
more influential than traditional media in their potential asso-
ciations with self-objectification. For example, Vandenbosch
and Eggermont (2012) found that in a model with both tradi-
tional media and SNS, only the use of SNS was directly related
to self-objectification. Girls in this study who reported view-
ing higher levels of sexually objectifying content on their
feeds also reported higher endorsement of Western beauty ide-
als, a greater valuation of appearance in their self-worth, and
higher levels body surveillance.
Other research has examined how different SNS or differ-
ent features of these SNS might be associated with women’s
self-objectification tendencies (e.g., differential influences
of photo-based SNS vs. text-based SNS; posting photos vs.
status updates; Manago et al., 2015; Mingoia, Hutchinson,
Wilson, & Gleaves, 2017; Saunders & Eaton, 2018). For
example, in the case of photo-based activity, research has
illustrated that women who report engaging in more photo-
heavy activity online also report higher levels of appearance
self-worth, greater endorsement of the thin ideal, higher lev-
els of body shame, a greater tendency to surveil their bodies,
and are more likely to incorporate self-sexualizing elements
in their own photos (based on profiles created in the context
of an experiment; R. Cohen, Newton-John, & Slater, 2017;
de Vries & Peter, 2013; Salomon & Brown, 2018). Women
who reported viewing a lot of photos on SNS also reported
greater weight dissatisfaction, stronger endorsement of the
thin ideal, and higher levels self-objectification (Meier &
Gray, 2014). Other work has shown that using SNS to moni-
tor attractive peers, or having appearance-oriented conversa-
tions with peers, is linked to adolescents’ self-reported
tendency to self-objectify, to surveil their bodies, and to self-
sexualize (Trekels, Ward, & Eggermont, 2018; Vandenbosch
& Eggermont, 2015).
This small body of work lends supports to the idea that
SNS may provide particularly relevant models of sexually
objectified women, even more so than other types of media.
Given this pattern of results and the unique way in which
women can engage with social media, we feel that it is
important to incorporate not only social media usage but also
social media investment into models linking media use, self-
objectification, and self-sexualization. By including diverse
forms of media, researchers may better capture individuals’
real-world media diets and thus construct a richer under-
standing of how exposure to sexually objectifying media
relates to individuals’ tendencies to self-sexualize.
Third Potential Culprit: Direct
Versus Mediated Connections
As a final possible explanation for the inconsistent results in
the current literature, we argue that it is possible that associa-
tions between women’s media use and their tendency to self-
sexualize may operate indirectly; because most studies have
focused on direct effects models, they may not be fully cap-
turing the process at hand. There is both theoretical and
empirical precedent in the literature to suggest that the link
between media use and other body image outcomes (e.g.,
body dissatisfaction) may involve several potential media-
tors, including body-related cognitions and affect. One medi-
ator proposed is women’s internalization of the thin ideal
(Rousseau, Beyens, Eggermont, & Vandenbosch, 2017;
Vandenbosch & Eggermont, 2015). Thin-ideal internaliza-
tion has been defined as the extent to which women are
aware of the importance placed on appearance in Western
cultures and internalize these beliefs by endorsing and desir-
ing to emulate these social standards (Groesz, Levine, &
Murnen, 2002; Thompson & Stice, 2001).
One common model examining the potential mediating
role of thin-ideal internalization is the three-step process of
self-objectification proposed by Vandenbosch and Eggermont
(2012). These authors propose that women consider the thin
4 SAGE Open
ideal presented in the media as an appropriate standard for
their own appearance and come to value the observable,
appearance-related aspects of their body more than the unob-
servable, competence-related aspects of their body. This
internalizing of the thin ideal and valuing body appearance
over body competence relates to women’s tendency to sur-
veil their bodies in an ongoing effort to ensure their bodies
are conforming to these media-driven standards of beauty
(Vandenbosch & Eggermont, 2012, 2015). This model has
been supported with empirical data and highlights the poten-
tially vital of role of internalization as a cognitive process
underlying the association between media and self-sexual-
ization, as internalization of the thin ideal was correlated
with almost all types of media assessed. In addition, body
surveillance was seldom directly related to media use and
instead operated through internalization of appearance ide-
als. Together, this work provides empirical evidence that the
media serve as purveyors of the thin ideal, and that the extent
to which women internalize this ideal may serve as a power-
ful mechanism undergirding their tendency to self-objectify
and potentially to self-sexualize.
A second potential mediator is the pressure women per-
ceive to conform to media ideals. Given the way in which
Western culture values the thin ideal, it has been theorized
that women are likely to perceive pressure from various
sources, including the media, their friends, and their fam-
ily, to conform to this ideal (Myers & Crowther, 2007;
Stice, 2002). Research has supported an association
between these perceived sociocultural pressures and body
dissatisfaction both experimentally and in correlational
research (Groesz et al., 2002; Myers & Crowther, 2007;
Stice & Whitenton, 2002). In a similar vein, researchers
have proposed that perceived pressure to conform to
media ideals might contribute to women’s tendency to
self-objectify. It stands to reason that chronic exposure to
sexually objectifying media creates a sense of pressure for
female consumers not only to obtain the bodies presented
in media but also to construct notions of the self that are
similarly reductive (i.e., objectified or sexualized).
Although this association has only been tested empirically
in a few studies (e.g., Tylka & Hill, 2004), the work that
does exist provides preliminary support to the idea that
pressure contributes to women’s tendency to self-objec-
tify. For example, Knauss, Paxton, and Alsaker (2008)
found that greater perceived pressure from the media
(both directly and through greater thin-ideal internaliza-
tion) was linked to adolescents’ tendency to monitor their
bodies. In line with this thinking, we would anticipate that
perceived sociocultural pressure might act as an important
cognitive mechanism mediating the association between
media use and women’s tendencies to self-objectify and
self-sexualize.
A third potential mediator may be women’s view of other
women as sexual objects. Objectification theorists posit that
women observe other women being objectified in society at
large and in the media, and that they then turn this tendency
to objectify upon themselves. Though explicitly stated in
objectification theory, this process of turning the objectifying
gaze onto the self has yet to be empirically tested. This
dynamic is supported not only by objectification theory but
also by cultivation theorists, who would posit that chronic
media exposure cultivates a shared sense of social reality and
social norms (Gerbner & Gross, 1976). Applying cultivation
theory to sexualized media, it is possible that exposure to
sexually objectified women in the media would cultivate a
sense that women are sexual objects. This cultivated belief,
particularly over time, might normalize the expectation that
women objectify themselves. Prior work supports the poten-
tial role of this mediating variable, as different types of media
exposure have demonstrated correlations with women’s ten-
dency to endorse objectifying statements about women
(Gordon, 2008; Hust & Lei, 2008), and with men’s tendency
to objectify women (Ward, Vandenbosch, & Eggermont,
2015). Even preferences for certain types of media (e.g., hip
hop music; explicit sexual content online) seem to cultivate a
particular view of women as sex objects (ter Bogt, Engels,
Bogers, & Kloosterman, 2010). Empirically testing how
women’s thoughts about other women influence their own
tendency to self-objectify and self-sexualize would shed
light on the potential cognitive mechanisms undergirding the
inconsistent media–body links in prior research.
The Current Study
Accordingly, the current study examined associations
between media exposure and young women’s self-
sexualization, operationalized as the extent to which they
report monitoring their appearance, report enjoyment of sexu-
alization by others, and report grounding their self-worth in
their sexual appeal. By including these broader measures of
self-sexualization as opposed to self-objectification alone, we
hoped to better capture the complex ways in which media use
is associated with young adult women’s beliefs, attitudes, and
behaviors toward their own bodies (Ward et al., 2018; Ward
et al., 2016). In addition, the current study examined whether
the link between media use and self-sexualization is statisti-
cally mediated by internalization of the thin ideal, perceived
pressure to achieve these ideals, and the view of other women
as sexual objects. In doing so, we built upon prior theory and
research supporting internalization of the thin ideal as a
mechanism linking media exposure and women’s tendency to
self-objectify and hoped to shed light on other potential cog-
nitive mechanisms potentially undergirding this link. We
chose to include a diverse set of media measures, including
three genres of television, magazine consumption, and
Facebook investment. By assessing facets of young women’s
use of traditional media, print media, and social media, we
hoped to better capture the breadth of young women’s mod-
ern media environment and to investigate whether different
types of media demonstrate particular potency in their
Grower et al. 5
connections to young women’s body image and objectifica-
tion. We hypothesized that:
Hypothesis 1: Heavier exposure to each of the different
types of media would be associated with higher self-
reported internalization of the thin ideal, greater subjec-
tive feelings of pressure to conform to cultural beauty
ideals, and stronger endorsement of the view that other
women are sex objects.
Hypothesis 2: Higher belief that women are sexual
objects, higher internalization of cultural beauty ideals,
and higher perceived pressure to conform to these ideals
would be associated with women’s self-reported self-
sexualization (as assessed by their self-reported tendency
to self-objectify, enjoyment of sexualization, and stronger
grounding of their self-worth in their sex appeal).
In addition, this article examined whether associations
between media use and self-sexualization are most accu-
rately represented as direct relations or as indirect relations.
Although the literature would suggest a mediated association
may be most appropriate, these alternative models were
compared, as they are founded on different theoretical
assumptions about how media use relates to women’s ten-
dency to self-sexualize. Support for a mediated model would
highlight the role of several potential cognitive mechanisms
in the self-objectification process.
Method
Participants
Participants were 663 undergraduate women aged 17 to 24
(M = 18.65, SD = 0.98) years. Regarding their ethnic/racial
identification, 63.7% (n = 422) self-identified as White,
19.0% (n = 126) as Asian, 7.7% (n = 51) as Black or African
American, 5.3% (n = 35) as Latina, 3% (n = 20) as Middle
Eastern, 0.6% (n = 4) as Multiracial, 0.3% (n = 2) as Native
American, and 0.2% (n = 1) as Other. An additional 0.3% (n
= 2) of the sample did not report their race. Participants
came from fairly well-educated backgrounds, reporting on
average that both their mother and father had graduated from
college (M = 16.71 years of schooling for mothers and M =
17.17 years of schooling for fathers).
Religiosity was assessed using three items, each scored on
a 1 (not at all/never) to 5 (very religious/all the time) scale.
Responses to the following three items were averaged: “How
religious are you?” “How often do you go to religious ser-
vices (like church or temple)?” and “How often do you pray?”
The sample was moderately religious, with a mean score fall-
ing slightly above the midpoint (M = 2.69, SD = 1.94).
Sexual experience was assessed with a single question, “How
would you describe your current level of experience with dat-
ing and sexual relationships?” Response options ranged from
0 “just starting out (virgin)” to 10 “have had several sexual
relationships.” Average sexual experience level for the cur-
rent sample was 4.32, roughly corresponding to the response
option “1-2 sexual partners (no longer a virgin).”
Measures
Media exposure. To examine magazine consumption, partici-
pants were asked to indicate how many issues per year
(between 0 and 12) they read of the following 12 monthly
women’s magazines: Allure, Cosmopolitan, Elle, Glamour, In
Style, Marie Claire, Self, Seventeen, Shape, Vanity Fair,
Vogue, and Women’s Health. This selection of magazines was
chosen partly from past research (e.g., Ward et al., 2018) and
partly because it includes lifestyle magazines, fashion maga-
zines, and health and fitness magazines, which may better cap-
ture the breadth of women’s magazine exposure. A mean
score was computed across the 12 magazines (Cronbach's
alpha = .73).
To assess consumption of situation comedies, we pro-
vided participants with a list of 24 situation comedy (sitcom)
programs. These programs represented all sitcoms currently
airing on primetime or syndication on major networks (ABC,
CBS, NBC, FOX, TNT, TBS, and CW) in our market.
Participants indicated how often they watched each sitcom
using the following response options, scored 0 to 3: never, a
little/a few episodes, sometimes/some episodes, and a lot/
most or all episodes. A mean was calculated across the 24
programs.
To assess exposure to reality and drama exposure, we pro-
vided participants with a list of 36 popular lifestyle reality
programs and 16 dramas (e.g., Real Housewives, The
Bachelor, How to Get Away with Murder, Shameless) cur-
rently airing on network TV or basic cable networks (e.g.,
MTV, VH1, Bravo; NBC, Fox). These programs were cho-
sen based on online rankings of popular programs (e.g., tv.
com). As with situation comedies, participants reported how
frequently they had ever viewed the 52 different programs
using the following response options scored 0 to 3: a little/a
few episodes, sometimes/some episodes, and a lot/most or all
episodes. A mean across all 36 reality television programs
and 16 dramas was calculated.
To assess Facebook investment, participants answered six
items regarding their use of Facebook and its importance to
their social life. This scale was adapted from the Facebook
Intensity Scale (Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007), with
two items modified. Participants responded to the items on a
3-point scale, from disagree to agree. A sample item is “I feel
like my social life would suffer if Facebook were shut down.”
A mean score was computed, with higher scores reflecting
greater Facebook investment. Cronbach’s alpha for the cur-
rent sample was .80, slightly higher than the alpha reported in
a prior study (Cronbach's alpha = .76; Manago et al., 2015).
Self-sexualization. Following the work of Ward et al. (2016),
self-sexualization was measured via three scales to capture
6 SAGE Open
both traditional components of self-objectification, such as
body surveillance, as well as components centered more on
women’s prioritizing and valuing of their sexual appeal. The
first scale included in the sexualization latent variable was
the Surveillance subscale of the Objectified Body Conscious-
ness Scale, which examined the extent to which individuals
engage in regular body monitoring. Respondents indicated
their level of agreement with each of eight items (e.g., “Dur-
ing the day, I think about how I look many times”) using a
6-point scale anchored by strongly disagree at 1 and strongly
agree at 6. A mean score was computed such that higher
scores indicate greater body surveillance. This scale was
originally developed and validated among three samples of
undergraduate women and their middle-aged mothers. Cron-
bach’s alpha for the surveillance subscale among the three
samples was .89, .79, and .76, respectively. Similarly, Cron-
bach’s alpha for the current sample was .77.
The second scale included in the sexualization latent vari-
able was the ESS (Liss et al., 2011), which measures the
extent to which individuals attempt to and enjoy emphasiz-
ing their own sexiness. Participants indicated their agree-
ment with eight items using a 6-point scale (1 = strongly
disagree; 6 = strongly agree). Sample items include “I love
to feel sexy” and “I like showing off my body.” Mean scores
were computed such that higher scores indicate greater
enjoyment of sexualization. The ESS was originally devel-
oped and validated among two predominantly White samples
of undergraduate women. The scale demonstrated strong
construct and discriminant validity. Cronbach’s alpha for the
development and validation samples was .85 and .86, respec-
tively. Cronbach’s alpha for the current sample was .88.
The third scale included in the sexualization latent variable
was the SASW (Gordon & Ward, 2000), which assesses the
extent to which participants base their self-worth on their sex-
ual appeal. Participants were first given the prompt “How
would you feel about yourself if . . .” and were asked to indi-
cate whether they would feel better or worse about themselves
in 23 situations, 13 of which reflected their sexual appeal/
attractiveness. Sample items include the following: “You were
asked to be a model for a calendar featuring college students”
and “A car accident left you with a permanent, two-inch scar
on your face.” Responses were indicated using a 7-point scale
anchored by –3 (“Ugh! I would feel really horrible about
myself”) and +3 (“Wow! I would feel really great about
myself”). Higher scores, based on absolute values, reflect
placing a stronger emphasis on sexual appeal in perceptions of
one’s self-worth. Cronbach’s alpha for the 13 items assessing
sexual appeal self-worth was .81. This level of internal consis-
tency is similar to or higher than in previous studies using this
scale (Cronbach’s alpha = .70, .79, and .81, in Manago et al.,
2015; Ward et al., 2018; Ward et al., 2016, respectively).
Potential mediators. To assess their views of other women as
sex objects, participants completed an extended version of the
Women are Sexual Objects Scale (Ward, 2002), which
included 13 items scored on a 6-point scale from 1 (strongly
disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). Sample items include “Women
who wear tight or revealing clothing are asking to be hit on by
men” and “Girls should do whatever they need to (e.g., use
make up, buy attractive clothes, work out) to look good
enough to attract a date/partner.” Items were averaged (Cron-
bach’s alpha = .80) so that higher scores indicated a stronger
endorsement of the belief that women are sex objects.
To assess women’s internalization of the thin ideal, par-
ticipants responded to the nine items of the Internalization-
General subscale of the Sociocultural Attitudes Toward
Appearance Questionnaire (SATAQ-3; Thompson, van den
Berg, Roehrig, Guarda, & Heinberg, 2004). Participants
rated their agreement on a 6-point scale (1 = definitely dis-
agree; 6 = definitely agree) to nine statements including “I
compare my body to the bodies of people who are on TV”
and “I wish I looked like the models in music videos.” Scores
were averaged such that higher scores indicated a greater
internalization of the thin ideal. Cronbach’s alpha for the cur-
rent sample was .93.
To assess women’s feelings of pressure to look like media
ideals, participants completed the seven items from the Pressure
subscale of the SATAQ-3 (Thompson et al., 2004). Participants
rated their agreement on a 6-point scale (1 = definitely disagree;
6 = definitely agree) to eight statements including “I’ve felt
pressure from TV or magazines to diet” and “I’ve felt pressure
from TV and magazines to be thin.” Responses to each item
were averaged (Cronbach’s alpha = .92) such that higher scores
indicated a greater perception of pressure from the media to con-
form to media ideals of beauty. Both subscales from the
SATAQ-3 were developed among a sample of 175 undergradu-
ate women (age range = 17-25 years) and validated among a
sample of 195 undergraduate women (age range = 18-22 years).
Cronbach’s alpha for the first sample was .96 for Internalization-
General and .92 for Pressure. For the second sample, Cronbach’s
alpha was .92 for Internalization-General and .94 for Pressure.
Procedure
Participants were recruited from an introductory psychology
subject pool and received research participation credit in return
for completing the survey. Students completed the survey in
small groups no larger than 10 participants on tablets in an on-
campus research lab. Participants were informed that the study
was interested in examining their media use and beliefs about
social relationships. Other measures were assessed, including
measures related to participants’ sexual agency, gender beliefs,
and mental health, but are not conceptually relevant to the
research questions under examination here. It took approxi-
mately 45 min to complete the full packet of measures.
Results
Descriptive Statistics
Table 1 presents the means, standard deviations, and zero-
order inter-correlations for the key variables in the study.
Grower et al. 7
Taking into consideration the skewness of the media vari-
ables, we conducted Spearman’s correlation test, which is
recommended for data with tailed distributions and outliers
(de Winter, Goslin, & Potter, 2016). The three self-sexualiza-
tion measures (body surveillance, sex appeal self-worth, and
enjoyment of sexualization) were moderately to strongly
correlated, with correlations ranging from .42 to .53. These
moderate-to-strong correlations suggest both conceptual
overlap and conceptual distinction of these constructs in line
with the aforementioned prior research. Concerning the three
mediators and three self-sexualization measures, the stron-
gest correlations were between internalization of the thin
ideal and pressure. A discriminant validity test, which
assesses the extent to which a latent variable is distinct from
other latent variables in the model, showed that there might
be some conceptual overlap between these constructs (i.e.,
the square root of average variance extracted [AVE] in inter-
nalization was equal to its correlation with pressure). This
overlap is likely due to the fact that both are subscales of the
SATAQ-3. In the next step, we checked for vertical collinear-
ity (i.e., collinearity between the mediating variables). We
retrieved tolerance and variance inflation factor (VIF) values
that take into consideration only the effects of these variables
on themselves (Kock & Lynn, 2012). These values did not
exceed 0.2 for tolerance and 4 for VIF, indicating no multi-
collinearity issues between these variables and their indica-
tor items (Kline, 1998).
Testing the Hypothesized Model
The integrative model was tested with structural equation
modeling (AMOS) using the maximum likelihood method.
The model controlled for age and sexual experience by
employing these variables as predictors for all of the
hypothesized endogenous variables. First, multiple imputation
was performed as the bootstrapping method does not allow the
sample to include missing data.1 Overall, 148 respondents
(22.2%) had missing data; 84 of those respondents had only
one missing value that had to be imputed. All variables had
less than 5% missing data. According to skewness and kurtosis
information, all variables, except for magazine exposure, were
normally distributed on a univariate level (i.e., skewness val-
ues did not exceed 2 and kurtosis values did not exceed 7; J.
Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003). However, Mardia’s
coefficient for the data was 36.57 with a critical ratio value of
16.67, indicating that the data were significantly non-normal
on a multivariate level. To resolve this issue, we decided to
perform bootstrapping in AMOS, which is an adequate way to
deal with non-normal data in structural equation modeling
(Nevitt & Hancock, 2001).
First, a measurement model was tested assessing the fit of
the self-sexualization latent variable. The media variables
and mediators (i.e., internalization, perceived pressure, and
objectification of women) were measured with observed
variables (i.e., total scale score). Self-sexualization was mea-
sured as a second-order latent variable comprising the enjoy-
ment of sexualization, body surveillance, and appearance
self-worth. Indicators for the three first-order measures were
first assigned to parcels using the item-to-construct balance
technique (Little et al., 2002). Specifically, a principal com-
ponents analysis was performed for each scale (separately).
SPSS was asked to extract one factor for each scale, and
items were assigned to three parcels based on their factor
loadings. The three highest factor loadings were distributed
across the three parcels followed by the next three highest
factor loadings until all items were assigned to one of the
three parcels. The measurement model showed a good fit of
the data, χ2(24) = 84.244, p < .001, root mean square error
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics and Zero-Order Correlations.
1234567891011
1. Magazines 1 .30** .17** .21** .11** .17** .22** .08* .18** .16** .16**
2. Reality television 1 .24** .27** .11** .19** .21** .10** .23** .25** .18**
3. Sitcoms 1 .30** –.01 .18** .16** –.13** .09* .11** .03
4. Dramas 1 .06 .11** .15** .06 .22** .21** .10**
5. Facebook investment 1 .11** .12** .03 .19** .17** .26**
6. Perceived pressure 1 .70** .05 .22** .41** .29**
7. Internalization of
appearance ideals
1 .18** .35** .55** .41**
8. Objectification of women 1 .35** .23** .22**
9. Enjoyment of sexualization 1 .42** .51**
10. Body surveillance 1 .53**
11. Appearance self-worth 1
Range 0-12 0-3 0-3 0-3 0-3 1-5 1-5 1-6 1-6 1-6 1-7
M (SD) for entire sample 0.55
(0.81)
0.64
(0.46)
0.75
(0.41)
0.79
(0.44)
1.15
(0.50)
3.44
(0.99)
3.36
(0.93)
2.86
(0.68)
3.99
(0.88)
4.23
(0.76)
1.67
(0.47)
*p < .05. **p < .01.
8 SAGE Open
of approximation (RMSEA) = .062, comparative fit index
(CFI) = .982.
Second, the hypothesized structural model was tested and
showed an adequate fit of the data (Figure 1). The model
yielded a chi-square value of 363.633 with 110 degrees of
freedom, p < .001, RMSEA = .059, CFI = .946, and χ2/df
= 3.306. The results indicated that 7.7% of the variance of
internalization of appearance ideals, 7.1% of the variance of
pressure to conform to media ideals, and 4.3% of the view of
women as sex objects were explained by media exposure.
Moreover, 58.5% of the variance of self-sexualization could
be explained by media exposure, internalization of appear-
ance ideals, pressure, and objectification of women.
Magazine exposure was only marginally significantly
related to the objectification of women, but was significantly
related to pressure, and internalization of appearance ideals.
Both sitcom exposure and reality TV exposure were signifi-
cantly related to the view of women as sex objects, pressure to
conform to media ideals, and thin-ideal internalization.
Interestingly, drama program exposure was unrelated to all
three proposed mediators. Finally, Facebook investment was
significantly associated with both perceived pressure to con-
form to media ideals and with thin-ideal internalization, but
unrelated to the view of women as sex objects.
User-defined estimands were created to examine the indi-
rect effects. The relative contribution of each mediator (i.e.,
internalization, perceived pressure, and objectification of
women) to the indirect relation between media exposure and
self-sexualization is presented in Table 2. Both magazine
exposure and Facebook investment had a significant indirect
effect on self-sexualization through internalization of appear-
ance ideals, whereas reality television use had a significant
indirect effect on self-sexualization through both internaliza-
tion of appearance ideals and through cultivating the view of
women as sex objects. These results indicate that the stron-
gest mediation was through internalization.
Alternative model. To better understand the role of mediators in
the relation between media exposure and self-sexualization,
we also tested a model that did not include the three mediators
(see Figure 2). This model also showed an adequate fit,
χ2 (80) = 215.03, p < .001, RMSEA = .051, CFI = .964,
χ2/df = 2.689; subsequently, we performed a nested model
comparison test. An unconstrained model (i.e., a model in
which the relation between the media variables and self-
sexualization was mediated by internalization, perceived pres-
sure, and objectification of women) was compared with a con-
strained model (i.e., a model in which the paths between the
media variables, the mediators, and self-sexualization were set
to zero and thus not estimated). This chi-square difference test
showed that the model involving the mediators had a signifi-
cantly better fit (Δχ2 = 326.795, p < .001, df = 18).
Discussion
The current study investigated associations between diverse
forms of media and women’s tendency to self-sexualize. We
extended prior research by including an array of media
Figure 1. Structural equation model (N = 663).
Note. — Significant path (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001); - - - Insignificant path (^p = .049). For clarity, error terms, covariances, and measurements are
not shown. Values reflect standardized coefficients.
Grower et al. 9
measures (three genres of television, magazines, and social
media), while also including broader measures of self-sexual-
ization. The current study also examined the role of three
potential mediating mechanisms: internalization of the thin
ideal, perceived sociocultural pressure to conform to the thin
ideal, and the view of women as sex objects. Results demon-
strate that women who reported more engagement with maga-
zines and investment in Facebook also reported higher
internalization of the thin ideal and greater perceived pressure
to conform to the thin ideal. Women who report greater expo-
sure to reality television also reported higher internalization of
the thin ideal, higher perceived pressure to conform to the thin
ideal, and higher endorsement of the view of women as sex
objects compared with women who reported lower levels of
reality television exposure. Greater sitcom exposure was also
related to greater internalization of the thin ideal and pressure
to conform to the thin ideal, but was related to lower endorse-
ment of the view of women as sex objects. Dramas were not
associated with any of the mediators examined. Of these var-
ied media sources, magazines and Facebook investment were
indirectly related to self-sexualization through internalization
of the thin ideal, whereas reality television was indirectly
related to self-sexualization through internalization of the thin
ideal and the view of women as sex objects. In turn, the view
of women as sex objects significantly related to self-sexualiza-
tion, as did internalization of appearance ideals; perceived
pressure did not.
Our findings contribute to the existing body of research in
several meaningful ways. First, prior studies examining links
between media use and women’s tendency to self-sexualize
have generally examined traditional or social media sepa-
rately and have not considered how women may be uniquely
invested with social media. Second, past research has often
examined self-objectification without consideration of other
components of women’s bodily experiences in the world. By
focusing on women’s tendency to self-sexualize more broadly
rather than their tendency to self-objectify alone, we advance
the understanding of how sexualized depictions of women in
the media are associated with women’s self-reported view of
their own bodies and sense of self. Third, by including more
Table 2. Test of Mediation for Specific Indirect Effects.
βp-value SE 95% CI %
Facebook investment > self-sexualization
Total effect .258 .003 .040 [.178, .332]
Direct effect .208 .002 .034 [.143, .275]
Indirect effect .050* .023 .022 [.006, .098]
Pressure .002 .656 .006 [–.010, .018] 3.39
Internalization .055* .012 .022 [.012, .103] 93.22
Objectification of women .002 .855 .012 [–.021, .026] 3.39
Reality > self-sexualization
Total effect .191 .002 .044 [.102, .275]
Direct effect .099 .014 .038 [.022, .170]
Indirect effect .092** .002 .025 [.040, .141]
Pressure .003 .657 .008 [–.012, .022] 2.54
Internalization .075** .009 .028 [.020, .134] 63.56
Objectification of women .041** .003 .015 [.014, .074] 34.74
Magazines > self-sexualization
Total effect .102 .015 .042 [.022, .187]
Direct effect .019 .602 .037 [–.047, .092]
Indirect effect .084** .002 .024 [.037, .133]
Pressure .001 .607 .003 [–.005, .010] 1.64
Internalization .046*** .001 .015 [.020, .079] 75.41
Objectification of women .014* .032 .008 [.002, .033] 22.95
Sitcoms > self-sexualization
Total effect .010 .789 .043 [–.247, .097]
Direct effect .000 .969 .038 [–.071, .072]
Indirect effect .010 .671 .026 [–.040, .061]
Drama > self-sexualization
Total effect .107 .010 .045 [.025, .204]
Direct effect .073 .044 .038 [.001, .153]
Indirect effect .033 .152 .025 [–.012, .085]
Note. CI = confidence interval.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
10 SAGE Open
diverse measures of women’s everyday media use and a
broader assessment of self-sexualization, which includes both
behavioral and cognitive components, we may more accu-
rately capture how media use may contribute to women’s
experiences of their bodies. Finally, the inclusion of several
potential mediators in our model contributes to a more
nuanced understanding of the cognitive processes that may
undergird women’s tendencies to self-sexualize. Although
examining direct links between media exposure and self-
objectification or self-sexualization has merit, examining
potential mediators provides insight into the mechanisms
through which exposure to sexualized bodies in the media
may influence women’s view of their own bodies.
In our results, heavier media consumption most consis-
tently predicted thin-ideal internalization and perceived
sociocultural pressure, as all media measures except for expo-
sure to television dramas significantly predicted these media-
tors. In contrast, only sitcoms and reality television cultivated
a more general view of women as sex objects. It may be that
the valuing of thin bodies and pressure to conform to those
ideals are nearly omnipresent in media, but that viewers are
more susceptible to the specific view that women are sex
objects when the women being presented as sex objects are
perceived to be more “real,” as in reality television, but are
less susceptible to the view of women as sex objects when
this objectification occurs in the context of comedy.
It is surprising that dramas did not emerge as a signifi-
cant predictor of any of the proposed mediators. One poten-
tial explanation for these null findings is that some dramas
may actually contain less sexually objectifying content than
other TV genres, or that consumption of certain programs
(e.g., Law & Order: SVU) may actually cultivate women’s
sense of empowerment (Hust, Marett, Lei, Ren, & Ran,
2015; Kim et al., 2007). For example, women in Hust
et al.’s (2015) study who frequently watched Law & Order:
SVU reported increased intentions to request consent in
their sexual encounters and endorsed rape myths less
strongly than those who reported less frequent viewing of
Law & Order: SVU. This work also highlights another
potential explanation of these null results, which is that dif-
ferent programs in the same genre (Law & Order: SVU ver-
sus NCIS) may have opposing effects on participants’
beliefs. Some programs may serve to empower women and
to diminish their view of themselves as sexual objects,
whereas other programs may serve to promote and rein-
force these messages.
Concerning indirect effects, both magazines and Facebook
investment acted indirectly on women’s tendency to self-
sexualize through internalization of the thin ideal, whereas
reality television operated through internalization of the thin
ideal and the view of women as sex objects to influence
women’s tendency to self-sexualize. The associations
between different types of media use and internalization of
the thin ideal are consistent with Vandenbosch and
Eggermont’s (2012) three-step process of self-objectifica-
tion. There were no indirect associations between sitcoms or
dramas and self-sexualization through any of the mediators,
which is also similar to Vandenbosch and Eggermont’s
Figure 2. Structural equation model (N = 663) for the associations between media use (i.e., Facebook investment, reality television,
dramas, sitcoms, and magazines) and self-sexualization.
Note. — Only significant paths are shown (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001). Values reflect standardized coefficients.
Grower et al. 11
(2012) model, which found no link between primetime tele-
vision and internalization of the thin ideal. Although these
results are not as consistent as expected, they do support the
need for future work to clarify the relation of these mediators
in women’s self-sexualization tendencies, as well as other
potential mediators.
Limitations, Future Directions, and Conclusions
Although the current study had several strengths, it also had
several limitations. The sample in the current study was rela-
tively homogeneous, comprising primarily White young
adult women from educated backgrounds. In addition, the
correlational nature of this work means we are unable to
make causal claims about the associations observed in
our model; it may be that having a strong tendency to self-
sexualize leads women to select particular media to consume
that validate these tendencies. Finally, although we included
assessments of both traditional and social media, the media
assessed in this study may not accurately represent women’s
full media diets. In the time since data collection was com-
pleted, several other social media platforms have risen in
popularity. In addition, these new social media platforms
may have an even greater influence on women’s tendency to
self-objectify or to self-sexualize, as they are image- rather
than text-based (e.g., Instagram). Though content analyses
do support that even more text-based SNS contain objecti-
fied content (e.g., through profile pictures), image-based
platforms are environments potentially rife with sexualized
self-representations.
Future studies should address these limitations by testing
this mediated model in a more diverse sample of women
(with regard to age and racial composition), by using longi-
tudinal data to verify the directionality of the proposed
model, and by including even more diverse media measures
(e.g., exposure to music genres, movies, or music videos;
other forms of social media). In addition, future work could
also expand on the current model by examining other media-
tors and moderators. For example, future work could exam-
ine the role of different gender ideologies or endorsement of
sexual scripts as potential mediators in this media–body
association. Although common in multivariate analyses (e.g.,
Hair, Black, Babin, & Anderson, 2009; Kock & Lynn, 2012),
the Internalization and Pressure scales—both derived from
the SATAQ 3—were highly correlated but not collinear.
High correlations between subscales could suggest that par-
ticipants may not actually differentiate between constructs
that researchers theoretically consider to be distinct (Hyde,
1998) and should, therefore, be handled cautiously in future
studies. In the realm of moderators, perceived realism, iden-
tification with characters in the media, and motives for view-
ing, has each been implicated in prior work examining media
effects (e.g., Gordon, 2008; Ward & Carlson, 2013). Women
who perceive television content as being highly realistic,
who identify strongly with characters in the media, or who
use media to learn about how the world operates may be par-
ticularly prone to self-sexualize themselves, as they view
media as a more appropriate model on which to base their
own behaviors.
The results of the current study have several implications
for the lives of women. These results suggest that chronic
exposure to various types of media may influence women’s
beliefs both about themselves (i.e., internalization of the thin
ideal and perceived pressure to conform to this ideal) and
about other women (i.e., the view of other women as sex
objects). Taken together, these findings highlight how expo-
sure to sexualized portrayals of women in the media is linked
with the ways in which women act to feel worthy. The reduc-
tion in women’s value to aspects of the self that are external,
to the exclusion of other aspects of the self, by definition
precludes women the possibility of navigating the social
world as agents. The current study builds our understanding
of the cognitive processes underlying media’s associations
with women’s self-sexualization tendencies by identifying
several potential mechanisms. Accordingly, these findings
may prove useful for developing media literacy programs or
other interventions targeting these mechanisms, intervening
both prior to and in the process of women’s problematic cog-
nitions before they manifest in the form of reduction via
self-sexualization.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect
to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding
The author(s) received no financial support for the research,
authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Note
1. In multiple imputation, each missing value is replaced by a
simulated value and m plausible alternative versions of the
complete dataset are generated. In our study, nine new datasets
were generated and we tested our hypothesized model with
each dataset. These analyses did not significantly differ and
the model that is reported in the article was analyzed with the
first dataset that was obtained from the MI method.
ORCID iD
Petal Grower https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0434-7645
References
American Psychological Association. (2007). Report of the APA
task force on the sexualization of girls. Washington, DC:
Author. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/pi/%20women/
programs/girls/report.aspx
Aubrey, J. S. (2006a). Effects of sexually objectifying media on
self-objectification and body surveillance in undergraduates:
Results of a 2-year panel study. Journal of Communication, 56,
366-386. doi:10.1111/jcom.2006.56.issue-2
12 SAGE Open
Aubrey, J. S. (2006b). Exposure to sexually objectifying media and
body self-perceptions among college women: An examination
of the selective exposure hypothesis and the role of moderating
variables. Sex Roles, 55, 159-172. doi:10.1007/s11199-006-
9070-7
Aubrey, J. S. (2007). The impact of sexually objectifying media
exposure on negative body emotions and sexual self-
perceptions: Investigating the mediating role of body self-
consciousness. Mass Communication and Society, 10, 1-23.
doi:10.1080/15205430709337002
Aubrey, J. S., & Frisby, C. M. (2011). Sexual objectification in
music videos: A content analysis comparing gender and genre.
Mass Communication and Society, 14, 475-501. doi:10.1080/
15205436.2010.513468
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied
multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sci-
ences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Cohen, R., Newton-John, T., & Slater, A. (2017). The relationship
between Facebook and Instagram appearance-focused activi-
ties and body image concerns in young women. Body Image,
23, 183-187. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.10.002
Common Sense Media. (2018). Social media, social life: Teens
reveal their experiences. Retrieved from https://www.com-
monsensemedia.org/research/social-media-social-life-2018
de Vries, D. A., & Peter, J. (2013). Women on display: The effect
of portraying the self online on women’s self-objectification.
Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 1483-1489. doi:10.1016/j.
chb.2013.01.015
de Winter, J. C. F., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2016). Comparing
the Pearson and Spearman correlation coefficients across
distributions and sample sizes: A tutorial using simulations
and empirical data. Psychological Methods, 21, 273-290.
doi:10.1037/met0000079
Duggan, M., Ellison, N., Lampe, C., Lenhart, A., & Madden, M.
(2015). Demographics of key social networking platforms.
Washington, DC: Pew Research Center.
Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits
of Facebook “friends”: Social capital and college students’
use of online social network sites. Journal of Computer-
Mediated Communication, 12, 1143-1168. doi:10.1111/j.1083-
6101.2007.00367.x
Fardouly, J., Diedrichs, P. C., Vartanian, L. R., & Halliwell, E.
(2015). The mediating role of appearance comparisons in the
relationship between media usage and self-objectification in
young women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 39, 447-457.
doi:10.1177/0361684315581841
Fardouly, J., & Vartanian, L. R. (2015). Negative comparisons
about one’s appearance mediate the relationship between
Facebook usage and body image concerns. Body Image, 12,
82-88. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.10.004
Flynn, M. A., Park, S. Y., Morin, D. T., & Stana, A. (2015). Anything but
real: Body idealization and objectification of MTV docusoap char-
acters. Sex Roles, 72, 173-182. doi:10.1007/s11199-015-0464-2
Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory:
Toward understanding women’s lived experiences and men-
tal health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173-206.
doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1997.tb00108.x
Gerbner, G., & Gross, L. (1976). Living with television: The
violence profile. Journal of Communication, 26, 172-194.
doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.1976.tb01397.x
Gordon, M. K. (2008). Media contributions to African American
girls’ focus on beauty and appearance: Exploring the con-
sequences of sexual objectification. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 32, 245-256. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.2008.00433.x
Gordon, M. K., & Ward, L. M. (2000, March). I’m beautiful, there-
fore I’m worthy: Assessing associations between media use and
adolescents’ self-worth. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting
of the Society for Research on Adolescence, Chicago, IL.
Groesz, L. M., Levine, M. P., & Murnen, S. K. (2002). The effect
of experimental presentation of thin media images on body
satisfaction: A meta-analytic review. International Journal of
Eating Disorders, 31, 1-16. doi:10.1002/eat.10005
Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E. (2009).
Multivariate data analysis. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice
Hall.
Holland, G., & Tiggemann, M. (2016). A systematic review of the
impact of the use of social networking sites on body image
and disordered eating outcomes. Body Image, 17, 100-110.
doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.02.008
Hust, S., & Lei, M. (2008). Sexual objectification, sports program-
ming, and music television. Media Report to Women, 36, 16-23.
Hust, S. J., Marett, E. G., Lei, M., Ren, C., & Ran, W. (2015).
Law & Order, CSI, and NCIS: The association between
exposure to crime drama franchises, rape myth acceptance,
and sexual consent negotiation among college students.
Journal of Health Communication, 20(12), 1369-1381. doi:
10.1080/10810730.2015.1018615
Hyde, S. (1998). Measuring feminist attitudes: A possible rap-
prochement between feminist theory and empirical data?
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 22, 361-362.
Kim, J. L., Sorsoli, C. L., Collins, K., Zylbergold, B. A.,
Schooler, D., & Tolman, D. A. (2007). From sex to sexual-
ity: Exposing the hetero- sexual script on primetime tele-
vision network. Journal of Sex Research, 44, 145-157.
doi:10.1080/00224490701263660
Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation
modeling. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Knauss, C., Paxton, S. J., & Alsaker, F. D. (2008). Body dissatisfac-
tion in adolescent boys and girls: Objectified body conscious-
ness, internalization of the media body ideal and perceived
pressure from media. Sex Roles, 59, 633-643. doi:10.1007/
s11199-008-9474-7
Kock, N., & Lynn, G. S. (2012). Lateral collinearity and misleading
results in variance-based SEM: An illustration and recommen-
dations. Journal of the Association for Information Systems,
13, 546-580.
Lenhart, A. (2015). Teens, social media, and technology overview,
2015. Washington, DC: Pew Internet.
Liss, M., Erchull, M. J., & Ramsey, L. R. (2011). Empowering or
oppressing? Development and exploration of the enjoyment
of sexualization scale. Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 37, 55-68. doi:10.1177/0146167210386119
Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K.
F. (2002). To parcel or not to parcel: Exploring the question,
weighing the merits. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 151-173.
doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_1
Manago, A. M., Ward, L. M., Lemm, K. M., Reed, L., & Seabrook,
R. (2015). Facebook involvement, objectified body conscious-
ness, body shame, and sexual assertiveness in college women
and men. Sex Roles, 72, 1-14. doi:10.1007/s11199-014-0441-1
Grower et al. 13
McKinley, N. M., & Hyde, J. S. (1996). The objectified body con-
sciousness scale development and validation. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 20, 181-215. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1996.
tb00467.x
Meier, E. P., & Gray, J. (2014). Facebook photo activity asso-
ciated with body image disturbance in adolescent girls.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 17, 199-
206. doi:10.1089/cyber.2013.0305
Messineo, M. J. (2008). Does advertising on black entertain-
ment television portray more positive gender representations
compared to broadcast networks? Sex Roles, 59, 752-764.
doi:10.1007/s11199-008-9470-y
Mingoia, J., Hutchinson, A. D., Wilson, C., & Gleaves, D. C.
(2017). The relationship between social networking site use and
the internalization of a thin ideal in females: A meta-analytic
review. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, Article 1351. doi:10.3389/
fpsyg.2017.01351
Moradi, B., & Huang, Y. P. (2008). Objectification theory and psy-
chology of women: A decade of advances and future directions.
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32, 377-398. doi:10.1111/j.
1471-6402.2008.00452.x
Morry, M. M., & Staska, S. L. (2001). Magazine exposure:
Internalization, self-objectification, eating attitudes, and body sat-
isfaction in male and female university students. Canadian Journal
of Behavioural Science, 33, 269-279. doi:10.1037/h0087148
Myers, T. A., & Crowther, J. H. (2007). Sociocultural pressures,
thin-ideal internalization, self-objectification, and body dissat-
isfaction: Could feminist beliefs be a moderating factor? Body
Image, 4, 296-308. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2007.04.001
Nevitt, J., & Hancock, G. R. (2001). Performance of bootstrapping
approaches to model test statistics and parameter standard error
estimation in structural equation modeling. Structural Equation
Modeling, 8, 353-377. doi:10.1207/S15328007SEM0803_2
Noll, S. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). A mediational model
linking self-objectification, body shame, and disordered
eating. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 22, 623-636.
doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1998.tb00181.x
Nowatzki, J., & Morry, M. M. (2009). Women’s intentions regard-
ing, and acceptance of, self-sexualizing behavior. Psychology
of Women Quarterly, 33, 95-107. doi:10.1111/j.1471-
6402.2008.01477.x
Perloff, R. M. (2014). Social media effects on young women’s body
image concerns: Theoretical perspectives and an agenda for
research. Sex Roles, 71, 363-377. doi:10.1007/s11199-014-0384-6
Rideout, V. J., Foehr, U. G., & Roberts, D. F. (2010). Generation
M 2: Media in the lives of 8- to 18-year-olds. Henry J. Kaiser
Family Foundation. Retrieved from https://www.kff.org/other/
event/generation-m2-media-in-the-lives-of/
Rousseau, A., Beyens, I., Eggermont, S., & Vandenbosch, L.
(2017). The dual role of media internalization in adolescent
sexual behavior. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 46, 1685-1697.
doi:10.1007/s10508-016-0902-4
Salomon, I., & Brown, C. S. (2019). The selfie generation:
Examining the relationship between social media use and early
adolescent body image. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 39,
539-560. doi:10.1177/0272431618770809
Saunders, J. F., & Eaton, A. A. (2018). Snaps, selfies, and shares:
How three popular social media platforms contribute to the
sociocultural model of disordered eating among young women.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 21, 343-
354. doi:10.1089/cyber.2017.0713
Slater, A., & Tiggemann, M. (2015). Media exposure, extracurricu-
lar activities, and appearance-related comments as predictors of
female adolescents’ self-objectification. Psychology of Women
Quarterly, 39, 375-389. doi:10.1177/0361684314554606
Smith, S. L., Choueiti, M., Prescott, A., & Pieper, K. (2012).
Gender roles & occupations: A look at character attributes
and job-related aspirations in film and television. Geena David
Institute on Gender in Media. Retrieved from https://seejane.
org/wp-content/uploads/key-findings-gender-roles-2013.pdf
Stice, E. (2002). Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology:
A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 825-848.
doi:10.1037//0033-2909.128.5.825
Stice, E., & Whitenton, K. (2002). Risk factors for body dissat-
isfaction in adolescent girls: A longitudinal investigation.
Developmental Psychology, 38, 669-678. doi:10.1037//0012-
1649.38.5.669
ter Bogt, T. F., Engels, R. C., Bogers, S., & Kloosterman, M.
(2010). “Shake it baby, shake it”: Media preferences, sexual
attitudes and gender stereotypes among adolescents. Sex Roles,
63, 844-859. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9815-1
Thompson, J. K., & Stice, E. (2001). Thin-ideal internaliza-
tion: Mounting evidence for a new risk factor for body
image disturbance and eating pathology. Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 10, 181-183. doi:10.1111/1467-
8721.00144
Thompson, J. K., van den Berg, P., Roehrig, M., Guarda, A. S., &
Heinberg, L. J. (2004). The Sociocultural Attitudes Towards
Appearance Scale-3 (SATAQ-3): Development and valida-
tion. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35, 293-304.
doi:10.1002/eat.10257
Tiggemann, M., & Slater, A. (2014). NetTweens: The internet and
body image concerns in preteenage girls. The Journal of Early
Adolescence, 34, 606-620. doi:10.1177/0272431613501083
Tiggemann, M., & Slater, A. (2015). The role of self-objectification
in the mental health of early adolescent girls: Predictors and
consequences. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 40, 704-711.
doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsv021
Tiggemann, M., & Slater, A. (2017). Facebook and body image
concern in adolescent girls: A prospective study. International
Journal of Eating Disorders, 50, 80-83. doi:10.1002/eat.22640
Trekels, J., Ward, L. M., & Eggermont, S. (2018). I “like” the way
you look: How appearance-focused and overall Facebook use
contribute to adolescents’ self-sexualization. Computers in
Human Behavior, 81, 198-208. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2017.12.020
Turner, J. S. (2011). Sex and the spectacle of music videos: An
examination of the portrayal of race and sexuality in music vid-
eos. Sex Roles, 64, 173-191. doi:10.1007/s11199-010-9766-6
Tylka, T. L., & Hill, M. S. (2004). Objectification theory as it
relates to disordered eating among college women. Sex Roles,
51, 719-730. doi:10.1007/s11199-004-0721-2
Vandenbosch, L., & Eggermont, S. (2012). Understanding sexual
objectification: A comprehensive approach toward media expo-
sure and girls’ internalization of beauty ideals, self-objectification,
and body surveillance. Journal of Communication, 62, 869-887.
doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01667.x
Vandenbosch, L., & Eggermont, S. (2015). The role of mass media
in adolescents’ sexual behaviors: Exploring the explanatory
value of the three-step self-objectification process. Archives of
Sexual Behavior, 44, 729-742. doi:10.1007/s10508-014-0292-4
Vandenbosch, L., Muise, A., Eggermont, S., & Impett, E. A. (2015).
Sexualizing reality television: Associations with trait and state
14 SAGE Open
self-objectification. Body Image, 13, 62-66. doi:10.1016/j.
bodyim.2015.01.003
Ward, L. M. (2002). Does television exposure affect emerging
adults’ attitudes and assumptions about sexual relationships?
Correlational and experimental confirmation. Journal of Youth
and Adolescence, 31, 1-15. doi:10.1023/A:1014068031532
Ward, L. M. (2016). Media and sexualization: State of empirical
research from 1995-2015. Journal of Sex Research, 53, 560-577.
Ward, L. M., & Carlson, C. (2013). Modeling meanness:
Associations between reality TV consumption, perceived real-
ism, and adolescents’ social aggression. Media Psychology, 16,
371-389. doi:10.1080/15213269.2013.832627
Ward, L. M., Seabrook, R., Giaccardi, S., & Zuo, A. (2015).
Television uses and effects in emerging adulthood. In J. Arnett
(Ed.), Oxford handbook of emerging adulthood (pp. 364-381).
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Ward, L. M., Seabrook, R. C., Grower, P., Giaccardi, S., &
Lippman, J. R. (2018). Sexual object or sexual subject: Media
use, self-sexualization, and sexual agency among undergradu-
ate women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 42, 29-43.
doi:10.1177/0361684317737940
Ward, L. M., Seabrook, R. C., Manago, A., & Reed, L. (2016).
Contributions of diverse media to self-sexualization among
undergraduate women and men. Sex Roles, 74, 12-23.
doi:10.1007/s11199-015-0548-z
Ward, L. M., Vandenbosch, L., & Eggermont, S. (2015). The
impact of men’s magazines on adolescent boys’ objectifi-
cation and courtship beliefs. Journal of Adolescence, 39,
49-58.
Zurbriggen, E. L., Ramsey, L. R., & Jaworski, B. K. (2011). Self-and
partner-objectification in romantic relationships: Associations
with media consumption and relationship satisfaction. Sex
Roles, 64, 449-462. doi:10.1007/s11199-011-9933-4
Author Biographies
Petal Grower is a developmental psychology PhD candidate at the
University of Michigan. Her research focuses on the impact of self-
objectification and self-sexualization on women’s sexual agency.
She is also interested in the conceptualization and measurement of
these constructs.
L. Monique Ward is a professor of Psychology at the University of
Michigan. Her research examines media contributions to gender
and sexual socialization, with particular emphasis on the impact of
sexually objectifying media content.
Jolien Trekels is a post-doctoral researcher at the School for Mass
Communication at the KU Leuven. Her research interests include
media use during adolescence and its influence on perceptions and
behaviors, with a particular focus on body image.
... Continuing, research has shown that even favorable appearance comments can result in body dissatisfaction and self-objectification in women and girls (Calogero et al., 2009;Rodgers et al., 2009;Tiggemann & Boundy, 2008). Viewing other women in an objectified and/or sexualized way can cultivate norms and expectations about the role of women and how they are valued in society (Grower et al., 2019). Despite the fact that mainstream body positivity encourages all to feel satisfied in their bodies, the focus on appearance and body satisfaction might make dominant sociocultural beauty standards salient and/or encourage self-objectification. Objectifying images of women can accompany both body-neutral and mainstream body-positive messages online. ...
... Our results also suggest that perceived moral appropriateness, or normative acceptability (Friestad & Wright, 1994), of body-positive content functions as a determinant of content effectiveness and influences endorsement of traditional beauty standards. Research suggests that perceived sociocultural pressures to conform to beauty ideals are often associated with body image disturbance in women (Frederick et al., 2022;Groesz et al., 2002;Grower et al., 2019;Thompson et al., 1999;Thompson & Stice, 2001). Experimental work also has shown the influence of social context on shifting body ideals, suggesting that women tend to shift their ideal to match perceived sociocultural body ideals (Bair et al., 2014;Mills et al., 2012). ...
... With this in mind, body neutrality's lack of appearance emphasis coupled with viewers' perceptions that content is normatively acceptable or endorsed (e.g., morally appropriate), may lead them to be more willing to shift their body ideals. Thus, alteration of sociocultural ideals may be contingent upon how approving viewers think others will be (see Grower et al., 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
The body-positive movement is one of many decentralized, user-generated movements on social media that seek to engender positive societal change. In support of the body-positive movement, social media users employ a variety of messages and images to advocate for more inclusive beauty standards across different online platforms. We examine how the nature of body-positive messaging (mainstream body positivity vs body neutrality), the degree to which images are sexualized (sexualized vs non-sexualized), and the platform that hosts body-positive content (Instagram/Flickr/blog) influence how people evaluate body-positive content online. The results indicate that the more participants felt messaging was body-neutral, the more morally appropriate and less self-interested they found the posts. The extent to which participants felt messaging was morally appropriate also led them to embrace more inclusive beauty standards. Moreover, non-sexualized (vs sexualized) images were rated more morally appropriate and less self-interested. Implications for promoting body positivity and other prosocial movements online are discussed.
... Since media -advertisements, reality TV programs, advertising, music videos-and social media portray women stereotypes based on body focus, sexual appeal risk behaviors have been associated with a sexualized aesthetic (Lynch, Tompkins, Van Driel & Fritz, 2016;Ward, Seabrook, Manago & Reed, 2016;McDade-Montez, Wallander & Cameron, 2017;Grower, Ward & Trekels, 2019;Condeza-Dall'Orso, Mattus & Vergara-Leighton, 2021;Glenn Cummins, Harrison Gong & Reichert, 2021;Mensa & Vargas-Bianchi, 2023). Couto, Hust, Rodgers et al. (2022) suggest media literacy education may be beneficial for adolescents to recognize and question the gender stereotypic portrayals in music media after a research of stanzas of songs in the Hot 100 Billboard chart where the most common category was sexual content (n = 222, 28.4%), followed by degrading terms toward women (n = 71, 9%), and violence (n = 55, 7%). ...
... Johnson & Yu (2021) published a recent systematic review from 2007 to 2020 on self-sexualization research, concluding that 31 papers operationalize the term with exposure to media and social media, self-objectification, internalization of sexualization, desiring attention from others and empowerment in a negative sense. The authors continue Grower et al., (2019) intuition that not only media but also women's tendency to self-sexualize influences selfsexualization. In fact, they concluded that media use was indirectly related to self-sexualization through internalization of the thin ideal and the view of women as sexual objects. ...
Article
Full-text available
In the recent report The Impact of Influencers on Advertising and Consumer Protection in the Single Market the European Parliament mentions the use of nudity and sexual content and the role of perceptions and expectations towards body image and perfect images. What most moves consumers' attitudes and behavioral intentions is the credibility, attractiveness (both physical and in terms of familiarity and likability), expertise, trustworthiness, popularity, prestige and high power of the influencers. In Spain two White Papers on influencer marketing indicate that a common form of socialization, especially for girls and women, consists of showing a sexy or a perfect body and projecting an egocentric image. The aim of this research is to analyze the presence of self-objectification in top influencers in Spain and to identify its relationship with either social values of success and recognition or stereotypes. The methodology carried out has been a content analysis of 246 images of the first 14 top influencers that appear in the Report of the 500 most influential men and women influencers in Spain in 2022. From a previous literature review, the analysis has been conducted with a deductive-inductive research paradigm through coding with QSR Nvivo the following categories of self-objectification: (1) sexy self-presentation; (2) appearance-centered attitude towards one's own perfect body - egocentric image; (3) stereotypical gender representation; and (4) success, social recognition, or popularity. The results show that self-objectification is more predominant in women than in men, and that it has an important correspondence with the self-perception of the professions they have as well as with age, being mainly belonging to the Z Generation. Both women and men who self-perceive themselves as having professions in which physical appearance is relevant (models or actors) tend to objectify themselves more. However, success and social recognition are more evident in those male influencers who emphasize their professional facet and do not resort to self-sexualization of their bodies (businessmen, advertisers or youtubers).
... As a consequence, women internalize this thirdperson perspective, valuing the observable characteristics of their body more than the internal, unobservable characteristics. As part of this process, women engage in body surveillance, whereby they constantly assess the extent to which their external appearance is in line with culturally valued ideals [1,2]. ...
... One of the most relevant socialization agents promoting objectification and body surveillance is mainstream media, whose content frequently equates a person's worth with their sexual attractiveness [2]. The power of these representations in triggering body surveillance stems from both their prevalence and accessibility [3]. ...
Article
Full-text available
As part of objectification processes, individuals engage in body surveillance, whereby they constantly assess the extent to which their external appearance conforms to culturally valued ideals. Mass media play a key role in fostering the objectification and internalization of media beauty standards and increases body surveillance. At the individual level, the literature has largely demonstrated that body surveillance leads to a variety of negative psychological outcomes, but little research has focused on the consequences of body surveillance in the context of romantic relationships. Using dyadic data from couples who identified as heterosexual, the present study examined relations among internalization of media standards, body surveillance, surveillance of the partner's body, surveillance from the partner, and relationship satisfaction. There were 438 participants (219 couples) recruited using snowball sampling. They were surveyed with an anonymous online questionnaire. Results showed that internalization of media standards was related to body surveillance in both men and women, and to surveillance of the partner's body and relationship satisfaction in men only. For both sexes, surveillance of the partner's body was negatively associated with relationship satisfaction. For women only, surveillance from the partner was also negatively related to relationship satisfaction. Implications are discussed.
... Our goal was to provide a list of TV programs popular with youth, especially Black, Asian American, and White youth. In constructing this list, we drew on Nielsen reports about digital lives of Asian American adults and families (Nielsen, 2018), published articles on popular programming among U.S. adults with some race-specific data (Levin, 2017), and programs preferred in earlier published work with adolescents (e.g., Grower et al., 2019). We also made sure to include programming that featured leads who were racial minorities (e.g., Basketball Wives, Jane the Virgin, Power). ...
Article
Full-text available
Men’s mainstream media exposure is often associated with traditional gender role endorsement, which in turn predicts negative mental health consequences. However, prior research often tested these associations among predominantly White, adult samples. The present study explored associations between consumption of three media genres that frequently feature men, endorsement of traditional gender roles, and mental health for a sample of Black, Asian American, and White male adolescents. Multigroup modeling indicated significant associations between media consumption and endorsement of traditional gender roles for all three groups and a significant association between traditional gender roles and mental distress among Asian American and White male adolescents. Results suggest that media may be a powerful force in young men’s gender beliefs and mental health regardless of racial background. Continued research is needed to elucidate how familial characteristics, such as mother’s education level, and features of boys’ cultural backgrounds, particularly for Black male adolescents, influence the extent to which traditional gender role endorsement predicts mental distress.
... It was also noted that some viewers had never seen such stereotypes of Asian women while abroad, suggesting that these stereotypes may be more prevalent in certain regions or media markets. The comments also revealed the dating preferences of Asian women with white identities, with some stating that they tend to date people of other races rather than Asian partners [10]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Stereotypes of Asian women have long persisted, portraying them as submissive, docile and over-sexualized individuals. These traits often remain overlooked, despite the fact that as human beings they are rich in unique personalities. Passive racism perpetuates these harmful generalizations, leading to the devaluation of the Asian race as a whole. This study aims to draw attention to this critical issue and to explore alternative perspectives that challenge traditional images of Asian women. To achieve this goal, ten films were analysed in depth to determine whether contemporary media representations continue to reinforce passive, docile images of Asian women. The results of the study show that despite some progress, the worrying trend of over-objectification and societal neglect of Asian women persists. This study delves into the psychology of society’s overemphasis on perpetuating stereotypes and the resulting impact on Asian women’s self-perceptions and social roles. In addition, this study emphasizes the historical roots of prejudice and misogyny that continue to influence Western society’s perception of Asian women.
... rin sa Merriam Webster (2018), nagagamit na ang thirst trap sa isang piguratibong pakahulugan. Maaari na itong tumukoy hindi lamang sa mga larawang probokatibo sa social media kundi maging sa kahit anong bagay na nakaaakit ng atensiyon ng mga tao. Maituturing din ang thirst trap bilang isang halimbawa ng self-sexualization. Ipinaliwanag nina Petal Grower, et. al. (2019) na nagaganap ang self-sexualization sa tuwing pinaiiral ng mga babae sa kanilang sarili ang mga aspekto ng seksuwalisayon sa kanilang mga sarili. Nangyayari ito sa kung tinutumbasan nila ang kanilang pagiging kaakit-akit sa kaseksihan o pag-objectify sa kanilang sarili. Sa konteksto ng pag-aaral ng thirst trap, hindi lang nakakahon ang ...
Article
Full-text available
Itinuturing ang YouTube bilang isang participatory culture. Naging makapangyarihan ang YouTube sa pagbibigay ng bagong plataporma hindi lamang sa mga batikan na sa larangan ng midya kundi maging sa mga baguhan o amateur pa lamang sa paggawa ng mga personal video. Habang patuloy na umuunlad ang mundo ng social media, lalo pang naging laganap ang paggamit ng self-sexualization at nagluwal pa ito ng bagong konsepto na mas kilala ngayon sa tawag na thirst trap. Isang halimbawa ang thirst trap ng isang mas pinalawig na selfie culture, madalas itong isang uri ng larawan na ginagamit upang akitin ang mga tao na purihin ang kanilang seksing katawan. Ang papel na ito ay isang eksploratoryong pagsusuri ng karakterisasyon ng thirst trap ng mga Filipino sa YouTube. Sinuri sa papel na ito ang mga video ng 10 sampung Filipinong thirst trapper sa YouTube. Sa kabuuang pagdalumat ng konsepto ng self-sexualization gamit ang thirst trap sa YouTube hangad ng papel na ito na makapagbukas ng bagong diskurso tungkol sa napapanahong paksang ito. Magagamit ang resulta ng pag-aaral upang mapalawak ang diskurso na may kinalaman sa new media at self-sexualization.
Article
Theoretical work on objectified body consciousness, comprised of body surveillance and body shame, proposes several negative sequelae of holding an objectified view of the self (i.e., valuing the body primarily for its attractiveness to others). Few studies have examined associations between objectified body consciousness and academic beliefs and strategies among adolescent girls, especially girls of color. In the present study, we investigated a conceptual model whereby exposure to Eurocentric appearance norms through TV consumption is related to body surveillance and body shame which, in turn, are related to academic beliefs and strategies among a racially diverse sample of United States girls. Participants were 650 self-identified Asian American/Pacific Islander, Black, and White adolescent girls ( M age = 15.45, SD age = 1.50). Higher Eurocentric appearance norms exposure was associated with higher body shame which, in turn, was associated with maladaptive academic beliefs and strategies for all three groups of girls. In contrast, higher Eurocentric appearance norms exposure was associated with higher body surveillance and just one academic belief, and in White girls only. Future work should continue to examine these associations among adolescent girls of color. In addition, we encourage educators to include body image content in media literacy curricula as a means of disrupting associations between Eurocentric appearance norms, body shame, and academic functioning.
Article
Past research suggests that sexualized women are dehumanized and viewing sexualized images negatively impacts viewers’ body image; however, plus-size women are mostly absent from this research. The current studies investigate how sexualization impacts dehumanization of plus-size women and participants’ body image. In Study 1 (N = 277, Mage = 19.52, SD =1.77) men and women viewed images of plus-size and thin sexualized and non-sexualized women and rated the women on traits linked to dehumanization. Results indicated that sexualized thin targets were perceived as less human than plus-size sexualized and non-sexualized targets. Plus-size sexualized targets were also perceived as less human than plus-size non-sexualized targets. In Study 2 (N = 500, Mage = 18.98, SD = 1.51) we investigated the impact of viewing sexualized images on participants’ feelings about their own body. Results indicated that sexualization, but not body size, impacted women’s objectified body consciousness. Men’s body esteem was impacted by the body size of the image. Perceived race of the image also impacted feelings of body control for both men and women. Taken together these results highlight that sexualization, at any body size, impacts women’s views about themselves and sexualized women, at any body size, are dehumanized.
Article
A prominent feature of mainstream television, especially reality programming, is a heterosexual script that outlines women’s and men’s traditional courtship roles. Although frequent media use is believed to produce greater acceptance of this script, existing analyses have not fully delineated contributions of scripted versus reality programming or tested these notions using a holistic heterosexual script scale. We addressed these limitations in two studies. In Study 1, 466 undergraduate women indicated their support of the heterosexual script and their consumption of popular reality programs, sitcoms, and dramas. Heavier viewing of reality programming predicted greater support for the heterosexual script, and heavier viewing of sitcoms predicted weaker support. In Study 2, we used longitudinal data to explore relations between viewing reality television, acceptance of the heterosexual script, and acceptance of sexualized aggression during undergraduate women’s first 2 years in college ( N = 244). We found that reality television consumption was not a direct predictor of acceptance of sexualized aggression but was a significant, indirect predictor through endorsement of the heterosexual script. These studies contribute to our understanding of unique media contributions to endorsement of the heterosexual script and illuminate one process by which women may come to normalize sexual mistreatment. Campus educational programming on sexuality, sexual assault, and healthy relationships may be able to intervene in this normalization through critique of the heterosexual script and media portrayals of dating and relationships.
Article
Full-text available
The current study aimed to integrate and test the sociocultural model of disordered eating with theories explaining the impact of mass media on the development of disordered eating for users of three popular social networking platforms. Young women social networking site (SNS) users (age 18-24) who had never received an eating disorder diagnosis (N = 637) completed questions capturing their SNS gratifications and usage, body surveillance, social comparisons, body dissatisfaction, and eating pathology. Measures were administered in one online session. Model relationships were similar across users of all three SNS platforms: Facebook, Instagram, and Snapchat. Users of all platforms demonstrated a significant positive relationship between upward comparisons and disordered eating outcomes, and between body surveillance and disordered eating outcomes, although differences between models did emerge. Empirical findings support extending the sociocultural model of disordered eating to include SNS uses and gratifications.
Article
Full-text available
Prior research has related the use of social networking sites (SNSs) to body image disturbances among adolescents, but studies examining how SNSs affect adolescents' engagement in self-sexualizing behaviors are lacking. The current correlational study among 640 adolescents (Mage = 16.27, SD = 1.60) aimed to take a more nuanced look at the influence of SNS use, by examining the influence of talking about appearance-related topics with friends on Facebook in addition to total amount of Facebook use. The findings showed that appearance conversations on Facebook, but not total amount of Facebook use, was directly related to self-sexualizing behaviors. In addition, both self-objectification and the belief that complying with the prevailing appearance ideals is rewarded (with, for example, popularity, increased self-esteem, and romantic success) proved to be valid explanatory mechanisms for the examined relations. Results further showed that both boys and girls were affected by appearance-focused Facebook use, although girls scored higher on the key variables and the model fit better among the girls.
Article
Full-text available
Previous research has indicated that exposure to traditional media (i.e., television, film, and print) predicts the likelihood of internalization of a thin ideal; however, the relationship between exposure to internet-based social media on internalization of this ideal remains less understood. Social media differ from traditional forms of media by allowing users to create and upload their own content that is then subject to feedback from other users. This meta-analysis examined the association linking the use of social networking sites (SNSs) and the internalization of a thin ideal in females. Systematic searches were performed in the databases: PsychINFO, PubMed, Web of Science, Communication and Mass Media Complete, and ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. Six studies were included in the meta-analysis that yielded 10 independent effect sizes and a total of 1,829 female participants ranging in age from 10 to 46 years. We found a positive association between extent of use of SNSs and extent of internalization of a thin ideal with a small to moderate effect size (r = 0.18). The positive effect indicated that more use of SNSs was associated with significantly higher internalization of a thin ideal. A comparison was also made between study outcomes measuring broad use of SNSs and outcomes measuring SNS use solely as a function of specific appearance-related features (e.g., posting or viewing photographs). The use of appearance-related features had a stronger relationship with the internalization of a thin ideal than broad use of SNSs. The finding suggests that the ability to interact with appearance-related features online and be an active participant in media creation is associated with body image disturbance. Future research should aim to explore the way SNS users interact with the media posted online and the relationship linking the use of specific appearance features and body image disturbance.
Article
Full-text available
Sexualizing media content is prevalent in various media types. Sexualizing media messages and portrayals emphasize unattainable body and appearance ideals as the primary components of sexual desirability. The internalization of these ideals is positively related to self-objectification and sexual body consciousness. In turn, self-objectification and sexual body consciousness affect adolescents' sexual behavior, albeit in opposing directions. While objectifying self-perceptions are linked to higher levels of sexual behavior, body consciousness during physical intimacy is linked to lower levels of sexual behavior. Based on this knowledge, the present three-wave panel study of 824 Belgian, predominant heterosexual adolescents (M age = 15.33; SD = 1.45) proposes a dual-pathway model that investigates two different pathways through which the internalization of media ideals may impact adolescents' sexual behavior. An inhibitory pathway links media internalization to lower levels of sexual behavior through sexual body consciousness, and a supportive pathway links media internalization to higher levels of sexual behavior through self-objectification. Structural equation analyses supported the proposed dual-pathway, showing that the impact of media internalization on adolescents' sexual behavior proceeds through an inhibitory pathway and a supportive pathway. Regarding the supportive pathway, media internalization (W1) positively predicted sexual behavior (W3), through valuing appearance over competence (W2). Regarding the inhibitory pathway, media internalization (W1) positively predicted body surveillance, which, in turn, positively predicted sexual body consciousness (all W2). Sexual body consciousness (W2) is negatively related to sexual behavior (W3). From a sexual developmental perspective, these findings emphasize the importance of guiding adolescents in interpreting and processing sexualizing media messages.
Article
Full-text available
The Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient (rp) and the Spearman rank correlation coefficient (rs) are widely used in psychological research. We compare rp and rs on 3 criteria: variability, bias with respect to the population value, and robustness to an outlier. Using simulations across low (N = 5) to high (N = 1,000) sample sizes we show that, for normally distributed variables, rp and rs have similar expected values but rs is more variable, especially when the correlation is strong. However, when the variables have high kurtosis, rp is more variable than rs. Next, we conducted a sampling study of a psychometric dataset featuring symmetrically distributed data with light tails, and of 2 Likert-type survey datasets, 1 with light-tailed and the other with heavy-tailed distributions. Consistent with the simulations, rp had lower variability than rs in the psychometric dataset. In the survey datasets with heavy-tailed variables in particular, rs had lower variability than rp, and often corresponded more accurately to the population Pearson correlation coefficient (Rp) than rp did. The simulations and the sampling studies showed that variability in terms of standard deviations can be reduced by about 20% by choosing rs instead of rp. In comparison, increasing the sample size by a factor of 2 results in a 41% reduction of the standard deviations of rs and rp. In conclusion, rp is suitable for light-tailed distributions, whereas rs is preferable when variables feature heavy-tailed distributions or when outliers are present, as is often the case in psychological research.
Article
Social media use among adolescents continues to increase each year. This cross-sectional study explored how the amount of time spent using social media and the frequency of specific behaviors on social media, namely, behaviors that involve self-objectification, were related to body surveillance and body shame among a sample of early adolescents (N = 142; 43 boys and 99 girls, X¯age = 12.44 years, SDage = 0.61). Utilizing self-report measures, three types of social media were examined: Twitter, Facebook, and Instagram. Analyses indicated that greater levels of self-objectifying social media use predicted greater body shame among youth, and this was mediated by an associated increase in body surveillance. This mediation was moderated by self-monitoring and gender, such that the mediating role of body surveillance was stronger among girls and adolescents who are particularly focused on others for approval (i.e., high in self-monitoring). Implications of these findings are discussed.
Article
Objectification theorists argue that repeated exposure to sexually objectifying media content leads to higher levels of self-objectification. Although consequences of self-objectification for women’s sexual health and sexual agency have been proposed, efforts to test these connections have been infrequent and have yielded inconsistent results. We used structural equation modeling to test connections between exposure to three media genres (women’s magazines, lifestyle reality TV, and situation comedies), self-sexualization, and four dimensions of sexual agency among 754 heterosexual and bisexual undergraduate women aged 16–23 (M = 18.5). Our assessments of sexual agency focused on sexual assertiveness, condom use self-efficacy, sexual affect, and alcohol use to feel sexual. Findings confirmed our expectations. More frequent consumption of women’s magazines, lifestyle reality TV programs, and situation comedies each predicted greater self-sexualization, which in turn predicted greater use of alcohol to feel sexual, less condom use self-efficacy, and more negative sexual affect. We discuss implications for women’s sexual well-being and for research on media sexualization. We also offer suggestions for practitioners, parents, and educators to disrupt the associations among media use, self-sexualization, and diminished sexual agency.
Article
The present study aimed to identify the specific social networking sites (SNS) features that relate to body image concerns in young women. A total of 259 women aged 18-29years completed questionnaire measures of SNS use (Facebook and Instagram) and body image concerns. It was found that appearance-focused SNS use, rather than overall SNS use, was related to body image concerns in young women. Specifically, greater engagement in photo activities on Facebook, but not general Facebook use, was associated with greater thin-ideal internalisation and body surveillance. Similarly, following appearance-focused accounts on Instagram was associated with thin-ideal internalisation, body surveillance, and drive for thinness, whereas following appearance-neutral accounts was not associated with any body image outcomes. Implications for future SNS research, as well as for body image and disordered eating interventions for young women, are discussed.
Article
Objective: The primary aim of the study was to examine the relationship across time between Facebook use and body image concern in adolescent girls. Method: A sample of 438 girls in the first two years (Years 8 and 9) of high school (aged 13-15 years) at Time 1 completed questionnaire measures of Facebook consumption and body image concerns, and again two years later (Time 2). Results: Facebook involvement increased substantially over the two year time period. Body image concerns also increased. Number of Facebook friends was found to prospectively predict the observed increase in drive for thinness. On the other hand, internalization and body surveillance prospectively predicted the observed increase in number of Facebook friends. Discussion: It was concluded that Facebook "friendships" represent a potent sociocultural force in the body image of adolescent girls. © 2016 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.(Int J Eat Disord 2016).