ChapterPDF Available

Alternative Reproductive Tactics

Authors:

Abstract

Alternative reproductive tactics (ARTs) refer to the consistent use of different reproductive behaviors between conspecifics of the same sex in one population at the same time. Hereby the difference between individuals is discontinuous, such that one can put individuals into two or more categories. A tactic refers to the behavioral phenotype, which results from a strategy. A strategy can have different tactics, e.g., territorial male versus satellite male. If a strategy has only one tactic, then more than one strategy must be present in the population for ARTs to be present. It is typically tactics which are studied, not strategies.
A
Alternative Reproductive
Tactics
Carsten Schradin
Université de Strasbourg, CNRS, IPHC UMR
7178, Strasbourg, France
School of Animal, Plant and Environmental
Sciences, University of the Witwatersrand,
Johannesburg, South Africa
Definitions
Strategy A set of decision rules, which has
a genetic basis, leading to a tactic.
Tactic The behavioral phenotype, which
results from a strategy. A strategy
can have one, two, or several
different tactics.
Alternative
reproductive
tactics ARTs
Consistent variation in
reproductive behaviors between
males or females of one species.
Alternative
strategies
Two (or more) genetically based
strategies of one sex that lead to
two (or more) alternative tactics.
Mixed
strategy
One strategy that, depending on
frequency depending selection,
leads to two or more ARTs which
have equal mean tness.
Conditional
strategy
One strategy which determines
that individuals follows a
subordinate tactic with low
reproductive success while they
are small with low
competitiveness, but switch to a
dominant tactic with high
reproductive success when
increasing competitiveness.
Single
strategy
One strategy with two or more
alternative tactics. The
environment determines which
tactic an individual chooses and
also whether ARTs have different
or same mean tness. The single
strategy combines the mixed and
the conditional strategy,
emphasizing the importance of
the environment in determining
tness outcomes.
Bourgeois
tactic
The tactic with the highest
possible tness.
Best of a bad
job
An individual follows a tactic
with a lower tness than an
alternative tactic, because it is not
competitive enough to
successfully follow the bourgeois
tactic.
Relative
plasticity
hypothesis
RPH
The RPH is based on the
assumption that differences
between ARTs are similarly
regulated via hormones as are the
differences between the sexes.
© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019
J. Vonk, T. K. Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior,
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_322-1
Introduction
Alternative reproductive tactics (ARTs) refer to
the consistent use of different reproductive behav-
iors between conspecics of the same sex in one
population at the same time. Hereby the difference
between individuals is discontinuous, such that
one can put individuals into two or more catego-
ries. A tactic refers to the behavioral phenotype,
which results from a strategy. A strategy can have
different tactics, e.g., territorial male versus satel-
lite male. If a strategy has only one tactic, then
more than one strategy must be present in the
population for ARTs to be present. It is typically
tactics which are studied, not strategies.
Examples of ARTs in Males and in
Females
ARTs are more common in males than in females,
probably because reproductive success in males is
more skewed than in females. For example, in
species in which a few competitive males monop-
olies the access to fertile females, smaller and less
competitive males might develop best of a bad job
tactics to mate with these females and gain at least
some reproductive success before being big
enough to be a dominant male. If more than one
reproductive tactic occurs in a population, then
ARTs occur. This is basically the case in every
species where apart from bourgeois males (see
below), another type of males occurs that are
trying to fertilize females. In males, the following
tactics have been described:
Bourgeois male: These are males which follow
the tactic with the highest mean tness. Thus,
these are the most competitive and most dom-
inant males, which often defend a territory
and/or a group of breeding females (harem).
Sneakers: These are small males trying to
sneak copulations with females defended by
bourgeois males. Satellite males, roamers, and
oaters are different types of sneaker males.
Sometimes this term is used for roamers and
oaters in reptiles, which means for subordi-
nate males that are not (like satellite males)
associated to a specic territory. Sneaker males
occur in many lizard species such as marine
iguana (Amblyrhynchus cristatus) and tree liz-
ard (Urosaurus ornatus), and in cichlid sh
such as Lamprologus callipterus.
Satellite males: These males are associated to a
specic territory but do not defend a territory
themselves. They are typically less competitive
and smaller than bourgeois males. They try to
mate with females in the territory they are
associated with. In some species like bluegills
(Lepomis macrochirus), these males have
female mimicry, meaning they look and
behave like females, explaining why the bour-
geois males do not chase them away. Satellite
males occur in many lizard species such as
marine iguana (Amblyrhynchus cristatus) and
tree lizard (Urosaurus ornatus), in tree frogs
and several species of crickets.
Roamers: This term is usually only used for
mammals. Roamers typically have a well-
dened home range that is very large and over-
laps the home range of several bourgeois males
but not defended as a territory. These males
roam over a large area, trying to avoid the
bourgeois males but to meet and to copulate
with any females they encounter. Example:
African striped mouse (Rhabdomys pumilio).
Floaters: This term is used mainly for birds and
mammals. Floaters are males that, in contrast to
roamers, do not have a well-dened home range,
but oat through the population, trying to copu-
late with females defended by bourgeois males.
Floaters occur in many bird species such as the
red-winged blackbird (Agelaius phoeniceus).
Helpers: Helpers are males that help a breeding
pair to raise the pairs offspring i.e., they help
to raise young that are not their own. In many
species, helpers also try to fertilize eggs/
females, either of the pair they are helping
(cichlid Neolamprologus pulcher;piedking-
sher Ceryle rudis), or of neighboring groups
(African striped mice, meerkats Suricatte
suricata; superb fairy-wrens, Malurus cyaneus).
While ARTs have been mostly studied in
males, female ARTs also occur. This is the case
when the investment of other females such as
2 Alternative Reproductive Tactics
maternal care can be exploited or when females
monopolies the parental investment of males by
forming one female-multiple male groups, mating
polyandreously. As ARTs are alternative ways to
gain tness via reproduction, alternative mating
tactics, such as monogamous versus polynadreous
mating of females also represent ARTs, but such
alternative mating tactics are difcult to measure
and received little attention. In females, the fol-
lowing ARTs have been described:
Solitary versus communal breeding in African
striped mice, house mice, and probably many
other small mammals.
In eusocial insects, workers that are laying
eggs in the nest dominated by a queen.
In some wasps, females can either join the nest
of another female (communal breeding) or start
their own nest (single breeder).
Female dunnocks can mate monogamously,
polyandrously, or polygynandrously.
Fire ants (Solenopsis invicta) females can form
colonies with either one single monogynous
queen or colonies with several breeding females.
Species With and Without
Morphological Differences
Between ARTs
In some species, males following different tactics
do not only differ in their behavior but also in
their morphology. For example, in several lizard
species with ARTs, the different tactics also dif-
fer in the coloration of their skin, such as the
coloration of dewlaps in tree lizards. In species
with genetically determined alternative strate-
gies, males can differ dramatically in size, for
example, in the cichlid sh Lamprologus
callipterus, where bourgeois males are 10 cm
long and weight 36 g, while dwarf males reach
only 2.5 cm and 0.9 g. In some case, such as
dwarf males parasitizing bourgeois males, the
reason for the phenotypic difference is obvious.
In contrast, why male lizards following ARTs
differ in coloration is still not understood, and
this question also received little scienticatten-
tion.ManystudiesonARTSfocusedonsuch
species where the different tactics can easily be
distinguished, as obviously this makes it easier to
study ARTs. However, in the huge majority of
studies, individuals following ARTs differ
mainly in behavior and often additionally to a
small, not always (to the observer) obvious
extent in body mass. Thus, species where ARTs
are also phenotypically different are a
special case.
What Does Not Represent ARTs?
No examples of ARTs are interspecic brood-
parasitism, sex change, simultaneous hermaphro-
ditism, and infanticide (Taborsky 2008). In many
species, each male might have different tactics to
fertilize females, without these representing
ARTs. For example, in most passerine birds,
males are pair-living, copulating with their female
partner, and additionally seeking to copulate with
neighboring females to gain extra-pair paternity.
As all males follow this strategy, it does not rep-
resent ARTs, as there is no consistent variation
between males. However, if it would be shown in
one species that some males invest heavily in
guarding their mate and not in extra-pair copula-
tions, while other males dont invest in mate
guarding but in extra-pair copulations, then this
consistent variation would represent ARTs.
Alternative tactics can also occur in other
behaviors than reproduction. For example, alter-
native dispersal tactics have been described in
African striped mice (Rhabdomys pumilio),
where the largest males disperse over short dis-
tances (less than 500 meters), intermediate males
over large distances (several kms), and small
males not at all (Solmsen et al. 2011). While
these alternative dispersal tactics are correlated
with ARTs (territorial male versus roaming male
versus philopatric males), they do not represent
ARTs themselves but rather a tactic of males of
intermediate body mass to reach the high tness
tactic of territorial male in a distant subpopulation
instead of becoming a roamer in the natal popula-
tion. Similar alternative tactics that are not repro-
ductive tactics can occur for different foraging
strategies.
Alternative Reproductive Tactics 3
The occurrence of an alternative male (or female)
morph that does not reproduce is not regarded as
ARTs. Thus, traditionally cooperatively breeding
species with nonbreeding helpers have not been
considered as examples of ARTs. However, more
detailed studies often found that helpers might breed
when the opportunity arises, without changing their
social status. Examples for this are female workers
of many hymenopterans, male helpers in meerkats,
in striped mice and in superb fairy-wrens, and
helpers of both sexes in the cichlid Neolamprologus
pulcher.
Strategies Underlying ARTs
It is important to differentiate between the observed
tactic and the underlying strategy (Gross 1996).
A strategy refers to decision rules which determine
which alternative tactic is shown. A strategy has a
strong genetic basic but is not totally genetically
xed, as there is variation between individuals, for
example, when exactly to switch from one tactic to
another. The strategy is determined by specic
mechanisms which are a response to environmental
cues. An example for a strategy might be the fol-
lowing decision rule: If you are larger than X,
increase your testosterone level and become a terri-
torial male, otherwise be a satellite male.
It has been stated that the differentiation
between strategies and tactics is not useful as
both relate to the same issue (Taborsky 2008).
However, the differentiation between strategy
and tactic can be very important, as it clearly
indicates one shortcoming in our understanding
of ARTs: Nearly all research has focused on the
tactics themselves, i.e., the tness consequences
and the underlying endocrine mechanisms leading
to ARTs (all the rest of the current article reviews
this), while the underlying strategies have been
ignored. Thus, for most species showing ARTs the
underlying decision rules have never been esti-
mated, tested, and validated. However, formulat-
ing these decisions rules can help us with coming
up with better and more meaningful predictions,
for example, about the expected tness conse-
quences of ARTs. On the other hand, if no clear
set of decision rules (a strategy) can be
formulated, this indicates a lack in our under-
standing of the ARTs of this specic species.
For male African striped mice, it has been
proposed that they have one strategy, the decision
rules of which determines which of the three pos-
sible ARTs a male follows, while the environment
determines the tness outcomes (Schradin and
Lindholm 2011):
1. Decision: To remain philopatric to your natal
group or to disperse?
(a) Remain a philopatric male if all females
breed in communal groups defended by
breeding males and your body mass is
below the population mean and there are
more sexually mature males than female
groups in the population. Mate with any
female from neighboring groups.
(b) Leave your natal group if:
(i) At any time of the year, a breeding
position becomes vacant in a neigh-
boring group which you can ll.
(ii) At the beginning of the breeding sea-
son if population density is low and
several single undefended females
are present in the population which
has a female biased sex ratio.
(iii) At the beginning of the breeding sea-
son if your body mass is above the
mean of the population, independent
of population density.
2. Decision: To become a roamer or try to become
a territorial breeder?
(a) If all female breed solitarily then become a
roaming male.
(b) If you nd a group of communally breed-
ing females that are not defended by a male
that is larger than you then become the
breeding male of this group, defend the
territory of this group against other males,
and try to copulate with all your females
plus all females from neighboring groups.
This set of decision rules makes clear that
being a roamer can be the result of two different
processes: (1) an active choice, because many
females breed solitarily and roaming is the best
tactic or as good as being a territorial breeder.
4 Alternative Reproductive Tactics
(2) Result of low competitive ability, i.e., the male
chose to disperse and would choose to become a
territorial breeder, but he cannot, because all com-
munal female groups are defended by larger
males. This is similar to the situation in stoplight
parrotsh (Sparisoma viridae) where males start
with the tactic of bachelor and the availability of
free territories and their relative body size deter-
mines when they can switch to the territorial tactic
(Cardwell and Liley 1991).
Development and ARTs
The tactic an individual follows to have reproduc-
tive success develops during its life and can
change during development (Fig. 1). Obviously,
the time point when an individual reaches sexual
maturity is important for the expression of its
reproductive tactic. In species where the
reproductive tactic is genetically xed, the tactic
that an individual will follow throughout its life
will then be expressed. If depending on the geno-
type two or more alternative tactics might be
expressed, then we observe ARTs in this popula-
tion (Fig. 1, top). In other species, its the envi-
ronment that determine which of two (or more)
ARTs is expressed (Fig. 1, middle). However,
especially in males, whether or not individuals
can reproduce not only depends on when it
reaches sexual maturity but also on its competi-
tiveness, for which body mass is often a very good
proxy. Large dominant males might reproduce,
while small males, even though sexually mature,
might have to wait and grow, possibly forming
bachelor groups of nonbreeding males or
remaining as nonbreeding subordinate males in a
group.
There is strong selection pressure on individuals
to reproduce, and accordingly in many species
Developmental (fixed) plasticity
Developmental (genetically fixed) sequence
Flexibility (reversible plasticity)
Genetically fixed
Plasticity
One
genotype
Two
genotypes
Alternative Reproductive Tactics, Fig. 1 A simplied
overview of the different pathways that can lead to ARTs,
adapted from (Taborsky 2008). Top: ARTs can arise out of
two (or more) different main genotypes determining which
of the alternative tactics is expressed in each individual.
Middle: One genotype can lead to ARTs via nonreversible
developmental plasticity, where one out of two (or more)
alternative pathways leading to ARTs is chosen. Empirical
evidence for his mechanism leading to ARTs is missing. In
contrast, most cases of ARTs are due to exibility, i.e.,
reversible phenotypic plasticity, for example, many cases
where males follow either a best of a bad job or a bourgeois
tactic, depending on their relative competitiveness. In
some tree frogs, the biggest males are calling, smaller
males are satellites, but when bigger males arrive/disap-
pear, males can immediately switch their tactic, and later
can switch again. Bottom: One main genotype can lead to
ARTs when individuals follow different tactics during their
development, for example, following a doing the best of a
bad tactic as young small adults, and changing to the
bourgeois tactic when older and more competitive
Alternative Reproductive Tactics 5
subordinate males are trying to mate with females,
even when being small and dominated by larger
males that try to monopolies access to receptive
females. Such small males might follow a best of a
bad job tactic, often called satellite or sneaker tactic,
until they become larger and can follow the domi-
nant bourgeois tactic (Fig. 1, bottom). In these spe-
cies, which tactic is followed is mainly determined
by growth after reaching adulthood, and as such
determined by development during adulthood.
While some authors do not regard such develop-
ment dependent tactics as ARTs, they clearly are, as
we need (1) ultimate explanations for the occurrence
of such best of a bad job tactics which are (2) con-
trolled by specic endocrine mechanisms, which
evolved to maximize lifetime reproductive success.
Tactics can be genetically xed or plastic
(Fig. 1). Within the plastic tactics, developmental
plasticity might lead permanently to one out of
several alternative tactics, which the individual
then cannot change anymore. The sequence of
tactics might also be developmentally xed, for
example, from small sneaker to large bourgeois
male, as described above. However, in many spe-
cies, individuals can exibly change their repro-
ductive tactic repeatedly. This is also a form of
plasticity, which in contrast to xed developmen-
tal plasticity is called exibility.
Evolution of Strategies Underlying ARTs
The phenomenon of alternative reproductive tac-
tics has been analyzed by game theory, where a
tactic refers to a specic behavior resulting from
individual decision rules, so-called strategies.
Reproductive tactics have a strong inuence on
individual tness and are thus under strong selec-
tion pressure. However, it is rather the underlying
strategies, i.e., developmental and cognitive deci-
sion rules that evolve and then produce the
observed tactics. In a classical paper, Gross
(1996)dened three categories of strategies
(Fig. 2):
1. Alternative strategies: Genetically polymor-
phic, based on frequency-dependent selection.
Different tactics yield the same average tness.
Examples are giant versus dwarf males of the
cichlid sh Lamprologus callipterus. Alterna-
tive strategies are represented by genetically
xed tactics in Fig. 1.
2. Mixed strategies: Genetically monomorphic,
based on frequency-dependent selection. Dif-
ferent tactics yield the same average tness. No
good empirical examples exist. Mixed strate-
gies have also been characterized by a proba-
bilistic basis, i.e., a probability x to play tactic
X and probability 1-x to play tactic Y. These
represent developmental xed plastic tactics in
Fig. 1, where tactics have the same average
tness and the chosen tactic then depends on
the frequency of all tactics in the population.
3. Conditional strategies: Genetically monomor-
phic, based on status-dependent selection. Dif-
ferent tactics yield different tness. The tactic
that an individual chooses depends on its com-
petitive abilities. The most competitive indi-
viduals follow the tactic that yields the
highest tness payoff, called the bourgeois
tactic. Less competitive males (often called
sneaker or satellite), that are smaller and youn-
ger than the bourgeois males, make the best of
a bad job, following a tactic with lower tness
that is still better than no reproductive success
at all. These males change to the bourgeois
tactic when they grow larger. Many examples
exist in both vertebrates and invertebrates.
These represent exible (reversible plasticity)
and developmental sequenced tactics in Fig. 1.
Empirical studies on the African striped mouse
revealed that tness consequences of ARTs can be
environment dependent. Male striped mice follow
one of three ARTs: (1) Territorial breeding males
defend a group of communally breeding females;
(2) Solitary roaming males trying to copulate with
any females they encounter; (3) Philopatric males
that remain in their natal group and try to sneak
copulations with females from neighboring
groups. Under high population density, breeding
males have 10 times higher reproductive success
than roamers and 100 times higher reproductive
success than philopatrics. However, under inter-
mediate population density, breeding males and
6 Alternative Reproductive Tactics
roamers have the same mean tness, and no
philopatrics occur (as all sexually mature males
leave their natal group). Under very low popula-
tion density, all males follow a roaming tactic,
and some of these roamers have a very high
reproductive success, higher than that of
breeders under high population densities. These
observations lead to the formulation of the single
strategy, which contains both the mixed and the
conditional strategy proposed by Gross (1996;
Fig. 2).
The single strategy is a new term introduced by
Schradin and Lindholm (2011) to replace the terms
mixed and conditional strategy, which differ mainly
in the predicted tness consequences (equal tness
for mixed versus different tness payoffs for tactics
arising from conditional strategies). Single strategies
are not based on genetic polymorphisms, but all
individuals follow the same or very similar decision
rules when choosing a tactic. Individuals have ex-
ible tactics, which means they can switch tactics,
often repeatedly. Environmental conditions deter-
mine whether the different tactics yield similar or
different tness. By formulating the single strategy
(=decision rules; see above) for striped mice, it
became evident that tness outcomes depend on
the prevailing environment. To become a roamer
can be the result of a male choosing to disperse
and to roam, as the tactic yields high tness. Alter-
natively, it can be the result of a male choosing to
disperse and attempts to become a territorial breeder
but not being competitive enough to do so. In the
latter case, being a roamer would be a best of a bad
job tactic.
The denitions by Gross (1996) have been
criticized on theoretical grounds because of its
focus on genetic polymorphism versus genetic
monomorphism: even if different animals follow
very similar decisions rules and show exibility,
i.e., they can change between tactics, they still
might differ genetically, e.g., in their decision
when to switch tactics (Shuster and Wade 2003).
Alternative Strategies
Genetic
Monomorphism
Frequency
depending selection
One strategy with
different tactics of the
same fitness
Genetic
Monomorphism
Status depending
selection
One strategy with
different tactics of
different fitness
Mixed Strategy C
onditional
Strategy
Genetic
polymorphism
Frequency
depending selection
Two or more
strategies with the
same
fitness.
Alternative Reproductive Tactics, Fig. 2 Bottom: Tra-
ditional categorization of reproductive strategies (decision
rules leading to tactics) by Gross (1996) and top modied
by Schradin and Lindholm (2011). Differences between
strategies refer to whether the shown tactic is genetically
xed (Alternative Strategies) or not (all other strategies).
While Gross (1996) further differentiated whether the t-
ness of ARTs is similar (Mixed Strategies) or different
(Conditional Strategies), Schradin and Lindholm (2011),
see the tness outcome as variable depending on the cur-
rent environmental conditions (Single Strategy)
Alternative Reproductive Tactics 7
(Tomkins and Hazel 2007) further concluded that
mathematical models can neither prove that t-
ness must be unequal (as was proposed by Gross
1996) nor that tness must be equal (as was pro-
posed by Shuster and Wade 2003).
Endocrine Mechanisms of ARTs
The relative plasticity hypothesis (RPH) has been
developed to explain the endocrine mechanisms
underlying ARTs (Moore 1991). The RPH bases
on the theoretical assumption that individuals of
one sex that follow ARTs differ in their hormonal
mechanisms in a similar way as is observed in
primary sex differentiation between males and
females (Moore 1991; Moore et al. 1998). Thus,
in the same way as males and females differ hor-
monally, males of different reproductive tactics
are also expected to differ hormonally. Hormones
can inuence behavior via two main ways
(Phoenix et al. 1959): (1) Early in life, hormones
can have organizational effects that permanently
change the central nervous system and as such
adult behavior. Organizational effects are non-
reversible. For example, male rodents that experi-
ence high levels of testosterone in utero will be
more aggressive when being adult. (2) During
adulthood, hormones have activational effects,
acting on the central nervous system (which was
formed via organizational effects during early
development), making the expression of specic
behaviors more likely. Activational effects are
reversible, i.e., when the hormone secretion
decrease, the behavior ceases. An example
would be singing in song birds, which is activated
by testosterone in spring and ceases in summer/
autumn when testosterone levels decrease.
The RPH predicts that individuals following
xed ARTS differ in their hormone proles early
in development (organizational effects; Table 1).
In contrast, when ARTs are sequential or exible,
individuals following different ARTs are pre-
dicted to differ in hormone proles in adulthood
(activational effects; Table 1). The main hormones
involved in the regulation of male ARTs are andro-
gens, such as testosterone. Bourgeois males typi-
cally have higher androgen levels than other males.
Progesterone and glucocorticoids are other hor-
mones that can be involved in the regulation of
ARTs. While originally the RPH was formulated
for these steroid hormones only, it is now known
that peptide hormones such as prolactin and neu-
ropeptides (oxytocin and vasopressin) can also be
involved. Figure 3shows as an example of how
hormone levels differ in male striped mice follow-
ing ARTs. In this species, it has also been shown
that males of the different tactics do not only differ
in their endocrine prole but also that individual
males change their hormone levels when
switching tactics, and that experimental increase
of testosterone might induce tactic change.
Alternative Reproductive Tactics, Table 1 The hor-
mone prole of males following ARTs as predicted by the
RPH (Moore et al. 1998) and a revised version of the RPH
which I suggest here. ACT: Activational effects leading to
different ARTs. ORG: Organizational effects leading to
different ARTs. PHYS: Physiological effects resulting
from different ARTs
ART type Relative plasticity hypothesis by Moore 1998 Modied relative plasticity hypothesis
Fixed ORG: Hormonal differences in early development.
Adults have similar hormone proles, except when
affected by different social experiences
ORG: Hormonal differences in early
development.
PHYS: Hormonal differences between ARTs
(adults) are similar to differences observed
between sexes.
Sequential ORG: Hormonal differences in adults when ARTs
are formed. Otherwise hormone proles are similar
ORG: Hormonal differences during adulthood
when ARTs are formed
PHYS: Hormone levels should differ between
ARTs
Reversible ACT: Adult morphs will always differ in their
hormone prole, but no differences in early
development
ACT: Hormone levels should differ between
ARTs
8 Alternative Reproductive Tactics
Furthermore, this has consequences for their
metabolism, as resting metabolic rate (metabolic
rate to maintain basal functions only, measured as
oxygen consumption when being at rest at
thermoneutrality) also differs between ARTs.
The endocrinology of ARTs has been investi-
gated in more than 50 vertebrate species, espe-
cially in sh and reptiles, but also in birds and
mammals, and a role of hormones in regulating
ARTs has been reported in nearly every study
(reviewed in Oliveira et al. 2008). However, the
support for the predictions of the RPH has been
low, both for correlative and experimental studies.
Oliveira et al. (2008) concluded that the RPH fails
to explain a large amount of the observed varia-
tion and thus reject the validity of the RPH. Spe-
cically, the existence of hormonal differences in
adults of species with xed ARTs, and an effect of
hormone manipulations in adults with xed ARTs
has led to a rejection of the RPH (Oliveira et al.
2008). However, these problems arise mainly
from two facts that should be taken into account
when considering the RPH (Table 2): (1) As the
RPH is based on the comparison between the male
and female sex, it does not predict that males of
two xed ARTs should only differ hormonally
during development but not during adulthood. In
the same way, as males and females (xed repro-
ductive tactics in most vertebrates) differ in their
hormonal prole, the same can be expected for
two ARTs in one sex. (2) The prediction that
hormone manipulations should have an effect in
early development in species with xed ARTs and
an effect in adults of species with plastic ARTs has
been interpreted to mean it should otherwise have
no effect. However, even in males of species with
no ARTs, experimentally administering testoster-
one can have strong effects, such that the extracted
conclusion it should not have an effect in species
with xed ARTs is not understandable. Further,
some species with plastic ARTs cannot change
their tactic at any stage, but only seasonally, for
example, from one breeding season to the next
(some blennies, a form of marine sh). Blennies
Cort
T
PRL
RMR
Cort
T ↓
PRL
RMR
Cort
T
PRL
RMR
Cort
T
PRL
RMR
Juvenile Breeder
Alternative Reproductive Tactics, Fig. 3 Physiological
change in male striped mice, which when reaching adult-
hood (not gray) can either remain as adult philopatric in
their natal group (blue), become a solitary living roamer
(black), or the breeding male of another group than their
natal group (red). These switches in tactics are associated
with changes in physiology, and it has been shown that
individual males change their physiological prole when
changing tactic. Cort corticosterone, PRL prolactin, RMR
resting metabolic rate, Ttestosterone, #decrease, "
increase, =does not change
Alternative Reproductive Tactics 9
with ARTs do not only show tactic specic behav-
ior but also tactic specic morphology, which is
difcult to change. Here, the effect of hormone
manipulation might be dependent on the time of
year, and experiments done during the breeding
season (when the tactic for the season is already
xed) might be less meaningful than in the pre-
breeding season. In sum, when taking into
account physiological effects resulting from dif-
ferent ARTs and articial effects that do not imply
a natural mechanistic pathway, then the RPH is
still highly valuable in understanding the endo-
crine control of ARTs.
I therefore suggest a modied version of the
RPH, which takes two facts into account: (1) Phys-
iological differences are expected in xed ARTs in
adulthood in the same way as hormonal differ-
ences are observed between males and females in
most species (Table 1). (2) Experimental changes
of hormones, potent mediators of physiology and
behavior, can cause articial effects on physiol-
ogy and behavior that are not observed under
natural conditions, as the imposed hormonal
changes (for example, articially high testoster-
one levels) also do not occur under natural condi-
tions (Table 2).
In sum, the expression of xed ARTs is regu-
lated by permanent organizational effects during
development which can then also lead to perma-
nent differences of hormone proles in adults. In
species with plastic ARTs, the transition from one
tactic to another tactic is associated with changes
in the hormone prole, representing activational
(reversible) endocrine effects. For example,
androgen levels typically increase and glucocorti-
coid levels decrease when males switch from a
sneaker to the bourgeois tactic.
ARTs and Intraspecific Variation in Social
Organization
Intraspecic variation in social organization
(IVSO) occurs when a species shows two or
more of the following forms of social organiza-
tion: living solitarily, in pairs, one breeding male
with several breeding females, one breeding
female with several breeding males, or multi-
male multi-female groups. Hereby, each form of
social organization must be composed of breeding
individuals, not only dispersing solitary individ-
uals or bachelor groups. Thus, each sex must have
the option to live under alternative forms of social
organization. IVSO becomes obvious when indi-
viduals of both sexes change their reproductive
tactics. In other words, if in one species both
males and females can show ARTs, then this can
lead to IVSO. For example, the house mouse (Mus
musculus) can live solitarily, in pairs or in com-
munal groups, with resource availability modify-
ing the intensity of intrasexual aggression in
males and female infanticide. In striped mice,
Alternative Reproductive Tactics, Table 2 Predicted
effects of experimental manipulations of hormones in males
following ARTs as predicted by the RPH (Moore et al.
1998), and a revised version of the RPH which I suggest
here. ACT: Activational effects leading to different ARTs.
ORG: Organizational effects leading to different ARTs.
ARTI: Articial effects that do not imply an evolved endo-
crine pathway that would occur under natural conditions
ART type
Relative plasticity hypothesis by
Moore et al. 1998 Modied relative plasticity hypothesis
Fixed ORG: Effective during early
development, but not in adults
ORG: Effects in early development long-lasting and similar to
effects observed in nature
ARTI ACT: Effects in adults short-lasting and similar or different
to observed differences between ARTs
Sequential ORG: Effective in adults but not
during early development
ORG: Effective in adults when ARTs are formed, long-lasting
and similar to effects observed in nature
ARTI ACT: Effects in nal ART in adults short-lasting and
similar to observed differences between ARTs
Reversible ACT: Effective in adults but not
during early development
ACT: Effective in adults but not during early development
10 Alternative Reproductive Tactics
both sexes show ARTs, and both sexes can live
solitarily or in groups. Thus, solitary living or
living in large communally breeding groups
consisting of one male and two to four breeding
females can occur. In sum, when both males and
females show ARTs, both sexes can live in differ-
ent forms of social organization, which is called
intraspecic variation in social organization.
Conclusion
When different individuals of one sex follow dif-
ferent tactics to reach reproductive success, this
represents an interesting phenomenon for all stu-
dents of animal behavior, both those interested in
the evolution and those interested in the physiol-
ogy and mechanisms of behavior. Such ARTs
occur in both sexes though they seem to be more
common in males and are clearly understudied in
females. ARTs can evolve when either the alter-
native tactics yield the same average tness, or if
following a suboptimal tactic allows individuals
to increase their lifetime reproductive success by
having some, even if low, tness when being
subordinate, waiting to grow and increase com-
petitiveness before adopting the bourgeois tactic
with the highest reproductive success. The expres-
sion of ARTs is regulated by the endocrine sys-
tem. Permanent organizational effects (typically
early in development) cause xed tactics, revers-
ible activational effects in adulthood cause
switches in plastic tactics. If ARTs occur in both
sexes, the social organization of an entire popula-
tion can change, causing intraspecic variation in
social organization. In sum, ARTs represent an
interesting phenomenon that allows us to study
the proximate causes and ultimate consequences
of different behavioral (and sometimes also mor-
phological) phenotypes within one single species.
Cross-References
Adaptation
Behavioral Flexibility
Development of Behavior
Ecology
Evolution
Natural Selection
Ontogeny
Proximate Causation
Reproductive Fitness
Social Behavior
Ultimate Causation
References
Cardwell, J. R., & Liley, N. R. (1991). Androgen control of
social status in males of a wild population of stoplight
parrotsh, Sparisoma viridae (Scaridae). Hormones
and Behavior, 25,118.
Gross, M. R. (1996). Alternative reproductive strategies
and tactics: Diversity within the sexes. TREE, 11,
9298.
Moore, M. C. (1991). Application of organization-
activation theory to alternative male reproductive strat-
egies: A review. Hormones and Behavior, 25, 154179.
Moore, M. C., Hews, D. K., & Knapp, R. (1998). Hor-
monal control and evolution of alternative male pheno-
types: Generalizations of models for sexual
differentiation. American Zoologist, 38, 133151.
Oliveira, R. F., Canario, A. V. M., & Ros, A. F. H. (2008).
Hormones and alternative reproductive tactics in verte-
brates. In R. F. Oliveira, M. Taborsky, & H. J.
Brockmann (Eds.), Alternative reproductive tactics
(pp. 132174). Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Phoenix, C. H., Goy, R. W., Gerall, A. A., & Young, W. C.
(1959). Organizing action of prenatally administered
testosterone propionate on the tissue mediating mating
behavior in the female guniea pig. Endocrinology, 65,
369382.
Schradin, C., & Lindholm, A. K. (2011). Relative tness of
alternative male reproductive tactics in a mammal
varies between years. Journal of Animal Ecology, 80,
908917.
Shuster, S. M., & Wade, M. J. (2003). Mating systems and
strategies. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Solmsen, N., Johannesen, J., & Schradin, C. (2011).
Highly asymmetric ne-scale genetic structure between
sexes of African striped mice and indication for condi-
tion dependent alternative male dispersal tactics.
Molecular Ecology, 20, 16241634.
Taborsky, M. (2008). The evolution of alternative repro-
ductive tactics: Concepts and questions. In R. F.
Oliveira, M. Taborsky, & H. J. Brockmann (Eds.),
Alternative reproductive tactics (pp. 121). Cam-
bridge: Cambridge University Press.
Tomkins, J. L., & Hazel, W. (2007). The status of the
conditional evolutionary stable strategy. TREE, 22,
522528.
Alternative Reproductive Tactics 11
... In some species, in which courtship is especially risky, males have evolved alternative tactics by which they may mate without first courting the female. Alternative tactics might include sneaking copulations or mating with females while they are occupied in some other activity and unable to repel or attack the male (Buzatto, Tomkins, & Simmons, 2014;Schradin, 2019;Shuster, 2010;Taborsky, Oliviera & Brockmann, 2008;Thornhill & Alcock, 1983). Sexual cannibalism can be especially pronounced in spiders (Buskirk, Frohlich, & Ross, 1984;Elgar, 1992;Robinson & Robinson, 1980), and the males of some spiders are known to circumvent the risk of being killed by mating with moulting or freshly moulted females, which are immobile while the cuticle is still soft (Danielson-François, Hou, Cole, & Tso, 2012;Foellmer & Fairbairn, 2003;Lubin, 1986;Uhl, Zimmer, Renner, & Schneider, 2015). ...
Article
Keywords: brown widow spider Latrodectus geometricus male mate choice mating with subadults sexual cannibalism Mating generally occurs with adult females, which undergo a suite of changes in morphology, physiology and behaviour during maturation. In the brown widow spider, Latrodectus geometricus, however, males can mate with immature females during a short period before they moult to the adult stage. Mating with immature females seems beneficial for males, because they are not at risk of being cannibalized, whereas cannibalism inevitably occurs in matings with adult females. We conducted choice experiments to elucidate male preference, courtship and mating behaviour with immature and adult females of different ages. We controlled for age of the females' webs to provide males with potential web-borne attractants of similar age. We tested whether males distinguish immature females that are ready to mate (late subadult stage) from adult females and from immature females that do not mate (early subadults), and we examined male response to young versus old adult females. Males approached and mated with adult females more frequently than late subadult females, but there were no differences in the frequencies of approach to early and late subadults or to adult females of different ages. Once on the web, however, males attempted to mate with the late subadults. We suggest that web-borne volatile cues, typical of adult females, may be reduced or lacking in late subadult females, yet less volatile cues may indicate receptivity.
Article
Full-text available
Polyandrous mating, where females mate with multiple males, can be caused by male manipulation, primarily benefiting males. Females, conversely, may suffer costs without direct benefits. According to the good gene hypothesis, females should choose superior males to pass on good genes to their offspring in lek-breeding species. However, when females are compelled to mate against their preference, which leads to polyandrous mating, indirect genetic costs arise, with lower-quality males fathering some offspring. Despite these costs, polyandrous mating may potentially benefit females through fertilization insurance and increased genetic diversity. This study investigated the costs and benefits of polyandrous mating in the lek-chorusing treefrog (Zhangixalus prasinatus) by comparing clutch fertilization success, offspring performance (embryo viability, survival, growth, development) linked to fitness, and allelic diversity between the clutches after monandrous and polyandrous mating. Our results showed that polyandrous mating (1) did not increase fertilization success, (2) did not affect embryo viability, offspring survival, growth, or development, but (3) increased genetic diversity. Additionally, we found that primary males (chosen by females) sired more offspring than peripheral males (not chosen by females). The small proportion of the clutch sired by peripheral males suggests that polyandrous mating in Z. prasinatus may be driven by males to pursue reproductive success, and may, as a by-product, potentially benefit females by increasing their offspring's genetic diversity. Therefore, peripheral males achieve higher reproductive success, while females lack apparent costs, which is suggested to be a likely reason for the moderate frequency of polyandrous mating in this species. Significance statement Our study investigates the impact of polyandrous mating on female emerald treefrogs (Zhangixalus prasinatus). Our findings reveal that while polyandrous mating neither enhances nor reduces fertilization success, embryo viability, offspring survival, growth, or development, it does increase genetic diversity within clutches. Although this genetic diversity may be a by-product of male reproductive strategies, it potentially offers females indirect genetic benefits, which may help explain the moderate frequency of polyandrous mating in this species. Our research contributes to a deeper understanding of mating systems and reproductive strategies in lek-breeding anurans.
Article
Full-text available
It is often argued that anisogamy causes alternative reproductive tactics (ARTs) to be more common in males than females. We challenge this view by pointing out logical flaws in the argument. We then review recent work on the diversity of female ARTs, listing several understudied types such as solitary versus communal breeding and facultative parthenogenesis. We highlight an important difference between male and female ARTs that caused female ARTs to be overlooked: male ARTs tend to focus on successful fertilization, whereas female ARTs occur at many stages of reproduction and often form complex networks of decision points. We propose to study correlated female ARTs as a whole to better understand their drivers and eco-evolutionary dynamics.
Book
Full-text available
This book presents the first unified conceptual and statistical framework for understanding the evolution of reproductive strategies. Using the concept of the opportunity for sexual selection, the authors illustrate how and why sexual selection, though restricted to one sex and opposed in the other, is one of the strongest and fastest of all evolutionary forces. They offer a statistical framework for studying mating system evolution and apply it to patterns of alternative mating strategies. In doing so, they provide a method for quantifying how the strength of sexual selection is affected by the ecological and life history processes that influence females' spatial and temporal clustering and reproductive schedules. Directly challenging verbal evolutionary models that attempt to explain reproductive behavior without quantitative reference to evolutionary genetics, this book establishes a more solid theoretical foundation for the field. Among the weaknesses the authors find in the existing data is the apparent ubiquity of condition-dependent mating tactics. They identify factors likely to contribute to the evolution of alternative mating strategies--which they argue are more common than generally believed--and illustrate how to measure the strength of selection acting on them. Lastly, they offer predictions on the covariation of mating systems and strategies, consider the underlying developmental biology behind male polyphenism, and propose directions for future research. Informed by genetics, this is a comprehensive and rigorous new approach to explaining mating systems and strategies that will influence a wide swath of evolutionary biology.
Article
Full-text available
CHAPTER SUMMARY The wide diversity of alternative tactics of reproduction found among vertebrates offers a unique opportunity to study the endocrine mechanisms underlying the phenotypic variation of reproductive traits. Here, we first assess the existing conceptual frameworks on the mechanisms underlying the expressionofalternative reproductive tactics (ARTs)byreviewing theavailabledataonhormonelevels in alternative phenotypes and on the effects of hormone manipulations in different vertebrate taxa. We then high- light recent studies that have opened new avenues of research on the neuroendocrine basis of ARTs, such as the use of functional genomics to study differential gene expression between morphs. Finally, we stress the need to integrate the study of ARTs with the mechanisms under- lying the expression of alternative phenotypes and with functional studies of ARTs. Only such an integrative approach will allow a comprehensive understanding of the evolution and development of ARTs.
Article
Full-text available
CHAPTER SUMMARY Here we outline the meaning of the term alternative reproductive tactics, or ARTs, and discuss why the existence of ARTs is so widespread in animals. We ask what we need to know to understand the evolution of ARTs and the importance of general principles such as frequency dependence, density dependence, and condition dependence, and what we need to know about proximate mechanisms involved in the regulation of ARTs to comprehend evolutionary patterns. We discuss current issues in the study of ARTs and list 12 questions that we think need particular attention. Throughout we shall provide representative examples of ARTs in animals to illustrate the ubiquitous nature of this phenomenon. WHAT IS THE MEANING OF ALTERNATIVE REPRODUCTIVE TACTIC Alternative The concept of ARTs refers to alternative ways to obtain fertilizations in both males and females. In its most common use, this term refers to traits selected to maximize fitness in two or more alternative ways in the context of intraspecific and intrasexual reproductive competition. In general, alternative phenotypes are characterized by a discontinuous distribution of traits evolved towards the same functional end. Examples include size dimorphism, color polymorphism, dimorphic morphological structures involved in the monopolization of resources or mates, and various behavioral alternatives such as territoriality vs. floating, monopolization vs. scramble competition, or investment in primary access to a resource vs. social parasitism. Individuals allocate resources to either one or the other (mutually exclusive) way of achieving the same functional end using evolved decision-making rules (Brockmann 2001). © Cambridge University Press 2008 and Cambridge University Press, 2009.
Article
Full-text available
SYNOPSIS. An organism's phenotype results from an interaction of environment and genotype. Sex steroids play a role in translating sexual genotype into phenotype. The focus of this research has been to extend the model of sex steroid hormone action in sexual differentiation to individual variation in reproductive phenotype. The hypothesis generated, called the relative plasticity hypothesis, has been tested in a species with alternative phenotypes, tree lizards ( Urosaurus ornatus ). Such species are useful models for tests of these ideas because variation is extreme and easily studied. These tests have shown that permanent differentiation of the territorial and nonterritorial phenotypes is accomplished by hormonal mechanisms operating during early development. These are similar to organizational actions of hormones classically described for sexual differentiation. A unique finding of this work is that the adrenal hormone progesterone regulates differentiation of the two male types. Furthermore, the nonterritorial male tree lizard appears to conditionally switch tactics between sedentary satellite and nomadic behavior. Nomadic behavior is triggered under stressful environmental conditions. Nonterritorial tree lizards show greater inhibition of reproductive hormones following stress than do territorial tree lizards, suggesting that a differential sensitivity of reproductive hormones to stress is the endocrine basis of conditional tactic switching in this morph. This mechanism is similar to the classical activational effects of hormones. Together, these findings indicate that individual variation in sexual phenotype is produced by mechanisms similar to sexual differentiation. Further refinement of the models and integration with some evolutionary ideas is explored.
Article
The sexual behavior of male and female guinea pigs from mothers receiving testosterone propionate during most of pregnancy was studied after the attainment of adulthood. As a part of the investigation, the responsiveness of the females to estradiol benzoate and progesterone and to testosterone propionate was determined. The larger quantities of testosterone propionate produced hermaphrodites having external genitalia indistinguishable macroscopicalty from those of newborn males. Gonadectomized animals of this type were used for tests of their responsiveness to estradiol benzoate and progesterone and to testosterone propionate. The capacity to display lordosis following administration of estrogen and progesterone was greatly reduced. Male-like mounting behavior, on the other hand, was displayed by many of these animals even when lordosis could not be elicited. Suppression of the capacity for displaying lordosis was achieved with a quantity of androgen less than that required for masculinization of the external genitalia. The hermaphrodites receiving testosterone propionate as adults displayed an amount of mounting behavior which approached that displayed by the castrated injected males receiving the same hormone. The data are uniform in demonstrating that an androgen administered prenatally has an organizing action on the tissues mediating mating behavior in the sense of producing a responsiveness to exogenous hormones which differs from that of normal adult females. No structural abnormalities were apparent in the male siblings and their behavior was essentially normal. The results are believed to justify the conclusion that the prenatal period is a time when fetal morphogenic substances have an organizing or “differentiating” action on the neural tissues mediating mating behavior. During adulthood the hormones are activational. Attention is directed to the parallel nature of the relationship, on the one hand, between androgens and the differentiation of the genital tracts, and on the other, between androgens and the organization of the neural tissues destined to mediate mating behavior in the adult.
Article
1. In many species, males can use different behavioural tactics to achieve fertilization, so called alternative reproductive tactics (ARTs). Few field studies have measured fitness consequences of ARTs under varying environmental conditions. 2. Here we describe fitness consequences of three phenotypically plastic ARTs in the African striped mouse (Rhabdomys pumilio), and show that relative fitness of ARTs differs between years. Each year represents a different generation. 3. For the generation living under high population density tactics differed in relative fitness in accordance with the theory of conditional strategies, with highly successful territorial breeding males having 10 times higher success than solitary roaming males and 102 times higher success than adult natally philopatric males. 4. For the generation living under intermediate population density the territorial breeding and roaming tactics yielded similar fitness which would be in agreement with the theory of mixed strategies. No philopatric males occurred. 5. For the generation living under low population density roaming was the only tactic used and some roamers had very high fitness. 6. The main prediction of status dependent selection for conditional strategies is a correlation between fitness and status, often measured as body mass, but we did not find this correlation within tactics when more than one tactic was expressed in the population. 7. Female distribution seems to have an effect on which reproductive tactics male chose: female defence polygyny when females are clumped (interference competition), but a searching tactic when females are dispersed (scramble competition). In contrast to predictions arising from theory on scramble competition, male body mass was important in determining fitness only in the year when females were dispersed, but not in other years. 8. Our results indicate that the differentiation between conditional and mixed strategies is not an absolute one. In many other species environmental conditions might fluctuate temporally and spatially so that the normally sub-optimal tactic yields similar fitness to the (usually) dominant tactic, or that only a single tactic prevails. 9. We suggest the term single strategy, independent of current fitness consequences, for systems where tactics are not genetically determined, in contrast to genetically determined alternative strategies.
Article
Sex-biased dispersal is observed in many taxa, but few studies have compared sex-biased dispersal among and within populations. We addressed the magnitude and habitat dependency of sex-biased dispersal in social African striped mice by separating group-related from population-related genetic variance to understand the contribution of each sex to deme structure. As dispersal over unoccupied habitat is likely to be more costly than dispersal within a population, we predicted that individuals leaving the natal population have a lower body condition, being inferior to heavier territorial individuals. Fine-scale genetic structure was detected in both sexes. Female relatedness decreased continuously from R = 0.21 at 25 m to zero at 500 m. Maximum male relatedness R = 0.05 was constant at distances between 25 and 75 m, becoming zero at 100 m. Genetic variance (F(ST) ) among seven locations was significantly higher in females than in males, while inbreeding estimates (F(IS) ) were significantly higher in males than in females. Assignment tests estimated significantly more migrants among males, while Bayesian clustering estimated only a single genetic unit cluster for males among the seven locations. The mean body mass of migrant males (44 g) was significantly lower than for males that remained resident and thus dispersed within their sub-population (48 g). Combined, the results showed habitat-independent male-biased dispersal and high female philopatry, and suggested that body condition was more important than kinship in male dispersal decisions. We suggest that locally inferior males are important for gene flow between sub-populations. Thus, males might follow alternative dispersal tactics.
Article
Not all members of a sex behave in the same way. Frequency- and statusdependent selection have given rise to many alternative reproductive phenotypes within the sexes. The evolution and proximate control of these alternatives are only beginning to be understood. Although game theory has provided a theoretical framework, the concept of the mixed strategy has not been realized in nature, and alternative strategies are very rare. Recent findings suggest that almost all alternative reproductive phenotypes within the sexes are due to alternative tactics within a conditional strategy, and, as such, while the average fitnesses of the alternative phenotypes are unequal, the strategy is favoured in evolution. Proximate mechanisms that underlie alternative phenotypes may have many similarities with those operating between the sexes.
Article
In the protogynous stoplight parrotfish (Sparisoma viride), large males defend territories that encompass the home-ranges of several mature females. However, high-quality habitat is in short supply, such that smaller, competitively inferior males do not defend territories. We investigated the role of 11-ketotestosterone (11KT) and testosterone (T) in the regulation of territorial behavior in a wild population of a protogynous reef fish, the stoplight parrotfish, at Glover's Reef, Belize. Radioimmunoassay of plasma samples from individuals of known social status revealed that nonterritorial males have lower levels of T and 11KT than territorial males. Nonterritorial males allowed access to vacant territories underwent pronounced increases in T and 11KT. When sampled 1 week after territory acquisition, levels of T and 11KT in new territorial males were significantly higher than the levels in established territorial males, but by 3 weeks after territory acquisition, there was no significant difference. We further investigated the hypothesis that such short-term increases in androgen levels are a response to intense male-male interactions during territory establishment. Simulated territorial intrusion promoted increased plasma levels of both T and 11KT while access to vacant territories without neighboring territorial males did not. These findings suggest that the endocrine system plays a role in fine-tuning the levels of territorial aggression exhibited by male stoplight parrotfish. We discuss these results in light of recent theory in behavioral endocrinology.
Article
Many species have extreme within-sex morphological and behavioral polymorphisms, most commonly different male phenotypes that practice different reproductive strategies. Although much is known about the role of hormones in sexual differentiation, little is known about what role hormones might play in within-sex differentiation. The relative plasticity hypothesis is derived from the classical organization-activation model of hormone action. It distinguishes between two types of polymorphic systems: a fixed system in which individual males assume one phenotype for their adult lives and a plastic system in which individual males can change phenotypes at least once. By analogy to sexual differentiation, the relative plasticity hypothesis generally predicts that organizational influences of hormones will be more important in fixed systems and activational influences of hormones will be more important in plastic systems. A review of our knowledge of the role of hormones in differentiation of within-sex polymorphisms indicates that the relative plasticity hypothesis accounts for otherwise diverse and contradictory results. This further supports the hypothesis that the organizational-activational model of hormone action derived from sexual differentiation generalizes to within-sex polymorphisms. However, studies of the effects of hormone manipulations on within-sex differentiation are rare but are desperately needed to further our understanding of this problem. Further studies of discontinuous behavioral variation characteristic of polymorphic species may further our understanding of the physiological basis of within-sex behavior variation in all species.