ArticlePDF Available

Oestrous females avoid mating in front of adult male bystanders in wild chacma baboons

Authors:
  • Institut des Sciences de l'Evolution - Montpellier 2
  • Institut des Sciences de l'Evolution de Montpellier

Abstract and Figures

In social species, female mating strategies can be constrained by both male and female groupmates through sexual conflict and reproductive competition, respectively. This study tests if females adjust their sexual behaviour according to the presence of male and female bystanders in wild chacma baboons (Papio ursinus) and assesses their relative importance. Our results show that oestrous females initiate fewer copulations in the presence of adult male bystanders, irrespective of whether they are mate-guarded or not. This inhibitory effect probably reflects a response to indirect sexual coercion by males, whose close proximity may dissuade females to initiate copulations with rival males to avoid punishment and/or aggressive mating interference. By contrast, females initiate more matings with their mate-guard in the presence of higher-ranking female bystanders, which may reflect an attempt to secure bodyguard services from their mate when they feel threatened. These results emphasize the importance of intra- and intersexual conflicts in shaping female sexual behaviour in this promiscuous society.
This content is subject to copyright.
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos
Research
Cite this article: Baniel A, Delaunay A,
Cowlishaw G, Huchard E. 2019 Oestrous females
avoid mating in front of adult male bystanders in
wild chacma baboons. R. Soc. open sci. 6:
181009.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsos.181009
Received: 21 June 2018
Accepted: 13 December 2018
Subject Category:
Biology (whole organism)
Subject Areas:
behaviour/evolution
Keywords:
social influence, intersexual conflicts, intrasexual
competition, chacma baboon
Author for correspondence:
Alice Baniel
e-mail: alice.baniel@gmail.com
Electronic supplementary material is available
online at https://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.
c.4344401.
Oestrous females avoid
mating in front of adult male
bystanders in wild chacma
baboons
Alice Baniel1,2,3, Axelle Delaunay1, Guy Cowlishaw3
and Elise Huchard2
1
Institute for Advanced Study in Toulouse, Toulouse, France
2
ISEM, Univ Montpellier, CNRS, EPHE, IRD, Montpellier, France
3
Institute of Zoology, Zoological Society of London, London, UK
AB, 0000-0001-7142-5864
In social species, female mating strategies can be constrained by
both male and female groupmates through sexual conflict and
reproductive competition, respectively. This study tests if
females adjust their sexual behaviour according to the presence
of male and female bystanders in wild chacma baboons (Papio
ursinus) and assesses their relative importance. Our results
show that oestrous females initiate fewer copulations in the
presence of adult male bystanders, irrespective of whether they
are mate-guarded or not. This inhibitory effect probably
reflects a response to indirect sexual coercion by males, whose
close proximity may dissuade females to initiate copulations
with rival males to avoid punishment and/or aggressive
mating interference. By contrast, females initiate more matings
with their mate-guard in the presence of higher-ranking female
bystanders, which may reflect an attempt to secure bodyguard
services from their mate when they feel threatened. These
results emphasize the importance of intra- and intersexual
conflicts in shaping female sexual behaviour in this promiscuous
society.
1. Introduction
In social species, male and female groupmates frequently
constrain the reproductive strategies of females. For example,
dominant females often harass other females and interfere with
their mating attempts, and monopolize resources that are
necessary to breed, such as shelters, mates or offspring care [1].
Similarly, males may attempt to control female sexuality
through the use of forced copulations, sexual harassment,
intimidation and punishment [2 4].
&2019 The Authors. Published by the Royal Society under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution License http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/, which permits
unrestricted use, provided the original author and source are credited.
In turn, females may evolve counter-strategies to alleviate the costs of social control, by inhibiting or
hiding copulations in the presence of potentially aggressive bystanders [5,6]. In many promiscuous
primates, copulations are conspicuous because females give loud vocalizations at the end of the
copulation. These copulation calls are thought to be sexually selected traits that advertise female
sexual receptivity to stimulate male– male competition and/or sperm competition—and ultimately
obtain higher quality offspring [7 10], or to confuse paternity and reduce infanticide risk [11].
However, for subordinate females, it may not always be advantageous to signal their copulations, and
they may even benefit from concealing or restraining their sexual activity in some contexts, to escape
aggression from harassing males and dominant females. In line with this, females and subordinate
males are more likely to copulate and emit copulation calls when dominant males are out of sight in
wild geladas (Theropithecus gelada) [12] and in captive macaques (Macaca mulatta and Macaca
fascicularis) [6,13,14], while in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), the presence of high-ranking females
around a mating couple inhibits the emission of copulation calls [15,16].
Further studies are needed to decipher the generality and evolutionary consequences of social
influences linked to the presence or absence of third party individuals on female sexual behaviour
in promiscuous species. While several reports have shown that dominant males inhibit the sexual
behaviour of group members [6,12,14], studies on the effects of dominant female bystanders remain
rare (but see [13,15]). Examining in parallel the effect of bystanders from both sexes may thus shed
light on the relative importance of the social constraints at play. Moreover, previous studies focusing
on social influence on copulations have rarely investigated whether the male or the female mating
partner is the most inhibited by bystanders, which would enhance our understanding of the
proximate and evolutionary determinants of such phenomena.
This study examines the relative importance of male and female bystanders on female mating
behaviour in wild chacma baboons (Papio ursinus). Chacma baboons live in multimalemultifemale
groups and mate promiscuously. Previous research has shown that adult males direct regular
aggression towards oestrous females as a form of sexual intimidation [17], and mate-guard them near
ovulation [18]. In addition to this direct coercion—where males are aggressive towards females to
increase their own reproductive success—it is possible that males also use indirect coercion, by
punishing oestrous females following copulations with rivals to dissuade them from mating with
those males again. Social constraints on female sexual behaviour do not arise exclusively from adult
males, as oestrous females also face intense aggression from other females, reflecting both mating
competition and reproductive suppression over paternal care [19– 21]. In particular, pregnant and
lactating females are often closely associated with an adult male, usually their offspring’s sire [22,23],
who provides them and their offspring with paternal services such as protection against predators
and conspecifics [2426]. These non-fertile females harass oestrous females who attempt to mate with
the sire of their offspring, which decreases the victim’s chances of conception with him [19]. The
mechanism mediating this reproductive suppression is not yet elucidated and could include
interference in copulations and/or a stress-induced disruption of reproductive physiology.
Female strategies to escape or alleviate those social constraints on their sexual behaviour have not been
investigated so far. Despite femalefemale harassment and male coercion, female baboons utter
conspicuous copulation calls at almost every copulation [8], but they may avoid copulating in the presence
of harassers. We hypothesize that oestrous females will be less likely to initiate copulations in the presence
of male bystanders (hypothesis 1, H1) due to sexual coercion [17] and/or higher-ranking females (H2) due
to intrasexual competition over mating opportunities or male social partners [19,20], in order to avoid
immediate aggression and copulation interference. We further hypothesize (H3) that male bystanders
would inhibit the sexual behaviour of oestrous females more strongly than female bystanders given the
importance of sexual dimorphism in this species, where male aggression is the main source of injury for
oestrous females [17]. In the course of these analyses, we consider two additional issues. First, that these
inhibitory effects will likely be altered by the mate-guarding status of oestrous females, because mate-
guarded females may be less able to modulate the occurrence and timing of copulations compared to
unguarded females and may further benefit from the protective effect of their mate-guard. We therefore
investigate the interaction between mate-guarding status and social influences in our models. Second, that
apparent social constraints on sexual behaviour could arise from other aspects affecting all copulations,
rather than from active female strategies. For example, if copulations always occur on the periphery of
the group, fewer neighbours would be present without involving inhibition. To rule out such a
possibility, we further compare social influences on female- and male-initiated copulations: similar
social influences on both sexes would raise questions over the interpretation that these effects
specifically reflect a female strategy.
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. open sci. 6: 181009
2
2. Material and methods
2.1. Data collection
Data were collected in JuneOctober 2013 and May– November 2014 from two groups of wild
chacma baboons living at Tsaobis Nature Park, Namibia (for details of the site and population,
see [27]). Group composition is given in electronic supplementary material, table S1. All
individuals were recognizable and habituated to observations at close range. The ages of
individuals (in years) were known from long-term life-history records. Males were considered
adult at eight years of age and females when they reached menarche [28]. Female dominance
ranks were established using ad libitum and focal observations of approach-avoid interactions
(displacements, supplants) and agonistic interactions (attacks, chases, threats) (see electronic
supplementary material, appendix 1).
In baboons, sexually receptive females develop perineal swellings during oestrus, which increase in
size as ovulation approaches [29]. Trained observers recorded the swelling size of females daily through a
visual assessment [29], using a 7-point scale scoring system. Mate-guarding episodes (or ‘consortships’)
were defined as periods when an oestrous female is constantly followed by a male who mates exclusively
with her [30]. The formation of consortships, and changes in the identity of partners involved, were
monitored ad libitum on a daily basis for all females of the group, and mate-guarding associations
were further confirmed at the start of each focal observation of an oestrous female to ensure the
accuracy of our records.
Observers on foot followed both groups daily from dawn to dusk, conducting focal animal sampling
on all adult females and males. All observers (except AB) were blind with respect to the research
questions. Focal follows lasted 60 min on average and were spread equally across the day (split into
four 3-h time blocks: 6.009.00 a.m., 9.0012.00, 12.0015.00, 15.0018.00). Focal individuals were
chosen in a semi-random manner, in order to balance observation time equally across individuals and
time periods, and each individual was never sampled more than once a day. We collected N¼487
observations of 32 oestrous females (mean +s.d.: 15.2 +8.8 per individual) and N¼551 observations
of 25 adult males (mean +s.d.: 22.0 +9.3 per individual). All occurrences of copulations were
recorded (with both juvenile and adult males), together with the identity of sexual partners and who
was responsible for initiating the copulation. Copulations were considered initiated by females when
they approached first and presented their hindquarters to males or when they solicited copulations
using facial expressions (come-here faces and/or lip-smacks), and by males when they approached
first and grabbed the female’s hindquarters or used the same facial expressions. Unclear cases were
recorded as ‘unknown initiator’. Immediately after each copulation (i.e. within 30 s maximum), we
performed a comprehensive audience scan (copulation scan) recording the identity of all adult males
and females present within 05 m of the mating couple during the copulation. Audience scans were
also performed routinely every 10 min (non-copulation scans). In both copulation and non-copulation
scans, individual neighbours were recorded even if they were out of sight of the focal subject (e.g.
because of vegetation). Non-copulation scans occurring within less than 10 min of copulations were
removed to ensure that non-copulation scans referred only to non-mating contexts. For mate-guarded
females, the male consort was omitted from the ‘non-copulation scans’ in consistency with ‘copulation
scans’ (where the copulating consort is not included) to avoid an artificial increase of the mean
number of adult males around mate-guarded females in non-copulation scans. Table 1 summarizes the
number of copulation and non-copulation scans where at least one adult male and higher-ranking
female were present within 5 m of the focal female.
2.2. Statistical analyses
We tested whether male and female bystanders inhibit the copulations of oestrous females by comparing
the composition of the audience (i.e. individuals standing in the immediate proximity of the mating
couple, and therefore presumably in visual and hearing range of their behaviour) in copulation versus
non-copulation scans. A binomial Generalized Linear Mixed Model (GLMM) was run with a logit link
function, using the occurrence of female-initiated copulations as the response variable (1/0, copulation
present/absent, drawing on female-initiated copulation scans and female-focal non-copulation scans,
respectively). We included female-initiated copulations collected during both male and female
observations (because the audience during copulation is the same for the male and female mating)
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. open sci. 6: 181009
3
but used only non-copulation scans from female observations, to control for the regular audience of
oestrous females only. Fixed factors included:
the number of adult male bystanders
the number of female bystanders that outranked the focal female
the mate-guarding status of the focal female (0: unguarded; 1: mate-guarded). Unguarded females
could copulate with any juvenile or adult male. Mate-guarded females could only copulate with
their consort male (we had only 2 cases of extra pair copulations out of 397 copulations; these two
cases were removed from subsequent analyses).
we also tested the significance of the interactions between the mate-guarding status of the focal female
and the number of (1) male bystanders and (2) higher-ranking female bystanders to test if social
influences vary according to mate-guarding status.
the swelling size of the focal female (to control for increasing sexual activity as females approach
ovulation)
the relative rank and age of the focal female (to control for the fact that sexual activity may differ
among females of various ranks and ages)
the group identity and year of study (to control for other sources of variation in the rates of copulation
across groups and years).
Random factors comprised the identity of the focal female, the identity of the focal observation and the
date of observation.
To ensure that any observed social influence on the probability of copulation reflected the responses
of oestrous females to their audience rather than any general property of copulations, we further
compared social influences on female- and male-initiated copulations to the same non-copulation
scans (in this case the regular audience of oestrous females). We ran a control model (same fixed and
random effects) with male-initiated copulations as the response variable (1/0, copulation present/
absent, drawing on male-initiated copulation scans and female-focal non-copulation scans
respectively) and fitted the same fixed and random effects.
All GLMMs were run using the glmer function of the lme4 package [31] in R v. 3.4.1 [32]. All
quantitative variables were z-transformed to have a mean of zero and a standard deviation of one (by
Table 1. Audience composition of non-copulation scans, female-initiated copulation scans and male-initiated copulation scans.
non-copulation
female-initiated
copulations
male-initiated
copulations
total no. of scans
a
2076 173 290
no. of scans with at least one higher-ranking female
bystander
a
254 17 35
average no. of higher-ranking female bystanders
b
0.16 +0.20 0.11 +0.16 0.08 +0.13
average no. of higher-ranking female bystanders for
unguarded females
b
0.21 +0.31 0.06 +0.15 0.08 +0.21
average no. of higher-ranking female
bystanders for mate-guarded females
b
0.13 +0.18 0.18 +0.25 0.13 +0.24
no. of scans with at least one male bystander
a
378 3 24
average no. of male bystanders
b
0.19 +0.11 0.01 +0.03 0.09 +0.22
average no. of male bystanders for
unguarded females
b
0.33 +0.14 0.01 +0.04 0.05 +0.15
average no. of male bystanders for
mate-guarded females
b
0.02 +0.03 0.00 +0.01 0.08 +0.21
a
These figures are calculated as the sum of available scans for each category of scans in the models.
b
These figures are calculated by averaging the number of male and female bystanders for each focal female and for each type of
scan (non-copulation scans, N¼32 focal females; female-initiated scans, N¼24 focal females; and male-initiated scans,
N¼24 focal females). We provide the mean value +standard deviation.
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. open sci. 6: 181009
4
subtracting the mean from each value and dividing by the standard deviation) to facilitate model
convergence. The significance of the fixed factors was tested using a likelihood ratio test, ‘LRT’
(assuming an asymptotic chi-square distribution of the test statistic), and using the full model to avoid
problems arising from stepwise model-selection procedures [33,34]. We only tested two-way interactions
for which we had a clear prediction. Non-significant interactions were omitted from the full model to
limit risks of over-parametrization and facilitate the interpretation of simple effects. The significance of
the fixed factors was assessed by computing their 95% Wald confidence intervals (using the
confint.merMod function) and by checking that they did not cross zero. To diagnose the presence
of multicollinearity, we calculated the variance inflation factor for each predictor. The maximal value of
VIF was 1.85 for the female-initiated model and 1.73 for the male-initiated model, which are well below
3, and thus do not indicate serious multicollinearity [35]. The correlation coefficient between the number
of male and higher-ranking female bystanders among audience scans was 0.22 for both the female-initiated
and male-initiated models, which falls below the critical threshold of 0.70 [36].
3. Results
Oestrous females initiated fewer copulations when the audience contained more adult males (table 2 and
figure 1a), and this was true for both unguarded and mate-guarded females (comparison of models with
and without an interaction between the number of male bystanders and mate-guarding status:
x
2
1¼2:06,
p¼0.151). By contrast, the effect of higher-ranking female bystanders differed markedly with the mate-
guarding status of oestrous females (comparison of models with and without an interaction between the
number of female bystanders and mate-guarding status:
x
2
1¼7:64, p¼0.006 and table 2). We ran the
GLMM separately for unguarded and mate-guarded females to explore these differences. Mate-
guarded females initiated more copulations when the audience contained more higher-ranking
females (electronic supplementary material, table S2; figure 1b). By contrast, unguarded females rarely
initiated copulations in the presence of dominant females (mean number of higher-ranking females
during female-initiated copulations: 0.06 +0.15 compared to 0.21 +0.31 outside mating context,
table 1 and figure 1b), though this trend did not reach statistical significance (see electronic
supplementary material, table S2). The contrasting mating patterns observed between mate-guarded
and unguarded females did not arise from passive differences in their audiences in non-copulation
scans because mate-guarding females have a similar number of higher-ranking females in proximity
during non-copulation scans (mean number +s.d.: 0.13 +0.18, table 1) than unguarded females
(0.21 +0.31, one sample t-test, t¼1.17, d.f. ¼48.70, p¼0.246).
For male-initiated copulations, the effect of male bystanders differed according to the mate-guarding
status of the female (comparison of models with and without an interaction between the number of
male bystanders and mate-guarding status:
x
2
1¼15:59, p,0.001 and table 3): males were less likely
occurrence of female-initiated copulations?
average no. male bystanders
0
0.1
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
0
0.1
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
no yes
*
occurrence of female-initiated copulations?
average no. higher-ranking
female bystanders
no yes no yes
unguarded
mate-guarded
n.s. *
(b)(a)
Figure 1. Barplot of the mean number of (a) male bystanders and (b) higher-ranking female bystanders during non-copulation and
female-initiated copulation scans. Barplots are drawn from the raw individual means. Error bars indicate the standard error of the
mean. ‘*’: p,0.05, ‘n.s.’: p.0.05.
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. open sci. 6: 181009
5
Table 2. Social influences on the occurrence of female-initiated copulations. Tests are based on 24 females, 43 males copulating and 173 copulation scans (70 of which occurred with juveniles), including 73
copulations involving mate-guarded females and 100 involving unguarded females, and 2076 non-copulation scans. Significant variables appear in bold. s.e.: standard error; LRT: statistic of a likelihood ratio test,
d.f.: degrees of freedom. LRT, d.f. and p-values are not given for fixed effects involved in a significant interaction.
response variable fixed factor estimate s.e. 95% confidence interval LRT d.f. p-value
occurrence of female-initiated
copulations (0/1)
number of higher-ranking female bystanders 20.34 0.36 [21.05 ; 0.37]
number of male bystanders 22.32 0.44 [23.19; 21.45] 49.02 1 <0.001
female is mate-guarded
a
20.85 0.30 [21.44; 20.27]
number of higher-ranking female bystanders : mate-guarding
a
1.08 0.43 [0.24; 1.92] 7.64 1 0.006
swelling size 0.45 0.17 [0.11; 0.79] 7.73 1 0.005
relative rank 20.56 0.47 [21.49; 0.37] 1.75 1 0.186
age 20.68 0.49 [21.64; 0.28] 2.29 1 0.130
troop
b
0.05 0.46 [20.85; 0.94] 0.00 1 0.950
year
c
2.55 0.66 [1.26; 3.84] 39.22 1 <0.001
a
Reference category: female is unguarded.
b
Reference category: J troop.
c
Reference category: 2013.
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. open sci. 6: 181009
6
Table 3. Social influences on the occurrence of male-initiated copulations. Parameters and tests are based on 30 focal females, 51 males copulating and 290 copulation scans (124 of which occurred with juveniles),
including 151 copulations involving mate-guarded females and 139 involving unguarded females, and 2076 baseline scans. Significant variables appear in bold. s.e.: standard error, LRT: statistic of a likelihood ratio
test, d.f.: degrees of freedom. LRT, d.f. and p-values are not given for fixed effects involved in a significant interaction.
response variable fixed factor estimate s.e. 95% confidence interval LRT d.f. p-value
occurrence of male-initiated copulations (0/1) number of higher-ranking female bystanders 0.09 0.16 [20.23; 0.41] 0.30 1 0.582
number of male bystanders 21.17 0.25 [21.67; 20.68]
female is mate-guarded
a
0.29 0.26 [20.22 ; 0.79]
number of male bystanders: mate-guarding
a
1.78 0.45 [0.90; 2.66] 15.59 1 <0.001
swelling size 0.58 0.25 [0.09; 1.07] 6.41 1 0.011
relative rank 20.77 0.45 [21.64; 0.11] 3.55 1 0.059
age 20.70 0.47 [21.61; 0.22] 2.77 1 0.096
troop
b
20.34 0.43 [21.18; 0.50] 0.87 1 0.350
year
c
1.51 0.35 [0.83; 2.20] 24.92 1 <0.001
a
Reference category: female is unguarded.
b
Reference category: J troop.
c
Reference category: 2013.
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. open sci. 6: 181009
7
to initiate copulations with unguarded females in the presence of male bystanders, while mate-guarding
males were not inhibited by male bystanders (table 3 and figure 2a; electronic supplementary material,
table S3). Female bystanders did not influence male-initiated copulations with guarded or unguarded
females (comparison of models with and without an interaction between the number of female
bystanders and mate-guarding status:
x
2
1¼1:60, p¼0.205, figure 2b), suggesting that the reported
social influences on copulations are not simply a general property of copulations. In addition, female-
initiated and male-initiated copulations did not differ in terms of the female audience (0.11 versus
0.08 female bystanders, table 1, one sample t-test, t¼0.69, d.f. ¼45.14, p¼0.491), but female-initiated
copulations have substantially fewer male bystanders than male-initiated copulations (0.01 versus 0.09
male bystanders, table 1, one sample t-test, t¼22.04, d.f. ¼30.54, p¼0.050), suggesting that the
effect of a male audience on female-initiated copulations reflects an active female avoidance strategy.
4. Discussion
Our results show that oestrous females modulate their mating activity in the presence of adult male
bystanders in wild chacma baboons. Although male mating interference and punishment after
copulation with rival males are rare in baboons [17], males that repeatedly harass oestrous females
before ovulation increase their mating success with the victim during the ovulatory period, in a form
of direct sexual coercion [17]. In this context, our current results suggest that females decrease sexual
solicitations in front of male bystanders to avoid subsequent aggression (even if that aggression does
not immediately follow a copulation), as well as the associated risk of injuries [17]. This suggests an
additional, indirect form of sexual coercion in baboons, such that repeated male aggression does not
only encourage oestrous females to mate with them (intimidation) but further dissuades them from
mating with their rivals (punishment). Although we did not find evidence of immediate punishment
(i.e. within 15 min) following copulations with rival males in a previous study [17], we cannot discard
the possibility that some form of punishment exists, which may be non-systematic and delayed. Even
if the risk of punishment is substantial, our ability to detect it is probably hampered by the evolution
of effective counter-strategies such as female avoidance of potential harassers, as suggested by these
results. We further found that male-initiated copulations with unguarded females (mainly by
juveniles) are also inhibited by the presence of adult males. This suggests that the suppressive effect
of male bystanders extends to most group members except high-ranking adult males and that most
copulations may occur away from them. Indeed, only copulations initiated by mate-guarding males,
who are typically high-ranking, were unaffected by the male audience. Overall, our results suggest
that the effects of male bystanders on mating activity are stronger than those of female bystanders
(based on the relative value of the estimate of the effect of male and female bystanders in table 2),
occurrence of male-initiated copulations?
average no. male bystanders
0
0.1
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
0
0.1
0.2
0.3
0.4
0.5
no yes
average no. higher-ranking female bystanders
(b)(a)
occurrence of male-initiated copulations?
no yes no yes
*n.s.
unguarded
mate−guarded
n.s.
Figure 2. Barplot of the mean number of (a) male bystanders and (b) higher-ranking female bystanders during non-copulation and
male-initiated copulation scans. Barplots are drawn from the raw individual means. Error bars indicate the standard error of the
mean. ‘*’: p,0.05, ‘n.s.’: p.0.05.
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. open sci. 6: 181009
8
possibly reflecting the substantial sexual dimorphism in body and canine size; males can induce severe
and life-threatening injuries to females. Such inhibiting effects of dominant males have been found in
many polygynous mammals (e.g. gelada: [12], rhesus macaque: [6,14]; long-tailed macaque: [6,13];
Japanese macaque, Macaca fuscata: [37], Southern elephant seal, Mirounga leonina: [38]) and may
represent an important underpinning mechanism in the male reproductive skew observed in these
species.
Despite previous findings of harassment from dominant females toward subordinate oestrous
females in chacma baboons [1921], we found no evidence that oestrous females inhibit their sexual
behaviour in the presence of dominant female bystanders. Instead, it seems that oestrous females, and
particularly unguarded females, avoid the proximity of high-ranking females both in copulation and
non-copulation contexts, suggesting that high-ranking females harass them continuously, and not just
after a copulation event. Our previous study revealed that pregnant and lactating females forming a
close bond with an adult male harass oestrous females who attempt to mate with him, thereby
decreasing their chances of conception [19]. Together with these new results, this suggests that the
main proximate mechanism mediating female-induced reproductive suppression is not copulation
interference. Rather, chronic stress resulting from repeated harassment directed towards oestrous
females across their cycle (and not just after copulations) may downregulate the reproductive
physiology of the victims [39].
Surprisingly, we found that mate-guarded females initiated more copulations in the presence than in
the absence of higher-ranking females. There was no such effect of high-ranking females on male-
initiated copulations in a context of mate-guarding, suggesting that it is not a passive consequence of
changes in a female’s social environment associated with mate-guarding episodes. This intriguing
result suggests that the mating activity of oestrous females is enhanced by the proximity of high-
ranking rivals in a mate-guarding episode: the presence of rivals may stimulate sexual activity in a
context of reproductive competition where oestrous females feel protected by the proximity of their
male consort. Alternatively, oestrous females may solicit more copulations with their consort when
they feel threatened by the proximity of high-ranking females in an attempt to keep him close, under
a bodyguard scenario. Further analyses on larger sample sizes, that take into account the intensity of
the competition between females by examining social influences on sexual activity in relation to the
friendship status of the female bystanders (i.e. testing whether this effect is stronger when female
bystanders are friends with the mating male, and therefore more likely to direct aggression towards
the female) could help to elucidate this possibility.
The proximate mechanisms underlying the social inhibition of mating activity may result from simple
processes, where females avoid copulating in the presence of certain bystanders or take advantage of
their absence. For example, subordinate male baboons monitor temporary separations between a
dominant male and his mate-guarded female [40], possibly to identify opportunities for copulations
and/or sneaky matings. The frequency of agonistic interactions faced by oestrous females may
similarly force them to monitor constantly the presence of conspecifics in close proximity.
Alternatively, this social inhibition may result from cognitively complex strategies, such as tactical
deception where copulation partners increase their spatial distance [6] or hide intentionally [5,12] from
specific bystanders. Further research will help to clarify the mechanisms at play in baboons, but this
study indicates that passive social influences on female mating strategies may be effective at shaping
female sexual behaviour, including the frequency of matings and the identity of mating partners, in
promiscuous societies.
5. Conclusion
Despite displaying conspicuous copulations, female chacma baboons seem to restrain their sexual
activity in the presence of male bystanders. Why do females hide from adult bystanders if they signal
their copulations so loudly? This apparent paradox may arise from a trade-off between the long-range
audience targeted by their copulatory signal and their immediate social environment. Copulatory
signals are thought to attract preferred mating partners—often high-ranking males—by signalling
female fertility (e.g. [10,41]). However, in the context of a coercive society, it may be safer for females
to avoid copulating in the immediate surroundings of high-ranking males if they do not mate with
them. Male recurrent aggression towards oestrous females may not only encourage females to mate
with them but further discourage them to mate with rivals. In promiscuous societies, females often
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. open sci. 6: 181009
9
seem to have little sexual freedom and may need to use complex tactics to implement their strategies in
an equally complex social landscape.
Ethics. Research procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee of the Zoological Society of London and the
Ministry of Environment and Tourism of Namibia (permit numbers 1786/2013 and 1892/2014) and adhered to the
ASAB/ABS Guidelines for the Treatment of Animals in Behavioural Research and Teaching.
Data accessibility. Data are available through Dryad Digital Repository (https://github.com/AliceBaniel/Social-
influences-on-baboon-matings).
Authors’ contributions. A.B. and E.H. designed the study. A.B and her field team collected the data. A.B. and A.D.
performed the statistical analyses. All authors contributed to draft the manuscript. All authors gave final approval
for publication.
Competing interests. We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding. A.B. was supported by the Agence Nationale de la Recherche Labex IAST and the Ministe
`re de l’Education
Nationale, de l’Enseignement Supe
´rieur et de la Recherche. E.H. was funded by the Agence Nationale pour la
Recherche (Research grant ANR-17-CE02-0008).
Acknowledgements. We are grateful to the Tsaobis Baboon Project volunteers in 2013 and 2014 for invaluable help in the
field, in particular to Ignacio Abadia Suanzes-Carpegna, Andrew Allan, Einat Bar-Ziv, Katie Hatton, Caitlin Miller,
Frieda Shikongo, Chris Smith and Agata Staniewicz. Our thanks also go to the Ministry of Environment and
Tourism for research permission in Namibia, the Gobabeb Research and Training Centre for affiliation, the Tsaobis
beneficiaries for permission to work at Tsaobis and the Snyman and Wittreich families for permission to work on
their land. We are also thankful to two anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments on an earlier version
of the manuscript. This paper is a publication of the ZSL Institute of Zoology’s Tsaobis Baboon Project.
Contribution ISEM 2018-276.
References
1. Clutton-Brock TH, Huchard E. 2013 Social
competition and its consequences in female
mammals. J. Zool. 289, 151–171. (doi:10.
1111/jzo.12023)
2. Muller MN, Wrangham RW. 2009 Sexual
coercion in primates and humans: an
evolutionary perspective on male aggression
against females. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
3. Clutton-Brock TH, Parker GA. 1995 Sexual
coercion in animal societies. Anim. Behav. 49,
1345–1365. (doi:10.1006/anbe.1995.0166)
4. Smuts BB, Smuts RW. 1993 Male aggression
and sexual coercion of females in nonhuman
primates and other mammals: evidence and
theoretical implications. Adv. Study Behav. 22,
1–63. (doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60404-0)
5. Byrne R, Whiten A. 1990 Tactical deception in
primates: the 1990 database. Prim. Rep. 27,
1–101.
6. Overduin-de Vries AM, Spruijt BM, de Vries H,
Sterck EHM. 2015 Tactical deception to hide
sexual behaviour: macaques use distance, not
visibility. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 69, 1333– 1342.
(doi:10.1007/s00265-015-1946-5)
7. Oda R, Masataka N. 1995 Function of copulatory
vocalizations in mate choice by female Japanese
macaques (Macaca fuscata). Folia Primatol. 64,
132– 139. (doi:10.1159/000156843)
8. Cowlishaw G, O’Connell SM. 1996 Male-male
competition, paternity certainty and copulation
calls in female baboons. Anim. Behav. 51,
235– 238. (doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0022)
9. Semple S. 1998 The function of Barbary
macaque copulation calls. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B
265, 287– 291. (doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0294)
10. Pradhan GR, Engelhardt A, Van Schaik CP,
Meastripieri D. 2006 The evolution of female
copulation calls in primates: a review and a new
model. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 59, 333– 343.
(doi:10.1007/s00265-005-0075-y)
11. O’Connell SM, Cowlishaw G. 1994 Infanticide
avoidance, sperm competition and mate choice:
the function of copulation calls in female
baboons. Anim. Behav. 48, 687– 694. (doi:10.
1006/anbe.1994.1288)
12. Le Roux A, Snyder-Mackler N, Roberts EK,
Beehner JC, Bergman TJ. 2013 Evidence for
tactical concealment in a wild primate. Nat.
Commun. 4, 1462. (doi:10.1038/ncomms2468)
13. Overduin-de Vries AM, Uhrenholt OC, de Vries
H, Spruijt BM, Sterck EHM. 2013 Sneak
copulations in long-tailed macaques (Macaca
fascicularis): no evidence for tactical deception.
Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 67, 101–111. (doi:10.
1007/s00265-012-1430-4)
14. Overduin-de Vries AM, Massen JJM, Spruijt BM,
Sterck EHM. 2012 Sneaky monkeys: an audience
effect of male rhesus macaques (Macaca
mulatta) on sexual behavior. Am. J. Primatol.
74, 217– 228. (doi:10.1002/ajp.21988)
15. Townsend SW, Deschner T, Zuberbuehler K.
2008 Female chimpanzees use copulation calls
flexibly to prevent social competition. PLoS ONE
3, e2431. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002431)
16. Townsend SW, Zuberbu¨hler K. 2009 Audience
effects in chimpanzee copulation calls. Commun.
Integr. Biol. 2, 282–284. (doi:10.4161/cib.2.3.
6796)
17. Baniel A, Cowlishaw G, Huchard E. 2017 Male
violence and sexual intimidation in a wild
primate society. Curr. Biol. 27, 2163– 2168.
(doi:10.1016/j.cub.2017.06.013)
18. Weingrill T, Lycett JE, Barrett L, Hill RA, Henzi
SP. 2003 Male consortship behaviour in chacma
baboons: the role of demographic factors and
female conceptive probabilities. Behaviour 140,
405– 427. (doi:10.1163/156853903321826701)
19. Baniel A, Cowlishaw G, Huchard E. 2018 Jealous
females? Female competition and reproductive
suppression in a wild promiscuous primate.
Proc. R. Soc. B 285, 20181332. (doi:10.1098/
rspb.2018.1332)
20. Baniel A, Cowlishaw G, Huchard E. 2018
Context-dependence of female reproductive
competition in wild chacma baboons. Anim.
Behav. 139, 37– 49. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.
2018.03.001)
21. Huchard E, Cowlishaw G. 2011 Female-female
aggression around mating: an extra cost of
sociality in a multimale primate society. Behav.
Ecol. 22, 1003–1011. (doi:10.1093/beheco/
arr083)
22. Huchard E, Alvergne A, Fejan D, Knapp LA,
Cowlishaw G, Raymond M. 2010 More than
friends? Behavioural and genetic aspects of
heterosexual associations in wild chacma
baboons. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 64, 769– 781.
(doi:10.1007/s00265-009-0894-3)
23. Moscovice LR, Di Fiore A, Crockford C, Kitchen
DM, Wittig R, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. 2010
Hedging their bets? Male and female chacma
baboons form friendships based on likelihood of
paternity. Anim. Behav. 79, 1007– 1015.
(doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.01.013)
24. Palombit RA, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL. 1997 The
adaptive value of ‘friendships’ to female
baboons: experimental and observational
evidence. Anim. Behav. 54, 599– 614. (doi:10.
1006/anbe.1996.0457)
25. Lemasson A, Palombit RA, Jubin R. 2008
Friendships between males and lactating
females in a free-ranging group of olive
baboons (Papio hamadryas anubis): evidence
from playback experiments. Behav. Ecol.
Sociobiol. 62, 1027–1035. (doi:10.1007/
s00265-007-0530-z)
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. open sci. 6: 181009
10
26. Buchan JC, Alberts SC, Silk JB, Altmann J. 2003
True paternal care in a multi-male primate
society. Nature 425, 179– 181. (doi:10.1038/
nature01866)
27. Cowlishaw G. 1997 Trade-offs between foraging
and predation risk determine habitat use in a
desert baboon population. Anim. Behav. 53,
667– 686. (doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0298)
28. Altmann J, Alberts SC. 2003 Variability in
reproductive success viewed from a life-history
perspective in baboons. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 15,
401– 409. (doi:10.1002/ajhb.10157)
29. Gesquiere LR, Wango EO, Alberts S, Altmann J.
2007 Mechanisms of sexual selection: sexual
swellings and estrogen concentrations as
fertility indicators and cues for male
consort decis ions in wild baboons. Horm.
Behav. 51, 114 125. (doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.
2006.08.010)
30. Alberts SC, Altmann J, Wilson ML. 1996 Mate
guarding constrains foraging activity of male
baboons. Anim. Behav. 51, 1269– 1277. (doi:10.
1006/anbe.1996.0131)
31. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. 2014
lme4: linear mixed-effects models using Eigen
and S4. R package version 1.1-7. R Package
version 1.1-7.
32. R Core Team. 2017 R: a language and
environment for statistical computing. Vienna,
Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
33. Whittingham MJ, Stephens PA, Bradbury RB,
Freckleton RP. 2006 Why do we still use
stepwise modelling in ecology and behaviour?
J. Anim. Ecol. 75, 1182–1189. (doi:10.1111/j.
1365-2656.2006.01141.x)
34. Mundry R, Nunn CL. 2009 Stepwise model
fitting and statistical inference: turning noise
into signal pollution. Am. Nat. 173, 119– 123.
(doi:10.1086/593303)
35. Zuur AF, Ieno EN, Elphick CS. 2010 A protocol
for data exploration to avoid common statistical
problems. Methods Ecol. Evol. 1, 3– 14. (doi:10.
1111/j.2041-210X.2009.00001.x)
36. Dormann CF et al. 2013 Collinearity: a review
of methods to deal with it and a simulation
study evaluating their performance. Ecography
36, 27–46. (doi:10.1111/j.1600-0587.2012.
07348.x)
37. Soltis J, Thomsen R, Takenaka O. 2001 The
interaction of male and female reproductive
strategies and paternity in wild Japanese
macaques, Macaca fuscata.Anim. Behav. 62,
485– 494. (doi:10.1006/anbe.2001.1774)
38. De Bruyn PJN, Tosh CA, Bester MN, Cameron EZ,
McIntyre T, Wilkinson SI. 2011 Sex at sea:
alternative mating system in an extremely
polygynous mammal. Anim. Behav. 82,
445– 451. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.06.006)
39. Beehner JC, Lu A. 2013 Reproductive suppression
in female primates: a review. Evol. Anthropol. 22,
226– 238. (doi:10.1002/evan.21369)
40. Crockford C, Wittig RM, Seyfarth RM, Cheney DL.
2007 Baboons eavesdrop to deduce mating
opportunities. Anim. Behav. 73, 885– 890.
(doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.10.016)
41. Maestripieri D, Roney JR. 2005 Primate
copulation calls and postcopulatory female
choice. Behav. Ecol. 16, 106– 113. (doi:10.1093/
beheco/arh120)
royalsocietypublishing.org/journal/rsos R. Soc. open sci. 6: 181009
11
... Whereas chacma baboon and macaque males, to some extent, are also less likely to initiate mating when additional (higher-ranking) males are in the vicinity(Overduin-de Vries et al., 2012;Overduin-de Vries et al., 2013), orang-utan males initiate copulations despite the presence of dominant males. In more social (group-living) species sanctions for sneaky mating may be high for both males and females: punishment of the female (sensuClutton-Brock & Parker, 1995;Baniel et al., 2019) and direct male aggression towards sneakily mating males(Overduin-de Vries et al., 2012) ...
Article
Full-text available
Sexual coercion is widespread in the animal kingdom. Its direct forms, including harassment and forced copulation, have largely been investigated as an expression of (alternative) male reproductive strategies, rather than the result of a sexual conflict between the sexes. Likewise, the frequent occurrence of forced copulations in orang-utans (Pongo spp.) has been attributed to male strategies and more recently also to concealed female fecundity. So far, however, the immediate contextual variables leading to forced copulations have rarely been examined. We compared two orang-utan populations, Suaq (Pongo abelii, Sumatra) and Tuanan (Pongo pygmaeus, Borneo), both characterized by an individual-based fission–fusion lifestyle, whereas their socioecology differs. We assessed how the occurrence of female-resisted and voluntary copulations was affected by female reproductive state, male morph (unflanged or flanged), measures of male–male competition, male–female relationship and ecological factors. Besides female reproductive state and male morph, predictors of female resistance were related to male–male competition. First, female resistance was more likely towards subordinate males who were displaced from proximity to the female by another male during that association. Second, the presence of additional flanged males increased the probability of female resistance. Third, the latency to both the arrival of another male and to the end of the association after sexual interactions was shorter if there was female resistance. We conclude that sexual coercion in orang-utans is highly dependent on the vicinity of more dominant males and can only be understood in the light of sexual conflict: While males force copulations when at risk of losing access to a female and thus follow a ‘now-or-never’ strategy, female resistance follows a ‘not-you-now’ pattern, which is ultimately consistent with an infanticide avoidance strategy.
... Audience effects are usually tested by comparing control scan conditions when the targeted behavior is not expressed with conditions when the targeted behaviors are expressed (e.g., in Baniel et al. 2019). In this study, I estimated that the effect of motivational state might be too important, and this traditional approach may have been irrelevant. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Animals exhibit an astonishing diversity of communicative systems, with substantial variation in both the nature and the number of signals they produce. Variation in communicative complexity has been conceptually and empirically attributed to social complexity and formalized as the “social-complexity hypothesis for communicative complexity” (SCHCC). Indeed, group-living animals face complex social environments where they engage in a wide range of interactions with different social partners triggering the need for transmission of a broader diversity of messages. In chapter I (Peckre et al. 2019), I review the literature on the current tests of the SCHCC, pointing out and discussing what I identified as the main gaps in the current state of the art. Specifically, three key issues emerged from my analysis. The first issue concerns the operational definition of the main variables, social and communicative complexity. Notably, when defining communicative complexity, most empirical tests of the SCHCC focus on a single modality (e.g., acoustic, visual, olfactory) whereas several good reasons exist for acknowledging the multimodal nature of both, signals and communicative systems in this framework. At the system level, focusing on only one modality may lead to over- or underestimation of the relationship between social and communicative complexity. The second issue relates to the fact that while numerous studies have highlighted a link between social and communicative complexity, their correlative nature does not permit conclusions about the direction of causality. Indeed, alternative hypotheses involving anatomical, phylogenetical, or ecological factors have also been proposed to explain the evolution of more complex forms of communication. Finally, I note that researchers rarely address the actual ways in which social factors directly affect variation in signaling. Indeed, the underlying mechanisms of this link are usually left unexplored, failing to uncover the specific attribute of communication that would be co-evolving with specific aspects of sociality. I, therefore, make a plea for expanding tests of the SCHCC in 1) scope (systematic approach across modalities) and 2) depth (characterization of the observed relationships) as I believe it may significantly advance our understanding of the intricate links between animal sociality and communication. To address point 1), I offer in chapter II a comprehensive approach of the cross-modal communicative systems of two closely related true lemur species having similar morphology, living in similar habitats, but differing in their social systems. I studied wild Eulemur rufifrons and E. mongoz in Madagascar, respectively in Kirindy and Ankatsabe forests for 12 months. I describe a new analytical framework to assess the complexity of signaling systems across modalities. Applying a multimodal approach may help to uncover the different selective pressures acting on the communicative system and to understand better adaptive functions that might be unclear from the study of its separate components independently. E. rufifrons, the species having the more complex social system, also had overall a more complex communicative system than the one of the E. mongoz. Both careful choices of the species to compare to limit the effect of possible additional selective pressures and exploration of the social function of the non-homologous signals allow concluding that this increased complexity of the communicative system in E. rufifrons is most likely associated with social selective pressures. I developed this new analytical framework, partly based on using a cross-modal network approach, with the perspective of facilitating cross-taxonomic comparisons. Moreover, this approach may be combined with new multi-dimensional approaches of social complexity and contribute to a more holistic approach to the tests of the SCHCC. By this, we should be able to derive new testable hypotheses that would contribute to better understand the course of events that have led to the evolution of communication diversity in its distinct dimensions. In chapter III, I address point 2) by investigating the impacts of sociality on the expression of a multimodal signal, the anogenital scent-marking behavior in wild red-fronted lemurs. I specifically investigated intragroup audience effects on anogenital scent-marking behaviors in a wild population of red-fronted lemurs and particularly whether males and females differed in this aspect and if these differences may reveal functional differences associated with anogenital scent-marking across sexes. I found an intragroup audience effect in males but not in females. Males deposited less often anogenital marks when more males were present within a three meters range compared to five- or ten-meters ranges. Males may prefer to reduce the risk of physical contact by avoiding to scent-mark near other males, and/or give priority to other males to scent-mark. With these results, I provide important insights into the functional significance of anogenital scent-marking in red-fronted lemurs and support the idea of greater intragroup social pressures associated with anogenital scent-marking in males than in females in egalitarian species. Studying the flexibility of complex signal usage (e.g., occurrence or structural modifications) across social contexts (audiences) should permit the identification of different individual social characteristics that may elicit or constrain complex signal expression. These social characteristics may later constitute social pressures acting for or against the evolution of these complex signaling behaviors. In chapters IV and V, I also address ethical questions related to this project and the way I tried to adapt and best address my responsibilities for animal welfare. In chapter IV, I expose some technical details and ethical concerns experienced during the choice of my field sites. While in chapter V (Buil and Peckre et al. 2019) I present a remote releasable collar system developed in collaboration with the Neurobiology Laboratory (German Primate Center, Göttingen, Germany) intending to provide a tool to significantly reduce the number of captures in studies using bio-logging for medium-sized mammal species. Overall, by emphasizing the importance of the multimodal nature of communicative systems and the social context in which signals are exchanged, I hope to stimulate the development of new tests of the SCHCC based on this expanded framework. I additionally argue for the importance of looking across research fields since striking parallels may be observed between animal behavior and linguistic research when addressing the origins of communication complexity, be it in the form of human language or animal signaling.
Article
For mammalian females, which bear the energetic costs of gestation and lactation, competition over food resources is generally expected to have a more important impact on reproductive success than competition over mates. However, there are some situations in which mammalian females compete over access to mates, limited supplies of sperm or caretaking for their offspring. Competition over both access to food and access to mates have been documented in female baboons (Papio spp.). Here, we examined the relative importance of competition over food and mates for wild female olive baboons, Papio anubis, in shaping the patterns of aggression among females. Lactating and pregnant females were more aggressive than females in other reproductive states, and sexually receptive females initiated and received relatively little aggression. There was no evidence that females competed over caretakers for their offspring or reduced future competition through reproductive suppression. Our data suggest that competition over food plays a more important role than competition over mates for female olive baboons. The body of current evidence suggests that evolution has finely tuned female baboons’ responses to the competitive pressures that they face in their local environments.
Article
Full-text available
Recent research reveals that female reproductive competition is common and may shape the social and reproductive strategies of female mammals. This study explores the determinants and intensity of female intrasexual conflicts in a wild promiscuous primate, the chacma baboon, Papio ursinus. We tested a suite of hypotheses to assess whether femaleefemale aggression was primarily driven by instantaneous competition for food, mates or paternal care, or aimed at reducing future competition among offspring via reproductive suppression. Behavioural data were gathered from 53 females in two groups over two contrasting 2-year periods (2005e2006, 2013e2014): the first characterized by stability in the male dominance hierarchies, the second by instability induced by several immigration events and male takeovers. In both periods, we found that sexually receptive females received high levels of aggression from other sexually receptive females, consistent with competition over mating opportunities. In the unstable period, females exchanged higher rates of aggression than in the stable period, regardless of reproductive state, but lactating females received most aggression, consistent with higher competition over social access to male partners in response to increased infanticide risk. There was no evidence that aggression between females reflected either competition over food or reproductive suppression. These findings indicate that patterns of aggression between females fluctuate with sociodemographic factors affecting sexual and social access to males and reflect reproductive competition more closely than resource competition in this promiscuous primate society.
Technical Report
Full-text available
Description Fit linear and generalized linear mixed-effects models. The models and their components are represented using S4 classes and methods. The core computational algorithms are implemented using the 'Eigen' C++ library for numerical linear algebra and 'RcppEigen' ``glue''.
Article
Full-text available
Although tactical deception (TD) may be employed to hide sexual behaviour, there is as yet no firm evidence for it. Hiding may be guided by cognitive mechanisms consistent with either no, low or high level TD, such as exploiting male peripheral positions (no TD), creating distance (TD level 1) or hiding behind screens (TD level 1.5 which involves visual perspective taking (VPT)). Macaques are capable of VPT in a food context, suggesting that they may employ TD level 1.5. We investigated, in an observational study with temporarily provided hiding screens, which strategy was used to hide sexual behaviour in captive groups of two macaque species (Macaca mulatta and Macaca fascicularis). Sexual behaviour only sporadically took place near screens, and the few copulations near screens were not systematically hidden from the alpha male, precluding TD level 1.5. Instead, both females and non-alpha males were at a larger distance from the alpha male during sexual interactions than otherwise, consistent with TD level 1. Creating peripheral locations (TD level 1) may be effective in improving sexual opportunities in many species. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s00265-015-1946-5) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Article
Female-female competition over paternal care has rarely been investigated in promiscuous mammals, where discreet forms of male care have recently been reported despite low paternity certainty. We investigated female competition over paternal care in a wild promiscuous primate, the chacma baboon (Papio ursinus), where pregnant and lactating females establish strong social bonds (friendships) with males that provide care to their offspring. We tested whether pregnant and lactating females interfere with the sexual activity of their male friend to prevent new conceptions that might lead to the subsequent dilution of his paternal care. We found that pregnant and lactating females were more aggressive towards oestrous females when they had recently conceived themselves, and when the oestrous female was mate-guarded by, and showed greater sexual activity with, their male friend. This aggression also reduced the likelihood of conception of the targeted female. These findings indicate that females can aggressively prevent further conceptions with their offspring's carer through reproductive suppression. Competition over access to paternal care may play an important and underestimated role in shaping female social relationships and reproductive strategies in promiscuous mammalian societies.
Article
Sexual violence occurring in the context of long-term heterosexual relationships, such as sexual intimidation, is widespread across human populations [1-3]. However, its evolutionary origins remain speculative because few studies have investigated the existence of comparable forms of sexual coercion in animals [4, 5], in which repeated male aggression toward a female provides the aggressor with delayed mating benefits [6]. Here, we test whether male aggression toward females functions as sexual coercion in wild chacma baboons (Papio ursinus). We found support for all three main predictions of the sexual coercion hypothesis [7]: male aggression (1) is greatest against cycling females, (2) is costly and represents the main source of injuries for cycling females, and (3) increases male mating success with their victims in the future. Detailed analysis of chronological sequences between aggression and matings ruled out other coercive mechanisms, such as short-term harassment and punishment, by showing that aggression and matings are temporally decoupled. This decoupling may explain why some forms of sexual violence have been largely overlooked in well-studied animal populations despite their likely impact on the fitness of both sexes. Finally, we found no support for alternative hypotheses such as a female preference for aggressive males [8, 9]. This new, detailed study of the forms and intensity of sexual intimidation in a wild primate suggests that it may be widespread across mammalian societies, with important implications for understanding the evolution of mate choice and sexual conflict in mammals, as well as the origins of human sexual violence.