ArticlePDF Available

Cognitive processes of emotion regulation, burnout and engagement at work

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background: Workers constantly resort to cognitive processes of emotion regulation to deal with emotions they experience in the workplace. These processes belong either to the “automatic” (preconscious and fast) or the “elaborative” (conscious and slow) mode. This study aims to determine the role of these variables in the work setting and to analyze their relationship with positive and negative affect, engagement and burnout. Method: 350 employees (54.8% men and 45.2% women) were presented with several instruments measuring burnout, engagement, affect and cognitive emotion regulation strategies in a prospective study. An explanatory model was tested through structural equation modeling analysis. Results: Acceptable fi t indices and a signifi cant explanatory value both for burnout (61%) and engagement (58%) were obtained. The use of “automatic” cognitive regulation strategies was associated with the presence of negative affect and burnout whereas “elaborative” processes were associated with positive affect and engagement. Conclusions: Our fi ndings underscore the importance of the role of emotion regulation in organizational settings.
Content may be subject to copyright.
73
Research on occupational well-being has focused particular
attention in recent years on two major topics, burnout and work
engagement. Burnout is a persistent mental state related to work,
predominated by negative affect such as stress, anxiety and worry,
and has a huge impact on people’s health (Grandey, 2015). This
syndrome is characterized by several dimensions: 1) exhaustion,
2) mental distance and 3) professional inef cacy. Since mental
distance includes cynicism and depersonalization as two separate
dimensions, a four-dimension model has more recently been
proposed for burnout (Salanova et al., 2005). Engagement has
been proposed as the opposite of the burnout state (Salanova,
Schaufeli, Llorens, Peiró, & Grau, 2000) and is de ned as a
positive motivational construct related to vigor (high energy
levels while working), dedication (manifested by signi cance,
pride, and goals related to the work done), and absorption (full
concentration levels at work). As indicated by Bakker & Albrecth
(2018), engaged employees tend to experience positive affect, such
as joy, enjoyment and enthusiasm, which expands the cognitive-
behavioral repertoire involved in their work. Moreover, engaged
employees can focus and devote all their resources and skills to
their work. They also tend to show greater collaboration with their
immediate environment, which allows them to effectively meet
the demands of their work and bene t the whole organization
(S a l an o va , Ll o re n s, & Sc h au f el i , 2 0 11) . I t is t he r e fo r e h i g hl y re l ev a nt
to identify those factors that promote “healthy” employees who are
“engaged” and feel ful llment in their work, since these aspects
have a positive in uence both on the workers themselves (Lent &
ISSN 0214 - 9915 CODEN PSOTEG
Copyright © 2019 Psicothema
www.psicothema.com
Cognitive processes of emotional regulation, burnout and work
engagement
Estanislao Castellano
1
, Roger Muñoz-Navarro
2
, María Sol Toledo
1
, Carlos Spontón
1
, and Leonardo Adrián Medrano
1,3
1 Universidad Siglo 21, Córdoba (Argentina), 2 Universitat de València (España), and 3 Universidad Nacional de Córdoba, Argentina
Abstract Resumen
Background: Workers constantly resort to cognit ive processes of emotion
regulation to deal with emotions they experience in the workplace.
These processes belong either to the “automatic” (preconscious and
fast) or the “elaborative” (conscious and slow) mode. This study aims to
determine the role of these variables in the work setting and to analyze
their relationship with positive and negative affect, engagement and
burnout. Method: 350 employees (54.8% men and 45.2% women) were
presented with several instruments measuring burnout, engagement,
affect and cognitive emotion regulation strategies in a prospective study.
An explanatory model was tested through structural equation modeling
analysis. Results: Acceptable t indices and a signi cant explanatory
value both for burnout (61%) and engagement (58%) were obtained. The
use of “automatic” cognitive regulation strategies was associated with the
presence of negative affect and burnout whereas “elaborative” processes
were associated with positive affect and engagement. Conclusions: Our
ndings underscore the importance of the role of cognitive emotion
regulation in organizational settings.
Keywords: Cognitive processes, emotion regulation, affect, engagement,
burnout.
Procesos cognitivos de regulación emocional, burnout y engagement
en el trabajo. Antecedentes: los trabajadores apelan constantemente a
procesos cognitivos de regulación emo cional para lidiar con las emociones
que experimentan en el trabajo. Estos procesos se pueden distinguir en
dos modos de procesamiento, uno es preconsciente y rápido, llamado
“automático”; y otro es consciente y más lento, llamado “elaborativo”. El
objetivo de este trabajo fue determinar el papel de estas variables en el
entorno de trabajo y analizar su relación con el afecto positivo y negativo,
el burnout y el engagement. Método: 350 trabajadores (54,8% hombres
y 45,2% mujeres) completaron varios instrumentos que miden burnout,
engagement, afecto y estrategias cognitivas de regulación emocional
en un estudio prospectivo. Se test un modelo explicativo a través del
análisis de ecuaciones estructurales. Resultados: se obtuvieron valores
aceptables en los índices de ajuste y un valor explicativo signi cativo
tanto para el burnout (61%) como para el engagement (58%). El uso
de estrategias cognitivas de regulación emocional “automáticas” se
relacionó con la presencia de afecto negativo y burnout, mientras que el
uso de procesoselaborativos” se relacio con la experiencia de afecto
y compromiso positivo. Conclusiones: estos resultados corroboran la
importancia del papel de la regulación cognitiva de las emociones en el
entorno organizacional.
Palabras clave: procesos cognitivos, regulación emocional, afecto,
engagement, burnout.
Psicothema 2019, Vol. 31, No. 1, 73-80
doi: 10.7334/psicothema2018.228
Received: August 24, 2018 • Accepted: December 10, 2018
Corresponding author: Roger Muñoz-Navarro
Facultad de Psicología
Universitat de València
46010 Valencia (Spain)
e-mail: roger.munoz@uv.es
Estanislao Castellano, Roger Muñoz-Navarro, María Sol Toledo, Carlos Spontón, and Leonardo Adrián Medrano
74
Brown, 2008) and on the well-being of the company as a whole
(Schaufeli, Bakker, & Van Rhenen, 2009). Furthermore, knowing
how to prevent the burnout syndrome, which has a negative impact
on productivity and people’s health, could help tackle one of the
biggest problems of the twenty- rst century (Prasad, 2016).
Recent studies on Positive Organizational Psychology have
highlighted the relevance of emotional factors in the work
setting owing to their explanatory value in assessing burnout and
engagement as mediators of occupational well-being (Castellano,
Cifre, Spontón, Medrano, & Maffei, 2013; Lisbona, Palaci,
Salanova, & Frese, 2018; Rodríguez-Mantilla & Fernández-
Díaz, 2017). Workers continually appeal to different processes of
emotion regulation (ER) in order to take charge of the emotions
they experience in their work setting (Grandey, 2015). These
same ER processes can be of relevance in preventing burnout
(Arnold, Connelly, Walsh, & Martin Ginis, 2015), promoting work
e n g a g e m e n t o n t h e p a r t o f e m p l o y e e s ( S a l a n o v a e t a l . , 2 0 11 ), c r e a t i n g
more adaptive organizational behavior (Rodríguez García, López-
Pérez, Férreo Cruzado, Fernández Carrascoso, & Fernández,
2017) and are closely related to job satisfaction (Côté & Morgan,
2002). Indeed, based on the model of emotional dissonance, the
latter authors showed that the suppression of pleasant emotions
decreased work satisfaction, while their ampli cation increased it.
These ndings are consistent with those reported by Grandey &
Melloy (2017) who, after a systematic review, concluded that ER
in the work setting has a direct impact on variables such as work
satisfaction, burnout, work performance and task abandonment.
The concept of ER has been de ned as the processes that
in uence the way in which people experience and express their
emotions (Gross, 1998). People are able to reroute the spontaneous
ow of their emotions, increasing, maintaining or decreasing them
(Gross, 2015). A recent meta-analysis of the most accepted ER
strategies in t he literature list s ten strategies: accepta nce, behavioral
avoidance, distraction, experiential avoidance, expressive
suppression, mindfulness, problem-solving, reappraisal, re ection
and concern (Naragon-Gainey, McMahon, & Chacko, 2017). As
seen, there are many strategies for regulating emotions and the
cognitive processes involved during an emotional episode play a
key role (Garnefski, Kraaij, & Spinhoven, 2001). Along these lines,
the latter authors propose a model of cognitive emotion regulation
by selecting nine ER cognitive strategies: self-blaming, blaming
others, rumination, catastrophizing, putting into perspective,
positive focus, positive reappraisa l, acceptanc e, and planni ng. Such
strategies can be grouped into one of two systems: “automatic”
or “elaborative” processing (Clore & Ortony, 2000; Medrano,
Muñoz-Navarro, & Cano-Vindel, 2017). Automatic processing is
characterized by being fast, preconscious and dif cult to control
whereas elaborative processing is voluntary, conscious, and slow.
Automatic ER processes contain functions such as rumination
or catastrophizing that enable situations of immediate threat to
be dealt with. However, they can in turn increase anxiety or alert
responses (Beck & Clark, 1997). Elaborative processes, such as
cognitive reappraisal, focusing on plans or acceptance, facilitate a
more rational and pro table interpretation of problems, decreasing
anxiety responses. These processes, as mentioned by Medrano et
al. (2016), could be explained from the perspective of evolutionary
psychology. Humans have developed different systems to detect
threats and react adaptively throughout evolutionary history, thus
increasing their chances of safety and survival. These processes
developed at earlier stages of evolution are automatic, simple, fast,
and without voluntary control. However, as our cognition system
evolved, we acquired more complex, rational and controlled
capabilities for conscious elaborative processes that can in uence
our experienced emotions in a bene cial manner.
Accordingly, this study aims to determine the role of ER in the
work setting and more speci cally, to analyze its relationship with
workers’ positive and negative affect, engagement and burnout.
The following hypotheses are proposed: There is a direct, positive
relationship between ER elaborative processes and positive affect
(hypothesis 1) and a direct relationship between ER elaborative
processes and engagement (hypothesis 2). There is a direct, negative
relationship between ER elaborative processes and negative affect
(hypothesis 3). There is a direct, positive relationship between
ER automatic processes and negative affect (hypothesis 4) and a
direct relationship between ER automatic processes and burnout
(hypothesis 5). There is a direct, negative relationship between
ER automatic processes and positive affect (hypothesis 6). There
is a direct, negative relationship between negative affect and
engagement (hypothesis 7) and a direct, positive relationship
between negative affect and burnout (hypothesis 8). Finally, a
direct, negative relationship is hypothesized between positive
affect and bur nout (hypothesis 9) and a direct, positive relationship
between positive affect and engagement (hypothesis 10).
Method
Participants
This study involved the participation of 350 workers belonging
to different companies in the city of Córdoba, Argentina. The
sample comprises workers of both genders (45% are women)
aged between 20 and 60 years (M = 36.44; SD = 8.28), selected
through non-probability, accidental sampling. To ensure a greater
heterogeneity in the sample, workers from different sectors and
areas were included (Table 1).
Instruments
Burnout: To assess exhaustion, cynicism, and inef cacy, the
Spanish version of the Maslach Burnout Inventory-General
Survey (MBI-GS) (Salanova et al., 2000) was used. To assess
depersonalization, we used the Spanish version of the Maslach
Burnout Inventory Human Services-Survey (MBI-HSS; Gil-
Monte, 2005). A total of 17 items corresponding to the four
dimensions of burnout were administered: exhaustion (4 items),
cynicism (4 items), depersonalization (5 items) and inef cacy
(4 items). To respond to the above-mentioned items a response
scale was used ranging from 0 (‘never’) to 6 (‘always/every
day’). We used the version adapted to the worker population of
Argentina (Spontón, Trógolo, Castellano, & Medrano, in press),
which has satisfactory psychometric properties. Analyses using
the Cronbach alpha coef cient showed that the scales have an
acceptable internal consistency (exhaustion = .77, cynicism =
.84 depersonalization = .71 and inef cacy = .80). The MBI-GS
scores correlated signi cantly and in the expected direction with
the levels of engagement, negative affect and positive affect, thus
providing external evidence of validity. The Cronbach alpha
coef cients found in the sample of this study for each dimension
were: .72 (exhaustion), .80 (cynicism), .87 (depersonalization) and
.64 (inef cacy).
Cognitive processes of emotional regulation, burnout and work engagement
75
Engagement: The Spanish version of the Utrecht Work
Engagement Scale (UWES; Salanova et al., 2000) was used,
enabling three dimensions of engagement to be evaluated: vigor (6
items), dedication (6 i t e ms ) an d absorption (5 items). All exam inees
used a seven-point scale (from 0 ‘never’ to 6 ‘always/every day’),
to respond to each item. Studies conducted in Argentina (Spontón,
Medrano, Maffei, Spontón, & Castellano, 2012) indicate that the
scale retains the same factorial structure as the original scale; the
reliability values calculated using the acceptable Cronbach alpha
coef cient were .69, .76 and .88 for the dimensions of absorption,
vigor and dedication, respect ively. In addition, Spontón et al. (2018)
p rov id e d ex te r na l e vi de nc e o f v al id it y b y c or r el at in g U W ES sc or es
with professional self-ef cacy. The values of internal reliability
for each dimension in this work were: .85 (dedication), .82 (vigor),
and .76 (absorption).
Affect: The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS;
Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) was used, which consists of
20 words describing different feelings and positive emotions
(for example, active, strong, inspired) and negative emotions (for
example, irritated, fearful, nervous). The evaluated subject used a
ve-point scale (from 1 = ‘very little or nothing’ to 5 = ‘always or
almost always) to show the extent to which he/she experienced
each of the mentioned emotions. The validated version for the
population of Córdoba, Argentina, was used (Moriondo, Palma,
Medrano, & Murillo, 2010; Medrano et al., 2015). The scale has
an acceptable level of internal consistency (α = .73 positive affect;
α = .82 negative affect). In order to achieve a greater delimitation
of the examined construct, the general scale prompt was slightly
modi ed. Thus, workers were asked to indicate how often they
experienced positive and negative affect in their workplace. In this
study, PANAS’s internal consistency was good (α = .88 positive
affect; α = .82 negative affect).
Cognitive Emotion Regulation: The Cognitive Emotion
Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ: Garnefski & Kraaij, 2007) is
an instrument consisting of 36 items that examines the ability to
regulate personal emotions through the use of nine cognitive-type
strategies. A ve-point scale as adapted by Medrano, Moretti,
Ortiz, and Pereno (2013) was used, ranging from ‘never or almost
never’ (1) to ‘always or almost always’ (5). The adapted version
has nine underlying factors: 1) Self-blame (α = .69), 2) Other-
blame (α = .82), 3) Rumination (α = .70), 4) Catastrophizing (α
= .68), 5) Positive refocusing (α = .83), 6) Planning (α = .66), 7)
Positive reappraisal (α = .77), 8) Putting into perspective (α = .70),
and 9) Acceptance (α = .59). The scales can be grouped into two
factors, termed automatic processes (rumination, catastrophizing,
self-incriminating and blaming others) and elaborative processes
(positive reinterpretation, focus on plans, acceptance, positive
focus and putting into perspective). CERQ scores have been shown
to be valid for predicting emotional interference, positive affect
and negative affect (Medrano et al., 2013). The values of internal
reliability for each dimension in the present study were low: 1)
Self-blame (α = .54), 2) Other-blame (α = .62), 3) Rumination (α
= .65), 4) Catastrophizing (α = .60), 5) Positive refocusing (α =
.81), 6) Planning (α = .61), 7) Positive reappraisal (α = .67), 8)
Putting into perspective (α = .58), and 9) Acceptance (α = .41).
Since some of the sub-dimensions of the variables had low levels
of internal consistency (alpha values below .70), it was decided to
“collapse” the items of each dimension into a single score in order
to achieve more reliable measures: automatic processes (α = .75)
and elaborative processes (α = .80)”.
Procedure
A prospective, ex-post facto study was conducted, with no
manipulation of independent variables. A standardized procedure
was used to ensure all participants received the same instructions.
The instr uments were administere d collectively and dur ing regular
working hours with prior authorization from the administration
of each company. The questionnaires were administered in paper
format, in a quiet physical space away from the place where the
workers habitually carry out their tasks. The approximate time
taken to complete the quest ionnaires was 20 m inutes per part icipant.
The investigators were present during the administration of the
tests in order to clear up any doubts that arose and to verify the
independent administration by the participants.
This research was evaluated and approved by the Institutional
Ethics Committee of Siglo 21 University, Córdoba, Argentina.
All participants gave informed consent, and the workers’
data con dentiality and the anonymity of their responses was
guaranteed. Once the data were analyzed, brief and anonymous
reports were offered to the participating companies.
Data analysis
To perform the statistical a nalysis the collected data were loaded
into the IBM SPSS 17 version. An initial exploratory data analysis
was conducted to evaluate the statistical assumptions required
for the use of Structural Equation Modeling (SEM). In this way,
univariate and multivariate normality were examined, descriptive
Table 1
Work and demographic characteristics of sample
Total sample
(n = 350)
N%
Gender
Female
Male
161
189
46
54
Type of work
Company
Independent
Government
Other
175
77
35
63
50
22
10
18
Sector
Public
Private
84
266
24
76
Size of the organization
Small (0-50 employees)
Medium (50 to 250 employees)
Large (more than 205 employees)
161
154
35
46
44
10
Type of Company
Trade
Services
Industry
Other
77
231
21
21
22
66
6
6
Income
Less than $15,000
Between $15,000 and $35,000
More than $35000
126
168
56
36
48
16
Estanislao Castellano, Roger Muñoz-Navarro, María Sol Toledo, Carlos Spontón, and Leonardo Adrián Medrano
76
statistics were calculated, and the existence of multicollinearity
was analyzed considering the bivariate relationships between
variables.
As the current study relies exclusively on self-report data, we
tested for possible bias due to common method variance using
Harmans single factor test (Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Lee, &
Podsakoff, 2003). Different models were speci ed to examine the
hypotheses. First, an orthogonal contribution model of cognitive
processes, in which automatic processes only in uence negative
affect and elaborative processes only in uence positive affect
(model 1), was compared with a cross-contribution model (model
2), in which automatic and elaborative processes in uence both
negative and positive affect. We proceeded in a similar manner
to contrast the hypotheses referring to the in uence of affect
on burnout and engagement. An orthogonal contribution model
of affect, where positive affect only in uences engagement, and
negative affect only in uences burnout (model 3) was compared
with a cross-contribution model, where positive and negative
affect in uence both burnout and engagement (model 4). All
models were speci ed in the AMOS program, 17 version.
To e valuat e t h e t of the models, several t indicators were
used: the absolute t index (χ2), the goodness of t index (GFI),
the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), the comparative t index (CFI),
and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). GFI,
TLI, and CFI values greater than .90 and RMSEA values smaller
than .08 indicate acceptable model t, while values greater than
.95 (for GFI, TLI and CFI) and smaller than .05 (for RMSEA)
are indicative of excellent t (Hu & Bentler, 1999). The division
of the coef cient χ² by the degrees of freedom (χ²/df) was also
considered; according to the literature, values lower than 3
indicate a good adjustment (Medrano & Moz-Navarro, 2017).
Finally, we calculated the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC;
Akaike, 1987) in order to compare competitive models, since it
is convenient to compare the suitability of non-tested models that
t into the same correlation matrix. The lower the AIC index, the
better the t (Byrne, 2001).
Results
An initial exploration and descriptive analysis was carried out
to assess the pattern of missing cases, to identify univariate and
multivariate atypical cases, to check the assumptions of normality
and to determ ine the behavior of the variables. The “missing values
analysis” of the SPSS revealed a random pattern of missing cases,
so a method of case allocation was used to replace the missing
values (mean in the series). Only 12 univariate atypical cases and
3 multivariate atypical cases were observed; these were retained in
the base considering their low ratio. Table 2 shows the descriptive
statistics of mean, standard deviation, skewness, and kurtosis
of the variables in the study. As can be seen, all variables have
levels of skewness and kurtosis lower than ±2, so the univariate
normality assumption is corroborated (George & Mallery, 2010).
Using the Pearson correlation coef cient, the bivariate
relationships (Table 2) were examined. These are theoretically
coherent and no r values higher than .90 were observed, thus
ruling out the existence of multicollinearity among variables
(Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001).
Since some of the sub-dimensions of the variables had low
levels of internal consistency (alpha values below .70), it was
decided to “collapse” the items of each dimension into a single
score, thus providing more reliable measures.
The multivariate normality assumption was examined using
the Mardia index (Mardia= 6.36; Z =1.54; p = 0.06). Considering
Table 2
Descriptive statistic of Mean (M), Standard deviation (DT), Asymmetry (A) y
Kurtosis (K)
Variables M DT A K
Exhaustion 2.43 1.34 .12 -.72
Depersonalization .92 .85 1.03 .99
Cynicism 1.18 1.13 1.06 .72
Inef cacy .80 .83 1.01 .36
Vigor 4.80 .91 -1.15 1.85
Dedication 4.61 1.14 -1.28 1.57
Absorption 4.24 1.02 -.45 -.37
Positive affect 3.86 .73 -.76 .03
Negative affect 1.54 .54 1.15 .83
Elaborative processes of emotion regulation* 55.40 8.28 -.21 -.21
Automatic processes of emotion regulation** 25.93 6.85 -.03 -.48
Note: * Values between 30 and 73; ** Values between 12 and 42
Table 3
Bivariate cor relations between affect regulation, emotions, burnout and engagement
Variables 12345678910
1 Elaborative processes
2 Automatic processes ,11
3 Positive affect ,46** -,10
4 Negative affect -,07 ,44** -,22*
5 Vigor ,43* -,13* ,75* -,21*
6 Dedication ,36* -,09 ,74* -,19* ,74**
7 Absorption ,21* ,01 ,45* -,18* ,58* ,59*
8 Exhaustion -,08 ,42* -,31* ,52* -,30* -,24* -,03
9 Depersonalization -,01 ,31* -,15* ,41* -,19* -,19* -,14* ,48*
10 Cynicism -,19* ,18* -,57* ,32* -,57* -,68* -,46* ,34* ,26*
11 Inef cacy -,17* ,23* -,28* ,19* -,29* -,13 -,07 ,20* ,17* ,26*
Note: * p<0,01
Cognitive processes of emotional regulation, burnout and work engagement
77
that the Mardia values are below the critical value of .70 suggested
by Rodríguez Ayán and Ruiz (2008), the “Maximum Likelihood”
(ML) was used as the calculation method. This method is
recommended in the literature as being the most ef cient in cases
of a wide sample of normally distributed data (Medrano & Muñoz-
Navarro, 2017). Firstly, we conducted Harman’s single factor test.
The results revealed a poor t to the data (χ2(9) = 95.62, GFI = 0.81,
CFI = 0.72, TLI = 0.53, RMSEA = 0.25). Consequently, common
method variance is not a serious de ciency in our dataset.
A series of four models was speci ed, tested and compared (see
Figure 1). Table 4 presents the t indices for these models. Model
4 is the only one of the four models to meet Hu and Bentler’s
(1999) criteria for a good t; models 1, 2, and 3 showed a much
worse t. From the analysis of each model it is clear that some of
the postulated hypotheses are not veri ed. The direct relationship
between elaborative processes, engagement and negative affect is
not statistically signi cant. It can also be observed that negative
affect does not contribute signi cantly to engagement. In other
words, engagement is explained fundamentally by positive affect
and not by levels of negative affect.
Elaborative processes of
emotional regulation
Engagement
Positive
affect
Negative
affect
Burnout
Automatic processes of
emotional regulation
.05
.72
.46
.11
.24
.44
.44
R2 = .55
R2 = .34
R2 = .22
R2 = .20
Model 1
Elaborative processes of
emotional regulation
Engagement
Positive
affect
Negative
affect
Burnout
Automatic processes of
emotional regulation
.05
.72
.48
.11
.24
.44
.46
R2 = .55
R2 = .34
R2 = .24
R2 = .21
Model 2
Elaborative processes of
emotional regulation
Engagement
Positive
affect
Negative
affect
Burnout
Automatic processes of
emotional regulation
.74
.48
.11
.24
.44
-.16
R2 = .55
R2 = .34
R2 = .24
R2 = .20
Model 3
.44
Elaborative processes of
emotional regulation
Engagement
Positive
affect
Negative
affect
Burnout
Automatic processes of
emotional regulation
.73
.48
.11
.24
.35
-.16
R2 = .55
R2 = .46
R2 = .24
R2 = .20
Model 4
.44
.06
-.41
-.13
-.16
Figure 1. The four variations of models of cognitive emotion regulation, affect, burnout and engagement
Table 4
Adjustment indices
χ² df CFI GFI TLI RMSEA (90% IC) χ²dif AIC
Model 1 60.54* 8 .83 .90 .68 .20 (.16 - .27) 7.56 86.54
Model 2 51.86* 6 .85 .92 .63 .22 (.17 - .28) 8.64 81.86
Model 3 55.74* 8 .85 .91 .71 .19 (.15 - .24) 6.96 81.74
Model 4 12.69* 6 .98 .98 .95 .08 (.01 - .15) 2.11 42.69
Final Model 13.80* 7 .97 .98 .95 .08 (.00 - .14) 1.97 41.78
Note: * p<0,01
Estanislao Castellano, Roger Muñoz-Navarro, María Sol Toledo, Carlos Spontón, and Leonardo Adrián Medrano
78
When these parameters were removed (Table 4), the t of
the model was improved in a Final Model (see Figure 2). The
standardized regression coef cients shown in Figure 2 are all
statistically signi cant. It should be mentioned that the model
has a high explanatory value, explaining 55% of the engagement
variance and 45% of the burnout variance. When considering
the total effects, it can be observed that elaborative process (β
total=.36 ) an d po sitive a ffe ct ( β total=.74) have a higher pre dictive
value for workers’ engagement. Automatic process (β total=.46)
and positive affect (β total= -.41) have a higher predictive value
for workers’ burnout.
Discussion
The role of emotions has been considered a predictor of burnout
(Castellano et al., 2013) and engagement (Lisbona et al., 2018) in
the workplace. However, there are still few studies that inquire into
the role of ER strategies. In this paper, an explanatory model of
burnout and engagement in response to worker ER was evaluated.
The t of the model was acceptable and many hypotheses were
corroborated with the exception of hypothesis 2 (direct, positive
relationship between ER elaborative processes and engagement),
hypothesis 3 (direct, negative relationship between ER elaborative
processes and negative affect) and hypothesis 7 (direct, negative
relationship between negative affect and engagement).
Basically, it can be seen that workers who activate elaborative
processes of ER (for example by positively reinterpret ing a negative
fact, or putting it into perspective) exp erience more positive affect,
greater engagement and less bur nout. On the contrary, workers who
tend to use mostly automatic ER processes (such as rumination or
catastrophizing) tend to have more negative affect, more burnout
and less positive affect. These ndings have a releva nt impact both
at the theoretical and practical levels.
As in previous studies (Garnefski et al., 2001; Gross, 2015),
our study shows a higher predictive value of positive affect and
elaborative processes of ER (for example, positive reappraisal).
Moreover, it can be observed that negative affect is not a signi cant
predictor of engagement in this model. These results point to the
importance of considering the “positive” factors of organizational
behavior. As emphasized by Positive Organizational Psychology
(Salanova et al., 2000; 2011), focusing on the dysfunctional factors
is not enough. To achieve a comprehensive approach to occupational
health it is necessary to pay due attention to those factors that
promote the optimal psychosocial functioning of employees. The
ndings of the present study therefore highlight the role played by
elaborative processes in the regulation of positive affect.
Our ndings are consistent with previous theoretical proposals.
Thus, Côté, Gyurak, & Levenson (2010) noted that the use of
elaborative processes of ER (such as positive reappraisal or focus
on pla ns) in creased wor kers’ feelings of sat isfact ion and de creased
their intention to quit. The opposite happened when automatic
processes of ER (such as catastroph izing) predominated: i ntentions
to quite increased and the levels of job satisfaction decreased.
The main contribution of ER elaborative processes in relation to
workers’ engagement may lie in the fact that they enable pleasant
emotions to be ampli ed. As Fredrickson & Joiner (2018) point
out, positive emotions, in addition to generating a pleasant feeling,
are a means to expand and develop a persons’ resources. This
coincides with the “resources/demands” theory (Schaufeli et al.,
2009), whereby workers with greater resources will experience
higher levels of engagement and those with fewer resources will
experience higher levels of burnout.
On a practical level, the results of this study provide a body
of evidence to support organizational intervention focused on
training to enable ER elaborative processes in workers. Programs
of this nature not only increase workers’ health, but also enhance
their degree of job satisfaction and engagement; this ultimately
affects their job performance, of considerable importance to the
organization as a whole (Côté et al., 2010). In this sense, it would
be bene cial for all concerned to design and implement training
programs in ER techniques (Braunstein, Gross, & Ochsner, 2017)
so that workers can boost the impact of positive affect and inhibit
or decrease their negative affect. Such programs will strengthen
workers’ self-regulatory capacity, providing them with resources
to lower the levels of negative affect and bur nout (in the di mensions
of cynicism, exhaustion, depersonalization and inef cacy) while
increasing positive affect and engagement (in the dimensions of
vigor, dedication and absorption).
Some limitations mer it consideration. The workers participating
in the present study were selected from a non-probability sample,
so its representativeness may be biased. In fact, the sample had
a higher proportion of private sector workers (76%). It would
be convenient to carry out studies with samples of public sector
workers in order to examine the invariance of the models.
Unfortunately, the sample size of this study does not allow such
analyses to be carried out (Medrano & Muñoz-Navarro, 2017).
Beyond this limitation, it is important to emphasize that the
sample is suf ciently heterogeneous in relation to income level,
type of work and size of the organization.
According to Salanova et al. (2011), the relationship between
engagement and positive affect is better conceptualized when it is
considered as a sequence of psychological experiences rather than
as a temporarily isolated episode. This aspect may constitute the
main limitation of this study, since the different instruments were
applied simultaneously, without leaving temporal intervals between
admin istrations. For this reason, the study should b e replicated in the
future, developing a plan for data collection in different phases, with
time intervals between administrations. This may provide greater
assurance that the independent variables precede one another and,
as a whole, of the effect of those on the dependent variable.
Elaborative processes of
emotional regulation
Automatic processes of
emotional regulation
Positive
affect
Negative
affect
Engagement
Burnout
R2 = .24
R2 = .20
R2 = .55
R2 = .46
.11
-.16
.44
.48 .73
-.41
.35
.24
Figure 2. Final model of cognitive, affect, burnout and engagement. Note:
All the path of the model was signi cant at level p<0.05
Cognitive processes of emotional regulation, burnout and work engagement
79
Another useful line of research would be to assess the effectiveness
of an ER training program considering as a dependent variable the levels
of positive and negative affect, as well as workers’ levels of engagement
and burnout. This type of training program would be aimed at increasing
the use of elaborative processes in ER, thus contributing to improved
levels of occupational health in organizations and companies.
References
Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52(3), 317-
332. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02294359
Arnold, K. A., Connelly, C. E., Walsh, M. M., & Martin Ginis, K. A.
(2015). Leadership styles, emotion regulation, and burnout. Journal
of Occupational Health Psychology, 20(4), 481-490. https://doi.
org/10.1037/a0039045
Bakker, A. B., & Albrecht, S. (2018). Work engagement: Current
trends. Career Development International, 23(1), 4-11. https://doi.
org/10.1108/CDI-11-2017-0207
Beck, A. T., & Clark, D. A. (1997). An information processing model of
anxiety: Automatic and strategic processes. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 35(1), 49-58. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(96)00069-1
Braunstein, L. M., Gross, J. J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2017). Explicit and
implicit emotion regu lation: A multi-level framework. Social Cognitive
and Af fective Neuroscience, 12(10), 1545-1557. https://doi.org/10.1093/
scan/nsx096
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Multivariate applications book series. Struct ural
equation modeling with AMOS: Basic concepts, applications, and
programming. Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Publishers.
Castellano, E., Cifre, E., Spontón, C., Medrano, L. A., & Maffei, L. (2013).
Positive and negative emotions on engagement and burnout prediction.
Revista Perua na de Psicología y Trabajo Social, 2(1), 75 -88.
Clore, G. L., & Ortony, A. (2000). Cognition in emotion: Always,
sometimes, or never? In R. D. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Series in
affective science. Cognitive neuroscience of emotion (pp. 24-61). New
York, NY, US: Oxford University Press.
Côté, S., & Morgan, L. M. (2002). A longitudinal ana lysis of the association
between emotion regulation, job satisfaction, and intentions to quit.
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 23(8), 947-962. htt ps://doi.
org/10.1002/job.174
Côté, S., Gyurak, A., & Levenson, R. W. (2010). The ability t o regulate emotion
is associated with greater well-being, income, and socioeconomic status.
Emotion, 10(6), 923-933. htt ps://doi.org/10.1037/a0021156
Fredrickson, B. L., & Joiner, T. (2018). Re ections on Positive Emotions
and Upward Spirals. Perspectives on Psychological S cience, 13(2),
194 -199. https://doi.org/10.1177/174569161769210 6
Garnefski, N., & Kraaij, V. (2007). The cognitive emotion regulation
questionnaire: Psychometric features and prospective relationships
with depression and anxiety in adults. European Journal of
Psych ologi cal A sses smen t, 23(3), 141-149. ht t ps :// do i. or g/ 10.1 02 7/10 15 -
5759.23.3.141
Garnefski, N., Kraaij, V., & Spinhoven, P. (2001). Negative life events,
cognitive emotion regulation and emotional problems. Person alit y
and Individual Dif ferences, 30(8), 1311-1327. https://doi.org/10.1016/
S0191-8869(00)00113-6
George, D., & Mallery, P. (2003). SPSS for Windows step by step: A simple
guide and reference. 11.0 update (4th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Gil-Monte, P. R. (2005). Factorial validity of the Maslach Burnout Inventory
(MBI-HSS) among Spanish professionals. Revista de Saúde Pública,
39(1), 1-8. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/S0034-89102005000100001
Grandey, A. A. (2015). Smiling for a Wage: What emotional labor teaches
us about emotion regulation. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 54 -60.
https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2015.962444
Grandey, A. A., & Melloy, R. C. (2017). The state of the heart: Emotional
labor as emotion regulation reviewed and revised. Journal of
Occupational Health Psychology, 22(3), 407- 422. https://doi.
org/10.1037/ocp0000067
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging eld of emotion regulation: An
integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 271. https://
doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Cur rent status and fut ure prospects.
Psychological Inquir y, 26(1), 1-26. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.
2014.940781
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for t indexes in covariance
structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives.
Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1),
1-55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118
Lent, R. W., & Brown, S. D. (2008). Social cognitive career theory and
subjective well-being in the context of work. Journal of Career
Assessment, 16(1), 6-21. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069072707305769
Lisbona, A., Palaci, F., Salanova, M., & Frese, M. (2018). The effects
of work engagement and self-ef cacy on personal initiative and
performance. Psicothema, 30(1), 89-96. https://doi.org/10.7334/
psicothema2016.245
Medrano, L. A., & Muñoz-Navarro, R. (2017). Conceptual and
practical approach to structural equations modeling. Revista Digital
de Investigación en Docencia Universitaria, 11(1). htt ps://doi.
org/10.19083/ridu.11.486
Medrano, L. A., Kanter, E. F., Trógolo, M., Ríos, M., Curerello, A., &
González, J. (2015). Adaptation of the Positive and Negative Affect
Scale (PANAS) to student university popu lation from Córdoba. Anuario
de Investigaciones de la Facultad de Psicología, 2(1), 22-36.
Medrano, L. A., Moretti, L., Ortiz, Á., & Pereno, G. (2013). Validation
of the Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire in University
Students of Córdoba, Argentina. Psykhe, 22(1), 83-9 6. http://dx.doi.
org/10.7764/psykhe.22.1.473
Medrano, L. A., Muñoz-Navarro, R., & Cano-Vindel, A. (2016). Cognitive
processes and emotion regulation: Contributions from a psycho-
evolutionary approach. Ansiedad y Estrés, 22(2-3), 47-54. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.anyes.2016.11.001
Moriondo, M., De Palma, P., Medrano, L. A., & Murillo, P. (2012).
Adaptation of Positive and Negative Affectivity Scale (PANAS) to
adults in Cordoba cit y: Preliminar y psychometric analysis. Universitas
Psych ologica, 11(1), 187-196.
Naragon-Gainey, K., McMahon, T. P., & Chacko, T. P. (2017). The
structure of common emotion regulation strategies: A meta-analytic
examination. Psychological Bulletin, 14 3(4), 38 4-427. https://doi.
org/10.1037/bul0000093
Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003).
Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review
of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of A pplied
Psych ology, 88(5), 879. doi:10.1037/0 021-9010. 88.5. 879
Prasad, A. B. (2016). Stress and burnout: Solutions to a global
problem. Adhyayan: A Journal of Management Sciences, 2(2). https://
doi.org/10.21567/adhyayan.v2i2.10248
Rodríguez Ayán, M. N., & Ruiz Díaz, M. Á. (2008). Atenuación de la
asimetría y de la curtosis de las puntuaciones observadas mediante
transformaciones de variables: incidencia sobre la estructura factorial
[Attenuation of the asymmetry and kurtosis of the observed scores
through transformations of variables: Incidence on the factorial
structure]. Psicológica, 29(2), 205-227.
Rodríguez Ga rcía, G. A., López-Pérez, B., Férreo Cru zado, M. A., Ferná ndez
Carr ascoso, M. E., & Fernández, J. (2017). Impact of t he Intensive Progra m
of Emotional Intelligence (IPEI) on work supervisors. Psicothema,
29(4), 508-513. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2016.396
Rodríguez-Mantilla, J. M., & Fernández-Díaz, M. (2017). The effect
of interpersonal relationships on burnout syndrome in Secondary
Education teachers. Psicothema, 29(3), 370-377. https://doi.
org/10.7334/psicothema2016.309
Salanova, M., Llorens, S., & Schaufeli, W. B. (2011). “Yes, I can, I feel
good, and I just do it!” On gain cycles and spirals of ef cacy beliefs,
Estanislao Castellano, Roger Muñoz-Navarro, María Sol Toledo, Carlos Spontón, and Leonardo Adrián Medrano
80
affect, and engagement. Applied Psychology, 60(2), 255-285. https://
doi.org/10.1111/j.14 64-0597.2010.00435. x
Salanova, M., Llorens, S., García-Renedo, M., Burriel, R., Bresó, E., &
Schaufeli, W. B. (2005). Toward a four-dimensional model of burnout:
A multigroup factor-analytic study including depersonalization and
cynicism. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 65, 901-913.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164405275662
Salanova, M., Schaufeli, W., Llorens, S., Peiró, J., & Grau, R. (2000).
Desde el “burnout” al “Engagement”: ¿una nueva perspectiva?
[From “burnout” to “Engagement”: A new perspective?]. Revista de
Psicología del Trabajo y las Organizaciones, 16(2), 117-134.
Schaufeli, W. B., Bakker, A. B., & van Rhenen, W. (2009). How changes
in job demands and resources predict burnout, work engagement, and
sickness absenteeism. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30(7),
893-917. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.595
Spontón, C., Castellano, E., Salanova, M., Llorens, S., Maffei, L., & Medrano, L.
(2018). Evaluation of a socio-cognit ive model of self-ef cacy, burnout and
work engagement: Invariance analysis between Argentina and Spain.
Psych ologia, 12(1), 89-101. doi: 10.21500/19002386.3226
Spontón, C., Medrano, L. A., Maffei, L., Spontón, M., & Castellano, E.
(2012). Validation of the engagement Questionnaire UWES for the
population of workers of Córdoba, Argentina. Liberabit, 18(2), 147-154.
Spontón, C., Trógolo, M., Castellano, E., & Medrano, L. (in press).
Psychometric properties of the Maslach Burnout Inventory- General
Survey in a sample of Argentinian workers. Interdisciplinaria: Revista
de Psicología y Ciencias A nes
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using Multiva riat e Sta tist ics.
Pearson Education, Inc. https://doi.org/10.1037/022267
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and
Validation of Brief Measures of Positive and Negative Affect: The
PANAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(6),
1063-1070. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063
... These findings align with a body of organizational studies addressing the foundational assumption of the disparity between cognition and emotion in the workplace (Kay & Skarlicki, 2020). According to previous studies, cognition (or perception) and emotion (or affection) play distinct roles in organizational management and behaviors, such as organizational change management (Rafferty & Minbashian, 2019), workplace energy (Baker, 2019), cognitive processing of emotions (Castellano et al., 2019), organizational empathy (Clark et al., 2019), creation of firms' positive cultures (Men & Yue, 2019), job satisfaction, team morale, and commitment to the group (Jehn et al., 1999), and so on. Our study extends this understanding by introducing the interpersonal workplace relationship context to the discourse of cognition vs. emotion in the workplace. ...
... Our results suggest that managers should understand team members' cognitive interpretations and social dynamics among them. Cognition involves a thorough, deliberate, and analytical mental process, while emotion entails a more intuitive, automatic, and holistic one (Castellano et al., 2019). People tend to break down stimuli into smaller pieces of information when they use cognitive interpretation. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to investigate the impact of relationships with supervisors (RS) and relationships with colleagues (RC) on two key employee attitudes toward organizations: organizational identification (OI) and affective commitment (AC). Data was collected from a survey of employees of a Korean conglomerate. The sample company was chosen due to its organizational culture that values interactions and relationships among its members. We conduct a response surface analysis and demonstrate that, while RS increases OI, RC enhances AC. We also discover that the imbalance between RS and RC has contrasting effects on OI and AC, which exerts a positive impact on OI and a negative impact on AC. In particular, the asymmetrical impacts of the imbalance are identified in which the imbalance of the relatively stronger RS has a greater influence than the imbalance of the relatively stronger RC. Although employees have two distinguished workplace relationships (i.e., RS and RC) which are governed by different mechanisms, studies that have distinguished these two relationships and examined the effect of each relationship are limited. By revealing the distinct patterns that OI and AC exhibit when they are affected by RS and RC and discovering the asymmetrical impacts of the imbalance between RS and RC, this study provides new insights into the effects of personal workplace relationships on employee behaviors. By offering valuable information for the conceptualization and differentiation of OI and AC, this study enriches the existing literature on these two key employee attitudes.
... Individuals use a variety of emotion regulation strategies, some of which are positively related to adaptive behaviors, reduced chances of exposure to psychological problems, and an increase in social and professional participation; Emotion regulation is positively associated with job satisfaction and effective performance at work (Salvarani et al., 2019;Lisbona et al., 2018;Rodríguez-Mantilla & Fernández-Díaz, 2017;Castellano et al., 2013). The emotion regulation process determines how an individual's emotions are experienced and how they are expressed after regulating and modifying the spontaneous emergence of those emotions or re-control of them (Castellano et al., 2019). Gross (1998) divided techniques for controlling one's feelings into two categories: In the first place, there are antecedent-focused emotion control methods, which an individual employ before the behavioral responses shift and the emotional response inclinations become active. ...
Article
Full-text available
The current study investigated the effect of gender, teaching experience, academic certificate, and specialization on emotional regulation difficulties. Participants were 172 faculty members at three private universities in Jordan and the United Arab Emirates (UAE). The difficulties in emotion regulation scale (DERS) was applied. The study showed that female faculty members had more limited access to emotion regulation strategies than their male counterparts did. The results also showed that experienced faculty members were more likely to accept emotional responses, but have difficulties with goal-directed activity, impulse control, and lack of emotional awareness. The findings suggest that effective use of emotion regulation strategies is affected by gender, moderate teaching experience affects faculty members' impulse control, emotional responses acceptance, and goal-directed activity, and doctorate holders from different specializations are more aware of their emotions.
... Given the difculty in eradicating the occurrence of these situations due to the infuence of multiple factors, it is necessary to cushion this negative impact by following an approach towards health and well-being at work, with the aim of trying to promote an optimal psychosocial functioning of workers [8,9]. In this context, the functional or dysfunctional links between workers and their work activity have an impact on their health and well-being, hence the importance of analysing the variables that may act as modulators of occupational health, such as engagement [10]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective. To analyse the relationship between workplace violence and work engagement in nursing professionals. Background. Violence in the workplace has become a phenomenon of growing concern. Nursing professionals are one of the most exposed groups to this type of violence. Method. A descriptive cross-sectional study was carried out on a national sample of Spanish nurses by means of convenience, non-probabilistic sampling. A questionnaire was administered that collected sociodemographic variables, the Utrecht Work Engagement Scale (UWES-9), and an “ad hoc” scale on violence in the workplace. Results. 1648 active nursing professionals participated in this study. The participants obtained higher scores in the “dedication” variable. It was observed that 42.17% had personally suffered some type of aggression or violence at work. Verbal aggression was the most frequent type of reported violence. Nurses who experience violence, threat, and psychological harassment showed lower levels of work engagement than those who did not. In the case of those who experience sexual harassment and those who do not, the values for the UWES-9 were similar. Conclusions. Having personally experienced violence, threats or psychological harassment can negatively affect nurses’ work engagement, the most common acts of violence being verbal and often perpetrated by family members and patients. Implications for Nursing Management. Adequate tools are not still being used to ensure that the assaulted professionals are adequately assisted by their institutions.
... it should also be noted that the relationship observed between automatic and elaborative strategies was weak. These results are consistent with previous research (castellano et al., 2019;Dominguez-Lara & medrano, 2016) and allow us to rule out the existence of an inverse relationship between the two types of processing. The use of elaborative strategies would not generate a decrease in automatic strategies, although it may possibly moderate their impact. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background. Emotion Regulation (ER) involves any explicit or implicit process that may alter the emotion felt, its duration and expression, and is a transdiagnostic factor of vulnerability involved in the etiology and maintenance of different emotional disorders. The Cognitive Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (CERQ) assesses nine cognitive strategies involved in ER and is a valuable tool. Its popularity and wide use led to the development of two abbreviated versions: a version with 18 items (two items per factor) and a 27-item version (three items per factor). Objective. To analyze the psychometric properties of both versions in the Argentinean population. Design. The research design was instrumental. The factor structure of the CERQ-18 and CERQ-27 as well as the reliability of the scores and the construct of each dimension were evaluated. In addition, we gathered validity evidence for its relationship with other variables by associating the CERQ scores with Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) scores. Results. The CERQ-18 presented more consistent evidence regarding its internal structure (adequate fit indices and factor loadings of moderate magnitude) and reliability. Given that the association of the two versions with the DERS is similar, we recommend that the 18-item version be used. Conclusion. The CERQ-18 has quite similar psychometric properties to the CERQ27 in the general population of Argentina and the findings contribute to an understanding of its internal structure.
... Although many studies have explored how emotional regulation influences other variables (e.g., Castellano et al., 2019;Hwang & Park, 2022), conflict management, in particular, has received little attention. Despite the knowledge gap on the influence of teachers' ERA in conflict management, studies on other professionals show that ERA influences the selection and use of strategies for conflict management (e.g., Chen et al., 2019;Villamediana et al., 2015). ...
Article
It has been postulated that emotions play essential roles in conflict situations and that excessive expression of emotions or inappropriate display can reduce the capacity to manage conflict. However, there is a lack of research that relates teachers' emotion-regulation ability to managing conflict. To bridge this gap, this pilot study aimed to examine the relationships between teachers' emotion-regulation ability and conflict management strategies used in the classroom. The sample consisted of 878 teachers (61% women) working in Portuguese schools, which completed an application of Mayer and Salovey's emotional intelligence model and Rahim's model of conflict management. Using the structural equation modeling, findings revealed that the teachers who tend to show a greater ability to regulate emotions use mainly integrating and compromising strategies to manage classroom conflicts more frequently than other strategies. In conclusion, it is necessary to create a curriculum in the pre-service teachers' education programs that includes emotional education, so that they can acquire emotional regulation skills, due to their importance in classroom conflict management.
... Although many studies have explored how emotional regulation influences other variables (e.g., Castellano et al., 2019;Hwang & Park, 2022), conflict management, in particular, has received little attention. Despite the knowledge gap on the influence of teachers' ERA in conflict management, studies on other professionals show that ERA influences the selection and use of strategies for conflict management (e.g., Chen et al., 2019;Villamediana et al., 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
It has been postulated that emotions play essential roles in conflict situations and that excessive expression of emotions or inappropriate display can reduce the capacity to manage conflict. However, there is a lack of research that relates teachers’ emotion-regulation ability to managing conflict. To bridge this gap, this pilot study aimed to examine the relationships between teachers’ emotion-regulation ability and conflict management strategies used in the classroom. The sample consisted of 878 teachers (61% women) working in Portuguese schools, which completed an application of Mayer and Salovey’s emotional intelligence model and Rahim’s model of conflict management. Using the structural equation modeling, findings revealed that the teachers who tend to show a greater ability to regulate emotions use mainly integrating and compromising strategies to manage classroom conflicts more frequently than other strategies. In conclusion, it is necessary to create a curriculum in the pre-service teachers’ education programs that includes emotional education, so that they can acquire emotional regulation skills, due to their importance in classroom conflict management.
... Diversos estudios realizados con trabajadores de Argentina evidencian la existencia de problemas y sintomatología que deterioran la salud mental y la calidad de vida, por ejemplo, cansancio crónico, síntomas de ansiedad, tensión, malestar emocional, sentimientos de ineficacia y baja realización personal, entre otros (Castellano, Muñoz-Navarro, Toledo, Spontón, & Medrano, 2019;Medrano & Trógolo, 2018;. Aunque indudablemente existen factores contextuales que contribuyen a explicar estos resultados, los problemas de salud vinculados al trabajo configuran un problema de carácter transnacional y transcultural (Bakker & Demerouti, 2013;Carod-Artal & Vázquez-Cabrera, 2012). ...
Article
Full-text available
El objetivo de este estudio consistió en examinar las propiedades psicométricas del “Cuestionario de Experiencias de Recuperación” para su uso en Argentina. La muestra estuvo compuesta por 505 trabajadores provenientes del sector público, privado y organizaciones no gubernamentales. Los resultados del análisis factorial confirmatorio evidenciaron que un modelo de cuatro factores ─distanciamiento psicológico del trabajo, relajación, búsqueda de retos y control sobre el tiempo libre─ presentó un buen ajuste a los datos. Por lo tanto, el cuestionario mantiene la misma estructura que la escala original. Los análisis de confiabilidad mostraron que la escala posee una adecuada consistencia interna (α = .75 - .92) y fiabilidad de constructo (H = .88 - .93). Se obtuvieron correlaciones teóricamente esperadas con medidas de burnout, work engagement y afecto negativo, aportando de esta manera evidencias externas de validez concurrente. En conclusión, la escala posee adecuadas propiedades psicométricas para ser utilizada en el contexto argentino, permitiendo disponer de un instrumento confiable que permite profundizar el conocimiento sobre la recuperación y brindando una herramienta valiosa para la práctica profesional. The aim of this study was to examine the psychometric properties of the Recovery Experience Questionnaire in Argentina. Five-hundred and five workers from public, private and non-governmental sectors participated in the study. Confirmatory factor analysis showed that the four-factor model has acceptable fit to the data. Thus, as the original scale, the Argentinean validation maintain the four recovery experiences—psychological detachment from work, relaxation, mastery experiences and control over leisure time. Reliability analyses showed good internal consistency (Cronbach’s α ranging from .75 to .92) and construct reliability (coefficient H ranging from .88 to .93) for all subscales. Correlations theoretically expected with measures of work engagement, burnout and negative affect supported for concurrent validity. Thus, the Recovery Experience Questionnaire shows adequate psychometric properties for its use in Argentina, make available a reliable instrument that allows deepening knowledge about recovery and providing a valuable tool for professional practice.
... Dada la dificultad en erradicar este tipo de situaciones, debido a la influencia de múltiples factores, se hace necesario amortiguar el impacto negativo mediante el enfoque de la salud y el bienestar en el trabajo, intentando así promover un funcionamiento psicosocial óptimo de los trabajadores (6,7). En ese contexto, los vínculos funcionales o disfuncionales entre la persona que trabaja y su actividad laboral tienen un impacto en su salud y bienestar, de ahí la importancia de analizar variables que puedan modular los efectos de la salud laboral, como el compromiso laboral (8). ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Nurses are one of the groups most exposed to violence in the workplace. The objective of this study was to analyze the relationship between violence at work and engagement in a sample of nurses in Spain. Methods: A descriptive cross-sectional study was carried out in a national sample of Spanish nurses. A questionnaire was administered that collected the sociodemographic variables, the Utrecht Work Engagement Scale (UWES-9) and an ad hoc scale on violence at work. The Mann-Whitney U test was performed as the statistical test with Bonferroni correction and the CHAID algorithm. Results: The sample consisted of a total of 1,648 active nurses. It was observed that 42.17% of them had personally suffered some type of aggression or violence in the workplace, verbal aggression being the most frequent. The results indicated that there was a negative association between work engagement and exposure to situations of violence at work. Conclusions: There is a relationship between having suffered attacks and the degree of work engagement against nurses, hence the need to establish effective preventive and intervention policies to promote an adequate work environment, and therefore stop episodes of violence in their initial stages.
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed at investigating the potential effect of emotional regulation on the medical staff ‎in ‎Algerian hospitals. A cross-sectional ‎ approach based on survey design was used in this study in ‎order ‎to answer the research questions. Data were collected by a questionnaire administered to a ‎sample ‎consisting of 153 randomly selected medical staff working at Algerian hospitals. The results ‎revealed ‎that the risk of COVID-19 transmission affected the emotional regulation of the medical staff ‎in ‎Algerian hospitals. It was also found that there were differences among participants in their ‎emotional ‎regulation that could be attributed to the variables of profession and workplace. The ‎results ‎highlighted the contributions of the positive and negative ‎emotional regulation strategies, ‎profession, ‎and workplace as mediating variables in ‎‏predicting the emotional regulation of medical ‎staff.‎ The results ‎have important implications for how best to help the medical staff fulfill their ‎emotions, thus being ‎better qualified for the response to the COVID-19 pandemic.
Article
Full-text available
University faculty members seek to regulate emotions to achieve professional and social goals in the work environment. The process of emotion regulation is influenced by self-awareness, as self-awareness is an important predictor of self-regulation, and the outcomes of the self-regulation process depend, in part, on the level of self-awareness. The purpose of the present paper was to examine whether or not self-awareness is used to predict emotional regulation difficulties among faculty members. The current quantitative study was designed using a survey research design. The participants comprised 172 faculty members from Philadelphia University in Jordan, the Arab Open University in Jordan, and Al Falah University in the U.A.E. Data were collected using the Self-Awareness Scale (SAS) and Emotional Regulation Difficulties Scale (ERDS). The researchers revealed that increasing the self-awareness subscale (self-critical) decreases the non-acceptance of emotional responses. The researchers also found that when there is an increase in the self-awareness subscale (desire for realistic awareness), there is a tendency toward lower levels of non-acceptance of emotional responses and difficulties engaging in goal-directed. Researchers also came to that an increase in the self-awareness subscale (self-reflection) decreases the non-acceptance of emotional responses, difficulties engaging in goal-directed and impulse control difficulties. The researchers concluded there is a need to work on university faculties' self-awareness and emotional regulation to balance realistic awareness and emotional responses related to task engagement and control difficulties. Based on the findings, it is concluded that it is necessary to pay attention to enhancing self-awareness and emotion regulation among faculty members in general and conduct more scientific studies on emotional regulation difficulties to examine their relationship with other variables. Doi: 10.28991/ESJ-2023-07-04-017 Full Text: PDF
Article
Full-text available
Background: Two popular concepts, work engagement and personal initiative, are different but related constructs. This study is based on and extends the Frese and Fay (2001) model of personal initiative (PI) by including work engagement (WE) and self-efficacy as antecedents of PI, and performance as a consequence. Method: Two studies (study 1, with a cross-sectional design using N = 396 participants from 22 organizations, and study 2, with a longitudinal design conducted in two waves with N = 118 participants from 15 organizations) test the hypotheses. Results: Structural equation modeling and the PROCESS SPSS Macro were used to test the hypothesized mediating role of personal initiative in work engagement and performance, and the results show the indirect effect of WE on performance through PI. Conclusions: The results of these two studies confirmed our hypotheses: WE and self-efficacy lead to higher PI, which, in turn, leads to higher performance. In addition to considering WE as an antecedent of PI, the results lead to considering PI as an antecedent of performance.
Article
Full-text available
The ability to adaptively regulate emotion is essential for mental and physical well-being. How should we organize the myriad ways people attempt to regulate their emotions? We explore the utility of a framework that distinguishes among four fundamental classes of emotion regulation strategies. The framework describes each strategy class in terms their behavioral characteristics, underlying psychological processes, and supporting neural systems. A key feature of this multi-level framework is its conceptualization of the psychological processes in terms of two orthogonal dimensions that describe (1) the nature of the emotion regulation goal (ranging from to implicit to explicit) and (2) the nature of the emotion change process (ranging from more automatic to more controlled). After describing the core elements of the framework, we use it to review human and animal research on the neural bases of emotion regulation and to suggest key directions for future research on emotion regulation.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Recent studies show that teachers, especially in Secondary Education present varying levels of burnout syndrome. This problem could be caused by internal factors unique to the subject (psychological characteristics) or external factors (overwork, social climate, etc.). Method: The objective of this study is to analyze the influence of interpersonal relationships on the development of burnout in a sample of 794 secondary education teachers from the Community of Madrid, applying structural equation modeling methodology. Results: it was observed that the teacher-student relationship has a significant effect on each of the three dimensions of the syndrome (exhaustion, cynicism, and inefficacy), and the teacher-superiors and teacher-coworker relationships show a moderate effect on these dimensions. Conclusions: The results show the importance of taking care of interpersonal relationships in schools to ensure the well-being of teachers and, ultimately, the quality of the learning process.
Article
Full-text available
Emotion regulation has been examined extensively with regard to important outcomes, including psychological and physical health. However, the literature includes many different emotion regulation strategies but little examination of how they relate to one another, making it difficult to interpret and synthesize findings. The goal of this meta-analysis was to examine the underlying structure of common emotion regulation strategies (i.e., acceptance, behavioral avoidance, distraction, experiential avoidance, expressive suppression, mindfulness, problem solving, reappraisal, rumination, worry), and to evaluate this structure in light of theoretical models of emotion regulation. We also examined how distress tolerance—an important emotion regulation ability —relates to strategy use. We conducted meta-analyses estimating the correlations between emotion regulation strategies (based on 331 samples and 670 effect sizes), as well as between distress tolerance and strategies. The resulting meta-analytic correlation matrix was submitted to confirmatory and exploratory factor analyses. None of the confirmatory models, based on prior theory, was an acceptable fit to the data. Exploratory factor analysis suggested that 3 underlying factors best characterized these data. Two factors—labeled Disengagement and Aversive Cognitive Perseveration—emerged as strongly correlated but distinct factors, with the latter consisting of putatively maladaptive strategies. The third factor, Adaptive Engagement, was a less unified factor and weakly related to the other 2 factors. Distress tolerance was most closely associated with low levels of repetitive negative thought and experiential avoidance, and high levels of acceptance and mindfulness. We discuss the theoretical implications of these findings and applications to emotion regulation assessment.
Chapter
Full-text available
This book, a member of the Series in Affective Science, is a unique interdisciplinary sequence of articles on the cognitive neuroscience of emotion by some of the most well-known researchers in the area. It explores what is known about cognitive processes in emotion at the same time it reviews the processes and anatomical structures involved in emotion, determining whether there is something about emotion and its neural substrates that requires they be studied as a separate domain. Divided into four major focal points and presenting research that has been performed in the last decade, this book covers the process of emotion generation, the functions of amygdala, the conscious experience of emotion, and emotion regulation and dysregulation. Collectively, the chapters constitute a broad but selective survey of current knowledge about emotion and the brain, and they all address the close association between cognitive and emotional processes. By bringing together diverse strands of investigation with the aim of documenting current understanding of how emotion is instantiated in the brain, this book will be of use to scientists, researchers, and advanced students of psychology and neuroscience.
Article
We reflect on our 2002 article and the impact this research report has had both within and beyond psychological science. This article was both one of the first publications to provide empirical support for hypotheses based on the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions and a product of the genesis of positive psychology. We highlight empirical and theoretical advancements in the scientific understanding of upward spiral dynamics associated with positive emotions, with particular focus on the new upward spiral theory of lifestyle change. We conclude by encouraging deeper and more rigorous tests of the prospective and reciprocal relations associated with positive emotions. Such progress is needed to better inform translations and applications to improve people’s health and well-being.
Article
Over the past two decades, the number of studies on work engagement has increased rapidly. Work engagement refers to a positive, affective-motivational state of high energy combined with high levels of dedication and a strong focus on work (Schaufeli and Bakker, 2010). It is highly desirable for contemporary public and private organizations to have engaged employees because engagement has been shown to coincide with high levels of creativity, task performance, organizational citizenship behavior, and client satisfaction (Bakker et al., 2014). In this paper, we briefly discuss the state of the art of the work engagement literature, and then outline new research trends and research questions. After that, we introduce the articles that have been included in this special issue of Career Development International that is devoted to work engagement.
Article
Background: This study aimed to evaluate the effect of the Intensive Program of Emotional Intelligence (IPEI; Fernández, 2016; Férreo, 2016) on middle managers’ emotional intelligence, as this variable may have a significant impact on personal satisfaction, task performance, and the work environment. Method: The intervention was applied to work team supervisors in a large call center, as it is an overlooked sector in this topic. Two-hundred and eighty-two supervisors from a Madrid-based, Spanish multinational (51.4% men and 48.6% women) participated in this study. Participants were assigned to the experimental group (n = 190) or the control group (n = 92) by availability, according to management decision. All supervisors filled in two questionnaires to evaluate the different components of intrapersonal emotional intelligence (i.e., attention, clarity, and repair; TMMS-24; Fernández-Berrocal, Extremera, & Ramos, 2004) and cognitive and affective empathy (i.e., perspective taking, emotion understanding, empathic joy, and personal distress; TECA; López-Pérez, Fernández, & Abad, 2008). Results: The findings showed an increase in the studied variables for the experimental group. Conclusions: The results obtained support middle managers’ training in emotional competences through short, efficient, economic programs. Potential limitations and implications of the results are discussed.
Article
Emotional labor has been an area of burgeoning research interest in occupational health psychology in recent years. Emotional labor was conceptualized in the early 1980s by sociologist Arlie Hochschild (1983) as occupational requirements that alienate workers from their emotions. Almost 2 decades later, a model was published in Journal of Occupational Health Psychology (JOHP) that viewed emotional labor through a psychological lens, as emotion regulation strategies that differentially relate to performance and wellbeing. For this anniversary issue of JOHP, we review the emotional labor as emotion regulation model, its contributions, limitations, and the state of the evidence for its propositions. At the heart of our article, we present a revised model of emotional labor as emotion regulation, that incorporates recent findings and represents a multilevel and dynamic nature of emotional labor as emotion regulation.