Access to this full-text is provided by Springer Nature.
Content available from Applied Network Science
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Applied Network Science
Leitão et al. Applied Network Science (2019) 4:3
https://doi.org/10.1007/s41109-018-0110-3
RESEARCH Open Access
Quantifying long-term impact of court
decisions
Jorge C. Leitão1,2, Sune Lehmann1* and Henrik Palmer Olsen2
*Correspondence: sljo@dtu.dk
1Technical University of Denmark,
Anker Engelundsvej 1, DK-2800 Kgs
Lyngby, Denmark
Full list of author information is
available at the end of the article
Abstract
In this work, we investigate how court decisions aggregate citations in the European
Court of Human Rights. Using the Bass model, we quantify the prevalence of the
rich-get-richer phenomenon. We find that the Bass model provides an excellent
description of how individual decisions accumulate citations. Our analysis reveals that
citations to a large fraction of decisions are, in fact, explained by the rich-get-richer
phenomenon. Based on our statistical model, we argue that network properties are
insufficient to explain the rich-get-richer effect, suggesting that intrinsic properties of
decisions drive a significant part of the observed citation patterns. We conclude by
discussing the legal implications of our findings.
Keywords: Law, Citation networks, Bass model, Preferential attachment
Introduction
The advance of computerized systems in the administrative systems of courts enables
systematic access to the complete database of their decisions. The use of statistical
methodologies to analyze such decisions has brought new insights into the collective
institutional behavior of courts. Behaviors that otherwise would not be immediately vis-
ible when studying a court’s decisions judgment by judgment. In this respect, network
science has played an important role in beginning to understand courts’ behavior through
the analysis of the citation patterns of a number of courts across the globe (Fowler et al.
2006; Fowler and Jeon 2008;LupuandVoeten2012;BlackandSpriggs2013).
A citation network of court decisions shares similarities with a network of scientific
citations: it is a directed acyclic network whose set of outgoing edges is fixed once the
node is created. Furthermore, a citation can be caused by numerous reasons as it can e.g.
depend on the semantic content of the two documents, on the semantic content of related
items, on the citations by the cited item, etc. Irrespectively of the reason an item is cited,
a citation reveals usage of that particular item by the relevant community and the study
of this usage reveals important features about the underlying dynamics of the community
(Wang et al. 2013; Derlén and Lindholm 2014; Tarissan and Nollez-Goldbach 2015), its
structure (Fowler et al. 2006; Fowler and Jeon 2008; Derlén and Lindholm 2017)and,in
some cases allows predicting the future usage of individual items (Wang et al. 2013).
A major question in law is how a court uses past decisions to legitimize new decisions
(Dworkin 1986). In the case of international courts, where the international law is set by
an immutable international treaty, such as the European Convention on Human Rights,
© The Author(s). 2019 Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0
International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the
Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Leitão et al. Applied Network Science (2019) 4:3 Page 2 of 15
a major question is whether the interpretation of the treaty changes over time, and by
how much (Popovic 2008). This process is closely related to how past decisions end up
being adopted by the court in future decisions. The European Court of Human Rights
has over time interpreted, and continues to interpret, the various rights provisions in a
way that can best be described as incrementally progressive. In each case decided, this
court tends to either stand firm on its previous decision or to go one step further in its
elaboration of what protection for the individual, the convention offers. If it stands firm it
will rely on a previous decision as a determinate interpretation of the convention (i.e. not
developing the interpretation). If it goes further it will rely on a principle articulated in the
earlier decision, and argue that that principle requires that the new case is included under
that principle (= developing the interpretation further). In both cases – whether it stands
firm or goes one small step further – it will cite it’s own previous case law in support
of it’s decision. In this way the court gradually builds a system of still more elaborate
standards and principles, and the citation of earlier decisions plays an important role in
this development (Gerards 2018).
In this paper we quantitatively describe the evolution of the usage of court decisions
within the European Court of Human Rights (ECHR). Previous research has inquired into
legal citations practices in both domestic courts, such as the United States Supreme Court
(Fowler et al. 2006) and international courts, such as the Court of Justice of the European U nion
(CJEU) (Mirshahvalad et al. 2012), the World Trade Organization (W TO) (Pauwel yn 2015),
and the International Court of Justice (ICJ) (Alschner and Charlotin 2018). By setting
out to quantitatively describe the evolution of the usage of court decisions within the
ECHR, our research, therefore, should be seen as a contribution to an emerging body of
knowledge on how Courts manage their case-law portfolio via their citation practices.
We propose a model for the usage of the decision by the court based on preferential
attachment (Bass model (Bass 1999;2004)) and show that this model provides an excel-
lent description of the evolution of the number of citations of individual court decisions.
We use the Bass model to quantify the extent to which preferential attachment drives
the increase in usage of individual decisions – and to predict the future usage of indi-
vidual decisions by the court. Being able to predict the future usage of a case in terms
of how many times it will be cited in the upcoming period, it is possible to identify
whether a specific case is on an upward trend or whether its usage has stabilized or is
perhaps descending. Combined with knowledge that can be gained from other research
(e.g. (Christensen et al. 2016; Šadl and Olsen 2017; Olsen and Küçüksu 2017;Aletrasetal.
2016)), about the legal themes and problems dealt with in the case, the prediction about
the intensity of future use can be used strategically, for example to guess how the court
will reason in the new case.
This paper is structured as follows: in the section “The ECHR and empirical data”
we describe the European Court of Human Rights, and the details of the dataset we
used. In the section “The use of decisions within the court” we show that the way in
which a decision reaches their current number of citations is a heterogeneous dynam-
ical process. In “Dynamical model for the evolution of number of citations”wemodel
each decision’s evolution in terms of number of citations as a dynamical process com-
promising two distinct processes, rich-get-richer and heterogeneous, external factors.
We demonstrate the model’s usefulness as a description of the vast majority of citation-
aggregation in the court’s decisions. In “Importance of rich-get-richer effect”weanalyze
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Leitão et al. Applied Network Science (2019) 4:3 Page 3 of 15
the prevalence of the rich-get-richer and heterogeneous processes as drivers of the court’s
decisions and argue that these do not depend on the properties of the network. In
the section “Predicting future usage” we use the model to make reliable predictions of
the future usage of court decisions, confirming that the model is useful to both describe
the past and predict the future. In “Conclusions” we conclude with an outlook of the main
results, including a discussion of the legal implications of our findings.
The ECHR and empirical data
The European Court of Human Rights was founded in 1959 and it rules on individual or
state applications alleging violations of the civil and political rights set out in the European
Convention on Human Rights.
Applications sent to the court can either be admitted or rejected. Some applications are
rejected outright as being manifestly ill-founded. Others are admitted or rejected after
more careful deliberation that is set out in the court’s admissibility decisions. The appli-
cations that are admitted to the court will be decided on its merits in a ruling, which will
say whether there has been a violation of the Convention. Decisions on the merits are
issued as Judgments. It is this final category of decisions (judgments) that we have focused
on. Moreover, we only focus on those decisions that have been written in English. While
the majority of the cases sent to the court are ruled inadmissible, the cases with an actual
judgment are the ones cited by the court in other decisions and therefore these define the
court’s jurisprudence. For this reason, we restricted this analysis to admitted cases with an
English text until the end of 2016. In total, we considered 17509 cases with 70 861 direct
citations between each other. Figure 1shows the yearly number of these decisions in the
database, showing a clear exponential increase of decisions.
The use of decisions within the court
Irrespectively of the content of each decision, past decisions are used by the court as part
of its justification for new decisions (see also “Introduction” section above). Operationally,
the court does this by citing past decisions. A decision can be cited for different reasons,
but, independent of the reason, each citation by definition provides evidence for the use of
a previous decision. Therefore, the number of citations is, by definition, a useful quantifier
of the usage of a decision within the court (Fowler et al. 2006; Fowler and Jeon 2008;Lupu
Fig. 1 The exponentially increasing number of decisions by the court from 1960 to present day. The x-axis
shows time and the y-axis shows number of decisions made per year
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Leitão et al. Applied Network Science (2019) 4:3 Page 4 of 15
and Voeten 2012;BlackandSpriggs2013), which we adopt here. The usage of an item
is often different from the average usage (Newman 2010); this is also the case here and
the resulting broad distribution of citations is also observed in ECHR decisions. Figure 2
(top) shows the ordered (by number of citations) sequence of all decisions of the court
until 2016, revealing a broad distribution of the number of citations; some decisions have
almost 1000 citations, while a large majority of decisions have zero or very few citations.
There are different possible explanations for the broad distribution of citations. The
most well-known is a rich-get-richer mechanism (Simon 1955), also known as preferen-
tial attachment (Barabási and Albert 1999). The rich-get-richer phenomenon is present
in a variety of natural and cultural contexts, ranging from city population sizes to the
number of neighbors in protein-protein interaction networks (Newman 2005; Clauset et al.
2009). In the context of citations between scientific papers, the hypothesis is that the more
attention an item has, the more attention it gets.
An important question that arises in the context of the court’s citations is whether rich-
get-richer is the mechanism that drives the importance of a decision. If yes, why is this the
case? What mechanisms contribute for the usage of a decision? To begin to address these
questions, we systematically analyze how each highly-cited decision of the ECHR today
aggregated their citations over time. As a starting point, we note that the time-evolution
of how court decisions are used is extremely heterogeneous (Fig. 3): different decisions
take very different paths towards becoming highly used, suggesting that there is no single
mechanism which explains the evolution of usage within the court.
Fig. 2 (top) The ranked number of citations. On the x-axis is every decision of the court, where 1 is the most
cited decision and the last number the least cited decision. The y-axis is the number of citations of the
decision. (Bottom) The average number of citations of the decisions as a function of time, showing that a
decision will get on average 5 citations, and that this value is roughly independent of time (y-axis)
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Leitão et al. Applied Network Science (2019) 4:3 Page 5 of 15
Fig. 3 The evolution of highly-cited decisions is heterogeneous. In order to illustrate the many different
shapes of citation curves, we plot the trajectories for all decisions with more than 30 citations. The x-axis is
the month after publication divided by the total number of months after publication so far (end of 2016). The
y-axis shows the number of citations of the decision at a given month divided by the total number of
citations so far. Each line corresponds to a decision. The individual decisions are color-coded from most cited
to least cited black>violet >orange (color); black>grey (gray scale)
Dynamical model for the evolution of number of citations
Our first goal is to describe the heterogeneous behaviour presented in Fig. 3quanti-
tatively. Motivated by the idea that decisions become used/adopted by the court, we
consider the Bass model of adoption processes (Bass 1999;2004). The bass model is one
of the simplest models of adoption processes, and has been used to describe different
adoption patterns ranging from technology to consumer products, language and citations
(Bass 2004). Given its simplicity and ease of interpretation, we consider it to be a relevant
model to approach this problem.
The first hypothesis underlying the Bass model is that there is a limit to the number
of citations that a decision will aggregate. In the context of law, this corresponds to the
notion that, with time, old decisions are less likely to be cited (Tarissan et al. 2016). There
are several reasons this might be the case. First of all, the social, economic and political
contexts in which a given decision is made will change over time. Even though the legal
principle remains the same, at some point the court will find it more appropriate to cite
a more recent decision. It will most likely only do so once the principle is so firmly estab-
lished that it will be seen as uncontroversial. Citing a new case for the same principle
may also allow the court to update the principle. By using a slightly changed formulation
it may introduce subtle new changes to the principle that will help the court to adapt its
case law to changing circumstances (Letsas 2013). Another reason why an old case may
not be cited is because the court has refined its case law and thereby the principles it uses
to decide cases. What was once one broad principle may have dispersed into two or more
more specialized principles, thereby leaving the original principle superfluous or extinct1.
The speed with which old cases cease to be cited differs according to which specific area
of law it belongs. This is because the court receives many cases in some areas and thereby
continuously updates and refines the legal principles with which it operates, whereas, in
other areas, the court may only get very few cases, which means that old cases may remain
important precedent for a much longer time. In some ways this process is similar to how
scientific publications (Wang et al. 2013) and technology (Bass 1999) tends to go out of
use over time.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Leitão et al. Applied Network Science (2019) 4:3 Page 6 of 15
The Bass model includes this hypothesis mathematically by stating that the increase in
number of citations, dx(t)/dt,isproportionalto(ci−x(t))where ciis a constant repre-
senting the maximum number of citations that decision iwill obtain. Notice that cidoes
not correspond to the maximum number of citations observed in the data, since decisions
may still be cited in the future.
The second hypothesis underlying the Bass model is that there are two mechanisms that
describe the increase in the number of citations. One mechanism is the rich-get-richer,
widely known in the community of network science as preferential attachment (Barabási
and Albert 1999;Newman2010). Specifically, this is included in the Bass model in the
form of linear preferential attachment,
dx(t)
dt ∝aix(t)(ci−x(t)),
where aiis known as the ‘fitness parameter’ in network science (Newman 2010). This
second mechanism generating attention in the Bass model is designed to incorporate
external factors to the decision (initial attractiveness in networks (Newman 2010)), bi,
and describes factors (not proportional to the number of citations) that contribute to the
increase of number of citations. These two mechanisms are assumed to be independent.
Therefore the full Bass can be written as
dx(t)
dt =(b+ax(t))(
c−x(t)),(1)
where we have dropped the sub-scripts for simplicity. The solution of this equation (with
initial condition x(0)=0) is given by
x(t)=ac et(a+bc)−1
aet(a+bc)+bc .(2)
To model the behavior observed for the each decision, we fit the parameters a,band
cto the empirical citation curves, using maximum likelihood estimates (see Additional
file 1)2.InFig.4we show two representative examples of this procedure and respective
best estimates.
To confirm the generality of this result, we used two approaches: firstly, we computed
theaveragerelativeerror
3of the models for every decision: this distribution of average
relative errors, Fig. 5), decays exponentially and averages at about 15%. Secondly, we split
the data in train (first 80% that we used to fit) and test (last 20% that we used to compute
the errors): the distribution of relative errors is similar to the original distribution, see
Fig. 9). Overall, these observations demonstrate an excellent representation of a generic
decision by the Bass model.
Importance of rich-get-richer effect
We now focus on quantifying the overall importance of each of the two mechanisms
described above, namely, rich-get-richer and external factors. We quantify the effect of
each mechanism on a given decision by computing the relative contribution, Grgr and Gext
to the expected total number of citations c. That is, of all ccitations a decision will have,
cGext describes the fraction explained by external factors and cGrgr describes the fraction
explained by the rich-get-richer mechanism (according to the model). The fractions Grgr
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Leitão et al. Applied Network Science (2019) 4:3 Page 7 of 15
Fig. 4 The best fit of the Bass describes the observed evolution of the number of citations well. The upper
panel corresponds to the decision (by merits and just satisfaction) in the case Boyle and Rice V. the United
Kingdom (1988); the lower panel corresponds to the decision in the case Edoardo Palumbo V. Italy (2001).
The gray region corresponds to the estimated standard deviation (see supp. information)
and Gext can be computed analytically for the Bass model from the parameters a,band c
and are given by Ghanbarnejad et al. (2014)
Gext =a
bc log 1+bc
a,Grgr =1−Gext .(3)
The relative contributions quantify whether the use of a decision is driven by the rich-
get-richer mechanism, external factors, or a combination of the two. A decision with high
Fig. 5 The empirical distribution of the average relative error of the model against the data (ensembled
ove r all decisions with more than 30 citations) decays exponentially with in creas ing e rror, ind icati ng th at th e mod el
des cribe s the majority of the cases. The relative error of a decision iis defined as i≡1
tmax tmax
t=0|xi(t)−x(t)|/xi(t)
where x(t)corresponds to Eq. 2with the best parameters obtained from MLE
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Leitão et al. Applied Network Science (2019) 4:3 Page 8 of 15
Grgr (low Gext) has citations that were obtained gradually throughout time, in a shape that
resembles an S curve (see Fig. 4, upper panel for an example). Conversely, a decision with
high Gext (low Grgr) characterizes a case where the number of citations were obtained
soon after the decision was published, with interest gradually fading (exponential increase
after publication, see Fig. 4, lower panel for an example). These contrasting behaviors
reflect different ways in which decisions are used by the court.
In Fig. 6we summarize Gext for all decisions. Analyzing this figure, we note that at
around decision rank 50 (approximately 10% of the cases considered) have a value for
Gext of 0.5 or larger. The remaining 90% percent of cases are explained primarily by the
rich-get-richer mechanism. In fact, for 80% of all decisions, the rich-get-richer mech-
anism explains more than 70% of all citations. In summary, the majority of citations
is well described by a combination of rich-get-richer and external factors, and that the
rich-get-richer factors tend to play a larger role in how decisions are cited.
The rich-get-richer mechanism is known to describe well how scientific publications
(Wang et al. 2013) and innovations (Bass 1999;2004) acquire attention. In the context
of scientific publications, the reason behind preferential attachment is that the more a
scientific publication is known, the more likely it is to be cited, and the more cited it is,
themorewellknownitis.
In legal practice, decisions are decided on the basis of a set of facts specific to the indi-
vidual case and references should reflect that by specifically referring to earlier cases with
a similar fact content. This is the basic content of the classic doctrine Stare Decisis4.
Therefore, it is not immediately obvious that we should expect to see the rich-get-richer
effect in legal practice.
We now make a case to why the rich-get-richer should be observed in this court. Our
argument is that with increasing case-load and faster paced societal development, legal
practices tend to become increasingly standardized and bureaucratized. This leads to a
reliance on general principles of law to decide cases. Herman Oliphant noted this devel-
opment in the US already in 1928 (Oliphant 1928). Oliphant notes a development from
Stare Decisis to what he calls Stare Dictis, on which Judges and Legal Scholars increas-
ingly rely on generalizations from prior cases to form abstract principles of law, which are
then more freely applied to new cases.
Fig. 6 The rank of all decisions (with more than 30 citations) by Gext .Thex-axis shows the decision rank,
ordered from highest external to lowest external; the y-axis shows the value of Gext of the decision
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Leitão et al. Applied Network Science (2019) 4:3 Page 9 of 15
To see how this practice may have emerged in the European Court of Human Rights,
we note that the European Court of Human Rights is overburdened with cases, see
Additional file 1. To manage its load, the court relies on various procedures that allows it
to decide groups of cases that are perceived by the court to derive from the same underly-
ing (structural problem). By definition, this is Stares Dictis: the underlying argument for
deciding for the group is no longer the particularities of the individual cases but rather a
common abstract principle of law. Since each of these principles are well represented by a
limited number of cases, these cases will be competing for their merit in representing the
specific principle. This mechanism is well known to lead to a rich-get-richer effect where
the more a case is used, the better it represents the principle.
To illustrate how this dynamics is present in the ECHR, we provide examples of two
procedures used by the court to mitigate the overburdeness of the court and that results
in cases competing for merit. One procedure is the courts’ use of ‘case law guides’5which
is aimed at potential litigants and which sets out to explain the main content of the court’s
case law. In these publications, the court refers to its own case law, another practice which
may also enforce the rich-get-richer principle by reinforcing the tendency to use case law
highlighted in these publications when deciding future cases. Another procedure is the
so-called ‘Pilot Judgment Procedure’ 6which allows the court to dismiss a whole group of
cases at a time. Those cases that are referenced in the pilot judgments will be considered
more important and will therefore feature as leading principles (Stare Dicta in Oliphant’s
terminology) in the Court’s case law.
In summary, while one should not immediately expect a rich-get-richer effect in legal
practice, the specific practical constraints of the European Court of Human Rights
can explain its occurrence. We hypothesise that rich-get-richer is observed in other
overburden courts that apply Stare Dictis to mitigate it.
Grgr is an intrinsic property of the decision
Recall that the factors Gext and Grgr are not related to how many citations a decision
has (or will have), but how these citations come about. A natural question is why certain
decisions are mainly driven by rich-get-richer factors, while others are driven by external
effects. We imagine that there is a number of factors that might play a role for a decision to
be driven by the rich-get-richer mechanism, such as the number of citations the decision
has, the year it was published, how broad the decision is judicially, etc. To understand the
role of these different factors, we used a multiple regression model7of y=Gext against
a broad range of different meta-data and network properties of the decision. Specifically,
for each case we considered (for the 2016 citation network)
•Number of citations (incoming links).
•The decision’s clustering coefficient in the citation network.
•The decision’s centrality in the citation network (implemented using Katz, page rank,
eigenvector, betweenness, and closeness centrality).
•The year it was published.
•The country the decision is about.
•The outcome of the decision (violation or non-violation of the treaty).
•The judicial breadth of the decision (number of convention articles to which it
relates).
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Leitão et al. Applied Network Science (2019) 4:3 Page 10 of 15
•What legal domains the decision relates to (implemented as one-hot encoding of the
articles to which the decision relates).
Surprisingly, we found that none of the features listed above have a strong linear correla-
tion with Gext (that is, R2<0.5 for each of the features). Further, using the features above,
we were unable to formulate a statistical model that explains the observed behaviors. This
indicates that Gext (and Grgr)isnotasimpleconsequenceofhowmuchthedecisionwas
cited, or properties of the network. For example, the fact that the clustering coefficient
does not play a role, rules out the hypothesis that high rich-get-richer phenomenon is due
to its neighbors being highly connected. Even though no linear correlation exists between
Grgr and the measures above, the most cited decisions all have a high Grgr (See top panel
of Fig. 7). In other words, a high Grgr is a necessary (but not sufficient) condition for a case
to develop into one of decisions most cited by the court. Given our inability to formulate
an accurate statistical model based on the features above, our findings suggest that Grgr
is an intrinsic property (Wang et al. 2013) of the decision. Such intrinsic properties could
be related to legal content and semantic context of a decision rather than the features we
have considered here.
Predicting future usage
Knowing which decisions will be used in the future is relevant for all stakeholders of the
court. This knowledge can be used to decide when to submit a new case on a specific
topic, to identify decisions that are most active in the court, etc. The Bass model describes
the evolution of the number of citations of decisions very well; it does so over a large
Fig. 7 Top: Grgr vs the number of citations in 2016; Botton: Grgr vs the local clustering coefficient in 2016
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Leitão et al. Applied Network Science (2019) 4:3 Page 11 of 15
period of time for decisions of different ages. This means that the model can be used to
predict decisions’ future usage by the court.
To confirm that this is indeed the case, we tested the predictive ability of the model
using a simple form of cross-validation. We once again fitted a simple Bass model to each
decision. This time, however, we trained (fitted) the model on only 80% of the data avail-
able for every decision, while withholding the most recent 20% of data for testing the
model. We can now compare the extrapolations based on our training data to the real cita-
tions aggregated over the 20% next months. The result of this analysis is shown in Fig. 8
for two examples. To confirm that this holds more generally, we computed the average
error over all decisions, which we measured to be less than 10% (see Fig. 9for the full his-
togram). This confirms the high predictive power of this model to future usage of court
decisions.
Conclusions
In this paper we quantitatively describe the patterns of decisions from one of the most
important international courts. Our main findings are that the Bass model provides an
excellent description of the time evolution of cases. We confirmed this through two differ-
ent results: a small RMS error when fitting the model and a overall good predictive power.
This model allows us to probe the importance of the rich-get-richer effect in the court’s
Fig. 8 The best fit of the model describes the future evolution of the number of citations well. The upper panel
corresponds to the decision (by merits and just satisfaction) in the case Boyle and Rice V. the United Kingdom
(1988) ; the lower panel corresponds to the decision in the case Edoardo Palumbo V. Italy (2001). The black
dots correspond to the number of citations of the decision up to 80% of its total age, the red dots correspond
to the remaining 20%, that were not used in the fit. The black curve with gray region correspond to the best fit
of the 80% of the months along with the extrapolated 20%, showing an overall excellent predictive power
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Leitão et al. Applied Network Science (2019) 4:3 Page 12 of 15
Fig. 9 The probability of the average relative error of the model on the last 20% of hidden months
(calculated as an ensemble over all decisions with more than 30 citations) decays exponentially, indicating
that the model describes the majority of the cases. The average of this histogram is 10%
citation network, and we find that some citation patterns in the ECHR are characterized
by this phenomenon.
The finding that the rich-get-richer effect applies to how decisions are used within the
court, is a clear signal that the court has a systematic approach to case citation that goes
beyond the traditional stare decisis doctrine of fitting the same law to same facts. Previous
research has indicated that case citation in very active international courts has become
routinized and/or standardized. In Ref. Panagis et al. (2017), for example, the authors
show that the CJEU makes use of so called judicial formulae, which are often repeated.
Such repeated formulae are used to continuously (re)establish the fundamental principles
and concepts of European Union law. Over time, however, the formulae detach from the
judgments in which they were first pronounced and acquire a broader relevance. They
begin to function as abstract rules and thereby take on a legal significance beyond the fact
constellation in which it was first articulated (Panagis et al. 2017). A similar point is made
by Ref. McAuliffe (2015), but via a study of how CJEU judgments are crafted8.
The rich-get-richer observed here effect suggest that the court may have routinized its
citation practice, offering the same citations for many decisions in cases that have broadly
similar facts. It is an indication, therefore, that the court – in its citation patterns – oper-
ates with a broader notion of which citations fit which cases, than one might otherwise
expect. The traditional characteristic of judicial decision-making is precisely that it is a
very individualized process in which the specific case is scrutinized under the law and
decided by carefully comparing it to previous cases that very closely share the same facts.
Our finding indicates that the ECHR operates with a broader notion of sameness or sim-
ilarity, when it cites previous case law as justification for its decisions. This could be seen
as a sign that the court operates more freely – and thereby more politically – when it
decides cases: There is a larger space between the facts of a case, the justification of the
decision, and the citations that is being used to justify that decision. These findings cor-
respond well, with previous legal research which indicated that the court operates a kind
of judicial diplomacy (Madsen 2010;Olsen2015).
Finally, to investigate the issue of preferential attachment in further depth, we then
attempt to understand how individual cases aggregate citations, using network and con-
textual (legal domain) features in order to estimate how much the rich-get-richer effect
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Leitão et al. Applied Network Science (2019) 4:3 Page 13 of 15
plays a role for a particular decision. Finally, we find support for explaining the rich-get-
richer effect by way of the legal science point-of-view (that we have presented above) in
the fact that we were unable to build a useful statistical model to describe the rich-get-
richer phenomenon. The features we use do not capture the batching which occurs as part
of the ‘Pilot Judgment Procedure’, nor the ‘case law guides’ which streamlines the cases
themselves. We expect that a promising avenue for future research on this topic could be
to explore the connection between these streamlining procedures and citation patterns
in the court, a question which has an interplay with the more general challenge of how
algorithms shape decision making in public institutions (O’Neil 2016).
Endnotes
1As illustrated for example by the court’s official case law guide for art. 8, which breaks
this short rights provision (“Everyone has the right to respect for his private and family
life, his home and his correspondence”) up into more than 50 different strings of case law.
2We also explored the model proposed in Ref. Wang et al. (2013). This model describes
the existing data equally well, but predicts an unrealistically high (1010 number of cita-
tions for about 30% of all decisions considered), suggesting lack of predictive power in
our case.
3We used relative error, as opposed to absolute error, because decisions have an skewed
number citations, see Fig. 2. A relative metric allows to better interpret the error of the
model in respect to the decision’s number of citations. A model of a decision whose
average relative error is 10% signifies that the model is on average 10% away ofeach datum
4There are numerous public accounts of how this doctrine works in modern law, see
for example: https://www.law.cornell.edu/wex/stare_decisis
5See: https://www.echr.coe.int/Pages/home.aspx?-p=-caselaw/
analysis&c=#n13794084798725475324837_-pointer.
6See: https://www.echr.coe.int/Documents/Pilot_-judgment_-
procedure_ENG.pdf.
7We also tested a more sophisticated method (a random forest regressor with hyper-
parameter optimization and k-fold cross-validation), and found similar results.
8Network analysis has also, as mentioned above, been used to investigate case cita-
tion behavior in other international courts such as the WTO appellate body and the ICJ.
Although network analysis is a useful method for inquiring into case citation behavior
in these courts, their case law is not sufficiently rich to fully validate a rich-get-richer
phenomenon in these courts. As an illustration of the difference in how much data is pro-
duced, Alschner and Charlotin (2018) observes 1 865 citations that the ICJ make to its
own case law. In our dataset we observe 70 861 citations that the ECHR makes to its own
case law.
Additional file
Additional file 1:Supplementary information. (PDF 36.3 kb)
Abbreviations
ECHR: European court of human rights; CJEU: Court of justice of the european union; ICJ: International court of justice
Acknowledgements
The authors thank unnamed reviewers for valuable help and feedback.
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Leitão et al. Applied Network Science (2019) 4:3 Page 14 of 15
Funding
Sune Lehmann acknowledges the Independent Research Fund Denmark (Project: Microdynamics of Influence in Social
Systems).
Availability of data and materials
GitHub repository with code and tests to recreate the database is available here: https://github.com/jorgecarleitao/echr_
network/.
Authors’ contributions
JCL, SL, HPO conceived the paper, JCL prepared and analyzed the data. JCL, SL, HPO wrote the paper. All authors read
and approved the final manuscript.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Author details
1Technical University of Denmark, Anker Engelundsvej 1, DK-2800 Kgs Lyngby, Denmark. 2University of Copenhagen,
Karen Blixens Plads 16, DK-2300 Copenhagen, Denmark.
Received: 24 August 2018 Accepted: 19 December 2018
References
Aletras N, Tsarapatsanis D, Preo¸tiuc-Pietro D, Lampos V (2016) Predicting judicial decisions of the european court of
human rights: A natural language processing perspective. PeerJ Comput Sci 2:93
Alschner W, Charlotin D (2018) The growing complexity of the international court of justice’s self-citation network. Eur J
Int Law 29(1):83–112
Barabási A-L, Albert R (1999) Emergence of scaling in random networks. Science 286(5439):509–512
Bass F (1999) A New Product Growth for Model Consumer Durables. Manag Sci 16(5):1825–1832. https://doi.org/10.1287/
mnsc.15.5.215
Bass FM (2004) Comments on “A New Product Growth for Model Consumer Durables The Bass Model”. Manag Sci
50(12_supplement):1833–1840. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.1040.0300
Black RC, Spriggs JF (2013) The Citation and Depreciation of U.S. Supreme Court Precedent. J Empir Leg Stud
10(2):325–358. https://doi.org/10.1111/jels.12012
Christensen ML, Olsen HP, Tarissan F (2016) Identification of case content with quantitative network analysis: An example
from the ecthr. In: JURIX. IOS Press, Amsterdam. pp 53–62
Clauset A, Shalizi CR, Newman ME (2009) Power-law distributions in empirical data. SIAM Rev 51(4):661–703
Derlén M, Lindholm J (2014) Goodbye van gend en loos, hello bosman? using network analysis to measure the
importance of individual cjeu judgments. Eur Law J 20(5):667–687
Derlén, M, Lindholm J (2017) Is it good law? network analysis and the cjeu’s internal market jurisprudence. J Int Econ Law
20(2):257–277
Dworkin R (1986) Law’s Empire. Harvard University Press, Cambridge
Fowler JH, Jeon S (2008) The authority of Supreme Court precedent. Soc Networks 30(1):16–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
socnet.2007.05.001
Fowler JH, Johnson TR, Spriggs JF, Jeon S, Wahlbeck PJ (2006) Network Analysis and the Law: Measuring the Legal
Importance of Precedents at the U.S. Supreme Court. Political Anal 15(3):324–346. https://doi.org/10.1093/pan/
mpm011
Gerards JH (2018) Dealing with divergence. margin of appreciation and incrementalism in the case-law of the european
court of human rights. Hum Rights Law Rev 18(3 1 Sept 2018):495–515. forthcominb Oxford University Press / Oxford
Academic. Oxford
Ghanbarnejad F, Gerlach M, Miotto JM, Altmann EG (2014) Extracting information from S-curves of language change. J R
Soc Interface 11(101)
Letsas G (2013) The echr as a living instrument: Its meaning and legitimacy. Constituting Eur: Eur Court Hum Rights
National, Eur Global Context 2:106
Lupu Y, Voeten E (2012) Precedent in International Courts: A Network Analysis of Case Citations by the European Court of
Human Rights. Br J Polit Sci 42(02):413–439. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0007123411000433
McAuliffe K (2015) Behind the scenes at the court of justice: A story of process and people. In: Davies B, Nicola F (eds). EU
Law Stories. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. pp 141–172
Madsen MR (2010) Legal diplomacy: law, politics and the genesis of postwar european human rights. In: Human Rights in
the Twentieth Century. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. pp 62–81
Mirshahvalad A, Lindholm J, Derlén M, Rosvall M (2012) Significant Communities in Large Sparse Networks. PLoS ONE
7(3):33721. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0033721
Newman ME (2005) Power laws, pareto distributions and zipf’s law. Contemp Phys 46(5):323–351
Newman M (2010) Networks: An Introduction. Oxford University Press
Oliphant H (1928) A return to stare decisis. ABAJ 14:71
Olsen HP (2015) International courts and the doctrinal channels of legal diplomacy. Transl Leg Theory 6(3-4):661–680
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
Leitão et al. Applied Network Science (2019) 4:3 Page 15 of 15
Olsen HP, Küçüksu A (2017) Finding hidden patterns in ecthr’s case law: On how citation network analysis can improve
our knowledge of ecthr’s article 14 practice. Int J Discrimination Law 17(1):4–22
O’Neil C (2016) Weapons of Math Destruction: How Big Data Increases Inequality and Threatens Democracy. Broadway
Books, Danvers
Panagis Y, Sadl U, Tarissan F (2017) Giving every case its (legal) due the contribution of citation networks and text
similarity techniques to legal studies of european union law. In: 30th International Conference on Legal Knowledge
and Information Systems (JURIX’17), vol 302. IOS Press, Amsterdam. pp 59–68
Pauwelyn J (2015) Minority rules: Precedent and participation before the wto appellate body. In: Jemielniak J, Nielsen L,
Olsen HP (eds). Judicial Authority in International Economic Law. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. pp 141–172
Popovic D (2008) Prevailing of judicial activism over self-restraint in the jurisprudence of the european court of human
rights. Creighton L Rev 42:361
Šadl U, Olsen HP (2017) Can quantitative methods complement doctrinal legal studies? using citation network and
corpus linguistic analysis to understand international courts. Leiden J Int Law 30(2):327–349
Simon HA (1955) On a class of skew distribution functions. Biometrika 42(3/4):425–440. Kettering
Tarissan F, Nollez-Goldbach R (2015) Temporal properties of legal decision networks: a case study from the international
criminal court. In: 28th International Conference on Legal Knowledge and Information Systems (JURIX’2015). IOS
Press, Amsterdam
Tarissan F, Panagis Y, Šadl U (2016) Selecting the cases that defined europe: complementary metrics for a network
analysis. In: Advances in Social Networks Analysis and Mining (ASONAM), 2016 IEEE/ACM International Conference
On. IEEE, Piscataway. pp 661–668
Wang D, Song C, Barabási A-L (2013) Quantifying Long-Term Scientific Impact. Science 342(6154):127–132. 10.1126/
science.1237825
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:
use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at
onlineservice@springernature.com
Available via license: CC BY
Content may be subject to copyright.