ArticlePDF Available

Effects of Meditation and Mind-Body Exercises on Older Adults' Cognitive Performance: A Meta-analysis

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background and objectives: Meditation and mind-body exercises are suggested to delay decline or enhance cognitive capabilities in older adults. However, their effectiveness remains uncertain. This study assessed the effectiveness of meditation and mind-body exercises to improve cognition in elderly people aged 60 years or above. Moderator variables were also explored. Research design and methods: A databases search (MEDLINE, EMBASE, CINAHL, PsycINFO, Cochrane Library, Web of Science, CNKI, and Wangfang) was conducted from the first available date to January 10, 2018. Inclusion criteria include (a) human older adults aged 60 years or above, (b) meditation, Tai Chi, Qigong, or yoga intervention, (c) intervention should be structured, (d) inclusion of a control group, (e) at least one outcome measure of cognition was measured at baseline and post-training, and (f) peer-reviewed journal articles in English or Chinese. Results: Forty-one studies (N = 3,551) were included in the meta-analysis. In general, meditation and mind-body exercises improve cognition in the elderly people (SMD = 0.34, 95% CI: 0.19 to 0.48), but the cognition-enhancing effects depend on the type of exercise. In addition, cognitive performance is only improved when the length of intervention is longer than 12 weeks, exercise frequency is 3-7 times/week, or duration of an exercise session is 45-60 min/session. Discussion and implications: This study suggests that meditation and mind-body exercises are effective to improve cognition of older adults aged 60 years or above, and exercise parameters should be considered for intervention planning.
Content may be subject to copyright.
e782
The Gerontologist
cite as: Gerontologist, 2019, Vol. 59, No. 6, e782–e790
doi:10.1093/geront/gnz022
Advance Access publication February 23, 2019
© The Author(s) 2019. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of The Gerontological Society of America. All rights reserved.
For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com.
Review Article
Effects of Meditation and Mind–Body Exercises on Older
Adults’ Cognitive Performance: AMeta-analysis
JohnS.Y. Chan, PhD Kanfeng Deng, MSc, Jiamin Wu, MSc, and JinH. Yan, PhD*
Laboratory of Neuromotor Control and Learning, Shenzhen University, China.
*Address correspondence to: Jin H.Yan, PhD, Laboratory of Neuromotor Control and Learning, Shenzhen University, 3688 Nan Hai Ave, Shenzhen,
Guangdong 518060, P.R. China. E-mail: jhyan@sfsu.edu
Received: October 24, 2018; Editorial Decision Date: January 15, 2019
Decision Editor: Patricia C.Heyn, PhD
Abstract
Background and Objectives: Meditation and mind–body exercises are suggested to delay decline or enhance cognitive
capabilities in older adults. However, their effectiveness remains uncertain. This study assessed the effectiveness of
meditation and mind–body exercises to improve cognition in elderly people aged 60years or above. Moderator variables
were also explored.
Research Design and Methods: A databases search (MEDLINE, EMBASE, CINAHL, PsycINFO, Cochrane Library, Web of
Science, CNKI, and Wangfang) was conducted from the rst available date to January 10, 2018. Inclusion criteria include
(a) human older adults aged 60years or above, (b) meditation, Tai Chi, Qigong, or yoga intervention, (c) intervention
should be structured, (d) inclusion of a control group, (e) at least one outcome measure of cognition was measured at
baseline and post-training, and (f) peer-reviewed journal articles in English or Chinese.
Results: Forty-one studies (N=3,551) were included in the meta-analysis. In general, meditation and mind–body exercises
improve cognition in the elderly people (SMD=0.34, 95% CI: 0.19 to 0.48), but the cognition-enhancing effects depend
on the type of exercise. In addition, cognitive performance is only improved when the length of intervention is longer than
12 weeks, exercise frequency is 3–7 times/week, or duration of an exercise session is 45–60min/session.
Discussion and Implications: This study suggests that meditation and mind–body exercises are effective to improve
cognition of older adults aged 60years or above, and exercise parameters should be considered for intervention planning.
Keywords: Alternative and complementary medicine/care/therapy, Cognition, Dementia, Exercise/physical activity
The world population is aging. The proportion of older
adults aged 60years or above is expected to increase from
12% to 22% between the years 2015 and 2050 (World
Health Organization, 2018). Normal aging is associated not
only with reduced physical health but also with cognitive
decline. Other than healthy aging, some older people may
have some forms of dementia. It is estimated that 47 million
people worldwide are having dementia, and 66 million
people could be affected by the year 2030 (Prince et al.,
2013). Given a large number of affected people and the
great burden imposed on the health care system, a search
for effective interventions to ameliorate cognitive decline
or improve cognitive functions in the older population is of
immense signicance.
Older adults of different cognitive statuses can improve
cognitive functions through exercise participation (Angevaren,
Aufdemkampe, Verhaar, Aleman, & Vanhees, 2008; Groot
etal., 2016). Although cognition is usually reduced in older
adults, abundant research has demonstrated the malleability of
elderly people’s neural system, which enables them to acquire
new skills or restore the affected capabilities (Cai, Chan,
Yan, & Peng, 2014; Wu, Chan, & Yan, 2016). Behavioral
and psychological interventions (including physical exercises
and cognitively stimulating activities) are recommended for
XX
XXXX
XXXX
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/59/6/e782/5363988 by Rijksuniversiteit Groningen user on 10 November 2020
improving cognitive functions of older adults. The association
between physical activity and reduced risks of cognitive
disorders in older adults has been documented (Buchman,
Boyle, Yu, Shah, Wilson, & Bennett, 2012). The benets of
physical activity on cognition are believed to be mediated
by the promotion of neurogenesis, synaptogenesis, and
capillarization (Colcombe & Kramer, 2003), and increases
in brain-derived neurotrophic factor and insulin-like growth
factors (Cotman, Berchtold, & Christie, 2007; Vaynman,
Ying, & Gomez-Pinilla, 2004). These interacting factors
are supposed to contribute to the neuroprotective effects of
exercises on elderly people’s cognition.
Besides traditional physical exercises, there is a growing
number of research into the benets of mind–body exercises
over the past decades. They are usually performed at a slow pace
and low intensity, and thus, are particularly suitable for older
adults (Guo, Shi, Yu, & Qiu, 2016; Taylor-Piliae etal., 2010).
Compared to physical exercises, mind–body exercises have a
higher cognitive demand and emphasize cognitive well-being.
It has been suggested that exercise interventions with a higher
cognitive demand are particularly efcacious to slow down
age-related cognitive decline (Raichlen & Alexander, 2017).
A recent meta-analysis has also shown that a combination
of physical and cognitive activities is ideal for treating or
preventing cognitive decline in older adults (Gheysen et al.,
2018). Tai Chi, Qigong, and yoga are prime examples of
mind–body exercises. Tai Chi is a multicomponent exercise
that trains exercisers’ aerobic, anaerobic, and exibility
capacities. Qigong involves a set of static or dynamic exercises
through coordinated breathing and physical movements to
cultivate one’s internal energy to achieve body healing, and
Baduanjin is one of the most common forms of Qigong (Chen
etal., 2013). Yoga originates from ancient India and includes
practice of postures, breathing, and meditation to support
optimal homeostasis. There is increasing evidence to show
the efcacy of mind–body exercises to improve the cognitive
functions of the elderly people (Gothe & McAuley, 2015; Wu,
Wang, Burgess, & Wu, 2013).
Mind–body exercises usually include meditation as a
part of training. Meditation involves various emotional and
attentional regulatory strategies to achieve cognitive well-
being and emotional balance (Lutz, Dunne, & Davidson,
2006). There is a growing body of literature to suggest that
meditation may benet the cognitive functions in older
adults and those with neurodegenerative diseases (Newberg
etal., 2014), possibly through enhancements of brain regions
related to interoception and attention (Hölzel etal., 2008).
In this study, we investigated if meditation and mind–
body exercises (meditation, Qigong, Tai Chi, yoga) benet
older adults’ cognition via the meta-analytic approach.
Further examinations will be conducted to compare their
effectiveness. To improve prescription of meditation and
mind–body exercises in the future, we also analyzed the
exercise moderators associated with cognitive benets in
older adults. It was hypothesized that, relative to the control
participants, older adults in meditation and mind–body
exercises show cognitive improvement. As suggested in
previous research, different types of exercise may have varying
inuences on different cognitive domains (Voss, Nagamatsu,
Liu-Ambrose, & Kramer, 2011). We hypothesized that
cognitive improvement depends on the type of exercise.
Methods
The systematic review with meta-analysis was conducted
in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for
Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) (Moher,
Liberati, Tetzlaff, & Altman, 2009).
Search Strategy
A computer search of MEDLINE, EMBASE, CINAHL,
PsycINFO, Cochrane Library, and Web of Science was
conducted up to January 10, 2018, using the search phrase,
(Tai Chi OR Tai Ji OR yoga OR Baduanjin OR Qigong OR
meditation) AND (cognition OR attention OR memory OR
executive function). The Chinese equivalent search phrase
was used in the CNKI and Wangfang databases to search
for Chinese articles.
All returned titles were screened by the rst author (J.
S. Y. Chan) to exclude duplicate and irrelevant studies
based on the article title. The abstract of each remaining
study was then independently reviewed by two investigators
(J. Wu and YD), this stage was overinclusive. In case of
disagreement of judgment, the article would be carried to
the next screening stage. Subsequently, the full texts of the
remaining studies were independently reviewed against
the inclusion criteria. The interrater agreement was high
(Cohen’s kappa= 0.78). In this stage, the reviewers had
to discuss with each other to reach a consensus in case of
disagreements. Reviewers also looked at the bibliography
of the included articles and published review articles to
avoid missing potentially relevant studies.
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
Studies were included if they met the following criteria:
(a) studies of human older adults (60 years or older);
(b) participants can be cognitively impaired (e.g., mild
cognitive impairment [MCI] or dementia), but should not
have other neurological (e.g., stroke) and/or mental illnesses
(e.g., depression); (c) a structured exercise program of any
duration, frequency, or intensity; (d) a control group was
included; (e) at least one outcome measure of cognition
was measured at baseline and post-training; and (f) peer-
reviewed articles in English or Chinese.
Data Extraction
Data of the study population, intervention, and outcome
measures were independently extracted with a standardized
The Gerontologist, 2019, Vol. 59, No. 6 e783
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/59/6/e782/5363988 by Rijksuniversiteit Groningen user on 10 November 2020
form by two investigators (K. Deng and CW). For both
intervention and control groups, sample size, mean values
of outcome measure at baseline and post-intervention,
and baseline standard errors were recorded. In addition
to outcomes measures, potential moderators were also
recorded. Type of exercise was categorically coded as
meditation, Qigong, Tai Chi, or yoga. Previous studies have
found impairments in attention (Chao & Knight, 1997),
processing speed (Finkel, Reynolds, McArdle, & Pedersen,
2007), verbal uency (Kempler, Teng, Dick, Taussig,
& Davis, 1998), and memory and executive functions
(Buckner, 2004) in elderly people. Thus, we focused on these
cognitive domains in this study. The cognitive domain was
coded as attention, processing speed, verbal uency, short-
term memory, working memory, executive function, or
global cognition. On the basis of the report in the included
studies, participants’ cognitive status was coded as healthy,
MCI, dementia, or unclear. Three exercise parameters
were recorded (program length, exercise frequency, and
duration). The program length (short: ≤12 weeks; medium:
13–26 weeks; long: >26 weeks), exercise frequency (per
week: low: ≤2; medium: 3–4; high: 5–7), and exercise
duration (per session: short: ≤45 min; medium: >45 to
≤60min; long: >60min) were coded using a prior meta-
analytic review as a guide (Northey, Cherbuin, Pumpa,
Smee, & Rattray, 2018). The mean age of participants, year
of education, and female percentage in a study were also
extracted as continuous moderators.
Risk of Bias Assessment
Two researchers independently assessed the risk of bias
of the included studies with the Cochrane Collaboration
Guidelines across the following domains: randomization;
allocation concealment; blinding (outcome assessor),
incomplete data, selective reporting, and any other risk of
bias. The Physiotherapy Evidence Database (PEDro) scale
was also used to evaluate the risk of bias of the included
studies (www.pedro.org.au). The scale contains 11 items
(yes/no questions), covering the domains of randomization,
concealed allocation, blinding and quality of reporting.
PEDro score for each study was obtained by counting the
number of yes (except for item 1), the score can range from
0 to 10. Aprevious study has suggested that a PEDro score
more than 6 indicates good quality of study (Gonzalez etal.,
2018). In this study, we considered studies with a PEDro
score more than 6 as having a low risk of bias. Interrater
agreement was high using either the Cochrane Collaboration
Guidelines (Cohen’s kappa=0.7) or the PEDro scale (Cohen’s
kappa= 0.72). Discrepancies in the risk of bias assessment
were resolved by discussion between the reviewers.
Statistical Analyses
Cognitive measures were the primary outcomes. To
account for the dependency of outcome measures within
studies, a multilevel random-effects meta-analysis
was conducted in R (version 3.5.0) with the metafor
package (version 2.0-0) using an unrestricted maximum
likelihood estimator (Viechtbauer, 2010). On the basis
of the approach of Becker (1988), standardized mean
difference (SMD) was computed as the effect size using
formulae 1–3, where gT and gC are the standardized
mean change for the intervention and control groups,
respectively.
X
pre
-
intervention,
X
post
-
intervention,
SD
pre
-intervention
,
and n indicate the pre-intervention and post-intervention
means, pre-intervention SD, and sample size, respectively.
c
(
n1
)=
2
/(
n1
) Γ [(
n1
)/
2
]/Γ[ (
n2
)/
2]
is a bias-correction factor. The formulae assume that
the pre-intervention and post-intervention variances
are homogeneous. A positive SMD indicates a greater
cognitive benet in the experimental group, relative to the
controlgroup.
g
T=c(nT1)
Ã
Xpost-intervention,T
Xpre-intervention,T
SDpre-intervention,T
(1)
gC=c(nC1)
Ã
Xpost-intervention,C
Xpre-intervention,C
SDpre-intervention,C
(2)
SMD =gTgC
(3)
Cochran’s Q test was used to assess heterogeneity. In
addition, I2 (proportion of variance due to heterogeneity)
was also computed to quantify the extent of heterogeneity
(Higgins & Thompson, 2002). Separate models were tted
to determine the main effects of different moderators.
To gain better insights into this issue, we also examined
the interaction of type of exercise and cognitive domain.
Statistical signicance was determined by omnibus
tests. Exploratory analyses of the moderating effects of
mean age of participants, year of education, and female
percentage were conducted by meta-regression. To evaluate
the inuence of risk of bias on the meta-analytic results,
separate random-effects models were tted for studies of
low risk of bias (PEDro score >6) and studies of high risk
of bias (PEDro score ≤6).
Publication bias was assessed with Egger’s regression. In
Egger’s regression, a signicant deviation of the y-intercept
from zero might indicate the presence of publication bias.
In addition, a Rosenthal fail-safe N test was also conducted
to estimate the number of unpublished null studies required
to nullify the overall effects at the 95% level.
Results
The PRISMA owchart was presented in Figure 1. In the
initial search, 5,113 records were returned of which 427
studies were retrieved for full-text reading. Eventually,
41 studies met all the inclusion criteria for quantitative
The Gerontologist, 2019, Vol. 59, No. 6
e784
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/59/6/e782/5363988 by Rijksuniversiteit Groningen user on 10 November 2020
analyses, involving 3,551 participants. The included studies
can be grouped by the type of exercise into meditation
(k=11), Qigong (k=9), Tai Chi (k=14), and yoga (k=9).
Characteristics of the included studies are summarized in
Supplementary Table 1 and a forest plot of the included
studies was presented in Supplementary Figure 1. As was
observed in Supplementary Table 1 and Supplementary
Figure 1, studies of a large sample size usually have a
narrower condence interval(CI).
Risk ofBias
Risk of bias of the included studies is shown in Figure 2.
Most of the studies have low risks in sequence generation,
incomplete outcome data, selective reporting, and other
bias. However, in terms of allocation concealment and
blinding, the risks are unclear in most of the included
studies. Risk of bias of each included study is summarized
in Supplementary Tables 2 and 3. On the basis of the PEDro
scale score, 78% of the included studies can be classied as
having a low risk ofbias.
Effects of Meditation and Mind–Body Exercises
on Cognition
Results of this meta-analysis show that meditation and
mind–body exercises, relative to controls, improve cognition
in the elderly people (SMD=0.34, 95% CI: 0.19 to 0.48,
p < .001), and there is signicant heterogeneity across
studies (Q(152)= 604.30, p < .001). Additional analyses
showed that the pooled effect size was lower for studies of
low risk of bias (SMD=0.27, 95% CI: 0.12 to 0.43) than
for studies of high risk of bias (SMD=0.59, 95% CI: 0.26
to 0.93) (Supplementary Figure 2). This suggests that an
inclusion of studies of high risk of bias may inate the pooled
effect size but not change the conclusion (i.e., meditation
and mind–body exercises have small-to-moderate effects to
improve cognition in the elderly people).
The funnel plot appears asymmetrical (Figure 3),
however, Egger’s regression indicates an absence of funnel
plot asymmetry (p=.26). According to previous simulation
results (Fragkos, Tsagris, & Frangos, 2014), for a meta-
analysis of 41 studies, the threshold of fail-safe N is 369.
The estimated fail-safe N (12,699) is far above the threshold.
On the basis of the Egger’s regression and fail-safe N, the
meta-analytic results should be robust to publication bias. I2
indicates that heterogeneity is high across studies (80.5%).
Moderator Analysis
To investigate the potential sources of heterogeneity,
moderators were analyzed in separate models. Results of
Figure 1. Study selection flowchart.
Figure 2. Risk of bias of the included studies.
Figure 3. Funnel plot for visual inspection of publication bias.
The Gerontologist, 2019, Vol. 59, No. 6 e785
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/59/6/e782/5363988 by Rijksuniversiteit Groningen user on 10 November 2020
moderator analyses are presented in Table 1. Although
separate analyses were conducted for different moderators,
heterogeneity remains high (I2 > 70%). This may implicate
that the high heterogeneity observed is attributed to an
interaction of multiple moderators or related to factors not
considered in the moderator analyses.
Exercise moderators
Concerning the type of exercise, only meditation (p < .001)
and Qigong (p= .001) produce signicant positive effects
on cognition. Studies, where the program length is medium
to high, have shown signicant cognitive improvement
(p < .001). Only medium-to-high exercise frequency is
related to cognitive improvement (p < .02). In terms of
exercise duration, only medium duration is associated with
positive effects on cognition (p < .001), whereas short and
long durations produce nonsignicant effects on cognition
(p > .34).
Cognitive moderators
The effect of meditation and mind–body exercises on
cognition is signicant for all cognitive domains (p < .01),
except processing speed (p=.06). The cognition-enhancing
effects are only observed in studies of participants with
MCI (p < .001) or unclear cognitive status (p = .006).
Furthermore, there is a signicant interaction between
type of exercise and cognitive domain (p < .001) (Table 2),
suggesting that the effectiveness of different types of
Table 1. Results of Moderator Analyses
Moderator
No. of
effect sizes
Estimated mean SMD
(95% CI) p value Q statistic I2
Omnibus test of
moderators
Type of exercise Q(149)=447.3,
p < .001
72.4% Q(4)=32.8, p < .001
Meditation 45 0.41 (0.16 to 0.65) .001
Qigong 21 0.72 (0.41 to 1.03) <.001
Tai Chi 48 0.19 (−0.03 to 0.41) .09
Yoga 39 0.15 (−0.13 to 0.44) .3
Cognitive domain Q(146)=577.5,
p < .001
79.6% Q(7)=26.5, p < .001
Attention 6 0.33 (0.01 to 0.65) .04
Executive function 32 0.39 (0.21 to 0.56) <.001
Global cognition 23 0.42 (0.24 to 0.61) <0001
Processing speed 6 0.36 (−0.02 to 0.74) .06
Short-term memory 32 0.28 (0.10 to 0.46) .002
Verbal uency 17 0.27 (0.06 to 0.48) .01
Working memory 37 0.27 (0.10 to 0.44) .002
Cognitive status Q(149)=592.5,
p < .001
79.9% Q(4)=25.8, p < .001
Healthy 21 0.17 (−0.20 to 0.53) .4
MCI 53 0.53 (0.28 to 0.78) <.001
Dementia 4 −0.14 (−1.08 to 0.79) .8
Unclear 75 0.29 (0.08 to 0.49) .006
Program length Q(150)=555.7,
p < .001
79% Q(3)=25.4, p < .001
Short (≤12 weeks) 42 0.14 (−0.15 to 0.44) .4
Medium (13–26 weeks) 82 0.36 (0.17 to 0.55) <.001
Long (>26 weeks) 29 0.72 (0.38 to 1.05) <.001
Exercise frequency Q(146)=540.4,
p < .001
78.8% Q(3)=23.7, p < .001
Low (≤2) 37 0.22 (−0.04 to 0.48) .1
Medium (3–4) 60 0.29 (0.04 to 0.55) .02
High (5–7) 52 0.50 (0.25 to 0.74) <.001
Exercise duration Q(131)=425.0,
p < .001
Q(3)=29.2, p < .001
Short (≤45min) 54 0.03 (−0.20 to 0.27) .8
Medium (>45 to ≤60min) 67 0.51 (0.32 to 0.70) <.001
Long (>60min) 13 0.21 (−0.22 to 0.65) .3
Note: CI=condence interval; MCI=mild cognitive impairment; SMD=standardized mean difference.
The Gerontologist, 2019, Vol. 59, No. 6
e786
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/59/6/e782/5363988 by Rijksuniversiteit Groningen user on 10 November 2020
exercise may vary in different cognitive domains. It is shown
that meditation improves attention, global cognition, and
working memory (moderate-to-large effects). Qigong
improves global cognition, short-term memory, and
working memory (moderate-to-large effects). Tai Chi
improves executive function and global cognition (small
effects). Yoga improves executive function (small effect)
and processing speed (large effect). There is no signicant
difference of change in executive function following Tai
Chi and Yoga interventions (p = .89). In addition, the
change in global cognition following meditation does not
differ from those following Qigong (p = .54) or Tai Chi
(p= .14). However, the improvement in global cognition
after Qigong intervention is greater than that after Tai Chi
intervention (p = .04). The changes in working memory
following meditation and Qigong interventions do not
differ signicantly (p=.95).
Participant moderators
Supplementary Table 4 shows the meta-regression results.
There are nonsignicant effects of mean age of participants
(β=−0.01, 95% CI: −0.04 to 0.01, p = .20) and year of
education (β = −0.02, 95% CI: −0.05 to 0.01, p= .21)
on the estimated effect size. However, the percentage of
female participants in a study is negatively associated
with the estimated effect size (β=−0.01, 95% CI: −0.02
to 0, p = .01), suggesting that a 1% increase in female
participants is associated with a reduction of estimated
effect size by 0.01. It is important to note that, when year
of education is included as a moderator, heterogeneity
becomes small (I2=33.9%), suggesting that difference of
participants’ education level is the main contributor to the
heterogeneity across studies.
Discussion
Effectiveness of Meditation and Mind–Body
Exercises on Older Adults’ Cognition
We examined the effectiveness of meditation and different
mind–body exercises on elderly peoples’ cognitive functions
through meta-analytic methods. In general, meditation
and mind–body exercises enhance older adults’ cognitive
performance, except processing speed. Meditation and
mind–body exercises are particularly benecial for elderly
people with MCI. However, this does not mean that
meditation and mind–body exercises do not affect cognitive
performance of participants of other cognitive statuses, as
most of the included studies did not explicitly mention the
cognitive status of participants. Because of this, it would be
inappropriate to conclude that meditation and mind–body
exercises favor older adults of a certain cognitive status
over the others at thisstage.
Although meditation and mind–body exercises can
improve older adults’ cognition in general, follow-up
moderator analyses indicate that only meditation and
Qigong benet cognitive performance. This means that
meditation and Qigong should be considered if our aim is
to boost the general cognitive performance in the elderly
people. Further analyses show an interaction between
type of exercise and cognitive domain, suggesting that the
cognition-enhancing effects depend on the type of exercise.
Specically, meditation can improve attention, global
cognition, and working memory. Qigong improves global
cognition, short-term memory, and working memory. Tai
Chi improves executive function and global cognition.
Yoga improves executive function and processing speed. It
has been previously shown that meditation and mind–body
exercises can have different impacts on brain structures
Table 2. The Effects of Different Types of Mind–Body
Exercise on Cognition
No. of
effect sizes
Estimated mean
SMD (95% CI) p value
Attention
Meditation 2 1.01 (0.20 to 1.81) .01
Qigong 1 0.53 (−0.12 to 1.19) .11
Tai Chi 0 NA NA
Yoga 3 0.20 (−0.23 to 0.62) .4
Executive function
Meditation 12 0.29 (−0.02 to 0.61) .07
Qigong 1 0.86 (−0.11 to 1.82) .08
Tai Chi 9 0.30 (0.04 to 0.56) .03
Yoga 10 0.33 (0.03 to 0.63) .03
Global cognition
Mediation 5 0.56 (0.21 to 0.92) .002
Qigong 9 0.72 (0.39 to 1.05) <.0001
Tai Chi 9 0.28 (0.02 to 0.53) .03
Yoga 0 NA NA
Processing speed
Meditation 5 −0.05 (−0.52 to 0.42) .8
Qigong 0 NA NA
Tai Chi 0 NA NA
Yoga 1 0.99 (0.36 to 1.63) .002
Short-term memory
Meditation 7 0.24 (−0.10 to 0.58) .2
Qigong 6 0.86 (0.48 to 1.24) <.0001
Tai Chi 7 0.25 (−0.04 to 0.53) .09
Yoga 12 −0.01 (−0.31 to 0.29) .9
Verbal uency
Meditation 9 0.24 (−0.10 to 0.57) .2
Qigong 0 NA NA
Tai Chi 6 0.15 (−0.13 to 0.44) .3
Yoga 2 0.24 (−0.22 to 0.70) .3
Working memory
Meditation 5 0.55 (0.18 to 0.93) .004
Qigong 4 0.54 (0.12 to 0.95) .01
Tai Chi 17 0.19 (−0.05 to 0.43) .1
Yoga 11 0.07 (−0.23 to 0.37) .6
Note: NA=not available; SMD=standardized mean difference.
The Gerontologist, 2019, Vol. 59, No. 6 e787
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/59/6/e782/5363988 by Rijksuniversiteit Groningen user on 10 November 2020
and functions (Fox etal., 2014; Gothe, Hayes, Temali, &
Damoiseaux, 2018; Tao etal., 2017), and this may result
in the differential benets observed. Thus, if an older
adult wants to improve a specic cognitive ability, he or
she should take part in the corresponding exercise. For
instance, meditation, rather than other mind/mind–body
exercises, should be chosen for improving attention.
Besides directly affecting brain structures and functions,
mind–body exercises may inuence cognition through
changing one’s tness level. Tai Chi and yoga usually
have a higher level of physical activity than meditation
and Qigong. As suggested in previous reports, improved
aerobic tness can be associated with enhanced executive
functions and reduced age-related neurodegeneration
(Colcombe & Kramer, 2003), and this is consistent to
the present results that only Tai Chi and yoga improve
executive functions.
Exercise Prescription
Analyses of the moderating effects of exercise parameters
may help prescribe meditation and mind–body exercises
optimally. It is shown that cognitive performance is only
improved when the length of exercise program is medium
to long (>12 weeks), exercise frequency is medium to high
(3–7 times/week), or duration of an exercise session is
medium (>45 to ≤60min/session).
The present results are similar to those in a previous
meta-analysis on the effectiveness of physical exercise on
elderly cognition. In that study, a medium exercise duration
is associated with cognitive benets in older adults
(Northey etal., 2018). That study also reveals that cognitive
benets are independent of intervention length and exercise
frequency; however, we found cognitive benets only when
the exercise frequency and intervention length are at least
medium. Apossible reason of such discrepancies may be
due to the fact that physical exercises are more physically
demanding and can improve one’s physical tness more
readily, thus, inducing more prominent changes in the brain
in the physical exercisers (Colcombe & Kramer, 2003).
Limitations and Future Research
Several limitations have been noted, which can be
improved in future studies. Heterogeneity is signicant
even after moderator analyses. Because of the nature of
meditation and mind–body exercises, it is conceivable
that variations within each type of exercise contribute a
signicant amount of heterogeneity. Currently, there is a
limited number of studies into different subtypes of mind/
mind–body exercise; hence, it would be difcult to discern
and compare their effectiveness. In addition, based on I2
results, we found that participants’ education level may
be the main source of heterogeneity across studies. Thus,
future researchers should consider reporting sample’s
educationlevel.
In addition, the quality of the included studies is usually
moderate, and the methods of allocation concealment
and blinding are usually unclear (Figure 2). Allocation
concealment is to ensure that the assignment of participants
is not foreseeable by investigators and/or participants. In
case of a breach of allocation concealment, investigators
may recruit participants they prefer, and participants
may decide to participate in the study or not based on
the allocation results (self-selection bias). Blinding of
outcome assessor is to ensure that the outcome assessor
is not aware of the group assignment of a participant,
such that he or she can objectively record the outcome
measures. The risk of bias assessment has shown that
allocation concealment and blinding of outcome assessors
are seldom reported in studies on meditation and mind–
body exercises. Selection and detection biases may affect
the accuracy and generalizability of results. Therefore, it is
important for researchers to avoid these biases and report
their procedures of allocation concealment and blinding in
greater detail in the future.
Furthermore, most of the included studies did not
report participants’ cognitive status. Conceivably, cognitive
status has signicant impacts on one’s baseline cognitive
performance and room for improvement, thus, the
cognitive status of participants should be assessed and
clearly stated. In order to have a more comprehensive
understanding of the effectiveness of meditation and body–
mind exercises, future research may compare the change of
performance in participants of different cognitive statuses
after interventions.
The scope of this study is limited to “traditional”
mediation and mind–body exercises. In recent years,
mindfulness interventions (e.g., mindfulness-based stress
reduction) have been proposed to improve emotional
disturbances (Baer, 2003). As mindfulness interventions
and other mind/mind–body exercises are similar, we would
expect mindfulness interventions to be an option for
improving older people’s cognition; however, research on
this topic is scarce and more effort should be invested in
this area of study in the future.
Conclusions
This study used a multilevel design to account for the
dependency of effect sizes within studies and examined
the effects of mind/mind–body exercises on cognitive
improvement in the elderly people. In addition, the effects
of exercise length, duration, and frequency on cognitive
improvement were also explored. This knowledge informs
us how to optimally prescribe these activities to the elderly
people for maximal benets. In summary, meditation and
mind–body exercises improve cognition of older adults
aged 60 years or above, and the cognition-enhancing
effects depend on the type of exercise. To obtain optimal
effectiveness, intervention length and exercise frequency
should be at least medium, and the duration of an exercise
The Gerontologist, 2019, Vol. 59, No. 6
e788
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/59/6/e782/5363988 by Rijksuniversiteit Groningen user on 10 November 2020
session should be medium. Meditation and mind–body
exercises offer an accessible and effective means to improve
cognition in the elderly people, and possibly counteract
against the age-related cognitive decline. Given that
meditation and mind–body exercises have a low physical
demand, they are particularly suitable for older adults who
have reduced physical ability and/or medical conditions not
permitting them from practicing high-intensity exercises.
As there is high heterogeneity across the included studies,
more high-quality studies should be included in the future
to ascertain the efcacy of meditation and mind–body
exercises.
Supplementary Material
Supplementary data are available at The Gerontologist
online.
Funding
This work was supported by the Natural Science Foundation of
Shenzhen University and the Knowledge Innovation Program of
Shenzhen (grant number JCYJ20170302143406192).
Acknowledgment
We thank Chuqian Wei and Yingyi Deng for their assistance in
literature search and screening.
Conflict of Interest
We have no conict of interest to declare.
References
Angevaren, M., Aufdemkampe,G., Verhaar, H. J., Aleman,A., &
Vanhees,L. (2008). Physical activity and enhanced tness to
improve cognitive function in older people without known
cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database of Systematic
Reviews, 3: CD005381. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD005381.
pub3
Baer, R.A. (2003). Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention:
A conceptual and empirical review. Clinical Psychology, 10,
125–143. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bpg015
Becker, B. J. (1988). Synthesizing standardized mean-change
measures. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical
Psychology, 41, 257–278. doi:10.1111/j.2044–8317.1988.
tb00901.x
Buchman,A. S., Boyle,P.A., Yu,L., Shah,R. C., Wilson,R.S., &
Bennett,D.A. (2012). Total daily physical activity and the risk
of AD and cognitive decline in older adults. Neurology, 78,
1323–1329. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182535d35
Buckner, R. L. (2004). Memory and executive function in aging
and AD: Multiple factors that cause decline and reserve
factors that compensate. Neuron, 44, 195–208. doi:10.1016/j.
neuron.2004.09.006
Cai,L., Chan,J.S., Yan,J. H., & Peng,K. (2014). Brain plasticity
and motor practice in cognitive aging. Frontiers in Aging
Neuroscience, 6, 31. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2014.00031
Chao,L.L., & Knight,R.T. (1997). Prefrontal decits in attention
and inhibitory control with aging. Cerebral Cortex (New York,
N.Y.: 1991), 7, 63–69. doi:10.1093/cercor/7.1.63
Chen,M., He,M., Min,X., Pan,A., Zhang,X., Yao,P., . . . Wu,T.
(2013). Different physical activity subtypes and risk of metabolic
syndrome in middle-aged and older Chinese people. PLoS ONE,
8: e53258. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0053258
Colcombe,S., & Kramer,A.F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive
function of older adults: Ameta-analytic study. Psychological
Science, 14, 125–130. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430
Cotman,C.W., Berchtold,N.C., & Christie,L.A. (2007). Exercise
builds brain health: Key roles of growth factor cascades
and inammation. Trends in Neurosciences, 30, 464–472.
doi:10.1016/j.tins.2007.06.011
Finkel, D., Reynolds, C. A., McArdle, J. J., & Pedersen, N. L.
(2007). Age changes in processing speed as a leading indicator
of cognitive aging. Psychology and Aging, 22, 558–568.
doi:10.1037/0882-7974.22.3.558
Fox,K.C., Nijeboer,S., Dixon,M. L., Floman,J.L., Ellamil,M.,
Rumak,S.P., . . . Christoff,K. (2014). Is meditation associated
with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-
analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation
practitioners. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 43,
48–73. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.016
Fragkos,K.C., Tsagris,M., & Frangos,C.C. (2014). Publication
bias in meta-analysis: Condence intervals for Rosenthal’s fail-
safe number. International Scholarly Research Notices, 2014,
825383. doi:10.1155/2014/825383
Gheysen, F., Poppe, L., DeSmet, A., Swinnen, S., Cardon, G.,
De Bourdeaudhuij, I., . . . Fias, W. (2018). Physical activity
to improve cognition in older adults: Can physical activity
programs enriched with cognitive challenges enhance the effects?
Asystematic review and meta-analysis. International Journal of
Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 15, 63. doi:10.1186/
s12966-018-0697-x
Gonzalez,G.Z., Moseley,A.M., Maher, C.G., Nascimento,D.P.,
Costa, L. D. C. M., & Costa, L. O. (2018). Methodologic
quality and statistical reporting of physical therapy randomized
controlled trials relevant to musculoskeletal conditions.
Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 99, 129–136.
doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2017.08.485
Gothe,N.P., Hayes,J.M., Temali,C., & Damoiseaux,J.S. (2018).
Differences in brain structure and function among yoga
practitioners and controls. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience,
12, 26. doi:10.3389/fnint.2018.00026
Gothe,N. P., & McAuley,E. (2015). Yoga and cognition: Ameta-
analysis of chronic and acute effects. Psychosomatic Medicine,
77, 784–797. doi:10.1097/PSY.0000000000000218
Groot,C., Hooghiemstra,A.M., Raijmakers,P.G., vanBerckel,B.N.,
Scheltens,P., Scherder, E.J., . . . Ossenkoppele,R. (2016). The
effect of physical activity on cognitive function in patients with
dementia: Ameta-analysis of randomized control trials. Ageing
Research Reviews, 25, 13–23. doi:10.1016/j.arr.2015.11.005
Guo, Y., Shi, H., Yu, D., & Qiu, P. (2016). Health benets of
traditional Chinese sports and physical activity for older adults:
The Gerontologist, 2019, Vol. 59, No. 6 e789
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/59/6/e782/5363988 by Rijksuniversiteit Groningen user on 10 November 2020
A systematic review of evidence. Journal of Sport and Health
Science, 5, 270–280. doi:10.1016/j.jshs.2016.07.002
Higgins,J.P., & Thompson,S.G. (2002). Quantifying heterogeneity
in a meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 21, 1539–1558.
doi:10.1002/sim.1186
Hölzel,B.K., Ott,U., Gard,T., Hempel,H., Weygandt,M., Morgen,K.,
& Vaitl, D. (2008). Investigation of mindfulness meditation
practitioners with voxel-based morphometry. Social Cognitive and
Affective Neuroscience, 3, 55–61. doi:10.1093/scan/nsm038
Kempler, D., Teng,E. L., Dick,M., Taussig, I. M., & Davis,D.S.
(1998). The effects of age, education, and ethnicity on verbal
uency. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society,
4, 531–538. doi:10.1017/S1355617798466013
Lutz, A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson,R. J. (2006). Meditation and
the neuroscience of consciousness: An introduction. In P.Zelazo,
M.Moscovitch, & E. Thompson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook
of consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Moher,D., Liberati,A., Tetzlaff,J., & Altman,D.G.; PRISMA Group.
(2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-
analyses: The PRISMA statement. Annals of Internal Medicine, 151,
264–269, W64. doi:10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135
Newberg,A.B., Serruya,M., Wintering,N., Moss,A.S., Reibel,D.,
& Monti, D. A. (2014). Meditation and neurodegenerative
diseases. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1307,
112–123. doi:10.1111/nyas.12187
Northey, J. M., Cherbuin, N., Pumpa, K. L., Smee, D. J., &
Rattray,B. (2018). Exercise interventions for cognitive function
in adults older than 50: Asystematic review with meta-analysis.
British Journal of Sports Medicine, 52, 154–160. doi:10.1136/
bjsports-2016–096587
Prince, M., Bryce, R., Albanese, E., Wimo, A., Ribeiro, W.,
& Ferri, C. P. (2013). The global prevalence of dementia:
Asystematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimer’s & Dementia,
9, 63–75.e2. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2012.11.007
Raichlen,D.A., & Alexander,G.E. (2017). Adaptive capacity: An
evolutionary neuroscience model linking exercise, cognition,
and brain health. Trends in Neurosciences, 40, 408–421.
doi:10.1016/j.tins.2017.05.001
Tao,J., Liu,J., Liu,W., Huang,J., Xue,X., Chen,X., . . . Kong,J.
(2017). Tai Chi Chuan and Baduanjin increase grey matter
volume in older adults: A brain imaging study. Journal of
Alzheimer’s Disease, 60, 389–400. doi:10.3233/JAD-170477
Taylor-Piliae,R.E., Newell,K.A., Cherin,R., Lee,M.J., King,A.C.,
& Haskell,W.L. (2010). Effects of Tai Chi and Western exercise
on physical and cognitive functioning in healthy community-
dwelling older adults. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity,
18, 261–279. doi:10.1123/japa.18.3.261
Vaynman,S., Ying,Z., & Gomez-Pinilla, F. (2004). Hippocampal
BDNF mediates the efcacy of exercise on synaptic plasticity
and cognition. European Journal of neuroscience, 20, 2580–
2590. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03720.x
Viechtbauer, W. (2010). Conducting meta-analyses in R with
the metafor package. Journal Statistical Software, 36: 48.
doi:10.18637/jss.v036.i03
Voss,M.W., Nagamatsu,L. S., Liu-Ambrose,T., & Kramer,A.F.
(2011). Exercise, brain, and cognition across the life span.
Journal of applied physiology (Bethesda, Md.: 1985), 111,
1505–1513. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.00210.2011
World Health Organization (2018). Ageing and health. Retrieved from
http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs404/en/ (accessed
July 5, 2018).
Wu,Q., Chan,J.S., & Yan,J.H. (2016). Mild cognitive impairment
affects motor control and skill learning. Reviews in the
Neurosciences, 27, 197–217. doi:10.1515/revneuro-2015-0020
Wu,Y., Wang,Y., Burgess, E. O., & Wu,J. (2013). The effects of
Tai Chi exercise on cognitive function in older adults: Ameta-
analysis. Journal of Sport and Health Science, 2, 193–203.
doi:10.1016/j.jshs.2013.09.001
The Gerontologist, 2019, Vol. 59, No. 6
e790
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/gerontologist/article/59/6/e782/5363988 by Rijksuniversiteit Groningen user on 10 November 2020
... To the best of our knowledge, there are four extant reviews that cover the effects of mindfulness interventions on cognition in older adults, all focusing on adults with MCI or AD (Berk et al., 2016;Chan et al., 2019;Farhang et al., 2019;Gard et al., 2014); one of those (Chan et al., 2019) is a meta-analysis combining mindfulness interventions with body-mind interventions. We extend on the previous reviews by including and comparing data on healthy older adults, which has not been done in the past. ...
... To the best of our knowledge, there are four extant reviews that cover the effects of mindfulness interventions on cognition in older adults, all focusing on adults with MCI or AD (Berk et al., 2016;Chan et al., 2019;Farhang et al., 2019;Gard et al., 2014); one of those (Chan et al., 2019) is a meta-analysis combining mindfulness interventions with body-mind interventions. We extend on the previous reviews by including and comparing data on healthy older adults, which has not been done in the past. ...
... The PEDro score for each study is calculated by adding the number of "yes" responses (excluding item 1). Two previous studies have suggested that a PEDro score of more than 6 indicates a good quality study (Chan et al., 2019;Gonzalez et al., 2018;Zhang et al., 2018). In this meta-analysis, we considered studies with a PEDro score equal to and more than 6 as having a low risk of bias and good quality, a score of 4 and 5 having fair quality, and a score less than 4 having poor quality and a high risk of bias (Zhang et al., 2018). ...
Article
Objectives: To examine the effect of mindfulness interventions on cognitive tasks in healthy older adults and older adults with diagnoses of MCI or dementia. Methods: Three-level meta-analysis and systematic review of 30 published randomized controlled trials. Results: Mindfulness interventions provided a small, yet significant positive effect on cognition compared to a control group (average weighted Hedges' g = 0.36, 95% confidence interval (CI) [0.06 to 0.65]). Attention (g = 0.22, 95% CI = [0.09, 0.35]), long-term memory (g = 0.32, 95% CI = [0.08, 0.56]), and visuospatial processing (g = 0.22, 95% CI = [0.10, 0.34]) all showed significantly meaningful changes regardless of cognitive status of the participants. There was no evidence for publication bias. Healthy older adults showed higher effect sizes than those with MCI (g = 0.27, 95% CI = [0.11, 0.43], vs. (g = -0.09, 95% CI = [-0.35, 0.17], resp.). Otherwise, there were no significant moderating effects of age, marital status, education, region, intervention type, length, number of sessions, adherence, or gender on effect size. Moderator analyses within cognitive domains suggest that focused attention practices might be the best for improving cognition. Lastly, meditation interventions seem to work as effectively as other mind-body interventions, but not as effectively as other interventions to improve cognitive functioning in older adults. Discussion: Mindfulness interventions appear to be a useful tool for improving cognitive functioning in older adults.
... [67] Therefore, meditative and mind-body activities enhance cognitive function and dementia prevention through neural, biological, and behavioral health strategies in older adults. [67,68] 3.3.4. Practice mindfulness. ...
Article
Full-text available
The number of older adults with dementia is predicted to markedly increase in the coming decades. A person suffers from dementia every 3 seconds globally, and one out of every 7 people suffers from dementia in Taiwan. The purpose of this narrative review is to integrate existing concepts of dementia prevention into health promotion and improve older adults’ quality of life. This narrative review was performed using the PubMed database by searching for basic research and systematic reviews on dementia prevention and health promotion among older adults. We established a framework for dementia prevention and health promotion with regard to the physical, mental, spiritual, and social health aspects. We identified the following strategies related to older adults towards dementia prevention and health promotion in follows: Physical health promotion: cognitive activities, physical activities, body mass index, balanced diet, rainbow diet, Mediterranean diet, dietary approaches to stop hypertension diet, mind diet, no smoking and drinking, avoiding the “three highs” (i.e., hyperglycemia, hyperlipidemia, and hypertension), and head trauma; Mental health promotion: Positive thinking, Brief Symptom Rating Scale (BSRS-5), depression scale, and ascertained dementia 8 questionnaire (AD8) screening; Spiritual health promotion: religious beliefs, spiritual music, meditative activities, mindfulness, yoga, Qi-gong, Tai-chi, and Baduanjin; and Social health promotion: A supportive family system, socialization, social support, social networks, social interaction, and social participation. The conclusion of this narrative review was to integrate the concepts of dementia prevention and health promotion among older adults.
... Findings from systematic reviews and meta-analyses further corroborate this evidence, with support found for MBIs' ability to enhance positive mood, decrease symptoms of anxiety and depression, improve sleep quality and decrease chronic insomnia (Hazlett-Stevens et al., 2019). Additionally, meta-analytic and review findings indicate substantial support for the beneficial effects of MBIs on memory, and executive functioning, in particular, processing speed, sustained attention accuracy, and subjective attentional control (Chan et al., 2019;Hazlett-Stevens et al., 2019;Zainal & Newman, 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
The global population is aging at an unprecedented rate, increasing the necessity for effective interventions targeting the mental health needs of older adults. Technology addressing the aging process of older adults (i.e., gerontechnology) is an avenue for the efficient delivery of programs that enhance adult well-being. Virtual reality (VR) is a type of gerontechnology with the potential to improve mental health and well-being (e.g., by increasing resilience, mindfulness, compassion, connection with nature, and decreasing stress, depression, anxiety); however, evidence in this area is currently lacking and more rigorous research on the acceptability, feasibility, and effectiveness of mental health programming via VR for older adults, such as nature, mindfulness, or compassion-based interventions, is necessary. The present literature review: 1) explores, synthesizes, and critically evaluates the literature on older adult mental health, well-being and gerontechnology, with a focus on virtual reality-based nature, mindfulness, and compassion-based interventions; 2) examines research to date on the relationship between virtual reality technology and nature, mindfulness, and self-compassion; 3) identifies gaps, contradictions, and limitations of existing research; 4) identifies areas for further investigation; and 5) discusses implications for research and clinical practice.
... Frente a estos cambios, existen tres factores del estilo de vida que desempeñan un papel importante en la desaceleración del deterioro cognitivo y la prevención de la demencia: las redes sociales, las actividades cognitivas de ocio y la actividad física regular (18) . En este sentido, el ejercicio mentecuerpo, muestra beneficios significativos sobre el rendimiento cognitivo, la cognición global, las funciones ejecutivas, el aprendizaje, la memoria y el lenguaje (19) . Así, la meditación y los ejercicios mente -cuerpo mejoran la cognición en las personas mayores. ...
Article
Full-text available
Obesity and overweight in older adults are associated with an increased risk of cognitive diseases, contributing to decreased functional performance. The objective of this review is to analyze the effect of exercise on memory in older adults with obesity or overweight. The methodology uses the words MESH: Population = aged, overweight, obesity; Intervention = exercise; Comparison = control groups; Result = Memory; Design: randomized controlled trial. Searching until December 2021 in PUBMED, SCIELO, BVSALUD, LILACS, TRIP DATABASE, and MEDLINE. Six thousand eight hundred thirty-two items are identified. We analyzed five studies with 214 participants. The mean body mass index is 28.6 ± 1.1 kg/m2, and the initial average Mini mental is 23.6 ± 2.1 points. The intervention with exercise is from twelve to twenty-six weeks. In the random-effects model, exercise improves the mean Mini Mental by MD = 2.6 points (1.7 to 3.4) p < 0.01. Heterogeneity (87.7 %). Exercise improves cognitive function (memory) in older adults who are overweight or obese.
... Interventional-based research comparing the effects of meditation practice on operation span test performance found improved working memory in young adults (Mrazek et al., 2013). The benefits of meditation on working memory have been reported in one recent meta-analytic study of cognitively healthy and impaired older adults aged 60 years or above (Chan et al., 2019), suggesting the potential benefits of mindfulness training for improving working memory capacity in older adults. Additionally, the beneficial effects of meditation on episodic memory have been observed (Van Vugt et al., 2012;Basso et al., 2019;Nyhus et al., 2019), suggesting the potential benefits of meditation for improving episodic memory capacity. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Aging is associated with cognitive decline, increased risk for dementia, and deterioration of brain function. Modifiable lifestyle factors (e.g., exercise, meditation, and social interaction) have been proposed to benefit memory and brain function. However, previous studies have focused on a single exercise modality or a single lifestyle factor. Consequently, the effect of a more comprehensive exercise program that combines multiple exercise modalities and lifestyle factors, as well as examines potential mediators and moderators, on cognitive function and brain health in late middle-aged and older adults remains understudied. This study's primary aim is to examine the effect of a multi-domain exercise intervention on memory and brain function in cognitively healthy late middle-aged and older adults. In addition, we will examine whether apolipoprotein E ( ApoE ) genotypes, physical fitness (i.e., cardiovascular fitness, body composition, muscular fitness, flexibility, balance, and power), and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) moderate and mediate the exercise intervention effects on memory and brain function. Methods The Western-Eastern Brain Fitness Integration Training (WE-BFit) is a single-blinded, double-arm, 6-month randomized controlled trial. One hundred cognitively healthy adults, aged 45–70 years, with different risks for Alzheimer's disease (i.e., ApoE genotype) will be recruited and randomized into either a multi-domain exercise group or an online educational course control group. The exercise intervention consists of one 90-min on-site and several online sessions up to 60 min per week for 6 months. Working memory, episodic memory, physical fitness, and BDNF will be assessed before and after the 6-month intervention. The effects of the WE-BFit on memory and brain function will be described and analyzed. We will further examine how ApoE genotype and changes in physical fitness and BDNF affect the effects of the intervention. Discussion WE-BFit is designed to improve memory and brain function using a multi-domain exercise intervention. The results will provide insight into the implementation of an exercise intervention with multiple domains to preserve memory and brain function in adults with genetic risk levels for Alzheimer's disease. Clinical trial registration ClinicalTrials.gov , identifier: NCT05068271.
... Mindfulness-based stress reduction programs have also been shown to reduce self-reported loneliness and pro-inflammatory gene expression in older adults [38]. Similarly, a meta-analysis on the effects of meditation and mind-body exercises on older adults' cognitive performance found improvements to cognition for older adults aged 60 years or above [39]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study was to explore the association between resilience and experiences of the COVID-19 pandemic among older adults. We used a sequential explanatory mixed methods study design to recruit older adults who spoke English and were 60 and above during the pandemic. Survey data investigated older adults’ resilience, post-traumatic growth, well-being, and demographics. Extreme case purposeful sampling of their resilience score was used to select interviewees. Qualitative data sought to understand the relationship between resilience and how older adults responded to the COVID-19 pandemic. Exploring the relationship between resilience (well-being in the face of challenge) and one’s experience of the COVID-19 pandemic revealed that participants categorized as having high resilience had long held behaviors of contemplative practices that helped them effectively adapt to the COVID-19 pandemic. As we continue to face global challenges, we must redefine care, guide interventions, and promote healthy aging by incorporating contemplative practices into the lives of older adults.
... Meditation and mind-body exercises improves cognition in the elderly people. According to Chan et al. (2019) meditation and mind-body exercises are effective to improve cognition of aging population of 60 years and above. ...
... Aside from mental symptoms, various persistent physical symptoms, such as shortness of breath, chest pain, altered smell and taste, cough, myalgia, diarrhea, and organ dysfunction, were observed following COVID-19 infection [108,109]. MBI often includes meditation, breathing training, and focused body movements, which could potentially relieve multiple symptoms of COVID-19 or long COVID, such as shortness of breath, fatigue, anxiety, sleep disturbance, cognitive impairment, and post-traumatic stress [104,[110][111][112][113][114][115]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent findings suggest a correlation between COVID-19 and diabetes, although the underlying causes are still little understood. COVID-19 infection tends to induce severe symptoms in patients with underlying diabetes, increasing their mortality rate. Moreover, COVID-19 itself appears to be a diabetogenic factor. In addition, mental health conditions, such as depression due to lockdown and anxiety about infection, were found to affect glycemic control and immunity, highlighting the importance of mental health care during the pandemic. Mind–Body Intervention (MBI), which includes meditation, yoga, and qigong, has emerged as a tool for mental health management due to its effects on stress reduction and the promotion of mental and physical well-being. Here, we review the latest randomized controlled trials to determine the effects of MBI on glycemic control and the immune system and discuss the underlying mechanisms by which MBI facilitates the virtuous cycle of stress management, glycemic control, and immune modulation. Furthermore, we examine the actual utilization of MBI during the COVID-19 pandemic era through recent studies. With proper online education, non-pharmacological MBI may be more widely used as an important tool for self-health care that complements the usual treatment of COVID-19 patients and survivors.
Article
Background Neurodegenerative diseases have become an important concern with the accelerated aging process. Tai Chi Quan (TCQ) has positive benefits for brain health and chronic diseases. The aim of this study was to summarize the protective effects of TCQ for motor function, cognition, quality of life, and mood in patients with neurodegenerative diseases. Methods A systematic search was conducted via PubMed database and the Web of Science core collection database until August 20, 2021. The available English systematic reviews, meta-analyses, and clinical trials were included. Two reviewers completed the screening and assessment process independently. Results A total of 28 studies on Parkinson's disease, 21 on cognitive impairment, and 9 on multiple sclerosis met the included criteria. The study found that TCQ remarkably improved general motor function and balance, and prevented falls for Parkinson's disease. TCQ significantly improved global cognitive function for cognitive impairment. TCQ was likely safe and beneficial for multiple sclerosis as result of heterogeneous outcomes and small samples. Conclusion TCQ exercise can effectively improve the motor function, global cognitive function, and falls in patients with neurodegenerative diseases. However, the positive effects of TCQ on the quality of life and mood of patients with neurodegenerative diseases need further evidence.
Article
Background Even though the effect of education on cognitive performance has been widely reported, the relationship between socio-cognitive mindfulness, cognitive performance, and memory complaint among the elderly with heterogeneous educational levels has not yet been investigated. Aim This study aimed to analyze the potential relationship between cognitive performance, memory complaint, and socio-cognitive mindfulness in a sample of healthy older adults with different years of education. Methods In this quantitative, cross-sectional, observational, and analytical study, participants (n = 68) were assessed with a sociodemographic questionnaire, cognitive performance test (ACE-III), levels of socio-cognitive mindfulness (LSM-21), and memory complaint (Memory Complaint Scale). Descriptive statistics, as well as Pearson’s correlation, and linear regression analysis were performed, and significance was assumed if p < .05. Results Years of education correlated with cognitive performance and socio-cognitive mindfulness, but not with memory complaint. Socio-cognitive mindfulness had a positive correlation with cognitive performance and a negative correlation with memory complaint. Also, socio-cognitive mindfulness predicted cognitive performance and memory complaint both in bivariate analysis and when controlling for years of education. Conclusion Our findings suggested that older adults with higher levels of socio-cognitive mindfulness showed better cognitive performance and less memory complaint.
Article
Full-text available
Abstract Background Aging-related cognitive decline and cognitive impairment greatly impacts older adults’ daily life. The worldwide ageing of the population and associated wave of dementia urgently calls for prevention strategies to reduce the risk of cognitive decline. Physical activity (PA) is known to improve cognitive function at older age through processes of neuroplasticity. Yet, emerging studies suggest that larger cognitive gains may be induced when PA interventions are combined with cognitive activity (CA). This meta-analysis evaluates these potential synergistic effects by comparing cognitive effects following combined PA + CA interventions to PA interventions (PA only), CA interventions (CA only) and control groups. Methods Pubmed, Embase, PsycInfo, CINAHL and Sportdiscus were searched for English peer-reviewed papers until April 2018. Data were extracted on cognition and factors potentially influencing the cognitive effects: mode of PA + CA combination (sequential or simultaneous), session frequency and duration, intervention length and study quality. Differences between older adults with and without mild cognitive impairments were also explored. Results Forty-one studies were included. Relative to the control group, combined PA + CA intervention showed significantly larger gains in cognition (g = 0.316; 95% CI 0.188–0.443; p
Article
Full-text available
Background: Yoga is a mind-body based physical activity that has demonstrated a variety of physiological, psychological and cognitive health benefits. Although yoga practice has shown to improve cognitive performance, few studies have examined the underlying neurological correlates. Objective: The current study aimed to determine the differences in gray matter volume of the hippocampus, thalamus and caudate nucleus and brain activation during the Sternberg working memory task. Method: Participants were 13 experienced yoga practitioners (mean age = 35.8), defined as having more than 3 years of regular yoga practice, and 13 age- and sex-matched controls (mean age = 35.7). All participants completed a 6-min walk test to assess fitness, psychosocial and demographic questionnaires; and underwent magnetic resonance imaging to assess gray matter volume and brain activation. Results: There were no group differences on demographic measures of income, education and on estimated VO2max or physical activity levels. Gray matter volume differences were observed in the left hippocampus, showing greater volume in experienced yoga practitioners compared to controls (p = 0.017). The functional MRI results revealed less activation in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in yoga practitioners compared to controls during the encoding phase of the Sternberg task (p < 0.05). Reaction time and accuracy on the task did not differ between the groups. Conclusions: Our results suggest an association between regular long-term yoga practice and differential structure and function of specific brain regions involved in executive function, specifically working memory, which has previously shown to improve with yoga practice. Future studies need to examine intervention effects of yoga and explore its potential to maintain and improve cognitive health across the lifespan through longitudinal and intervention studies.
Article
Full-text available
Age-related cognitive decline is a significant public health concern. Recently, non-pharmacological methods, such as physical activity and mental training practices, have emerged as promising low-cost methods to slow the progression of age-related memory decline. In this study, we investigated if Tai Chi Chuan (TCC) and Baduanjin modulated the fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations (fALFF) in different frequency bands (low-frequency: 0.01–0.08 Hz; slow-5: 0.01–0.027 Hz; slow-4: 0.027–0.073 Hz) and improved memory function. Older adults were recruited for the randomized study. Participants in the TCC and Baduanjin groups received 12 weeks of training (1 h/day for 5 days/week). Participants in the control group received basic health education. Each subject participated in memory tests and fMRI scans at the beginning and end of the experiment. We found that compared to the control group: (1) TCC and Baduanjin groups demonstrated significant improvements in memory function; (2) TCC increased fALFF in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) in the slow-5 and low-frequency bands; and (3) Baduanjin increased fALFF in the medial PFC in the slow-5 and low-frequency bands. This increase was positively associated with memory function improvement in the slow-5 and low-frequency bands across the TCC and Baduanjin groups. Our results suggest that TCC and Baduanjin may work through different brain mechanisms to prevent memory decline due to aging.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of this study is to investigate and compare how 12-weeks of Tai Chi Chuan and Baduanjin exercise can modulate brain structure and memory function in older adults. Magnetic resonance imaging and memory function measurements (Wechsler Memory Scale-Chinese revised, WMS-CR) were applied at both the beginning and end of the study. Results showed that both Tai Chi Chuan and Baduanjin could significantly increase grey matter volume (GMV) in the insula, medial temporal lobe, and putamen after 12-weeks of exercise. No significant differences were observed in GMV between the Tai Chi Chuan and Baduanjin groups. We also found that compared to healthy controls, Tai Chi Chuan and Baduanjin significantly improved visual reproduction subscores on the WMS-CR. Baduanjin also improved mental control, recognition, touch, and comprehension memory subscores of the WMS-CR compared to the control group. Memory quotient and visual reproduction subscores were both associated with GMV increases in the putamen and hippocampus. Our results demonstrate the potential of Tai Chi Chuan and Baduanjin exercise for the prevention of memory deficits in older adults.
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive impairment is one of the most common problem saffecting older adults. In this study, we investigated whether Tai Chi Chuan and Baduanjin practice can modulate mental control functionand the resting state functional connectivity (rsFC) of the cognitive control network in older adults. Participants in the two exercise groups practiced either Tai Chi Chuan or Baduanjin for 12 weeks, and those in the control group received basic health education. Memory tests and fMRI scans were conducted at baseline and at the end of the study. Seed-based (bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, DLPFC) rsFC analysis was performed. We found that compared to the controls, 1) both Tai Chi Chuan and Baduanjin groups demonstrated significant improvements in mental control function; 2) the Tai Chi Chuan group showed a significant decrease in rsFC between the DLPFC and the left superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and anterior cingulate cortex; and 3) the Baduanjin group showed a significant decrease in rsFC between the DLPFC and the left putamen and insula. Mental control improvement was negatively associated with rsFC DLPFC-putamen changes across all subjects. These findings demonstrate the potential of Tai Chi Chuan and Baduanjin exercises in preventing cognitive decline.
Article
Objectives: To investigate the methodological quality and statistical reporting of reports of trials indexed on the Physiotherapy Evidence Database (PEDro) classified in the musculoskeletal subdiscipline, and to analyse the characteristics of the trials that can predict trial report quality. Design: Cross-sectional study based on a collection of randomised controlled. We randomly selected 19% of trial reports coded as "musculoskeletal" from the PEDro database. Methodological quality was assessed using the PEDro scale. We assessed aspects of the trial report using nine items from the CONSORT statement. We performed multivariate linear regression analysis models to predict the total PEDro score. Setting: NOT Applicable. Participants: NOT Applicable. Interventions: NOT Applicable. Main outcome measure: NOT Applicable. Results: A total of 1404 trial reports were included in the analysis. The mean total PEDro score was 5.27 points (SD = 1.63), which reflects low methodological quality. There was a slight improvement in the quality of trial reports over time. The characteristics that predicted the total PEDro score were: endorsement of the CONSORT statement, sample size calculation, a lower number of primary outcomes, evaluation of electrotherapy as intervention, if the trial reported the research design in the title, reporting of participant flow diagram, years since publication (most recent trials), and trials published in English. Conclusion: The quality of the trials in musculoskeletal physical therapy is suboptimal. The use of reporting checklists such as the CONSORT statement should be mandatory in all journals. Journal reviewers and journal editors should also use the CONSORT statement during the review process.
Article
The field of cognitive neuroscience was transformed by the discovery that exercise induces neurogenesis in the adult brain, with the potential to improve brain health and stave off the effects of neurodegenerative disease. However, the basic mechanisms underlying exercise–brain connections are not well understood. We use an evolutionary neuroscience approach to develop the adaptive capacity model (ACM), detailing how and why physical activity improves brain function based on an energy-minimizing strategy. Building on studies showing a combined benefit of exercise and cognitive challenge to enhance neuroplasticity, our ACM addresses two fundamental questions: (i) what are the proximate and ultimate mechanisms underlying age-related brain atrophy, and (ii) how do lifestyle changes influence the trajectory of healthy and pathological aging?
Article
Background Physical exercise is seen as a promising intervention to prevent or delay cognitive decline in individuals aged 50 years and older, yet the evidence from reviews is not conclusive. Objectives To determine if physical exercise is effective in improving cognitive function in this population. Design Systematic review with multilevel meta-analysis. Data sources Electronic databases Medline (PubMed), EMBASE (Scopus), PsychINFO and CENTRAL (Cochrane) from inception to November 2016. Eligibility criteria Randomised controlled trials of physical exercise interventions in community-dwelling adults older than 50 years, with an outcome measure of cognitive function. Results The search returned 12 820 records, of which 39 studies were included in the systematic review. Analysis of 333 dependent effect sizes from 36 studies showed that physical exercise improved cognitive function (0.29; 95% CI 0.17 to 0.41; p<0.01). Interventions of aerobic exercise, resistance training, multicomponent training and tai chi, all had significant point estimates. When exercise prescription was examined, a duration of 45–60 min per session and at least moderate intensity, were associated with benefits to cognition. The results of the meta-analysis were consistent and independent of the cognitive domain tested or the cognitive status of the participants. Conclusions Physical exercise improved cognitive function in the over 50s, regardless of the cognitive status of participants. To improve cognitive function, this meta-analysis provides clinicians with evidence to recommend that patients obtain both aerobic and resistance exercise of at least moderate intensity on as many days of the week as feasible, in line with current exercise guidelines.
Article
Background: While effective therapies for preventing or slowing cognitive decline in at-risk populations remain elusive, evidence suggests mind-body interventions may hold promise. Objectives: In this study, we assessed the effects of Kirtan Kriya meditation (KK) and music listening (ML) on cognitive outcomes in adults experiencing subjective cognitive decline (SCD), a strong predictor of Alzheimer's disease. Methods: Sixty participants with SCD were randomized to a KK or ML program and asked to practice 12 minutes/day for 3 months, then at their discretion for the ensuing 3 months. At baseline, 3 months, and 6 months we measured memory and cognitive functioning [Memory Functioning Questionnaire (MFQ), Trail-making Test (TMT-A/B), and Digit-Symbol Substitution Test (DSST)]. Results: The 6-month study was completed by 53 participants (88%). Participants performed an average of 93% (91% KK, 94% ML) of sessions in the first 3 months, and 71% (68% KK, 74% ML) during the 3-month, practice-optional, follow-up period. Both groups showed marked and significant improvements at 3 months in memory and cognitive performance (MFQ, DSST, TMT-A/B; p's ≤0.04). At 6 months, overall gains were maintained or improved (p's ≤ 0.006), with effect sizes ranging from medium (DSST, ML group) to large (DSST, KK group; TMT-A/B, MFQ). Changes were unrelated to treatment expectancies and did not differ by age, gender, baseline cognition scores, or other factors. Conclusions: Findings of this preliminary randomized controlled trial suggest practice of meditation or ML can significantly enhance both subjective memory function and objective cognitive performance in adults with SCD, and may offer promise for improving outcomes in this population.