ArticlePDF Available

Domestic cats ( Felis catus ) discriminate their names from other words

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Two of the most common nonhuman animals that interact with humans are domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and cats (Felis catus). In contrast to dogs, the ability of domestic cats to communicate with humans has not been explored thoroughly. We used a habituation-dishabituation method to investigate whether domestic cats could discriminate human utterances, which consisted of cats’ own names, general nouns, and other cohabiting cats’ names. Cats from ordinary households and from a ‘cat café’ participated in the experiments. Among cats from ordinary households, cats habituated to the serial presentation of four different general nouns or four names of cohabiting cats showed a significant rebound in response to the subsequent presentation of their own names; these cats discriminated their own names from general nouns even when unfamiliar persons uttered them. These results indicate that cats are able to discriminate their own names from other words. There was no difference in discrimination of their own names from general nouns between cats from the cat café and household cats, but café cats did not discriminate their own names from other cohabiting cats’ names. We conclude that cats can discriminate the content of human utterances based on phonemic differences.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
1
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:5394 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-40616-4
www.nature.com/scientificreports
Domestic cats (Felis catus)
discriminate their names from
other words
Atsuko Saito1,2,3, Kazutaka Shinozuka4, Yuki Ito1 & Toshikazu Hasegawa1
Two of the most common nonhuman animals that interact with humans are domestic dogs (Canis
familiaris) and cats (Felis catus). In contrast to dogs, the ability of domestic cats to communicate
with humans has not been explored thoroughly. We used a habituation-dishabituation method to
investigate whether domestic cats could discriminate human utterances, which consisted of cats’ own
names, general nouns, and other cohabiting cats’ names. Cats from ordinary households and from a
‘cat café’ participated in the experiments. Among cats from ordinary households, cats habituated to the
serial presentation of four dierent general nouns or four names of cohabiting cats showed a signicant
rebound in response to the subsequent presentation of their own names; these cats discriminated their
own names from general nouns even when unfamiliar persons uttered them. These results indicate
that cats are able to discriminate their own names from other words. There was no dierence in
discrimination of their own names from general nouns between cats from the cat café and household
cats, but café cats did not discriminate their own names from other cohabiting cats’ names. We
conclude that cats can discriminate the content of human utterances based on phonemic dierences.
Domestic cats (Felis catus) and dogs (Canis familiaris) are the most popular companion animals; worldwide, over
600 million cats live with humans1, and in some countries their number equals or exceeds the number of dogs
(e.g., Japan: dogs: 8,920,000, cats: 9,526,000)2,3. Cats started to cohabit with humans about 9,500 years ago4; their
history of cohabitation with humans is shorter than that of dogs5, and they have been domesticated by natural
selection, not by articial selection68. Despite these dierences in their process of domestication compared to
that of dogs, cats too have developed behaviours related to communication with humans; for example, for human
listeners, the vocalisations of domestic cats are more comfortable than those of African wild cats (Felis silvestris
lybica)9. In addition, purring has dierent acoustical components during solicitation of foods than at other times,
and humans perceive such solicitation purrs as more urgent and unpleasant than non-solicitation purrs10. ese
facts clearly indicate that domestic cats have developed the ability to communicate with humans and frequently
do so; Bradshaw8 suggested that this inter-species communicative ability is descended from intra-species com-
municative ability.
Researchers have only recently begun to investigate cats’ ability to communicate with humans. Miklósi et
al. showed that cats are able to use the human pointing gesture as a cue to nd hidden food, similarly to dogs11.
e researchers also suggested that cats do not gaze toward humans when they cannot access food, unlike dogs.
However, a recent study revealed that cats show social referencing behaviour (gazing at human face) when
exposed to a potentially frightening object, and to some extent cats changed their behaviour depending on the
facial expression of their owner (positive or negative)12. Cats in food begging situations can also discriminate the
attentional states of humans who look at and call to them13. In addition, Galvan and Vonk demonstrated that cats
were modestly sensitive to their owners emotions14, and other research has indicated that cats’ behaviour is inu-
enced by human mood15,16. Further, cats can discriminate their owner’s voice from a stranger’s17. is research
evidence illustrates that domestic cats have the ability to recognize human gestural, facial, and vocal cues.
1Department of Cognitive and Behavioral Science, Graduate School of Arts and Sciences, the University of Tokyo,
3-8-1 Komaba, Meguro-ku, Tokyo, Japan. 2Department of Childhood Education, Musashino University, 1-1-20
Shinmachi, Nishitokyo-shi, Tokyo, Japan. 3Department of Psychology, Faculty of Human Sciences, Sophia University,
7-1 Kioicho, Chiyoda-ku, Tokyo, Japan. 4RIKEN Center for Brain Science, 2-1 Hirosawa, Wako, Saitama, Japan. Atsuko
Saito and Kazutaka Shinozuka contributed equally. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed
to A.S. (email: atsaito@sophia.ac.jp)
Received: 21 June 2016
Accepted: 20 February 2019
Published online: 04 April 2019
Corrected: Author Correction
OPEN
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
2
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:5394 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-40616-4
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
In contrast to cats, numerous research studies have shown the ability of domestic dogs to communicate with
humans. Dogs are skilful at reading human communicative gestures, such as pointing (reviewed in Miklósi &
Soproni18). Dogs can dierentiate human attentional states1922 and distinguish human smiling faces from blank
expressions23. ey are also capable of using some human emotional expressions to help them nd hidden food
and fetch objects24,25.
Although the majority of prior studies have focused on visual communication between humans and dogs26,
some studies have investigated the dog’s ability to respond to human vocalisations. For example, the pitch of a
human voice aects dog behaviour27: dogs obey high-pitched voices to a greater extent than low-pitched voices.
Dogs can discriminate expressions of emotion with voice28, and obey a command with angry voice more slowly
than with happy voice. Dogs trained to sit and come in response to tape-recorded commands change their perfor-
mance when the phonemes of commands are changed29. Many dog owners believe their dogs understand about
30 utterances30. Extensively trained dogs are able to dierentiate 200–1000 human words or labels31,32. e ability
to understand human verbal utterances is also shown in other species, such as apes33, dolphins34, and parrots35;
however, whether such an ability exists in domestic cats remains untested.
In the present study, we investigated the ability of domestic cats to discriminate human verbal utterances. Cats
are sensitive to dierences in human voice characteristics17. Some owners insist that their cats can recognize their
own names and words related to food. erefore, we can make the following hypothesis: cats can discriminate
words uttered by humans from other words—especially their own names, because a cat’s name is a salient stim-
ulus as it may be the human utterance most frequently heard by domestic cats (cats kept by humans) and may be
associated with rewards, such as food, petting, and play.
We conducted experiments in cats’ homes, using a habituation-dishabituation method, as in our previous
study17. In general, dogs’ ability to recognize human utterances are tested using command and retrieval tasks31,36.
ese tasks require pre-training, and the training of cats to perform on command would require a lot of eort
and time. On the other hand, habituation-dishabituation method enabled us to measure cats’ natural reactions
during a single visit, without extensive training. To test the hypothesis, we presented four dierent words serially
as habituation stimuli, then presented the cats’ own names as test stimuli. If the cats were habituated to the other
4 words and dishabituated to their own names, a rebound response to the presentation of their own names would
be observed, indicating the ability to discriminate their own names from other words.
We conducted four experiments to test the hypothesis. In Experiment 1, we investigated whether cats can dis-
criminate their own names from general nouns with the same length and accents as their own names. If cats can
discriminate their own names from other words by using phonetic characteristics other than length of or accent
of stimuli, cats habituated to the other 4 words should show dishabituation when hearing their own names. e
test cats were living either with no other cats or with a small number of other cats. In this experiment, although
we equalized the familiarity of the nouns, the relative familiarity of names and other nouns was markedly dif-
ferent, that is, cats heard their own names more frequently than other nouns. erefore, cats discriminated their
own names depending both on phonetic characteristics and on familiarity. In Experiment 2, we investigated cats’
ability to discriminate their own names from other cats’ names, by using cats living with 4 or more other cats. It
can be assumed that the test cats were exposed to the other cats’ names as well as their own names; stimuli were
prepared using cohabiting cats’ names. en, in Experiment 3, we examined eects of multiple-cat living envi-
ronments on discrimination of general nouns and cats’ own names, similar to Experiment 1. In Experiments 2
and 3, we tested cats both from ordinary households and from a ‘cat café’, a business establishment where visitors
can freely interact with cats. In Experiments 1 to 3, stimuli used cats’ owners’ own voices, because they exhibit a
marked response to their owner’s voice17. However, this leaves open the possibility that cats can discriminate their
own names only when their owners utter them. us, in Experiment 4, we tested whether cats can discriminate
their own names from general nouns even when unfamiliar persons utter them; if they showed discrimination
ability in this experiment, we would take them to recognize their own names based on common phonetic char-
acteristics in human verbal utterances.
Results
Behaviour score. e upper panels of Fig.1 summarise the cats’ responses to the stimuli, as scored by the
experimenter. rough all the experiments, more than half of the cats responded to voice stimuli by moving
their ears and heads; fewer than 10% of the cats demonstrated vocalisation, tail movement, and displacement.
is trend did not dier contingent on whether stimuli were nouns, other cats’ names, or tested cats’ own names.
Fisher’s exact test revealed that number of cats which showed orienting response (moving ear and/or moving
head) were signicantly higher than which showed communicative response (vocalization and/or tail movement)
in all trials from Experiment 1 to 4 (Supplementary TableS1).
e total scores (Fig.1 lower panels) were moderately correlated with the average response magnitude eval-
uated by the raters, as shown in the next section (Spearman’s rank correlation, ρ = 0.70, P < 0.001; ρ = 0.61,
P < 0.001; ρ = 0.64, P < 0.001, ρ = 0.60, P < 0.001 for Experiments 1, 2, 3, and 4, respectively). us, the raters
evaluations of the response magnitudes might have partly depended on the number of simultaneously occurring
responses by the cats.
Response magnitude. In Experiment 1, the raters’ evaluations revealed that eleven out of the 16 test cats
decreased their average response magnitude from noun 1 to noun 4. ese cats were considered to have suc-
cessfully habituated to the general nouns vocalised by the owners. en, nine out of the eleven habituated cats
increased their response magnitude from noun 4 to their own name. Group-level analysis using a generalized
linear mixed model (GLMM) revealed a signicant eect of stimulus category (F(1,10) = 11.18, P = 0.007),
indicating eleven habituated cats signicantly increased in response magnitude from noun 4 to their own name
(t(10) = 3.34, P = 0.007, Fig.2a). us, habituated cats dishabituated when they heard their own names.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
3
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:5394 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-40616-4
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
In Experiment 2, 15 out of the 34 test cats decreased average response magnitude from name 1 to name 4, and
were considered to have successfully habituated to stimuli consisting of the names of other cohabiting cats. e
ratio of successfully habituated cats was dierent between the ordinary households and the cat café (ordinary
households: 6 out of 24, cat café: 9 out of 10; χ2 = 9.60, df = 1, P = 0.002). Although the ratio of successfully habit-
uated cats from ordinary households is very low, we analysed the data from these six cats because of methodolog-
ical restriction. We added housing environment (ordinary households or cat café) as a xed eect for group-level
analysis. GLMM revealed a signicant eect of interaction (stimulus category * environment; F(1,13) = 8.26,
P = 0.013). All six habituated cats from ordinary households increased their response magnitudes from name
Figure 1. Response style to vocal stimuli in overall cats. Upper panels: Behaviour observed in response to voice
stimuli and the percentage of cats that expressed each behaviour in (a) Experiment 1, (b) Experiment 2, (c)
Experiment 3, and (d) Experiment 4. Black solid lines indicate orienting response. Black dashed lines indicate
communicative response. Gray solid lines indicate displacement. Lower panels: Mean total behavioural scores
for all cats in (a) Experiment 1, (b) Experiment 2, (c) Experiment 3, and (d) Experiment 4. Error bars indicate
SEs.
Figure 2. Mean magnitude of responses to each voice in habituated cats in (a) Experiment 1, (b) Experiment
2, (c) Experiment 3, and (d) Experiment 4. Error bars indicate SEs. Asterisks indicate signicant dierences
(P < 0.05).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
4
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:5394 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-40616-4
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
4 to their own names. Post-hoc analysis revealed that response magnitudes to stimuli for cats’ own names were
signicantly higher than those for name 4 in these six habituated cats (t(13) = 3.43, P = 0.005, Fig.2b). In con-
trast, only three out of nine habituated cats from the cat café increased their response magnitudes from name 4 to
their own name. e response magnitudes to cats’ own names did not dier from those to name 4 in these nine
habituated cats (t(13) = 0.35, P = 0.732, Fig.2b). Signicantly higher response was also seen in household cats
compared to café cats in response to own name (t (20.24) = 2.39, P = 0.027), but not in response to noun 4 (t
(20.24) = 0.38, P = 0.705, Fig.2b).
In Experiment 3, following the results of Experiment 2, we again included the environment as a xed eect.
Fourteen out of the 20 household cats decreased average response magnitude from noun 1 to noun 4. Seven out
of nine café cats decreased average response magnitude from noun 1 to noun 4. ese cats were considered to
have successfully habituated to the stimuli consisting of spoken nouns. In contrast to Experiment 2, interaction
of stimulus category * environment was not signicant (F (1,19) = 1.52, P = 0.233). A nal model only included
the eect of stimulus category (F (1,20) = 6.05, P = 0.023). irteen out of the 21 habituated cats increased their
response magnitude from noun 4 to their own name. e response magnitude to cats’ own names was signif-
icantly higher from that to noun 4 in these 21 habituated cats (t(20) = 2.46, P = 0.023, Fig.2c). us, these
habituated cats dishabituated when they heard their own names.
In Experiment 4, the raters’ evaluations revealed that 20 out of the 33 test cats decreased their average response
magnitude from noun 1 to noun 4; these cats were considered to have successfully habituated to the general nouns
vocalised by unfamiliar persons. en, 13 out of the 20 habituated cats increased their response magnitude from
noun 4 to their own name. Group-level analysis revealed a signicant eect of stimulus category in twenty habit-
uated cats (F(1,19) = 4.41, P = 0.049), who dishabituated signicantly when they heard their own name uttered
by an unfamiliar person as compared to noun 4 (t(19) = 2.10, P = 0.049, Fig.2d).
We also analysed habituated cats’ sum of behaviour score (total score) to test whether number of responses
simultaneously elicited in response to a vocal stimulus increased from trial 4 (noun or other cats name) to trial
5 (test cat’s name). However, unlike response magnitude, signicant increase in the total score was not observed
except for Experiment 2 (Supplementary Fig.S1). is result suggests that qualitative analysis of behaviour with
present/absent manner is less sensitive to detect dishabituation. It is conrmed that eectiveness of quantitative
analysis with the response magnitude coded by blind raters.
Discussion
In Experiments 1, 3, and 4, cats that habituated to general nouns with the same length and accent as their own
names dishabituated to their own names. is was true both when their owner’s voice was presented (Experiments
1 and 3) and when the unfamiliar person’s voice was presented (Experiment 4), in spite of the fact that cats distin-
guish owners’ voices from unfamiliar persons’ voices17. ese results show that cats can identify their own names
from other words that consisted of the same number of mora but with dierent phonemes when they are uttered
both by familiar person and by unfamiliar person. e results of Experiment 2 suggest that cats from ordinary
households discriminate their own names from those of cohabiting cats but that cats from a cat café may not.
From the results of all experiments, it thus appears that at least cats living in ordinary households can distinguish
their own names from general words and names of other cats. is is the rst experimental evidence showing cats’
ability to understand human verbal utterances.
How can we explain this ability and behaviour on the part of the cats? eir own names must be one of the
most-heard human utterances by cats. If they have no meaning, frequently experienced stimuli should be habitu-
ated and not elicit reaction from cats. However, the results of our experiment were to the contrary; thus, the asso-
ciation between hearing their names and receiving rewards or punishments might aect the behaviour of cats.
is implies that cats’ names can be associated with rewards, such as food, petting, and play, or with punishments,
such as taking them to a veterinary clinic or to a bath. Sometimes, owners who keep multiple cats will call all of
their cats’ names at the same time. In that situation, a cat may associate both its own name and those of cohabiting
cats with reward. ese situations could explain the results of Experiment 2: the ratio of ordinary household cats
that successfully habituated to names of other cohabiting cats was very low (6 out of 24). ere is a possibility
that cats housed with other multiple cats may associate other cats’ names with rewarding or unpleasant events.
However, in some situations, for example, when the owner wishes to take it to a veterinary clinic, or to pet a cat,
they may call only one cat’s name. Taking them to clinic should be a stressor. Petting could be rewarding to the
cat37, although depending on the cats’ personality, it could also be a stressor38. ese situations would facilitate a
cat’s learning to discriminate its own name from those of other cats.
If cats associate their own name with rewards or stressors, it is reasonable to think that they react to their
name. In these experiments, cats responded to owner vocalisation not with communicative behaviour (vocal-
isation and tail moving)39 but just with orienting behaviour (ear moving and head moving)40. is tendency
replicated that reported in our previous study17. is may be caused by the dierence between the situation where
we conducted the experiments and the natural situation. In normal reward or stress situations, name calling by
owners may elicit more dynamic, or communicative reaction from cats.
Next, we consider the results from the cat café. e café cats did not discriminate their own names from the
names of cohabiting cats, though their performance in the discrimination of their own names from general nouns
did not dier from that of ordinary household cats. e social environment may explain this dierence in results.
Many dierent humans visit cat cafés, and since the cats’ names are listed in cafés, visitors can call the names of
the cats. However, the way names are called may vary by visitor (e.g., intonation may vary); such a condition may
hinder cats in discriminating their name from those of other cats. Or, café cats may hear their name mentioned
along with other cat names frequently without accurate reward discrimination by visitors. For example, if a visitor
calls cat A, but cat B approaches to the visitor and cat B gets petting and treats instead of cat A. ese situations
would make name discrimination less relevant for these cats. Additionally, the number of cohabiting cats may
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
5
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:5394 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-40616-4
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
have aected the results. Usually, the number of cats in a cat café is greater than the number in an ordinary house-
hold. Further, because we conducted the experiment in only one cat café, we cannot assure their generalisability
or reach a denitive conclusion.
Nevertheless, this study has demonstrated that cats can discriminate human utterances based on phonemic
dierences. Although such discrimination is acquired without explicit discrimination training, instead emerging
from the patterns of natural daily communication between humans and cats, we may utilise this ability positively
for cats’ quality of life. For example, perhaps we can get cats to learn that dangerous objects or places are referred
to by specic utterances. is work has shed new light on the ability of cats to communicate with humans; further
clarifying cats’ abilities with respect to cat–human communication will potentially enhance the welfare of both
humans and cats.
Methods
Subjects. In Experiment 1, the participants were 16 domestic cats (8 males and 8 females; age range: 1–11
years, mean age: 3.69 years, SD = 3.01) living with 11 families (three male and eight female owners), each of whom
lived with 2 or fewer other cats. By breed, there were 12 mongrels, two Scottish Folds, an American Shorthair, and
a Himalayan. Fieen of the cats had begun to live with their owner within one year of birth, and one cat when it
was 5 years old. Fieen of the cats were neutered (one female was not).
In Experiment 2, 34 domestic cats (16 males and 18 females) each of which was living with 4 or more other
cats, participated. Twenty-four cats were owned by four families and the remaining 10 were part of a ‘cat café, a
business establishment where visitors can freely interact with cats. e cats had six female owners (two owners
were members of the same household). Of the 34 cats, there were 24 mongrels, three LaPerms, a Devon Rex, a
Somali, a Scottish Fold, an American Curl, a LaPerm Shorthair, a Tonkinese, and a Munchkin. eir ages ranged
from 0.5 to 10 years (mean age: 5.51 years, SD = 2.95), and the ages when they began to live with their owners
ranged from birth to 36 months aer birth. All cats were neutered.
In Experiment 3, participants were 29 domestic cats (16 males and 13 females) living with 4 or more other
cats. ey were kept by three families and one cat café, which had four female owners; of the 29 cats, 9 were from
the cat café. Breeds were 21 mongrels, three LaPerms, a Scottish Fold, an American Curl, a LaPerm Shorthair,
a Tonkinese, and a Munchkin. eir ages ranged from 1 to 11 years (mean age: 6.48 years, SD = 3.29). e ages
when they began to live with their owners ranged from birth to 36 months aer birth. All cats were neutered. Of
these 29 cats, 26 cats participated in Experiment 2. Interval between Experiment 2 and 3 was at least 2 weeks.
In Experiment 4, participants were 33 domestic cats (14 males and 19 females) living with from 0 to 5 other
cats. Of them, 30 cats were kept in 21 families (2 male and 19 female owners) and 3 cats were kept in univer-
sity laboratories. Of the 33 cats, 24 were mongrels, two LaPerms, two American Shorthair, a Scottish Fold, a
Himalayan, a Russian Blue, a Norwegian Forest Cat, and a Bengal. eir ages ranged from 1 to 17 years (mean
age: 6.48 years, SD = 4.14), and the ages when they began to live with their owners ranged from one month
to 36 months aer birth. All cats were neutered, excepting one female. Of these 33 cats, 3 had participated in
Experiment 1 and 5 had participated in Experiments 2 and 3. Experiment 4 was conducted about 3 years aer
Experiment 3. In all experiments, all cats were indoor only except one, and cats were not subjected to food dep-
rivation during the study period. Detailed information is presented in the electronic Supplementary Material
(TablesS2–S5).
Apparatus and Stimuli. Before the experiments began, for each cat, ve sound stimuli consisting of human
voice were recorded. One stimulus consisted of a human calling the cat’s name. e other four stimuli consisted of
a human vocalising four dierent general nouns (Experiments 1, 3, and 4) or four names of other cats living with
the test cats (Experiment 2). For Experiments 1, 2, and 3, the stimuli were recorded by the owners of the tested
cats. For Experiment 4, the stimuli were recorded by two women unfamiliar to the tested cats. Each owner was
instructed to vocalise the cat’s names as he/she normally would; if the owner usually called the cat by a nickname
instead of its real name, the nickname was used. In Experiments 1, 3, and 4, four dierent general Japanese nouns
were selected from the list of Matsumoto41; all nouns had the same level of familiarity and were emotionally neu-
tral. e numbers of moras and accents in the nouns were the same as in the cats name. Speakers were instructed
to vocalise the nouns with the same intonation and manner as they vocalised the cats’ names. In Experiment 2,
four of the other cohabiting cats’ names were recorded similarly to the test cats’ names. e orders of presentation
of general nouns and cohabiting cats’ names were pseudo-randomized.
We recorded the vocalisations with a handheld digital audio recorder (ZOOM H2 Handy Recorder) in WAV
format; the sampling rate was 44100 Hz with 16-bit quantisation. e sound stimuli were adjusted to the same
volume level using sound editing soware (Adobe Soundbooth CS4 or Adobe Audition CS6). During the exper-
iment, the handheld recorder was used to present the stimuli through a speaker (Sony SRS-Z100), which was
hidden from the test cat. e distance between the test cat and the speaker was about 3 m, and the volume of
the voices was approximately 65 dB at 3 m from the speaker. A video camera (Sanyo DNX-CA9 or Panasonic
HX-WA20) placed in front of the test cats recorded their reactions during the playback of the stimuli.
For Experiment 1, 3, and 4, the discriminant analysis was performed to conrm that there was no implicit
dierence in acoustic characteristics between noun and name stimuli. Vocal stimuli for cats which showed dis-
habituation (habituated cats with increasing response magnitude from noun 4 to own name: N = 9, 13, and 13
in Experiment 1, 3, and 4, respectively) were selected for analysis. Six acoustic parameters were extracted from
each vocal stimulus by using Praat 6.0.43 soware: total duration (sec), mean pitch (Hz), f1 (Hz), f2 (Hz), f3
(Hz), and mean intensity (dB). en the discriminant analysis was applied with IBM SPSS Statistics 21. Above
acoustic parameters were set as independent variables, and type of stimulus (noun or name) was set as a group.
As a result of the analysis, high values of Wilks lambda were obtained (Experiment 1, Wilks lambda = 0.930,
χ2 = 2.884, df = 6, P = 0.823; Experiment 3, Wilks lambda = 0.821, χ2 = 11.866, df = 6, P = 0.065; Experiment 4,
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
6
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:5394 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-40616-4
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
Wilks lambda = 0.979, χ2 = 1.294, df = 6, P = 0.972), indicating that it was dicult to discriminate between noun
and name stimuli by using implicit acoustical characteristics as a cue.
Procedure. Experiments 1, 2, and 3 were conducted from December 2012 to November 2013; Experiment
4 was conducted from September 2016 to April 2017. All experiments were held in each owner’s home or in the
cat café, wherever the particular cats lived. e experimenter waited until cats were calm before beginning the
experiment. During the experiment, the owners were out of their cat’s sight. We used a habituation-dishabituation
procedure in which prepared stimuli were played serially with a 15-s inter-stimulus interval (ISI); the order of
presentation was word 1, word 2, word 3, word 4, and test cat’s name. e number of habituation stimuli and the
ISI were improved versions of those used in a previous study17. Cats’ responses to the stimuli were expected to
decrease during the presentation of words 1 through 4 due to habituation; then, if the cats could discriminate
their own names from the other words, responses were expected to increase again when their own names were
presented, due to dishabituation. e experiment lasted around 1.5 minutes. During presentation, the test cat
was not actively isolated from cohabiting cats, to keep the test cats behaviour natural. ere was no need for any
interruption in the experimental sessions due to cohabiting cats’ behaviour.
All procedures related to animal care and experimentation in our research adhered to the ‘Guidelines for
the treatment of animals in behavioural research and teaching’ as published by the Association for the Study
of Animal Behaviour in Animal Behaviour 71, 245–253 (2006) and to the ethical guidelines of the University
of Tokyo. e study was approved by the Animal Experiments Committee of the Graduate School of Arts and
Sciences of the University of Tokyo and by the Animal Experiments Committee of Musashino University.
Behavioural analysis. Video-recordings of cats’ responses were trimmed to show from 5 s before stimulus
onset to 10 s aer stimulus oset, using Adobe Premiere CS6. Vocalisation of the words and cats’ names in the
clips was masked by pure tones to facilitate blind evaluation of the clips. In total, 80, 170, 145, and 165 clips were
created for Experiments 1, 2, 3, and 4, respectively.
We conducted two kinds of analyses to investigate the cats’ response styles and magnitudes, as in our previous
study17. e rst analysis describes response style. One of the experimenters (KS) observed the clips of each cat
in random order and classied the cat’s responses to the stimuli into ve categories: ear moving, head moving,
vocalising, tail moving, and displacement; each category is described in Table1. ese categories cover orienting
responses (ear moving and head moving)40 and communicative responses (vocalising and tail moving)39. Each
category was scored separately as 0 (absent) or 1 (present) for each clip, to determine the proportion of cats show-
ing each response in each presentation trial. en, the summed score was calculated as the total score for each
clip, to enable examination of the correlation between the numbers of categories occurring simultaneously and
response magnitude rated by blind raters (described in the next section). To check for reliability, the other exper-
imenter (AS) observed a random selection of one-fourth of the clips and scored the cats’ behaviours. e indices
of concordance were 0.75 for ear moving, 0.81 for head moving, 0.99 for vocalising, 0.97 for tail moving, and 0.99
for displacement (κ = 0.76, P < 0.001 for overall observation).
e second analysis was conducted to examine response magnitude. Raters who were blind to the stimuli and
their presentation order scored each cat’s responses in the clips, which were presented in random order within
each test cat. In Experiment 1, there were ten blind raters (6 men and 4 women; mean age = 21.7 years), whereas
in Experiment 2 and 3 there were six blind raters (all women; mean age = 27.5 years), and in Experiment 4, nine
blind raters (one man and 8 women; mean age = 22.9 years). e raters were instructed to compare each cats
behaviours before and aer the presentation of each stimulus and rate the magnitude of the cats responses to the
stimuli from 0 (no response) to 3 (marked response). Kendall’s coecient of concordance showed signicant,
moderate concordance among the raters (W = 0.73, df = 79, P < 0.001; W = 0.73, df = 169, P < 0.001; W = 0.65,
df = 144, P < 0.001, W = 0.55, df = 164, P < 0.001 for Experiments 1, 2 3, and 4, respectively).
Mean response magnitude was calculated for each video clip and used for subsequent analysis. GLMM was
applied using the lme4 package version 1.1–13 on R soware version 3.4.1. Stimulus category (Experiment
1; noun 4 v. own name, Experiment 2; other cat’s name 4 v. own name, Experiment 3; noun 4 v. own name,
Experiment 4; noun 4 v. own name) was set as a xed eect. Environment (ordinary households v. cat café) and
interaction of stimulus category * environment were also set as xed eects for Experiments 2 and 3. Subjects
were set as a random eect. Gaussian distribution with identity link function was specied for lmer function.
en, post-hoc analysis was conducted using the step function in the lmerTest package version 2.0–33; the step
function reduced non-signicant xed eects and determined a nal model. e random eect (subjects) was
manually kept regardless of signicance, to control pseudo-replication.
Data Availability
e data supporting this article are included in Supplementary Electronic Information.
Category Description
Ear moving Any change in ear(s) angle from ear root
Head moving Any change in head angle at the neck
Vocalising Any vocalisation
Tail moving Any movement of tail between its root and tip
Displacement More than one step of displacement of both
hind paws in any direction
Table 1. Descriptions of categories for behavioural scores.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
7
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:5394 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-40616-4
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
References
1. Driscoll, C. A., Clutton-Broc, J., itchener, A. C. & O’Brien, S. J. e taming of the cat: Genetic and archaeological ndings hint that
wildcats became housecats earlier—and in a dierent place—than previously thought. Sci. Am. 300, 68–75, https://doi.org/10.1038/
scienticamerican0609-68 (2009).
2. Ferdman, . A. & Ingraham, C. Where cats are more popular than dogs in the U.S.—and all over the world. Washington Post, https://
www.washingtonpost.com/news/won/wp/2014/07/28/where-cats-are-more-popular-than-dogs-in-the-u-s-and-all-over-the-
world (2014).
3. Japan Pet Food Association. e breeding rate and number of breeding of dogs and cats. Japan Pet Food Association, http://www.
petfood.or.jp/data/chart2017/3.pdf (2017).
4. Vigne, J. D., Guilaine, J., Debue, ., Haye, L. & Gerard, P. Early taming of the cat in Cyprus. Science 304, 259, https://doi.org/10.1126/
science.1095335 (2004).
5. Clutton-Broc, J. Origins of the dog: Domestication and early history in e domestic dog: Its evolution, behaviour, and interactions
with people (ed. Serpell, J. A.) 7–20 (Cambridge University Press, 1995).
6. Clutton-Broc, J. e British Museum Book of Cats (British Museum Publications Ltd., 1988).
7. Driscoll, C. A., Macdonald, D. W. & O’Brien, S. J. From wild animals to domestic pets, an evolutionary view of domestication. Proc.
Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 9971–9978, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0901586106 (2009).
8. Bradshaw, J. W. S. Sociality in cats: A comparative review. J. Vet. Behav. 11, 113–124, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2015.09.004
(2016).
9. Nicastro, N. Perceptual and acoustic evidence for species-level dierences in meow vocalizations by domestic cats (Felis catus) and
African wild cats (Felis silvestris lybica). J. Comp. Psychol. 118, 287–296 (2004).
10. McComb, ., Taylor, A. M., Wilson, C. & Charlton, B. D. e cry embedded within the purr. Curr. Biol. 19, 507–508, https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.cub.2009.05.033 (2009).
11. Milósi, Á., Pongrácz, P., Laatos, G., Topál, J. & Csányi, V. A comparative study of the use of visual communicative signals in
interactions between dogs (Canis familiari s) and humans and cats (Felis catus) and humans. J. Comp. Psychol. 119, 179–186, https://
doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.179 (2005).
12. Merola, I., Lazzaroni, M., Marshall-Pescini, S. & Prato-Previde, E. Social referencing and cat-human communication. Anim. Cogn.
18, 639–648, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0832-2 (2015).
13. Ito, Y., Watanabe, A., Taagi, S., Arahori, M. & Saito, A. Cats beg for food from the human who loos at and calls to them: Ability to
understand humans’ attentional states. Psychologia 59, 112–120, https://doi.org/10.2117/psysoc.2016.112 (2016).
14. Galvan, M. & Von, J. Man’s other best friend: domestic cats (F. silvestris catus) and their discrimination of human emotion cues.
Anim. Cogn. 19, 193–205, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-015-0927-4 (2016).
15. ieger, G. & Turner, D. C. How depressive moods aect the behavior of singly living persons toward their cats. Anthrozoös 12,
224–233, https://doi.org/10.2752/089279399787000066 (1999).
16. Turner, D. C. & ieger, G. Singly living people and their cats: a study of human mood and subsequent behavior. Anthrozoös 14,
38–46, https://doi.org/10.2752/089279301786999652 (2001).
17. Saito, A. & Shinozua, . Vocal recognition of owners by domestic cats (Felis catus). Anim. Cogn. 16, 685–690, https://doi.
org/10.1007/s10071-013-0620-4 (2013).
18. Milósi, Á. & Soproni, . A comparative analysis of animals’ understanding of the human pointing gesture. Anim. Cogn. 9, 81–93,
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-005-0008-1 (2006).
19. Call, J., Brauer, J., aminsi, J. & Tomasello, M. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) are sensitive to the attentional state of humans. J.
Comp. Psychol. 117, 257–263 (2003).
20. Gácsi, M., Milósi, Á., Varga, O., Topál, J. & Csányi, V. Are readers of our face readers of our minds? Dogs (Canis familiar is) show
situation-dependent recognition of human’s attention. Anim. Cogn. 7, 144–153, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-003-0205-8 (2004).
21. Virányi, Z., Topál, J., Gácsi, M., Milósi, Á. & Csányi, V. Dogs respond appropriately to cues of humans’ attentional focus. Behav.
Process. 66, 161–172, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2004.01.012 (2004).
22. S chwab, C. & Huber, L. Obey or not obey? Dogs (Canis familiar is) behave dierently in response to attentional states of their owners.
J. Comp. Psychol. 120, 169–175, https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.120.3.169 (2006).
23. Nagasawa, M., Murai, ., Mogi, . & iusui, T. Dogs can discriminate human smiling faces from blan expressions. Anim. Cogn.
14, 525–533, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-011-0386-5 (2011).
24. Buttelmann, D. & Tomasello, M. Can domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use referential emotional expressions to locate hidden food?
Anim. Cogn. 16, 137–145, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-012-0560-4 (2013).
25. Turcsán, B., Szántho, F., Milósi, Á. & ubinyi, E. Fetching what the owner prefers? Dogs recognize disgust and happiness in human
behaviour. Anim. Cogn. 18, 83–94, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-014-0779-3 (2015).
26. Udell, M. A., Dorey, N. . & Wynne, C. D. What did domestication do to dogs? A new account of dogs’ sensitivity to human actions.
Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 85, 327–345, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-185X.2009.00104.x (2010).
27. Scheider, L., Grassmann, S., aminsi, J. & Tomasello, M. Domestic dogs use contextual information and tone of voice when
following a human pointing gesture. Plos One 6, e21676, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0021676 (2011).
28. uffman, T. & Morris-Trainor, Z. Do dogs understand human emotional expression? J. Vet. Behav. 6, 97–98, https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.jveb (2011).
29. Fuuzawa, M., Mills, D. S. & Cooper, J. J. e eect of human command phonetic characteristics on auditory cognition in dogs
(Canis familiar is). J. Comp. Psychol. 119, 117–120, https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.119.1.117 (2005).
30. Pongrácz, P., Milósi, Á. & Csányi, V. Owners’ beliefs on the ability of their pet dogs to understand human verbal communication:
A case of social understanding. Curr. Psychol. Cogn. 20, 87–107 (2001).
31. aminsi, J., Call, J. & Fischer, J. Word learning in a domestic dog: Evidence for ‘fast mapping’. Science 304, 1682–1683, https://doi.
org/10.1126/science.1097859 (2004).
32. Pilley, J. W. & eid, A. . Border collie comprehends object names as verbal referents. Behav. Process. 86, 184–195, https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.beproc.2010.11.007 (2011).
33. Williams, S. L., Brae, . E. & Savage-umbaugh, E. S. Comprehension sills of language-competent and nonlanguage-competent
apes. Lang. Commun. 17, 301–317 (1997).
34. Herman, L. M. eceptive competencies of language-trained animals. Adv. Stu dy Be hav. 17, 1–60 (1987).
35. Pepperberg, I. M. e Alex studies: Cognitive and communicative abilities of grey parrots. (Harvard University Press, 1999).
36. Young, C. A. Verbal commands as discriminative stimuli in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 32, 75–89,
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(05)80165-0 (1991).
37. Ellis, S. L. H., ompson, H., Guijarro, C. & Zulch, H. E. e inuence of body region, handler familiarity and order of region
handled on the domestic cat’s response to being stroed. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 173, 60–67, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
applanim.2014.11.002 (2015).
38. amos, D. et al. Are cats (Felis catus) from multi-cat households more stressed? Evidence from assessment of fecal glucocorticoid
metabolite analysis. Physiol. Behav. 122, 72–75, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.08.028 (2013).
39. Bradshaw, J. & Cameron-Beaumont, C. e signalling repertoire of the domestic cat and its undomesticated relatives in e domestic
cat: e biology of its behaviour, 2nd edn. (eds Turner, D. C. & Bateson, P.) 67–93 (Cambridge University Press, 2000).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
8
SCIENTIFIC REPORTS | (2019) 9:5394 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-40616-4
www.nature.com/scientificreports
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
40. Olmstead, C. E. & Villablanca, J. . Development of behavioral audition in the itten. Physiol. Behav. 24, 705–712, https://doi.
org/10.1016/0031-9384(80)90401-1 (1980).
41. Matsumoto, . elation between the emotional stroop tas and the probe detection tas (In Japanese). Socio-Environ. Stud. 11,
203–216 (2006).
Acknowledgements
We wish thank the owners of the cats and Marsa Smith (cat café). We also thank Saho Takagi, Minori Arahori,
and Hitomi Chijiiwa for helping with data collection, Hiroki Koda for helping with auditory stimulus analysis. A.
Saito and T. Hasegawa were granted funding by Kakenhi (No. 25118003). A. Saito was supported with Incentive
Allowance for Dissemination of Individual Research Results by Sophia University.
Author Contributions
A.S., Y.I. and T.H. conceived and designed the experiments. A.S., K.S. and Y.I. performed the experiments and
analysed the behavioural data. K.S. conducted statistical analysis. A.S. and K.S. prepared the manuscript. K.S.
analysed auditory stimuli. A.S. and T.H. organized the research project. All the authors read and approved the
nal manuscript.
Additional Information
Supplementary information accompanies this paper at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-40616-4.
Competing Interests: e authors declare no competing interests.
Publisher’s note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and
institutional aliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or
format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Cre-
ative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. e images or other third party material in this
article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the
material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not per-
mitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the
copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
© e Author(s) 2019
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
... Ellis et al. (2015) reported human guardians could identify the context of a given vocalisation for their own cats but not of an unfamiliar cat. From the feline side, it has been demonstrated that many cats can learn their own names (Saito et al., 2019) and can discriminate the names given to cohabiting cats from random words (Takagi et al., 2022). Cats and humans might not always understand what each other are trying to communicate, but the literature supports the assertion that the potential exists for both species to develop signals with shared meaning. ...
... How cats are named also provided insight into how guardians related to their cats. All participants consented to not having their cats' names anonymised, and a couple ability to discriminate their own names from other words (Saito et al., 2019). The study included both family companion cats and cat café residents, and both groups were able to recognise recordings of their names from nouns spoken by both familiar and unfamiliar humans (Saito et al., 2019). ...
... All participants consented to not having their cats' names anonymised, and a couple ability to discriminate their own names from other words (Saito et al., 2019). The study included both family companion cats and cat café residents, and both groups were able to recognise recordings of their names from nouns spoken by both familiar and unfamiliar humans (Saito et al., 2019). Although the café cats did not distinguish their names from those of other cats, the results from the study by Saito et al. (2019) suggested cats cohabiting in the same household could. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
This thesis employs thematic discourse analysis to elucidate prominent themes and points of contention associated with roaming cats (Felis catus). The data comprised 2476 online user comments responding to content related to roaming cats, 75 qualitative survey responses, 771 Facebook responses, and biographies reconstructed from eight case studies of cat-human relationships. These reflect broader social discourses surrounding more-than-human animals and human governance over other animals. Notions of guardian (owner) responsibilities are underpinned by different perceptions of companion cats (pets), ranging from childlike dependents who need to be protected and ‘parented’ to wild-like animals whose confinement would be morally wrong. Comments reveal how discourses from scientists, cat and wildlife advocacy groups, and the media are filtered through a local lens and often applied out of context. The data supports the notion that media reporting is instigating a moral panic over roaming cats by evoking emotive responses to predation by cats. These invariably become entangled within discourses related to cat safety, welfare, and complaints of ‘nuisance’ behaviours. Discourses surrounding cats in the community are further examined within a morethan- human biopolitical framework that describes how cohesive social mechanisms exert control over feline bodies through normalisation of practices such as desexing and confinement. Language was found to play a key role in biopolitical control by dominating the narrative of ‘responsible’ cat guardianship. Language is also central to moral panic theory, and the term ‘feral’ was shown to reinforce a ‘folk devil’ trope of free-living cats as transgressive and inherently different from companion cats. ‘Feral’ also invoked pity among those adamant cats need human love and care. However, cats are not without agency and can co-create meaning within a multispecies home or community. Case studies demonstrated cat-human intersubjectivity (joint meaning-making) and the various relationships formed between cats and non-feline animals (including human), both inside and outside of their homes.
... Each test was performed indoors. Cats were tested in the home of their owners, because in the case of most companion cats, testing them away from home (e.g., in an unknown testing room at the institute) generates strong stress that would impede their successful participation in the tests (for other instances of cats being tested at their owners' homes, see [51][52][53]). Each test was recorded with a Panasonic HDC-SD10 (Panasonic Corporation, Kadoma, Osaka, Japan) and Moto G (Motorola Mobility LLC, Chicago, IL, USA) mobile phone camera (Panasonic was replaced with Moto G in the later stage of data collection as it was damaged) camcorder for later analysis. ...
... The involvement of companion cats in cognitive tests often suffers from the high dropout rate of subjects due to the difficulty of finding effective motivation for cats [59]; and their refusal to cooperate with the experimenter [51]. Recently, testing cats in their owners' home proved to be a promising approach for comparative ethologists (e.g., [52,53]); thus, the satisfactory level of motivation and confidence of our feline subjects in the detour tests could partially be explained by the choice of home-based testing. The fact that companion dogs were tested at the university's laboratory does not make the testing conditions markedly different because the most important aspect of the 'natural environment' for (socialized) dogs is the presence of their owner. ...
... In our study, instead of providing them with a social learning task, both cats and dogs were tested under the same treatment conditions with laser light demonstration. Although there are recent indications that cats engage in complex sociocognitive interactions with humans (e.g., attachment-like bonds, [63]; gaze-following, [51]; responding to their name, [52]), at this time, we wanted to compare cats' and dogs' responses to non-social stimuli connected to the detour task, because there are still some arguments that there could be fundamental differences between the social bonds formed by the two species with humans [45]. This aspect was also indirectly supported in this study by the descriptive results of the animals looking back at their owner, as more dogs referred back to their owner, and with higher frequency than cats did. ...
Article
Full-text available
We tested companion cats and dogs in similar indoor conditions using identical procedures in the classic detour task around a V-shaped transparent wire-mesh fence. Besides the control group, we used two types of laser light-pointing demonstration (moving around the fence, or pointing straight at the reward). We found that dogs reached the food reward faster than cats; across consecutive trials, only the dogs showed improvement in their speed and dogs continued to use the same side for detouring after a preceding successful attempt, while cats chose the side for detouring irrespective of their previous successful trials. In addition, ‘demonstrating’ a detour with the laser did not influence the speed or direction of the detour of the subjects; and dogs looked back to their owner more frequently than the cats did. We discuss the possibility that for dogs, detouring along a transparent obstacle represents a more problematic task than for cats; therefore, dogs strongly rely on their previous experiences. This is the first time that cats were successfully tested in this detour paradigm in direct comparison with dogs. The results are relevant from the aspect of testing cognitive performance in companion cats, which are known to be notoriously reluctant to engage with novel experimental situations.
... As is the case of inter-cat sociality, the social behavior of pet cats towards humans can also vary. However, an increasing number of studies demonstrate that cats not only have the capacity to form strong social bonds with humans (e.g., Vitale et al., 2019 showed that 68% of cats studied were securely attached to their owners), they also show great social sensitivity; e.g., they preferentially approach attentive humans and follow human points or gazes to a location containing concealed food (Miklosi et al., 2005;Saito et al., 2019;Vitale and Udell 2019;Pongracz et al., 2019). Furthermore, Vitale Shreve et al. (2017) found that cats often exhibit preferences for social stimuli when given the choice. ...
... Further, pet cats form attachment bonds to human caretakers, forming secure attachments at roughly the same rate as has been observed in dog-human bonds and human infant-caregiver bonds (Vitale et al., 2019). Cats have also been shown to recognize when a human says their name (Saito et al., 2019), engage in social referencing (Merola et al., 2015), use some forms of synchronized non-verbal communication with human owners, such as slow blink-matching, wherein a cat opens and closes its eyes repeatedly in a series of slow blinks more frequently when their owners blink at them than when no such social interaction is taking place (Humphrey et al., 2020). ...
Article
Cats are among the most popular pets worldwide, but there are still major gaps in the public's general understanding of their social behaviors and related needs, including for socialization. In addition to these knowledge gaps, people often have negative or ambivalent attitudes about cats, which can directly impact their welfare outcomes. Insufficient attention to the behavioral ecology and development of cat sociality, along with failure to account for their highly variable individual preferences and tolerance for social behaviors can lead them to experience distress that undermines both their welfare and the human-animal bond. As Part 1 of a two-part series addressing common myths about cats, the purpose of this first paper is to review and debunk common misperceptions about the social needs and behaviors of cats, including misunderstandings about their social lives and abilities to bond with humans. We also identify where opportunities exist to improve socialization of cats and to advance research in related areas that might better support their behavior and welfare needs.
... Cats and dogs also attend to human speech and learn to distinguish between words spontaneously (Young, 1991;Fukuzawa et al., 2005;Saito et al., 2019;Takagi et al., 2022). Studies suggest that owners believe that their dogs understand a large number of words (Pongrácz et al., 2001;Reeve & Jacques, 2022), with owners reporting an average of 89 words over 165 family and working dogs (Reeve & Jacques, 2022). ...
Article
The first studies that sought to establish two-way communication between humans and great apes led to important findings but were nevertheless heavily criticized for their training methods, testing procedures, and claims. More recently, hundreds of pet owners around the world have begun training domesticated animals to use Augmentative Interspecies Communication (AIC) soundboard devices, contributing to the first ever large-scale study on interspecies communication. Here, we introduce our scientific approach to our global citizen science project, where we will investigate how dogs and cats use AIC devices, building an incremental research program starting from their associative learning of buttons to determining how AIC device use might impact their welfare and their capacity for symbolic representation. We discuss how our multi-faceted approach can alleviate many of the concerns regarding the original studies performed with apes, achieving larger sample sizes, ample documentation of training techniques, and testing animals’ performance in controlled experimental settings.
... Domestic house cats are sensitive to traits of individual identity encoded in the voices of their owners and can recognize them among the voices of other people (Saito & Shinozuka, 2013). Domestic house cats are capable of recognizing their names among other words (Saito et al., 2019). One months old kittens display stronger responses to chirps and meows of their own mothers compared with calls of other mother cats (Szenczi et al., 2016). ...
Article
This study investigates the frequency, temporal and power parameters in 11 (5 males, 6 females) captive feral domestic cats Felis silvestris catus, vocalising in their individual outdoor enclosures during the mating season. Discriminant function analysis (DFA) classified the meows to correct callers with 79.2% accuracy, which exceeded the chance level of 22.9 ± 2.8%, calculated with permutation test. Male meows were lower-frequency, with the maximum fundamental frequency of 0.37 ± 0.05 kHz vs 0.61 ± 0.16 kHz in females. Sex differences in the maximum, beginning and end fundamental frequencies varied from 32 to 39%, depending on acoustic parameter. The DFA classified the meows to correct sex with accuracy of 88.0%, which exceeded the chance level of 58.2 ± 3.1%. We discuss that the meows encode information about individual identity and sex and that acoustic differences in frequency parameters of the meows exceed sexual dimorphism of body size in domestic cat.
... Similarly, domestic cats (Felis catus) and dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) demonstrate discriminatory abilities in auditory cues. Domestic cats have demonstrated the ability to ascribe human speech as name identifiers for both themselves and other cohabitating cats (Saito et al. 2019;Takagi et al. 2022). Domestic dogs behave similarly in relation to names but can also learn the names of inanimate objects as well as general terms of object categories-demonstrating both discrimination and generalization (Dror et al. 2021;Fugazza et al. 2021a,b;Kaminski and Nitzschner 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
The field of animal cognition has advanced rapidly in the last 25 years. Through careful and creative studies of animals in captivity and in the wild, we have gained critical insights into the evolution of intelligence, the cognitive capacities of a diverse array of taxa, and the importance of ecological and social environments, as well as individual variation, in the expression of cognitive abilities. The field of animal cognition, however, is still being influenced by some historical tendencies. For example, primates and birds are still the majority of study species in the field of animal cognition. Studies of diverse taxa improve the generalizability of our results, are critical for testing evolutionary hypotheses, and open new paths for understanding cognition in species with vastly different morphologies. In this paper, we review the current state of knowledge of cognition in mammalian carnivores. We discuss the advantages of studying cognition in Carnivorans and the immense progress that has been made across many cognitive domains in both lab and field studies of carnivores. We also discuss the current constraints that are associated with studying carnivores. Finally, we explore new directions for future research in studies of carnivore cognition.
Article
Full-text available
Cat social behaviour and cognition has received a growing interest during the last decades. Recent studies reported that cats efficiently engage in interspecific communication with humans and suggest that cats are sensitive to human emotional visual and auditory cues. To date, there is no evidence on the social and informative role of human emotional odours, which may affect human-cat communication. In this study, we presented cats with human odours collected in different emotional contexts (fear, happiness, physical stress and neutral) and evaluated the animals’ behavioural responses. We found that “fear” odours elicited higher stress levels than “physical stress” and “neutral”, suggesting that cats perceived the valence of the information conveyed by “fear” olfactory signals and regulate their behaviour accordingly. Moreover, the prevalent use of the right nostril (right hemisphere activation) with the increase of stress levels, particularly in response to “fear” odours, provides first evidence of lateralized emotional functions of olfactory pathways in cats.
Article
Full-text available
In contemporary western cultures, most humans talk to their pet companions. Speech register addressed to companion animals shares common features with speech addressed to young children, which are distinct from the typical adult-directed speech (ADS). The way dogs respond to dog-directed speech (DDS) has raised scientists’ interest. In contrast, much less is known about how cats perceive and respond to cat-directed speech (CDS). The primary aim of this study was to evaluate whether cats are more responsive to CDS than ADS. Secondarily, we seek to examine if the cats’ responses to human vocal stimuli would differ when it was elicited by their owner or by a stranger. We performed playback experiments and tested a cohort of 16 companion cats in a habituation–dishabituation paradigm, which allows for the measurement of subjects’ reactions without extensive training. Here, we report new findings that cats can discriminate speech specifically addressed to them from speech addressed to adult humans, when sentences are uttered by their owners. When hearing sentences uttered by strangers, cats did not appear to discriminate between ADS and CDS. These findings bring a new dimension to the consideration of human–cat relationship, as they imply the development of a particular communication into human–cat dyads, that relies upon experience. We discuss these new findings in the light of recent literature investigating cats’ sociocognitive abilities and human–cat attachment. Our results highlight the importance of one-to-one relationships for cats, reinforcing recent literature regarding the ability for cats and humans to form strong bonds.
Article
Full-text available
The ability of domestic dogs (C. lupus famaliaris) to follow and attend to human emotion expressions is well documented. It is unknown whether domestic cats (F. silvestris catus) possess similar abilities. Because cats belong to the same order (Carnivora), but did not evolve to live in complex social groups, research with them enables us to tease apart the influence of social structure versus domestication processes on the capacity to recognize human communicative cues, such as emotions. Two experiments were conducted to determine the extent to which domestic cats discriminate between human emotion cues. The first experiment presented cats with facial and postural cues of happiness and anger from both an unfamiliar experimenter and their familiar owner in the absence of vocal cues. The second experiment presented cats with vocal cues of human emotion through a positively or negatively charged conversation between an experimenter and owner. Domestic cats were only modestly sensitive to emotion, particularly when displayed by their owner, suggesting that a history of human interaction alone may not be sufficient to shape such abilities in domestic cats.
Article
Full-text available
Cats’ (Felis catus) communicative behaviour towards humans was explored using a social referencing paradigm in the presence of a potentially frightening object. One group of cats observed their owner delivering a positive emotional message, whereas another group received a negative emotional message. The aim was to evaluate whether cats use the emotional information provided by their owners about a novel/unfamiliar object to guide their own behaviour towards it. We assessed the presence of social referencing, in terms of referential looking towards the owner (defined as looking to the owner immediately before or after looking at the object), the behavioural regulation based on the owner’s emotional (positive vs negative) message (vocal and facial), and the observational conditioning following the owner’s actions towards the object. Most cats (79 %) exhibited referential looking between the owner and the object, and also to some extent changed their behaviour in line with the emotional message given by the owner. Results are discussed in relation to social referencing in other species (dogs in particular) and cats’ social organization and domestication history.
Article
We tested whether cats (Felis catus) could recognize human attentional states when begging for food from one of two unfamiliar actors. Cats were tested under three conditions that differed in the actors’ actions: Visual only condition—the actor looking at the cat silently versus facing sideways silently; Visual and Auditory condition—the actor looking at and calling to the cat versus looking at the cat silently; and Auditory only condition—the actor facing down and calling to the cat versus facing down silently. In the Visual and Auditory condition, cats preferred the actor who was calling to them. In the Visual only and the Auditory only conditions, the cats showed no preference for the actors’ attentional states. There was a modest difference in the preference between the Visual and Auditory condition and the Auditory only condition. These results suggest that cats can use vocal cues of attention toward them only in situations in which humans are looking at them.
Article
This paper presents results of almost 30 years of study of the cognitive and communicative activities of Grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus), conventionally regarded as mindless mimics. These studies have demonstrated that Grey parrots can solve various cognitive tasks and acquire and use English speech in ways that often resemble those of very young children. Examples include the concepts of same/different, colour, size and shape. The parrot Alex can also recognize and distinguish numbers up to six, and spontaneously demonstrated his ability to grasp the concept of "none". Given the evolutionary distance between birds and mammals, these results have intriguing implications for the evolution of intelligence, the study of comparative intelligence, and the care and maintenance of birds held in captivity in zoos and as companion animals. (c) 2006 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
Chapter
The chapter discusses on what is known of the receptive competencies of animals in language like tasks. The emphasis is on the extensive work with apes, as well as the recent work with marine mammals, bottle-nosed dolphins. Sign language projects have given little attention to receptive competencies. Generally, data from these studies are insufficient for judging the degree to which apes understand the signs of their trainers. The most extensive attempt to quantify sentence comprehension in apes was that by Premack. The chimps Sarah, Peony, and Elizabeth were tutored in a system in which plastic symbols of arbitrary shape and color were used to represent objects, properties, and actions within an artificial language. Sentences could be constructed, by the experimenter or by the chimp, by arranging the symbols in a linear array on a board. There is evidence for an asymmetry of comprehension and production in the development and maintenance of language in humans. During early childhood, comprehension generally precedes and exceeds production. The ability of dolphins to understand imperative and interrogative sentences expressed within artificial acoustic or gestural languages is examined. Two young female bottle-nosed dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), housed together in a large seawater tank, were tutored in the artificial languages.
Article
The domestic cat is now one of the most common pet species in the Western world. As part of its role as a pet, cats are expected to not only tolerate but enjoy being touched. This study consisted of two experiments, with the first investigating the influence of body region touched and handler familiarity on the domestic cat's behavioural response to being stroked. The second experiment extended this work by investigating the influence of order of body region touched on behavioural responses. Both handler familiarity and body region stroked significantly influenced negative behavioural responses. Familiar handling, in comparison to unfamiliar handling, led to significantly higher negative behavioural scores displayed by the cats (Z = −3.235, N = 34, p = 0.001). When considering the different body regions investigated, the caudal region produced the highest negative scores both when handled by the unfamiliar person (Experiment 1: χ2 = 14.330, N = 34, p = 0.046) and by the familiar person (Experiment 2: χ2 = 18.387, N = 20, p = 0.002). Order of body region touched had no significant bearing on behavioural responses exhibited. Results suggest that handling of cats should avoid the caudal region and highlight the need for further investigation into the owner–cat relationship.
Article
The domestic cat is the only member of the Felidae to form social relationships with humans, and also, the only small felid to form intraspecific social groups when free ranging. The latter are matriarchies, and bear only a superficial similarity to those of the lion and cheetah, which evolved separately and in response to very different selection pressures. There is no evidence for intraspecific social behavior in the ancestral species Felis silvestris, and hence, the capacity for group formation almost certainly evolved concurrently with the self-domestication of the cat during the period 10,000 to 5,000 years before present. Social groups of F. catus are characterized by cooperation among related adult females in the raising of kittens from parturition onward and competition between adult males. Unlike more social Carnivora, cats lack ritualized submissive signals, and although "peck-order" hierarchies can be constructed from exchanges of aggressive and defensive behavior, these do not predict reproductive success in females, or priority of access to key resources, and thus do not illuminate the basis of normal cat society. Cohesion in colonies of cats is expressed as, and probably maintained by, allorubbing and allogrooming; transmission of scent signals may also play a largely uninvestigated role. The advantages of group living over the ancestral solitary territorial state have not been quantified adequately but are likely to include defense of permanent food sources and denning sites and protection against predators and possibly infanticide by invading males. These presumably outweigh the disadvantages of communal denning, enhanced transmission of parasites, and diseases. Given the lack of archaeological evidence for cats kept as pets until some 4,000 years before present, intraspecific social behavior was most likely fully evolved before interspecific sociality emerged. Signals directed by cats toward their owners fall into 3 categories: those derived from species-typical actions, such as jumping up, that become signals by association; signals derived from kitten-to-mother communication (kneading, meow); and those derived from intraspecific cohesive signals. Social stress appears widespread among pet cats, stemming from both agonistic relationships within households and territorial disputes with neighborhood cats, but simple solutions seem elusive, most likely because individual cats vary greatly in their reaction to encounters with other cats.