ArticlePDF Available

Perfectionism, worry, rumination, and distress: A meta-analysis of the evidence for the perfectionism cognition theory

Authors:

Abstract

The perfectionism cognition theory (PCT) provides a theoretical account of the cognitive mechanisms of perfectionism, with an emphasis on the function of cognitive perseveration. The framework of PCT provides three central themes: (1) that both self-oriented perfectionism (SOP) and socially prescribed perfectionism (SPP) are correlated with rumination and worry; (2) that perfectionists are more likely to suffer from interrelated forms of cognitive perseveration; (3) that rumination and worry are important variables that contribute to vulnerability to emotional distress and physical illness. A meta-analysis was performed to examine the three themes of PCT. The results suggested that both SOP and SPP were positively correlated with worry and rumination. Moderator analyses suggested that both SOP and SPP are correlated with various forms of cognitive perseveration and meta-analytic mediation models suggested worry and rumination contribute to the relationship between perfectionism and distress. The findings of this meta-analysis provide evidence to support the three themes of PCT and shed light on the mechanisms and processes of perfectionism cognition.
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
Perfectionism, Worry, Rumination, and Distress: A Meta-Analysis of the Evidence for
the Perfectionism Cognition Theory
Abstract
The perfectionism cognition theory (PCT) provides a theoretical account of the
cognitive mechanisms of perfectionism, with an emphasis on the function of cognitive
perseveration. The framework of PCT provides three central themes: (1) that both
self-oriented perfectionism (SOP) and socially prescribed perfectionism (SPP) are
correlated with rumination and worry; (2) that perfectionists are more likely to suffer
from interrelated forms of cognitive perseveration; (3) that rumination and worry are
important variables that contribute to vulnerability to emotional distress and physical
illness. A meta-analysis was performed to examine the three themes of PCT. The
results suggested that both SOP and SPP were positively correlated with worry and
rumination. Moderator analyses suggested that both SOP and SPP are correlated with
various forms of cognitive perseveration and meta-analytic mediation models
suggested worry and rumination contribute to the relationship between perfectionism
and distress. The findings of this meta-analysis provide evidence to support the three
themes of PCT and shed light on the mechanisms and processes of perfectionism
cognition.
Keywords: rumination, worry, cognitive perseveration, perfectionism cognition
theory
1
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
1. Introduction
During the past two decades, important studies have provided empirical evidence
for and theoretical models of perfectionism (e.g. Frost, Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate,
1990; Hewitt & Flett, 1991; Stoeber & Otto, 2006). Researchers have agreed that
perfectionism is a vulnerability factor for psychological issues like depression and
anxiety (e.g. Flett, Madorsky, Hewitt, & Heisel, 2002; Frost & DiBartolo, 2002).
Recently, Flett, Nepon and Hewitt (2016) proposed the perfectionism cognition theory
(PCT) to provide a theoretical account of the cognitive mechanisms of perfectionism,
with an emphasis on the function of cognitive perseveration. As two comparable
forms of cognitive perseveration, rumination and worry play an important role in the
perfectionism-distress link because perfectionists who are at risk of mental health
issues tend to ruminate and worry. Therefore, the correlation between cognitive
perseveration and perfectionism could shed light on the cognitive processes and
mechanisms of perfectionism.
1.1. Perfectionism
Perfectionism is defined as the setting of and striving for excessively high
standards of personal performance and overly critical evaluation of oneself (Frost et
al., 1990). Early researchers treated perfectionism as a unitary construct, describing
perfectionism as a fear of failure due to poor self-esteem (Beck, 1976; Ellis, 1962;
Missildine, 1963), and early unidimensional perfectionism measurements, such as the
perfectionism subscales of the Eating Disorder Inventory (Garner, Olmstead, &
Polivy, 1983) and the Burns Perfectionism Scale (Burns, 1980), were adopted.
2
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
Hamachek (1978) was the first to propose a multifarious conceptualisation of
perfectionism and to suggest that the different dimensions of perfectionism have
significant functional influence on positive and negative outcomes. Consequently,
researchers developed multidimensional perfectionism measurements that provided
theoretical and empirical advances in terms of the multidimensional construct of
perfectionism (Stoeber & Otto, 2006).
The Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (MPS-F) was the first
multidimensional perfectionism measurement to assess six dimensions of
perfectionism, including personal standards, organisation, concern over mistakes,
doubts about actions, parental expectations, and parental criticism (Frost et al., 1990).
The MPS-F has been widely used due to its good psychometric properties (Frost &
DiBartolo, 2002). Hewitt and Flett (1991) subsequently looked at the clinical features
of perfectionism and reviewed the relevant literature and developed the
Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (MPS-HF; Hewitt & Flett, 1991). The MPS-
HF emphasises the importance of both intrapersonal and interpersonal factors and has
three dimensions: self-oriented perfectionism (SOP), socially prescribed
perfectionism (SPP), and other-oriented perfectionism (OOP). Other
multidimensional perfectionism measurements that assess traits of perfectionism have
also emerged, such as the Almost Perfect Scale-Revised (APS-R; Slaney, Rice,
Mobley, Trippi, & Ashby, 2001), the Perfectionism Inventory (PI; Hill et al., 2004),
and the Perfectionism Questionnaire (PQ; Rhéaume, Ladouceur, & Freesto, 2000).
3
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
This meta-analysis adopted MPS-HF as the measurement for perfectionism, because
the PCT focuses on SOP and SPP.
In the multidimensional model of perfectionism proposed by Hewitt and Flett
(1991), the three forms of perfectionism relate to beliefs, motivations, and behaviours.
Self-oriented perfectionists believe that becoming perfect is important to themselves
and set very high personal standards. If a self-oriented perfectionist fails to meet these
standards, they will criticise themselves. In comparison, socially prescribed
perfectionists believe that becoming perfect is important to others and that they need
to be perfect for others. If socially prescribed perfectionists fail to meet their high
expectations, they believe that they will be criticised by others.
In both SOP and SPP, there is an element of self-criticism, whereas other-
oriented perfectionists set high standards for others and expect others to become
perfect. If others fail to meet these standards, other-oriented perfectionists will
criticise them. Therefore, SOP and SPP focus on criticism of the self, whereas OOP
focuses on criticism of others (Hewitt & Flett, 2004). It is clear that concerns about
criticism of the self are correlated with various forms of perseverative cognition (Flett
et al., 2016), therefore, the framework of PCT focuses on SOP and SPP, but not OOP.
SOP has both adaptive and maladaptive features. On the one hand, self-oriented
perfectionists strive for high standards by using adaptive strategies, such as
achievement motivation (Klibert, Langhinrichsen-Rohling, & Saito, 2005), self-
efficacy (Mills & Blankstein, 2000), and positive affect (Frost, Heimberg, Holt,
Mattia, & Neubauer, 1993). On the other hand, SOP is also found to correlate with
4
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
depressive symptoms (e.g. Hewitt & Flett, 1991; Olson & Kwon, 2008) as unrealistic
self-standards and harsh evaluations of the self may generate stress associated with
failure and negative emotions. In contrast, SPP is only associated with negative
psychological outcomes, such as depressive symptoms (Klibert, Langhinrichsen-
Rohling, & Saito, 2005), trait anger (Dunkley & Blankstein, 2000), suicide proneness
(Blankstein, Lumley, & Crawford, 2007), and procrastination (Xie, Yang, & Chen,
2018).
1.2. Worry and rumination
Worry and rumination are similar, but distinguishable forms of perseverative
cognition. Worry is defined as thoughts and images about affectively negative and
relatively uncontrollable future events (Borkovec, Ray, & Stober, 1998; Borkovec,
Robinson, Pruzinsky, & DePree, 1983). Rumination is characterised by repetitive,
intrusive, and past-oriented cognitions about negative experiences (Papageorgiou &
Siegle, 2003). Worry and rumination have similar thought processes (i.e. repetitive
and negative) and are both chronic (Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky, 2008),
but the type of goal orientation associated with them differs. Worry is more future-
oriented, while rumination is considered to be much more past-oriented (Watkins,
Moulds, & Mackintosh, 2005). It should also be noted that worry is focused on
events, while rumination is focused on feelings (Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, &
Lyubomirsky, 2008).
Self-report questionnaires are typically adopted to assess worry and rumination.
The most common measurement of worry is the Penn State Worry Questionnaire
5
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
(PSWQ; Meyer, Miller, Metzger, & Borkovec, 1990), which is widely used to assess
trait worry. Similarly, the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale (IUS; Buhr & Dugas,
2002) assesses worry-related cognitive factors. The most common measurements of
rumination are the Response Styles Questionnaire (RSQ; Nolen-Hoeksema &
Morrow, 1991) and the Ruminative Response Scale (RRS; Treynor, Gonzalez, &
Nolen-Hoeksema, 2003), which require participants to recall depressive experiences
and to assess the degree of their ruminative symptoms. Although these two
questionnaires are the most widely used, researchers also have developed other self-
report questionnaires of rumination (Whitmer & Gotlib, 2013), such as the
Rumination-Reflection Questionnaire (RRQ; Trapnell & Campbell, 1999) and
Rumination About an Interpersonal Offense (RIO; Wade, Vogel, Liao, & Goldman,
2008).
Individual differences and situational characteristics have been found to be the
determinants of perseverative cognition. Low self-confidence in problem-solving
abilities causes worrisome thinking (Davey, 1994) and a low internal locus of control
causes rumination (Watson , Clark, & Tellegen, 1984). Gender is a common factor
that impacts on the ruminative response style according to the response styles theory
(RST; Nolen-Hoeksema, 1987) and Nolen-Hoeksema, Girgus, and Seligman (1991)
found that women are more likely to be ruminative. Personality traits like neuroticism
and perfectionism are found to be important predictors of perseverative cognition
(Blankstein & Lumley, 2008; Wänke & Schmid, 1996). Several models of
perseverative cognition also highlight the importance of goals as the failure of goal
6
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
attainment leads to repetitive thought, which is likely to continue until the goal is
achieved (Higgins & Carver, 1990; Martin & Tesser, 1996). These models suggest that
the disruption of goals that are important to an individual’s life is more likely to cause
perseverative cognition.
Several findings have provided evidence that perseverative cognition is
correlated with distress and that it can maintain and exacerbate negative affect in
several ways. Repetitive thoughts make individuals dwell on their problems rather
than solve them or reduce the difficulties they face, which causes a high level of
perceived stress (Just & Alloy, 1997; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000). Perseverative
cognition can prolong negative affect as individuals tend to interpret their life events
in an irrational and negative way (Nolen-Hoeksema, Morrow, & Fredrickson, 1993).
Distress can also trigger worry and rumination, which then makes individuals feel
more distressed, leading to an emotional and cognitive vicious cycle (Spasojević &
Alloy, 2001). Lyubomirsky and Tkach (2004) suggested that the correlation between
perseverative cognition and distress is due to impaired motivation, biased memory,
and poor health behaviours.
1.3. Perfectionism Cognition Theory
PCT is a theoretical model that provides a framework for perfectionism to
explicate its cognitive processes and mechanisms (Flett et al., 2016). This model
highlights the importance of two forms of perseverative cognition: worry and
rumination. The framework of PCT provides three central themes: (1) that both SOP
and SPP are correlated with rumination and worry; (2) that perfectionists are more
7
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
likely to suffer from interrelated forms of cognitive perseveration; (3) that rumination
and worry are important variables that contribute to vulnerability to emotional distress
and physical illness.
The first theme of PCT focuses on the relationship between perfectionism and
perseverative cognition and suggests that perfectionists are more likely to be chronic
overthinkers. Perfectionists fear negative evaluations by themselves and others, which
can be magnified by worry and rumination. It is clear that evaluation fears, which
activate various forms of cognitive perseveration, can contribute to the relationship
between perfectionism and cognitive perseveration. Self-evaluative and socially
evaluative concerns are important to the nature of perfectionism and provide a
theoretical account of perseverative cognition among perfectionists. The findings of
empirical studies have provided evidence that both SOP and SPP are correlated with
worry and rumination in college and community samples (Olson & Kwon, 2008;
Randles, Flett, Nash, McGregor, & Hewitt, 2010; Senra, Merino, & Ferreiro, 2017;
Wilson, Hunter, Rasmussen, & McGowan, 2015).
Although there are evidences supported the links between perfectionism and
perseverative cognition, the magnitude of the correlation is still unclear. Meta-analysis
is a statistical method to aggregate and synthesize the existing findings from those
studies which focus on a particular topic (Glass, 1976). From this perspective, meta-
analysis is an appropriate method to evaluate the magnitude of correlations between
perfectionism and perseverative cognition. However, to the best of our knowledge,
there is no meta-analysis in terms of perfectionism and perseverative cognition.
8
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
Therefore, the first aim of this study is to meta-analysis method to evaluate the
magnitude of the four comparable correlations between perfectionism and
perseverative cognition (SOP and worry, SPP and worry, SOP, and rumination, and
SPP and rumination).
The second theme of PCT assumes that both SOP and SPP are correlated with
various forms of cognitive perseveration. Flett et al. (2016) used case studies to
survey several types of cognitive perseveration, including mistake rumination, post-
event overthinking, and social comparison rumination, which can exacerbate and
prolong the stress and distress of perfectionists. A range of stressors can elicit various
types of cognitive perseveration that make perfectionists more likely to be concerned
about possible or actual failures. The constant pressure from the physical and social
environment can impair cognitive capacity in chronically stressed perfectionists and
empirical studies indicate that perfectionists are prone to many types of cognitive
perseveration (see Cox & Chen, 2014; Hewitt & Flett, 2007; Treynor, Gonzalez, &
Nolen-Hoeksema, 2003).
Clinical cases of perfectionists can provide evidence regarding the link between
perfectionism and various forms of cognitive perseveration, but research comparing
these links is minimal. There are various types of cognitive perseveration, therefore, it
is necessary to compare the correlations between perfectionism and the multiple types
of overthinking. To assess different forms of cognitive perseveration, researchers
adopt different measurements of worry and rumination, as mentioned above.
Moderator analysis can examine every group of correlation that depends on the
9
0
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
measurements of the included studies. Therefore, the second aim of this study is to
evaluate the correlations between perfectionism and the various forms of cognitive
perseveration.
The third theme of PCT states that worry and rumination can contribute to both
mental and physical health outcomes in perfectionists. Perseverative cognition causes
perfectionists to have more intense negative emotions and stress reactions, which can
lead to perseverating perfectionists having reduced attention and cognitive capacity
and suffering from emotional and physical exhaustion. The cognitive perseveration
hypothesis proposed by Brosschot, Gerin and Thayer (2006) highlights the role of
perseverative cognition in response to a variety of stressors and suggests that worry
and rumination prolong stress-related responses and lead to poor health outcomes.
Many existing studies show that worry and rumination are correlated to psychological
distress among perfectionists (Blankstein & Lumley, 2008; Cox & Chen, 2014;
Nepon, Flett, Hewitt, & Molnar, 2011), but the correlation between cognitive
perseveration and physical illness among perfectionists has not been explored
empirically. Therefore, the present meta-analysis only focuses on the mental health of
perseverating perfectionists.
PCT highlights two contributors (rumination and worry) to the perfectionism
cognitive process and suggests that they mediate the link between perfectionism and
distress. O’Connor, O’Connor and Marshall (2007) found that rumination mediates
the correlation between SOP/SPP and depression and hopelessness, while Short and
Mazmanian (2013) found that both rumination and worry mediate the correlation
10
1
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
between SPP and negative affect. To the best of our knowledge, however, there is a
lack of studies that explore the mediating role of worry in the link between SOP and
mental health. To illuminate the mechanisms of perfectionism cognition, it is therefore
necessary to explore whether worry and rumination mediate the link between
SOP/SPP and psychological distress.
PCT is a theoretical model of cognitive perfectionism’s processes and
mechanisms that provides a better understanding of the role of cognitive perseveration
in perfectionists’ psychological and physical health. The three aims of the present
meta-analysis reflect the three themes of the PCT. The first aim is to evaluate the
magnitude of the correlation between perfectionism and perseverative cognition. The
second aim is to examine the correlations between two dimensions of perfectionism
and the various forms of cognitive perseveration. The third aim is to examine the
mediating roles of cognitive perseveration between multidimensional perfectionism
and distress.
2. Methods
2.1. Literature search
The literature search was conducted using seven databases (ERIC, MEDLINE,
ProQuest Dissertations and Thesis, PsycARTICLES, PsycINFO, PubMed, and Web of
Science) and by using the following search terms: (perfectionism or perfection or
perfectionist or perfectionistic) and (rumination or ruminate or ruminative or worry
or overthinking or repetitive thought or repetitive think or cognitive perseveration).
The publication date was limited to between 1991 and 2018 because the MPS-HF was
11
2
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
first developed in 1991 (Hewitt, et al., 1991). In addition, one chapter of the book by
Flett et al. (2016) that first proposed PCT was reviewed and the reference list was
examined for potential studies. Finally, we contacted several authors via email to
request potential studies and unpublished data sets. The literature search was finalised
in March 2018.
2.2. Inclusion criteria
Each study included in this meta-analysis had to meet four inclusion criteria: the
MPS-HF had to have been used to measure perfectionism; at least one form of
cognitive perseveration, including rumination and worry, had to have been measured;
the correlation coefficient r had to have been reported; and the study had to be written
in English. If a published study and an unpublished study (dissertation) used the same
data set, the published study was included. After screening the full text of the articles,
15 studies met the inclusion criteria. The study selection procedure (see Fig.1)
followed the PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-
Analyses) guidelines (Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, & Altman, 2009).
Insert Fig. 1 about here
Fig.1. The PRISMA diagram (Moher et al., 2009).
2.3. Coding
Two authors coded the effect sizes and the characteristics of studies
independently. Any disagreement in the coding was resolved before conducting any
analyses. The correlation coefficient r was coded as the measure of effect size and
every study included in this meta-analysis had to have reported at least one effect size.
12
3
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
We extracted four comparisons of the correlation between perfectionism and cognitive
perseveration: (i) SOP and worry (k = 7, n = 1278); (ii) SPP and worry (k = 7, n =
1278); (iii) SOP and rumination (k = 30, n = 5339); (iv) SPP and rumination (k = 31, n
= 5387). Mean age, percentage of females, country of origin, publication type,
rumination measures, worry measures, depression measures, and anxiety measures
were coded for moderators. Table 1 shows the studies included in this meta-analysis
and their corresponding study characteristics. To test the mediation model, we coded
four correlation matrices: (i) SOP, worry, depression, and anxiety (k = 7, n = 1278);
(ii) SPP, worry, depression, and anxiety (k = 7, n = 1278); (iii) SOP, rumination,
depression, and anxiety (k = 36, n = 6285); (iv) SPP, rumination, depression, and
anxiety (k = 37, n = 6333).
Insert Table 1 about here
2.4. Data analysis
2.4.1. Overall estimated effect sizes. A meta-analysis is used to estimate the
effect sizes from a series of empirical studies addressing the same question (Field &
Gillett, 2010). We conducted the current meta-analysis by using the correlation
coefficient r as the measure of effect size. Although Hedges and Olkin (1985)
suggested that Fisher’s r-to-z transformation can be performed for combining
correlation coefficients, researchers do not completely agree with this procedure (see,
Field, 2001; Hafdahl, 2009; Hunter & Schmidt, 1990). As a meta-analytic mediation
model requires raw correlation matrices, the correlations in this meta-analysis were
not transformed to Fisher’s z for the sake of consistency. In addition, the 95%
13
4
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
confidence intervals of the overall mean effect sizes were reported. Fixed-effects
models are adopted for studies with homogeneity, whereas random-effects models are
adopted for studies with heterogeneity (Cooper, Hedges, & Valentine, 2009). A
significant Q value represents the statistical heterogeneity between studies and
indicates the variation among the effect sizes. I2 reflects the percentage of total
variability due to heterogeneity (Higgins, Thompson, Deeks, & Altman, 2003).
As studies with significant findings are more likely to be published than those
with findings supporting the null hypothesis (Field & Gillett, 2010), publication bias
should be estimated in a meta-analysis. Three strategies were adopted to assess
publication bias in the current meta-analysis. Firstly, Duval and Tweedie’s (2000) trim
and fill method was used to assess the symmetry of the effect size distributions in the
funnel plot. This method estimates the number of missing studies according to the
most extreme results in the meta-analysis and recalculates the estimated effect size to
make the funnel plot more symmetrical. Publication bias exists when the estimated
effect size changes significantly after using the trim and fill method.
Secondly, the fail-safe N method was used to assess publication bias by
calculating the number of studies that would be needed to change the conclusion from
significant to non-significant (Vevea &Woods, 2005). If the fail-safe N is greater than
5k + 10 (k is the number of effect sizes), the result can be considered robust. Thirdly,
Begg’s adjusted rank test (Begg & Mazumdar, 1994) and Egger’s linear regression
test (Egger, Smith, Schneider, & Minder, 1997) were examined for each meta-
analysis. Begg’s adjusted rank test computes Kendall’s tau for the correlation between
14
5
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
the effect size and the sample size and Egger’s linear regression test assesses the
asymmetry of the funnel plots for every effect size. When the statistical p value is less
than .05, it indicates that there is publication bias in the meta-analysis. The metafor
package (Viechtbauer, 2010) was performed for overall effect sizes analyses in the R
environment.
2.4.2. Moderator analyses. Moderators were tested for in the correlations
between perfectionism and cognitive perseveration. As two types of moderators
(categorical and continuous) exist, we conducted corresponding analyses to examine
the moderators that accounted for the heterogeneity in the true effects. Subgroup
analyses were conducted for categorical moderators and meta-regressions were
conducted for continuous moderators (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein,
2009). Moderator analyses were also carried out in the metafor package.
2.4.3. Meta-analytic mediation model. To examine the mediating role of
cognitive perseveration between multidimensional perfectionism and distress, we
proposed four mediation models. In these models, two dimensions of perfectionism
(SOP and SPP) were the independent variables, rumination and worry were the
mediating variables, and distress was the dependent variable. Distress was a latent
variable loading from depression and anxiety as these are the most common distress
symptoms. Four correlation matrices were extracted to test the mediation models.
Two-stage structural equation modelling (TSSEM) was performed to test these
four mediation models (Cheung, 2015; Cheung & Chan, 2005; Cheung & Chan,
2009). TSSEM combines meta-analysis procedure and structural equation modelling.
15
6
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
In the first stage, the pooled correlation matrix is estimated and the homogeneity of
the correlation matrices is tested. In the second stage, the proposed model is tested by
using the fixed-effects model and the random-effects model according to the results of
the homogeneity test from the first stage. The path coefficients and the standard errors
of those path coefficients were calculated to conduct the Sobel test, the Aroian test,
and the Goodman test for the mediation tests. The metaSEM package (Cheung, 2015)
and the lavaan package (Rosseel, 2012) were used to test the mediation effect in the R
environment.
3. Results
3.1. Study characteristics
Fifteen articles were included in the current meta-analysis (see Table 1) and four
published articles reported seven samples for the effect sizes of the SOP-worry link
and the SPP-worry link. A total of 1,278 participants with a mean age of 22.18 (78%
female) were from one country (Canada). The 12 articles (two unpublished
dissertations and 10 published journals) reported 30 samples for the effect size of the
SOP-rumination link. A total of 5,339 participants with a mean age of 25.62 (70%
female) were from four countries (Canada, Spain, UK, and USA). Thirteen articles
(two unpublished dissertations and 11 published journals) reported 31 samples for the
effect size of the SPP-rumination link. A total of 5,387 participants with a mean age of
25.61 (70% female) were from five countries (Australia, Canada, Spain, the UK, and
the USA). The number of effect sizes for the SPP-rumination link was larger than that
16
7
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
for the SOP-rumination link because one article reported only one dimension of
perfectionism (SPP).
3.2. Overall estimated effect sizes
Each comparison of the two dimensions of perfectionism and the two forms of
cognitive perseveration were examined. Table 2 shows the overall mean effect sizes
(with corresponding confidence interval), the estimates of heterogeneity (Q value and
I2), and the results of the publication bias tests, i.e. results of Duval and Tweedie’s
(2000) trim and fill method, fail-safe N, and estimates of Begg’s adjusted rank test and
Egger’s linear regression test.
Insert Table 2 about here
3.2.1. SOP and worry. The overall mean correlation between SOP and worry
was significant: r = 0.33, with a 95% CI from 0.28 to 0.38, p < 0.001 under the fixed-
effects model. The results of the homogeneity tests showed existing homogeneity
across studies (Q = 5.00, p = 0.54, I2 = 0). There were no missing studies according to
Duval and Tweedie’s (2000) trim and fill method (ka = 0) and the overall mean
correlation remained the same (rb = 0.33, p < 0.001). The result of fail-safe N was 416,
which suggested that the result was robust. The results of Begg’s adjusted rank test
(Kendall's tau = -0.24, p = 0.56) and Egger’s linear regression test (z = -0.32, p =
0.75) were not significant, suggesting that there was no potential publication bias.
3.2.2. SPP and worry. The overall mean correlation between SPP and worry
was significant: r = 0.43, with a 95% CI from 0.38 to 0.47, p < 0.001 under the fixed-
effects model. The results of the homogeneity tests showed existing homogeneity
17
8
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
across studies (Q = 6.46, p = 0.54, I2 = 18%). There were no missing studies
according to Duval and Tweedie’s (2000) trim and fill method (ka = 0) and the overall
mean correlation remained the same (rb = 0.43, p < 0.001). The result of fail-safe N
was 878, which suggested that the result was robust. The results of Begg’s adjusted
rank test (Kendall's tau = -0.14, p = 0.77) and Egger’s linear regression test (z = -0.03,
p = 0.98) were not significant, suggesting that there was no potential publication bias.
3.2.3. SOP and rumination. The overall mean correlation between SOP and
rumination was significant: r = 0.20, with a 95% CI from 0.17 to 0.22, p < 0.001
under the fixed-effects model. The results of the homogeneity tests showed existing
homogeneity across studies (Q = 40.97, p = 0.07, I2 = 34%). Three studies were
imputed by Duval and Tweedie’s (2000) trim and fill method, but the interpretation of
the overall mean correlation did not change (rb = 0.19, p < 0.001). The result of fail-
safe N was 2,379, which suggested that the result was robust. The results of Begg’s
adjusted rank test (Kendall's tau = -0.01, p = 0.91) and Egger’s linear regression test
(z = 1.36, p = 0.17) were not significant, suggesting that there was no potential
publication bias.
3.2.4. SPP and rumination. The overall mean correlation between SPP and
rumination was significant: r = 0.32, with a 95% CI from 0.27 to 0.36, p < 0.001
under the random-effects model. The results of the homogeneity tests showed existing
heterogeneity across studies (Q = 97.56, p < 0.001, I2 = 69%). Nine studies were
imputed by Duval and Tweedie’s (2000) trim and fill method, but the interpretation of
the overall mean correlation did not change (rb = 0.27, p < 0.001). The result of fail-
18
9
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
safe N was 6,941, which suggested that the result was robust. The results of Begg’s
adjusted rank test (Kendall's tau = -0.02, p = 0.92) and Egger’s linear regression test
(z = 0.83, p = 0.40) were not significant, suggesting that there was no potential
publication bias.
3.3. Moderator analyses
Several moderator analyses were conducted to examine the moderation effects of
mean age, percentage of females, country of origin, publication type, and measures
using the mixed-effects model. A summary of coefficients (k, N, r, 95% CI, df, and
Qwithin) among categorical moderators is shown in Table 3. A summary of coefficients
(intercept, ß, SE, 95% CI for ß, and Z) among continuous moderators is shown in
Table 4. Publication type significantly moderated the correlation between SOP and
rumination (Qwithin = 7.36, p < 0.01) and country significantly moderated the
correlation between SPP and rumination (Qwithin = 21.11, p < 0.001).
Insert Table 3 about here
Insert Table 4 about here
3.4. Mediation analysis
To explore the mediating role of cognitive perseveration between perfectionism
and distress, corresponding correlation matrices were extracted (as mentioned in the
coding procedure). Four mediation models were proposed using TSSEM. In the first
stage, the correlations among variables were calculated using the same method as the
estimation of effect sizes. In addition, the homogeneity of the correlation matrices was
19
0
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
estimated by the value of fit indices, including the chi-square test (
2), degree of
freedom (df), the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA), the
standardised root mean square residual (SRMR), and the comparative fit index (CFI).
In the second stage, the fit indices of the mediation models were estimated to test
these four proposed models. To examine the significance of the indirect effects, raw
regression coefficients for the associations and their standard errors were calculated to
conduct the Sobel test, the Aroian test, and the Goodman test.
3.4.1. Mediating role of worry between SOP and distress. In the first stage, all
the correlations among variables were significant, indicating that it was appropriate to
conduct TSSEM. The model-fitting statistics (
2 = 7.26, df = 10, RMSEA = 0.00,
SRMR = 0.03, CFI = 1.00) indicated homogeneity of the correlation matrices,
therefore, the TSSEM analysis used a fixed-effects model in both stages. In the
second stage, the fit indices of the mediation model were
2 = 0.62, df = 1, RMSEA =
0.00, SRMR = 0.02, and CFI = 1.00, indicating that the proposed model fitted the data
very well. Fig. 2 shows the proposed mediation model and the corresponding
parameters. The results show that the indirect effect of SOP on distress via worry
(Sobel test = 7.77, p < 0.001; Aroian test = 7.76, p < 0.001; Goodman test = 7.79, p <
0.001) was significant, indicating that worry plays a mediating role in the relationship
between SOP and distress.
Insert Fig. 2 about here
20
1
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
Fig.2. Path model of the mediating role of worry in the relationship between SOP and
distress. N (sample size) = 1278.
3.4.2. Mediating role of worry between SPP and distress. In the first stage, all
the correlations among variables were significant, indicating that it was appropriate to
conduct TSSEM. The model-fitting statistics (
2 = 8.35, df = 10, RMSEA = 0.00,
SRMR = 0.04, CFI = 1.00) indicated homogeneity of the correlation matrices,
therefore, the TSSEM analysis used a fixed-effects model in both stages. In the
second stage, the fit indices of the mediation model were
2 = 1.35, df = 1, RMSEA =
0.02, SRMR = 0.02, and CFI = 1.00, indicating that the proposed model fitted the data
very well. Fig. 3 shows the proposed mediation model and the corresponding
parameters. The results show that the indirect effect of SPP on distress via worry
(Sobel test = 6.80, p < 0.001; Aroian test = 6.79, p < 0.001; Goodman test = 6.81, p <
0.001) was significant, indicating that worry plays a mediating role in the relationship
between SPP and distress.
Insert Fig. 3 about here
Fig.3. Path model of the mediating role of worry in the relationship between SPP and
distress. N (sample size) = 1278.
3.4.3. Mediating role of rumination between SOP and distress. In the first
stage, all the correlations among variables were significant, indicating that it was
21
2
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
appropriate to conduct TSSEM. The model-fitting statistics (
2 = 590.60, df = 125,
RMSEA = 0.15, SRMR = 0.12, CFI = 0.84) indicated heterogeneity of the correlation
matrices, therefore, the TSSEM analysis used a random-effects model in both stages.
In the second stage, the fit indices of the mediation model were
2 = 4.17, df = 1,
RMSEA = 0.02, SRMR = 0.04, and CFI = 1.00, indicating that the proposed model
fitted the data very well. Fig. 4 shows the proposed mediation model and the
corresponding parameters. The results show that the indirect effect of SOP on distress
via worry (Sobel test = 6.84, p < 0.001; Aroian test = 6.82, p < 0.001; Goodman test =
6.85, p < 0.001) was significant, indicating that rumination plays a mediating role in
the relationship between SOP and distress.
Insert Fig. 4 about here
Fig.4. Path model of the mediating role of rumination in the relationship between SOP
and distress. N (sample size) = 6285.
3.4.4. Mediating role of rumination between SPP and distress. In the first
stage, all the correlations among variables were significant, indicating that it was
appropriate to conduct TSSEM. The model-fitting statistics (
2 = 609.13, df = 128,
RMSEA = 0.15, SRMR = 0.12, CFI = 0.88) indicated heterogeneity of the correlation
matrices, therefore, the TSSEM analysis used a random-effects model in both stages.
In the second stage, the fit indices of the mediation model were
2 = 1.13, df = 1,
22
3
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
RMSEA = 0.00, SRMR = 0.02, and CFI = 1.00, indicating that the proposed model
fitted the data very well. Fig. 5 shows the proposed mediation model and the
corresponding parameters. The results show that the indirect effect of SPP on distress
via worry (Sobel test = 6.48, p < 0.001; Aroian test = 6.47, p < 0.001; Goodman test =
6.50, p < 0.001) was significant, indicating that rumination plays a mediating role in
the relationship between SPP and distress.
Insert Fig. 5 about here
Fig.5. Path model of the mediating role of rumination in the relationship between SOP
and distress. N (sample size) = 6333.
4. Discussion
The overall purpose of the present study was to examine the three themes of PCT
by conducting a meta-analysis. The specific aims were to evaluate the magnitude of
the correlations between perfectionism and perseverative cognition, to examine the
correlations between two dimensions of perfectionism and various forms of cognitive
perseveration, and to examine the mediating roles of cognitive perseveration between
multidimensional perfectionism and distress. Correspondingly, we first calculated the
overall estimated effect sizes of four comparable correlations between perfectionism
and cognitive perseveration. We then analysed two moderators, especially the
measurements of worry and rumination, and finally examined four mediation models
to test the roles of worry and rumination in the relationships between SOP and SPP
and psychological distress.
23
4
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
This study is the first meta-analytic review of the relationship between
perfectionism and cognitive perseveration and finds that both SOP and SPP are
positively correlated with worry and rumination. These results are consistent with the
previous qualitative summary (Flett et al., 2016) and provide further evidence to
support the first theme of PCT. The findings also suggest that according to Cohen’s
(1992) guidelines, SOP has a medium positive relationship with worry and a small-to-
medium positive relationship with rumination, while SPP has a medium-to-large
positive relationship with worry and a medium positive relationship with rumination.
As described above, both SOP and SPP capture the evaluation fears that activate
worry and rumination. Perfectionists tend to perceive constant pressure in their life,
focus on negative events, fear being criticised, and subsequently worry about the
future and ruminate about the past (Flett & Hewitt, 2002; Hewitt & Flett, 1996).
The present understanding of perfectionism dimensions indicates that
perfectionism involves two higher order dimensions: perfectionistic strivings (PS) and
perfectionistic concerns (PC) (see Stoeber & Otto, 2006). SOP as an indicator of PS is
regarded as an adaptive form of perfectionism correlated with positive characteristics,
whereas SPP as an indicator of PC is regarded as a maladaptive form of perfectionism
correlated with negative characteristics. The results of this study indicate that SOP
and SPP have a positive relationship with worry and rumination, however, which is
not consistent with this model of perfectionism. Some other models of perfectionism
also regard SOP as adaptive dimension of perfectionism (see Bieling, Israeli, &
Antony, 2004; Dunkley & Blankstein, 2000). Stoeber and Otto (2006) outlined that
24
5
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
PS and PC are typically correlated and they show considerable overlap. Consequently,
PS (an equivocal dimension) links to both adaptive and maladaptive outcomes.
To investigate this problem, the relationships between the dimensions of
perfectionism and the outcomes were examined in three reviews of perfectionism by
conducting both bivariate and partial correlations (i.e. controlling for correlations in
different dimensions of perfectionism) (Gotwals, Stoeber, Dunn, & Stoll, 2012; Hill &
Curran, 2015; Stoeber & Otto, 2006). To compare the partial correlation coefficients
with the bivariate correlation coefficients, researchers could understand correlations
between PS and PC. In the future, researchers may take the results of partial
correlations among dimensions of perfectionism into consideration to explore the
effects of the pure dimensions of perfectionism. Moreover, PCT states that other
elements of evaluative concerns perfectionism (concerns over mistakes and doubts)
are correlated with worry and rumination, therefore future studies can adopt various
measurements of perfectionism to examine the relationship between two higher order
dimensions (PS and PC) and cognitive perseveration. The Big Three Perfectionism
Scale (BTPS) developed by Smith and colleagues (2016) could provide an
understanding of the correlations between SOP and rumination and worry.
Specifically, the BTPS disentangles SOP from self-worth contingencies and this might
make sense of the link between the rigid insistence of perfectionists and rumination
and worry.
In terms of moderation, the current study examined two moderators: measures of
worry and rumination. The results of the subgroup analyses suggest that most
25
6
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
correlations between perfectionism and various forms of cognitive perseveration are
positively significant, although two correlations were not significant when studies
adopted CERQ as the rumination measure. The findings of this meta-analysis provide
evidence that partly supports the second theme of PCT. Some forms of cognitive
perseveration mentioned in PCT have not been examined in the current study,
however, because there is a lack of specific measurements for these types of cognitive
perseveration.
In the four comparisons of the correlation between perfectionism and cognitive
perseveration, only the correlation between SPP and rumination showed existing
heterogeneity across studies. Country may alter this relationship, indicating that the
link between SPP and rumination in English-speaking countries is stronger than in
Spain. This could be because Senra, Merino and Ferreiro (2017) adopted the Spanish
version of the MPS and the RRS in which some items of measurement might be
adjusted for Spanish, or because cultural differences impact this relationship. It is
evident that perfectionism and its correlates show cultural variations (e.g. Stoeber,
Kobori, & Tanno, 2013). Considering the cultural differences in the link between
perfectionism and cognitive perseveration, future studies could compare the influence
of Western and Eastern culture on this relationship.
In terms of mediation, the current study examined four mediation models and
found that worry and rumination have mediating roles in the relationship between
perfectionism and distress. The findings of this meta-analysis provide evidence to
support the third theme of PCT, which states that worry and rumination contribute to
26
7
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
the link between perfectionism and distress. The findings are partly inconsistent with
the previous research, however. For example, Short and Mazmanian (2013) found that
worry and rumination mediate the relationship between SPP and negative affect, but
that SOP was not significantly correlated with distress. In contrast to the findings of
Short and Mazmanian, some studies provide evidence that the link between SOP and
distress is significant (e.g. Hewitt & Flett, 1991; Olson & Kwon, 2008). This meta-
analysis synthesises several studies and finds a significantly positive relationship
between SOP and distress and a mediating role of rumination and worry in this
relationship. The results of the meta-analytic mediation model illuminate the
mechanisms of perfectionism cognition and provide a better understanding of the link
between perfectionism and distress.
To examine the mediating roles of worry and rumination in the link between
perfectionism and distress, longitudinal studies would be appropriate to test mediation
models and help understand causality. Olson and Kwon (2008) designed a four-week
longitudinal study to examine perfection, rumination, and depression, and found that
perfectionism interacted with rumination and stress and could predict depression.
More longitudinal studies like this are needed as they could help identify the
dynamics of the perfectionism-distress relationship and might clarify the cognitive
process over time.
In fact, the third theme of PCT proposes that cognitive perseveration not only
contributes to psychological distress, but also contributes to physical illness among
perfectionists. The cognitive perseveration hypothesis mentioned in the introduction
27
8
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
suggests that cognitive perseveration may negatively impact physical health by
prolonging the stress response (Brosschot, Gerin, & Thayer, 2006). Several studies
provide evidence that cognitive perseveration is correlated with poor physical well-
being, e.g. cardiovascular disease (Brosschot, Van, & Thayer, 2007; Chalmers,
Quintana, Abbott, & Kemp, 2014), an impoverished immune system (Thomsen et al.,
2004), and poor sleep quality (Zoccola, Dickerson, & Lam, 2009). To the best of our
knowledge, however, only one study has explored the link between perfectionism and
health symptoms from a cognitive perspective that highlights the role of
perfectionistic automatic thoughts (Flett, Molnar, Nepon, & Hewitt, 2012). Future
studies could explore the contributory roles of worry and rumination in the
relationship between perfectionism and physical illness.
It should be noted that this meta-analysis has some limitations. Firstly, all the
studies included examined perfectionism using the MPS-HF (Hewitt & Flett, 1991),
because PCT focuses on SOP and SPP. PCT also states that other elements of
evaluative concerns perfectionism, such as concern over mistakes (COM) and doubts
about actions (DAA), are correlated with worry and rumination, but the current study
did not examine other dimensions of perfectionism. Future studies could examine
other dimensions of perfectionism by using several different measurements. Secondly,
all the studies included in this meta-analysis were done with non-clinical sample.
When researchers try to study perfectionism in clinical samples, they prefer to use the
MPS-F because of it’s good psychometric properties (Frost & DiBartolo, 2002).
Theoretically, the PCT could provide an explanation for both clinical and non-clinical
28
9
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
samples, so future studies could examine the PCT in clinical samples. Thirdly,
although the number of studies in this area is growing, the literature is relatively small
and only 15 articles met the inclusion criteria for this meta-analysis. Lastly, the
mediation analyses required correlation matrices for perfectionism, worry, rumination,
depression, and anxiety, but some of the included research used more than one
measurement to evaluate a variable. This would lead to a large inflation of effect
sizes, which should be taken into consideration when interpreting the results.
To conclude, the current study provides evidence for the three themes of PCT.
Firstly, it was found that both SOP and SPP were positively correlated with worry and
rumination. Specifically, SOP had medium and small-to-medium positive
relationships with worry and rumination, respectively, while SPP had medium-to-
large and medium positive relationships with worry and rumination, respectively.
Secondly, there was evidence that both SOP and SPP were correlated with various
forms of cognitive perseveration. Thirdly, the mediation results suggested that worry
and rumination have mediating roles in the relationship between perfectionism and
distress. In other words, cognitive perseveration contributes to mental health
outcomes among perfectionists. Overall, the findings of this meta-analysis support the
main arguments of PCT and shed light on the mechanisms and processes of
perfectionism cognition.
29
0
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
References
*Reference marked with asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-analysis
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York:
International Universities Press.
Begg, C. B., & Mazumdar, M. (1994). Operating characteristics of a rank correlation
test for publication bias. Biometrics, 1088-1101. doi:10.2307/2533446
Bieling, P. J., Israeli, A. L., & Antony, M. M. (2004). Is perfectionism good, bad, or
both? Examining models of the perfectionism construct. Personality and
Individual Differences, 36, 1373–1385. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00235-6
*Blankstein, K. R., & Lumley, C. H. (2008). Multidimensional perfectionism and
ruminative brooding in current dysphoria, anxiety, worry, and anger. Journal of
Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 26(3), 168-193. doi:
10.1007/s10942-007-0068-z
Blankstein, K. R., Lumley, C. H., & Crawford, A. (2007). Perfectionism,
hopelessness, and suicide ideation: revisions to diathesis-stress and specific
vulnerability models. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior
Therapy, 25(4), 279-319. doi: 10.1007/s10942-007-0053-6
Borenstein M, Hedges L. V, Higgins J. P. T, Rothstein H. R. (2009). Introduction to
Meta-analysis. Chichester, UK: John Wiley.
Borkovec, T. D., Ray, W. J., & Stober, J. (1998). Worry: a cognitive phenomenon
intimately linked to affective, physiological, and interpersonal behavioral
processes. Cognitive Therapy & Research, 22(6), 561-576. doi:
10.1023/A:1018790003416
30
1
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
Borkovec, T. D., Robinson, E., Pruzinsky, T., & Depree, J. A. (1983). Preliminary
exploration of worry: some characteristics and processes. Behaviour Research
and Therapy, 21(1), 9-16. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(83)90121-3
Brosschot, J. F, Gerin, W., & Thayer, J. F. (2006). The preservative cognition
hypothesis: A review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation,
and health. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 60, 113–124. doi
10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.06.074
Brosschot, J. F., Van, D. E., & Thayer, J. F. (2007). Daily worry is related to low heart
rate variability during waking and the subsequent nocturnal sleep period.
International Journal of Psychophysiology Official Journal of the International
Organization of Psychophysiology, 63(1), 39-47. doi:
10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.07.016
Buhr, K., & Dugas, M. J. (2002). The Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale: psychometric
properties of the English version. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 931–
945. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00092-4
*Buhr, K., & Dugas, M. J. (2006). Investigating the construct validity of intolerance of
uncertainty and its unique relationship with worry. Journal Of Anxiety Disorders,
20(2), 222-236. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2004.12.004
Burns, D. (1980). The perfectionist’s script for self-defeat. Psychology Today, 34–51.
Chalmers, J. A., Quintana, D. S., Abbott, M. J. A., & Kemp, A. H. (2014). Anxiety
disorders are associated with reduced heart rate variability: a meta-analysis.
Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5(80), 80. doi; 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00080
31
2
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
Cheung, M. W. L. (2015). metaSEM: An R package for meta-analysis using structural
equation modeling. Frontiers in Psychology, 5:1521. doi:
10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01521
Cheung, M. W. L., & Chan, W. (2005). Meta-analytic structural equation modeling: a
two-stage approach. Psychological methods, 10(1), 40. doi: 10.1037/1082-
989X.10.1.40
Cheung, M. W. L., & Chan, W. (2009). A two-stage approach to synthesizing
covariance matrices in meta-analytic structural equation modeling. Structural
Equation Modeling, 16(1), 28-53. doi: 10.1080/10705510802561295
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159. doi:
10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155
Cooper, H., Hedges, L. V., & Valentine, J. C. (2009). The handbook of research
synthesis and meta-analysis (2nd ed.). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
*Cox, S. L., & Chen, J. (2015). Perfectionism: A contributor to social anxiety and its
cognitive processes. Australian Journal of Psychology, 67(4), 231-240. doi:
10.1111/ajpy.12079
Davey, G. C. L. (1994). Worrying, social problem-solving abilities, and social
problem-solving confidence. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32(3), 327. doi:
10.1016/0005-7967(94)90130-9
Dunkley, D. M., & Blankstein, K. R. (2000). Self-critical perfectionism, coping,
hassles, and current distress: A structural equation modeling approach. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 24(6), 713–730. doi: 10.1023/A:1005543529245
32
3
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
Duval, S., & Tweedie, R. (2000). A nonparametric “trim and fill” method of
accounting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Journal of the American
Statistical Association, 95(449), 89-98. doi: 10.1080/01621459.2000.10473905
Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis
detected by a simple, graphical test. Bmj, 315(7109), 629-634. doi:
10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629
Ellis, A. (1962). Reason and emotion in psychotherapy. New York: Lyle Stuart.
Field AP, & Gillett R. (2011). How to do a meta-analysis. British Journal of
Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 63(3), 665-694. doi:
10.1348/000711010X502733
Field, A. P. (2001). Meta-analysis of correlation coefficients: a Monte Carlo
comparison of fixed- and random-effects methods. Psychological Methods, 6(2),
161–180. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.6.2.161
Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2002). Perfectionism and maladjustment: An overview of
theoretical, definitional, and treatment issues. In G. L. Flett & P. L. Hewitt
(Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory, research, and treatment. Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
*Flett, G. L., Madorsky, D., Hewitt, P. L., & Heisel, M. J. (2002). Perfectionism
cognitions, rumination, and psychological distress. Journal of Rational-Emotive
and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 20(1), 33-47. doi: 10.1023/A:1015128904007
Flett, G. L., Molnar, D. S., Nepon, T., & Hewitt, P. L. (2012). A mediational model of
perfectionistic automatic thoughts and psychosomatic symptoms: The roles of
33
4
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
negative affect and daily hassles. Personality and Individual Differences, 52,
565–570. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.09.010
Flett, G. L., Nepon, T., & Hewitt, P. L. (2016). Perfectionism, Worry, and Rumination
in Health and Mental Health: A Review and a Conceptual Framework for a
Cognitive Theory of Perfectionism. Perfectionism, Health, and Well-Being.
Springer International Publishing.
*Fracalanza, K., Koerner, N., Deschenes, S. S., & Dugas, M. J. (2014). Intolerance of
uncertainty mediates the relation between generalized anxiety disorder symptoms
and anger. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 43(2), 122-132. doi:
10.1080/16506073.2014.888754
Frost, R. O., & DiBartolo, P. (2002). Perfectionism, anxiety and obsessive compulsive
disorder. In G. Flett & P. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfectionism: Theory, research, and
treatment. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press.
Frost, R. O., Heimberg, R. G., Holt, C. S., Mattia, J. I., & Neubauer, A. L. (1993). A
comparison of two measures of perfectionism. Personality and Individual
Differences, 14, 119–126. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(93)90181-2
Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimensions of
perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14(5), 449-468. doi:
10.1007/BF01172967
Garner, D., Olmstead, M., & Polivy, J. (1983). Development and validation of a
multidimensional eating disorder inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia.
34
5
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
International Journal of Eating Disorders, 2, 15–34. doi: 10.1002/1098-
108X(198321)2:2%3C15::AID-EAT2260020203%3E3.0.CO;2-6
Glass, G. V. (1976). Primary, secondary, and meta-analysis of research. Educational
Researcher, 5(10), 3-8. doi: 10.3102/0013189X005010003
Gotwals, J. K., Stoeber, J., Dunn, J. G. H., & Stoll, O. (2012). Are perfectionistic
strivings in sport adaptive? A systematic review of confirmatory, contradictory,
and mixed evidence. Canadian Psychology, 53(53), 263-279. doi:
10.1037/a0030288
Hafdahl, A. R. (2009). Improved Fisher z estimators for univariate random-effects
meta-analysis of correlations. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical
Psychology, 62, 233–261. doi: 10.1348/000711008X281633
Hamachek, D. E. (1978). Psychodynamics of normal and neurotic perfectionism.
Psychology, 15, 27–33.
Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. New
Directions for Program Evaluation, 1984(24), 25-42. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-08-
057065-5.50022-1
Hewitt PL, & Flett GL. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social contexts:
conceptualization, assessment, and association with psychopathology. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 60(3), 456-70. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.60.3.456
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1996). Personality traits and the coping process. In M.
Zeidner & N. S. Endler (Eds.), Handbook of coping: Theory, research,
applications. Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons.
35
6
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (2004). Multidimensional perfectionism scale (MPS):
Technical manual. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (2007). When does conscientiousness become
perfectionism? Traits, self-presentation styles, and cognitions suggest a persistent
psychopathology. Current Psychiatry, 6(7), 49-56.
Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring
inconsistency in meta-analyses. Bmj, 327(7414), 557-560. doi:
10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557
Higgins, E. T., & Carver, C. S. (1990). Principles of self-regulation. New York:
Guilford.
Hill, A. P., & Curran, T. (2015). Multidimensional Perfectionism and Burnout: A
Meta-Analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20(3), 269-288. doi:
10.1177/1088868315596286
Hill, R. W., Huelsman, T. J., Furr, M., Kibler, J., Vicente, B. B., & Kennedy, C.
(2004). A new measure of perfectionism: The Perfectionism Inventory. Journal
of Personality Assessment, 82, 80–91. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa8201_13
Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Methods of meta-analysis: correcting error
and bias in research findings. Newbury Park: Sage.
Just, N., & Alloy, L. B. (1997). The response styles theory of depression. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology, 106. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.106.2.221
Klibert, J. J., Langhinrichsen-Rohling, J., & Saito, M. (2005). Adaptive and
maladaptive aspects of selforiented versus socially prescribed perfectionism.
36
7
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
Journal of College Student Development, 46, 141–156. Doi:
10.1353/csd.2005.0017
*Laderoute, K. T. (2008). Perfectionism as a vulnerability factor for psychological
distress: Exploring the roles of self-consciousness and perceived discrepancies.
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, York University.
Liberati, A., Altman, D. G., Tetzlaff, J., Mulrow, C., Gøtzsche, P. C., & Ioannidis, J. P.
A., et al. (2009). The prisma statement for reporting systematic reviews and
meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and
elaboration. Epidemiology Biostatistics and Public Health, 6(4), e1-e34. doi:
10.1371/journal.pmed.1000100
Lyubomirsky, S., & Tkach, C. (2004). The consequences of dysphoric rumination. In
C. Papageorgiou & A. Wells (Eds.), Depressive rumination: Nature, theory, and
treatment. New York: Wiley.
Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1996). Clarifying our thoughts. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.),
Advances in social cognition ruminative thoughts. Hillsdale, NJ, US: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Metzger, R. L., Miller, M. L., Cohen, M., Sofka, M., & Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Worry
changes decision making: the effect of negative thoughts on cognitive
processing. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46(1), 78-88. doi: 10.1002/1097-
4679(199001)46:1<78::AID-JCLP2270460113>3.0.CO;2-R
Mills, J. S., & Blankstein, K. R. (2000). Perfectionism, intrinsic vs extrinsic
motivation, and motivated strategies for learning: a multidimensional analysis of
37
8
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
university students. Personality and Individual Differences, 29(6), 1191-1204.
doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00003-9
Missildine, W. H. (1963). Your inner child of the past. New York: Simon and Schuster.
*Nepon, T., Flett, G. L., Hewitt, P. L., & Molnar, D. S. (2011). Perfectionism, negative
social feedback, and interpersonal rumination in depression and social anxiety.
Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science / Revue canadienne des sciences du
comportement, 43(4), 297-308. doi: 10.1037/a0025032
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1987). Sex differences in unipolar depression: evidence and
theory. Psychological Bulletin, 101(2), 259. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.259
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2000). The role of rumination in depressive disorders and
mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(3),
504-511. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.3.504
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and
distress following a natural disaster: the 1989 loma prieta earthquake. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 105-121. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.61.1.115
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Girgus, J. S., & Seligman, M. E. (1991). Sex differences in
depression and explanatory style in children. Journal of Youth & Adolescence,
20(2), 233-245. doi: 10.1007/BF01537610
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Morrow, J., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1993). Response styles and
the duration of episodes of depressed mood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
102(1), 20-28. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.102.1.20
38
9
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
Nolenhoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking
rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science A Journal of the Association
for Psychological Science, 3(5), 400-424. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-
6924.2008.00088.x
*O'Connor, D. B., O'Connor, R. C., & Marshall, R. (2007). Perfectionism and
psychological distress: Evidence of the mediating effects of rumination.
European Journal of Personality, 21(4), 429-452. doi: 10.1002/per.616
*Olson, M. L., & Kwon, P. (2008). Brooding perfectionism: Refining the roles of
rumination and perfectionism in the etiology of depression. Cognitive Therapy
And Research, 32(6), 788-802. doi: 10.1007/s10608-007-9173-7
Papageorgiou, C., & Siegle, G. J. (2003). Rumination and depression: advances in
theory and research. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(3), 243-245. doi:
10.1023/A:1023918331490
*Randles, D., Flett, G. L., Nash, K. A., McGregor, I. D., & Hewitt, P. L. (2010).
Dimensions of perfectionism, behavioral inhibition, and rumination. Personality
and Individual Differences, 49(2), 83-87. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.002
Rhéaume, J., Ladouceur, R., & Freeston, M. H. (2000). The prediction of obsessive–
compulsive tendencies: Does perfectionism play a significant role? Personality
and Individual Differences, 28, 583–592. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00121-X
Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of
Statistical Software, 48(2), 1-36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02
39
0
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
*Rudolph, S. G., Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2007). Perfectionism and deficits in
cognitive emotion regulation. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-
Behavior Therapy, 25(4), 343-357. doi: 10.1007/s10942-007-0056-3
*Rudowski, M. (2010). Exploring the effects of brooding rumination as a mediator in
the relationship between positive and negative perfectionism and depressive
symptoms in a clinical population. Unpublished bachelor thesis, University of
Michigan
*Senra, C., Merino, H., & Ferreiro, F. (2017). Exploring the link between
perfectionism and depressive symptoms: contribution of rumination and defense
styles. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 74(3). doi: 10.1002/jclp.22571
*Short, M. M., & Mazmanian, D. (2013). Perfectionism and negative repetitive
thoughts: Examining a multiple mediator model in relation to mindfulness.
Personality and Individual Differences, 55(6), 716-721. doi:
10.1016/j.paid.2013.05.026
Slaney, R. B., Rice, K. G., Mobley, M., Trippi, J., & Ashby, J. S. (2001). The revised
almost perfect scale. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and
Development, 34(3), 130–145.
Smith, M. M., Saklofske, D. H., Stoeber, J., & Sherry, S. B. (2016). The big three
perfectionism scale: a new measure of perfectionism. Journal of
Psychoeducational Assessment, 34(7), 670-687. doi:
10.1177/0734282916651539
40
1
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
Spasojević, J., & Alloy, L. B. (2001). Rumination as a common mechanism relating
depressive risk factors to depression. Emotion, 1(1), 25-37. DOI: 10.1037/1528-
3542.1.1.25
Stoeber, J., & Otto, K. (2006). Positive conceptions of perfectionism: approaches,
evidence, challenges. Personality and Social Psychology Review An Official
Journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology Inc, 10(4), 295.
doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1004_2
Stoeber, J., Kobori, O., & Tanno, Y. (2013). Perfectionism and self‐conscious
emotions in British and Japanese students: predicting pride and embarrassment
after success and failure. European Journal of Personality, 27(1), 59-70. doi:
10.1002/per.1858
Thomsen, D. K., Mehlsen, M. Y., Hokland, M., Viidik, A., Olesen, F., & Avlund, K.,
et al. (2004). Negative thoughts and health: associations among rumination,
immunity, and health care utilization in a young and elderly sample.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 66(3), 363. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200405000-00012
Trapnell, P. D., & Campbell, J. D. (1999). Private self-consciousness and the five-
factor model of personality: distinguishing rumination from reflection. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(2), 284-304. doi: 10.1037/0022-
3514.76.2.284
Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered:
A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259. doi:
10.1023/A:1023910315561
41
2
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
Vevea, J. L., & Woods, C. M. (2005). Publication bias in research synthesis:
sensitivity analysis using a priori weight functions. Psychological Methods,
10(4), 428. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.10.4.428
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting Meta-Analyses in R with the metafor Package.
Journal of Statistical Software, 36(3). doi: 10.18637/jss.v036.i03
Wade, N. G., Vogel, D. L., Liao, Y. H., & Goldman, D. B. (2008). Measuring state-
specific rumination: development of the rumination about an interpersonal
offense scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 55(3), 419-426. doi:
10.1037/0022-0167.55.3.419
Wänke, M., & Schmid, J. (1996). Rumination: When all else fails. In R. S. Wyer, Jr.
(Ed.), Advances in social cognition ruminative thoughts. Hillsdale, NJ, US:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
Watkins, E., Moulds, M., & Mackintosh, B. (2005). Comparisons between rumination
and worry in a non-clinical population. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 43(12),
1577-1585. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.11.008
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1984). Cross-cultural convergence in the
structure of mood: A Japanese replication and a comparison with U.S. findings.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(47), 127-144. doi:
10.1037/0022-3514.47.1.127
Whitmer, A. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2013). An attentional scope model of rumination.
Psychological Bulletin, 139(5), 1036-1061. doi: 10.1037/a0030923
42
3
PERFECTIONISM, WORRY, RUMINATION, AND DISTRESS
*Wilson, C., Hunter, S. C., Rasmussen, S., & McGowan, A. (2015). They made you
perfect: A test of the Social Reaction Model of Perfectionism. Aggressive
Behavior, 45(1), 421-431. doi: 10.1002/ab.21572
Yu Xie, Jiyu Yang, & Faxiang Chen. (2018). Multidimensional Perfectionism and
Procrastination: A Meta-Analysis of Main, Moderator, and Mediator Effects.
Social Behavior and Personality, 46(3), 395-408. doi: 10.2224/sbp.6680
Zoccola, P. M., Dickerson, S. S., & Lam, S. (2009). Rumination predicts longer sleep
onset latency after an acute psychosocial stressor. Psychosomatic Medicine,
71(7), 771-775. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181ae58e8
43
... However, it is important to differentiate between maladaptive (i.e., concerns about mistakes and criticism) and adaptive perfectionism (i.e., being focused on organisation and aiming for higher achievement; Frost et al., 1993). Maladaptive perfectionists experience more frequent negative emotional experiences and tend to use primarily maladaptive emotion regulation strategies compared to adaptive perfectionists (Molnar et al., 2006;Vois & Damian, 2020;Xie et al., 2019). Flett et al. (2021) identify several reasons why perfectionists struggle with mindfulness: activity-based type of selfworth, ruminating on past or future mistakes, difficulty expressing negative emotions, low self-acceptance, and heightened stress reactivity. ...
... Previous research indicated that perfectionistic self-presentation is associated with reduced levels of mindfulness (Flett et al., 2021). Maladaptive perfectionists, in particular, struggle with emotion regulation and engaging in mindfulness-based practices (Flett et al., 2021;Miley et al., 2022;Vois & Damian, 2020;Xie et al., 2019). ...
Article
The present study explored the associations between orthorexia nervosa, social media addiction, emotion regulation difficulties, perfectionism and BMI with four facets of mindful eating, using path analysis to assess these relationships. A sample of 551 students (127 males, 424 females, mean age = 22.6 years) completed an online self‐report questionnaire evaluating these constructs. Analyses revealed that mindful eating contributed to emotion dysregulation and orthorexia nervosa and was related to social media addiction. Significant indirect paths were identified from mindful eating, social media addiction and adaptive perfectionism through emotion dysregulation to maladaptive perfectionism. While no direct paths were observed between mindful eating and perfectionism, a direct path was found between adaptive perfectionism and orthorexia nervosa. These findings suggest a more nuanced understanding of eating behaviours is required. Individuals susceptible to eating disorders should approach mindful eating with caution and seek support from healthcare providers to ensure it is used in a way that supports overall wellbeing. Future research should aim to replicate and further clarify these associations to reveal the long‐term effects of mindful eating.
... Rumination is also associated with perfectionism (DiBartolo et al., 2001;Flett et al., 2002) particularly with PC (Frost et al., 1997;Xie et al., 2019). Perfectionistic individuals are characterized by an excessive tendency toward evaluation and criticism (Di Schiena et al., 2012;Patterson et al., 2021) and by low decision-making skills (Frost & Shows, 1993). ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Identifying factors that contribute to the development of suicidal ideation (SI) is crucial for prevention and treatment. Perfectionistic Concerns (PC) represent the experience of external pressure to be perfect and have been consistently linked to SI, but it is unclear how PC and SI are associated in depressed adults. This study examined the association between PC and SI and whether self-esteem, loneliness and rumination (brooding, reflection and depression-related rumination) moderated this association in a clinical sample. Methods This cross-sectional study used a psychiatric outpatient sample of depressed adults, selected for the presence of SI. In total 110 adults (mean age 33.54 (SD = 12.03) and 60% female) participated in this study. Associations were estimated by regression analyses. Results PC was not associated with SI in the sample (r = .16, p = .087). However, high levels of PC in combination with low self-esteem (β = -.29, p = .007) or with high levels of depression-related rumination (β = .22, p = .017) were positively associated with SI. Loneliness, brooding and reflection did not moderate the PC-SI relationship. Discussion These findings underline the importance of taking into account the individual characteristics in depressed, perfectionistic individuals when estimating SI severity and selecting the focus of intervention. Due to the cross-sectional design no causal inferences can be made.
... Perfectionism is a multifaceted personality trait characterized by setting exceedingly high standards for performance, accompanied by critical self-evaluations and concerns over mistakes and others' evaluations [35]. Given its consistent association with negative repetitive thinking and psychological distress [36], perfectionism may contribute to variations in how individuals respond psychologically to the climate crisis. ...
Article
Full-text available
Psychological reactions to the climate change (CC) crisis, encompassing worry, distress, and impairment, are influenced by personality. Also, these psychological reactions to CC and future anxiety can impact an individual’s overall psychological distress. The role of perfectionism in psychological distress through CC reactions and future anxiety (FA) has not yet been explored. Our aim was to explore the mediator roles of CC worry, CC distress, CC impairment, and FA in the link between perfectionism and general psychological distress while controlling for the effect of gender. A path analysis was conducted on a sample of 577 adults (mean age = 32.62 ± 14.71) from the general population (64.5% women). The final model with all statistically significant path coefficients (p ≤ 0.01) presented an excellent model fit: χ2(6) = 1.56, p = 0.152; CFI = 0.995; GFI = 0.995; TLI = 0.986; RMSEA = 0.031, CI = 0.000–0.068, p = 0.764. Perfectionism was associated with general PD through several indirect pathways, including CC worry, CC distress, CC impairment, and FA, while controlling for the effect of gender. The model explained 3.89% of CC worry, 39.70% of CC distress, 20.64% of CC impairment, 23.73% of FA, and 20.20% of general PD. These findings suggest that cognitive and emotional responses to CC and future anxiety are exacerbated with higher perfectionism levels, which leads to higher overall psychological distress. These data underscore that individuals with high levels of perfectionism may require help to psychologically adapt to CC. Interventions aimed at lowering the maladaptive cognitive and emotional responses to climate threats should be implemented.
... Rumination's distinction in apprehend psychological unease is further indicate by its relation to the hike of anxiety escort on by outermost stresses (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000). It's compulsive to note that new research specify rumination may alleviate the connection between anxiety and particular tech-related behaviors, like irresistible social media use and smartphone dependence, indicating a composite interplay between rumination and modern stressors (Xie et al., 2019); (Zhang et al., 2018). According Maslach et al. (2001), rumination has been associate to the emergence of job exhausted in the setting of workplace fluctuation. ...
Article
Pakistan's banking sector is at the forefront of digital transformation, but this progress comes with a hidden cost-techno-stress. Our study, designed with your professional needs, explores how information overload, work-life intrusion, and other tech-related anxieties impact employee well-being and commitment. We examine the prevalence of techno-stress and its links to negative thinking, emotional detachment, and career satisfaction. We aim to develop practical strategies to reduce techno-stress, fostering employee well-being and organizational effectiveness. Using surveys, we analyze how different forms of rumination affect mental health and productivity. We propose interventions to boost employee satisfaction and organizational efficiency by minimizing negative tech stressors and promoting healthy detachment. We analyze the complex relationships between techno-stress and related factors through rigorous methodology. This research underscores the need for context-specific solutions in Pakistan's banking sector, offering valuable insights and actionable recommendations to cultivate a supportive work environment that thrives alongside technological advancements.
... Echoing theories, maladaptive perfectionism has been consistently found to be related to psychological distress. Cross-sectional studies showed that it is related to stress (Einstein et al., 2001), depression (Dunkley et al., 2000;Minarik & Ahrens, 1996;Sherry et al., 2014;Wang et al., 2009, Xie et al., 2019), anxiety (Anthony et al., 1998Coles et al., 2003;Dunkley et al., 2000;Einstein et al., 2001), and general distress (Kong et al., 2021). Longitudinal studies also revealed that maladaptive perfectionism predicted increases in depressive symptoms in non-clinical samples (Smith et al., 2017), and the persistence of depressive symptoms in participants with depression (Enns & Cox, 2005;Hewitt et al., 1996). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Perfectionism as a personality trait can be seen as having both adaptive and maladaptive dimensions. Nevertheless, their relationships with psychological distress remain mixed in the current literature. Previous studies were also limited by the use of impure measurements and the failure to statistically control for the effects of the other dimension. By addressing these major limitations and exploring the mediating and moderating roles of self-esteem and self- compassion, the current study provides an in-depth examination of the relationships between bidimensional perfectionism and psychological distress. In a community sample of 503 adults, results supported a bidimensional view of perfectionism, with maladaptive perfectionism positively predicting psychological distress and adaptive perfectionism being unrelated to psychological distress. Self-esteem was found to mediate the relationships between both dimensions of perfectionism and psychological distress. Self-compassion was only found to moderate the relationship between maladaptive perfectionism and self-esteem. Whilst the maladaptive nature of maladaptive perfectionism was supported in this study, findings suggested that adaptive perfectionism remains a more complicated construct. Future studies should aim at clarifying the nature and psychological outcomes of adaptive perfectionism.
... In this study, we focus on quantitative job insecurity, because short-time work is used when employees may otherwise be laid off (i.e., the job as such is at risk). We operationalize job insecurity in terms of employees' worries about the future of their job, without distinguishing between cognitive and affective facets (by definition, worrying is a cognitive process closely intertwined with negative affect; Borkovec et al., 1998;Xie et al., 2019). This approach is supported by empirical evidence: While cognitive and affective job insecurity may be distinguished from one another, both tend to correlate negatively with wellbeing, and positively to one another (Jiang & Lavaysse, 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Many countries rely on short-time work to prevent mass layoffs in economic crises. Despite serving to protect jobs, short-time work may trigger job insecurity perceptions, which may impair employee well-being. Moreover, past experiences of unemployment may increase susceptibility to job insecurity in response to short-time work. Drawing on Conservation of Resources (COR) Theory, Appraisal Theory and temporal stressor-strain models, this study investigates effects of short-time work on the development of job satisfaction, life satisfaction and affective well-being via perceived job insecurity across 6 years, considering previous unemployment as a moderator. We used propensity score matching to account for selectivity into short-time work and calculated latent growth curve models with N = 1211 employees in Germany (619 affected by short-time work, 592 controls). Short-time work predicted higher levels and an immediate increase in job insecurity, followed by a decrease over time. Both levels and changes in job insecurity were associated with levels and changes in well-being. Indirect effects of short-time work on well-being via job insecurity persisted 2 years after short-time work. We found no difference between previously unemployed respondents and others in their reactions to short-time work. The findings support COR theory and a prolonged stress-reaction model, showing lingering effects on well-being via job insecurity even after short-time work ends. The study supports short-time work as an antecedent of job insecurity and reveals temporal dynamics between job insecurity, its antecedents and outcomes over time. When implementing short-time work, employers should aim to mitigate concerns about job security to protect employee well-being.
... Both SOP and SPP are linked to cognitive perseveration [19]. Another study supports the idea of perfectionism cognition theory that perfectionists are more likely to suffer from interrelated forms of cognitive perseveration, and they may experience emotional distress due to the positive interactions between SOP and SPP with worry and rumination [20]. Our study found a positive correlation between perfectionism and ruminative thoughts in the rhinoplasty group. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective This study investigates differences in personality characteristics, including perfectionism, ruminative thinking style, and self-compassion, between individuals who have undergone rhinoplasty and a control group without any history of cosmetic surgery. Methods The study included 33 adult patients who underwent rhinoplasty between 2021 and 2023 at Bursa Uludağ University Faculty of Medicine Hospital and 33 adult patients who visited our centre for other complaints as a control group. The rhinoplasty group consisted of primary surgical patients with functional and cosmetic concerns, excluding those who sought revision surgery or had only functional problems. The control group consisted of individuals with no prior cosmetic surgery history and no expectations of aesthetic interventions. Psychiatric analysis was performed using Frost multidimensional perfectionism scale, ruminative thinking style questionnaire, and self-compassion scale. Results This research revealed that individuals who had rhinoplasty scored higher in perfectionism 109.3 (±23.3) and ruminative thinking 87.9 (±22) compared to those who did not undergo surgery 94.15 (±22.2) and 77.7 (±23), respectively. Additionally, the rhinoplasty group had lower self-compassion scores, 80.4 (±17.3), than the control group, 86.1 (±11.2). Statistically significant differences were observed in perfectionism between the groups ( p = 0.009). In rhinoplasty patients, a notably positive correlation was found between perfectionism and ruminative thinking scores ( r = 0.482; p = 0.005), while a moderately significant negative correlation was observed between self-compassion and ruminative thinking scores ( r = − 0.465; p = 0.006). Conclusion Individuals who undergo rhinoplasty generally show increased levels of perfectionism and are more prone to ruminative thinking. They also demonstrate reduced self-compassion compared to non-surgical control groups. Cosmetic surgeons should be aware of these psychological trends and consider using appropriate scales during pre-surgery consultations and follow-up visits. Adopting this informed approach can improve the surgeon–patient relationship and help overcome communication challenges. Level of Evidence III This journal requires that authors assign a level of evidence to each article. For a full description of these Evidence-Based Medicine ratings, please refer to the Table of Contents or the online Instructions to Authors www.springer.com/00266 .
Article
Full-text available
p style="text-align: justify;">This paper reviews the experiences of researchers from Western and Eastern countries, including the USA, Germany, Canada, the United Kingdom, China, and others, in studying internalized disorders in children and adolescents. The diversity of countries with different public health systems in the review broadened the perspective on a diagnostic paradigm based on the transdiagnostic model of mental disorder classification. The paper examines various approaches to conceptualizing internalizing disorders in overseas research. It introduces the concept of "internalized disorders" and contrasts it with "externalized disorders", with a focus on the symptoms, diagnosis, and interventions for internalized disorders in childhood and adolescence. It also presents data on the epidemiology and aetiology of internalized disorders, analyzing their prevalence in populations, which vary by age, gender, and region of residence. The paper also includes a discussion on the comorbidity of internalized disorders in children and adolescents. This scope of information provides a comprehensive understanding of the essence of "internalized disorders" and their key characteristics, creating a foundation for developing effective diagnostic and therapeutic strategies in national psychological practice.</p
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study is to examine the role of mediators including rumination, empathy and forgiveness in the relationship between social goals and school adjustment. The research sample consisted of 404 first-year high school boys who were selected by multi-stage cluster sampling and answered to social achievement goals (Ryan & Shim, 2008), Rumination about an Interpersonal Offense (Wade, Vogel & Goldman,2008),Empathy(Bar-Ann,1997), Trait Forgivingness (Berry, Worthington, O'Connor, Parrott &Wade,2005) and Adjustment Inventory For High School (Sinha and Singh, 1993). In this study, descriptive method and correlational research design of path analysis type were used. The results showed that the assumed pattern of Intermediate rumination, empathy and forgiveness related to social achievement goals, including social development, social demonstration approach, and social demonstration avoid to emotional, social, educational adjustment has good fitness. In addition, all correlational pathways (except for Social development and emotional adjustment, social demonstration approach and academic adjustment, social demonstration avoid and socio-academic adjustment) were statistically significant. The results of the present study show that the goals of social progress through predicting the increase of empathy and forgiveness and reduction of rumination have an important role in the occurrence of emotional, social and educational adjustment of students.
Preprint
Youth perfectionism levels have increased significantly over the last decades. Given the dominance of picture-perfect content, social media are often designated as contributors of this rise. Accordingly, this study examined how exposure to positive social media content might increase adolescents’ perfectionistic traits and vice versa. Moreover, it was explored whether upward social comparison and adolescents’ sociocultural context (on a micro and macro level) play, respectively, a mediating and moderating role. A three-wave panel study was conducted among 1697 Belgian, French, and Slovenian adolescents (Mage=15.14, SDage=1.78, 57.2% girls). The between-person results showed that higher trait-like levels of exposure to positive social media content is significantly related to higher trait-like levels of perfectionism. Adolescents’ upward social comparison trait-like levels related to exposure to positive social media content and to socially prescribed perfectionism. A more complex pattern emerged at the within-person level with different results across time intervals, countries, and adolescents’ individualistic values.
Article
Full-text available
Rumination was examined as a potential common mechanism linking risk factors with depression. Initially nondepressed individuals (N = 137) were assessed for presence of a ruminative response style and 4 other hypothesized risk factors for depression. They were followed for 2.5 years. Negative cognitive styles, self-criticism, dependency, neediness, and history of past depression were all significantly associated with rumination. Rumination mediated the predictive relationships of all risk factors except dependency with the number of prospective Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed., rev.; American Psychiatric Association, 1987) major depressive episodes (MDEs; definite and subthreshold) during the follow-up period. In contrast, private self-consciousness did not mediate any relationships between risk factors and subsequent MDEs. Thus, rumination, as a special kind of self-focus, may act as a general proximal mechanism through which other vulnerability factors affect depression.
Article
Full-text available
Several studies have shown that people who engage in ruminative responses to depressive symptoms have higher levels of depressive symptoms over time, after accounting for baseline levels of depressive symptoms. The analyses reported here showed that rumination also predicted depressive disorders, including new onsets of depressive episodes. Rumination predicted chronicity of depressive disorders before accounting for the effects of baseline depressive symptoms but not after accounting for the effects of baseline depressive symptoms. Rumination also predicted anxiety symptoms and may be particularly characteristic of people with mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms.
Article
Full-text available
The authors tested and extended S. Nolen-Hoeksema’s (1991) response styles (RSs) theory of depression by assessing the role of RSs in the onset of depressive episodes (DEs), controlling for the effects of concurrent depression, examining the dispositional aspects of RS, clarifying the role of distraction in the course of a DE, and studying the predictive validity of the Response Styles Questionnaire. Nondepressed (ND) participants who reported that they ruminate in response to their depressive symptoms were more likely to experience a DE over 18 months than were participants who reported that they distract themselves from their symptoms. Both a ruminative RS as measured in an ND state and the use of rumination during the first DE predicted the severity of that episode. In contrast, neither trait nor state rumination predicted the duration of the first DE. Participants showed moderate stability of RSs over 1 year and responded in a consistent manner when depressed.
Article
Full-text available
We examined the relationship between ruminative and distracting styles of responding to depressed mood and the duration of mood. Seventy-nine subjects kept accounts of their moods and responses to their moods for 30 consecutive days. The majority of subjects (83%) showed consistent styles of responding to depressed mood. Regression analyses suggested that the more ruminative responses subjects engaged in, the longer their periods of depressed mood, even after taking into account the initial severity of the mood. In addition, women were more likely than men to have a ruminative response style and on some measures to have more severe and long-lasting periods of depression.
Article
Full-text available
A large body of evidence indicates that women are more likely than men to show unipolar depression. Five classes of explanations for these sex differences are examined and the evidence for each class is reviewed. Not one of these explanations adequately accounts for the magnitude of the sex differences in depression. Finally, a response set explanation for the sex differences in depression is proposed. According to this explanation, men are more likely to engage in distracting behaviors that dampen their mood when depressed, but women are more likely to amplify their moods by ruminating about their depressed states and the possible causes of these states. Regardless of the initial source of a depressive episode (i.e., biological or psychological) men’s more active responses to their negative moods may be more adaptive on average than women’s less active, more ruminative responses.
Article
Full-text available
As results of researchers’ examination of the relationship between perfectionism and procrastination have often been inconsistent, we conducted a meta-analysis of the relationship between procrastination and multidimensional perfectionism. Results indicated that perfectionistic strivings were negatively linked to procrastination, whereas perfectionistic concerns were positively linked to procrastination. Gender, and the measures of perfectionism and procrastination were found to moderate the relationship between procrastination and multidimensional perfectionism. We found that self-efficacy played a mediating role in the relationship between self-oriented perfectionism and procrastination. Our findings fill a gap in the literature and provide confirmatory evidence that the temporal motivational theory can be applied to gain further understanding of the perfectionism–procrastination relationship. © 2018 Scientific Journal Publishers Limited. All Rights Reserved.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: This study investigated the mediating and moderating roles of emotion-focused coping strategies (rumination and immature defenses) in the relationship between perfectionism and depressive symptoms in a Spanish community sample. Method: 438 participants (67.4% female; mean age = 36.94 years) completed self-reports assessing perfectionism, rumination, immature defenses and depression. Multiple mediation, moderation and moderated mediation analyses were conducted. Results: Brooding-rumination and immature defenses mediated the relationship between perfectionism and depressive symptoms. Also, brooding-rumination moderated the impact of perfectionism on depressive symptoms. The mediating effect of brooding in the relationship between perfectionism and depressive symptoms turned out to be moderated by immature defenses, such that the vulnerability link between perfectionism and depressive symptoms through brooding was stronger in individuals scoring high on immature defenses. Conclusion: Brooding-rumination and immature defenses may be essential mechanisms to explain the paths and conditions whereby maladaptive perfectionism gives rise to depression.