ArticlePDF Available

Young children’s perceptions of ubiquitous computing and the Internet of Things

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Ubiquitous computing (ubicomp) and the Internet of things (IoT) are turning into everyday household technology in ever increasing pace, for example, in the form of connected toys. Whilst, ubicomp and IoT are changing and shaping children’s digital and technological landscape not much is known how children perceive these omnipresent and concealed forms of digital technology. This qualitatively oriented paper explores 3–6-year-old Finnish children’s perceptions of ubicomp and IoT via interviews and a design task. Initially, the children were skeptical towards the idea that tangible objects, such as toys, could be computer and/or Internet enabled. However, these perceptions were a subject to change when children were introduced a scientific conception of what computers and the Internet are and asked to apply their knowledge in a technological design task. Implications for early years digital literacy education are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Young children’s perceptions of ubiquitous computing and the Internet of Things
Abstract
Ubiquitous computing (ubicomp) and the Internet of things (IoT) are turning into everyday
household technology at an ever-increasing pace, for example, in the form of connected toys.
However, while ubicomp and IoT are changing and shaping children’s digital and technological
landscape, not much is known about how children perceive these omnipresent and concealed
forms of digital technology. This qualitatively oriented paper explores 3- to 6-year-old Finnish
children’s perceptions of ubicomp and IoT via interviews and a design task. Initially, the
children were skeptical toward the idea that tangible objects, such as toys, could be computer
and/or Internet enabled. However, these perceptions were subject to change when children
were introduced to a scientific conception of what computers and the Internet are and asked to
apply their knowledge to a technological design task. Implications for early years digital
literacy education are discussed in the paper.
Keywords
Children; digital literacy; early childhood; the Internet of things; ubiquitous computing
Practitioner notes
What is already known about this topic
The role of computer- and Internet-enabled tangible objects, such as connected toys,
is expanding rapidly in young children’s lives.
The need for early years digital literacy education has been acknowledged.
What this paper adds
This study explores young children’s initial perceptions of ubiquitous computing and
the Internet of things.
Children are initially skeptical toward the idea that tangible objects can be
computer/Internet enabled.
This initial skepticism is subject to change when children are introduced to a scientific
conception of what computers and the Internet are.
Implications for practice and policy
The study provides novel insights into how children’s digital literacy can be supported
in early childhood education.
Introduction
Research on young children and digital technologies has been a subject of rapid growth
(Mertala, 2016); especially, children’s encounters with tablet computers have attracted
scholarly attention (Couse & Chen, 2010; Falloon, 2014; Kucirkova, Messer, Sheehy, &
Fernández Panadero, 2014; Neumann, 2018). Notably, there has been less interest in children’s
perceptions of ubiquitous computing (ubicomp) and the Internet of Things (IoT). Specifically,
2
studies that explore how young children perceive and understand these technologies are sparse
(cf. Manches, Duncan, Plowman, & Sabeti, 2015).
Such research and knowledge, however, is urgently needed because ubicomp and IoT are
changing and shaping children’s digital and technological landscape and life worlds. According
to one forecast, there will be more than 75 billion IoT-connected devices installed worldwide
as of 2025.
1
Several market research reports also predict that the already notable sales figures
of computer- and/or Internet-enabled toys will grow rapidly in the near future.
2
Thus, although
IoT toys may not be everyday playthings for the vast majority of children today (Brito, Dias,
& Oliveira, 2018), they most likely will be in a few years. This qualitative study contributes
to filling this knowledge gap by exploring 3- to 6-year-old Finnish children’s perceptions of
ubicomp and IoT via picture-enhanced interviews and a design task.
Background
Ubicomp, as defined by Abowd and Mynatt (2000), refers to the proliferation of computing in
the physical world. In other words, the core idea of ubicomp is that any tangible object can
either include or be a computer. The idea behind IoT, in turn, is that any ‘thing’ or object that
is appropriately tagged can communicate through an Internet-like structure with other objects
that are similarly tagged (Pascual-Espada, Sanjuán-Martínez, Pelayo G-Bustelo, & Cueva-
Lovelle, 2011). In other words, when appropriately tagged, any tangible object can include
Internet connectivity. As can be seen from the definitions above, to a notable extent, ubicomp
and IoT are overlapping concepts. The main difference is that ubicomp objects do not
necessarily require Internet connectivity, whereas IoT objects need a computer chip (i.e., a
microcontroller or microprocessor) to function. To provide a concrete example, there are
programmable toys that have no Internet connectivity (i.e., BeeBot and Coderpillar), as well as
programmable toys with Internet connectivity (i.e., Dash & Dot and Evolution Robot; (Velicu
& Lampert, 2017). The first group can be referred to as ubicomp toys, whereas the latter ones
can be labeled IoT toys.
1
https://www.statista.com/statistics/471264/iot-number-of-connected-devices-worldwide/
2
https://www.prnewswire.com/news-releases/juniper-research-smart-toy-sales-to-grow-threefold-to-exceed-155-
billion-by-2022-628177033.html; https://globenewswire.com/news-release/2018/09/05/1565750/0/en/Global-
Smart-Toys-Market-Will-Reach-USD-5-410-00-Million-By-2024-Zion-Market-Research.html;
https://www.statista.com/statistics/320941/smart-toys-revenue/
3
Research-based knowledge of how children perceive ubiquitous computing and IoT also has
notable pedagogical value. The importance of supporting young children’s digital literacy has
been addressed by various stakeholders, including scholars (Marsh, 2017); global agents, such
as the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (Taguma, Makowiecki, &
Litjens, 2013); and national educational administrations (Finnish National Agency for
Education, 2016). As the pedagogics of early years digital literacy education are in the
emerging stage (Edwards, Mantilla, et al., 2018) (Edwards, Mantilla, et al., 2018; Salomaa &
Mertala, 2019), the development of appropriate and research-based methods requires up-to-
date knowledge of children’s initial understanding of ubicomp and IoT.
The understanding that tangible, everyday objects can be computer and Internet enabled
represents one form of operational digital literacy, which includes the skills needed to
understand the functional properties of digital artifacts (Marsh, 2017). It has been stated that
the proliferation of IoT toys has provided a new world of Internet experience for young
children, and this should be acknowledged in their cyber-safety education (Edwards, Mantilla,
et al., 2018). Put differently, due to the rapidly increasing market share and availability of
connected toys, it is important to teach children that toysand other tangible objectscan be
connected to the Internet, and especially, that these toys can collect data from them and
distribute the data to third parties. To cite the Electronic Privacy Information Center’s (2016)
complaint and request for investigation,
By purpose and design, these toys record and collect the private conversations of young
children without any limitations on collection, use, or disclosure of this personal
information. The toys subject young children to ongoing surveillance and are deployed
in homeswithout any meaningful data protection standards. They pose an imminent
and immediate threat to the safety and security of children. (p. 2)
These concerns are highlighted by the notion that many connected toys have been identified as
vulnerable for hacking (Chu, Apthorpe, & Feamster, 2018). In March 2017, it was announced
that 2 million voice recordings between parents and children were allegedly exposed to
potential hackers, along with 800,000 emails and passwords to their accounts from the database
of the IoT toy company CloudPets,
3
to provide one example.
3
https://www.marketwatch.com/story/your-childs-teddy-bear-may-now-be-hacked-2017-03-01
4
Toys are also the tangibles most often used when studying children’s encounters and meaning
making with and around ubicomp and IoT. The existing research has approached the
phenomenon from the viewpoints of how children perceive, for example, the questions of
privacy (Mcreynolds et al., 2017) and learning (Heljakka & Ihamäki, 2017) in relation to
Internet-enabled toys. However, the focus with the greatest semblance to and importance for
the present paper is Manches et al.’s (2015) study of how cognizant 10- to 11-year-old-children
are of IoT toys. According to their findings, even children who commonly played with IoT toys
were not aware of how the technology worked. That said, it is important to apprehend that
children’s limited knowledge is not restricted to ubicomp and IoT; rather, previous research
has suggested that children’s understanding of the Internet and computers is generally narrow
(Edwards, Nolan, et al., 2018; Mertala, 2019; Robertson, Manches, & Pain, 2017). Many
children, for example, find it difficult to distinguish whether they are online or offline when
playing games or using on-demand streaming services (Mertala, 2019). This also applies to
children’s understanding of computers. In his study on 5- to 7-year-old children’s conceptions
of computers, code, and the Internet, Mertala (2019) observed that children conceptualized
computers as traditional desktop and laptop computers (or even monitors), whereas
smartphones and tablets were considered to be different forms of technology. Accordingly, the
children did not spontaneously express that computers could be located in tangible everyday
objects, such as washing machines and toys. However, a study by Robertson et al. (2017)
suggested that when children are introduced to a scientific concept of computers (i.e.,
computers as chips) they can identify various devices containing such chips, including tablets,
smartphones, video cameras, traffic lights, and watches. This study first tests Robertson’s et al
(2017) findings and then further examines whether the children are able to apply the new
knowledge when engaged in a design task, which is explained in detail in the Methods section.
Scientific concept here refers to an explanation of what things are and how and why they work
(Edwards, Nolan, et al., 2018).
Theoretically, this paper draws on a sociocultural tradition in which learning of and about
things is understood to occur in interaction with the social and cultural (including material)
environment in which the subject acts (Lantolf & Thorne, 2007; Vygotsky, 1978). According
to previous research, much of children’s learning about digital technologies is based on
intentional and/or unintentional tutoring by guardians, older siblings, and other close figures
(Edwards, Nolan, et al., 2018; Mertala, 2019); thus, children’s digital literacy varies with the
5
quality and quantity of these interactions. One benefit of drawing on sociocultural theory is
that acknowledging the role of the social, material, and cultural contexts enables the researcher
to go beyond the (unfounded) generational dichotomy discourse in which children are
portrayed as born-competent “digital natives” and adults are viewed as unskilled “digital
immigrants(Prensky, 2001). Both of these images are popular in the field of educational
research and practice (Kirschner & De Bruyckere, 2017). Put differently, the way one
understands and conceptualizes digital technologies is more dependent on the sociocultural
context in which one lives than on one’s age and/or generation.
Research aims and questions
The aim of this study is understanding how young children perceive ubicomp and IoT. The first
objective was exploring children’s initial perceptions, which was formulated into the following
research question:
What types of initial perceptions do young children have about ubicomp and IoT?
Based on previous research, it was expected that children’s initial conceptions
4
would be that
computers and the Internet are tool- (i.e., computers are desktops) and activity-based (i.e., the
Internet is for playing games; Edwards, Nolan, et al., 2018; Mertala, 2019) and they would not
be cognizant of what ubicomp and the IoT are (Manches et al., 2015). Previous research also
suggested that children’s perceptions can change when a scientific conception is introduced to
them (Robertson et al., 2017). On these grounds, the second objective was examining how
children’s perceptions change when they encounter a new scientific conception. This was
formulated in the following research question:
How do children’s perceptions of ubicomp and IoT change when a scientific conception
of computers and the Internet is introduced to them?
Methods
Participants, research context, and data collection
4
In this paper, conceptions refers to childrens explanations of what things are (Edwards, Nolan, et al., 2018;
Mertala, 2019). Perceptions, in turn, is a broader term that includes reflection on whether the thing/phenomenon
under discussion, here ubicomp and IoT, is possible in the first place.
6
The data were collected from 33 children from one Finnish early childhood center in December
2018. Consent to participate in the research was requested verbally from the children and in
written form from their guardians. The distribution of the children’s ages and genders is
displayed in Table 1. The center was chosen via convenience sampling (Patton, 2002). I have
been collaborating with the educators since 2013, and I am familiar with the children as well.
The participating center was also a teaching practicum placement for the university’s early
childhood teacher education program, and the children had become accustomed with the
culture of new people working alongside their own educators for fixed short-term periods.
Table 1
Age and Gender Distribution of Participating Children
Age
3
4
5
6
Total
Girls
-
-
5
3
8
Boys
2
4
4
15
25
Total
2
4
9
18
33
Providing detailed verbal accounts on functional principles of digital technologies is sometimes
difficult for young children (Robertson et al., 2017). Thus, they should be offered alternative
mediums for self-expression for ensuring rich data. The use of visual methods and materials,
such as drawing and pictures, is typical in contemporary childhood research (Lipponen, Rajala,
Hilppö, & Paananen, 2016), and this has proven to provide rich data for exploring young
children’s meaning making around digital technologies (Brito et al., 2018; Edwards, Mantilla,
et al., 2018; Mertala, 2016, 2019; Robertson et al., 2017). In this study, visual materials were
used as supports for verbal narration (pictures shown to children) and forms of visual narration
(drawing task). From a sociocultural viewpoint, children’s drawings do not emerge in a
“cultural vacuum” (Mertala, 2016), but instead, they are always influenced by the
communication and symbol systems around them (Anning & Ring, 2004).
In practice, the data were gathered via individual picture-enhanced research interviews and a
drawing task that took place during the first day of a three-day pedagogical project carried out
by a group of preservice early childhood teachers.
5
The project was part of a compulsory course
about technology-enhanced learning. The method for data collection, as well as the entire
5
Performing the data collection as part of a broader project was partly based on ethical reflection in research. As
data collection was carried during the first day, the following two days were considered requital for the time and
effort the children had invested into us during the data collection period.
7
project, was designed in collaboration with the educators of the participating center to ensure
the project respects their pedagogical values, as well as the children’s interests. The preservice
teachers were trained in how to carry out the interviews and drawing task prior to data
collection. They were, for example, encouraged to create a relaxed atmosphere by playing,
reading, and chatting with the children before addressing the issues related to the research
objectives. Before data collection was performed, the children were introduced to the aim and
methods of the research (Einarsdottir, Dockett, & Perry, 2009) by me during the morning circle
time. Then, the preservice teachers introduced themselves to the children and began to
familiarize themselves with the children with support from the educators.
The actual data collection process consisted of five phases. These entailed the following:
i. The children’s initial conceptions were explored by asking them to freely explain what
they knew about and understood by computers and the Internet.
ii. Next, the children were shown pictures of a car, washing machine, and teddy bear
6
one
by one and asked whether these objects could include a computer or the Internet. These
specific objects were chosen because they present a pool of everyday objects that are
likely familiar to all children. All these objects can contain computers and connectivity.
For example, all modern cars have at least one computer in them, and many have
integrated on-board computers that can display error signals and/or be used for
navigation purposes;
iii. In the third phase, a short scientific explanation of computers and the Internet were
introduced to the children. The explanations were based on two children’s nonfiction
books Kuinka tietokone toimii? Kurkista ja Koodaa (Flip-the-flap: computers and
coding; (Dickins & Nielsen, 2015) and Miten Internet toimii (How the Internet works;
(Nilsson, 2015). The size of a computer chip was also concretized for the children by
letting them examine a Raspberry Pi computer. Scientific explanations are provided in
Table 2. Reference pictures of the books and Raspberry Pi are provided in Figure 1.
iv. Next, the children were shown the same three pictures again, and they were asked
whether they could contain a computer or the Internet.
v. Finally, the children were asked to use drawing to design a toy that would have a
computer or Internet connectivity in it. The children were oriented for the task with a
fairy tale about a Christmas elf who hit his/her head on a tree in a sledging accident,
6
See supplementary file 1.
8
and therefore, was unable to invent any new toys for the coming Christmas and needed
help from the children.
7
The children’s responses to the questions and their
presentations of toy designs were recorded by writing them down (Einarsdottir et al.,
2009).
All the interviews were conducted with one child at a time.
Table 2
Scientific Explanations of Computers and the Internet
7
See supplementary file 2
Explanation
A computer is a device that can follow instructions and solve problems. However, computers do
not come up with solutions independently; instead, they follow the instructions given by people
using the buttons, mouse, or keyboard. For example, pressing A on the keyboard is a command
to write an A on a computer-connected display. Computers can be really small. Many devices,
such as cameras and remote-control cars, have computers inside them.
The Internet is a large network of computer cables and computers that enables devices to
communicate with each other. You can connect to the Internet, for example, over a telephone
network, fiber optic cable, or wireless network. The wireless network is also known as WiFi.
The Internet sends things called digital information. This information is made from ones and
zeros because computers and computer programs can only read information in that form.
Computers then convert this information so that it can be viewed, listened to, and used by
humans
9
Figure 1. Pictures of the books and of Raspberry Pi
Analysis
The analysis process was guided by an abductive approach in which the researcher moved
between deductive and inductive reasoning to open up new ways of theorizing the phenomenon
(Dey, 2003). To put the results in context, the author reviewed the existing research and
acknowledged its findings by using them as the basis for initial analytical readings of the data.
However, due to the novelty of the research objective and exploratory nature of the study, data-
driven interpretations were also performed to refine the existing theoretical views.
In practice, the data were analyzed via qualitative oriented monotype mixed analysis (MMA;
(Onwuegbuzie, Slate, Leech, & Collins, 2007) and the constant comparison method (Boeije,
2002). In MMA, the datawhether qualitative or quantitativeare analyzed using both
qualitative and quantitative methods. The use of MMA requires that qualitative data are altered
into a form that can be analyzed statistically, while quantitative data are transformed into a type
that can be analyzed qualitatively (Onwuegbuzie et al., 2007). This mixing can be characterized
as a combination of measurement and interpretation (Biesta, 2010). In the present study,
transforming the data meant quantifying the responses containing specific types of information,
for example, children’s conceptions of what computers and the Internet are. These frequency
counts were then converted to percentages for calculating the frequency effect size
(Onwuegbuzie, 2003).
Interpretative analysis was carried out by reading the data, comprising both the drawings and
interviews, in a holistic manner; the aim of doing this was identifying the essential qualities of
the phenomenon under investigation (Miles, Huberman, & Saldana, 2013) and making
comparisons between the theory and data, between the data from different participants, and
within the data from individual participants (Boeije, 2002). In other words, the analytical focus
was not only on what the children said but also on how they expressed their views and
perceptions. For instance, it was noted that some children used more intense narration when
describing why there could not be a computer inside a teddy bear than why there could not be
a computer inside a washing machine or car.
First, children’s descriptions of what computers and the Internet are were coded and
categorized based on content using three literature-informed (Edwards, Nolan, et al., 2018;
10
Mertala, 2019) categories as starting points. The categories were as follows: function-based
explanation (i.e., how computers and the Internet work), tool-based explanation (i.e., what
counts as a computer/the Internet), and activity-based explanation (i.e., what can be done
with computers/online). Next, the children’s answers to the first-round questions on whether
there could be a computer and/or Internet in a car, washing machine, and teddy bear were coded
and categorized based on whether the children thought these items could or could not include
computers and/or Internet connectivity. The answers were then further coded and categorized
regarding the nature of their reasoning. Three categories were formed, as follows: “function-
based explanation” (the child explained what a computer and/or connectivity would afford for
the object), “non-function-based explanation” (the child explained where a computer and/or
connectivity could be located but did not provide an explanation of what this would afford for
the object), and “no explanation” (the child agreed that there could be a computer and/or
connectivity in the object with no further explanation). A similar procedure was conducted for
data from the second round of questions. In the last phase, children’s toy designs—that is, the
drawings and what the children said about themwere coded in relation to how ubicomp
and/or IoT features were exhibited in them. Data extracts from each category are provided in
the Findings section to improve the clarity and transparency of the analysis process.
Before presenting the findings, the limitations of the data should be addressed. The types of
objects used as examples and the way the scientific concept was introduced potentially
influenced the data by providing cues to the children. To put this in context, a remote-controlled
toy car was used as one example of a computer-enabled toy, and remote controllability was one
of the ubicomp features included most often in the children’s toy designs. This skewness in the
data is considered in the analysis and conclusions made from the findings. Another possible
limitation is that the unbalanced age and gender distribution of the participating children
prevented gender- or age-based comparisons between them.
Findings
The findings of this study are presented in four subsections. The first discusses the children’s
initial conceptions of computers and the Internet, while the second considers their initial
perceptions of ubicomp and IoT. The last two subsections focus on the change of children's
perceptions after the introduction of the scientific concept of computers and the Internet.
Children’s initial conceptions of computers and the Internet
11
Table 3 displays how children’s initial views of what computers and the Internet are were
distributed regarding the nature of their conceptions. In some cases, a child’s response included
examples from several categories, and thus, the number of examples is higher than the number
of children.
Table 3.
Distribution of Children’s Initial Conceptions of Computers and the Internet
Function
Tool
Activity
Appearance
“I don’t know”
or unclear
response
Computer
3
6
23
5
6
Internet
-
6
16
-
14
Function-based explanations of computers and the Internet were rare in the children’s initial
perceptions. One child, for example, commented that computers are built from different types
of parts. His knowledge had strong sociocultural roots, as he related that he knew this because
his grandfather had different types of computer parts. Relatively few tool-based references
were made. Some children commented that computers include laptops and desktops, while a
few noted that tablets and smartphones are computers as well (“Tablet is a computer” [Child#6,
4y]; Smartphone, it is a computer too [Child#20, 6y]). In terms of the Internet, tool-based
explanations included notions like “Mother’s phone has Internet” (Child#23, 5y).
Activity-based conceptions were the most prominent category. Most often, the mentioned
activities were playing games and watching movies and children’s programs. Some of the
children commented that an Internet connection is needed for buying things and ordering food:
One child remarked, You can buy stuff” (Child#14, 4y), while another stated, “You can order
pizza” (Child#13, 5y). In addition, one child commented that an Internet connection is needed
for video streaming services to function, stating, “You cannot watch YouTube if you don’t
have [an] Internet [connection] (Child#27, 6y). Nevertheless, the main trend in the data was
that the difference between being online and being offline was unclear for the children. One
child, for instance, stated that he had searched for ideas for Christmas presents with a computer,
but at the same time, he stated that he had never used the Internet. Finally, five children made
references to computers appearance, that is, size, shape, and color, in their responses. Two
children commented that computers are big, which is essential information regarding children’s
initial perceptions of ubicomp and IoT: If computers are big, they cannot be found inside small
objects.
12
Children’s initial perceptions of ubicomp and IoT
Table 4 summarizes the quantitative distribution of the children’s initial perceptions of
ubicomp and IoT. As the table shows, the great majority of the children thought that cars,
washing machines, and teddy bears could not include a computer or Internet connection.
Table 4
Distribution of Children’s Initial Perceptions of Ubicomp and IoT
Car
Washing machine
Teddy bear
Yes
Don’t
know
No
Yes
Don’t
know
No
Yes
Don’t
know
No
Computer
9
(27%)
4
(12%)
20
(61%)
8
(24%)
1
(3%)
24
(73%)
7
(21%)
3
(9%)
23
(70%)
Internet
9
(27%)
7
(21%)
17
(52%)
8
(24%)
5
(15%)
20
(61%)
5
(15%)
6
(18%)
22
(67%)
However, there were notable variations in the depth and level of detail in the children’s
descriptions of why there could or could not be computers and/or Internet included in cars,
washing machines, and toys. In terms of cars, for example, there were children who were aware
that some modern cars are self-directed. One child stated, “My cousin’s father has an electric
car. The car can reverse by itself” (Child#26, 6y). In addition, some of the children knew that
the navigating systems of modern cars require computers and/or an Internet connection. A data
extract from Child#33 (6y) provides an illustrative example: When asked whether a car could
contain a computer, he answered, “Yeah, because it has a map that guides you to where you
are going. However, at the same time, several children commented that there could be
computers and/or Internet in cars, but they were not able to provide explanations for what these
components could do in them.
Table 5 presents how the children’s answers were distributed on a function-based/non-
function-based/no explanation scale. “You can put on a navigator with an Internet
[connection] (Child#6, 4y) is an example of function-based reasoning, whereas, It [washing
machine] has a screen that has a computer in it” (Child#1, 4y) was categorized under non-
function-based reasoning. The main difference between these categories is that the first
describes how the specific technology affords the essential functions of the Internet-enabled
device in question, whereas the second includes no such description; instead, it merely states
13
where a computer could be located. Comments with no concrete explanation, such as, “Yeah,
there could be Internet” (Child#21, 6y), were labeled with the “No reasoning” code.
Table 5
Qualitative Distribution of Children’s Yes Responses in the First Interview Round
Function-based explanation
Non-function-based
explanation
No explanation
Computer
Internet
Computer
Internet
Computer
Internet
Car
3
3
4
2
2
4
Washing
machine
2
-
4
3
2
5
Teddy bear
4
4
3
1
-
2
Changes in children’s perceptions of ubicomp & IoT
Table 6 summarizes the quantitative distribution of the children’s perceptions of ubicomp and
IoT in the second round of interviews. As can be seen in the table, there were notable
quantitative changes in the children’s views after the introduction of the scientific concept of
computers and the Internet, the most prominent being the increase of 49% points in the
perception that cars could contain a computer.
Table 6
Distribution of Children’s Perceptions of Ubicomp and IoT After the Introduction of Scientific
Concepts of Computers and the Internet
Car
Washing machine
Teddy bear
Yes
Don’t
know
No
Yes
Don’t
know
No
Yes
Don’t
know
No
Computer
25
(76%)
1
(3%)
7 (21%)
17
(52%)
3
(9%)
13
(39%)
14
(42%)
1
(3%)
18
(55%)
Internet
16
(48%)
5
(15%)
12
(36%)
15
(45%)
3
(9%)
15
(45%)
14
(42%)
1
(3%)
18
(55%)
When comparing the distribution of answers, it appears to be easier for children to apprehend
computers and connectivity in objects that are mechanical to begin with. The teddy bear was
the only object that more than half the children said could not contain a computer or Internet
connection. In other words, even the introduction of scientific concepts was sufficient for
shaping the children’s perceptions about cars and washing machines; the teddy bear, at least in
part, was a different matter. For instance, Child#26 initially thought that there could not be a
14
computer or connectivity in any of the three objects. After the introduction of scientific
concepts, he changed his mind about cars and washing machines, but not about the toy. In his
words, a teddy bear “is a soft-toy. It helps you to fall asleep. There can’t be a computer in it”
(Child#26, 6y). Put another way, the plush toy bear and digital technology were mutually
exclusive categories for him.
Comparably to the first interview round, there was notable variation in the depth and level of
detail in the children’s responses. Table 7 displays how the children’s answers were distributed
on the function-based/non-function-based/no explanation scale. As can be seen from the table,
the increase mainly took place in the “no explanation” and “non-function-based” categories. In
other words, although the shift of perceptions was notable in a quantitative sense, much of this
change took place at a rather superficial level.
Table 7
Qualitative Distribution of Children’s Yes Responses in the Second Interview Round
Function-based explanation
Non-function-based
explanation
No explanation
Computer
Internet
Computer
Internet
Computer
Internet
Car
5
3
7
2
13
11
Washing
machine
2
1
8
5
7
9
Teddy bear
6
6
2
2
6
6
There were also cases in which the children’s perceptions had evolved significantly. The data
from Child#3 (6y) provide a piquant example of such a case. When asked about his initial
conceptions, he stated that there could be no computers or Internet connection in a washing
machine. In the second round, however, he was able to provide a rather detailed description of
how an IoT-enabled washing machine could work. In his words, “There could be an Internet
connection in it. You could control it with a computer and turn it on.”
Ubiquitous computing and the Internet of Things in children’s toy designs
In the last phase, the children were asked to design a toy via a drawing that included computers
and/or Internet connectivity. Once again, there was notable variation in the children’s
responses. Some of the children provided detailed descriptions of what a computer and/or
connectivity enabled in their toy. For instance, one child drew a toy robot and stated, “[This is]
a robot. The computer is inside the robot. The computer makes the robot move (Child#17, 5y;
15
see Figure 2); another related, Somebody moves this [toy car] with a smartphone. It has an
Internet connection” (Child#4, 6y; see Figure 3).
Figure 2. Ubicomp robot Figure 3. IoT car
Nevertheless, designs in which references to ubicomp and IoT were more implicit appeared
more commonly. One child, for instance, designed a toy hamster (Figure 4) and stated that it
was “remote controlled. It has three buttons. One button makes it dance. [One button] makes it
walk forward. One button makes it make sounds (Child#13, 5y). However, she did not explain
what the specific technology was that enabled these features. Furthermore, in some cases, the
children’s initial activity- and tool-based conceptions of computers and the Internet were
reflected in their toy designs. Child#13 related that computers can be used for playing games
and games can be downloaded from the Internet. These activity-based conceptions were also
present in her toy design (Figure 5). In her words, “[This is] Barbie’s smartphone. You can
download games on it. It has a heart-shaped screen that you can watch and play stuff
(Child#31, 6y). In other words, she did not incorporate ubicomp and IoT into the doll, but
instead, she designed a ubicomp- and IoT-enabled accessory for her.
16
Figure 4. Toy hamster and its house Figure 5. Barbie’s smartphone
The types of toys the children designed can be understood to reflect the material environment
they live in, and all four examples above can be traced back to existing toys. Remote control
cars and Barbies (and other fashion dolls), for instance, have been regular items in young
children’s “toy pool” for decades. Accordingly, technology-enhanced hamsters (and other
animals) designed by several children recall popular interactive “care toys” that are either IoT
enhanced (i.e., Hatchimals) or traditionally battery operated (i.e., Chatimals). One more
example comprises the remote controllable and camera-enabled helicopters and airplanes
designed by four children, as these designs notably resembled miniature unmanned aerial
vehicles, known as drones in colloquial language. Figure 6 provides a piquant example of this;
here, the child has designed “an airplane that can make YouTube videos on the Internet
(Child#28, 6y). The arrow indicates the location of the camera.
17
Figure 6. IoT-enabled airplane
Discussion and conclusions
This qualitative study has explored 3- to 6-year-old Finnish children’s perceptions of ubicomp
and IoT. It was found that the children were initially skeptical about whether tangible objects
could include computers or connectivity. This was mainly due to the children’s initial
conceptions of computers and the Internet, which were profoundly activity- and tool-based, as
also identified in previous research (Edwards, Nolan, et al., 2018; Mertala, 2019). The findings
also suggest that children’s perceptions of ubicomp and IoT can be shaped and refined by
providing them an age-appropriate scientific definition of what computers and the Internet are
and having them apply this new knowledge to a design task. In some cases, there were notable
qualitative changes in the children’s perceptions. However, in most cases, the changes in the
children’s perceptions were superficial rather than profound. Thus, more research is needed to
explore the further development and persistence of children’s changed perceptions.
To conclude, this study has provided original knowledge involving young children’s
perceptions of ubicomp and IoT. The use of multiple child-centered data collection methods,
such as picture-enhanced interviews, drawing-based design tasks, and reading of non-fiction
books and thought-provoking stories, has made it possible to gather rich and deep data, which
is one of the prerequisites of credible and trustworthy qualitative research (Fusch & Ness,
2015). Furthermore, detailed descriptions of the research context, data, and methods support
the transferability of the findings to other contexts in the areas of research and practice
(Shenton, 2004). In other words, while the findings cannot be automatically generalized to
18
other populations, the study provides several implications for early years’ digital literacy
education in early childhood education centers, which are discussed next.
According to the present study, the huge majority of young children do not possess an initial
understanding that tangible objects can be connected to the Internet. This finding implies, that
we should move beyond the prominent screen-based understanding of technology toward a
more holistic approach. It is important that both, children’s guardians and their professional
educators, demonstrate for children that computing and connectivity are not features restricted
only into screen-based devices. This, as shown in this paper, can be done by using children’s
non-fiction books and drawing-based design tasks. This notion challenges the contemporary
discourses around digital literacy that are dominated by a device-centered view in which the
focus is on the affordances of different digital tools (i.e., tablets, interactive whiteboards, and
apps; (Neumann, Finger, & Neumann, 2017).
This notion positions this study in the emerging branch of research indicating that traditional
(and non-digital) early childhood education practices, such as drama, drawing, and crafting,
are sound methods of exploring our digitized lifeworld with young children (Edwards,
Mantilla, et al., 2018; Salomaa & Mertala, 2019). These are valuable notions for early years
professionals, who have been found to struggle with how to provide digital literacy education
(Edwards, Mantilla, et al., 2018; Salomaa & Mertala, 2019) and who find it difficult to integrate
digitality and technology into the traditional practices of early childhood education (Lindahl &
Folkesson, 2012). As Bassey (1981) proposes, if practitioners believe their situations to be
similar to the one described in the study, they may relate the findings to their own positions.
The present study was carried out in a natural early childhood education setting by using means
and materials familiar to all practitioners. Thus, the findings presented in this paper are
potentially empowering for practitioners struggling with the new and somewhat ambiguous
curricular alignments around supporting and developing young children’s digital literacy.
Statements on potential conflicts of interest
The author does not report any conflicts of interest.
Statement on open data and ethics
19
The researcher has followed the ethical guidelines of the University of Oulu. The data cannot
be distributed openly as the participants are underaged. However, requests for data may be
made to the corresponding author.
References
Abowd, G. D., & Mynatt, E. D. (2000). Charting past, present, and future research in
ubiquitous computing. ACM Transactions on Computer-Human Interaction, 7(1), 29
58. doi:10.1145/344949.344988research in ubiquitous comp. ACM Transactions on
Computer-Human Interaction, 7(1), 2958. https://doi.org/10.1145/344949.344988
Anning, A., & Ring, K. (2004). Making Sense of Children’s Drawings. Maidenhead: Open
University Press. Retrieved from
http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=nlebk&AN=233835&site=ehost
-live
Bassey, M. (1981). Pedagogic research: On the relative merits of search for generalization
and study of single events. Oxford Review of Education, 7(1), 7394.
https://doi.org/10.1080/0305498810070108
Biesta, G. (2010). ). Pragmatism and the philosophical foundations of mixed methods
research. In A. Tashakkori & C. Teddlie (Eds.), Sage handbook of mixed methods in
social and behavioral research (2nd ed., pp. 95117). SAGE.
Boeije, H. (2002). A Purposeful Approach to the Constant Comparative Method in the
Analysis of Qualitative Interviews. Quality and Quantity, 36, 391409.
Brito, R., Dias, P., & Oliveira, G. (2018). Young children, digital media and smart toys: How
perceptions shape adoption and domestication. British Journal of Educational
Technology, 49(5), 807820. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjet.12655
Buchanan, R., Murray, T., & Buchanan, R. (2018). ’ The Internet is all around us ’: How
children come to understand the Internet ‘ The Internet is all around us ’: How children
come to understand the Internet . Digital Culture & Education, 10(July), 121.
Retrieved from http://www.digitalcultureandeducation.com/cms/wp-
content/uploads/2018/07/Murray and Buchanan.pdf
Chu, G., Apthorpe, N., & Feamster, N. (2018). Security and Privacy Analyses of Internet of
Things Children’s Toys. IEEE Internet of Things Journal, PP(5), 1.
https://doi.org/10.1109/JIOT.2018.2866423
Couse, L. J., & Chen, D. W. (2010). A Tablet Computer for Young Children? Exploring its
Viability for Early Childhood Education. Journal of Research on Technology in
Education, 43(1), 7596. https://doi.org/10.1080/15391523.2010.10782562
Dey, I. (2003). Qualitative data analysis: A user-friendly guide for social scientists. New
York, NY: Routledge.
Dickins, P., & Nielsen, S. (2015). Kuinka tietokone toimii? Kurkista ja Koodaa [Lift-the-flap:
Computers and coding]. Helsinki, Finland: Tammi.
Edwards, S., Mantilla, A., Henderson, M., Nolan, A., Skouteris, H., & Plowman, L. (2018).
Teacher Practices for Building Young Children’s Concepts of the Internet through Play-
Based Learning. Educational Practice and Theory, 40(1), 2950. Retrieved from
http://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=eric&AN=EJ1191511&site=eh
ost-live%0Ahttp://dx.doi.org/10.7459/ept/40.1.03
Edwards, S., Nolan, A., Henderson, M., Mantilla, A., Plowman, L., & Skouteris, H. (2018).
Young children’s everyday concepts of the Internet: A platform for cyber-safety
education in the early years. British Journal of Educational Technology, 49(1), 4555.
https://doi.org/10.1111/bjet.12529
20
Einarsdottir, J., Dockett, S., & Perry, B. (2009). Making meaning: Children’s perspectives
expressed through drawings. Early Child Development and Care, 179(2), 217232.
https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430802666999
Falloon, G. (2014). What’s going on behind the screens? Researching young students’
learning pathways using iPads. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 30(4), 318336.
https://doi.org/10.1111/jcal.12044
Finnish National Agency for Education. (2016). National Core Curriculum for Early
Childhood Education and Care 2016.
Fusch, P. I., & Ness, L. R. (2015). Are we there yet? Data saturation in qualitative research,
20(9), 14081416.
Heljakka, K., & Ihamäki, P. (2017). Preschoolers Learning with the Internet of Toys : From
Toy-Based Edutainment to Transmedia Literacy. Seminar.Net, 14(1), 85102.
Kirschner, P. A., & De Bruyckere, P. (2017). The myths of the digital native and the
multitasker. Teaching and Teacher Education, 67, 135142.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2017.06.001
Kucirkova, N., Messer, D., Sheehy, K., & Fernández Panadero, C. (2014). Children’s
engagement with educational iPad apps: Insights from a Spanish classroom. Computers
and Education, 71, 175184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2013.10.003
Lantolf, J. P., & Thorne, S. L. (2007). Sociocultural Theory and Second Language Learning.
In B. van Patten & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition (pp.
197220). Routledge.
Lindahl, M. G., & Folkesson, A.-M. (2012). Can we let computers change practice?
Educators’ interpretations of preschool tradition. Computers in Human Behavior, 28(5),
17281737. https://doi.org/http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.04.012
Lipponen, L., Rajala, A., Hilppö, J., & Paananen, M. (2016). Exploring the foundations of
visual methods used in research with children. European Early Childhood Education
Research Journal, 24(6), 936946. https://doi.org/10.1080/1350293X.2015.1062663
Manches, A., Duncan, P., Plowman, L., & Sabeti, S. (2015). Three questions about the
Internet of things and children. TechTrends, 59(1). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11528-014-
0824-8
Marsh, J. (2017). Introduction. Makerspaces in the early years: A literature review.
DigiLitEY. Retrieved from http://makeyproject.eu/wp-
content/uploads/2017/02/Makey_Literature_Review.pdf
Mcreynolds, E., Hubbard, S., Lau, T., Saraf, A., Cakmak, M., & Roesner, F. (2017). Toys
that Listen: A Study of Parents, Children, and Internet-Connected Toys. Chi, 5197
5207. https://doi.org/10.1145/3025453.3025735
Mertala, P. (2016). Fun and games - Finnish children’s ideas for the use of digital media
in preschool. Nordic Journal of Digital Literacy, 10(04), 207226.
https://doi.org/10.18261/issn.1891-943x-2016-04-01
Mertala, P. (2018). Young children’s conceptions of computers, code, and the Internet.
International Journal of Child-Computer Interaction.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijcci.2018.11.003
Miles, M. B., Huberman, A. M., & Saldana, J. (2013). Qualitative data analysis. SAGE.
Neumann, M. M. (2018). Using tablets and apps to enhance emergent literacy skills in young
children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 42(October 2016), 239246.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2017.10.006
Neumann, M. M., Finger, G., & Neumann, D. L. (2017). A Conceptual Framework for
Emergent Digital Literacy. Early Childhood Education Journal, 45(4), 471479.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643-016-0792-z
Nilsson, H. (2015). Miten Internet toimii [How the Internet works]. Helsinki, Finland:
21
Kansallinen audiovisuaalinen instituutti.
Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2003). Effect Sizes in Qualitative Research: A Prolegomenon. Quality
and Quantity, 37, 393409.
Onwuegbuzie, A. J., Slate, J. R., Leech, N. L., & Collins, K. M. T. (2007). Conducting mixed
analyses: A general typology. International Journal of Multiple Research Approaches,
1(1), 417. https://doi.org/10.5172/mra.455.1.1.4
Pascual Espada, J., Sanjuán Martínez, O., Pelayo G-Bustelo, B. C., & Cueva Lovelle, J. M.
(2011). Virtual Objects on the Internet of Things. International Journal of Interactive
Multimedia and Artificial Intelligence, 1(4), 23. https://doi.org/10.9781/ijimai.2011.144
Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative research and evaluation methods (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks,
CA: SAGE.
Prensky, M. (2001). Digital Natives , Digital Immigrants. On the Horizon, 9(5), 16.
https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1108/10748120110424816
Robertson, J., Manches, A., & Pain, H. (2017). “It’s Like a Giant Brain With a Keyboard”:
Children’s Understandings About How Computers Work. Childhood Education, 93(4),
338345. https://doi.org/10.1080/00094056.2017.1343589
Rücker, M. T., & Pinkwart, N. (2016). Review and Discussion of Children’s Conceptions of
Computers. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 25(2), 274283.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10956-015-9592-2
Salomaa, S., & Mertala, P. (2019). An education-centred approach to digital media education.
In C. Gray & I. Palaiologou (Eds.), Early learning in the digital age (pp. 151164).
SAGE.
Shenton, A. K. (2004). Strategies for ensuring trustworthiness in qualitative research projects.
Education for Information, 22, 6375.
Taguma, M., Makowiecki, K., & Litjens, I. (2013). Quality Matters in Early Childhood
Education and Care: Norway 2013. https://doi.org/10.1787/9789264176713-en
Velicu, A., & Lampert, C. (2017). The composite world of IoToys. In G. Mascheroni & D.
Holloway (Eds.), The Internet of Toys: A Report on Media and Social Discourses
around Young Children and IoToys (pp. 1524). DigiLitEY.
Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes.
Massachusetts, MA: Harvard university press.
... If students have a misconception prior to learning a subject, this may prevent them from learning the new subject properly, thereby leading to new misconceptions (Biber et al., 2013). Research on children's initial conceptions of abstract digital technologies like the Internet (Eskelä-Haapanen & Kiili, 2019), search engines (Kodama, 2016) the Internet of Things (Mertala, 2020), digital data (Pangrazio & Selwyn, 2018), and programming / coding (Mertala, 2019) suggest that informal encounters provide children only a limited understanding of what these technologies actually are. AI as an "opaque technology'' (Long & Magerko, 2020) and a "fuzzy concept" (Kaplan & Haenlein, 2019) should not be an exception. ...
... The data was analyzed using an abductive approach that combines deductive and inductive reasoning (Grönfors, 2011). The role of deduction is to offer theoretical threads, which are complemented and/or refined via interpretive inductive analysis (Mertala, 2020). As a result, inductive approach forms a "hermeneutic circle" of reading, reflective writing, and interpretation (see, Kafle, 2013). ...
... This implies that while the misconception is quite fundamental, it should not be that difficult to fix since the degree of certitude (Usó-Doménech & Nescolarde-Selva, 2016) is low. Indeed, there is evidence (see, Kim et al., 2023;Mertala, 2020) that superficial technology-related misconceptions can be replaced with (more) correct ones via simple pedagogical practices. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Research on children's initial conceptions of AI is in an emerging state, which, from a constructivist viewpoint, challenges the development of pedagogically sound AI-literacy curricula, methods, and materials. To contribute in resolving this need in the present paper, qualitative survey data from 195 children were analyzed abductively to answer the following three research questions: What kind of misconceptions do Finnish 5th and 6th graders' have about AI?; 2) How do these misconceptions relate to common misconception types?; and 3) How profound are these misconceptions? As a result, three misconception categories were identified: 1) Non-technological AI, in which AI was conceptualized as peoples' cognitive processes (factual misconception); 2) Anthropomorphic AI, in which AI was conceptualized as a human-like entity (vernacular, non-scientific, and conceptual misconception); and 3) AI as a machine with a pre-installed intelligence or knowledge (factual misconception). Majority of the children evaluated their AI-knowledge low, which implies that the misconceptions are more superficial than profound. The findings suggest that context-specific linguistic features can contribute to students' AI misconceptions. Implications for future research and AI literacy education are discussed.
... Although access to technologies gives teachers the opportunity to integrate technology into the classroom, personal attitudes predict the actual adoption and use of technology: teachers who strongly agree that technology can benefit children's learning use technology more often (Blackwell et al., 2014;Jack & Higgins, 2019b;Marklund, 2020). Against this background, in curriculum design, paying attention to the unique pedagogical characteristics of ECE is vital (Mertala, 2020). One important aspect to consider is that teachers emphasize caring with the youngest children, but promote education with older children (Mertala, 2020); hence, the curriculum framework should provide support for teachers not only by clearly indicating the content areas and targeted learning outcomes in general, but also by supporting teachers to orient appropriate teaching and learning practices for children of different ages. ...
... Against this background, in curriculum design, paying attention to the unique pedagogical characteristics of ECE is vital (Mertala, 2020). One important aspect to consider is that teachers emphasize caring with the youngest children, but promote education with older children (Mertala, 2020); hence, the curriculum framework should provide support for teachers not only by clearly indicating the content areas and targeted learning outcomes in general, but also by supporting teachers to orient appropriate teaching and learning practices for children of different ages. This important aspect was missing from both curricula and therefore we recommend adding more appropriate teaching and learning practices for children of different ages in curricula texts. ...
... However, our analysis indicated that learning outcomes were not precisely mentioned for children's media criticism. If initial steps towards learning media criticism and related learning objectives and activities do not become clear for EC educators, it can be confusing and leave room for misinterpretation (Mertala, 2020). Based on these results, we recommend clarifying types, genres, and functions of practicing media criticism in curriculum texts and include current technology trends giving examples such as computational thinking and robotics. ...
Article
Full-text available
Children’s digital competence (DC) is often poorly supported in early childhood education (ECE). Furthermore, common definition of DC is difficult to find. Therefore, the aim of this comparative curriculum study was to better understand how objectives and content of DC are defined in ECE. Australia and Finland curricula were analyzed applying theory-driven content analysis. The results indicated that in both countries theoretical basis of DC was present: objectives and content of DC referred to (1) technical skills and practices, (2) applying digital technologies, (3) evaluate digital technologies critically, and (4) motivation to participate in a digital culture. However, aims, content, and practical guidelines on the curricula were unclear. This study recommends that ECE curricula should better emphasis and make explicit the key elements of DC and how to holistically foster children’s DC in practice. Further studies to clarify the important elements of DC in ECE curricula frameworks is required.
... Research shows that children develop their own conceptions of abstract digital technologies like the Internet, code, and ubiquitous computing before their formal introduction in (pre)school (e.g., Edwards et al., 2018;Eskelä-Haapanen & Kiili, 2019;Mertala, 2019Mertala, , 2020Wennås Brante & Walldén, 2021). This stands for AI as well (Kreinsen & Schultz, 2021;Ottenbreit-Leftwich et al., 2021). ...
... Unlike in deductive analysis, in the abductive method, the role of the theory is not tested by the data. Instead, theory and previous research are treated as "threads" (Grönfors, 2011), which can provide working categories for the initial analyses but are a subject to be refined via data-driven interpretations (Mertala, 2020). The main theoretical threads in this study were the stages of AI (Kaplan & Haenlein, 2019), "Big Ideas" in AI (Touretzky et al., 2019) Vygotskian-based idea of everyday concepts (Edwards et al., 2018;Mertala, 2019), existing research on people's conceptions of AI Kreinsen & Schultz, 2021;Zhang and Dafoe, 2020 ), and the public representations of AI (e.g., Brokensha, 2020;Chuan et al., 2019;Fast & Horvitz, 2017;Jokela, 2018;Obozintsev, 2018;Slotte Dufva & Mertala, 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
In the present paper, we report the findings of a qualitative survey study of 195 Finnish 5th and 6th grade students' pre-instructional conceptions of artificial intelligence (AI). An exploration of these initial conceptions provides insight into students' preliminary understanding of the topic and informs curriculum designers and teachers about misconceptions that might jeopardize student learning. The findings suggest that students' initial conceptions of AI are varied and often uninformed. For instance, references to the role of data in training AI applications were practically nonexistent. Instead, AI was often described as an anthropomorphic technology that possesses cognitive qualities equivalent to those of humans––a conception that notably resembles how AI is portrayed in the media. As a pedagogical implication, our findings suggest that it would be valuable to “demystify” AI by exploring its technical principles (i.e., the role of data) of the “human-like” AI solutions students encounter in their everyday lives.
... Unlike in deductive analysis, in the abductive method, the role of the theory is not tested by the data. Instead, theory and previous research are treated as "threads" (Grönfors, 2011), which can provide working categories for the initial analyses but are a subject to be refined via data-driven interpretations (Mertala, 2020). The main theoretical threads in this study were the stages of AI (Kaplan and Haenlein, 2019), "Big Ideas" in AI (Touretzky et al., 2019) Vygotskian-based idea of everyday concepts (Edwards et al., 2018;Mertala, 2019), existing research on people's conceptions of AI (Cave et al., 2020;Kreinsen & Schultz, 2021;Zhang & Defoe, 2019), and the public representations of AI (e.g., Brokensha, 2020;Chuan et al., 2019;Fast & Horvitz, 2016;Jokela, 2018, Obozintsev, 2018Slotte Dufva & Mertala, 2021. ...
... Our findings suggest that the formation of an accurate scientific conception of AI is unlikely to happen through informal learning among primary school students. This finding is hardly surprising, as studies on children's and adolescents' conceptions of digital data (Pangrazio & Selwyn, 2018), the Internet of Things (Mertala, 2020), machine learning , and search engines (Kodama et al., 2017) have provided similar results. Thus, while seeking to improve students' critical capabilities to make sense of their surrounding worlds now and in the future, our findings support the call for including a form of "AI literacy" in the compulsory education curricula (e.g., Lee et al., 2021;Long & Magerko, 2020;Vartiainen et al., 2020;2021;Yang, 2022) as well as contribute to the formation of a knowledge base for these curricula to build upon. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
In the present paper, we report the findings of a qualitative survey study of 195 Finnish 5th and 6th grade students’ pre-instructional conceptions of artificial intelligence (AI). An exploration of these initial conceptions provides insight into students’ preliminary understanding of the topic and informs curriculum designers and teachers about misconceptions that might jeopardize student learning. The findings suggest that students’ initial conceptions of AI are varied and often uninformed. For instance, references to the role of data in training AI applications were practically nonexistent. Instead, AI was often described as an anthropomorphic technology that possesses cognitive qualities equivalent to those of humans––a conception that notably resembles how AI is portrayed in the media. As a pedagogical implication, our findings suggest that it would be valuable to “demystify” AI by exploring its technical principles (i.e., the role of data) of the “human-like” AI solutions students encounter in their everyday lives.
... We focus on pre-teens (8-12-year olds), in line with Bauman and Tatum's (2009) study of websites and young children. Children nowadays stay digitally connected via mobile devices (Mertala, 2020). In western society, children are required to be active in their own socialization, which in the case of interacting with technology, is done without the benefit of leveraging intergenerational knowledge (Ling, 2000). ...
Article
Full-text available
Social media (SM) platforms are frequently used by pre‐teen (8‐12‐year old) consumers for curating their self‐identity, developing social relationships and for learning. This paper identifies the building blocks that drive pre‐teen SM engagement. We use the Gioia method to analyse interview data collected from 32 pre‐teens and parents, in France. Findings show that the primary building blocks are FoMO (Fear of Missing Out) and social inclusive experience, being noticed online, multiplicity, excessive use (without guidelines) and self‐regulation. Identity constructs (self‐identity and social‐identity) are used to explain SM engagement – and to empirically define core conceptual building blocks (aggregate dimensions) that drive SM engagement. We contribute to consumer theory by developing a holistic research framework to examine pre‐teen SM engagement. Self‐identity and social‐identity theories help explain the factors that drive pre‐teen SM engagement and explain push/pull influences of parents and schools in encouraging or discouraging certain behaviour. We build on current research into SM usage, drawing from the fragmented existing literature, to reveal causes of both excessive screen‐time and SM usage among pre‐teen consumers, which may indicate antecedents of future adult behaviour. Practical and regulatory policy issues are considered and addressed.
Article
Full-text available
Early Childhood Education and Care (ECEC), like much of broader society, has entered the postdigital. The postdigital pertains to a moment in human history whereby practices and digital technologies are intertwined with the daily actions and interactions of people. As ECEC enters the postdigital, attention has been directed towards to understanding the implications of this particular social situation for young children’s learning and development. Drawing on sociomaterialism, contemporary explanations often describe the postdigital mostly in terms of children’s play as both ‘messy’ and ‘entangled’. This paper considers such descriptions a necessary starting point for further engaging with the implications of the postdigital for young children, their families and educators, including in terms of online safety, digital data privacy and protection, and the use of networked technologies by educators for supporting children’s learning. Drawing on the notion of modes of meaning-making from community of practices thinking (Wenger 1998) and aligning these with Jandrić's (2019) call for a collective intelligence for the postdigital through ‘we-act’, ‘we-learn’, and ‘we-think’, this paper proposes the examination of the contemporary literature concerning young children and digital technologies in terms of pedagogy, theory, and method. Three generations of research about young children and digital technologies are identified, leading to the current postdigital moment in time, in which pedagogy, theory and method suggest five new concepts for ECEC in the postdigital, these being convergence, subjectivities, social systems, networks and human experience. The deployment of these concepts in practice and research is examined, illustrating the emergence of a new collective intelligence for ECEC in the postdigital that remains responsive to the ongoing learning and developmental needs of young children over time.
Chapter
Disaster prevention education covers disaster risks and their management, and practice in disaster escape. The aims of this study were to integrate internet of things (IoT) programming education into mudslide disaster prevention education and to explore the effects of this approach on elementary students’ mudslide concepts and computational thinking. This study had a quasi-experiment design. Four classes of fifth-grade students (n = 105) in one elementary school participated for 10 weeks. Two classes comprised the experimental group, and the other two classes comprised the control group. Students in the experimental group had significantly higher computational thinking test scores than did students in the control group; mudslide test scores did not differ significantly between groups. These results the use of IoT programming in mudslide disaster prevention education to improve children’s computational thinking.
Article
Many households include children who use voice personal assistants (VPA) such as Amazon Alexa. Children benefit from the rich functionalities of VPAs and third-party apps but are also exposed to new risks in the VPA ecosystem. In this paper, we first investigate “risky” child-directed voice apps that contain inappropriate content or ask for personal information through voice interactions. We build SkillBot — a natural language processing (NLP)-based system to automatically interact with VPA apps and analyze the resulting conversations. We find 28 risky child-directed apps and maintain a growing dataset of 31,966 non-overlapping app behaviors collected from 3,434 Alexa apps. Our findings suggest that although child-directed VPA apps are subject to stricter policy requirements and more intensive vetting, children remain vulnerable to inappropriate content and privacy violations. We then conduct a user study showing that parents are concerned about the identified risky apps. Many parents do not believe that these apps are available and designed for families/kids, although these apps are actually published in Amazon’s “Kids” product category. We also find that parents often neglect basic precautions such as enabling parental controls on Alexa devices. Finally, we identify a novel risk in the VPA ecosystem: confounding utterances, or voice commands shared by multiple apps that may cause a user to interact with a different app than intended. We identify 4,487 confounding utterances, including 581 shared by child-directed and non-child-directed apps. We find that 27% of these confounding utterances prioritize invoking a non-child-directed app over a child-directed app. This indicates that children are at real risk of accidentally invoking non-child-directed apps due to confounding utterances.
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to determine the usability of the internet of things technology in open and distance learning environments. In this context, the phenomenology method was used in order to get the opinions of field experts. The criterion sampling method, which is one of the purposeful sampling methods, was used in determining the participants. As a criterion, it is taken into account that the participants are experts in the field of open and distance learning. In this context, eight field experts contributed to the study. The findings obtained at the end of the study clarify how internet of things technology can be used in traditional face-to-face learning environments and open and distance learning environments, what benefits it can provide, what are its limitations, its future usage trends, and how it can be used outside of learning environments.
Article
Full-text available
The Internet of Toys (IoToys) as an emerging type of edutainment presents a new research area, especially in the context of learning. This study investigates four connected toys played with in the preschool context. By turning to preschool-aged children and their educators observed and interviewed during and after a play test and group interview session, we study how the educational value of IoToys is actualized in a play situation in an early learning environment. In order for IoToys to work as tools in toy-based learning in the preschool context, we suggest that educators acknowledge the engagement with these toys as a form of transmedia play which demands transmedia literacy skills.
Technical Report
Full-text available
The aim of this report is therefore twofold. First, we aim to provide a critical introduction to the Internet of Toys, by setting its conceptual boundaries and discussing the theoretical, methodological and policy challenges it raises. Second, we aim to report on the findings of a small comparative project we have carried out as part of the activities of Working Group 4 of the COST Action DigiLitEY. At this stage, Internet-connected toys are an emerging market, thus making empirical research on their appropriation and use in the everyday lives of children and their families difficult. As a consequence, and in order to understand whether and how IoToys have entered play discourses, we examine the discursive environment of smart toys, i.e. its representations in media commentaries and commercial advertisements.
Article
Full-text available
While children are living more of their lives online, little is known about what they understand about the implications of their online participation. Here we report on the Best Footprint Forward project which explored how children come to understand the internet. Thirty-three children (ranging in age from 10 to 12 years old) from three primary schools in regional Australia participated in focus groups and created a work sample depicting the internet. Analysis of the focus group transcripts and work samples revealed that while the children's understanding of the internet was not technical, their knowledge was developed through the social activities that they engaged in online, and influenced by the interactions they have in their 'real life' with parents, teachers and friends. The children in the study demonstrated an ambivalence about the internet; they regularly went online for a variety of purposes but these positive experiences were tempered by concerns and fears. This research presents a nuanced perspective of children's knowledge of the internet; by rejecting the notion that children are naïve, passive consumers of digital culture, analysis of their understanding reveals it to be balanced and sophisticated.
Article
Young children aged 4-5 years are online in rapidly increasing numbers. This is due to the accessibility of the internet afforded young children via touchscreen technologies. In Australian, the United Kingdom and United States of America, calls have been made for cyber-safety education to be provided for young children in their early childhood settings. However, little knowledge exists regarding the pedagogical provision of this education. This paper reports on study conducted with three Australian early childhood teachers focussed on the development of cyber-safety education for young children.
Article
Touch screen tablets (e.g., iPads) are being increasingly used by young children due to their stimulating multimodal features and intuitive touch-based interface. However, little is known about the effects of tablets and apps on the development of emergent literacy skills. This pre-post-test randomised controlled study explored the effects of using literacy apps on emergent literacy skills in English speaking children aged 2–5 years (N = 48). There were 24 children in the iPad group and 24 children in the waitlist control group. The 9-week (30 min/week) iPad literacy program focussed on three new alphabet letters each week using three apps (letter matching, letter tracing, and drawing). Following the program, children in the iPad group showed significantly higher letter name and sound knowledge, print concepts and name writing skills than children in the control group. No significant group differences were found for letter writing skills or numeral knowledge. The findings showed that tablets can positively support letter name and sound learning and aspects of emergent writing development. How teachers can best utilise these digital tools in early childhood classrooms to support emergent literacy requires further investigation.
Article
Contemporary homes are filled with digital technologies, and children are exposed to them almost since birth, initiating their first digital experiences at very early ages (Chaudron et al., 2015, Young children (0–8)). This trend is expected to become stronger, as our future has been envisioned around the concept of the IoT (Internet of Things), and the first smart toys are arriving at the homes of digitally savvy families. This study focuses on the digital practices of young children, looking particularly at smart toys, aiming to: a) explore how smart toys are being adopted by families, considering the perceptions of children and parents about drivers and barriers to adoption; and b) understand the domestication of smart toys. Our research is exploratory and builds on the theoretical framework, methodological protocols and ethical guidelines of the international projects “Young Children (0–8) and Digital Technologies” (Chaudron et al., 2015, Chaudron et al., Young children (0–8); Dias and Brito, 2016, 2017, Crianças (0 a 8 anos)) and “Media and Social Discourses around Young Children and IoToys” (Mascheroni & Holloway, 2017). Our methodology is qualitative, based on visits to families including interviews with children and parents and participant observation. We selected a purposive sample of 21 medium‐ or high‐income and digitally savvy families in Portugal. Concerning drivers and barriers for adoption, most children identify several smart toys and express interest in having them. Parents are motivated by the amount of satisfaction that the toy will afford the children and the added‐value in terms of learning or developing skills. However, most parents prefer mechanical toys, sports or outdoors activities and the high price is an obstacle. Concerning domestication, smart toys are still scarce in homes and perceived as novelty, thus being in a very early stage of domestication.
Article
This paper investigates the security and privacy of Internet-connected children’s smart toys through case studies of three commercially-available products. We conduct network and application vulnerability analyses of each toy using static and dynamic analysis techniques, including application binary decompilation and network monitoring. We discover several publicly undisclosed vulnerabilities that violate the Children’s Online Privacy Protection Rule (COPPA) as well as the toys’ individual privacy policies. These vulnerabilities, especially security flaws in network communications with first-party servers, are indicative of a disconnect between many IoT toy developers and security and privacy best practices despite increased attention to Internet-connected toy hacking risks.