ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Background: A positive relationship between problematic gaming and escapism motivation to play video games has been well established, suggesting that problematic gaming may result from attempts to deal with negative emotions. However, to date, no study has examined how emotion dysregulation affects both escapism motives and problematic gaming patterns. Methods: Difficulties in emotion regulation, escapism, and problematic involvement with video games were assessed in a sample of 390 World of Warcraft players. A structural equation modeling framework was used to test the hypothesis that escapism mediates the relationship between emotion dysregulation and problematic gaming. Results: Statistical analyses showed that difficulties in emotion regulation predicted both escapism motives and problematic gaming, and that escapism partially mediated this relationship. Conclusion: Our findings support the view that problematic players are likely to escape in online games as a maladaptive coping strategy for dealing with adverse emotional experiences.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Problematic video game use as an emotional coping strategy: Evidence from a
sample of MMORPG gamers
MARIA DI BLASI
1
*, ALESSANDRO GIARDINA
1
, CECILIA GIORDANO
1
, GIANLUCA LO COCO
1
, CRISPINO TOSTO
2
,
JOEL BILLIEUX
3
and ADRIANO SCHIMMENTI
4
1
Department of Psychological and Educational Sciences, University of Palermo, Palermo, Italy
2
Istituto per le Tecnologie Didattiche, Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche, Palermo, Italy
3
Addictive and Compulsive Behaviours Laboratory, Institute for Health and Behaviours, University of Luxembourg,
Esch-sur-Alzette, Luxembourg
4
Faculty of Human and Social Sciences, UKE Kore University of Enna, Enna, Italy
(Received: September 2, 2018; revised manuscript received: November 20, 2018; accepted: December 23, 2018)
Background: A positive relationship between problematic gaming and escapism motivation to play video games has
been well established, suggesting that problematic gaming may result from attempts to deal with negative emotions.
However, to date, no study has examined how emotion dysregulation affects both escapism motives and problematic
gaming patterns. Methods: Difculties in emotion regulation, escapism, and problematic involvement with video
games were assessed in a sample of 390 World of Warcraft players. A structural equation modeling framework was
used to test the hypothesis that escapism mediates the relationship between emotion dysregulation and problematic
gaming. Results: Statistical analyses showed that difculties in emotion regulation predicted both escapism motives
and problematic gaming, and that escapism partially mediated this relationship. Conclusion: Our ndings support the
view that problematic players are likely to escape in online games as a maladaptive coping strategy for dealing with
adverse emotional experiences.
Keywords: problematic gaming, emotion regulation, Internet gaming disorder, escapism, MMORPG, gaming
disorder
INTRODUCTION
Massively multiplayer online role-playing games
(MMORPGs) are virtual environments in which players
can interact with each other through customizable avatars,
co-operating, role-playing, and competing with millions of
players around the world. Previous literature on problem-
atic gaming (PG) has largely focused on MMORPGs. PG is
currently considered a potential disorder, listed within
Emerging Measures and Models(Section III) in the
most recent edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychi-
atric Association, 2013) under the label Internet gaming
disorder (IGD). Furthermore, the International Classica-
tion of Diseases (World Health Organization, 2018)
includes Gaming Disorder in its section devoted to dis-
orders resulting from addictive behaviors. Nevertheless,
what exactly constitutes PG is still a topic of debate (King
et al., 2018;Van Rooij et al., 2018). One concern arises
from the risk of pathologizing healthy, highly engaged
gamers who could t the diagnostic criteria for IGD
without experiencing signicant life impairment (Deleuze,
Long, Liu, Maurage, & Billieux, 2018). Moreover, it
remains unclear whether there is a causal relationship
between life impairments and addictive-like gaming
behavior (Kardefelt-Winther, 2014a,2014b;Snodgrass
et al., 2014;Van Rooij et al., 2018).
Although PG has often been conceptualized within a
biomedical model of addiction (Petry et al., 2014), alternative
etiological hypotheses have emerged. These include PG
resulting from maladaptive coping strategies displayed to
deal with negative affect states (Maroney, Williams, Thomas,
Skues, & Moulding, 2018;Reinecke, 2009;Schimmenti &
Caretti, 2010;Snodgrass et al., 2018). In this respect,
Kardefelt-Winther (2014b) proposed that some individuals
might use the online environment to alleviate negative
feelings arising from stressful or adverse life situations,
potentially leading to problematic use and addictive-like
symptoms. Therefore, PG may represent a maladaptive cop-
ing strategy for individuals with limited self-regulation skills.
Emotion dysregulation (ED) and problematic gaming (PG)
In the past two decades, the construct of ED has received
growing interest in psychology and psychopathology. ED
* Corresponding author: Maria Di Blasi; Department of Psycholog-
ical and Educational Sciences, University of Palermo, Viale delle
Scienze, Edicio 15, Palermo 90100, Italy; Phone: +39 091 238
97717; Fax: +39 091 651 3825; E-mail: maria.diblasi@unipa.it
This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License,
which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium for non-commercial purposes, provided the original author and
source are credited, a link to the CC License is provided, and changes if any are indicated.
ISSN 2062-5871 © 2019 The Author(s)
FULL-LENGTH REPORT Journal of Behavioral Addictions 8(1), pp. 2534 (2019)
DOI: 10.1556/2006.8.2019.02
First published online February 11, 2019
has been related to several types of psychopathological and
relational problems (DAgostino, Covanti, Monti, &
Starcevic, 2017;Dimaggio et al., 2017;Gross & Jazaieri,
2014), leading some authors to consider it a hallmark of
psychopathology (Beauchaine, Gatzke-Kopp, & Mead,
2007). Emotion regulation is generally referred to as a set
of psychological processes aimed at differentiating,
recognizing, and modulating feelings with exibility and
according to environmental and relational stimuli (David,
2016;Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist, & Target, 2002;Gross &
Thompson, 2007).
Emotional processing and emotion regulation have been
widely analyzed in previous studies of smartphone, social
networks, and general Internet use, showing both a positive
effect on well-being (Elhai et al., 2018;Hoffner & Lee,
2015;Kardefelt-Winther, 2014a) and a link to problematic
Internet use (Casale, Caplan, & Fioravanti, 2016;Hormes,
Kearns, & Timko, 2014;Schimmenti, Starcevic, Gervasi,
Deleuze, & Billieux, 2018;Yildiz, 2017). In the context of
online gaming, many studies have reported a higher use of
video games for emotion regulation purposes (Gaetan,
Bréjard, & Bonnet, 2016;Hemenover & Bowman, 2018;
Hussain & Grifths, 2009;Villani et al., 2018). For
example, Villani et al. (2018) suggested that some video
game features, such as interactivity, the sense of mastery,
and the ability to deal safely with failure and experiment
with identity, may play a part in facilitating emotion
regulation.
Despite the conicting evidence, few studies have
addressed the role of ED in PG (Billieux et al., 2011;
Estévez, Jáuregui, Sánchez-Marcos, L´opez-González, &
Grifths, 2017;Yen et al., 2018). Billieux et al. (2011)
found that a tendency to act rashly when experiencing
intense negative feelings was the main impulsivity dimen-
sion promoting problematic involvement in World of War-
craft (WoW) in a sample of gamers. Similarly, Estévez et al.
(2017) showed that a lack of emotional clarity and control
over emotional responses predicted problematic video game
involvement in their sample. Therefore, it might be hypoth-
esized that some people with emotion regulation difculties
use video game environments to deal with negative affec-
tivity, thus increasing the risk of negative consequences
(Kardefelt-Winther, 2014b). Clearly, there is a need to
address the paucity of research on the role of ED among
problematic gamers.
Escapism motivations to play and emotional coping
The construct of motivations to play (Yee, 2006) could be
critical in explaining the relationship between ED and
PG. For example, Billieux et al. (2013) found that motiva-
tions to play have specic, meaningful associations with
in-game behaviors, suggesting that psychological motives
are reliable indices of playersinterests and needs.
Among the various motivations to play, escapism has
been studied extensively. Escapism refers to the involve-
ment in video games to relax or to distract oneself from
real-life problems (Yee, 2006). It may represent one of the
main features contributing to the enjoyment of online games
and is not necessarily related to negative outcomes
(Bowditch, Chapman, & Naweed, 2018;Hussain &
Grifths, 2009;Kardefelt-Winther, 2014c). Indeed, Kuo,
Lutz, and Hiler (2016) conceived escapism as an active,
emotion-focused, coping mechanism that brings relief from
stress and a sense of efcacy and empowerment to players
lives. However, several studies have shown escapism moti-
vation to be one of the strongest predictors of PG with
regard to its avoidant, passive, and disengaging nature
(Ballabio et al., 2017;Billieux et al., 2013;Bowditch
et al., 2018;Kuss, Louws, & Wiers, 2012;Schimmenti,
Infanti, Badoud, Laloyaux, & Billieux, 2017;Zanetta-
Dauriat et al., 2011).
To account for this contradiction, some authors have
argued that the negative potential of escapism is related to
its interactions with other vulnerability factors, such as
psychological distress, poor psychosocial well-being, or
poor coping resources (Ballabio et al., 2017;Bowditch
et al., 2018;Demetrovics et al., 2011;Kardefelt-Winther,
2014b,2014c). Moreover, some studies have suggested
that an interaction between negative affect and escapism is
likely to play a pivotal role in PG (Billieux et al., 2015;
Bowditch et al., 2018;Chang, Hsieh, & Lin, 2018;
Kardefelt-Winther, 2014a;Maroney et al., 2018). For
example, Kardefelt-Winther (2014a) showed that escapism
mediated the relationship between distress and problematic
use of WoW. Another study (Király et al., 2015)
suggested that psychiatric symptoms were both directly
and indirectly (via escapism and competition motives,
respectively) negatively associated with PG in a sample
of online gamers. In addition, Billieux et al. (2015)
postulated that among participants characterized by in-
creased negative affect in their sample of 1,057 WoW
players, PG was probably the result of a maladaptive
coping strategy to overcome negative affect and negative
life events. Finally, Maroney et al. (2018) recently found
that escapism motivations partially mediated the
signicant effects of depression, loneliness, and social
anxiety on PG among rst-person shooter and MMORPG
players. However, to date, no studies have explored the
link between ED and escapism motivations in problematic
MMORPGs use.
The present study
The aim of this study was to test the relationships between
ED, escapism motivations, and PG. More specically, we
hypothesized that WoW players with higher levels of ED
use video games to escape negative affect, and that this
greater need to escape into virtual reality in turn triggers PG.
Hence, the present investigation has the potential to shed
light on the underlying mechanisms of PG and thus to
inform clinical practice. According to the theoretical frame-
work of the study, three operational hypotheses can be
formulated:
H1: Emotion dysregulation statistically predicts prob-
lematic gaming.
H2: Emotion dysregulation statistically predicts
escapism.
H3: Emotion dysregulation statistically predicts prob-
lematic gaming via escapism.
26 |Journal of Behavioral Addictions 8(1), pp. 2534 (2019)
Blasi et al.
METHODS
Participants and procedure
Since every game genre has specic features and structural
characteristics that are likely to have confounding effects on
ndings, we contacted players of the MMORPG WoW only.
WoW is the most popular title in the MMORPGs genre and
still counts millions of players around the world today. The
game has in fact been the subject of several previous studies
designed to assess motivations to play MMORPG and
potential psychological factors associated with problematic
gaming (Billieux et al., 2013;Bowditch et al., 2018;
Yee, 2006). In WoW, players interact with each other
through customizable avatars within the heroic-fantasy
world of Azeroth. Much of the gameplay revolves around
co-operation (player vs. environment) or competition
(player vs. player) with other players (often via virtual
social networks called guilds), in order to complete
quests, improve character skills and items, and achieve a
better in-game ranking.
Participants were recruited through an advertisement
posted on ofcial WoW forums, Facebook groups and
pages, and general video game forums. The advertisement
briey explained the aim of the study and invited players to
participate through an online survey. The survey started in
May 2017 and ended June 2018. Inclusion criteria were
being uent Italian speakers, active WoW players, and aged
18 years or older. Anonymity was assured (no data on the
gamersidentication, including their Internet Protocol
address, were collected). Information on sociodemographic
variables including sex, age, and ethnicity, as well as game-
related variables, such as participantscharacters race
(e.g., elf, human, orc), faction (horde or alliance), character
level, self-reported weekly hours spent playing, and the
number of months played, were collected through a struc-
tured questionnaire administered along with standardized
self-reports. Self-reported weekly hours spent playing
was measured as the number of hours of online gaming
in a week reported by the participant. Respondents took
approximately 25 min to complete the survey.
In total, 827 participants started the survey. Four hundred
and fourteen (50.06% of the initial sample) completed the
entire survey. Of these, 24 were excluded from analyses for
the following reasons: no age reported (n=8), anomalous
hours of play (n=1), inconsistent matches between item
level and character level (n=10), and impossible matches
between character faction and character race (n=5). The
nal sample consisted of 390 players. Participantsages
ranged from 18 to 67 years (M=28.28, SD =8.24), and the
sample was predominantly composed of men (n=289,
74.10%).
Measures
Difculties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS-18). The
DERS is a widely used self-report measure of emotion
regulation difculties. Items are rated on a scale from 1
(almost never)to5(almost always). Scores for each
subscale and the total score are sums of the relevant items
(with three items reverse-coded). Higher DERS scores
reect greater emotion regulation difculties. In this study,
we used the Italian translation (Sighinol, Norcini Pala, &
Chiri, 2010) of the 18-item version by Victor and Klonsky
(2016). The DERS-18 includes six factor analytically de-
rived subscales: lack of emotional awareness (Awareness;
e.g., I pay attention to how I feel,reverse scored; Cron-
bachsα=.68); lack of emotional clarity (Clarity; e.g., I
am confused about how I feel;Cronbachsα=.83); lack of
acceptance of ones emotions when distressed (Non-
acceptance; e.g., When I am upset, I feel ashamed with
myself for feeling that way;Cronbachsα=.83); lack of
ability to engage in goal-directed cognition and behavior
when distressed (Goals; e.g., When I am upset, I have
difculty focusing on other things;Cronbachsα=.88);
lack of ability to manage ones impulses when distressed
(Impulse; e.g., When I am upset, I lose control of my
behavior;Cronbachsα=.88); and lack of access to
effective strategies for feeling better when distressed (Strat-
egies; e.g., When I am upset, I believe that I will remain
that way for a long time;Cronbachsα=.84). Cronbachs
αs for the subscales in this study were similar to those found
in other studies (Estévez et al., 2017).
Internet Addiction Test WoW Version (IAT-WoW). The
IAT-WoW scale was used for assessing PG. The Italian
translation of the IAT (Ferraro, Caci, DAmico, & Blasi,
2007) was adapted specically for WoW rather than the
Internet in general, in accordance with Billieux et al.s
(2013,2015) suggestion. This adapted IAT comprised
20 items scored on a 5-point Likert scale measuring addic-
tive use of WoW and related negative consequences on
various aspects of everyday life (e.g., Did you ever play
WoW longer than you had intended?). For each item,
respondents can choose from ve response options scored
15(never, rarely, occasionally, often, and always). Higher
IAT-WoW scores indicate higher levels of problematic
involvement in WoW. Cronbachsαfor IAT-WoW in this
study was .88, similar to that reported in other studies
(Billieux et al., 2013,2015).
Motivation to Play in Online Games Questionnaire
(MPOGQ-WoW version). The MPOGQ, developed by Yee
(2006), measures the reasons why players engage in
MMORPG. In this study, we adapted the original version
to apply only to WoW, in accordance with a procedure
used by Billieux et al. (2013). The questionnaire measures
three broad motivations divided into 10 specicmotives:
Achievement (advancement, competition, and mechan-
ics), Social (socializing, relationship, and teamwork), and
Immersion (customization, escapism, discovery, and role-
play). In accordance with the hypotheses of this study,
only the escapism items were used. The escapism scale
measures involvement in the video game to relax and/or to
escape from real-life problems (e.g., How often do
you play WoW so that you can avoid thinking about
some of your real-life problems or worries?). Participants
rate each statement using a 5-point Likert scale, with
higher scores indicating higher escapism motives. Cron-
bachsαof the escapism scale in this study was .69,
similar to that found in other studies (Billieux et al., 2013,
2015).
Journal of Behavioral Addictions 8(1), pp. 2534 (2019) |27
Problematic gaming and emotional dysregulation
Measurement model
We used a structural equation modeling (SEM) framework
to test the hypothesis that escapism would mediate the
relationship between ED and PG. This model is based on
the notion that the six DERS subscales (Non-acceptance,
Goal, Impulse, Awareness, Strategies, and Clarity) form the
latent variable of ED. The three items comprising
the escapism subscale of the MMOPGQ-WoW formed the
latent variable of escapism. Since the IAT measure has
shown inconsistent factor structures in previous research
(Faraci, Craparo, Messina, & Severino, 2013), we rst ran
an exploratory factor analysis on the IAT-WoW to identify a
clear factor structure for our measurement model. Five
factors had eigenvalues above 1 (the eigenvalues being
6.50, 1.47, 1.13, 1.12, and 1.07), but only the rst factor
explained more than 5% of the variance of the IAT-WoW
(29.77% of variance); moreover, solutions with more than
one factor displayed signicant and inconsistent loadings
across factors. Therefore, we decided to use the ve items
with the highest loading on the general factor of the IAT-
WoW to determine an appropriate measurement of PG and
thus to form its latent factor. The ve items selected to
measure the IAT-WoW latent factor (items 2, 5, 12, 15, and
20; Cronbachsα=.76) represent core symptoms of PG
(neglecting household chores to spend more time online;
others complaining about the amount of time spent on
WoW; fears that life without WoW would be joyless; being
preoccupied with WoW when ofine and fantasizing
about playing it; and feelings of depression or nervousness
when not playing WoW, which go away when going back to
playing it). Moreover, these items with the highest
loadings on the IAT-WoW general factor are relevant for
understanding PG patterns according to both the compen-
satory hypothesis of PG as a coping strategy to deal with
current stressors (Kardefelt-Winther, 2014b) and to the
symptom-based model of behavioral addictions (Brown,
1993;Grifths, 2005). The measurement model and its
predicted relationships between variables are depicted in
Figure 1.
Statistical analysis
Descriptive statistics were calculated for all the variables in
the study. Gender differences were examined using inde-
pendent t-tests. Pearsonsrcorrelations between weekly
hours of play and the psychological variables were also
considered. SEM analysis of the measurement model was
performed using the lavaan package (Rosseel, 2012) for R
software. Multiple goodness-of-t indices were assessed.
Assessment of the absolute indices included the following:
the χ
2
/df ratio, for which values 2 indicate a good t; the
root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), for
which values 0.05 indicate a good t; and the standardized
root mean square residual (SRMR), for which a value 0.08
is recommended for accepting a model. Assessment of
incremental t indices included the non-normed t index
(NNFI) and the comparative t index (CFI), with
values >.95 indicating a good t for both (Brown, 2006;
Hu & Bentler, 1999). The χ
2
test of difference was further
used to determine whether the model presented in Figure 1
provided a better t than did a non-mediated model in which
ED independently predicted escapism and PG, without
escapism mediating PG.
Ethics
The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration
of Helsinki and ethical guidelines for psychological research
laid down by the Italian Psychological Association. All
subjects were informed of the anonymity of the study and
all gave their informed consent.
RESULTS
Descriptive statistics for the full sample and differentiated
by gender are presented in Table 1.
The majority of the players (92.8%) had been playing
WoW for more than 12 months, showing high game expe-
rience in terms of number of months played (M=71.33,
Figure 1. Measurement model
28 |Journal of Behavioral Addictions 8(1), pp. 2534 (2019)
Blasi et al.
SD =42.97). On average, participants did not report high
ED scores (M=38.51, SD =13.07) with respect to other
samples (Sighinolet al., 2010), although there was a
slightly higher tendency to play to escape from reality
(M=10.00, SD =2.64) with respect to other studies
(e.g., Kaczmarek & Drążkowski, 2014). Moderate symp-
toms of PG were reported on the IAT-WOW (M=46.97,
SD =11.89), similar to those found in other studies
(Schimmenti, Infanti, et al., 2017;Wang et al., 2017).
The results of the ttests examining gender differences
showed that males did not differ signicantly from females
with regard to PG [M=47.56, SD =11.84 vs. M=45.28,
SD =11.91, t(388) =1.661, p=.098], ED [M=38.21,
SD =12.47 vs. M=39.36, SD =2.73, t(388) =0.70,
p=.48], or escapism [M=9.95, SD =2.61 vs. M=10.12,
SD =4.68, t(388) =0.56, p=.57]. Nor did they differ in
terms of weekly time spent playing WoW [M=23.26,
SD =15.32 vs. M=22.89, SD =15.19, t(388) =0.21,
p=.83] or months since they had started to play [M=73.47,
SD =42.42 vs. M=65.21, SD =44.16, t(388) =1.66,
p=.096]. Pearsonsrcorrelations between age, weekly
time spent playing WoW, and all the psychological mea-
sures are presented in Table 2.
ED was signicantly and positively related to both PG
and escapism, with relations being of moderate size. Nota-
bly, weekly hours spent playing was signicantly, yet
weakly, associated with escapism and PG but not with ED.
Moreover, all the DERS subscales were signicantly related
to both PG and escapism motivations. Specically, the
strategies scale was the one mostly related to escapism
motivations (r=.40) and PG (r=.33).
After examining the intercorrelations between variables,
the model presented in Figure 1was tested via SEM using
maximum likelihood estimation. The model yielded a sig-
nicant χ
2
value [χ
2
(74) =126.16, p<.001]; however, the
χ
2
/df ratio was 1.70, suggesting a good t. Further exami-
nation of t indices conrmed that the mediation model
tted very well (RMSEA =0.043 [90% CI: 0.0290.055],
SRMR =0.038, NNFI =0.96, CFI =0.97). All the esti-
mates, intercepts, and variances in this model were signi-
cant at p<.01. The mediation model explained 31% of
the escapism variance and 53% of the PG variance.
Table 1. Psychosocial and game-related variables: Descriptive statistics and gender differences
Overall
Gender differences
Range t(388) p
Males (N=289) Females (N=101)
M(±SD)orN(%) M(±SD)M(±SD)
DERS 38.51 (±13.07) 38.21 (±12.47) 39.36 (±2.73) 1890 0.706 .481
Awareness 7.44 (±2.78) 7.41 (±2.59) 7.50 (±3.24) 315 0.241 .810
Clarity 5.92 (±2.89) 5.84 (±2.67) 6.15 (±3.44) 315 0.842 .401
Goals 7.81 (±13.61) 7.87 (±3.71) 7.63 (±3.33) 315 0.600 .549
Impulse 5.22 (2.88) 5.16 (±2.78) 5.38 (±3.14) 315 0.660 .510
Non-acceptance 5.89 (±3.19) 5.80 (±3.15) 6.12 (±3.29) 315 0.883 .378
Strategies 6.23 (±3.38) 6.11 (±3.34) 6.55 (±3.49) 315 1.135 .268
IAT-WOW 46.97 (±11.89) 47.56 (±11.84) 45.28 (±11.91) 20100 1.661 .098
Escapism 10 (±2.64) 9.95 (±2.61) 10.12 (±14.68) 315 0.568 .570
Weekly hours of play 23.26 (±15.32) 23.26 (±15.32) 22.89 (±15.19) 1105 0.214 .830
Months of play 71.33 (±42.97) 73.47 (±42.42) 65.21 (±44.16) 1168 1.666 .096
Note.M: mean; SD: standard deviation; DERS: Difculties in Emotion Regulation Scale; IAT-WOW: Internet Addiction Test World of
Warcraft version.
Table 2. Correlations between problematic involvement in WoW, escapism motives, and difculties in emotion regulation
with its dimensions
Measures 1 2 3 4 5678910
1. Age
2. Hours a week .02
3. Escapism .18*** .17***
4. IAT-WOW .17*** .21*** .47***
5. DERS total score .30*** .03 .43*** .39***
6. Awareness .02 .07 .11** .12*** .40***
7. Clarity .27*** .03 .35*** .31*** .73*** .30***
8. Goals .27*** .00 .34*** .30*** .76*** .05 .41***
9. Impulse .21*** .08* .30*** .30*** .73*** .20*** .41*** .55***
10. Acceptance .19*** .03 .29*** .25*** .70*** .15*** .40*** .46*** .37***
11. Strategies .27*** .06 .40*** .33*** .80*** .12*** .54*** .57*** .52*** .50***
Note. WoW: World of Warcraft; DERS: Difculties in Emotion Regulation Scale; IAT-WOW: Internet Addiction Test WoW version.
*p<.05. **p<.01. ***p<.001.
Journal of Behavioral Addictions 8(1), pp. 2534 (2019) |29
Problematic gaming and emotional dysregulation
The non-mediated model, in which ED predicted both
escapism and PG with no relationship between these two
latter variables, displayed only acceptable t indices
(RMSEA =0.055 [90% CI: 0.0440.067], SRMR =0.060,
NNFI =0.96, CFI =0.93), and explained 37% of the
escapism variance but only 36% of the PG variance. As
expected, the mediation model was superior to the non-
mediated model in which escapism did not mediate the
predictive association between ED and PG [Δχ
2
(1) =38.89,
p<.001]. Notably, the effect of ED on PG decreased
from a standardized estimate of 0.55 (p<.001) in the
non-mediated model to one of 0.26 (p=.008), thus showing
partial mediation of escapism in the relationship between
ED and PG. The standardized total effect of the model
was 0.51 (p<.001); the standardized indirect effect of
ED on PG via escapism was 0.26 (p<.001). The standard-
ized estimates of the mediation model are presented in
Figure 2.
DISCUSSION
All our hypotheses were conrmed in our sample. Analyses
conducted within an SEM framework showed that ED
predicted both escapism motives and PG, and that escapism
partially mediated this relationship. The t indices of the
mediation model were excellent and support the view that
ED directly and indirectly affects levels of PG.
Consistent with H1, individuals reporting higher ED were
more likely to show problematic involvement in WoW. This
nding supports the view that ED is related to PG (Billieux
et al., 2011;Estévez et al., 2017;Gaetan et al., 2016).
Moreover, our results are consistent with other research
showing that difculties in emotion regulation are a part of,
or give a rise to, psychological symptoms such as depression
and social anxiety (Klemanski, Curtiss, McLaughlin, &
Nolen-Hoeksema, 2017;Paulus, Vanwoerden, Norton, &
Sharp, 2016), which have been established as risk factors
for PG (Maroney et al., 2018;Yen et al., 2018).
Our ndings are also well aligned with recent literature
suggesting that video game environments are particularly
suited to experimenting and coping with negative affect
(Gaetan et al., 2016;Hemenover & Bowman, 2018;Villani
et al., 2018). For example, Gaetan et al. (2016, p. 347)
suggested that video games may function as a means of
rendering emotions psychologically meaningful and to
curb alexithymic dynamic;that is, they can help gamers
to reduce difculties in identifying, describing, and proces-
sing their feelings (Schimmenti, Passanisi, et al., 2017).
Nevertheless, for individuals who display difculties in
emotion regulation, the video game environment may be
the main domain in which they can approach the regulation
of their emotions without negative consequences (Billieux
et al, 2015;Chang et al., 2018;Gaetan et al., 2016;
Kardefelt-Winther, 2014b;Maroney et al. 2018;
Schimmenti & Caretti, 2010).
In accordance with our second hypothesis, our
results showed higher ED to be associated with escapism.
Previous studies have found an association between escap-
ism motivation to play and negative emotions, as well as
stressful life events (Hussain & Grifths, 2009;Kardefelt-
Winter, 2014a,2014c;Kuo et al., 2016). However, to our
knowledge, this is the rst study to focus on a potential
underlying process, namely ED. As such, our ndings are
consistent with the results of Kuo et al. (2016) highlighting
that escapism can act as a coping mechanism for WoW
players motivated by the need to regulate their negative
affect linked to external stressors. It could be argued that
escapism in online games acts as an emotion-focused coping
strategy aimed at better managing negative emotions and
feelings. This strategy may paradoxically have the function
of helping individuals to cope with the inefcacy or unavail-
ability of other emotion regulation processes, thus allowing
them temporarily to escape from negative mood states
experienced in their ofine lives through their immersion
into alternative and more comfortable environments.
With regard to H3, we found that escapism partially
mediated the relationship between ED and PG. This
Figure 2. Mediation model. χ
2
=126.16*** (df =74); RMSEA =0.043 [90% CI: 0.0290.055]; SRMR =0.038; NNFI =0.96;
CFI =0.97. ***p<.001
30 |Journal of Behavioral Addictions 8(1), pp. 2534 (2019)
Blasi et al.
observed mediating effect of escapism is also meaningful at
the theoretical level, in which it shows that ED may lead
individuals to use video games as either a psychic retreat
from current and past problems (Schimmenti & Caretti,
2010;Schimmenti, Guglielmucci, Barbasio, & Granieri,
2012) and/or a coping strategy to escape from psychosocial
stressors (Kardefelt-Winther, 2014b). However, the use of
video games as a preferred strategy for coping with or
escaping from psychological difculties might foster exces-
sive involvement in video games, and might eventually lead
to clinically impairing gaming patterns. Similarly, a recent
study (Maroney et al., 2018) found that escapism mediated
the relationship between psychosocial distress and PG in a
sample of online gamers. The results from this study suggest
that, for gamers with higher ED, escapism might temporari-
ly serve as an emotion-focused coping strategy for seeking
temporary relief from disturbing affect states, while
simultaneously exercising a pusheffect (Chang et al.,
2018) that presses players into gaming to achieve short-term
well-being, thus increasing the risk of PG.
Therefore, these results raise some relevant questions
concerning the distinction between healthy and maladaptive
escapism that need to be eshed out. Indeed, many authors
have shown that escapism motivations can be related to
problematic gaming as well as highly engaged gaming, and
that different psychological factors can interact with
escapism to inuence its functionality (Bowditch et al.,
2018;Deleuze et al., 2018;Kardefelt-Winther, 2014c;Kuo
et al., 2016). For example, Kuo et al. (2016) pointed out that
while passive escapism provides relief from stressful
emotions by simply refocusing attention on the fantastic
narratives provided by the medium, active escapism may
allow the gamer to directly confront specic psychological
threats (e.g., low self-esteem) by redirecting awareness into
an interactive fantasy world that can provide us with
experiences of self-afrmation and empowerment. Accord-
ingly, in our SEM, clear differences in the magnitude of
loadings of the escapism items into their latent factor could
be observed, suggesting that the escapism construct is
multidimensional. Therefore, clarifying the nature of escap-
ism as reecting avoidant or active coping processes should
be seen as a crucial task for future research on this topic.
Overall, these ndings call for the development of new
measurement tools able to distinguish between adaptive
versus maladaptive escapism in the context of video game
playing.
LIMITATIONS
This study comes with some limitations that need to be
acknowledged. First, it is a cross-sectional study, and
therefore no causality can be inferred. Second, it relies on
self-report measures, which can result in bias effects and
under- or overreporting of behaviors. Third, the self-selected
nature of the sample limits the ability to generalize our
ndings to other video game players (Khazaal et al., 2014).
Fourth, partial attrition was high, with only 50% of initial
participants completing the whole survey. Many respon-
dents probably felt that the survey was too long, and as such
a shorter survey may have counteracted this attrition. Final-
ly, other psychosocial factors, such as social anxiety, de-
pression, and social isolation, as well as other gaming
motives that could play a role in PG, were not assessed.
Therefore, future research using longitudinal designs and
more representative samples of gamers are needed.
CONCLUSIONS
The ndings of this study provide new information to
support a better understanding of the psychological factors
behind the development and maintenance of PG among
WoW players. They suggest that escapism motivation in
emotionally dysregulated individuals may strengthen PG. In
line with a compensatory framework of PG (Kardefelt-
Winther, 2014b;Schimmenti & Caretti, 2010;Snodgrass
et al., 2018), the ndings also show the interplay between
difculties in emotion regulation, escapism, and problem-
atic involvement in WoW. Furthermore, they provide
evidence that gamers characterized by emotion regulation
difculties may rely on game experiences in order to regu-
late their emotional experiences, which can ultimately
translate into a maladaptive coping strategy. While the
deeply immersive nature of gaming experience can promote
regulation of, and relief from, negative mood states, this
maladaptive, emotionally focused coping strategy increases
the risk of problematic involvement in video games. It is
worth noting that this study also has some clinical implica-
tions. Problematic players, pushed by overwhelming
emotions to escape into the video game, may benet from
interventions focused on enhancing their emotion regulation
strategies and processes, such as those aimed at promoting
interception and emotional awareness (Murphy, Brewer,
Catmur, & Bird, 2017) in order to reduce affect-laden
impulsivity (Billieux, Gay, Rochat, & Van der Linden,
2010) and strengthen mentalized affectivity (Schimmenti
& Caretti, 2010).
Funding sources: No nancial support was received for this
study.
Authorscontribution: All authors contributed equally to
this work. They had full access to all data in the study and
take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the
accuracy of the data analysis.
Conict of interest: The authors declare no conict of
interest.
REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statis-
tical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5). Arlington, VA:
American Psychiatric Association.
Ballabio, M., Grifths, M. D., Urbán, R., Quartiroli, A.,
Demetrovics, Z., & Király, O. (2017). Do gaming motives
Journal of Behavioral Addictions 8(1), pp. 2534 (2019) |31
Problematic gaming and emotional dysregulation
mediate between psychiatric symptoms and problematic
gaming? An empirical survey study. Addiction Research &
Theory, 25(5), 397408. doi:10.1080/16066359.2017.1305360
Beauchaine, T. P., Gatzke-Kopp, L., & Mead, H. K. (2007).
Polyvagal theory and developmental psychopathology:
Emotion dysregulation and conduct problems from preschool
to adolescence. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 174184.
doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2005.08.008
Billieux, J., Chanal, J., Khazaal, Y., Rochat, L., Gay, P., Zullino,
D., & Van der Linden, M. (2011). Psychological predictors of
problematic involvement in massively multiplayer online role-
playing games: Illustration in a sample of male cybercafé
players. Psychopathology, 44(3), 165171. doi:10.1159/
000322525
Billieux, J., Gay, P., Rochat, L., & Van der Linden, M. (2010). The
role of urgency and its underlying psychological mechanisms
in problematic behaviours. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
48(11), 10851096. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.07.008
Billieux, J., Thorens, G., Khazaal, Y., Zullino, D., Achab, S., &
Linden, M. V. (2015). Problematic involvement in online
games: A cluster analytic approach. Computers in Human
Behavior, 43, 242250. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.10.055
Billieux, J., Van der Linden, M., Achab, S., Khazaal, Y.,
Paraskevopoulos, L., Zullino, D., & Thorens, G. (2013). Why
do you play World of Warcraft? An in-depth exploration of
self-reported motivations to play online and in game beha-
viours in the virtual world of Azeroth. Computers in Human
Behavior, 29(1), 103109. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2012.07.021
Bowditch, L., Chapman, J., & Naweed, A. (2018). Do coping
strategies moderate the relationship between escapism
and negative gaming outcomes in World of Warcraft
(MMORPG) players? Computers in Human Behavior, 86,
6976. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2018.04.030
Brown, R. I. F. (1993). Some contributions of the study of
gambling to the study of other addictions. In W. R. Eadington
& J. Cornelius (Eds.), Gambling behavior and problem
gambling (pp. 241272). Reno, NV: University of Nevada
Press.
Brown, T. A. (2006). Conrmatory factor analysis for applied
research. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Casale, S., Caplan, S. E., & Fioravanti, G. (2016). Positive
metacognitions about Internet use: The mediating role in
the relationship between emotional dysregulation and prob-
lematic use. Addictive Behaviors, 59, 8488. doi:10.1016/
j.addbeh.2016.03.014
Chang, S. M., Hsieh, G. M., & Lin, S. S. (2018). The mediation
effects of gaming motives between game involvement and
problematic Internet use: Escapism, advancement and social-
izing. Computers & Education, 122, 4353. doi:10.1016/
j.compedu.2018.03.007
DAgostino, A., Covanti, S., Monti, M. R., & Starcevic, V. (2017).
Reconsidering emotion dysregulation. Psychiatric Quarterly,
88(4), 807825. doi:10.1007/s11126-017-9499-6
David, S. (2016). Emotional agility: Get unstuck, embrace change,
and thrive in work and life. London, UK: Penguin.
Deleuze, J., Long, J., Liu, T.-Q., Maurage, P., & Billieux, J. (2018).
Passion or addiction? Correlates of healthy versus problematic
use of videogames in a sample of French-speaking regular
gamers. Addictive Behaviors, 82, 114121. doi:10.1016/
j.addbeh.2018.02.031
Demetrovics, Z., Urbán, R., Nagygyörgy, K., Farkas, J., Zilahy, D.,
Merv´o, B., Reindl, A., Ágoston, C., Kertész, A., & Harmath, E.
(2011). Why do you play? The development of the Motives for
Online Gaming Questionnaire (MOGQ). Behavior Research
Methods, 43(3), 814825. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0091-y
Dimaggio, G., Popolo, R., Montano, A., Velotti, P., Perrini, F.,
Buonocore, L., Garofalo, C., DAguanno, M., & Salvatore, G.
(2017). Emotion dysregulation, symptoms, and interpersonal
problems as independent predictors of a broad range of
personality disorders in an outpatient sample. Psychology
and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 90(4),
586599. doi:10.1111/papt.12126
Elhai, J. D., Tiamiyu, M. F., Weeks, J. W., Levine, J. C., Picard,
K. J., & Hall, B. J. (2018). Depression and emotion regulation
predict objective smartphone use measured over one
week. Personality and Individual Differences, 133, 2128.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.051
Estévez, A., Jáuregui, P., Sánchez-Marcos, I., L´opez-González, H.,
& Grifths, M. D. (2017). Attachment and emotion regulation
in substance addictions and behavioral addictions. Journal
of Behavioral Addictions, 6(4), 534544. doi:10.1556/
2006.6.2017.086
Faraci, P., Craparo, G., Messina, R., & Severino, S. (2013).
Internet Addiction Test (IAT): Which is the best factorial
solution? Journal of Medical Internet Research, 15(10),
e225. doi:10.2196/jmir.2935
Ferraro, G., Caci, B., DAmico, A., & Blasi, M. D. (2007). Internet
addiction disorder: An Italian study. CyberPsychology &
Behavior, 10(2), 170175. doi:10.1089/cpb.2006.9972
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E., & Target, M. (2002). Affect
regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self.
New York, NY: Other Press.
Gaetan, S., Bréjard, V., & Bonnet, A. (2016). Video games in
adolescence and emotional functioning: Emotion regulation,
emotion intensity, emotion expression, and alexithymia.
Computers in Human Behavior, 61, 344349. doi:10.1016/
j.chb.2016.03.027
Grifths, M. (2005). A componentsmodel of addiction within a
biopsychosocial framework. Journal of Substance Use, 10(4),
191197. doi:10.1080/14659890500114359
Gross, J. J., & Jazaieri, H. (2014). Emotion, emotion regulation,
and psychopathology: An affective science perspective. Clini-
cal Psychological Science, 2(4), 387401. doi:10.1177/
2167702614536164
Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation:
Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook
of emotion regulation (pp. 324). New York, NY: Guilford
Press.
Hemenover, S. H., & Bowman, N. D. (2018). Video games,
emotion, and emotion regulation: Expanding the scope. Annals
of the International Communication Association, 42(2),
125143. doi:10.1080/23808985.2018.1442239
Hoffner, C. A., & Lee, S. (2015). Mobile phone use, emotion
regulation, and well-being. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and
Social Networking, 18(7), 411416. doi:10.1089/cyber.
2014.0487
Hormes, J. M., Kearns, B., & Timko, C. A. (2014). Craving
Facebook? Behavioral addiction to online social networking
and its association with emotion regulation decits. Addiction,
109(12), 20792088. doi:10.1111/add.12713
32 |Journal of Behavioral Addictions 8(1), pp. 2534 (2019)
Blasi et al.
Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for t indexes
in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus
new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6(1), 155.
doi:10.1080/10705519909540118
Hussain, Z., & Grifths, M. D. (2009). The attitudes, feelings,
and experiences of online gamers: A qualitative analysis.
CyberPsychology & Behavior, 12(6), 747753. doi:10.1089/
cpb.2009.0059
Kaczmarek, L. D., & Drążkowski, D. (2014). MMORPG escapism
predicts decreased well-being: Examination of gaming time,
game realism beliefs, and online social support for ofine
problems. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking,
17(5), 298302. doi:10.1089/cyber.2013.0595
Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2014a). Problematizing excessive online
gaming and its psychological predictors. Computers in Human
Behavior, 31, 118122. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.017
Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2014b). A conceptual and methodological
critique of Internet addiction research: Towards a model of
compensatory Internet use. Computers in Human Behavior, 31,
351354. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.059
Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2014c). The moderating role of psychoso-
cial well-being on the relationship between escapism and
excessive online gaming. Computers in Human Behavior,
38, 6874. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.020
Khazaal, Y., van Singer, M., Chatton, A., Achab, S., Zullino, D.,
Rothen, S., Khan, R., Billieux, J., & Thorens, G. (2014). Does
self-selection affect samplesrepresentativeness in online
surveys? An investigation in online video game research.
Journal of Medical Internet Research, 16(7), e164.
doi:10.2196/jmir.2759
King, D. L., Delfabbro, P. H., Potenza, M. N., Demetrovics, Z.,
Billieux, J., & Brand, M. (2018). Internet gaming disorder
should qualify as a mental disorder. Australian & New Zealand
Journal of Psychiatry, 52(7), 615617. doi:10.1177/
0004867418771189
Király, O., Urbán, R., Grifths, M. D., Ágoston, C., Nagygyörgy,
K., Kökönyei, G., & Demetrovics, Z. (2015). Psychiatric
symptoms and problematic online gaming: The mediating
effect of gaming motivation. Journal of Medical Internet
Research, 17(4), e88. doi:10.2196/jmir.3515
Klemanski, D. H., Curtiss, J., McLaughlin, K. A., & Nolen-
Hoeksema, S. (2017). Emotion regulation and the transdiag-
nostic role of repetitive negative thinking in adolescents with
social anxiety and depression. Cognitive Therapy and Re-
search, 41(2), 206219. doi:10.1007/s10608-016-9817-6
Kuo, A., Lutz, R., & Hiler, J. (2016). Brave new World of
Warcraft: A conceptual framework for active escapism. Jour-
nal of Consumer Marketing, 33(7), 498506. doi:10.1108/
JCM-04-2016-1775
Kuss, D. J., Louws, J., & Wiers, R. W. (2012). Online gaming
addiction? Motives predict addictive play behavior in massive-
ly multiplayer online role-playing games. Cyberpsychology,
Behavior, and Social Networking, 15(9), 480485.
doi:10.1089/cyber.2012.0034
Maroney, N., Williams, B. J., Thomas, A., Skues, J., & Moulding,
R. (2018). A stress-coping model of problem online
video game use. International Journal of Mental Health and
Addiction. Advance online publication. 114. doi:10.1007/
s1146
Murphy, J., Brewer, R., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2017). Interocep-
tion and psychopathology: A developmental neuroscience
perspective. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 23,
4556. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2016.12.006
Paulus, D. J., Vanwoerden, S., Norton, P. J., & Sharp, C. (2016).
Emotion dysregulation, psychological inexibility, and shame
as explanatory factors between neuroticism and depression.
Journal of Affective Disorders, 190, 376385. doi:10.1016/
j.jad.2015.10.014
Petry, N. M., Rehbein, F., Gentile, D. A., Lemmens, J. S., Rumpf,
H. J., Mößle, T., Bischof, G., Tao, R., Fung, D. S., Borges, G.,
Auriacombe, M., González Ibá˜nez, A., Tam, P., & OBrien,
C. P. (2014). An international consensus for assessing Internet
gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach. Addiction,
109(9), 13991406. doi:10.1111/add.12457
Reinecke, L. (2009). Games and recovery: The use of video and
computer games to recuperate from stress and strain. Journal of
Media Psychology, 21(3), 126142. doi:10.1027/1864-
1105.21.3.126
Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: An R package for structural equation
modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 136.
doi:10.18637/jss.v048.i02
Schimmenti, A., & Caretti, V. (2010). Psychic retreats or psychic
pits? Unbearable states of mind and technological addiction.
Psychoanalytic Psychology, 27(2), 115132. doi:10.1037/
a0019414
Schimmenti, A., Guglielmucci, F., Barbasio, C., & Granieri, A.
(2012). Attachment disorganization and dissociation in virtual
worlds: A study on problematic Internet use among players of
online role-playing games. Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 9(5),
187195.
Schimmenti, A., Infanti, A., Badoud, D., Laloyaux, J., & Billieux,
J. (2017). Schizotypal personality traits and problematic
use of massively-multiplayer online role-playing games
(MMORPGs). Computers in Human Behavior, 74, 286293.
doi:10.1016/j.chb.2017.04.048
Schimmenti, A., Passanisi, A., Caretti, V., La Marca, L., Granieri,
A., Iacolino, C., Gervasi, A. M., Maganuco, N. R., & Billieux,
J. (2017). Traumatic experiences, alexithymia, and Internet
addiction symptoms among late adolescents: A moderated
mediation analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 64, 314320.
doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.11.002
Schimmenti, A., Starcevic, V., Gervasi, A. M., Deleuze, J., &
Billieux, J. (2018). Interference with processing negative
stimuli in problematic Internet users: Preliminary evidence
from an emotional Stroop task. Journal of Clinical Medicine,
7(7), 177. doi:10.3390/jcm7070177
Sighinol, C., Norcini Pala, A., & Chiri, L. R. (2010). Difculties in
Emotion Regulation Scale: Traduzione e adattamento Italiano.
Psicoterapia Cognitiva e Comportamentale, 16(2), 141170.
Snodgrass, J. G., Bagwell, A., Patry, J. M., Dengah II, H. F.,
Smarr-Foster, C., Van Oostenburg, M., & Lacy, M. G. (2018).
The partial truths of compensatory and poor-get-poorer Internet
use theories: More highly involved videogame players experi-
ence greater psychosocial benets. Computers in Human
Behavior, 78, 1025. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2017.09.020
Snodgrass, J. G., Lacy, M. G., Dengah II, H. F., Eisenhauer, S.,
Batchelder, G., & Cookson, R. J. (2014). A vacation from your
mind: Problematic online gaming is a stress response. Com-
puters in Human Behavior, 38, 248260. doi:10.1016/
j.chb.2014.06.004
Van Rooij, A. J., Ferguson, C. J., Colder Carras, M., Kardefelt-
Winther, D., Shi, J., Aarseth, E., Bean, A. M., Bergmark, K. H.,
Journal of Behavioral Addictions 8(1), pp. 2534 (2019) |33
Problematic gaming and emotional dysregulation
Brus, A., Coulson, M., Deleuze, J., Dullur, P., Dunkels, E.,
Edman, J., Elson, M., Etchells, P. J., Fiskaali, A., Granic, I.,
Jansz, J., Karlsen, F., Kaye, L. K., Kirsh, B., Lieberoth, A.,
Markey, P., Mills, K. L., Nielsen, R. K. L., Orben, A.,
Poulsen, A., Prause, N., Prax, P., Quandt, T., Schimmenti,
A., Starcevic, V., Stutman, G., Turner, N. E., van Looy, J., &
Przybylski, A. K. (2018). A weak scientic basis for gaming
disorder: Let us err on the side of caution. Journal of Behav-
ioral Addictions, 7(1), 19. doi:10.1556/2006.7.2018.19
Victor, S. E., & Klonsky, E. D. (2016). Validation of a brief version
of the Difculties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS-18) in
ve samples. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
Assessment, 38(4), 582589. doi:10.1007/s10862-016-9547-9
Villani, D., Carissoli, C., Triberti, S., Marchetti, A., Gilli, G., &
Riva, G. (2018). Videogames for emotion regulation: A sys-
tematic review. Games for Health Journal, 7(2), 8599.
doi:10.1089/g4h.2017.0108
Wang,L.,Wu,L.,Wang,Y.,Li,H.,Liu,X.,Du,X.,&Dong,G.
(2017). Altered brain activities associated with craving and
cue reactivity in people with Internet gaming disorder: Evidence
from the comparison with recreational Internet game users.
Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1150. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01150
World Health Organization. (2018). ICD-11 for mortality and
morbidity statistics. Retrieved from https://icd.who.int/
browse11/l-m/en#/http://id.who.int/icd/entity/1448597234
Yee, N. (2006). Motivation for play in online games. CyberPsychol-
ogy & Behavior, 9(6), 772775. doi:10.1089/cpb.2006.9.772
Yen, J. Y., Yeh, Y. C., Wang, P. W., Liu, T. L., Chen, Y. Y., & Ko,
C. H. (2018). Emotional regulation in young adults with Internet
gaming disorder. International Journal of Environmental Re-
search and Public Health, 15(1), 30. doi:10.3390/ijerph15010030
Yildiz, M. A. (2017). Emotion regulation strategies as predictors
of Internet addiction and smartphone addiction in adolescents.
Journal of Educational Sciences and Psychology, 7(1), 6678.
Zanetta-Dauriat, F., Zermatten, A., Billieux, J., Thorens, G.,
Bondol, G., Zullino, D., & Khazaal, Y. (2011). Motivations
to play specically predict excessive involvement in massively
multiplayer online role-playing games (MMORPGs): Evidence
through an online survey. European Addiction Research, 17,
185189. doi:10.1159/000326070
34 |Journal of Behavioral Addictions 8(1), pp. 2534 (2019)
Blasi et al.
... In 2019, gaming disorder was included as a mental disorder in the 11th edition of the International Classification of Diseases [4], and its worldwide prevalence is estimated to be around 1-2% [5]. Psychological models posit that over-involvement in video games and virtual worlds may serve to fulfil needs unachieved in real life through a compensatory process [6][7][8]. A systematic review on maladaptive player-game relationships reported that gamers who report diminished self-concept (e.g. ...
... This three-week baseline corresponds to international standards for defining an adequate quasi-experimental single-case methodology with enough data points per phase [34]. 6 The baseline assessed gaming disorder symptoms, social anxiety symptoms and a series of psychological factors that we Figure 2. Study design and representation of the various steps of the psychological assessment. (Note: the intersections between the 'month and year' columns and the 'psychometric instruments' rows indicate which psychometric instrument was administered at which time during the baseline, intervention and three-month follow-up phases. ...
... However, this was eventually not feasible, so we created groups based on participant availability and ensured that none of the participants in the same group knew each other. 6 We initially planned to have baselines of three to six measurement points (i.e. different number of measurement points for each group) with the onset of the experimental phase randomly determined for each group (group 1 starts after a three-week baseline, group 2 starts after a four-week baseline, etc.). ...
Article
Full-text available
Gamers with poor self-concept, high social anxiety and high loneliness are more at risk of problematic involvement in video games, such as massively multiplayer online role-playing games. There is a research gap concerning treatment approaches to cater to socially anxious gamers with problematic patterns of gaming involvement. This registered exploratory pilot programme tests the feasibility and initial effect of a structured protocol in which socially anxious online gamers are exposed to real-life social interactions while playing an offline tabletop role-playing game (TTRPG). Our structured protocol lasted 10 weeks and involved 10 sessions organized into three modules in which participants played a TTRPG inspired by the game ‘Dungeons and Dragons’. Each module deployed a role-playing scenario designed to challenge the players in game terms and involve them in a story based on maturing relationships with other characters and solving challenges by social means and investigation. Our study used a quasi-experimental multiple single-case design with a three-week baseline across groups (four groups of five gamers with sub-clinical problematic video game use and social anxiety) and a three-month follow-up. Primary outcomes were time spent gaming, gaming disorder symptoms and social anxiety symptoms. Secondary outcomes were assertiveness/social skills, self-concepts and perceived loneliness. In terms of feasibility, we observed that most participants completed the programme (two of the 20 participants dropped out) and were involved in terms of participation and weekly psychometric assessments. Moreover, participants were largely able to attain the progressively more difficult objectives implemented in the TTRPG programme. Multiple single-case analyses showed that most participants benefited from the intervention through a reduction in social anxiety symptoms and problematic gaming symptoms, although to varying degrees. Some participants also reduced their gaming time or presented with reduced perceived loneliness. Assertiveness and self-concepts were not improved. This pilot study shows that a TTRPG intervention approach is feasible and may be used to reduce social anxiety and gaming disorder symptoms. The present programme must now be tested with clinical participants.
... Individuals suffering from IGD have been shown to withdraw from real-life activities, seeking to satisfy psychological needs by engaging in compensatory behaviors within the in-game environment (Antons et al., 2023;Blasi et al., 2019;Li et al., 2021;Stavropoulos et al., 2023;Zhang et al., 2022). This prioritization of in-game activities over those in gamers' real lives may result from heightened flow experiences during gaming sessions (Hu et al., 2021). ...
Article
Full-text available
The experience of online flow, characterized by immersion and time distortion, may enhance gaming enjoyment but also contribute to disordered gaming patterns. The present paper examined online flow components and their associations with disordered gaming symptoms among 565 role-playing-gamers. Network analysis identified the centrality of time distortion and loss of control within flow states and highlighted their connections to disordered gaming indicators, such as impaired control and gaming prioritization. Enjoyment and positive challenge were core features within flow, suggesting they support adaptive engagement. However, pathways linking prolonged immersion with gaming disorder symptoms indicate that intense flow experiences may increase problematic gaming risks. These findings reveal a nuanced interaction between online flow and disordered gaming, where adaptive enjoyment and challenge can coexist with maladaptive control loss. Overall, the findings highlight how elements of flow can differentially affect gaming outcomes, contributing both to healthy engagement and potential disorder.
... A list of the measures in alphabetical order, including a description, reference, number of items, and the reliability coefficient.Gaming behaviour variables included years of play, hours played per week, preferred platform (e.g., computer), preferred game genre (e.g., MOBA), and preferred game (e.g., Fortnite).EscapismThe original scale was developed by Yee[66] to measure motivations to play MMORPGs. This scale was adapted by Di Blasi et al.[67] to measure gaming ESCAPISM motivations of World of Warcraft (WoW) players. In the current study, this scale was adapted to refer to a broader online gamer sample. ...
Article
Full-text available
Internet gaming is a popular pastime, which has surged in popularity in light of COVID‐19. While positive outcomes are common, some gamers experience problems associated with their gaming. For this reason, understanding how factors can influence healthy versus unhealthy internet gaming outcomes is crucial. This study developed gamer typologies based on boredom proneness, escapism, and coping style. Typologies were compared on problematic gaming outcomes, gaming behaviours, engagement, and health and wellbeing–related variables. Cluster analysis of a diverse international sample ( N = 913) identified three distinct profiles: immersed, balanced, and detached gamers. Detached gamers reported the highest boredom proneness, escapism, and disengaged coping. They also had worse health and wellbeing–related outcomes than other clusters, despite engaging in similar gaming behaviours to immersed gamers. These novel findings emphasise the importance of an engaged coping style and highlight the risk of using solely addiction‐based measures to identify problematic internet gaming, which may overlook potentially adaptive behaviours.
... Moreover, compensation feelings and negative reinforcement may play an important role even in the very early stages of addictive behaviors or even in casual behaviors, for example, when using games or pornography to cope with stress. Particularly at the beginning, the experience of compensation appears to be closely related to (maladaptive) coping strategies (e.g., Di Blasi et al., 2019;Kardefelt-Winther, 2014, 2017. However, at a conceptual level, a greater distinction should be made between the experience during the use itself (i.e., compensation) and the application of the behavior based on a specific motivation (i.e., coping strategies). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and aims The Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model of behavioral addictions is used relatively often as a scientific framework to specify research hypotheses and to interpret empirical findings in behavioral addiction research. There are, however, controversial interpretations in the literature regarding some specific elements of the model, which may require a more precise definition of specific constructs and processes that are central to the I-PACE model. Methods This is neither a comprehensive literature review nor a proposal for a new version of the I-PACE model. We aim to provide a selective, critical evaluation of some interpretations of the model and to include recent developments regarding addiction theories and controversial debates. Results The role of gratification and compensation and therefore positive and negative reinforcement are specified. The concepts of cue-reactivity and craving are considered in the context of desire thinking and permissive beliefs. The relationships between impulsive, habitual, and compulsive behaviors in behavioral addictions are discussed. The effects of general self-control and situation-specific executive functions are elaborated. Punishment (in)sensitivity is discussed as a further important process potentially involved in behavioral addictions. These constructs and processes (through their interactions) are considered in the context of changes over time in the course of addictive behaviors. Conclusion This viewpoint article aims to provide greater precision and clarity regarding some specific elements of the I-PACE model, which may help stimulate research and theory building and advance clinical care in the behavioral addiction field.
... Social anxiety and loneliness were chosen as measures of social self-concept, as they are commonly measured in the relevant literature [40,41]. Emotional regulation was chosen to assess emotional self-concept for similar reasons, being often used in literature regarding related topics [42]. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Tabletop gaming is becoming increasingly relevant in mainstream culture and psychological research, making it important to understand the factors that drive people to engage in this activity. In this article, we present the design of a study aimed at investigating the relationship between self-concept constructs-such as self-esteem and emotional self-concept-and the motivation to play tabletop games. Additionally, we explore how these factors interact with the tendency to identify with game characters during play. We discuss the theoretical framework underpinning the study and outline the methodological approach we intend to follow.
Article
This study aims to explore the relationship between LGBTQ+ culture in video games and brand resonance, aiming to understand to what extent the use of LGBTQ+ cultural assets can affect brand awareness, quality, and loyalty between LGBTQ+ and non-LGBTQ+ consumers. An online survey collected data from 488 people, and a proposed model was tested using SEM multigroup analysis. Findings show that the impact of the “LGBTQ+ cultural assets” differs between LGBTQ+ and non-LGBTQ+ consumers. The results of this study suggest the LGBTQ+ culture in a video game is the key predictor of brand loyalty among LGTBQ+ consumers.
Article
Full-text available
Although it has been proposed that problematic Internet use (PIU) may represent a dysfunctional coping strategy in response to negative emotional states, there is a lack of experimental studies that directly test how individuals with PIU process emotional stimuli. In this study, we used an emotional Stroop task to examine the implicit bias toward positive and negative words in a sample of 100 individuals (54 females) who also completed questionnaires assessing PIU and current affect states. A significant interaction was observed between PIU and emotional Stroop effects (ESEs), with participants who displayed prominent PIU symptoms showing higher ESEs for negative words compared to other participants. No significant differences were found on the ESEs for positive words among participants. These findings suggest that PIU may be linked to a specific emotional interference with processing negative stimuli, thus supporting the view that PIU is a dysfunctional strategy to cope with negative affect. A potential treatment implication for individuals with PIU includes a need to enhance the capacity to process and regulate negative feelings.
Article
Full-text available
It is argued that problem video game use (PVGU) has similarities with behavioral addictions such as problem gambling. Unlike other addictions, the predictors of online PVGU have not been studied extensively. We applied a stress-coping model, previously developed for electronic gambling addiction, to PVGU. In this model, stressors lead to excessive behavior via maladaptive coping strategies involving the behavior. Video game players (N = 2261) completed an online questionnaire about their gaming habits, and self-report measures of depression, loneliness, social anxiety, and escapism and social interaction motives for gaming. Consistent with the stress-coping model, depression, loneliness, and social anxiety predicted levels of PVGU, these effects being partially mediated by escapism and social interaction motives for gaming. The pattern of mediation differed by gamers’ preferred game genre in a way that suggested “First Person Shooter” games provide an escape from aversive states, while, in addition to providing escape, massively multiplayer online role playing games, which emphasize collaborative play, may also be supplementing or substituting for face-to-face social interactions.
Article
Full-text available
We greatly appreciate the care and thought that is evident in the ten commentaries that discuss our debate paper, the majority of which argued in favor of a formalized ICD-11 gaming disorder. We agree that there are some people whose play of video games isrelated to life problems. We believe that understanding this population and the nature and severity of the problems they experience should be a focus area for future research. However, moving from research construct to formal disorder requires a much stronger evidence base than we currently have. The burden of evidence andthe clinical utility should be extremely high because there is a genuine risk of abuse of diagnoses. We provide suggestions about the level of evidence that might be required: transparent and preregistered studies, a better demarcation of the subject areathat includes a rationale for focusing on gaming in particular versus a more general behavioral addictions concept, the exploration of non-addiction approaches, and the unbiased exploration of clinical approaches that treat potentially underlying issues such as depressive mood or social anxiety first. We acknowledge there could be benefits to formalizing gaming disorder, many of which were highlighted by colleagues in their commentaries, but we think they do not yet outweigh the wider societal and public health risks involved. Given the gravity of diagnostic classification and its wider societal impact, we urge our colleagues at the WHO to err on the side of caution for now and postpone the formalization.
Article
Full-text available
Recent years have seen important advances in the study of video games, with many scholars examining how video games impact emotional experiences of players. While this research borrows heavily from traditional media effects paradigms, it has proceeded without effectively integrating developments in emotion and emotion regulation. Therefore, this paper first reviews the nature of emotion and emotion regulation, and then reviews the extant video game literature. We next draw on theoretical accounts of emotion to frame three broad research questions that may advance future work on video games and emotions: (1) What is the nature of video game-evoked emotional experiences? (2) What are the processes through which video games produce emotions? (3) How are video games used to regulate emotions?
Article
Full-text available
Introduction: Emotion regulation (ER) supports multiple individual functions and promotes mental health and wellbeing. Among the tools that may be used to help people in managing their affective states, videogames are reaching attention and are showing positive effects. Yet, little is known about their effectiveness. Objective: This study aims to assess the amount and quality of studies investigating the effects and modalities of the use of videogames for ER. Materials and methods: A systematic literature search according to PRISMA guidelines was performed. Subsequently, according to expert advice other few studies have been added. Results: Twenty-three studies met the inclusion criteria and were included in the review; they can be categorized into three groups, namely (1) cross-sectional and qualitative studies, (2) experimental studies investigating the effects of videogame experience on ER and (3) ER intervention with serious games. Discussion: Discussion of the reviewed studies highlights that frequent gaming with commercial games offers more opportunities for ER improvement (related to gameplay and enjoyment of fictional properties) than limited-time experiences, such as those supported by bespoke serious games. This research area is still in its infancy and findings need to be interpreted with caution; furthermore, future reviews are encouraged to include clinical populations. Conclusion: Videogames offer several opportunities for ER and a challenge for educational and psychological interventions.
Article
Full-text available
Background Risky behaviors have been related to emotional regulation and attachment, which may constitute risk factors for developing an addictive behavior. However, there may also be differences between substance and non-substance-related addictions. Aims This study aimed to examine the relationship of emotional regulation and attachment, with substance (alcohol and drug abuse), and non-substance-related addictions (gambling disorder, video game addiction, and problematic Internet use) in adolescents and emerging adults. The study also aimed to examine gender differences for such predictors. Methods The sample comprised 472 students aged 13–21 years recruited from high schools and vocational education centers. Results Findings demonstrated that emotion regulation was predictive of all addictive behaviors assessed in this study (alcohol and drug abuse, gambling disorder, video game addiction, and problematic Internet use), whereas attachment predicted non-substance-related addictions (gambling disorder, video game addiction, and problematic Internet use). In addition, gender differences were found, with females scoring significantly higher in maternal and peer attachment, whereas males scored significantly higher in gambling disorder and video game addiction. Conclusion The findings may be useful for preventive and clinical interventions conducted with youth regarding addictive behaviors.
Article
The main purpose of this study was to examine the longitudinal mediation effects of multiple gaming motives from online game involvement to problematic Internet use (PIU). In terms of mediators, the present study included three specific gaming motivations - escapism, advancement, and socializing. A sample of 389 gamers participated in the study from freshman to junior college years. Gaming involvement was collected at the freshman year, motives at sophomore, and PIU at junior. Initially, the three gaming motives were examined separately for their longitudinal mediation effects in a series of single-mediator models. Next, the three motives were simultaneously included in a multiple-mediator model to compare the relative levels of their mediation effects from gaming involvement to PIU across a period of two years. In the multiple-mediator model, the most compelling results showed that escapism and advancement were positively associated with PIU, yet socializing was not; socializing failed to significantly predict later PIU. Also, the direct effect of gaming involvement on PIU was weak and failed to reach significant level, contradicting to the findings of previous cross-sectional studies. Many game players experience great pleasure from immersing themselves in a virtual world, and often to the extent of developing addictive symptoms. This phenomenon is referred to as “pull” effect which is considered comparable to the motive of “pursuing in-game advancement” in the present study. Conversely, frustrations or other negative experiences in real life might also bring about a “push” effect that tugs players into gaming. The motive of escapism from the real-life in this study resembles the so-called “push” effect. The authors suggest when gamers demonstrated social motive during game time, such as participating in a gamer community and collaborating with the others despite being socially withdrawn in real life, it would be less reasonably to be regarded as PIU. During game time, if gamers experience a temporary sense of well-being, it might help to vent their real-life frustration. However, an excessive use of this emotion-focused coping strategy might still increase the risk of PIU.