PreprintPDF Available
Preprints and early-stage research may not have been peer reviewed yet.

Abstract and Figures

This study examined how scrupulosity, depression, anxiety, stress, and neuroticism may statistically predict problematic pornography viewing dimensions. Participants (n = 507 women and n = 250 men) responded to an online survey. Structural equation modeling indicated scrupulosity as a significant positive predictor across all problematic pornography viewing dimensions across genders. Depression was also a significant positive predictor, but only for those who use pornography to escape negative emotions, and for women with functional problems related to pornography use. The relationship between scrupulosity and functional pornography problems was significantly stronger for men. Interaction analyses suggested that low scrupulosity may buffer the relationship between mental health concerns and problematic pornography viewing in men, while high scrupulosity may exacerbate the relationship between anxiety and excessive pornography use in women. The full model accounted for 14 – 34% of the variance of various dimensions of problematic pornography viewing. Clinical implications and areas of further research are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at
https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=usac20
Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity
The Journal of Treatment & Prevention
ISSN: 1072-0162 (Print) 1532-5318 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/usac20
Is scrupulosity behind the relationship between
problematic pornography viewing and depression,
anxiety, and stress?
Nicholas C. Borgogna, Jessica Duncan & Ryon C. McDermott
To cite this article: Nicholas C. Borgogna, Jessica Duncan & Ryon C. McDermott (2018)
Is scrupulosity behind the relationship between problematic pornography viewing and
depression, anxiety, and stress?, Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity, 25:4, 293-318, DOI:
10.1080/10720162.2019.1567410
To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/10720162.2019.1567410
View supplementary material
Published online: 18 Feb 2019.
Submit your article to this journal
Article views: 52
View Crossmark data
Citing articles: 1 View citing articles
Is scrupulosity behind the relationship between
problematic pornography viewing and depression,
anxiety, and stress?
Nicholas C. Borgogna , Jessica Duncan, and Ryon C. McDermott,
University of South Alabama, Mobile, Alabama
ABSTRACT
This study examined how scrupulosity, depression, anxiety,
stress, and neuroticism may statistically predict problematic
pornography viewing dimensions. Participants (n¼507
women and n¼250 men) responded to an online survey.
Structural equation modeling indicated scrupulosity as a sig-
nificant positive predictor across all problematic pornography
viewing dimensions across genders. Depression was also a sig-
nificant positive predictor, but only for those who use pornog-
raphy to escape negative emotions, and for women with
functional problems related to pornography use. The relation-
ship between scrupulosity and functional pornography prob-
lems was significantly stronger for men. Interaction analyses
suggested that low scrupulosity may buffer the relationship
between mental health concerns and problematic pornog-
raphy viewing in men, while high scrupulosity may exacerbate
the relationship between anxiety and excessive pornography
use in women. The full model accounted for 14 34% of the
variance of various dimensions of problematic pornography
viewing. Clinical implications and areas of further research
are discussed.
Over the past two decades researchers and clinicians have become increas-
ingly interested in understanding predictors of problematic pornography
viewing (Borgogna & McDermott, 2018; Brand, Antons, Wegmann, &
Potenza, 2018; Grubbs & Perry, 2018; Grubbs, Perry, Wilt, & Reid, 2018;
Kor et al., 2014; Price, Patterson, Regnerus, & Walley, 2016; Short, Black,
Smith, Wetterneck, & Wells, 2012; Sniewski, Farvid, & Carter, 2018;
Twohig, Crosby, & Cox, 2009). Defined as personal/interpersonal problems
associated with compulsive/addictive pornography use, problematic pornog-
raphy viewing has been conceptualized as a multidimensional construct.
Using Kor et al.s(2014) framework specifically, these dimensions include
functional problems related to pornography viewing; i.e., relationship/work
CONTACT Nicholas C. Borgogna nicholascborgogna@gmail.com University of South Alabama, Mobile,
Alabama, USA.
Supplemental data for this article can be accessed on the publishers website.
ß2019 Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
SEXUAL ADDICTION & COMPULSIVITY
2018, VOL. 25, NO. 4, 293318
https://doi.org/10.1080/10720162.2019.1567410
problems (Kor et al., 2014; Leonhardt, Willoughby, & Young-Petersen,
2017; Manning, 2006; Minarcik, Wetterneck, & Short, 2016; Muusses,
Kerkhof, & Finkenauer, 2015; Perry, 2016; Poulsen, Busby, & Galovan,
2013); problems associated with excessive use and control difficulties
(DOrlando, 2011; Kor et al., 2014; Kraus, Potenza, Martino, & Grant,
2015); and, using pornography as a dysfunctional means of escaping mental
health problems (Kor et al., 2014; Perry, 2017). While multiple studies have
observed significant positive relationships between pornography viewing
and various mental health outcomes (e.g., Butler, Pereyra, Draper,
Leonhardt, & Skinner, 2018; Egan & Parmar, 2013; Levin, Lillis, & Hayes,
2012; Perry, 2017), few have included important cultural and individual dif-
ference factors that may be relevant in determining how general mental
health concerns (i.e., anxiety/depression) are related to problematic pornog-
raphy viewing. Such factors may be helpful for informing clinical
interventions.
Gender and religiosity constructs have both been offered as avenues that
may moderate frequencies of pornography viewing and subjective feelings
of problematic pornography viewing behaviors (e.g., perceived addiction;
Grubbs, Stauner, Exline, Pargament, & Lindberg, 2015; Grubbs, Wilt,
Exline, Pargament, & Kraus, 2018). Scrupulosity specifically has gained
recent attention as a particular aspect of religiosity that may be strongly
predictive of problematic pornography behaviors (Borgogna & McDermott,
2018; Grubbs et al., 2018; Short, Kasper, & Wetterneck, 2015), although
this view has been debated (Brand et al., 2018). However, to date, no study
has examined mental health and scrupulosity variables simultaneously in
the prediction of problematic pornography viewing. Nor has any study
examined how gender, scrupulosity, and mental health may interact in the
prediction of problematic pornography viewing. We, therefore, sought to
address these gaps in the literature.
Pornography viewing and mental health
While research on problematic pornography viewing is relatively nascent,
multiple studies have indicated concerning outcomes associated with porn-
ography viewing frequency. These include modest positive correlations with
loneliness (Butler et al., 2018; Yoder, Virden, & Amin, 2005), depression
(Kraus et al., 2015; Nelson, Padilla-Walker, & Carroll, 2010; Perry, 2017;
Wolak, Mitchell, & Finkelhor, 2007; Ybarra & Mitchell, 2005), anxiety
(Harper & Hodgins, 2016; Levin et al., 2012), narcissism (Kasper, Short, &
Milam, 2015), headaches (Anand & Dhikav, 2012), and neuroticism (Egan
& Parmar, 2013). Moreover, pornography use has also been modestly nega-
tively associated with general life satisfaction (Harper & Hodgins, 2016;
294 N. C. BORGOGNA ET AL.
Wright, Tokunaga, Kraus, & Klann, 2017), relationship satisfaction/relation-
ship problems (Bergner & Bridges, 2002; Bridges, Bergner, & Hesson-
McInnis, 2003; Daspe, Vaillancourt-Morel, Lussier, Sabourin, & Ferron,
2018; Perry, 2018), and sexual satisfaction/desire (Carvalheira, Traeen, &
Stulhofer, 2015; Daspe et al., 2018; Sun, Bridges, Johnson, & Ezzell, 2016;
Wright, Bridges, Sun, Ezzell, & Johnson, 2018; Wright, Sun, Steffen, &
Tokunaga, 2017). While mental health factors are implicated in relation to
pornography viewing (both as a statistical predictor and as an outcome;
Butler et al., 2018; Perry, 2017), cultural and individual factors have also
been reported to be important in determining who may develop problem-
atic pornography viewing behaviors. Religiosity and gender have both been
identified has variables important to this consideration (e.g., Nelson et al.,
2010; Perry, 2017; Twohig et al., 2009).
Pornography and religiosity
Multiple studies have noted that the negative outcomes related to pornog-
raphy viewing are pronounced in religious populations (Borgogna &
McDermott, 2018; Bradley, Grubbs, Uzdavines, Exline, & Pargament, 2016;
Grubbs, Exline, Pargament, Volk, & Lindberg, 2017; Grubbs & Perry, 2018;
Grubbs, Stauner, et al., 2015; Nelson et al., 2010; Patterson & Price, 2012;
Perry, 2017; Perry & Snawder, 2017; Short et al., 2015; Whitehead & Perry,
2018), and more specifically religious men (e.g., Perry, 2017). Prior reports
have demonstrated that religious individuals may perceive their pornog-
raphy use as addictive even if it may not follow an addictive paradigm
(termed perceived addiction; Grubbs, Exline, Pargament, Hook, & Carlisle,
2015; Wilt, Cooper, Grubbs, Exline, & Pargament, 2016; however, see
Brand et al., 2018). Perceived addiction is statistically predictive of psycho-
logical distress even when pornography viewing frequency is controlled
(Grubbs, Stauner, et al., 2015; Grubbs, Volk, Exline, & Pargament, 2015;
Wilt et al., 2016). While these finding broadly suggest that religious indi-
viduals may be at-risk for developing problems related to pornography use,
other researchers have suggested religiosity may act a potential protective
factor (e.g., Hardy, Steelman, Coyne, & Ridge, 2013). Moreover, religiosity
as a broad-construct may have dimensions that function as both protective
and risk-factors. Scrupulous-religiosity (scrupulosity) has been suggested as
a potential religious dimension that may serve as a risk-factor for problem-
atic pornography viewing (N.C. Borgogna & McDermott, 2018).
Characterized as a psychological condition defined by pathological guilt
and/or obsession associated with moral or religious issues (Abramowitz,
Huppert, Cohen, Tolin, & Cahill, 2002; Miller & Hedges, 2008), scrupulos-
ity was suggested as a potential risk factor in the role between religiosity
SEXUAL ADDICTION & COMPULSIVITY 295
and problematic pornography viewing by Short et al. (2015). Grubbs et al.
(2018) similarly identified religious scruplesas relevant to the develop-
ment of perceived addiction to pornography viewing. Most recently, scru-
pulosity was reported as a significant statistical predictor of problematic
pornography viewing in men and women across all dimensions in Kor
et al.s(2014) framework (Borgogna & McDermott, 2018).
While these studies indicate scrupulosity may be relevant to the develop-
ment of problematic pornography viewing, no studies have examined how
mental health issues and scrupulosity may interact to exacerbate and/or
buffer problematic pornography viewing tendencies. Similarly, there is a
need to further examine how gender may influence such possible interac-
tions. This is important as men typically report more problematic tenden-
cies associated with their viewing and higher viewing frequencies than
women (e.g. Albright, 2008; Carroll et al., 2008; Harper & Hodgins, 2016;
Price, Patterson, Regnerus, & Walley, 2016;W
ery & Billieux, 2017). Thus,
there is need to examine the suggested moderating roles of scrupulosity
and gender on the relationships between general mental issues and prob-
lematic pornography viewing.
The current study
We addressed these considerations by conducting a study examining how
depression, anxiety, and stress may statistically predict the problematic
dimensions suggested in Kor et al.s(2014) framework, while simultan-
eously examining the contribution of scrupulosity and gender. Four
hypotheses guided our analyses: First, consistent with Borgogna and
McDermott (2018), we hypothesized that scrupulosity would emerge as a
strong significant statistical positive predictor across all problematic porn-
ography viewing constructs. Second, consistent with examinations of men-
tal health in relation to problematic pornography viewing (e.g., Kor et al.,
2014; Kraus et al., 2015; Levin et al., 2012), we hypothesized that anxiety,
depression, and stress would modestly and positively statistically predict all
dimensions as well. Third, because men view pornography more often than
women (Albright, 2008; Carroll et al., 2008; Paul, 2009; Price et al., 2016),
we hypothesized that gender would moderate these relationships, such that
the relationship between scrupulosity and mental health as positive statis-
tical predictors of problematic pornography viewing would be significantly
stronger for men. Fourth, as a means of further exploration, and consistent
with research indicating religious populations experience worse mental
health outcomes associating with pornography viewing in general (Grubbs,
Stauner, et al., 2015; Nelson et al., 2010; Patterson & Price, 2012; Perry,
2017; Perry & Whitehead, 2018; Volk, Thomas, Sosin, Jacob, & Moen,
296 N. C. BORGOGNA ET AL.
2016), we also examined how scrupulosity may interact with anxiety,
depression, and stress. Specifically, we hypothesized that an exacerbation
effect would occur at high levels of scrupulosity, with the interactions being
significantly predictive of all dimensions of problematic pornography view-
ing. We further hypothesized that gender would additionally moderate the
interaction effect of scrupulosity, such that the interaction paths would be
significantly stronger for men.
Method
Participants/Procedure
After review board approval, participants were gathered online via a subject
pool located at a university in the southeastern United States, with add-
itional snowball sampling through professional research social media for-
mats (such as the Psychology on the Net web service), and postings on
craigslist and Reddit. Data were gathered from January 2017 to January
2018. The study was advertised as a social survey exploring mental health
and sexuality. Participants gathered through the subject pool were offered
extra-credit, and those participating through the snowball procedure could
optionally enter a raffle for one $100 Visa-gift card. We only included par-
ticipants who completed at least 80% of all measures and passed a random
response check in the survey. Since gender as a binary variable was critical,
16 non-cisgender participants were removed from analyses, yielding a final
sample of 757 (507 women and 250 men). Demographics are available in
Table 1.
Measures
Problematic pornography viewing
We used the Problematic Pornography Use Scale (PPUS; Kor et al., 2014)
as our measure of problematic pornography viewing. The PPUS is a 12-
item measure with four, three-item factors. The factors are: distress and
functional problems (FP, I risked or put in jeopardy a significant relation-
ship, place of employment, educational or career opportunity because of
the use of pornographic materials), excessive use (EU, I spend too much
time being involved in thoughts about pornography), control difficulties
(CD, I feel I cannot stop watching pornography), and use for escape/
avoidance negative emotions (ANE, I watch pornographic materials when
am feeling despondent). Items are scored on a Likert scale (1-never true to
6-almost always true). The four-factor model has been validated across gen-
ders via confirmatory factor analyses, though some research has indicated
questionable reliability for the functional problems scale in women
SEXUAL ADDICTION & COMPULSIVITY 297
(Borgogna, Lathan, & Mitchell, 2018). The measure has further been shown
to have appropriate convergent and construct validity (Kor et al., 2014).
Internal consistencies were adequate on all four subscales: FP a¼.70, EU
a¼.89, CD a¼.88, and ANE a¼.90. Pornography was defined as view-
ing materials that depict sexual activity, organs, and/or experiences for the
purpose of sexual arousal (Kalman, 2008).
Scrupulosity
We used the 12-item Fear of Sinsubscale of the Penn Inventory of
Scrupulosity (PIOS; Abramowitz et al., 2002) as our measure of scrupulos-
ity (I feel guilty about immoral thoughts I have had). Items are scored on
a Likert-Scale (1-never to 4-constantly). The PIOS has demonstrated good
internal consistency and adequate convergent and discriminant validity
(Abramowitz et al., 2002; Olatunji, Abramowitz, Williams, Connolly, &
Table 1. Demographic Information.
Men (n¼250) Women (n¼507)
Race
White or Caucasian 72.40% 65.30%
Black/African American 12.40% 18.30%
Hispanic/Latino(a) 5.20% 5.10%
Asian American/Middle Eastern 6.80% 7.10%
Native American/Pacific Islander 0.40% 1.00%
Other/Multiracial 2.80% 3.20%
Sexual Orientation
Heterosexual 86.00% 83.20%
Homosexual 5.20% 3.20%
Bisexual 5.60% 9.70%
Questioning 1.60% 2.40%
Other Sexual Minority 1.60% 1.60%
Relationship Status
Single 46.80% 37.10%
Casually Dating 8.40% 13.00%
Seriously Dating 30.40% 36.10%
Engaged/Married 11.60% 9.30%
Separated/Divorced 2.00% 2.60%
Widow 0.40% 1.20%
Other 0.40% 0.80%
Highest Level of Education
High School 70.40% 73.60%
Associates/Technical 12.00% 11.80%
Bachelors 12.00% 9.50%
Masters 4.00% 3.90%
Doctorate/Professional 1.60% 1.20%
Religion
Christian 62.00% 72.80%
Agnostic/Atheist 24.00% 15.00%
Hindu 0.80% 0.00%
Muslim 4.80% 1.00%
Jewish 1.20% 1.80%
Buddhist 1.20% 2.40%
Other 6.00% 6.90%
Missing/Did Not Respond 0.00% 0.20%
Mean (SD) Mean (SD)
Age 24.2 (9.9) 24.1 (10.7)
Age of First Pornography Viewing 13.2 (2.4) 15.2 (4.3)
298 N. C. BORGOGNA ET AL.
Lohr, 2007). The Fear of Sin subscale alone has been shown to be an effi-
cient way of measuring scrupulosity (Borgogna & McDermott, 2018;
Olatunji, 2008). Internal consistency for the current sample was excellent: a
¼.93.
General mental health
We used the Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS; Lovibond &
Lovibond, 1995) as our measure of general mental health. The DASS is a
21-item measure with three, seven-item factors: Depression (DASS-D,
Over the past week, I couldnt seem to experience any positive feeling at
all), anxiety (DASS-A, Over the past week, I felt I was close to panic),
and stress (DASS-S, Over the past week, I found it hard to wind down).
Items are presented using a Likert scale (1-did not apply to me at all to 4-
applied to me very much, or most of the time). The DASS has been vali-
dated across genders, races, and cultures, and has demonstrated adequate
construct, convergent, and discriminate validity (Crawford & Henry, 2003;
Norton, 2007). Internal consistencies were excellent on all three subscales:
DASS-D a¼.90, DASS-A a¼.81, and DASS-S a¼.84.
Neuroticism
We used the 8-item Neuroticism scale from the Big Five Inventory (BFI-N;
John, Donahue, & Kentle, 1991) as our measure of neuroticism (I see
myself as someone who worries a lot). Items are presented using a Likert
scale (1-disagree strongly to 5-agree strongly). The BFI has been validated
across genders, races, and cultures via exploratory and confirmatory factor
analyses and has demonstrated adequate convergent and discriminant val-
idity (John, Naumann, & Soto, 2008). Internal consistency was good: a
¼.85.
Primary analysis plan
We used multi-group structural equation modeling (SEM) to examine the
hypothesized relationships between depression, anxiety, stress, and scrupu-
losity on problematic pornography viewing dimensions (i.e., functional
problems, excessive use, control difficulties, and avoidance of negative emo-
tions), while controlling for neuroticism (as it has been shown to be a stat-
istical predictor of general mental health problems (Lahey, 2009) and
problematic pornography viewing tendencies (Egan & Parmar, 2013)).
Figure 1 depicts the basic conceptual model that was examined across gen-
ders. Following best practices for SEM (Kline, 2016), we first tested a meas-
urement model to ensure that all latent variables were adequately
SEXUAL ADDICTION & COMPULSIVITY 299
represented by their manifest indicators. We then examined a structural
model with paths included to test the hypothesized relationships. For evalu-
ating model-fit, we used the following indices and cutoffs (Hu & Bentler,
1999; Kline, 2016): comparative fit index (CFI) and the Tucker-Lewis index
(TLI; values above .90 indicate acceptable fit for both the CFI and TLI), the
root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) with 90% confidence
intervals (CIs; low values of .06 or less and high values less than .10 indi-
cate a good fit), and the standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR;
values of .08 or less indicate a good fit). The chi-square test statistic was
also reported (a non-significant value indicates a good fit to the data); how-
ever, it was interpreted with caution, given its sensitivity to sample size.
Figure 1. Conceptual Model.
Note: Error and disturbance terms are not shown for readability.
300 N. C. BORGOGNA ET AL.
Because the present study also examined the potential moderating effect
of gender, we tested measurement and structural invariance between groups
(Kline, 2016). Three forms of invariance were examined: configural invari-
ance (determining whether the same basic pattern of factor loadings was
evidenced across genders), metric invariance (testing the equivalence of fac-
tor loadings for each latent variable between genders), and direct-effects
invariance (determining whether the direct effects between depression, anx-
iety, stress, neuroticism, and scrupulosity on the problematic pornography
viewing factors were equivalent between genders). Configural and metric
invariance are necessary to ensure that any moderation effects are not due
to underlying differences in the measurement of the constructs between
groups. Direct-effects invariance is the final step of the analysis, and if sig-
nificant differences in the strength of the direct relationships are found,
then moderation is evident (Kline, 2016).
To evaluate measurement invariance in each model, we used a scaled
chi-square difference test in which a more parsimonious model was tested
against a less parsimonious model. A non-significant chi-square difference
provides support for invariance (Kline, 2016). However, the chi-square dif-
ference test is sensitive to sample size; thus, even small changes in the chi-
square can be statistically significant (Kline, 2016). Therefore, we also used
two alternative approaches to measurement invariance testing: examining
the change in CFI (Cheung & Rensvold, 2002) and calculating the bias-cor-
rected bootstrapped confidence intervals (CIs) of the difference between
groups on parameters of interest (Cheung & Lau, 2012). A change of CFI
less than or equal to .01 (Cheung & Rensvold, 2002) and a CI containing
zero for the between-groups difference on a particular unstandardized par-
ameter suggest invariance (Cheung & Lau, 2012).
Because we also examined the potential interaction effect of scrupulosity
with anxiety, depression, and stress, we constructed several latent variable
interaction models. Our interactions were created using the latent moder-
ated structural equations method suggested by Klein and Moosbrugger
(2000). In these models, mental health variables were examined in predic-
tion of dimensions of problematic pornography viewing at high (1 SD
above the mean) and low (1 SD below the mean) levels of scrupulosity.
Results
Preliminary analyses
Of the 757 participants, few had missing responses (no more than four on
any item). A small number of participants were identified as univariate out-
liers (<2.8% on the PPUS scales). Some (<4.1%) multivariate outliers
were also identified by examining the Mahalanobis distances in the total
SEXUAL ADDICTION & COMPULSIVITY 301
sample. Most of the predictor variables were distributed normally, with
exception to depression (positively skewed). Additionally, each of the PPUS
subscales showed a positive skew. Thus, we used a maximum likelihood
estimator with robust standard errors in our primary analyses to fit the
model, while taking into account potential normality violations. Table 2
displays the bivariate correlations, means, and standard deviations across
men and women.
Primary analysis
After our preliminary analyses, we tested the specified SEM measurement
and structural models. These analyses were conducted in Mplus version 8
(Muth
en & Muth
en, 2017). Item parcels were generated for the DASS-D,
DASS-A, DASS-S, PIOS, and BFI-N scales. Our parceling procedure
involved conducting an exploratory factor analysis for items in each scale
fitted to a one-factor solution. Items were then assigned to three parcels in
an iterative fashion to ensure that all loadings were balanced (Russell,
Kahn, Spoth, & Altmaier, 1998). Since each of the PPUS subscales consists
of three items, we used the individual items to form the PPUS subscale
latent variables. All analyses (except bootstraps) used a maximum likeli-
hood estimator with robust standard errors.
Measurement model
The measurement model provided a good fit for men, (n¼250) v
2
(288) ¼
441.89, p<.001, CFI ¼.96, TLI ¼.95, RMSEA ¼.046 (90% CI ¼.037,
.055), and SRMR ¼.046; and an acceptable fit for women, (n¼507) v
2
(288) ¼655.49, p<.001, CFI ¼.94, TLI ¼.93, RMSEA ¼.05 (90% CI ¼
Table 2. Correlations, Means, and Standard Deviations.
Men Women
Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 MSDMSD
1. PPUS-FP - .51 .48 .43 .24 .25 .25 .39 .23 1.71 .99 1.27 .59
2. PPUS-EU .52 - .72 .59 .16-.11.24 .31.21 2.01 1.16 1.41 .88
3. PPUS-CD .49 .66 - .58 .13 .15.25 .36 .161.28 1.36 1.27 .74
4. PPUS-ANE .39 .69 .63 - .37 .23 .32 .36 .25 2.37 1.43 1.59 1.16
5. DASS-D .19 .17 .15 .31 - .62 .70 .26 -.15 1.81 .76 1.74 .74
6. DASS-A .14 .21 .19 .24 .64 - -.23 .29 .58 1.63 .59 1.74 .62
7. DASS-S .15 .20 .13 .28 .73 .72 - .40 .59 1.88 .64 2.02 .65
8. PIOS .20 .28 .25 .26 .29 .31 .35 - .29 2.07 .85 1.98 1.00
9. BFI-N .04 .11.08 .22 .55 .51 .62 .29 - 2.82 .88 3.25 .85
Note: Men above the midline, women below. PPUS-FP ¼Problematic Pornography Use Scale - Functional
Problems, PPUS-EU ¼Excessive Use, PPUS-CD ¼Control Difficulties, PPUS-ANE ¼Avoidance of Negative
Emotions, Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale - D ¼Depression, DASS-A ¼Anxiety, DASS-S ¼Stress,
PIOS ¼Penn Inventory of Scrupulosity, BFI-N ¼Big Five Inventory - Neuroticism.
p<.05.
p<.01.
p<.001.
302 N. C. BORGOGNA ET AL.
.045, .055), and SRMR ¼.043. A configural invariance model, in which all
paths were freely estimated between men and women, provided an accept-
able fit overall, v
2
(576) ¼1117.88, p<.001, CFI ¼.95, TLI ¼.94,
RMSEA ¼.05 (90% CI ¼.045, .054), and SRMR ¼.044. Thus, the meas-
urement model appeared to be capturing the same general pattern of factor
loadings between men and women.
We then tested a metric invariance measurement model by constraining
the factor loadings to be equal across genders. The constrained model pro-
vided acceptable fit, v
2
(594) ¼1166.50, p<.001, CFI ¼.94, TLI ¼.93,
RMSEA ¼.05 (90% CI ¼.046, .055), and SRMR ¼.051. The scaled chi-
square difference test indicated that the metric invariance model was sig-
nificantly worse than the configural model, v
2
(18) ¼44.73, p<.001; how-
ever, the change in CFI was within appropriate limits (DCFI ¼-.003).
Furthermore, bias-corrected bootstrap confidence intervals of the differen-
ces between factor loadings (Cheung & Lau, 2012) were all non-significant,
with exception to the items on the excessive use scale. Supplementary
Table 1 displays the factor loadings in the measurement model.
Structural model
We then examined a structural model with paths specified between men and
women. The configural invariance structural model provided acceptable fit,
v
2
(612) ¼1210.52, p<.001, CFI ¼.94, TLI ¼.93, RMSEA ¼.051 (90%
CI ¼.047, .055), and SRMR ¼.053. Bias-corrected bootstrap samples
(n¼1000) were then used to estimate the confidence intervals of each path
from depression, anxiety, stress, and scrupulosity to the problematic pornog-
raphy viewing variables. Table 3 displays the unstandardized and standar-
dized coefficients for each path and the 95% confidence intervals. Results
indicated several significant paths for both men and women. Specifically,
scrupulosity significantly predicted functional problems, excessive use, con-
trol difficulties, and avoidance of negative emotions, while depression signifi-
cantly predicted avoidance of negative emotions in the sample of men. For
women, nearly the same relationships were found across scales, with scrupu-
losity predicting functional problems, excessive use, control difficulties, and
avoidance of negative emotions, and depression significantly predicting func-
tional problems and avoidance of negative emotions. All other relationships
between mental health constructs and problematic pornography viewing
dimensions were non-significant (see Table 3 for specific coefficients).
Moderation analyses
To examine the potential moderation effect of gender in the structural
model, we constrained the direct effects between each latent variable to be
SEXUAL ADDICTION & COMPULSIVITY 303
equalacrossgroups(i.e.,directeffectsinvariance).Thescaledchi-square
difference test indicated that the direct effect invariance model was a sig-
nificantly worse fit compared to a model in which direct effects were
freely estimated, v
2
(18) ¼41,46, p<.001, though the CFI did not
change. The bootstrap procedure only suggested significant differences on
the relationship between scrupulosity and functional problems, with the
relationship being significantly stronger for men than women. For men,
the structural model accounted for 27% of the variance for functional
problems,15%ofthevarianceforexcessiveuse,16%ofthevariancefor
control difficulties, and 25% of the variance for avoidance of negative
emotions. For women, the structural model accounted for 13% of the
variance for functional problems, 12% of the variance for excessive use,
12% of the variance for control difficulties, and 15% of the variance for
avoidance of negative emotions.
Table 3. Standardized and Unstandardized Structural Model Results.
Gender
Predictor
Variables !
Criterion
Variables
Direct
Effect BSE
Standardized
Direct Effect b95% CI
Men Scrupulosity !FP .53 .14 .46 (.244, .792)
Women Scrupulosity !FP .13 .05 .21(.042, .251)
Men Scrupulosity !EU .42 .14 .30 (.133, .549)
Women Scrupulosity !EU .28 .07 .26 (.147, .447)
Men Scrupulosity !CD .48 .14 .32 (.198, .744)
Women Scrupulosity !CD .22 .06 .26 (.100, .341)
Men Scrupulosity !ANE .50 .15 .31 (.226, .811)
Women Scrupulosity !ANE .25 .09 .19 (.092, .432)
Men Depression !FP .16 .15 .12 (-.082, .478)
Women Depression !FP .16.08 .23(.022, .386)
Men Depression !EU -.11 .21 -.07 (-.435, .225)
Women Depression !EU .04 .16 .03 (-.305, .383)
Men Depression !CD -.22 .21 -.12 (-.645, .239)
Women Depression !CD .18 .14 .18 (-.074, .488)
Men Depression !ANE .69 .24 .36 (.188, 1.159)
Women Depression !ANE .52 .20 .33(.147, .927)
Men Anxiety !FP .15 .42 .07 (-.466, .961)
Women Anxiety !FP -.03 .18 -.03 (-.501, .301)
Men Anxiety !EU -.02 .45 -.01 (-.601, .690)
Women Anxiety !EU .20 .34 .11 (-.557, .958)
Men Anxiety !CD -.28 .45 -.10 (-1.136, .615)
Women Anxiety !CD .46 .34 .34 (-.183, 1.128)
Men Anxiety !ANE -.67 .54 -.23 (-1.719, .187)
Women Anxiety !ANE .15 .43 .07 (-.679, .999)
Men Stress !FP -.15 .32 -.09 (-.848, .307)
Women Stress !FP .15 .23 .18 (-.303, .731)
Men Stress !EU .28 .31 .15 (-.317, .678)
Women Stress !EU .12 .44 .08 (-.725, 1.153)
Men Stress !CD .66 .37 .33 (-.032, 1.454)
Women Stress !CD -.40 .41 -.36 (-1.323, .287)
Men Stress !ANE .38 .41 .17 (-.386, 1.219)
Women Stress !ANE -.22 .54 -.12 (-1.300, .788)
FP ¼Functional Problems, EU ¼Excessive Use, CD ¼Control Difficulties, ANE ¼Avoidance of Negative Emotions.
Contact first author for non-significant neuroticism path coefficients.
p<.05.
p<.01.
p<.001.
304 N. C. BORGOGNA ET AL.
Interaction analyses
We then examined interactions between scrupulosity and depression, anx-
iety, and stress for each dimension of problematic pornography viewing
(see Table 4 for all significant interactions, with a value approaching sig-
nificance at p<0.05 (p¼0.051) also included). We chose to examine gen-
der samples separately since Mplus is currently unable to consider
categorical grouping variables within latent variable interaction models. For
men, a significant interaction occurred between depression and scrupulosity
on excessive use, control difficulties, and avoidance of negative emotions.
Figure 2 demonstrates these relationships. For men, anxiety also interacted
with scrupulosity to statistically predict control difficulties (see Figure 2).
Interactions also occurred between stress and problematic pornography
viewing across excessive use, control difficulties, and avoidance of negative
emotions (see Table 4 and Figure 3). In each case, low levels of scrupulosity
buffered the relationship between mental health problems and problematic
pornography use; however, the buffering effect is non-significant for those
high in scrupulosity, and becomes somewhat exacerbating for those strug-
gling with depression who are also high in scrupulosity (see Figure 2).
Unlike men, no interactions occurred between depression or stress with
scrupulosity across any of the dimensions of problematic pornography use
in the sample of women. However, anxiety and scrupulosity interacted with
an exacerbation effect on excessive use in women (see Figure 3). For men,
the depression X scrupulosity interaction model increased variance
accounted for to 31% for functional problems, 22% for excessive use, 22%
of the variance for control difficulties, and 32% of the variance for avoid-
ance of negative emotions. The anxiety X scrupulosity interaction model
increased variance accounted for to 31% for functional problems, 20% for
excessive use, 22% for control difficulties, and 31% for avoidance of nega-
tive emotions. The stress X scrupulosity interaction model increased
Table 4. Latent Variable Interactions Reaching or Approaching Significance at p <0.05.
Gender Interaction !
Criterion
Variables
Direct
Effect BSE
Standardized
Direct Effect bpValue
Men SCRUPXDEP !EU -.27 .11 -.17p¼.018
Men SCRUPXDEP !CD -.32 .16 -.14p¼.040
Men SCRUPXDEP !ANE -.44 .19 -.19p¼.021
Men SCRUPXANX !CD -.41 .21 -.13 p¼.051
Men SCRUPXSTRESS !EU -.33 .11 -.12 p¼.003
Men SCRUPXSTRESS !CD -.35 .17 -.18p¼.035
Men SCRUPXSTRESS !ANE -.48 .19 -.18 p¼.010
Women SCRUPXANX !EU .36 .18 .16p¼.041
Note: FP ¼Functional Problems, EU ¼Excessive Use, CD ¼Control Difficulties, ANE ¼Avoidance of Negative
Emotions, SCRUPXDEP ¼Scrupulosity X Depression Interaction, SCRUPXANX ¼Scrupulosity X Anxiety
Interaction, SCRUPXSTRESS ¼Scrupulosity X Stress Interaction. Contact first author for other non-significant
interaction coefficients.
p<.05.
p<.01.
p<.001.
SEXUAL ADDICTION & COMPULSIVITY 305
variance accounted for to 33% for functional problems, 23% for excessive
use, 21% for control difficulties, and 34% for avoidance of negative emo-
tions. For women, the depression X scrupulosity and anxiety X scrupulosity
interaction models did not account for any additional variance. The stress
X scrupulosity interaction model increased variance accounted for to 14%
Figure 2. Unstandardized simple slope coefficients are given for high (1 SD above) and low (1
SD below) levels of scrupulosity.
Figure 3. Unstandardized simple slope coefficients are given for high (1 SD above) and low (1
SD below) levels of scrupulosity.
306 N. C. BORGOGNA ET AL.
for functional problems but did not account for additional variance across
the other problematic pornography viewing dimensions.
Discussion
This study advanced prior work by examining how scrupulosity and gender
moderate the relationships across a range of mental health concerns
(depression, anxiety, and stress) and multiple dimensions of problematic
pornography viewing (functional problems, excessive use, control difficul-
ties, and avoidance of negative emotions). Broadly, results were consistent
with our hypotheses, with a few notable exceptions. Consistent fully with
our first hypothesis, scrupulosity emerged as a strong significant positive
statistical predictor across all measures of problematic pornography viewing
in both men and women. These results are consistent with Borgogna and
McDermott (2018), and further suggest scrupulosity as a construct within
the broader domain of religiosity, that may be relevant to the construct,
impact and potential treatment of problematic pornography use.
Partially consistent with our second hypothesis, depression was a strong
significant positive statistical predictor of pornography use to avoid nega-
tive emotions across men and women. This is logical, as those using porn-
ography in a dysfunctional manner to escape unpleasant emotions must
first be experiencing unpleasant emotions. Furthermore, depression was a
significant moderate positive statistical predictor of functional problems
only in the sample of women. This could be indicative of the relationship
problems some women experience as the result of pornography use with a
partner. Recent findings have indicated that women who hold traditionally
masculine conceptualizations of what men should be may be more likely to
experience functional problems associated with their pornography use
(Borgogna, McDermott, Browning, Beach, & Aita, 2018). However, these
results could also be related to findings that indicate viewing pornography
in relationships is associated with more positive outcomes for men than for
women (Poulsen et al., 2013). This would be especially relevant for scrupu-
lous women who may be opposed to pornography use but view it at their
partners behest. Future qualitative studies examining the context in which
functional problems develop for both men and women and how they are
associated with depression and scrupulosity would be beneficial for under-
standing these findings further. Interestingly, stress, anxiety, and neuroti-
cism were not significant statistical predictors of any dimensions of
problematic pornography viewing across men or women within the full
model, despite being significantly related to many of the variables at the
bivariate level.
SEXUAL ADDICTION & COMPULSIVITY 307
Inconsistent with our third hypothesis, the gender moderation analyses
(via direct effects invariance) were broadly non-significant with one excep-
tion: the relationship between scrupulosity and functional problems was
moderated by gender, with men reporting a significantly stronger relation-
ship. This is consistent with literature indicating that men may be more at-
risk than women for having pornography effect their relationships in a
negative way (e.g., Willoughby, Carroll, Busby, & Brown, 2016; Wright,
Bridges, et al., 2018; Zitzman & Butler, 2009), have pornography jeopardize
their employment (Kor et al., 2014; Maltz & Maltz, 2009), or jeopardize
their respect (such as work examining the role of pornography use among
clergy; Ahmad et al., 2015; Ferree, 2002). More importantly, these findings
suggest a stronger need to examine gender-related differences across mental
health issues and problematic pornography viewing in future studies, espe-
cially as many of the studies suggesting a relationship between mental
health issues and pornography viewing have used male-exclusive samples
(see Egan & Parmar, 2013; Kraus et al., 2015; Levin et al., 2012; Nelson
et al., 2010; Twohig et al., 2009 for examples).
The interaction analyses were partially consistent with our fourth
hypothesis. For men, high scrupulosity was related to more problematic
pornography viewing compared to low scrupulosity across depression, anx-
iety, and stress. However, only in a few cases was there an exacerbation
effect in the sample of men. Rather, low scrupulosity acted as a buffer
across most of the interactions in the sample. Additionally, for most of the
significant interactions, high levels of mental health concerns lowered prob-
lematic pornography viewing in highly scrupulous men (see Figures 2 and
3), while increasing problematic pornography viewing for those low in
scrupulosity.
The interaction analyses were largely non-significant across the sample of
women. However, consistent with our fourth hypothesis, a single inter-
action occurred with anxiety and scrupulosity exacerbating problems
related to perceptions of excessive pornography use. Interestingly, anxiety
was non-significant in the direct effects model for women, suggesting scru-
pulosity as a key contributor to the excessive use problems. This is the first
study to identify such a nuanced finding and suggests that women who are
highly anxious and scrupulous may perceive their pornography use as
excessive. Despite neuroticism being controlled, this may indicate add-
itional personality dimensions worth examining in future studies. For
instance, scrupulosity has been shown to partially mediate the relationship
between perfectionism and mental health concerns (Allen & Wang, 2014).
Thus, researchers may want to consider perfectionism as a variable in
future examinations, given that excessive use problems may be based on
subjective perceptions that behaviors are excessive or interfering (see
308 N. C. BORGOGNA ET AL.
discussions on perceived addiction; e.g., Brand et al., 2018 and Grubbs,
Stauner, et al., 2015).
Limitations
While this study contributes novel findings, it is not without limitations.
Namely, the cross-sectional nature of the data precludes causal inferences.
Furthermore, we only explored general mental health problems. Past
research has implicated severe mental health issues, such as bipolar dis-
order, as being associated with problematic internet use (W
olfling, Beutel,
Dreier, & M
uller, 2015). Thus, a potential area of further research would
be examining how problematic pornography use relates too other (and pos-
sibly more severe) psychopathologies. The self-report nature of this study
places the data at risk of participant biases. The study also used a relatively
liberal threshold for setting statistical significance, as multiple comparisons
were not controlled. The generalizability of these results is also limited to a
primarily white population from the United States. While the majority of
the extant research has been conducted on American and European sam-
ples, recent data have indicated that black Americans may view more porn-
ography than white Americans, with religiosity only moderating the
relationship for white Americans (Perry & Schleifer, 2019). Thus, further
research across different ethnic and cultural groups appears to be an
important area of study.
The interaction between scrupulosity and anxiety on control difficulties
in men should also be examined carefully in future studies. The standard
error of .21 is slightly larger than half the unstandardized coefficient of
-.41. Indeed, the p-value of .051 is technically outside of the domain for
assigning statistical significance by traditional methods. Thus, we emphasize
a need for replication of this interaction, as well as our findings
more broadly.
Additionally, future studies should consider examining these variables
with alternative measures of problematic pornography use. Specifically, the
problematic pornography consumption scale (PPCS; B}
othe et al., 2018) has
recently been published which contains additional measurements for toler-
ance and withdrawal factors. This scale was not available when the current
study began data collection, but has strong theoretical and psychometric
support. Additional nuanced measures such as compulsive pornography
consumption scale (CPC; Noor, Rosser, & Erickson, 2014) and the cyber
pornography use inventory (CPUI; Grubbs, Sessoms, Wheeler, & Volk,
2010) may also provide nuanced information about relationship between
problematic pornography use, scrupulosity, and mental health problems,
and should thus be considered in future studies.
SEXUAL ADDICTION & COMPULSIVITY 309
Implications
The results provide considerable implications for researchers and clinicians.
For researchers, these findings highlight the importance of considering
multiple potentially related constructs when examining variables related to
problematic pornography use. For instance, several researchers report sig-
nificant bivariate relationships between problematic pornography viewing
and anxiety (e.g., Kor et al., 2014). However, in the current study, this rela-
tionship disappears when accounting for the role of depression, stress,
neuroticism, and scrupulosity in a structural model. Interestingly, anxiety
then reemerges as a significant statistical predictor but only in interaction
with scrupulosity for control difficulties in men and excessive use problems
in women. Similarly, unlike Egan and Parmar (2013), neuroticism was not
related to any dimensions of problematic pornography use in our analyses
for men or women when controlling for the additional roles of mental
health and scrupulosity, despite being significantly related at the bivariate
level. Thus, continued nuanced approaches utilizing sophisticated analyses
while controlling for multiple co-varying factors across studies of problem-
atic pornography use are recommended.
For clinicians, these findings contribute to a growing literature (e.g.,
Bradley et al., 2016; Grubbs, Stauner, et al., 2015; Grubbs et al., 2018;
Perry, 2017; Wilt et al., 2016) implicating religiosity, and particularly scru-
pulosity (Borgogna & McDermott, 2018; Grubbs et al., 2018; Short et al.,
2015), as a variable relevant to the problematic pornography use. Multiple
nationwide analyses have indicated that internet searches for pornographic
material are highest in regions of the United States that are more religious
and traditionally conservative (Edelman, 2009; Macinnis & Hodson, 2015;
Whitehead & Perry, 2018). Thus, exploring religious factors in the context
of treatment of problematic pornography use should be considered an inte-
gral part of assessment and conceptualization (Kraus and Sweeney, 2018).
Clinicians may consider investigating dysfunctional methods clients may
have used to control their pornography viewing. For instance, Borgogna
and McDermott (2018) suggest avoidant-based mechanisms, such as
thought suppression techniques (as examined in scrupulous populations),
may serve to increase pornography-related thoughts. Therefore, approaches
that emphasize mindful awareness techniques paired with values-based
behavioral goals may provide more meaningful results (Crosby &
Twohig, 2016; Twohig & Crosby, 2010).
Clinicians should also assess what particularly about the clients pornog-
raphy use is problematic. Measures such as the PPUS (Kor et al., 2014),
PPCS (B}
othe, T
oth-Kir
aly, Zsila, et al., 2018), CPUI (Grubbs et al., 2010);
and CPC (Noor et al., 2014) can be helpful in this process. Importantly,
many individuals in treatment may refer to their pornography use as
310 N. C. BORGOGNA ET AL.
addictive;however, clinicians should not assume and/or treat the use as a
traditional addiction without proper assessment. Indeed, there is a consid-
erable debate as to whether problematic pornography viewing should be
conceptualized as an addiction (B}
othe, T
oth-Kir
aly, Potenza, et al., 2018;
Kowalewska et al., 2018; Ley, Prause, & Finn, 2014; Stark, Klucken,
Potenza, Brand, & Strahler, 2018; Potenza et al., 2017; Prause et al., 2017).
Moreover, formal categorization of compulsive/addictive pornography use
is not included in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2013).
Grubbs and colleagues (2015; 2018) have reported that religious individ-
uals may perceive their viewing as addictive, and may describe their prob-
lems using such terminology, even if their behavior may not follow an
addictive pattern (i.e., with features such as withdrawal and tolerance).
Thus, some individuals in treatment may not be viewing pornography very
frequently, yet may find it highly distressing when it occurs. Moreover,
exploring values and perceptions on a case-by-case basis may be beneficial
in establishing achievable goals for therapeutic work. Clinical trials investi-
gating the treatment of problematic pornography viewing (Crosby &
Twohig, 2016; Twohig & Crosby, 2010) are consistent with these recom-
mendations, and may be used as additional sources for recommendations
and guides for therapists.
Conclusion
In conclusion, problematic pornography viewing is likely going to continue
being a clinical concern (Brand et al., 2018; Cooper, Delmonico, & Burg,
2000; Grubbs & Perry, 2018; Sniewski et al., 2018). Our findings extend
prior research implicating scrupulosity as a primary factor relevant to prob-
lematic pornography use (Borgogna & McDermott, 2018). We extended
prior work by examining the potential contributions of scrupulosity, anx-
iety, depression, and stress, while also controlling for neuroticism, as pre-
dictors of functional problems related to pornography viewing, perceived
excessive pornography use and control difficulties, as well as the use of
pornography to avoid uncomfortable emotions. Scrupulosity was the stron-
gest factor in relation to all dimensions of problematic pornography use
across genders. Multiple interactions were noted in the sample of men with
a single interaction in the sample of women between scrupulosity and men-
tal health constructs (see figures 2 and 3). Low scrupulosity broadly buf-
fered the relationship between mental health issues and problematic
pornography viewing in men, though high scrupulosity exacerbated exces-
sive use problems in anxious women. Our models broadly accounted for a
moderate portion of the variance across all problematic pornography
SEXUAL ADDICTION & COMPULSIVITY 311
viewing dimensions for both men and women. We recommend examining
scrupulosity as a potentially relevant factor in clinical interventions
designed to reduce problems associated with pornography use. Given the
novelty of our findings, as well as the high degree of nuance, we addition-
ally recommend continued research across these variables, with an
emphasis on replication.
The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest. The data used
for this study has not been presented elsewhere.
Author Note
Nicholas C. Borgogna and Jessica Duncan: Department of Psychology, University of South
Alabama; Ryon C. McDermott: Counseling and Instructional Sciences, University of South
Alabama. We thank two additional members of the Culture and Individual Differences
(CID) research team within the undergraduate Psychology program at the University of
South Alabama for their help collecting data: Katelyn Baker and Davidson Meador.
Correspondence may be directed to Nicholas C. Borgogna, Department of Psychology, 75 S.
University Blvd., Mobile, AL 36608, nicholascborgogna@gmail.com.
ORCID
Nicholas C. Borgogna http://orcid.org/0000-0002-5085-3656
References
Abramowitz, J. S., Huppert, J. D., Cohen, A. B., Tolin, D. F., & Cahill, S. P. (2002).
Religious obsessions and compulsions in a non-clinical sample: The Penn Inventory of
Scrupulosity (PIOS). Behaviour Research and Therapy,40(7), 825838. doi:10.1016/
S0005-7967(01)00070-5
Ahmad, Z. S., Thoburn, J., Perry, K. L., Mcbrearty, M., Olson, S., & Gunn, G. (2015).
Prevalence rates of online sexual addiction among Christian clergy. Sexual Addiction &
Compulsivity,22, 344356. doi:10.1080/10720162.2015.1082079
Albright, J. M. (2008). Sex in America online: an exploration of sex, marital status, and sex-
ual identity in internet sex seeking and its impacts. Journal of Sex Research,45(2),
175186. doi:10.1080/00224490801987481
Allen, G. E. K., & Wang, K. T. (2014). Examining religious commitment, perfectionism,
scrupulosity, and well-being among LDS individuals. Psychology of Religion and
Spirituality,6(3), 257264. doi:10.1037/a0035197
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders, 5th Edition: DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
Anand, K. S., & Dhikav, V. (2012). Headaches induced by pornography use. Archives of
Sexual Behavior,41(5), 1077. doi:10.1007/s10508-012-9988-5
Bergner, R. M., & Bridges, A. J. (2002). The significance of heavy pornography involvement
for romantic partners: Research and clinical implications. Journal of Sex & Marital
Therapy,28, 193206. doi:10.1080/009262302760328235
312 N. C. BORGOGNA ET AL.
Borgogna, N. C., Lathan, E. C., & Mitchell, A. L. (2018). Is womens problematic pornog-
raphy viewing related to body image or relationship satisfaction? Sexual Addiction &
Compulsivity,25(4), 345366. doi:10.1080/10720162.2018.1532360
Borgogna, N. C., & McDermott, R. C. (2018). The role of gender, experiential avoidance,
and scrupulosity in problematic pornography viewing: A moderated-mediation model.
Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity,25(4), 319344. doi:10.1080/10720162.2018.1503123
Borgogna, N. C., McDermott, R. C., Browning, B. R., Beach, J. D., & Aita, S. L. (2018).
How does traditional masculinity relate to men and womens problematic pornography
viewing? Sex Roles. doi:10.1007/s11199-018-0967-8
B}
othe, B., T
oth-Kir
aly, I., Potenza, M. N., Griffiths, M. D., Orosz, G., & Demetrovics, Z.
(2018). Revisiting the role of impulsivity and compulsivity in problematic sexual behav-
iors. Journal of Sex Research,1,114. doi:10.1080/00224499.2018.1480744
B}
othe, B., T
oth-Kir
aly, I., Zsila,
A., Griffiths, M. D., Demetrovics, Z., & Orosz, G. (2018).
The development of the Problematic Pornography Consumption Scale (PPCS). Journal of
Sex Research,55(3), 395406. doi:10.1080/00224499.2017.1291798
Bradley, D. F., Grubbs, J. B., Uzdavines, A., Exline, J. J., & Pargament, K. I. (2016).
Perceived addiction to internet pornography among religious believers and nonbelievers.
Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity,23, 225243. doi:10.1080/10720162.2016.1162237
Brand, M., Antons, S., Wegmann, E., & Potenza, M. N. (2018). Theoretical assumptions on
pornography problems due to moral incongruence and mechanisms of addictive or com-
pulsive use of pornography: Are the two Conditionsas theoretically distinct as sug-
gested? Archives of Sexual Behavior. doi:10.1007/s10508-018-1293-5
Bridges, A. J., Bergner, R. M., & Hesson-McInnis, M. (2003). Romantic partnersuse of
pornography: its significance for women. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy,29(1), 114.
doi:10.1080/00926230390154790
Butler, M. H., Pereyra, S. A., Draper, T. W., Leonhardt, N. D., & Skinner, K. B. (2018).
Pornography use and loneliness: A bidirectional recursive model and pilot investigation.
Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy,44(2), 127137. doi:10.1080/0092623X.2017.1321601
Carroll, J. S., Padilla-Walker, L. M., Nelson, L. J., Olson, C. D., Barry, C. M., & Madsen,
S. D. (2008). Generation XXX pornography acceptance and use among emerging adults.
Journal of Adolescent Research,23(1), 630. doi:10.1177/0743558407306348
Carvalheira, A., Traeen, B., & Stulhofer, A. (2015). Masturbation and pornography use
among coupled heterosexual men with decreased sexual desire: How many roles of mas-
turbation?. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy,41(6), 626635. doi:10.1080/
0092623X.2014.958790
Cheung, G. W., & Lau, R. S. (2012). A direct comparison approach for testing measure-
ment invariance. Organizational Research Methods,15(2), 167198. doi:10.1177/
1094428111421987
Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing
measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling,9(2), 233255. doi:10.1207/
S15328007SEM0902_5
Cooper, A., Delmonico, D. L., & Burg, R. (2000). Cybersex users, abusers, and compulsives:
New findings and implications. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity,7,529. doi:10.1080/
10720160008400205
Crawford, J. R., & Henry, J. D. (2003). The Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS):
Normative data and latent structure in a large non-clinical sample. British Journal of
Clinical Psychology,42(2), 111131. doi:10.1348/014466503321903544
SEXUAL ADDICTION & COMPULSIVITY 313
Crosby, J. M., & Twohig, M. P. (2016). Acceptance and commitment therapy for problem-
atic internet pornography use: A randomized trial. Behavior Therapy,47(3), 355366.
doi:10.1016/j.beth.2016.02.001
DOrlando, F. (2011). The demand for pornography. Journal of Happiness Studies,12,
5175. doi:10.1007/s10902-009-9175-0
Daspe, M., Vaillancourt-Morel, M., Lussier, Y., Sabourin, S., & Ferron, A. (2018). When
pornography use feels out of control: The moderation effect of relationship and sexual
satisfaction. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy,44, 343353. doi:10.1080/
0092623X.2017.1405301
Edelman, B. (2009). Red light states: Who buys online adult entertainment?. Journal of
Economic Perspectives,23(1), 209220. doi:10.1257/jep.23.1.209
Egan, V., & Parmar, R. (2013). Dirty habits? Online pornography use, personality, obses-
sionality, and compulsivity. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy,39, 394409. doi:10.1080/
0092623X.2012.710182
Ferree, M. C. (2002). Sexual addiction and co-addiction: Experiences among women of
faith. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity,9, 285292. doi:10.1080/10720160216048
Grubbs, J. B., Exline, J. J., Pargament, K. I., Hook, J. N., & Carlisle, R. D. (2015).
Transgression as addiction: Religiosity and moral disapproval as predictors of perceived
addiction to pornography. Archives of Sexual Behavior,44(1), 125136. doi:10.1007/
s10508-013-0257-z
Grubbs, J. B., Exline, J. J., Pargament, K. I., Volk, F., & Lindberg, M. J. (2017). Internet
pornography use, perceived addiction, and religious/spiritual struggles. Archives of Sexual
Behavior,46(6), 17331745. doi:10.1007/s10508-016-0772-9
Grubbs, J. B., & Perry, S. L. (2018). Moral Incongruence and Pornography Use: A Critical
Review and Integration. Journal of Sex Research,1,19. doi:10.1080/
00224499.2018.1427204
Grubbs, J. B., Perry, S. L., Wilt, J. A., & Reid, R. C. (2018). Pornography problems due to
moral incongruence: An integrative model with a systematic review and meta-analysis.
Archives of Sexual Behavior,1, 1. doi:10.1007/s10508-018-1248-x
Grubbs, J. B., Sessoms, J., Wheeler, D. M., & Volk, F. (2010). The Cyber-Pornography Use
Inventory: The development of a new assessment instrument. Sexual Addiction &
Compulsivity,17, 106126. doi:10.1080/10720161003776166
Grubbs, J. B., Stauner, N., Exline, J. J., Pargament, K. I., & Lindberg, M. J. (2015).
Perceived addiction to internet pornography and psychological distress: Examining rela-
tionships concurrently and over time. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors,29(4),
10561067. doi:10.1037/adb0000114
Grubbs, J. B., Volk, F., Exline, J. J., & Pargament, K. I. (2015). Internet pornography use:
Perceived addiction, psychological distress, and the validation of a brief measure. Journal
of Sex & Marital Therapy,41,83106. doi:10.1080/0092623X.2013.842192
Grubbs, J. B., Wilt, J. A., Exline, J. J., Pargament, K. I., & Kraus, S. W. (2018). Moral disap-
proval and perceived addiction to internet pornography: a longitudinal examination.
Addiction,113(3), 496506. doi:10.1111/add.14007
Hardy, S. A., Steelman, M. A., Coyne, S. M., & Ridge, R. D. (2013). Adolescent religious-
ness as a protective factor against pornography use. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology,34(3), 131139. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2012.12.002
Harper, C., & Hodgins, D. C. (2016). Examining correlates of problematic internet pornog-
raphy use among university students. Journal of Behavioral Addictions,5(2), 179191.
doi:10.1556/2006.5.2016.022
314 N. C. BORGOGNA ET AL.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure ana-
lysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling,6(1),
155. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118
John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The Big Five Inventory: Versions 4a
and 54. Berkley, CA: University of California, Berkley, Institute of Personality and Social
Research.
John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative Big-
Five Trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. In O. P. John, R. W.
Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp.
114158). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Kalman, T. (2008). Clinical encounters with internet pornography. The Journal of the
American Academy of Psychoanalysis and Dynamic Psychiatry,36(4), 593618. doi:
10.1521/jaap.2008.36.4.593
Kasper, T. E., Short, M. B., & Milam, A. C. (2015). Narcissism and internet pornography
use. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy,41, 481486. doi:10.1080/0092623X.2014.931313
Klein, A., & Moosbrugger, H. (2000). Maximum likelihood estimation of latent interaction
effects with the LMS method. Psychometrika,65(4), 457474. doi:10.1007/BF02296338
Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (4th ed.). New
York, NY: Guilford Press.
Kor, A., Zilcha-Mano, S., Fogel, Y. A., Mikulincer, M., Reid, R. C., & Potenza, M. N.
(2014). Psychometric development of the Problematic Pornography Use Scale. Addictive
Behaviors,39(5), 861868. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.01.027
Kowalewska, E., Grubbs, J. B., Potenza, M. N., Gola, M., Draps, M., & Kraus, S. W. (2018).
Neurocognitive mechanisms in compulsive sexual behavior disorder. Current Sexual
Health Reports,10(4), 255264. doi:10.1007/s11930-018-0176-z
Kraus, S. W., Potenza, M. N., Martino, S., & Grant, J. E. (2015). Examining the psychomet-
ric properties of the Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale in a sample of compulsive
pornography users. Comprehensive Psychiatry,59, 117122. doi:10.1016/
j.comppsych.2015.02.007
Kraus, S. W., & Sweeney, P. J. (2018). Hitting the target: Considerations for differential
diagnosis when treating individuals for problematic use of pornography. Archives of
Sexual Behavior, doi:10.1007/s10508-018-1301-9.
Lahey, B. B. (2009). Public health significance of neuroticism. American Psychologist,64(4),
241256. doi:10.1037/a0015309
Leonhardt, N. D., Willoughby, B. J., & Young-Petersen, B. (2017). Damaged goods:
Perception of pornography addiction as a mediator between religiosity and relationship
anxiety surrounding pornography use. Journal of Sex Research,1,112. doi:10.1080/
00224499.2017.1295013
Levin, M., Lillis, J. E., & Hayes, S. C. (2012). When is online pornography viewing prob-
lematic among college males? Examining the moderating role of experiential avoidance.
Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity,19, 168180. doi:10.1080/10720162.2012.657150
Ley, D., Prause, N., & Finn, P. (2014). The emperor has no clothes: A review of the
Pornography Addictionmodel. Current Sexual Health Reports,6(2), 94105. doi:
10.1007/s11930-014-0016-8
Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states:
Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression
and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy,33(3), 335343. doi:10.1016/
0005-7967(94)00075-U
SEXUAL ADDICTION & COMPULSIVITY 315
Macinnis, C. C., & Hodson, G. (2015). Do American states with more religious or conser-
vative populations search more for sexual content on google?. Archives of Sexual
Behavior,44(1), 137147. doi:10.1007/s10508-014-0361-8
Maltz, W., & Maltz, L. (2009). The porn trap: The essential guide to overcoming problems
caused by pornography. New York, New York: HarperCollins Publishers.
Manning, J. C. (2006). The impact of internet pornography on marriage and the family: A
review of the research. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity,13, 131165. doi:10.1080/
10720160600870711
Miller, C. H., & Hedges, D. W. (2008). Scrupulosity disorder: An overview and introduc-
tory analysis. Journal of Anxiety Disorders,22(6), 10421058. doi:10.1016/
j.janxdis.2007.11.004
Minarcik, J., Wetterneck, C. T., & Short, M. B. (2016). The effects of sexually explicit
material use on romantic relationship dynamics. Journal of Behavioral Addictions,5(4),
700707. doi:10.1556/2006.5.2016.078
Muth
en, B. O., & Muth
en, L. K. (2017). Mplus users guide (8th ed.). Los Angeles, CA:
Muth
en & Muth
en.
Muusses, L. D., Kerkhof, P., & Finkenauer, C. (2015). Internet pornography and relation-
ship quality: A longitudinal study of within and between partner effects of adjustment,
sexual satisfaction and sexually explicit internet material among newly-weds. Computers
in Human Behavior,45,7784. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.077
Nelson, L. J., Padilla-Walker, L. M., & Carroll, J. S. (2010). I believe it is wrong but I still
do it: A comparison of religious young men who do versus do not use pornography.
Psychology of Religion and Spirituality,2(3), 136147. doi:10.1037/a0019127
Noor, S. W., Rosser, B. R. S., & Erickson, D. J. (2014). The brief scale to measure problem-
atic sexually explicit media consumption: Psychometric properties of the Compulsive
Pornography Consumption (CPC) Scale among men who have sex with men. Sexual
Addiction & Compulsivity,21, 240261. doi:10.1080/10720162.2014.938849
Norton, P. J. (2007). Depression Anxiety and Stress Scales (DASS-21): Psychometric ana-
lysis across four racial groups. Anxiety, Stress & Coping,20, 253265. doi:10.1080/
10615800701309279
Olatunji, B. O. (2008). Disgust, scrupulosity and conservative attitudes about sex: Evidence
for a mediational model of homophobia. Journal of Research in Personality,42(5),
13641369. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.04.001
Olatunji, B. O., Abramowitz, J. S., Williams, N. L., Connolly, K. M., & Lohr, J. M. (2007).
Scrupulosity and obsessive-compulsive symptomsw: Confirmatory factor analysis and val-
idity of the Penn Inventory of Scrupulosity. Journal of Anxiety Disorders,21(6), 771787.
doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.12.002
Patterson, R., & Price, J. (2012). Pornography, religion, and the happiness gap: Does porn-
ography impact the actively religious differently?. Journal for the Scientific Study of
Religion,51(1), 7989. doi:10.1111/j.1468-5906.2011.01630.x
Paul, B. (2009). Predicting internet pornography use and arousal: the role of individual dif-
ference variables. The Journal of Sex Research,46(4), 344357. doi:10.1080/
00224490902754152
Perry, S. L. (2016). Does viewing pornography reduce marital quality over time? Evidence
from longitudinal data. Archives of Sexual Behavior.11,4499, 1. doi:10.1007/s10508-016-
0770-y
Perry, S. L. (2017). Pornography use and depressive symptoms: examining the role of moral
incongruence. Society and Mental Health,1,112. doi:10.1177/2156869317728373
316 N. C. BORGOGNA ET AL.
Perry, S. L. (2018). Pornography use and marital separation: Evidence from two-wave panel
data. Archives of Sexual Behavior,47,112. doi:10.1007/s10508-017-1080-8
Perry, S. L., & Schleifer, C. (2019). Race and trends in pornography viewership, 1973-2016:
Examining the moderating roles of gender and religion. Journal of Sex Research,56,
6273. doi:10.1080/00224499.2017.1404959
Perry, S. L., & Snawder, K. J. (2017). Pornography, religion, and parentchild relationship
quality. Archives of Sexual Behavior,46(6), 17471761. doi:10.1007/s10508-016-0927-8
Perry, S. L., & Whitehead, A. L. (2018). Only bad for believers? Religion, pornography use,
and sexual satisfaction among American men. Journal of Sex Research,1, 1. doi:10.1080/
00224499.2017.1423017
Potenza, M. N., Gola, M., Voon, V., Kor, A., & Kraus, S. W. (2017). Is excessive sexual
behaviour also an addictive disorder?. Lancet Psychiatry,4(9), 663664. doi:10.1016/
S2215-0366(17)30316-4
Poulsen, F. O., Busby, D. M., & Galovan, A. M. (2013). Pornography use: who uses it and
how it is associated with couple outcomes. Journal of Sex Research,50(1), 7283. doi:
10.1080/00224499.2011.648027
Prause, N., Janssen, E., Georgiadis, J., Finn, P., & Pfaus, J. (2017). Data do not support sex
as addictive. Lancet Psychiatry,4(12), 899. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30441-8
Price, J., Patterson, R., Regnerus, M., & Walley, J. (2016). How much more XXX is
Generation X consuming? Evidence of changing attitudes and behaviors related to porn-
ography since 1973. Journal of Sex Research,53(1), 1220. doi:10.1080/
00224499.2014.1003773
Russell, D. W., Kahn, J. H., Spoth, R., & Altmaier, E. M. (1998). Analyzing data from
experimental studies: A latent variable structural equation modeling approach. Journal of
Counseling Psychology,45(1), 1829. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.45.1.18
Short, M. B., Black, L., Smith, A. H., Wetterneck, C. T., & Wells, D. E. (2012). A review of
internet pornography use research: Methodology and content from the past 10 years.
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking,15(1), 1323. doi:10.1089/
cyber.2010.0477
Short, M. B., Kasper, T. E., & Wetterneck, C. T. (2015). The relationship between religiosity
and internet pornography use. Journal of Religion and Health,54(2), 571583. doi:
10.1007/s10943-014-9849-8
Sniewski, L., Farvid, P., & Carter, P. (2018). The assessment and treatment of adult hetero-
sexual men with self-perceived problematic pornography use: A review. Addictive
Behaviors,77, 217224. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.10.010
Stark, R., Klucken, T., Potenza, M. N., Brand, M., & Strahler, J. (2018). A current under-
standing of the behavioral neuroscience of compulsive sexual behavior disorder and
problematic pornography use. Current Behavioral Neuroscience Reports,5(4), 218231.
doi:10.1007/s40473-018-0162-9
Sun, C., Bridges, A., Johnson, J. A., & Ezzell, M. B. (2016). Pornography and the male sex-
ual script: An analysis of consumption and sexual relations. Archives of Sexual Behavior,
45(4), 983994. doi:10.1007/s10508-014-0391-2
Twohig, M. P., & Crosby, J. M. (2010). Acceptance and commitment therapy as a treatment
for problematic internet pornography viewing. Behavior Therapy,41(3), 285295. doi:
10.1016/j.beth.2009.06.002
Twohig, M. P., Crosby, J. M., & Cox, J. M. (2009). Viewing internet pornography: For
whom is it problematic, how, and why?. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity,16, 253266.
doi:10.1080/10720160903300788
SEXUAL ADDICTION & COMPULSIVITY 317
Volk, F., Thomas, J., Sosin, L., Jacob, V., & Moen, C. (2016). Religiosity, developmental
context, and sexual shame in pornography users: A serial mediation model. Sexual
Addiction & Compulsivity,23, 244259. doi:10.1080/10720162.2016.1151391
W
ery, A., & Billieux, J. (2017). Problematic cybersex: Conceptualization, assessment, and
treatment. Addictive Behaviors,64, 238246. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.11.007
Whitehead, A. L., & Perry, S. L. (2018). Unbuckling the Bible Belt: A state-level analysis of
religious factors and google searches for porn. Journal of Sex Research,55(3), 273283.
doi:10.1080/00224499.2017.1278736
Willoughby, B. J., Carroll, J. S., Busby, D. M., & Brown, C. C. (2016). Differences in porn-
ography use among couples: Associations with satisfaction, stability, and relationship
processes. Archives of Sexual Behavior,45(1), 145158. doi:10.1007/s10508-015-0562-9
Wilt, J. A., Cooper, E. B., Grubbs, J. B., Exline, J. J., & Pargament, K. I. (2016).
Associations of Perceived Addiction to Internet Pornography with Religious/Spiritual
and Psychological Functioning. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity,23, 260278.
Associations doi:10.1080/10720162.2016.1140604
Wolak, J., Mitchell, K., & Finkelhor, D. (2007). Unwanted and wanted exposure to online
pornography in a national sample of youth Internet users. Pediatrics,119(2), 247257.
doi:10.1542/peds.2006-1891
W
olfling, K., Beutel, M. E., Dreier, M., & M
uller, K. W. (2015). Bipolar spectrum disorders
in a clinical sample of patients with Internet addiction: Hidden comorbidity or differen-
tial diagnosis?. Journal of Behavioral Addictions,4(2), 101105. doi:10.1556/
2006.4.2015.011
Wright, P. J., Bridges, A. J., Sun, C., Ezzell, M. B., & Johnson, J. A. (2018). Personal pornog-
raphy viewing and sexual satisfaction: A quadratic analysis. Journal of Sex and Marital
Therapy, 44(3), 308315. doi:10.1080/0092623X.2017.1377131
Wright, P. J., Sun, C., Steffen, N. J., & Tokunaga, R. S. (2017). Associative pathways
between pornography consumption and reduced sexual satisfaction. Sexual and
Relationship Therapy,1,118. doi:10.1080/14681994.2017.1323076
Wright, P. J., Tokunaga, R. S., Kraus, A., & Klann, E. (2017). Pornography consumption
and satisfaction: A meta-analysis. Human Communication Research,43(3), 315343. doi:
10.1111/hcre.12108
Ybarra, M. L., & Mitchell, K. J. (2005). Exposure to internet pornography among children
and adolescents: a national survey. Cyberpsychology & Behavior,8, 473486. doi:10.1089/
cpb.2005.8.473
Yoder, V. C., Virden, T. B., & Amin, K. (2005). Internet pornography and loneliness: An
association?. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity,12,1944. doi:10.1080/
10720160590933653
Zitzman, S. T., & Butler, M. H. (2009). Wivesexperience of husbandspornography use
and concomitant deception as an attachment threat in the adult pair-bond relationship.
Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity,16, 210240. doi:10.1080/10720160903202679
318 N. C. BORGOGNA ET AL.
... A comprehensive review of measurement invariance testing is beyond the scope of the current article 22,27,28 . However, a brief synopsis is available in Supplementary File 1. Thus far, gender is perhaps the most common grouping for PPU measurement invariance analyses, with most studies demonstrating that PPU instruments typically achieve metric invariance across genders, but fail scalar 18,29,30 . Though depending on fit criteria employed, other PPU measures might achieve higher levels of invariance for gender 11,13,31 . ...
... Structural invariance is predicated upon achieving at least metric measurement invariance across groups (or correlation coefficient differences may be attributable to measurement biases rather than latent construct differences) 34 . However, little is known about PPU structural invariance in general, with most extant tests being relegated to the same studies in which metric invariance was investigated 17,29 ; none of which examined how coefficients might change according to race. ...
... Anxiety was chosen as historical models have suggested that some expressions of CSBD (and thus PPU) may be related to maladaptive attempts to reduce anxiety 35 . Moreover, prior research has shown that anxiety problems modestly correlate with PPU 29,36 . Given that PPU is a new mental health classification, understanding how it converges with general psychological problems, such as anxiety, across groups might help contextualize findings. ...
Article
Full-text available
Problematic pornography use (PPU) is a burgeoning area of clinical interest. The Brief Pornography Screen (BPS) is a new PPU measure. The BPS has not been psychometrically studied within specific cultural groups. We sought to broaden the PPU literature by examining the confirmatory factor analytic (CFA) fit, measurement invariance, and structural invariance of the BPS across eight identity groups. Participants came from a survey administered at three United States universities. In total, n=2,475 participants were analyzed, with the following identity group participant breakdowns: Asian American and Pacific Islander (AAPI) Male=268, AAPI Female=303, Black Male=101, Black Female=189, Latin Male=208, Latin Female=372, White Male=432, and White Female=602. BPS CFA fit was good across all groups. Measurement invariance analyses suggested metric, but not scalar invariance across all groups. We then split participants by sex assignment, full residual invariance was evident across groups for male participants and partial residual invariance was evident for female participants. Structural invariance analyses indicated that anxiety as a weak positive BPS correlate in AAPI, Latin, and White Male participants (β's=.25-.27), but not meaningfully related for the other groups. Pornography viewing frequency was positively correlated with BPS scores across most groups with a wide range (β's=.29-.52), but not Black Male participants (β=.15). Our results suggest that the BPS is an appropriate PPU measure across the tested identity groups. While between group measurement is relatively accurate within sex assignment groups, correlates differed in strength, meaning different variables likely predict PPU for different groups of people. Abstract Problematic pornography use (PPU) is a burgeoning area of clinical interest. The Brief
... Individuals with substance use disorders (SUDs) may be vulnerable to developing PPU in part due to shared neurocognitive mechanisms, including those linked to impulsivity (Gola et al., 2017;Mestre-Bach, Fernández-Aranda, Jiménez-Murcia, & Potenza, 2020;Stark, Klucken, Potenza, Brand, & Strahler, 2018). Mental health concerns may also relate to PPU, as individuals may use pornography to escape psychological distress (Borgogna, Duncan, & McDermott, 2018). Studies of OUD have identified certain genetic variants that influence susceptibility, including genetic markers related to dopamine systems that are involved in reward and impulse control processing (Mistry, Bawor, Desai, C Marsh, & Samaan, 2014). ...
... Other studies have linked PPU to psychological distress and psychiatric symptomatology involving impulsivity, anxiety, post-traumatic stress disorder, and SUDs, particularly to alcohol (Borgogna et al., 2018;Borgogna, Kraus, & Grubbs, 2021;Moisson et al., 2019;Shirk, Saxena, Park, & Kraus, 2021). Pornography use has been associated with depression and anxiety (Guidry, Floyd, Volk, & Moen, 2020;Perry, 2018;Willoughby, Busby, & Young-Petersen, 2019), although the precise nature of the relationship remains incompletely understood. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and Aims Many jurisdictions are experiencing opioid epidemics. Opioid use disorder (OUD) often co-occurs with other psychiatric disorders including behavioral addictions like gambling disorder. However, little is known regarding the frequency and correlates of problematic pornography use (PPU) among people seeking treatment for OUD. Here we aimed to investigate PPU and its correlates in people seeking OUD treatment. Method From October 2018 to March 2020, 1,272 individuals seeking OUD treatment were screened for PPU by completing the Brief Pornography Screen (BPS), a 5-item instrument validated for assessing PPU. Self-reported data were used. Results Among the sample there were 707 (60%) males and 565 (40%) females. The mean age of participants was 37.9 ± 10.5 years (range 18–73), there were 707 (60%) males and 565 (40%) females, 14.4% ( n = 183) exhibited low positive BPS scores (1 ≤ score ≤4), and 4.5% of the sample ( n = 57) screened positive for PPU (BPS score ≥4). Individuals screening positive for PPU versus negative were mostly male (77%), scored higher on measures of impulsivity in the domains of positive urgency, negative urgency, and sensation-seeking and demonstrated more psychopathology on measures of substance use, psychotic symptoms, emotional lability, depression/functioning and self-harm. Discussion and Conclusion A minority of individuals seeking treatment for OUD screened positive for PPU. Among individuals with OUD, those screening positive (versus negative) for PPU were more impulsive and experienced more psychiatric symptoms, suggesting the need for additional investigation and screening for and addressing PPU in people with OUD.
... Higher BPS values were associated with more severe psychopathology, consistent with previous findings linking PPU to psychopathology or poor mental health (Bibi et al., 2022;Borgogna et al., 2018;Hernández et al., 2023;Hernandez-Mora & Varescon, 2022;Shirk et al., 2021;Whitfield et al., 2018). Correlations between the BPS and measures of depression and anxiety were also reported in the validation study of BPS in a general population (Islam et al., 2024). ...
Article
Full-text available
Problematic pornography use (PPU) shares characteristics with behavioral addictions, such as gambling disorder (GD), and PPU and GD may frequently co-occur. In order to fill existing gaps in screening instruments for PPU validated in GD, the study examined the psychometric properties of the Spanish version of the Brief Pornography Screen (BPS) within a clinical sample of individuals with GD. A total of 200 individuals seeking treatment for GD were consecutively recruited. The psychometric properties of the BPS were examined by performing confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and testing for evidence of convergent and discriminant validity. The CFA supported a one-factor solution showing high internal consistency (α = 0.81). The BPS showed convergent validity and correlated with psychological distress, general psychopathology, impulsive tendencies, and low self-directedness. The BPS demonstrated adequate psychometric properties and is therefore recommended as a brief screening tool for identifying probable PPU in individuals seeking treatment for GD.
... Anxiety and depression have bidirectional relationships with pornography use. They can emerge as responses to distressing elements associated with pornography use (Borgogna et al., 2018;Guidry et al., 2020;Leonhardt et al., 2018;Willoughby et al., 2019) or serve as motivators for consumption (Bőthe et al., 2021a(Bőthe et al., , 2021b(Bőthe et al., , 2021c. These mental health factors also play a role in difficulties with sexual function and sexual distress (McCabe et al., 2016). ...
Article
This study explores the connection between pornography use, sexual functioning, and mental health using latent profile analysis (LPA) to identify distinct profiles among pornography users and assess variations in mental health, including anxiety, depression, and emotion regulation. The aim of this study was to identify profiles among pornography users, specifically distinguishing those with sexual distress and sexual function problems, characterize these profiles, and assess variations in mental health. Data were collected from 463 participants through an anonymous online survey. Three distinct pornography user profiles were identified based on their consumption and sexual problems: high-frequency problematic pornography use (PPU, which is defined as uncontrollable patterns of pornography use resulting in adverse consequences and significant distress) with sexual problems, high-frequency non-PPU without/with low sexual problems, and no/low-frequency non-PPU without sexual problems. Differences among the profiles emerged in anxiety and depressive symptoms as well as emotion regulation capabilities. The high-frequency PPU with sexual problems group exhibited lower emotion regulation capabilities, while the no/low-frequency non-PPU without sexual problems group reported significantly lower depressive symptoms. This study emphasizes the multifaceted nature of pornography use, revealing that certain patterns may be more problematic. The findings highlight the interplay between emotional dysregulation, anxiety, depression, problematic pornography use, and sexual problems and distress. Recognizing these distinct profiles is crucial for understanding the relationship between pornography use and sexual and mental well-being, ultimately contributing to more targeted interventions and improved outcomes for all populations.
... Apart from this, the destructive properties of pornography on the individual lead to physical and psychological problems that attract clinicians attention, such as diminished sexual desire when engaging in activity (e.g. pornography) (Pizzol et al., 2016), erectile dysfunction (Park et al., 2016), and anxiety (Borgogna et al., 2018). Despite its clinical concern, the diminished confidence in the quality of these assessments is due to their lack of reliability (Marshall & Miller, 2019). ...
Article
The Problematic Pornography Consumption Scale (PPCS) was developed to assess problematic pornography use (PPU). A Cronbach’s alpha generalization meta-analysis was conducted to estimate the average Cronbach’s alpha score of the PPCS.Seventeen Cronbach’s alpha scores from 12 studies (N = 41974) yielded excellent Cronbach’s alpha score estimates. The average Cronbach’s alpha score of the PPCS was .91 indicating excelltent internal consistency (95% CI = .883, .931; p < .001). Clinicians and researchers can be more confident in knowing that the PPCS is a reliable scale for measuring problematic pornography use across different populations.
Article
Full-text available
El consumo de pornografía es una práctica muy extendida. En la mayoría de casos, no conlleva consecuencia negativa alguna; no obstante, algunas personas desarrollan un patrón de consumo excesivo y problemático. Uno de los factores que aumenta el riesgo de desarrollar un uso problemático de pornografía (UPP) es el estado de ánimo disfórico (concretamente, el estado de ánimo depresivo). Asimismo, la rumiación (forma disfuncional de regulación emocional) perpetúa el malestar emocional, lo cual podría exacerbar el vínculo entre depresión y UPP. El objetivo de este estudio fue examinar si la rumiación mediaba o moderaba la relación entre depresión, frecuencia de uso de pornografía (FUP) y UPP. Con este propósito, 220 participantes entre 18-74 años completaron una batería de cuestionarios online donde se evaluaban las principales variables del estudio. La depresión tuvo un efecto directo, positivo y significativo en el UPP ( =0.96***), pero no en la FUP ( =-0.02). De igual modo, la rumiación tuvo un efecto directo, positivo y significativo en el UPP ( =0.12*), pero no en la FUP ( =0.008). En cuanto a los análisis de mediación, la rumiación no mediaba entre la depresión y el UPP ( =0.01), pero sí entre la depresión y la FUP ( =0.03*). Asimismo, la rumiación no moderó la relación entre depresión y FUP ( =0.001) ni entre depresión y UPP ( =-0.003). Estos resultados sugieren que el estado emocional modula fuertemente el consumo de pornografía (aumentando el riesgo de desarrollar un UPP), pero que la rumiación no media este vínculo ni modera su relación. El único caso en el que la rumiación desempeña un papel a la hora de explicar el nexo entre depresión y pornografía es en el caso de la FUP. Así, es posible que personas con depresión y tendencia a rumiar usen la pornografía con mayor frecuencia, pero no por ello de forma más problemática.
Article
29 Sports participation is associated with heightened sexual behavior, while its determinants are 30 unclear. We examined hypersexuality in 104 kayakers and 77 mixed exercisers, considering the 31 roles of gender, exercise volume, and perceived stress. Participants, 89 men and 92 women 32 (Mage=26.1±8.1 years), completed the Hypersexual Behavior Inventory, Perceived Stress 33 Scale, and demographic questions online. Path analyses tested the relationships between 34 perceived stress and exercise volume, considering gender and sports form-related differences. 35 Women reported more stress than men (p<.001, Cohen's d=.70). Men reported higher 36 hypersexuality than women (p<.001, d=.96). Kayakers reported higher training volumes 37 (p<.001, d=.97) and hypersexuality than mixed exercisers (p=.003, d=.46). Perceived stress was 38 positively and moderately associated with hypersexuality, while exercise volume was positively 39 but weakly related to hypersexuality only among men. Exercise volume was unrelated to 40 hypersexuality in kayakers, while a positive, moderate association emerged in mixed exercisers. 41 These results suggest that hypersexuality and its associations with perceived stress and exercise 42 volume could vary based on the sports' form and the gender of the athlete. While the perceived 43 stress may relate to hypersexuality in both men and women, the relationship between exercise 44 volume and hypersexuality may vary more according to gender and the sport's form. 45
Article
Full-text available
The pornography problems due to moral incongruence (PPMI) model is a premier framework for understanding problematic pornography use (PPU). However, past studies have generally examined men or entered gender as a covariate in primary analyses. Such approaches mask between-gender differences. Additionally, dysregulation constructs are also thought to be relevant to PPU, yet it is unclear the degree to which they incrementally predict PPU beyond moral incongruence constructs in non-pathological populations. We addressed these gaps by gathering a large sample of college students (n = 295 men, n = 838 women). Analyses with pornography users (n = 251 men, n = 407 women) were consistent with the PPMI model, adjusted for pornography use frequency. Findings did not change when dysregulation constructs of impulsivity and emotional resilience were added to the model. No paths significantly differed between genders. Altogether, among college student pornography users, religiosity was strongly positively correlated with moral disapproval (β = .65 men, β = .62 women), moral disapproval was moderately positively correlated with PPU (β = .41 men, β = .29 women), religiosity was initially moderately positively correlated with PPU (r = .21 men, r = .22 women), but became non-significant in the full model (β = − .21 men, β = − .04 women), and indirect effects of religiosity to PPU through moral disapproval were significant (indirect β = .27 men, β = .18 women). None of the dysregulation constructs significantly predicted PPU. The full model accounted for 23-22% of the PPU variance in men and women, respectively. Implications, future directions, and limitations are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose of Review In the recently released eleventh edition of the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-11), compulsive sexual behavior disorder (CSBD) was for the first time included and classified as an impulse control disorder. The present report aims at summarizing the empirical results concerning the neurobiological underpinnings of CSBD, including problematic pornography use. Insight into mechanistic factors underlying CSBD may promote the development of more effective therapeutic interventions for people affected. Recent Findings Recent neurobiological studies have revealed that compulsive sexual behaviors are associated with altered processing of sexual material and differences in brain structure and function. Summary Although few neurobiological studies of CSBD have been conducted to date, existing data suggest neurobiological abnormalities share communalities with other additions such as substance use and gambling disorders. Thus, existing data suggest that its classification may be better suited as a behavioral addiction rather than an impulse-control disorder.
Article
Full-text available
Pornography viewing is a growing practice that has been understudied in samples of women. As an answer to calls within the literature, we explored the role of problematic pornography viewing constructs, body image, and relationship satisfaction in a sample of women (n = 949). Structural equation modeling indicated pornography viewing frequency, perceptions of excessive use, and control difficulties were unrelated to body image or relationship satisfaction. However, problematic pornography use to escape negative emotions significantly predicted participants’ body image and relationship dissatisfaction. Future directions, limitations, and clinical implications are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose of Review The current review summarizes the latest findings concerning neurobiological mechanisms of compulsive sexual behavior disorder (CSBD) and provides recommendations for future research specific to the diagnostic classification of the condition. Recent Findings To date, most neuroimaging research on compulsive sexual behavior has provided evidence of overlapping mechanisms underlying compulsive sexual behavior and non-sexual addictions. Compulsive sexual behavior is associated with altered functioning in brain regions and networks implicated in sensitization, habituation, impulse dyscontrol, and reward processing in patterns like substance, gambling, and gaming addictions. Key brain regions linked to compulsive sexual behavior features include the frontal and temporal cortices, amygdala, and striatum, including the nucleus accumbens. Summary Despite much neuroscience research finding many similarities between CSBD and substance and behavioral addictions, the World Health Organization included CSBD in the ICD-11 as an impulse-control disorder. Although previous research has helped to highlight some underlying mechanisms of the condition, additional investigations are needed to fully understand this phenomenon and resolve classification issues surrounding CSBD.
Article
Full-text available
Problematic pornography viewing (PPV) is a growing concern. Based on a masculine gender role strain framework, individuals endorsing traditional masculinity ideology (TMI) may be especially drawn to pornography. However, relatively few studies have explored how TMI is related to PPV. Furthermore, no known studies have explored how these connections differ in men and women. To address these gaps, we conducted a large survey of 310 men and 469 women in the United States assessing multiple PPV and TMI dimensions. A bifactor structural equation model was used to regress PPV domains onto global and specific TMI factors. Invariance testing further examined the moderating effects of participants’ gender in the model. Results indicated that global TMI was unrelated to men’s PPV. However, men’s dominance ideologies predicted greater functional problems and excessive pornography use. Men’s restrictive emotionality and heterosexist ideologies predicted control difficulties with pornography use and using pornography to escape negative emotions. Additionally, men’s avoidance of femininity ideology predicted excessive pornography use and control difficulties. For women, only global TMI was associated with functional problems. Invariance testing suggested the observed gender differences were not due to underlying discrepancies in the measurement of TMI or PPV. Clinical interventions for PPV that incorporate gender role themes are recommended.
Article
Full-text available
Impulsivity and compulsivity are transdiagnostic features associated with clinically relevant aspects of psychiatric disorders, including addictions. However, little research has investigated how impulsivity and compulsivity relate to hypersexuality and problematic pornography use. Thus, the aims of the present study were to investigate (a) self-reported impulsivity and compulsivity with respect to hypersexuality and problematic pornography use and (b) the similarities and possible differences between hypersexuality and problematic pornography use in these domains. Utilizing structural equation modeling (SEM) in a large community sample (N = 13,778 participants; female = 4,151, 30.1%), results indicated that impulsivity (β = .28, β = .26) and compulsivity (β = .23, β = .14) were weakly related to problematic pornography use among males and females, respectively. Impulsivity had a stronger relationship (β = .41, β = .42) with hypersexuality than did compulsivity (β = .21, β = .16) among males and females, respectively. Consequently, impulsivity and compulsivity may not contribute as substantially to problematic pornography use as some scholars have proposed. On the other hand, impulsivity might have a more prominent role in hypersexuality than in problematic pornography use. Future research should examine further social and situational factors associated with problematic pornography use.
Article
Full-text available
Internet pornography use (IPU) remains a controversial topic within sexual behavior research fields. Whereas some people report feeling dysregulated in their use of pornography, mental health and medical communities are divided as to whether or not IPU can be addictive. The present review seeks to examine this issue more closely, with a focus on how variables other than pornography use, such as moral disapproval and moral incongruence (i.e., feeling as if one’s behaviors and one’s values about those behaviors are misaligned) might specifically contribute to self-perceived problems around pornography use. Through an examination of recent literature, the present work reviews evidence that moral incongruence about IPU is a common phenomenon and that it is associated with outcomes relevant to current debates about pornography addiction. Specifically, moral incongruence regarding IPU appears to be associated with greater distress about IPU, greater psychological distress in general, greater reports of problems related to IPU, and greater reports of perceived addiction to IPU. The implications of this body of evidence for both clinical and research communities are discussed, and future directions for research are considered.
Article
Full-text available
There is an understudied, meaningful distinction between high frequency of pornography use and the subjective feeling that this behavior is out of control. We examined whether the quality of a couple's relationship and sex life can strengthen or weaken the association between frequency of Internet pornography use and perceived lack of control over this behavior. In a sample of 1036 participants, results showed that frequency of pornography use was more strongly associated with feeling out of control when relationship and sexual satisfaction were lower. Findings suggest that couple dissatisfaction puts the individual at risk of reporting out-of-control pornography use.
Article
Full-text available
Research has often demonstrated a negative association between pornography use and various intrapersonal and relationship outcomes, particularly for men. Several recent studies, however, have suggested that the negative association between pornography use and these indicators is stronger among more religious Americans, suggesting that moral incongruence (engaging in an activity that violates one's sacred values) and the attendant shame or cognitive dissonance, rather than the pornography use per se, may be the primary factor at work. The current study tested and extended this theory by examining how religion potentially moderates the link between pornography use and sexual satisfaction in a national random sample of American adults (N=1,501). Analyses demonstrated that, while pornography use was negatively associated with sexual satisfaction for American men (not women), among men who rarely attended religious services or held a low opinion of the Bible, this negative association essentially disappeared. Conversely, the negative association between frequency of pornography consumption and sexual satisfaction was more pronounced for men with stronger ties to conventional religion. These findings suggest that the connection between pornography use and sexual satisfaction, especially for men, depends largely on what viewing pornography means to the consumer and their moral community, and less so on the practice itself.
Article
Full-text available
Marc Potenza and colleagues1 advocated classifying “excessive sexual behaviour” as an addictive disorder in ICD-11. Sex has components of liking and wanting that share neural systems with many other motivated behaviours.2 However, experimental studies do not support key elements of addiction such as escalation of use, difficulty regulating urges, negative effects, reward deficiency syndrome, withdrawal syndrome with cessation, tolerance, or enhanced late positive potentials. A key neurobiological feature of addiction is the increased responsiveness of glutamate neurons that synapse on the nucleus accumbens. These changes might affect long-term sensitisation of the mesocorticolimbic dopamine pathway, as manifested by a range of symptoms including cue-induced craving and compulsive drug use.3 To date, research on the effects of sex on glutamate function and its modulation of dopamine pathways is scarce. Sex is a primary reward, with unique peripheral representation. Engagement in sex is positively associated with health and life satisfaction. Sex does not allow for supraphysiological stimulation. Research in this area has yet to investigate actual partnered sexual behaviours. Experimental work has been limited to sexual cues, or secondary rewards, using images. More research is needed, but data concerning frequent or excessive sex do not support its inclusion as an addiction. Also, data are not sufficient to differentiate between compulsive and impulsive models. Many other approaches exist, including well-supported non-pathological models.4 Potenza and colleagues5 also stated that addiction criteria were not met for sexual behaviours: we agree with this earlier conclusion.
Article
Full-text available
While some research has uncovered racial differences in patterns of pornography viewership, no studies to date have considered how these patterns may be changing over time or how these trends may be moderated by other key predictors of pornography viewership, specifically, gender and religion. Using nationally representative data from the 1973-2016 General Social Surveys (N = 20,620), and taking into account different ethno-religious histories with pornography as a moral issue, we examine how race, gender, and religion intersect to influence trends in pornography viewership over 43 years. Analyses reveal that black Americans in general are more likely to view pornography than whites, and they are increasing in their pornography viewership at a higher rate than whites. Moreover, black men are more likely to consume pornography than all other race-gender combinations, but only differ from white women in their increasing rate of pornography viewership. Lastly, frequent worship attendance only moderates trends in pornography viewership for white men. By contrast, regardless of attendance frequency, black men and women show increasing rates of pornography use while white women show flat rates. We conclude by discussing the implications of these findings for research on the intersections of race, gender, religion, and sexuality.