Content uploaded by Mark R Dadds
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Mark R Dadds on May 27, 2019
Content may be subject to copyright.
American Psychologist
What Is It to Discipline a Child: What Should It Be? A
Reanalysis of Time-Out From the Perspective of Child
Mental Health, Attachment, and Trauma
Mark R. Dadds and Lucy A. Tully
Online First Publication, February 25, 2019. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/amp0000449
CITATION
Dadds, M. R., & Tully, L. A. (2019, February 25). What Is It to Discipline a Child: What Should It Be?
A Reanalysis of Time-Out From the Perspective of Child Mental Health, Attachment, and Trauma.
American Psychologist. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/amp0000449
What Is It to Discipline a Child: What Should It Be? A Reanalysis of
Time-Out From the Perspective of Child Mental Health, Attachment,
and Trauma
Mark R. Dadds and Lucy A. Tully
The University of Sydney
Parental discipline strategies are a necessary and critical aspect of positive child development.
Their qualities confer risk versus protection for the development of mental health problems.
Time-out from positive reinforcement is now one of the most common and well-researched
discipline procedures across the world, with overwhelming evidence to support its efficacy
and acceptability. It has also recently attracted considerable criticism from writers evoking
child well-being considerations based on attachment theory. The main concern is that the
removal of a child to time-out exposes the child to a break in attachment security and, for
children with trauma histories, potentially causes harm. Here, we consider what a discipline
strategy should be from a mental health perspective and, utilizing the best available models
of developmental mental health and psychopathology, derive five axioms for judging and
guiding the worth and acceptability of any particular discipline strategy. We then use these
axioms to evaluate and specify how time-out can be used in a way that maximizes positive
child outcomes, and then review its use with children who have experienced complex trauma.
We show that time-out, when conceptualized and enacted consistently with contemporary
models of learning, attachment, self-regulation, and family systems theory, is actually a
positive perturbation to these systems that can rapidly remediate problems the child is
experiencing, and thereby generally enhances child well-being. Clinical, research, and policy
implications are briefly discussed.
Keywords: parent discipline, child mental health, time-out, attachment, trauma
In 2014, Time magazine published an article titled “Time-
Outs Are Hurting Your Child” by Siegel and Bryson
(2014a). The article warned parents against using time-out
with children, arguing that harmful effects were supported
by neuroplasticity research. These authors largely recanted
most of what they wrote in a later article in the Huffington
Post (Siegel & Bryson, 2014b), arguing that they were
simply cautioning against inappropriate or punitive use of
time-out. However, Time magazine followed up with an-
other article in 2016 by psychologist Vanessa Lapointe,
again specifying the reasons why time-out may be damag-
ing children (Lapointe, 2016). These articles have been
associated with a widespread and growing rejection of time-
out as an acceptable and effective parental discipline strat-
egy, especially for children who are believed to have at-
tachment problems or trauma histories. As noted by
Quetsch, Wallace, Herschell, and McNeil (2015), there is no
empirical evidence to show that time-out is ineffective or
harmful; however, Internet-based literature warning of the
harmful nature of time-out is now rife.
The aim of this article is to conduct a first analysis of
time-out from the perspective of the child’s developing
mental health. We believe that the analysis of child disci-
pline from a mental health perspective is an important yet
neglected area. The quality and effectiveness of parental
socialization, especially parental discipline, are associated
with lifelong mental and social health (Kessler et al., 2010).
At extremes, most episodes of child abuse arise in the
context of ineffective and problematic parental– child disci-
pline encounters (Chaffin et al., 2004; Straus, 2000). Each
incident of parent– child discipline has the potential to in-
vest in the building of a more socially skilled, resilient, and
self-regulating child or, on the other hand, a child who is
more vulnerable to interpersonal conflict, intense unregu-
lated emotions, and impulsive selfishness (Patterson, 1982;
Straus, 2000; United Nations Children’s Fund, 2017). In
Mark R. Dadds and Lucy A. Tully, School of Psychology, The Univer-
sity of Sydney.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Mark R.
Dadds, Child Behaviour Research Clinic, School of Psychology, Univer-
sity of Sydney, 97 Church Street, Camperdown, Sydney, Australia, 2006.
E-mail: mark.dadds@sydney.edu.au
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
American Psychologist
© 2019 American Psychological Association 2019, Vol. 1, No. 1, 000
0003-066X/19/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/amp0000449
1
this sense, child discipline is one of the most common,
ongoing, and critical events or processes that drives the
social and psychological development of children, and thus
society, as it invests in the next generation. As such, a
comprehensive understanding of the effects of various child
discipline techniques should be a critical priority for child
development and mental health research.
The term time-out is an abbreviation of “time out from
positive reinforcement.” It thus requires the presence of a
reinforcing environment as well as removal from access to
reinforcers for a specified period of time for some form of
misbehavior. Although the abbreviated term time-out is
widely used in scientific literature, we believe that use of
this term has led to the misunderstanding that it is a
stand-alone strategy rather than being one part of a set of
procedures in which enhancing responsive parenting and
strengthening the parent–child relationship is a critical first
step. Thus, we believe that the use of the phrase time out
from positive reinforcement is a more descriptive and accu-
rate one to use, and we adopt the acronym TOPR for the
remainder of the article. In line with its behavioral history,
we define TOPR as a parenting strategy in which a child’s
access to rewards, usually parental attention, is temporarily
removed contingent upon a problem behavior and reinstated
following a specified period of nonproblem behavior. Al-
though the removal of access to rewards can be imple-
mented at any age, we specifically focus this article on its
common usage with children 2 to 8 years of age, as speci-
fied by the major evidence-based parenting programs (see
Everett, Hupp, & Olmi, 2010). The use of the acronym
TOPR in this article refers to both nonexclusionary time-out
(or the time-out chair) and exclusionary time-out, which is
often used as a backup for nonexclusionary time-out.
The analysis of TOPR presented here is embedded in a
broader discussion of what discipline is and should be.
Specifically, in the first part of the article, we derive a set of
axioms for evaluating the worth of parental discipline strat-
egies from the perspective of emerging child mental health,
using what we believe to be the best available contemporary
theories in developmental psychopathology. We then scru-
tinize TOPR from this perspective, leading to a set of
guidelines for use of this technique that maximize its con-
tribution to the positive development and mental health of
growing children. In the second part of the article, we
review evidence on the use of TOPR with children who
have trauma histories given the recent concerns about the
appropriateness of TOPR with these children. In this sec-
tion, we revisit the axioms and guidelines set out in the first
part of the article and aim to answer the following ques-
tions: Is there any evidence that treatments that include
TOPR are harmful? How does TOPR fit within best-
practice treatments for children with trauma symptoms?
A History of Time-Out
According to Risley (2005), it was Montrose Wolf who
first focused on the reinforcing power of adult attention for
children and, based on that, invented and named the non-
violent parenting procedure “time-out” (Wolf, Risley, &
Mees, 1963). More generally, experimental research on
TOPR commenced in the 1960s, when principles of behav-
ioral analysis were first applied to a child’s home or school
environment. Behavioral analysis grew out of Skinner’s
work in the 1930s and 1940s on operant conditioning with
laboratory animals, which described the association be-
tween a particular behavior and a consequence (Skinner,
1950): Actions that are followed by reinforcers are strength-
ened and more likely to occur again in the future, whereas
punishers are an adverse event or outcomes that cause a
decrease in the behavior it follows. Applied to the parenting
context, reinforcers are parenting behaviors (such as praise,
affection, quality time with the child) that increase the
frequency of positive behaviors in a child, whereas punish-
ers (such as ignoring misbehavior, removal of toys or priv-
ileges, and TOPR) reduce or extinguish negative behaviors.
It was clear from some of the first trials close to 50 years
ago (e.g., O’Leary, O’Leary, & Becker, 1967; Patterson,
Ray, & Shaw, 1968) that TOPR was not a stand-alone
parenting strategy; rather, it was part of a stepped process,
in which effective implementation was dependent on mas-
tery of positive reinforcement of the child as a first step. In
other words, enhancing the positive relationship between
parent and child (“time-in”) was necessary for time-out to
be effective. TOPR was also not considered a singular
intervention but part of a sequence of procedural steps.
Mark R. Dadds
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
2DADDS AND TULLY
From the earliest trials, TOPR was considered a temporary
strategy that was inherently self-limiting in its use. That is,
effective implementation of TOPR resulted in rapid im-
provements in behavior, and thus the need to use the strat-
egy subsequently decreased (e.g., see O’Leary et al., 1967).
Early trials of applied behavioral analysis in the home and
school contexts were formalized into a two-stage model of
parenting skill delivery by Hanf (1969). The first stage
involved differential reinforcement of appropriate child be-
havior (via praise for positive behavior and ignoring of
misbehavior) and strengthening the parent– child relation-
ship. The second stage involved delivering calm and con-
sistent behavioral management skills in the form of effec-
tive instructions and providing appropriate consequences
for misbehavior via TOPR and other consequences.
During the 1970s and 1980s, research on TOPR largely
focused on the effectiveness of different procedural parameters
for TOPR. For example, research examined effects of differing
procedural variables such as duration of TOPR (e.g., Hobbs,
Forehand, & Murray, 1978), provision of warnings before
TOPR (e.g., Roberts, 1982), and escape prevention strategies
(e.g., Bean & Roberts, 1981). From the 1990s, research com-
menced on contemporary evidence-based parenting interven-
tions, which included TOPR, with randomized controlled trials
examining the effectiveness of these interventions as a preven-
tion or treatment for child mental health problems. These new
manualized and evidence-based parenting interventions—
including Triple P (Sanders, Kirby, Tellegen, & Day, 2014),
Parent Child Interaction Therapy (PCIT; Querido, Bearss, &
Eyberg, 2002), The Incredible Years (Webster-Stratton, 2001),
Helping the Noncompliant Child (McMahon, Long, & Fore-
hand, 2011), and Parent Management Training Oregon Model
(Patterson, Reid, & Eddy, 2002)—moved beyond simple use
of reinforcers and punishers, and integrated elements of social
interaction theory, cognitive– behavioral theory, and attach-
ment theory. In these interventions, TOPR was delivered as
one discipline strategy within a suite of positive parenting
strategies. That is, the behavioral approach saw discipline as
embedded within a context of positive nurturing parent– child
relationship.
There have now been hundreds of randomized controlled
trials of parenting interventions that include TOPR. These
parenting programs are now considered one of the most
significant achievements of clinical psychology (Brestan &
Eyberg, 1998), having clearly established effectiveness for
preventing or treating a range of child behavioral and emo-
tional problems (Kaminski & Claussen, 2017; Sanders et
al., 2014). Importantly, a meta-analytic review of studies of
parenting interventions examined program components as-
sociated with larger interventions effects and found that
TOPR was a key strategy associated with larger effects in
improving child behavioral adjustment (Kaminski, Valle,
Filene, & Boyle, 2008).
TOPR is now one of the most common discipline proce-
dures used by parents, second only to verbal explanations,
as the most common parental discipline technique in the
United States (Regalado, Sareen, Inkelas, Wissow, & Hal-
fon, 2004). Its use has accelerated over the last few decades
(Ryan, Kalil, Ziol-Guest, & Padilla, 2016). Despite this,
however, other aversive forms of discipline, such as spank-
ing, have continued, such that worldwide as many as three
in four young children are subjected to physical discipline
by their caregivers on a regular basis (United Nations Chil-
dren’s Fund, 2017). In addition, a recent survey found that
although a majority of parents reported using TOPR, many
procedural elements were implemented incorrectly, and this
was associated with parents’ perception that TOPR was
ineffective (Riley, Wagner, Tudor, Zuckerman, & Freeman,
2017). When parents view TOPR as ineffective, it is likely
to be replaced with more harsh or coercive strategies (Dray-
ton et al., 2014) or permissive parenting strategies. Thus, the
effective implementation of TOPR by parents appears to be
hampered by a lack of accurate information regarding ef-
fective procedural elements, along with inaccurate and mis-
leading information about the potential harmful effects of
the strategy.
The Theoretical Basis of Time-Out
Throughout the recent widespread adoption of TOPR, the
conceptual definition has remained largely unexplored, non-
controversial, and firmly grounded in operant theory. That is,
TOPR is defined as any procedure that reduces unacceptable
child behavior by the child experiencing an enforced reduction
in available reinforcement for a brief defined period contingent
upon an unacceptable behavior. Enacted procedures range
Lucy A. Tully
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
3
TIME-OUT IN THE AGE OF ATTACHMENT AND TRAUMA
from a caregiver briefly withdrawing attention from a child, to
placing a child on a specified chair or corner of the room, to the
use of a specially designed segregated TOPR area (see Cor-
ralejo, Jensen, Greathouse, & Ward, 2018). There is a distinc-
tion between nonexclusionary and exclusionary TOPR: In non-
exclusionary TOPR, the child remains in the environment
where the problem occurred, whereas in exclusionary TOPR,
the child is removed to another location (Scarboro & Forehand,
1975). Exclusionary versions are typically reserved for
younger children, commonly 2 to 8 years of age, who at first
struggle to regulate their behavior and may need to be physi-
cally removed in order to achieve an effective time-out from
reinforcers. Exclusionary TOPR is usually used as a backup to
nonexclusionary TOPR if a child does not follow the rules
(such as remaining on the chair or staying quiet).
Given its widely accepted operant base, few writers have
addressed the adequacy of its conceptual underpinnings,
and attention has largely focused on the procedural tech-
niques defining TOPR. For example, MacDonough and
Forehand (1973) outlined and reviewed available research
on eight parameters of the TOPR procedure: location,
schedule, warning, signal, form of administration (which we
elected to call instructional vs. physical administration),
verbalized reason, contingent release, and duration. A num-
ber of updates of these procedures have taken place since
(Corralejo et al., 2018; Everett et al., 2010; Harris, 1985;
Hobbs & Forehand, 1977). As we will see, subtle variations
in procedure are critical to how TOPR functions when it is
scrutinized from a contemporary mental health perspective.
Researchers and practitioners have occasionally presented
innovative ideas about the underlying goals and meaning of
TOPR as a general discipline and child mental health strat-
egy. For example, Dadds and Hawes (2006) described a
parenting program in which TOPR is conceptualized within
a behavioral, attachment, and systems perspective (see also
Scott & Dadds, 2009). Johns and Levy (2013) presented a
case study in which TOPR and time-in are used within both
behavioral and attachment frameworks to treat a child with
severe emotional and behavioral problems. Although not
focusing on TOPR per se, a number of authors have pointed
to the compatibility of learning and attachment theories in
the processes and outcomes associated with behavioral par-
ent training interventions for child behavior problems (e.g.,
Allen, Timmer, & Urquiza, 2014; O’Connor, Matias, Futh,
Tantam, & Scott, 2013; Troutman, 2015).
What is missing, however, is a thorough analysis of what
TOPR is and should be in consideration of emerging child
mental health. In the nearly 60 years since TOPR was first
used in the scientific and health literature, there has never
been a thorough reanalysis of the full set of parent– child
dynamics that ensue when a parent implements TOPR with
a child. We argue here that a parent placing a child in TOPR
represents a dynamic far greater than a mere “operant”
punishment. It speaks to issues of power, attachment, self-
versus other- behavioral/emotional regulation, and contex-
tual family dynamics.
The following section presents an analysis of parental
discipline strategies, and TOPR specifically, in terms of
four contemporary pillars of child development theory: so-
cial learning theory, attachment theory, self- and emotion
regulation theory, and ecological/family systems theory. We
use these models to derive a set of axioms to guide how any
discipline strategy should be evaluated in terms of its impact
on child mental health. We then apply these axioms to
TOPR in order to clarify how it should be used. We show
that when conceptualized and enacted according to the joint
considerations and indications of these highly compatible
models, TOPR is a positive perturbation across these be-
havioral, attachment, self-regulatory and ecological/family
systems that can rapidly remediate problems the child has in
these systems, and thereby generally enhance child devel-
opment.
Parental Discipline and Child Mental Health
Discipline From the Perspective of Learning Theory
In terms of learning theory, broadly sampled here from
early operant to later social and cognitive learning theory,
we take the core criterion of effectiveness—that is, parental
discipline should work. Unfortunately, judging effective-
ness can be somewhat complicated, especially for parents
and many social commentators in society, as the worth of a
discipline strategy may be judged according to visible ef-
fects on the child (that is, it might be assumed that effec-
tiveness is indicated by the distress it causes in the person
being disciplined). In formal learning terms, the effects on
the emotion of the child is not the criterion for effectiveness;
discipline— or “punishment,” in formal learning terms—is
effective if it reduces the frequency or intensity of the
problematic child behavior it is contingent upon. That is,
discipline reduces the likelihood of the behavior it follows.
Discipline strategies such as TOPR can also be thought of as
extinction procedures or “response costs,” in that they re-
move a positive reinforcer from the behavior that previously
elicited this reward. Thus, in the instance of a child being
placed in TOPR for kicking a parent, TOPR is both a
punisher that may reduce the likelihood of future kicking
and an extinction event that removes parental attention that
may have previously rewarded the kicking behavior.
Further, discipline functions in at least four ways in terms
of broader social learning theory. First, effective discipline
should prevent the likely escalation of parent– child conflict
that is fundamental to social learning models of antisocial
behavior, family violence, and associated psychopathology.
Patterson’s (1982) classic coercive family process remains
the best explanatory model of how reinforcement traps, in
which parent and child are simultaneously reinforced for
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
4DADDS AND TULLY
mutual attack and capitulation, move from the innocuous to
the pathological as small struggles over daily life escalate
into chronic and frequent aggression and abuse. When no
effective discipline strategy is available to parents, mundane
events like getting a child ready for school can escalate into
severe conflict (Straus, 2000). In this sense, effective disci-
pline procedures prevent escalating coercion cycles and
give families a sense of predictable control over otherwise
uncontainable escalations.
Second, learning theory specifies modeling and imitation
as core mechanisms for learning behavior. Effective disci-
pline should present parents as positive mental health mod-
els to the child. The use of a brief calm parenting discipline
procedure models to the child that conflict can be dealt with
calmly without resorting to physical and verbal aggression.
It thus minimizes the modeling of aggression that often
ensues when parents are unable to contain escalating coer-
cion cycles.
Third, a core component of social learning theory is
self-efficacy, defined as the expectation caregivers hold
about their ability to parent successfully (Johnston & Mash,
1989). The evidence is clear that the use of escalating
coercive discipline strategies is associated with low parental
self-efficacy (Johnston & Mash, 1989; Jones & Prinz, 2005)
and poor child mental health (Regalado et al., 2004). An
effective discipline procedure provides parents with a
method that enhances self-efficacy and reduces the likeli-
hood of the use of aversive coercive parenting borne of
frustration and anger. For the child, it is critical that disci-
pline strategies are consistent, predictable, and controllable.
As such, the child clearly understands parental expectations
for child behavior, that discipline is not random or emotion-
ally driven, and, thus, that it can be successfully avoided
through self-regulation. Fourth, programs based on social
learning theory are not designed to simply remove a nega-
tive behavior without consideration of a replacement behav-
ior; some of the most successful programs show reductions
in problematic behavior by the establishment or strengthen-
ing of an alternative, often incompatible, positive behavior
(Baldwin & Baldwin, 1986).
Finally, the issue of the causes of the particular child
behavior is an important point that is addressed in social
learning theory. TOPR was designed as a response to an
operant child behavior, that is, an inappropriate behavior
that is purposeful or goal directed, and thus influenced by a
reinforcement contingency, often parental attention. The
operant specifies that the child’s behavior is occurring to
achieve some goal. Thus, TOPR is a punisher or extinction
event applied to a child “operant.” It was not designed to be
used with child behaviors that are “elicited” by powerful
environmental stimuli and are not under the child’s control,
such as fear and panic and distress reactions in the absence
of behavioral problems.
Thus, from the perspective of learning theory, successful
parental discipline is that which satisfies six criteria. Disci-
pline should (a) reduce the likelihood of the inappropriate
behavior recurring; (b) prevent the ongoing escalation of
coercive aggressive cycles; (c) model calm, positive inter-
personal processes; (d) strengthen an alternative positive
behavior; (e) increase self-efficacy that one is an effective
agent; and (f) limit active discipline strategies to operant
child behavior, not elicited or accidental behavior like fear
or making mistakes.
Discipline From the Perspective of Attachment Theory
From Bowlby through Ainsworth to contemporary mod-
els of attachment and parent– child processes, attachment
theory has provided a rich framework for understanding
developmental psychopathology (Cassidy & Shaver, 1999).
At its core is the idea of secure attachment as a fundamental
element of well-being. It is conceptualized and operation-
alized in terms of separation and rapprochement. A young
child who is securely attached to a caregiver is able to
separate in order to explore the environment, and develop
creative independence, self-soothing, and regulation, due to
the availability of rapprochement with a secure base in the
form of a predictable nurturing relationship with the care-
giver. Although attachment theory has developed broadly to
include several corollaries of child and adult relationships
(see Cassidy & Shaver, 1999), the core idea remains that
children require a secure caregiving relationship that allows
them to effectively separate, explore, reunite, and seek
comfort in order to successfully negotiate and master the
changing demands of their social environment and devel-
opment.
From an attachment perspective, a discipline strategy
should, at a minimum, not threaten or break the attachment
bond; at its best, it should enhance the bond. Attachment
bonds are threatened when a caregiver is abusive, unpre-
dictable, unreliable, or unavailable, such as associated with
death, separation, and rejection. Insecure attachments are
characterized by the inability to separate and reunite suc-
cessfully (Ainsworth & Bell, 1970); hence, the clingy child
and/or parent who struggle with anxiety and anger at sepa-
ration and reunite with avoidance and hostility. Avoiding
separation or reunification does not solve these attachment
threats; rather, attachment theory specifies that a secure
attachment is marked by the ability to successfully separate
and reunite without threats to the bond (Ainsworth & Bell,
1970; Cassidy & Shaver, 1999). Thus, discipline is effective
from an attachment perspective when it provides a clear
message to the child that the attachment bond is maintained
solidly throughout the discipline event. That is, the strategy
used provides the message that the caregiver cares about the
child, does not approve of the behavior, and believes in their
capacity to demonstrate self-control and positive behavior.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
5
TIME-OUT IN THE AGE OF ATTACHMENT AND TRAUMA
This core process of successful separation and rapproche-
ment is enacted precisely in many common discipline strat-
egies when delivered effectively. TOPR, for example, by
definition, involves a temporary separation in which access
to parental attention is reduced and is contingent upon the
child regulating their emotions and behavior, at which time
rapprochement is enacted. The essential ingredient that
makes a discipline strategy compatible with attachment
theory is that the “separation” inherent in the strategy is not
a threat to the attachment bond—rather, it is a temporary
and predictable separation solely based on the need for an
operant disciplinary event—and that the basic parent– child
relationship, its attachment security, remains unchanged by
this temporary separation.
The major concern of writers criticizing TOPR and other
forms of discipline from an attachment perspective (e.g.,
Siegel & Bryson, 2014a) is that the separation damages or
breaks the attachment bond by communicating to the child
that the parent is not available to the child to help them
manage and soothe difficult emotions and interpersonal
conflicts. This is a worthy concern and should be axiomatic
to how discipline should be used, but it also misunderstands
the underlying message that contemporary discipline strat-
egies such as TOPR have been designed to communicate,
that is, that discipline is about a brief predictable response to
a specific problem behavior; it is not about isolating and
abandoning a child. It effectively says, for example, “You
are loved, you are my child, you are safe, this is not about
you or us, it is just about this instance of aggression.”
This provides a stark contrast to the experience of many
parent– child dyads who are not able to use discipline tech-
niques without getting upset and impacting the attachment
relationship. As noted above, from a social learning per-
spective, effective discipline provides a preventative mea-
sure against escalating coercive cycles. These escalations
frequently drift into dark areas of attachment threat and
abuse as parents and children grow increasingly emotional
and frustrated with each other and say and do things they
might not wish to resort to. When this happens, discipline
procedures become “attachment rich”—they stray into is-
sues of attachment (e.g., “I am sick of you, I’m fed up, why
are you like this? Get out of my sight”). These are unsuc-
cessful separations from an attachment perspective, as they
damage the secure base and make rapprochement difficult.
Thus, from an attachment perspective, successful disci-
pline does not threaten, and preferably strengthens, the
parent– child bond, thereby increasing each person’s capac-
ity for successful attachments more generally. By success-
fully negotiating the symbolic “separation and rapproche-
ment” that is represented by the initiation and resolution of
a discipline event, especially TOPR, successful discipline
thus normalizes and reinforces the daily manifestations of a
secure attachment bond.
Discipline From the Perspective of Self- and
Emotion Regulation Theory
Here, we draw upon the rich and overlapping areas of
self-control, behavioral regulation, and emotion regulation in
the science of child development. Although these are diverse
fields each with their own methods and issues, they share a
common focus on the individual’s ability to regulate self-
processes in order to achieve a desired outcome, and we refer
to this commonality of self-regulation in what follows. The
ability to successfully regulate across domains of behavior,
emotions, cognition, and attention has been shown to promote
positive child development and mental health (Gross, 1998,
2015). That is, there is a huge literature showing that skill in
these regulatory processes is associated with, and predictive of,
a diverse range of physical, social, and health outcomes (Al-
dao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010; Fergusson, Boden,
& Horwood, 2013; Gross, 1998; Moffitt et al., 2011; Southam-
Gerow & Kendall, 2002). Further, there is an impressive lit-
erature that formally links parental discipline with self-
regulation in children. The child’s regulatory abilities develop
early in life and in the context of parent– child relationships and
interactions, before generalizing to school and peer settings
(Fabes, Eisenberg, & Miller, 1990). A wealth of evidence
suggests a clear link between coercive, dysregulated parenting
styles and deficits in a child’s own capacity for emotion reg-
ulation (Chang, Schwartz, Dodge, & McBride-Chang, 2003;
Gottman, Katz, & Hooven, 1997).
Thus, parental behavior and parent– child interactions can
promote the positive development and mental health of
children by promoting the child’s ability to effectively reg-
ulate their emotions, attention, and behavior. In relation to
parental discipline strategies, then, self-regulation theory
would regard an effective discipline strategy as one that, at
a minimum, does not challenge or reduce, but preferably
enhances, a child’s ability to self-regulate their behavioral,
emotional, and attentional/cognitive processes. This is chal-
lenging to achieve with many discipline strategies, espe-
cially with young children, as discipline inherently contains
elements of control, whereby the child is, by definition and
necessity, being regulated by the parent. A common element
of broad models of child development (e.g., Vygotsky,
1978) is the idea that healthy child development entails
some form of transfer of control from parents to the child,
such that the child gradually internalizes disciplinary con-
trol into skills for autonomous regulation.
For discipline in general, then, and TOPR in particular,
the development of self-regulation should be a broad goal.
Although TOPR typically involves parental control, there
are multiple opportunities in the process whereby the child
can be empowered to learn self-regulatory skills. By under-
standing that a predictable, nonemotional consequence will
reliably follow a problematic behavior, a child is given
maximum opportunity to regulate in advance to avoid the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
6DADDS AND TULLY
discipline. Similarly, a calm instruction given before TOPR
maximizes the likelihood of self-regulation. Explaining to a
child how TOPR works in advance of implementation max-
imizes the chances that the child can stay calm and regulate.
By making removal from TOPR contingent upon the child
successfully regulating behavior and emotions, autonomous
regulation and a sense of self-agency and control can be
strengthened in the child.
This approach to discipline overlaps with the previous
comments on self-efficacy, such that self-regulation refers
to actual regulatory processes, and self-efficacy refers to an
individual’s beliefs about their ability to successfully per-
form a behavior, in this case, self-regulatory processes, in
order to achieve a desired goal. Developmental psychopa-
thology has identified self-regulatory processes at the core
of various models of vulnerability and resilience to mental
health problems (e.g., Gross, 2015), as well as models of
specific disorders, including cognitive and behavioral reg-
ulation in ADHD, attention control in anxiety disorders and
psychopathy, and emotion regulation in conduct problems
(Cavanagh, Quinn, Duncan, Graham, & Balbuena, 2017).
Thus, from the perspective of self-regulation theory, an
effective discipline strategy will be a positive force in the
development of mental health by enhancing, over time, the
child’s ability to regulate behavior, emotion, arousal, and
attention without the need for external, disciplinary con-
straints. The development of autonomous self-regulation
skills is a core aspect of early child development. Thus,
disciplinary strategies should be sensitive to and promote
development such that external control inherent in disci-
pline should be gradually relinquished as the child learns to
internalize the regulatory skills.
Discipline From the Perspective of Ecological/
Family Systems Models
Children develop in a complex social field. Huge con-
tributions to understanding developmental psychopathol-
ogy have come from models that focus on the ecological
system in which development is embedded. This covers a
range of diverse models that have in common the core
idea that the development of mental health is in part
determined by the relationship of the child to functional
and structural properties of the social context. This con-
text includes the family, neighborhood, school, sporting
and other activity groups, religion, and larger cultural
groupings. Some of the most influential ideas come from
Bronfenbrenner (1979) and the ecology of child devel-
opment; the 1970s family therapy thinkers such as
Minuchin et al. (1975), who detailed how healthy child
development is in part dependent on structural hierar-
chies and groupings with clear boundaries within the
family; and a wealth of empirical work showing that
effective parent– child and family communication pat-
terns are a clear predictor of developmental health versus
disorder (Davies & Cummings, 2006).
For the purposes of evaluating discipline from the
ecological perspective, we propose a general tenet
emerging from the core of these models, especially Bron-
fenbrenner’s (1979) ecological model, that can be sum-
marized as follows: Child development and mental health
is in part determined by both how the child interacts with
his or her immediate family and social systems, and how
these systems interact with each other to promote child
development. This, then, includes the child’s interactions
with individual parents and other family members,
school, and broader social groups, as well as how these
individuals and groups interact with each other with
regard to the child. The latter is determined by how well
the broader social ecology holds compatible and positive
values about child development, moral and cultural sys-
tems, and meaning in general.
Thus, discipline, both in terms of its procedures and the
behavior it is applied to, should be consistent with a
larger set of shared values held by the child and parent’s
social ecology. Discipline is thus not erratic, impulsively
emotional, random, or applied differentially; it is based
on higher goals of moral and social behavior. This fit
between the discipline and its social and moral context is
a critical principle for understanding the worth of any
discipline strategy. As argued by Grusec, Danyliuk, Kil,
and O’Neill (2017), discipline is effective when it is
perceived by the child to be fair, reasonable, and clearly
embedded in explicit community moral values of right
and wrong. That is, the discipline event has meaning
clearly embedded in a family and ecological system of
shared perceptions of what is right; it is not arbitrary, out
of scale to the problem behavior, unfair, and based on
parental emotion or impulse. We also note that the con-
struct of how well individual behavioral events are em-
bedded in, and consistent with, larger cultural and spiri-
tual meaning is emerging as one of the most robust
predictors of mental health and resilience in the “happi-
ness” literature (see Ryan & Deci, 2001).
This literature indicates that to understand the value and
acceptability of TOPR, there must be consideration of the
social context in which it is implemented. Primarily, this
refers to the child’s family and how fairly and explicitly its
moral and social principles are used to guide the various
child behaviors that are selected for discipline. From the
perspective of ecological and family systems thinking, ef-
fective discipline is one that responds to a behavior that is
wrong or inappropriate according to broader system and
cultural mores, and enacts a corrective process that is fair,
reasonable, acceptable, and aligned with the general cultural
mores and meaning of the system.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
7
TIME-OUT IN THE AGE OF ATTACHMENT AND TRAUMA
Integrating the Prescriptions of Major Mental
Health Models Into a Set of Working Axioms
The above leads us to propose a set of working axioms by
which to guide and evaluate the worth of specific parental
discipline strategies. It should be noted that elements of
these recommendations are specified in existing evidence-
based interventions that employ TOPR, such as Triple P
(Sanders et al., 2014), PCIT (Querido et al., 2002), and the
Incredible Years (Webster-Stratton, 2001). This is the first
time, we believe, that these axioms have been brought
together into an expanded, integrated set. Thus, in terms of
the development of mental health in children, discipline
strategies should be (a) effective in terms of their success in
reducing recurrence of the problem behavior, preventing
escalation of further coercive aggression, providing effec-
tive modeling of interpersonal process, and developing and
strengthening alternative behaviors to the problem behavior;
(b) attachment secure, as in not a threat to, and preferably
facilitative of, ongoing secure attachment relationship
between the parent and child; (c) promoting of the child’s
self-efficacy and skills for behavioral, emotional, and
cognitive/attentional self-regulation; and (d) fair and
meaningful, not arbitrary and personal, and be clearly
embedded in, and consistent with, an explicit set of
shared family, social, and cultural mores about what is
right and meaningful.
Guidelines for Implementation of TOPR From a
Mental Health Perspective
These axioms about how discipline should be conceptu-
alized and implemented to enhance positive child develop-
ment and mental health can now be used to inform the
appropriate use of TOPR, our main focus in this article.
Thus, in response to the question “How then should TOPR
be implemented in terms of promoting the ongoing mental
health of children?” we propose the following specific
guidelines:
1. TOPR should only be used for inappropriate child
behavior that has an operant or deliberate quality or
component, that is, behavior over which the child
has some control and that is functional in producing
some desired outcome for the child. It should not be
used for behavior that represents an inability to
perform an action, lack of understanding, mistakes,
fear or other overwhelming emotions. These behav-
iors warrant supportive teaching responses from par-
ents, not discipline. TOPR can be used for inappro-
priate child behavior such as aggression that
accompanies distress reactions but not for the dis-
tress reactions themselves.
2. The effectiveness of TOPR implementation should
be judged in terms of observable and timely reduc-
tions in the problem behavior and. thus, in the rap-
idly diminishing need to use TOPR.
3. Parental behavior during the TOPR procedure should
provide a positive role model of calm, attachment-
secure interpersonal process.
4. The use of TOPR must be a part of a broader behav-
ioral program that promotes a warm and rewarding
relationship, and explicitly teaches alternative positive
child behaviors to replace the problem behavior to
improve the child’s self-efficacy in meeting their own
needs.
5. TOPR should be seen as a microcosm of the funda-
mental attachment process of separation and reunion. It
must not carry any parental communication of aban-
donment, isolation, and rejection during the TOPR and
return to time-in phases. Implemented appropriately,
TOPR can be seen as microtheater for enacting and
repairing attachment problems, conveying the explicit
message that this discipline event is focused on a
specific problem behavior, and throughout, the child
remains safe, valued, and loved.
6. The child should have an active role in influencing the
unfolding of TOPR such that their self-regulatory ca-
pacities are enhanced rather than externally controlled
and diminished. Thus, TOPR should end in a way that
is contingent upon the child’s self-regulation, not an
arbitrary time period. For example, return to time-in
can be contingent upon the child showing a brief but
stable period of regulated emotions and behavior.
7. Discipline strategies should function to improve the
child’s ability to effectively regulate emotions and be-
havior. Thus, the child should be taught, at a positive
time outside of conflict and prior to TOPR being used,
some basic rules for TOPR and skills for regulating
their emotions and behavior that can be used in TOPR
and elsewhere.
8. TOPR should be used for behaviors that are prespeci-
fied and explicitly explained to the child as being
problematic and inappropriate in terms of generally
accepted mores to the child, the family, the school, and
so on. These should be open to discussion at positive
times outside of discipline events.
9. TOPR implementation should be embedded in a family
and ecological system of shared perceptions of what is
right; it is not arbitrary, out of scale to the problem
behavior, unfair, and based on parental emotion or
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
8DADDS AND TULLY
impulse. Children should be encouraged to be active
participants in understanding the cultural, moral, and
pragmatic context of family discipline.
10. TOPR should be applied democratically. That is, in
order to embed the discipline process firmly within
accepted ideas of fairness, time-out should be applied
equally and fairly across children in a family, depend-
ing on developmental levels.
Conceptualized and enacted within these guidelines for
promoting mental health, we propose that TOPR is not only
an acceptable parental discipline strategy—it is a positive
perturbation of the parent– child system that can enhance
and repair behavioral problems as well as broader problems
of self- and emotion regulation and parent– child attachment
problems. Studies show that behavioral parenting programs
that include TOPR have clear benefits for these broader
aspects of child development, including attachment pro-
cesses (O’Connor et al., 2013). We thus propose that the
widespread use of TOPR is warranted, and when concep-
tualized and implemented appropriately, as described
above, is a positive force for the mental health of children
and families.
Negative Information About TOPR
What, then, drives the recent flurry of criticism of TOPR
as a harmful procedure for children? It would be easy to
dismiss this as based on inaccurate ideas of what TOPR is.
We think it better to look carefully at these criticisms for
ideas of worth, as there is no doubt that they are motivated
by a positive concern for children. From reviewing the
literature on the Internet about TOPR (e.g., Australian As-
sociation for Infant Mental Health [AAIMH], 2009; Coul-
son, 2016; Lapointe, 2016; Siegel & Bryson, 2014a), it
would seem that the criticisms can be summarized into three
categories: (a) TOPR produces an attachment threat, for
example, “it deliberately cuts off the child from the rela-
tionship with parent or carer so that the child feels power-
less to connect with the adult” (AAIMH, 2009); (b) TOPR
does not teach new behaviors; and (c) TOPR fails to address
the underlying causes of the problem behavior. These are
reasonable concerns and they should be carefully consid-
ered rather than dismissed by proponents of TOPR. These
are the same concerns noted by Quetsch et al. (2015) in their
analysis of the literature that claims TOPR to be harmful for
children. Importantly, the analysis we presented above
about how parent– child discipline should be conceptualized
and implemented, and thus specifically how TOPR should
be used, converged on the same issues. Further, the analysis
showed that when implemented in terms of current knowl-
edge of child development and mental health, TOPR is
actually a positive discipline strategy that addresses each of
these concerns and can be a positive perturbation for attach-
ment problems and the development of self-regulation
skills. There is a further concern, however, about the use of
TOPR in relation to children with trauma histories and the
potential of TOPR to harm these children, and this will be
addressed in the following section.
Is TOPR Safe and Effective for Children With
Trauma Histories?
Exposure to traumatic or adverse events is unfortunately
common in childhood and adolescence, and is associated
with the onset of a broad range of mental health disorders
(e.g., McLaughlin et al., 2012) as well as behavioral symp-
toms of “trauma,” such as reexperiencing, avoidance, and
hyperarousal, which may lead to a diagnosis of posttrau-
matic stress disorder (PTSD). Concerns about the harmful
effects of TOPR for children with trauma histories are
prolific on the Internet, although these concerns have not
been documented in the published scientific literature, and it
is not clear how widespread these concerns are among
practitioners and caregivers. For the sake of this analysis, it
is important to distinguish between acute trauma, which
results from exposure to a discrete traumatic event, and
complex trauma, which results from the experiences of
multiple, chronic, and prolonged developmentally adverse
events, often of an interpersonal nature and with early life
onset (van der Kolk, 2005). Exposure to child abuse and
neglect is considered a complex trauma and can result in
difficulties in emotion perception, processing and regula-
tion, and long-lasting physical, emotional, and psychologi-
cal problems as well as changes in structural and functional
brain development (Jedd et al., 2015).
We believe it is important to focus on the appropriateness
and effectiveness of TOPR as a discipline procedure for
children who have experienced complex trauma in the form
of abuse and neglect specifically, as it is for these children
that concerns about potential harmful effects of TOPR are
paramount. Additionally, because the trauma is often expe-
rienced within the context of a dysfunctional caregiving
system, it is important to explore the potential of TOPR to
reduce trauma-related mental health problems. Thus, we
address two key questions in relation to use of TOPR with
children with abuse and neglect histories: Is there any
evidence that treatments that include TOPR are harmful,
and how does TOPR fit within current best-practice treat-
ments for children with trauma symptoms? We also revisit
the axioms and guidelines for implementation of TOPR
covered earlier in relation to children with trauma histories.
Regarding the first question about whether interventions
that include TOPR are harmful for children with abuse and
neglect histories, there is considerable research that has
examined the efficacy of parenting interventions that in-
clude TOPR within child welfare populations, and this
research has found no evidence of adverse effects. For
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
9
TIME-OUT IN THE AGE OF ATTACHMENT AND TRAUMA
children who have experienced physical abuse and remain
with their parents, parenting interventions have been found
to significantly reduce rereports of physical abuse; ineffec-
tive, harsh, and dysfunctional parenting practices; and neg-
ative parenting attitudes, and to enhance positive parenting
practices, attitudes, and attributions (see reviews by Chen &
Chan, 2016; Vlahovicova, Melendez-Torres, Leijten, Knerr,
& Gardner, 2017). Many of these interventions include
TOPR as part of a focus on teaching effective parental
discipline, along with enhancing the parent– child affective
bond. PCIT includes TOPR, and a review of 11 PCIT
studies targeting maltreating families found significant re-
ductions in child welfare rereferrals, negative parenting
practices, and child externalizing behaviors (Batzer, Berg,
Godinet, & Stotzer, 2018). There is much less research for
children who have experienced parental neglect, but there is
some evidence that interventions including TOPR are also
effective in reducing neglect recidivism (Chaffin, Funder-
burk, Bard, Valle, & Gurwitch, 2011). We could find no
evidence of iatrogenic effects stemming from the use of
parenting programs that include TOPR with these popula-
tions of children.
Use of TOPR for children who have been removed from
their parents and placed with foster or kinship carers war-
rants a particular focus. Not only have these children expe-
rienced abuse and/or neglect severe enough to result in
removal from their parents—they have also experienced
separation from parents and other attachment figures. For
these children, there is evidence that parenting interventions
are effective in improving child mental health and parenting
(see reviews by Hambrick, Oppenheim-Weller, N=zi, &
Taussig, 2016; Uretsky & Hoffman, 2017), and TOPR is
often included as a key discipline procedure in these inter-
ventions (e.g., Mersky, Topitzes, Grant-Savela, Brondino,
& McNeil, 2016; Price et al., 2008). Thus, there appears to
be evidence that interventions involving TOPR improve
parenting and child adjustment for children in the welfare
system, and there is no evidence to date of harmful effects.
A few evidence-based parenting interventions have also
been adapted to be appropriate for children with trauma
symptoms (Akin, Lang, McDonald, Yan, & Little, 2019;
Gurwitch, Messer, & Funderburk, 2017). For example, Par-
ent Management Training Oregon Model was adapted to
include trauma content and a focus on emotion regulation
and mindfulness techniques for children in foster care with
serious emotional disturbance (Akin et al., 2019). This
intervention was found to be effective in improving child
socioemotional well-being in an randomized controlled trial
when compared with services as usual (Akin et al., 2019).
Importantly, the adaptations to this program did not include
changes to core parenting strategies such as TOPR.
There have been concerns among practitioners and par-
ents that the use of TOPR with children with abuse and
neglect histories may retraumatize children, which is de-
fined as “traumatic stress reactions, responses and symp-
toms that occur consequent to multiple exposures to trau-
matic events that are physical, psychological, or both in
nature” (Duckworth & Follette, 2012, p. 2). It is argued that
retraumatization can occur through discipline practices that
are distressing for a child and may cause reexperiencing of
the traumatic event (McNeil, Costello, Travers, & Norman,
2013). For example, TOPR may serve as a reminder of a
previous traumatic event and cause intense distress (see
McNeil et al., 2013, and Quetsch et al., 2015, for detailed
discussions about retraumatizaton in relation to TOPR).
However, in the unlikely event that TOPR should remind a
child of a previous traumatic event (e.g., limit setting by
parents reminding a child of previous physical abuse) and
trigger temporary arousal or fear, repeated use of the pro-
cedure as part of a calm, consistent response to misbehavior
may potentially serve as psychological exposure (McNeil et
al., 2013; Quetsch, Lieneman, & McNeil, 2017; Quetsch et
al., 2015). Gradual exposure to trauma reminders is a key
component of interventions for child trauma symptoms. In
this context, exposure to TOPR has the potential to reduce
a child’s arousal or fear of the trauma reminder (McNeil et
al., 2013). Through the implementation of TOPR as a calm
and predictable discipline strategy—and one that does not
disrupt secure attachment— children may learn to replace
feelings of fear and pain with feelings of control and safety
(see Quetsch et al., 2015, 2017).
In support of the argument that interventions that include
TOPR do not increase trauma symptoms, a study of a
standard PCIT found significant reductions in child trauma
symptoms along with child behavior problems from pre- to
posttreatment (Pearl et al., 2012), although it should be
noted that this was not a randomized controlled trial. The
children in this study were considered high risk, with the
majority of children having experienced some form of trau-
matic event. It is important to caution that we are not
suggesting that TOPR be used as a discipline procedure
specifically for trauma symptoms (as noted earlier in Guide-
line 1), but it may be indicated as part of a parenting
intervention for inappropriate child behavior that accompa-
nies these symptoms Cohen, Berliner, & Mannarino, 2010.
The second question focuses on how TOPR fits within
current best-practice treatments for child trauma symptoms.
Recent reviews have identified that one of the most evidence-
based interventions for reducing child trauma symptoms is
trauma-focused cognitive behavioral therapy (TF-CBT; Leen-
arts, Diehle, Doreleijers, Jansma, & Lindauer, 2013). This
intervention— described by the acronym PRACTICE—
involves a focus on parenting, along with psychoeducation,
relaxation skills, affective modulation skills, cognitive cop-
ing and processing skills, trauma narrative, in vivo mastery
of trauma reminders (exposure), conjoint child–parent ses-
sions, and enhancing future safety and development (Cohen,
Berliner, et al., 2010). In the parenting component, caregiv-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
10 DADDS AND TULLY
ers learn effective parenting skills, stress management, and
skills to support gradual exposure and to correct their own
trauma-related cognitive errors (Kliethermes, Drewry, &
Wamser-Nanney, 2017). The parenting skills taught in TF-
CBT comprise standard discipline strategies, including the
use of TOPR (Cohen, Berliner, et al., 2010). However,
interventions such as TF-CBT may not feasible if child
disruptive behaviors prevent the child and parent from ef-
fectively engaging in the intervention, so in this case, a
parenting intervention may be warranted as a first-line treat-
ment (Cohen, Berliner, et al., 2010; Gurwitch et al., 2017).
Cohen, Berliner, et al. (2010) provide a detailed discussion
about treatment for comorbid behavior problems and trauma
symptoms. Thus, it would seem that TOPR is already a
standard procedure within evidence-based interventions for
child trauma symptoms such as TF-CBT.
It is also important to revisit the likely putative mecha-
nisms for how TOPR works within the context of maltreat-
ing parents and trauma-exposed children, and in doing so,
revisit the axioms set out earlier. Child physical abuse
mostly occurs within a context of an escalating coercive
parent– child interactions, and interactions may be driven by
negative parental attributions and intolerance toward the
child’s behavior as well as increasing reliance on harsh
discipline (Chaffin et al., 2004). Through successful imple-
mentation of TOPR, discipline becomes calm, controlled,
predicable, and brief, instead of being emotional, escalating,
chaotic, and prolonged, with caregivers modeling effective
emotion regulation skills (Axiom a). Use of TOPR ensures
that the positive parent– child relationship is maintained and
not damaged by discipline encounters (Axiom b). Children
practice and achieve success at emotional and behavioral
regulation in TOPR, thus improving their skills in self-
regulation (Axiom c). Through consistent and predictable
use of TOPR, which is discussed and rehearsed with chil-
dren prior to implementation, children will replace beliefs
about discipline being unsafe, unfair, and erratic with an
understanding that TOPR is safe, fair, reasonable, and
meaningful (Axiom d). Consequently, children may replace
the distress and fear that was once associated with discipline
with feelings of safety, security, and predictability (Quetsch
et al., 2017).
Implementation of TOPR should be considered with each
child’s abuse and neglect histories in mind (Quetsch et al.,
2015), along with their behavioral presentations. It is im-
portant that comprehensive assessments include standard-
ized measures about a child’s traumatic experiences, PTSD
symptoms, and behavioral problems, as well as the quality
of parenting and the parent– child relationship. A thorough
assessment is critical to determine treatment planning for
children with trauma histories in order to determine what
problems to focus on during treatment (see Cohen, Berliner,
et al., 2010, and Cohen, Bukstein, et al., 2010, for more
information about assessment and treatment). Based on
each child’s individual history and presentation, TOPR may
require modification. Such modifications may include iden-
tifying alternative strategies to exclusionary TOPR, which
is often used as a backup to nonexclusionary TOPR. The use
of exclusionary TOPR may be particularly concerning for
caregivers of children with trauma histories, and they may
be reluctant to implement it. Pearl et al. (2012) described
how the strategy was modified in response to organizational
policies preventing the use of exclusionary TOPR. In this
example, loss of privileges was implemented as a backup
procedure to nonexclusionary TOPR, and this modification
did not appear to reduce the effectiveness of the strategy.
Additionally, some strategies or procedures may require an
increased emphasis for a child with trauma histories, such as
an a more intense focus on establishing/strengthening the
positive parent– child relationship prior to introducing
TOPR. Ongoing assessment of child symptoms throughout
the duration of intervention is also critical, as this will
determine the effectiveness of TOPR, as described in
Guideline 2, and guide any modifications to the procedure.
We thus conclude that there is no evidence to show that
TOPR is contraindicated for children showing trauma symp-
toms, either as a discipline strategy or as part of a parenting
intervention to address trauma-related behavior problems, al-
though the procedure should not be used to manage trauma
symptoms specifically. TOPR is used in evidence-based inter-
ventions that show benefits for improving parenting and child
adjustment in child welfare populations. This is not to say that
these research findings constitute evidence that TOPR per se is
effective, as the intervention effects may arise primarily from
strengthening the parent– child relationship or other strate-
gies in the intervention. The type of studies that would be
needed to evaluate the independent contribution of TOPR
per se, such as dismantling studies and meta-analyses of
intervention components, have not yet been conducted.
The evidence base for making these conclusions is small
at present, and more research into the use of TOPR with
highly vulnerable populations should be a priority. We
also note that procedural variations of TOPR might aid
applicability and acceptability with traumatized children
without compromising effectiveness. This would be rec-
ommended, with an important emphasis on monitoring
implementation to ensure that caregivers adhere to its
evidence-based procedural parameters, guidelines for im-
plementation as outlined in this article, and careful mon-
itoring of changes in child behavior to determine effec-
tiveness of the strategy.
Summary and Conclusions
We reviewed contemporary theory and empirical studies
with the aim of developing a set of axioms for evaluating
the value and acceptability of parental discipline strategies
from the perspective of emerging mental health using the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
11
TIME-OUT IN THE AGE OF ATTACHMENT AND TRAUMA
best available contemporary theory in developmental psy-
chopathology. We then scrutinized TOPR, one of the most
common but increasingly controversial parental discipline
strategies from this perspective, leading to a set of guide-
lines for use of this technique that maximize its contribution
to the mental health of growing children. Given recent
concerns about the use of TOPR with children who have
experienced trauma, we also reviewed evidence on the use
of discipline with such children, especially the use of
TOPR. We conclude the following:
1. Parental discipline represents a group of common
and ongoing events in the lives of children that
contribute to their mental health, and thus society’s
health at large. Thus, its usage should be a priority
concern for the health, education. and social sec-
tors of society.
2. The quality of any particular discipline strategy as
a mental health perturbation can be evaluated using
the best contemporary models of developmental
psychopathology, that is, social learning, attach-
ment, self-regulation, and family/ecological sys-
tems theories.
3. Each of these models contributes to a set of clear
guidelines, presented in this article, about how
discipline can and should be used to facilitate pos-
itive child outcomes.
4. TOPR is an increasingly common but somewhat
controversial discipline strategy that has been
shown to be effective in reducing inappropriate
behavior and improving parent– child relations, but
that has not been comprehensively scrutinized in
terms of contemporary models of developmental
mental health.
5. The application of the guidelines we derived from
models of developmental psychopathology to the
practice of TOPR leads to specific guidelines for
its use, specified earlier in this article.
6. When implemented within these guidelines, TOPR
is an effective and positive discipline strategy that
has the potential to enhance all aspects of the
child’s development and mental health.
7. The appropriate use of TOPR is also compatible
with our best understanding of the needs of chil-
dren with a history of exposure to trauma, although
the procedure is not appropriate for trauma symp-
toms specifically.
8. Research into the use of TOPR with children with
a history of trauma exposure indicates that when
presented as part of broad parenting intervention, it
improves behavioral and emotional problems.
9. Notwithstanding Points 7 and 8 above, further re-
search is needed to confirm the effectiveness and
safety of using TOPR with children who have
experienced complex trauma.
These conclusions have important implications for clini-
cal practice and policy. The evidence presented here indi-
cates that the adoption of policies that prohibit the use of
TOPR with children may be ill-considered and deny access
of children in need to an effective evidence-based proce-
dure. Given the wealth of evidence showing that TOPR is a
positive perturbation in child mental health, and the absence
of evidence showing it is harmful after five decades of
research, clinical, and common usage, claims that it is
harmful should be considered extraordinary, and thus re-
quire an extraordinary level of evidence to back them up. A
clear parallel is claims that vaccinations cause autism. The
evidence showing this to be false is so strong that claims to
the contrary require an extraordinary quality and quantity of
evidence to back them up. When TOPR is used, however, it
is crucial that its underlying theory, therapeutic mecha-
nisms, and procedural subtleties are well understood and
explicit to all parties concerned. The evidence is clear that
inappropriate parental discipline strategies have been, and
continue to be, implemented in the name of TOPR and these
are widespread, ineffective, and potentially harmful. Thus, a
priority for improving child mental health literacy is to
disseminate accurate information about the mechanisms and
procedures of this and other forms of discipline.
Finally, we have clearly seen that the use of TOPR is
synergistic with attachment theory and, in fact, offers a
concrete way of preventing and addressing attachment prob-
lems in everyday life. Similarly, the use of TOPR is com-
patible with the needs of children who have been exposed to
complex trauma and appears to be effective for treating
behavioral and emotional problems in these children. The
literature on this latter conclusion is tentative, however, and
should be a priority for future research.
References
Australian Association for Infant Mental Health (AAIMH). (2009). Time out:
Position Paper 3. Retrieved from https://aaimhi.sslsvc.com/key-issues/
position-statements-and-guidelines/AAIMHI-Position-paper-3-Time-
out.pdf
Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bell, S. M. (1970). Attachment, exploration, and
separation: Illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situa-
tion. Child Development, 41, 49 – 67. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1127388
Akin, B. A., Lang, K., McDonald, T. P., Yan, Y., & Little, T. (2019).
Randomized trial of PMTO in foster care: Six-month child well-being
outcomes. Research on Social Work Practice, 29, 206 –222.
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-
regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
12 DADDS AND TULLY
Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j
.cpr.2009.11.004
Allen, B., Timmer, S. G., & Urquiza, A. J. (2014). Parent– child interaction
therapy as an attachment-based intervention: Theoretical rationale and
pilot data with adopted children. Children and Youth Services Review,
47, 334 –341. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2014.10.009
Baldwin, J. D., & Baldwin, J. I. (1986). Behavior principles in everyday
life. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Batzer, S., Berg, T., Godinet, M. T., & Stotzer, R. L. (2018). Efficacy or
chaos? Parent-child interaction therapy in maltreating populations: A
review of research. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 19, 3–19. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1177/1524838015620819
Bean, A. W., & Roberts, M. W. (1981). The effect of time-out release
contingencies on changes in child noncompliance. Journal of Abnormal
Child Psychology, 9, 95–105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00917860
Brestan, E. V., & Eyberg, S. M. (1998). Effective psychosocial treatments
of conduct-disordered children and adolescents: 29 years, 82 studies, and
5,272 kids. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 27, 180 –189. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp2702_5
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cam-
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (1999). Handbook of attachment: Theory,
research and clinical applications, 5, 113–120.
Cavanagh, M., Quinn, D., Duncan, D., Graham, T., & Balbuena, L. (2017).
Oppositional defiant disorder is better conceptualized as a disorder of
emotional regulation. Journal of Attention Disorders, 21, 381–389.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1087054713520221
Chaffin, M., Funderburk, B., Bard, D., Valle, L. A., & Gurwitch, R. (2011).
A combined motivation and parent-child interaction therapy package
reduces child welfare recidivism in a randomized dismantling field trial.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79, 84 –95. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/a0021227
Chaffin, M., Silovsky, J. F., Funderburk, B., Valle, L. A., Brestan, E. V.,
Balachova, T.,...Bonner, B. L. (2004). Parent-child interaction therapy
with physically abusive parents: Efficacy for reducing future abuse
reports. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 500 –510.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.72.3.500
Chang, L., Schwartz, D., Dodge, K. A., & McBride-Chang, C. (2003).
Harsh parenting in relation to child emotion regulation and aggression.
Journal of Family Psychology, 17, 598 – 606. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
0893-3200.17.4.598
Chen, M., & Chan, K. L. (2016). Effects of parenting programs on child
maltreatment prevention: A meta-analysis. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse,
17, 88 –104. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1524838014566718
Cohen, J. A., Berliner, L., & Mannarino, A. (2010). Trauma focused CBT
for children with co-occurring trauma and behavior problems. Child
Abuse & Neglect, 34, 215–224. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2009
.12.003
Cohen, J. A., Bukstein, O., Walter, H., Benson, S. R., Chrisman, A.,
Farchione, T. R.,...AACAP Work Group On Quality Issues. (2010).
Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and
adolescents with posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of the American
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 414 – 430.
Corralejo, S. M., Jensen, S. A., Greathouse, A. D., & Ward, L. E. (2018).
Parameters of time-out: Research update and comparison to parenting
programs, books, and online recommendations. Behavior Therapy, 49,
99 –112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2017.09.005
Coulson, J. (2016). The negative effects of time out on children. Retrieved
from https://ifstudies.org/blog/the-negative-effects-of-time-out-on-
children
Dadds, M., & Hawes, D. (2006). Integrated family intervention for child
conduct problems: A behaviour-attachment-systems intervention for
parents. Samford, Queensland, Australia: Australian Academic Press.
Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (2006). Interparental discord, family
process, and developmental psychopathology. In D. Cicchetti & D. J.
Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Volume 3: Risk, disor-
der, and adaptation (pp. 86 –128). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Drayton, A. K., Andersen, M. N., Knight, R. M., Felt, B. T., Fredericks,
E. M., & Dore-Stites, D. J. (2014). Internet guidance on time out:
Inaccuracies, omissions, and what to tell parents instead. Journal of
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 35, 239 –246. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000000059
Duckworth, M., & Follette, V. (Eds.). (2012). Retraumatization: Assess-
ment, treatment, and prevention. New York, NY: Routledge. http://dx
.doi.org/10.4324/9780203866320
Everett, G. E., Hupp, S. D., & Olmi, D. J. (2010). Time-out with parents:
A descriptive analysis of 30 years of research. Education & Treatment
of Children, 33, 235–259. http://dx.doi.org/10.1353/etc.0.0091
Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1990). Maternal correlates of
children’s vicarious emotional responsiveness. Developmental Psychol-
ogy, 26, 639 – 648.
Fergusson, D. M., Boden, J. M., & Horwood, L. J. (2013). Childhood
self-control and adult outcomes: Results from a 30-year longitudinal
study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychi-
atry, 52, 709 –717.e1. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.04.008
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1997). Meta-emotion: How
families communicate emotionally. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integra-
tive review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271–299. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects.
Psychological Inquiry, 26, 1–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2014
.940781
Grusec, J. E., Danyliuk, T., Kil, H., & O’Neill, D. (2017). Perspectives on
parent discipline and child outcomes. International Journal of Behavioral
Development, 41, 465– 471. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0165025416
681538
Gurwitch, R. H., Messer, E. P., & Funderburk, B. W. (2017). Parent-child
interaction therapy. In M. Landolt, M. Cloitre, & U. Schnyder (Eds.),
Evidence-based treatments for trauma related disorders in children and
adolescents (pp. 341–361). Cham, Switzerland: Springer. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1007/978-3-319-46138-0_16
Hambrick, E. P., Oppenheim-Weller, S., N=zi, A. M., & Taussig, H. N.
(2016). Mental health interventions for children in foster care: A sys-
tematic review. Children and Youth Services Review, 70, 65–77. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2016.09.002
Hanf, C. (1969). A two stage program for modifying maternal controlling
during mother-child (MC) interaction. Paper presented at the Western
Psychological Association Meeting, Vancouver, BC.
Harris, K. R. (1985). Definitional, parametric, and procedural consider-
ations in timeout interventions and research. Exceptional Children, 51,
279 –288.
Hobbs, S. A., & Forehand, R. (1977). Important parameters in the use of
timeout with children: A re-examination. Journal of Behavior Therapy and
Experimental Psychiatry, 8, 365–370. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-
7916(77)90004-0
Hobbs, S. A., Forehand, R., & Murray, R. G. (1978). Effects of various
durations of timeout on the noncompliant behavior of children. Behavior
Therapy, 9, 652– 656. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(78)80142-7
Jedd, K., Hunt, R. H., Cicchetti, D., Hunt, E., Cowell, R. A., Rogosch,
F.A.,...Thomas, K. M. (2015). Long-term consequences of childhood
maltreatment: Altered amygdala functional connectivity. Development
and Psychopathology, 27, 1577–1589. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S095
4579415000954
Johns, A., & Levy, F. (2013). “Time-in” and “time-out” for severe emo-
tional dysregulation in children. Australasian Psychiatry, 21, 281–282.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1039856212475327
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
13
TIME-OUT IN THE AGE OF ATTACHMENT AND TRAUMA
Johnston, C., & Mash, E. J. (1989). A measure of parenting satisfaction and
efficacy. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 18, 167–175. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1207/s15374424jccp1802_8
Jones, T. L., & Prinz, R. J. (2005). Potential roles of parental self-efficacy
in parent and child adjustment: A review. Clinical Psychology Review,
25, 341–363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2004.12.004
Kaminski, J. W., & Claussen, A. H. (2017). Evidence base update for
psychosocial treatments for disruptive behaviors in children. Journal of
Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 46, 477– 499. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1080/15374416.2017.1310044
Kaminski, J. W., Valle, L. A., Filene, J. H., & Boyle, C. L. (2008). A
meta-analytic review of components associated with parent training
program effectiveness. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36,
567–589. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10802-007-9201-9
Kessler, R. C., McLaughlin, K. A., Green, J. G., Gruber, M. J., Sampson,
N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M.,...Williams, D. R. (2010). Childhood
adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO World Mental Health
Surveys. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 197, 378 –385. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080499
Kliethermes, M. D., Drewry, K., & Wamser-Nanney, R. (2017). Trauma-
focused cognitive behavioral therapy. In M. Landolt, M. Cloitre, & U.
Schnyder (Eds.), Evidence-based treatments for trauma related disor-
ders in children and adolescents (pp. 167–186). Cham, Switzerland:
Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-46138-0_8
Lapointe, V. (2016, January 27). 3 reasons why time-out may be damaging
your child. Time. Retrieved from http://time.com/4195647/three-
reasons-why-time-outs-may-be-damaging-your-child
Leenarts, L. E., Diehle, J., Doreleijers, T. A., Jansma, E. P., & Lindauer,
R. J. (2013). Evidence-based treatments for children with trauma-related
psychopathology as a result of childhood maltreatment: A systematic
review. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 22, 269 –283. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00787-012-0367-5
MacDonough, T. S., & Forehand, R. (1973). Response-contingent time out:
Important parameters in behavior modification with children. Journal of
Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 4, 231–236. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7916(73)90079-7
McLaughlin, K. A., Greif Green, J., Gruber, M. J., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky,
A. M., & Kessler, R. C. (2012). Childhood adversities and first onset of
psychiatric disorders in a national sample of U.S. adolescents. JAMA Psy-
chiatry, 69, 1151–1160. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011
.2277
McMahon, R. J., Long, N., & Forehand, R. L. (2011). Parent training for
the treatment of oppositional behavior in young children: Helping the
noncompliant child. In R. Murrihy, A. Kidman, & T. Ollendick (Eds.),
Clinical handbook of assessing and treating conduct problems in youth
(pp. 163–191). New York, NY: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-
1-4419-6297-3_7
McNeil, C. B., Costello, A., Travers, R., & Norman, M. (2013). Parent-
child interaction therapy for children traumatized by physical abuse and
neglect. In S. Kimura & A. Miyazaki (Eds.), Physical and emotional
abuse: Triggers, short and long-term, consequences and prevention
methods (pp. 53– 86). Hauppage, New York: Nova.
Mersky, J. P., Topitzes, J., Grant-Savela, S. D., Brondino, M. J., & McNeil,
C. B. (2016). Adapting parent– child interaction therapy to foster care:
Outcomes from a randomized trial. Research on Social Work Practice,
26, 157–167. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1049731514543023
Minuchin, S., Baker, L., Rosman, B. L., Liebman, R., Milman, L., & Todd,
T. C. (1975). A conceptual model of psychosomatic illness in children.
Family organization and family therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry,
32, 1031–1038. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1975.01760260
095008
Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J.,
Harrington, H.,...Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-
control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108,
2693–2698. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010076108
O’Connor, T. G., Matias, C., Futh, A., Tantam, G., & Scott, S. (2013).
Social learning theory parenting intervention promotes attachment-based
caregiving in young children: Randomized clinical trial. Journal of
Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 42, 358 –370. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1080/15374416.2012.723262
O’Leary, K. D., O’Leary, S., & Becker, W. C. (1967). Modification of a
deviant sibling interaction pattern in the home. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 5, 113–120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(67)90005-8
Patterson, G. R. (1982). Coercive family process (Vol. 3). Eugene, OR:
Castalia.
Patterson, G. R., Ray, R. S., & Shaw, D. A. (1968). Direct intervention in
families of deviant children. Oregon Research Institute Research Bul-
letin, 8(No. 9). Eugene, OR: Oregon Research Institute and University of
Oregon.
Patterson, G. R., Reid, J. B., & Eddy, J. M. (2002). A brief history of the
Oregon model. In J. B. Reid, G. R. Patterson, & J. Snyder (Eds.),
Antisocial behavior in children and adolescents: A developmental anal-
ysis and model for intervention (pp. 3–20). Washington, DC: American
Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10468-001
Pearl, E., Thieken, L., Olafson, E., Boat, B., Connelly, L., Barnes, J., &
Putnam, F. (2012). Effectiveness of community dissemination of parent–
child interaction therapy. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research,
Practice, and Policy, 4, 204 –213. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022948
Price, J. M., Chamberlain, P., Landsverk, J., Reid, J. B., Leve, L. D., &
Laurent, H. (2008). Effects of a foster parent training intervention on
placement changes of children in foster care. Child Maltreatment, 13,
64 –75. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1077559507310612
Querido, J. G., Bearss, K., & Eyberg, S. M. (2002). Parent/child interaction
therapy. In F. W. Kaslow & T. Patterson (Eds.), Comprehensive hand-
book of psychotherapy: Cognitive-behavioral approaches (Vol. 2, pp.
91–113). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Quetsch, L., Lieneman, C., & McNeil, C. (2017). The role of time-out in
trauma-informed treatment for young children. Retrieved from http://
societyforpsychotherapy.org/role-time-trauma-informed-treatment-
young-children/
Quetsch, L., Wallace, N., Herschell, A., & McNeil, C. (2015). Weighing in
on the time-out controversy: An empirical perspective. Clinical Psychol-
ogist, 68, 4 –19.
Regalado, M., Sareen, H., Inkelas, M., Wissow, L. S., & Halfon, N. (2004).
Parents’ discipline of young children: Results from the National Survey
of Early Childhood Health. Pediatrics, 113(Suppl.), 1952–1958.
Riley, A. R., Wagner, D. V., Tudor, M. E., Zuckerman, K. E., & Freeman,
K. A. (2017). A survey of parents’ perceptions and use of time-out
compared to empirical evidence. Academic Pediatrics, 17, 168 –175.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.acap.2016.08.004
Risley, T. (2005). Montrose M. Wolf (1935–2004). Journal of Applied Be-
havior Analysis, 38, 279 –287. http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2005.165-04
Roberts, M. W. (1982). The effects of warned versus unwarned time-out
procedures on child noncompliance. Child & Family Behavior Therapy,
4, 37–53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J019v04n01_04
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A
review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual
Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev
.psych.52.1.141
Ryan, R. M., Kalil, A., Ziol-Guest, K. M., & Padilla, C. (2016). Socio-
economic gaps in parents’ discipline strategies from 1988 to 2011.
Pediatrics, 138, e20160720. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-0720
Sanders, M. R., Kirby, J. N., Tellegen, C. L., & Day, J. J. (2014). The
Triple P-Positive Parenting Program: A systematic review and meta-
analysis of a multi-level system of parenting support. Clinical Psychol-
ogy Review, 34, 337–357. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.04.003
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
14 DADDS AND TULLY
Scarboro, M. E., & Forehand, R. (1975). Effects of two types of response-
contingent time-out on compliance and oppositional behavior of chil-
dren. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 19, 252–264. http://dx
.doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(75)90089-2
Scott, S., & Dadds, M. R. (2009). Practitioner review: When parent training
doesn’t work: Theory-driven clinical strategies. Journal of Child Psychol-
ogy and Psychiatry, 50, 1441–1450. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610
.2009.02161.x
Siegel, D., & Bryson, T. P. (2014a, September 23). “Time-outs” are hurting
your child. Time. Retrieved from http://time.com/3404701/discipline-
time-out-is-not-good
Siegel, D., & Bryson, T. P. (2014b, October 21). You said WHAT about
time-outs?! Huffington Post. Retrieved from https://www.huffington
post.com/daniel-j-siegel-md/time-outs-overused_b_6006332.html
Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological
Review, 57, 193–216. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0054367
Southam-Gerow, M. A., & Kendall, P. C. (2002). Emotion regulation and
understanding: Implications for child psychopathology and therapy.
Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 189 –222. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
S0272-7358(01)00087-3
Straus, M. A. (2000). Corporal punishment and primary prevention of
physical abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect, 24, 1109 –1114. http://dx.doi
.org/10.1016/S0145-2134(00)00180-0
Troutman, B. (2015). Integrating behaviorism and attachment theory in
parent coaching. Cham, Switzerland: Springer. http://dx.doi.org/10
.1007/978-3-319-15239-4_3
United Nations Children’s Fund. (2017). A familiar face: Violence in the
lives of children and adolescents. Retrieved from https://www.unicef
.org/publications/files/Violence_in_the_lives_of_children_and_
adolescents.pdf
Uretsky, M. C., & Hoffman, J. A. (2017). Evidence for group-based foster
parent training programs in reducing externalizing child behaviors: A
systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Public Child Welfare,
11, 464 – 486. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15548732.2017.1326360
van der Kolk, B. (2005). Developmental trauma disorder: Toward a ratio-
nal diagnosis of children with complex trauma histories. Psychiatric
Annals, 35, 401– 408. http://dx.doi.org/10.3928/00485713-20050501-06
Vlahovicova, K., Melendez-Torres, G. J., Leijten, P., Knerr, W., & Gard-
ner, F. (2017). Parenting programs for the prevention of child physical
abuse recurrence: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Child
and Family Psychology Review, 20, 351–365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/
s10567-017-0232-7
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher
psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Webster-Stratton, C. (2001). The incredible years: Parents, teachers, and
children training series. Residential Treatment for Children & Youth, 18,
31– 45. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J007v18n03_04
Wolf, M., Risley, T., & Mees, H. (1963). Application of operant condi-
tioning procedures to the behaviour problems of an autistic child. Be-
haviour Research and Therapy, 1, 305–312. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
0005-7967(63)90045-7
Received July 17, 2018
Revision received December 18, 2018
Accepted January 2, 2019 䡲
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
15
TIME-OUT IN THE AGE OF ATTACHMENT AND TRAUMA
A preview of this full-text is provided by American Psychological Association.
Content available from American Psychologist
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.