ArticlePDF Available

Slow Down: Behavioural and Physiological Effects of Reducing Eating Rate

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Slowing eating rate appears to be an effective strategy for reducing food intake. This feasibility study investigated the effect of eating rate on post-meal responses using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), plasma gastrointestinal hormone concentrations, appetite ratings, memory for recent eating, and snack consumption. Twenty-one participants (mean age 23 years with healthy body mass index) were randomly assigned to consume a 600 kcal meal at either a “normal” or “slow” rate (6 vs. 24 min). Immediately afterwards, participants rated meal enjoyment and satisfaction. FMRI was performed 2-h post-meal during a memory task about the meal. Appetite, peptide YY, and ghrelin were measured at baseline and every 30 min for 3 h. Participants were given an ad-libitum snack three hours post-meal. Results were reported as effect sizes (Cohen’s d) due to the feasibility sample size. The normal rate group found the meal more enjoyable (effect size = 0.5) and satisfying (effect size = 0.6). Two hours post-meal, the slow rate group reported greater fullness (effect size = 0.7) and more accurate portion size memory (effect sizes = 0.4), with a linear relationship between time taken to make portion size decisions and the BOLD response in satiety and reward brain regions. Ghrelin suppression post-meal was greater in the slow rate group (effect size = 0.8). Three hours post-meal, the slow rate group consumed on average 25% less energy from snacks (effect size = 0.5). These data offer novel insights about mechanisms underlying how eating rate affects food intake and have implications for the design of effective weight-management interventions.
Content may be subject to copyright.
nutrients
Article
Slow Down: Behavioural and Physiological Effects of
Reducing Eating Rate
Katherine Hawton 1, * , Danielle Ferriday 2, Peter Rogers 1,2, Paula Toner 1, Jonathan Brooks 3,4,
Jeffrey Holly 5, Kalina Biernacka 5, Julian Hamilton-Shield 1and Elanor Hinton 1,3
1
National Institute for Health Research Bristol Biomedical Research Centre, University Hospitals Bristol NHS
Foundation Trust and University of Bristol, Bristol BS8 1TU, UK; peter.rogers@bristol.ac.uk (P.R.);
pt13294@my.bristol.ac.uk (P.T.); j.p.h.shield@bristol.ac.uk (J.H.-S.); elanor.hinton@bristol.ac.uk (E.H.)
2Nutrition and Behaviour Unit, School of Psychological Science, University of Bristol, 12A Priory Rd,
Bristol BS8 1TU, UK; danielle.ferriday@bristol.ac.uk
3Clinical Research and Imaging Centre, University of Bristol, 60 St Michael’s Hill, Bristol BS2 8DX, UK;
jon.brooks@bristol.ac.uk
4School of Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol, 12A Priory Rd, Bristol BS8 1TU, UK
5
IGFs and Metabolic Endocrinology, University of Bristol, Second Floor, Learning and Research, Southmead
Hospital, Westbury-on-Trym, Bristol BS10 5NB, UK; jeff.holly@bristol.ac.uk (J.H.);
mdxkz@bristol.ac.uk (K.B.)
*Correspondence: katherine.hawton@bristol.ac.uk; Tel.: +44-117-342-1750
Received: 27 October 2018; Accepted: 19 December 2018; Published: 27 December 2018


Abstract:
Slowing eating rate appears to be an effective strategy for reducing food intake.
This feasibility study investigated the effect of eating rate on post-meal responses using functional
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), plasma gastrointestinal hormone concentrations, appetite ratings,
memory for recent eating, and snack consumption. Twenty-one participants (mean age 23 years
with healthy body mass index) were randomly assigned to consume a 600 kcal meal at either a
“normal” or “slow” rate (6 vs. 24 min). Immediately afterwards, participants rated meal enjoyment
and satisfaction. FMRI was performed 2-h post-meal during a memory task about the meal. Appetite,
peptide YY, and ghrelin were measured at baseline and every 30 min for 3 h. Participants were given
an ad-libitum snack three hours post-meal. Results were reported as effect sizes (Cohen’s d) due to
the feasibility sample size. The normal rate group found the meal more enjoyable (effect size = 0.5)
and satisfying (effect size = 0.6). Two hours post-meal, the slow rate group reported greater fullness
(effect size = 0.7) and more accurate portion size memory (effect sizes = 0.4), with a linear relationship
between time taken to make portion size decisions and the BOLD response in satiety and reward
brain regions. Ghrelin suppression post-meal was greater in the slow rate group (effect size = 0.8).
Three hours post-meal, the slow rate group consumed on average 25% less energy from snacks (effect
size = 0.5). These data offer novel insights about mechanisms underlying how eating rate affects food
intake and have implications for the design of effective weight-management interventions.
Keywords:
eating rate; satiety; functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI); memory for recent
eating; appetite hormones; meal enjoyment
1. Introduction
Obesity is a major public health issue with approximately 60% of adults and nearly 30% of children
aged 2–15 years in the UK being overweight or obese [
1
]. There is a paucity of strategies proven to be
effective for preventing, or treating, overweight or obesity. Fast eating is associated with excess body
weight [
2
] and reducing rate of eating appears to reduce energy intake [
3
]. These systematic reviews
have highlighted that further work is needed to ascertain the mechanism underlying effects of eating
Nutrients 2019,11, 50; doi:10.3390/nu11010050 www.mdpi.com/journal/nutrients
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 2 of 23
rate on weight control and whether eating rate influences self-reported appetite [
4
]. For example, a
recent study found that participants who consumed a test meal more slowly reported a greater increase
in fullness, yet with reduced enjoyment and satisfaction from the meal [5].
Research investigating the physiological basis of the effect of eating rate on satiety has yielded
inconsistent results for gastrointestinal (GI) hormone responses. One study utilising a cross-over design
(N= 17) found greater post-prandial peptide YY (PYY) response after slowing eating rate (30 min vs.
5 min meal) however no effect was seen on ghrelin suppression [
6
]. Similar findings were elicited by
another study examining the effects of eating rate and eating density on GI hormone responses (N= 20),
where slow eating rate (20 g/min vs. 80 g/min) led to greater PYY response but no effect on ghrelin
suppression [
7
]. By contrast, one cross-over study (N= 25), found no effect of eating rate by comparing
7 min, 14 min and 28 min meals on PYY release post-prandially [
8
]. A study of overweight adolescents
(N= 27) utilising a Mandometer (device providing real-time feedback about consumption of meal by
weight of plate) to slow eating rate vs. control, found slowing eating rate led to increased PYY response
and greater ghrelin suppression post-prandially [
9
]. Thus, there is no current consensus regarding the
effect of eating rate on satiety hormone response and further research is needed.
Other work has explored whether the mechanism underlying differences in food intake following
different eating rates may relate to enhancing or disrupting memory for recent eating episodes [
10
,
11
].
There is accumulating evidence that memory for recent eating plays a pivotal role in the control of
energy intake [
12
14
]. A recent study (N= 40) found that participants who consumed a meal more
slowly remembered eating a larger portion but eating more slowly was not found to affect self-reported
memory vividness [
10
], as was found in another study [
11
]. Memory for recent eating is assumed to
involve brain regions classically associated with memory, such as the hippocampus [
15
,
16
]. More recent
functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies utilising a “What, Where, When” paradigm of
episodic memory [
17
,
18
] suggest that, in addition to the hippocampus and other medial temporal lobe
regions, frontal and parietal brain regions are also important for memory-related processes.
The objectives of this feasibility study were two-fold: first, to assess the feasibility of conducting a
further, full-powered study combining a range of different measures (appetite ratings, satiety hormones,
fMRI and subsequent consumption) to assess the effect of manipulating eating rate on these measures
relating to energy intake. Feasibility outcomes were (i) combining novel and technically challenging
measures (how many full data sets were obtained), (ii) a novel fMRI task (were the participants able to
perform the task), (iii) ability to measure imaging signal in the brain regions of interest using fMRI,
(iv) the practicality and acceptability of the blood sampling protocol (number of samples obtained)
and (v) whether participants were aware of the aim of the study. Neuroimaging, a novel memory for
recent eating task and GI hormones measurements were combined to ascertain whether top-down
cognitive mechanisms or bottom-up physiological mechanisms are driving the effect of slowing eating
rate. We developed a new “what, where, when” task to assess for memory for recent eating to be
utilised during fMRI, as no such paradigm was previously available. Secondary objectives were to
provide preliminary data using measures of appetite, hormones, fMRI and ad-libitum consumption to
gain a greater understanding of the mechanisms underlying the effect of eating rate on energy intake.
To our knowledge, this is the first study to apply neuroimaging to investigate the effects of
eating rate. The extensive fMRI literature on the satiating effects of food and food cues predicts a
“network” of brain regions whose response might be influenced by eating rate, including insula [
19
]
and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) [
20
], in addition to subcortical reward regions [
21
23
]. We predicted
that slowing eating rate would lead to greater signal change during a memory for recent eating
task in memory-, satiety- and reward-responsive brain areas, including hippocampal, frontal and
parietal regions. We also hypothesised that experimentally slowing eating rate would lead to reduced
enjoyment and satisfaction, a greater feeling of fullness post-meal and improved memory of the meal.
In relation to GI hormones, we hypothesised that slowing eating rate would lead to increased ghrelin
suppression and increased PYY secretion post-meal. We predicted that the combined effect of these
mechanisms would lead to a reduced subsequent food intake at an ad-libitum snack meal.
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 3 of 23
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
Twenty-one participants aged between 18–35 years of healthy BMI (18.5–25.0 kg/m
2
) were
recruited using posters and an online advert. Participants were asked to complete an online
questionnaire to check eligibility which also included the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire
(DEBQ) [
24
]. Ethical approval was granted by the Faculty of Science Human Research Ethics
Committee, University of Bristol.
2.2. Eligibility
To be eligible, the body mass index (BMI) of participants had to be within the normal range
(18.5 kg/m
2
–25 kg/m
2
). Any volunteers who had a history of eating disorders, neurological disorders
or traumatic brain injury, psychiatric disorders or diabetes were excluded. Participants who were on a
diet to control weight, were pregnant or had food allergies, were taking any medication that might
influence appetite or who smoked more than five cigarettes per day were also excluded. It was also
a requirement for participants to be fluent in English. To ensure safety in the MRI scanner, further
contraindications included metal implants and any tattoos with metallic ink, which were screened
for using the standardised Clinical Research Imaging Centre (CRIC Bristol) screening form. A signed
consent form was completed by each participant prior the participating in the study.
2.3. Randomisation
Participants were allocated on the basis of age, gender, BMI and DEBQ restraint scores to either a
“normal” or “slow” eating rate group. To balance the groups on these factors, a minimisation method
proposed by Pocock & Simon [
25
] was employed. This adaptive randomization technique is designed
to reduce any imbalance between the experimental groups in the distribution of scores over several
factors [
26
]. A 4:1 element of chance was incorporated and automated using Microsoft Excel so that
allocation to groups was pseudo-random [
27
]. Eleven participants were randomised to the “normal”
group and 10 participants were randomised to the “slow” group.
2.4. Measures
Computerised visual analogue scales (VAS) utilising a 100-point scale between “Not at all” and
“Extremely” with a moveable cursor, were used for appetite (fullness, hunger, thirst, desire to eat,
bloated, nauseous, empty), pleasantness and desire to eat, enjoyment and satisfaction ratings. Wording
of the instructions for appetite ratings is reported in Appendix A. Pleasantness and desire to eat ratings
were given following one mouthful (two pieces of macaroni) before and after the meal (referred to as
“Taste tests”; wording of instructions given in the Appendix Bin Table A2, as per [
28
]). Satisfaction
may be considered to be a composite of both enjoyment (how tasty the meal was) and post-meal
fullness [
29
] and was included with the following instructions (“How much did you enjoy eating the
meal?”; “How satisfied did you feel by the meal?”).
PYY and ghrelin were measured at baseline and every 30 min for 3 h. In addition, blood
glucose was measured at baseline and 120 min post-meal to ensure that none of the participants
showed insulin resistance or impaired glucose tolerance. The samples were collected via a peripheral
intravenous cannula. Blood samples for ghrelin and PYY were collected into aprotinin containing
EDTA tubes, inverted and centrifuged in 4
C at 2500 rpm for 15 min. 1N hydrochloric acid (HCl) and
phenylmethylsulfonyl fluoride (PMSF) were added as preservatives. Plasma samples were kept in
80 C until assayed. Details of the measurement assays can be found in Appendix A.
A novel memory task was developed for this study to examine and distinguish between different
aspects of an eating episode that might contribute to an individual’s memory for recent eating, namely,
memory for the portion size consumed, the time since eating that portion, and spatial context in which
the meal was consumed. This task was designed to measure the behavioural (% correct responses and
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 4 of 23
reaction times) and neural correlates of each trial type. The paradigm was based on an episodic “What,
where, when” fMRI task by Kwok et al. [
17
], in which short video clips were presented, followed by
images of the scenes in three different trial types that required recall of what was in the video (scene
recognition), when in the sequence of the clip was the image shown (scene chronology), and where items
were arranged (correct spatial layout). In our newly adapted paradigm, for each of the three trial types,
an image was shown in the centre of the screen. The participant was instructed by an on-screen cue to
make a response based on three particular trial types; (a) portion size (b) interval since last ate and (c)
spatial memory trials (see Figure 1for details of trial-types). A total of 150 trials, 50 trials of each type,
were pseudo-randomly displayed, in one of two orders (counterbalanced between participants) with
no more than two of any one trial type in a row (Figure A1, Appendix A). The 50 portion photographs
used in the portion trial tasks were in 20 kcal equicaloric steps either side of the true portion size of the
test meal used in the study (600 kcal). Each trial was presented for 4 s, with a mean ISI of 5.5 s (random
jitter of between 3–8 s of fixation cross, no more than two of any length in a row). The data captured
when the fixation cross appeared was used as the baseline fMRI measure (see Section 2.6 below for
details). Response mapping to left/right button presses were counterbalanced between participants.
Behavioural performance on the task was measured by percentage of correct responses and response
times (RT) for each trial type.
Nutrients 2018, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 4 of 24
namely, memory for the portion size consumed, the time since eating that portion, and spatial context
in which the meal was consumed. This task was designed to measure the behavioural (% correct
responses and reaction times) and neural correlates of each trial type. The paradigm was based on an
episodic “What, where, when” fMRI task by Kwok et al. [17], in which short video clips were
presented, followed by images of the scenes in three different trial types that required recall of what
was in the video (scene recognition), when in the sequence of the clip was the image shown (scene
chronology), and where items were arranged (correct spatial layout). In our newly adapted paradigm,
for each of the three trial types, an image was shown in the centre of the screen. The participant was
instructed by an on-screen cue to make a response based on three particular trial types; (a) portion
size (b) interval since last ate and (c) spatial memory trials (see Figure 1 for details of trial-types). A
total of 150 trials, 50 trials of each type, were pseudo-randomly displayed, in one of two orders
(counterbalanced between participants) with no more than two of any one trial type in a row (Figure
A1, Appendix A). The 50 portion photographs used in the portion trial tasks were in 20 kcal
equicaloric steps either side of the true portion size of the test meal used in the study (600 kcal). Each
trial was presented for 4 s, with a mean ISI of 5.5 s (random jitter of between 38 s of fixation cross,
no more than two of any length in a row). The data captured when the fixation cross appeared was
used as the baseline fMRI measure (see analysis section below for details). Response mapping to
left/right button presses were counterbalanced between participants. Behavioural performance on
the task was measured by percentage of correct responses and response times (RT) for each trial type.
Figure 1. Example screenshots from memory for recent eating task(a) portion size memory trials,
in which participants indicated whether the portion on the screen was bigger or smaller than the
portion they ate for the meal (b) interval since last ate trials, in which participants indicated whether
the time on the screen was a shorter or longer time period than when they finished eating (c) spatial
memory trials, in which participants indicated whether the image on the screen was in the correct or
incorrect (mirror image) spatial layout of the environment in which the meal was consumed.
Prior to the study session, participants had been advised that the aim of the study was to assess
physiological responses to a meal. The main aim of studying the effect of eating rate was partially
disclosed from participants. The study session took place at CRIC Bristol utilising a novel protocol
(Figure 2). Participants were asked to abstain from eating or consuming calorie-containing beverages
for 12 h overnight before the session. On arrival, height (cm) was measured with shoes removed
using a stadiometer and weight (kg) was measured using electronic scales. Participants were asked
to complete baseline appetite and mood ratings using a VAS on the computer. A cannula was then
inserted and a baseline blood sample taken (for glucose, PYY and ghrelin).
a
b
c
Figure 1.
Example screenshots from memory for recent eating task—(
a
) portion size memory trials, in
which participants indicated whether the portion on the screen was bigger or smaller than the portion
they ate for the meal (
b
) interval since last ate trials, in which participants indicated whether the time
on the screen was a shorter or longer time period than when they finished eating (
c
) spatial memory
trials, in which participants indicated whether the image on the screen was in the correct or incorrect
(mirror image) spatial layout of the environment in which the meal was consumed.
Prior to the study session, participants had been advised that the aim of the study was to assess
physiological responses to a meal. The main aim of studying the effect of eating rate was partially
disclosed from participants. The study session took place at CRIC Bristol utilising a novel protocol
(Figure 2). Participants were asked to abstain from eating or consuming calorie-containing beverages
for 12 h overnight before the session. On arrival, height (cm) was measured with shoes removed
using a stadiometer and weight (kg) was measured using electronic scales. Participants were asked
to complete baseline appetite and mood ratings using a VAS on the computer. A cannula was then
inserted and a baseline blood sample taken (for glucose, PYY and ghrelin).
Participants were then asked to consume a 600 kcal meal of macaroni cheese (Tesco
®
Italian
Kitchen Macaroni Cheese Pasta, 1.78 kcal/g) in either 6 min (normal rate group) or 24 min (slow
rate group) by eating a mouthful, of specified size, cued by an audible bleep. The normal rate group
consumed a larger bite size (2 pieces vs. 1 piece of macaroni) with a shorter inter-bite interval (12 s
vs. 24 s). A larger portion (562 g, 1000 kcal) was presented to participants such that they never saw
the exact portion size consumed. The exact timing of each eating rate group was designed to ensure
that only 600 kcal was consumed. This was to test memory of the meal actually consumed rather than
relying on recall of the portion size seen on the plate (N.B. pilot work prior to this study showed that
presenting participants with the exact 600 kcal portion led to ceiling effects on the novel memory task).
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 5 of 23
To calculate the length of the meals in each condition, a separate group of eight participants (same
inclusion criteria) were asked to consume the meal without time constraints (mean time = 6 min).
The length of the slow meal was then extrapolated from this mean. Participants were provided with a
250 mL glass of water with their meal, and if they requested extra water they were provided with a
further 250 mL glass. Participants then completed the pleasantness and their desire to eat taste tests
(pre and post meal), and enjoyment and satisfaction ratings (post-meal only). Appetite ratings and
blood samples for measurement of PYY and ghrelin concentrations were collected half-hourly for 3 h
post-meal, along with a second glucose measurement at 120 min post-meal.
Nutrients 2018, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 5 of 24
Procedure
Figure 2. Study design.
Participants were then asked to consume a 600 kcal meal of macaroni cheese (Tesco® Italian
Kitchen Macaroni Cheese Pasta, 1.78 kcal/g) in either 6 min (normal rate group) or 24 min (slow rate
group) by eating a mouthful, of specified size, cued by an audible bleep. The normal rate group
consumed a larger bite size (2 pieces vs. 1 piece of macaroni) with a shorter inter-bite interval (12 s vs
24 s). A larger portion (562 g, 1000 kcal) was presented to participants such that they never saw the
exact portion size consumed. The exact timing of each eating rate group was designed to ensure that
only 600 kcal was consumed. This was to test memory of the meal actually consumed rather than
relying on recall of the portion size seen on the plate (N.B. pilot work prior to this study showed that
presenting participants with the exact 600 kcal portion led to ceiling effects on the novel memory
task). To calculate the length of the meals in each condition, a separate group of eight participants
(same inclusion criteria) were asked to consume the meal without time constraints (mean time = 6
min). The length of the slow meal was then extrapolated from this mean. Participants were provided
with a 250 mL glass of water with their meal, and if they requested extra water they were provided
with a further 250 mL glass. Participants then completed the pleasantness and their desire to eat taste
tests (pre and post meal), and enjoyment and satisfaction ratings (post-meal only). Appetite ratings
and blood samples for measurement of PYY and ghrelin concentrations were collected half-hourly
for 3 h post-meal, along with a second glucose measurement at 120 min post-meal.
An fMRI scan was performed 2 h post-meal. Neuroimaging took place at CRIC Bristol on a
Siemens 3T Magnetom Skyra MRI scanner using a 32-channel head coil. Participants were supine
with cushions around the head in the coil to restrict movement. A pulse-oximeter and respiratory
bellows were used to measure heart rate and respiration. Details of the acquisition can be found in
Appendix A. During this scan, the novel memory for recent eating task was performed (details
above). After the scan, following final blood collection and appetite ratings, participants were asked
to report how vividly they remembered consuming the meal using a VAS (How vividly do you
remember the lunch you ate earlier today?), and were asked to estimate the size of the portion they
had consumed using computerised images which each differed by 20 kcal (similar to the ideal portion
size task described in [30]). Finally, participants were offered an ad-libitum snack 3 h post-meal
consisting of 500 kcal of cookies and 500 kcal of crisps, and were advised to “eat until comfortably
full”. A further 250 mL glass of water was provided with this snack meal. At the end of the study
session, participants were asked to complete a free text awareness question (Please describe in as
much detail as possible what you believe the aim of this study was”).
2.5. Behavioural Data Analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (IBM SPSS Statistics 23, Armonk, NY, USA). A
priori, we decided to report effect sizes (Cohen’s d) due to the sample size of this feasibility study.
Reporting effect sizes as a statistical approach is advocated by numerous authors because of concerns
Figure 2. Study design.
An fMRI scan was performed 2 h post-meal. Neuroimaging took place at CRIC Bristol on a
Siemens 3T Magnetom Skyra MRI scanner using a 32-channel head coil. Participants were supine with
cushions around the head in the coil to restrict movement. A pulse-oximeter and respiratory bellows
were used to measure heart rate and respiration. Details of the acquisition can be found in Appendix A.
During this scan, the novel memory for recent eating task was performed (details above). After the
scan, following final blood collection and appetite ratings, participants were asked to report how
vividly they remembered consuming the meal using a VAS (“How vividly do you remember the lunch
you ate earlier today?”), and were asked to estimate the size of the portion they had consumed using
computerised images which each differed by 20 kcal (similar to the ideal portion size task described
in [
30
]). Finally, participants were offered an ad-libitum snack 3 h post-meal consisting of 500 kcal of
cookies and 500 kcal of crisps, and were advised to “eat until comfortably full”. A further 250 mL glass
of water was provided with this snack meal. At the end of the study session, participants were asked
to complete a free text awareness question (“Please describe in as much detail as possible what you
believe the aim of this study was”).
2.5. Behavioural Data Analysis
Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS (IBM SPSS Statistics 23, Armonk, NY, USA).
A priori, we decided to report effect sizes (Cohen’s d) due to the sample size of this feasibility study.
Reporting effect sizes as a statistical approach is advocated by numerous authors because of concerns
about null-hypothesis significance testing and because pvalues do not indicate the magnitude of any
reported effects [
31
33
]. Confidence intervals for the effect sizes, calculated using SPSS [
34
], were
included to allow interpretation at the population level, as recommended for feasibility studies [
35
].
For comparisons between the slow rate (experimental) group and normal rate (control) group we have
reported standard error and effect sizes, using the Cohen’s d formula as below:
Effect size =(mean of slow rate group)(mean of normal rate group)
Pooled standard deviation (1)
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 6 of 23
Descriptors of magnitude of effect size were 0.20 for small effect size, 0.50 for medium effect size
and 0.80 for large effect size [
36
]. For correlations, Pearson’s correlation coefficient was used and the
following descriptors were applied for the magnitude of the correlation coefficient with r= 0.10 for
small effect size, r= 0.30 for medium effect size and r= 0.50 for large effect size. Appetite ratings are
reported as change from pre-meal baseline to each time point (e.g., 120 min rating—pre-meal baseline).
These were calculated on an individual participant basis, by subtracting the participant’s baseline
score from each of the later time points.
2.6. FMRI Data Analysis
Details of the image pre-processing can be found in Appendix A. Time-series statistical analysis
was carried out using FMRIBs Improved Linear Model (FILM) with local auto-correlation correction
(pre-whitening) [
37
]. Explanatory variables (EVs) were added to the general linear model for each trial
type, with correctly answered trials and incorrect trials as separate regressors. Incorrect trials were not
modelled further in the analysis (proportion incorrect portion trials: normal 32%; slow 21%; interval
trials: normal 29%; slow 27%; spatial trials: normal 39%; slow 42%). Trials were modelled in two
ways (within a single design) by including EVs that accounted for portion, interval and spatial trials
versus baseline (a fixation cross) with a weighting of one, and a second set of EVs where the weighting
reflected the time taken to respond to each trial (regressors specified onset, duration and RT for each
trial type). This second set of regressors determined brain areas whose response was linearly related to
trial RT—and constitutes a parametric analysis [
38
]. These EVs provided additional understanding of
the processes underlying the three trial-types as the mean RT to the three trial-types were different.
The remaining six EVs modelled the incorrect trials in the same format as the correct trials (weighting
of one or with RT). Contrasts were defined to examine the response to each trial type modelled via
the different EVs, these contrast of parameter estimates (COPEs) were subsequently used to perform
second-level group analyses using two approaches. The first approach was a mixed effect analysis
in FEAT using FLAME (FMRIB’s Local Analysis of Mixed Effects stage 1) (details of registration in
Appendix A). For each COPE, this analysis estimated the whole sample mean, and unpaired t-tests
were conducted to estimate differences between normal and slow rate groups. Due to the risk of
false positives when analysing imaging data (when analyses are conducted on over 100,000 voxels in
the brain), some thresholding is necessary. The conservative approach taken here follows the recent
discussion about cluster-based thresholding [
39
] and employed a cluster threshold of z= 3.09, with a
corrected pvalue of 0.05. Reporting of cluster location used the FSL tool (Oxford, UK) ATLASQUERY
and associated script, AUTOAQ, based on three atlases in FSL: Cerebellar Atlas in MNI152 space after
normalization with FNIRT; Harvard-Oxford Cortical Structural Atlas; Harvard-Oxford Subcortical
Structural Atlas. The closest to estimates of effect size in fMRI data is to extract the percentage BOLD
signal change in the regions of interest and plot the values for each group. Note that the corresponding
results for parametric analyses reflect changes in the magnitude of the slope between BOLD and RT.
The second approach was a masked analysis using RANDOMISE, FSL’s tool for nonparametric
permutation inference on neuroimaging data [
40
]. Based on previous literature we defined a priori
anatomical regions of interest (ROI) for further analysis. The masks comprised two “networks”:
(i) food-related brain regions known from previous literature to respond to satiation and food
cues, including hypothalamus, amygdala, nucleus accumbens, striatum, insula and orbitofrontal
cortex [
19
,
20
,
22
,
23
,
41
]; (ii) memory-related regions known from previous literature to respond to
episodic memory, including hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, angular gyrus, frontal pole and
precuneus cortex [
17
,
18
,
42
]. Masks were created by thresholding the corresponding anatomically
defined brain areas in the Harvard-Oxford Cortical and Subcortical structural atlases in FSLview,
except the hypothalamus mask that was drawn by hand using the Atlas of the Human Brain [
43
] as
a guide.
The RANDOMISE analysis uses the COPEs for each trial type taken from the first level analyses
and transformed into standard space (details in Appendix A). Unpaired t-tests were conducted
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 7 of 23
between the normal and slow rate groups for the response in each of the chosen masks separately, and
significance determined using threshold-free cluster enhancement (TFCE) [
44
], and a FWE-corrected
value of p< 0.05. Clusters of more than 10 voxels were first reported without accounting for the
number of ROIs tested (11). In order to address this potential limitation, we performed additional
masked analyses combining all the masks for (i) the food-related regions, (ii) the memory-related
regions, and (iii) all masks combined. Unpaired t-tests were conducted between the normal and slow
rate groups for the response in each of these three combined masks, as above.
3. Results
3.1. Feasibility Outcomes
3.1.1. Combination of Novel and Technically Challenging Measures
It was possible to collect full data sets from the majority of participants, combining a range of
novel and technically challenging measures. Due to unforeseen staff circumstances, one of the study
participant’s study sessions was terminated part way through and therefore a further participant was
recruited (with a total of 11 participants in the normal rate group and 10 participants in the slow
rate group). Specifics details of data points collected for each measure are as follows: 100% (21/21)
of data points collected for ratings of baseline appetite, enjoyment, satisfaction, pre and post meal
pleasantness and desire to eat, appetite ratings at 0 and 30 min post meal, and ghrelin and PYY levels
at 0 and 30 min post-meal. 95% (20/21) of data points were collected for the novel memory for recent
eating task in the scanner, memory vividness VAS, portion size task, and post-meal appetite ratings
from 60 min time-point onwards. 91% (19/21) of data points were collected for glucose levels at 0 and
120 post-meal, ghrelin and PYY levels at 90, 120 and 180 min post-meal, and ad libitum consumption
of crisps and cookies. Finally, 86% (18/21) data points were collected for ghrelin and PYY levels at
60 min post-meal.
3.1.2. Novel fMRI Task
The novel fMRI task was successful; participants were able to perform the task and individual
participant scores were neither at floor or ceiling values (i.e., no participant scored the minimum or the
maximum score). As a result, it was possible to demonstrate differences between the two groups and
between different task types, which is promising for a future larger study.
3.1.3. Regions of Interest Using fMRI
Ability to measure imaging signal in the brain regions of interest was investigated through
examination of the first level maps for each participant. These showed that signal change was observed
in the regions of interest in the brain.
3.1.4. Blood Sampling Protocol
It was possible to cannulate all study participants on the first attempt. 115/126 (91.4%) blood
samples were successfully obtained, and excluding the participant whose study session was terminated
early due to external circumstances 113/120 (94.2%) of samples were obtained. Therefore, these young,
healthy adult volunteers were willing to have blood taken for research purposes.
3.1.5. Participants Awareness of the Aim of the Study
None of the study participants guessed that the aim of the study was related to eating rate and
therefore the actual aim of the study was concealed successfully; removing a potential source of bias.
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 8 of 23
3.2. Preliminary Results
3.2.1. Baseline Characteristics
Following the randomisation procedure, the groups were well matched for each of the variables
(Table 1). Baseline appetite ratings are reported in the Appendix B(Table A1).
Table 1. Baseline characteristics of normal and slow rate groups. (mean; SD).
Normal Rate Group Slow Rate Group
N11 10
Male/Female 6/5 5/5
BMI (kg/m2)21.8 (2.0) 21.4 (1.7)
Age (years) 23.4 (4.7) 22.7 (3.3)
DEBQ restraint 2.7 (0.5) 2.5 (1.2)
3.2.2. Post-Meal Outcome Measures
Pleasantness and desire to eat ratings performed after one mouthful of the macaroni cheese (taste
test), both before and after the meal are shown in the Appendix B(Table A2). The participants in the
slow rate group consumed more water (342 mL; S.D 126) than the normal rate group (257 mL; S.D 156)
with a moderate effect size (0.6) between the two groups. The normal rate group reported enjoying the
meal (57.5; S.D 21.6) more than the slow rate group (46.6; S.D 24.5) with an effect size of 0.5 (lower
CI =
0.4; upper CI = 1.3). The normal rate group also reported higher satisfaction levels immediately
post-meal (62.9.0; S.D 13.6) compared to the slow rate group (49.4; S.D 28.2) with an effect size of 0.6
(lower CI = 0.3; upper CI = 1.5).
3.2.3. Memory for Recent Eating Task
The slow rate group achieved a higher percentage of correct responses (effect size 0.4) and
responded more quickly (effect size 0.4) than the normal rate group on the portion size trials (Table 2).
There were only small differences between the two groups in their performance in the spatial trials
(effect size 0.2) and negligible differences in the interval trials (effect size 0.1). Response times to the
spatial trials were longer than for the other two trial types.
Table 2.
Percentage correct and response time on the memory for recent eating task (performed in MRI
scanner) (mean; SD).
Trial Type Normal
Rate Group
Slow Rate
Group
Effect Size
(Cohen’s d)
Lower
CI of d
Upper
CI of d
Portion % responses 67.6 (29.5) 79.0 (20.8) 0.4 0.4 1.3
Response time (ms) 1760 (376) 1631 (356) 0.4 0.5 1.2
Interval % responses 70.5 (30) 73.4 (22.7) 0.1 0.8 1.0
Response time (ms) 1822 (570) 1814 (373) 0.0 0.7 0.7
Spatial % responses 60.7 (5.7) 58.4 (13.3) 0.2 0.6 1.1
Response time (ms) 2258 (295) 2196 (419) 0.2 0.7 1.0
3.2.4. Portion Task and Vividness
Participants in the slow rate group reported remembering the meal slightly more vividly (71.3;
SD 8.0) compared to the normal rate group (64.9; SD 22.5), with a moderate effect size of 0.4 (lower
CI =
0.5; upper CI = 1.3). In the portion size task, participants in both the normal (
55.3 kcal; SD
156.4) and slow (
44.0 kcal; SD 131.6) groups remembered having consumed less food than they
actually had (effect size = 0.03; lower CI = 0.8; upper CI = 0.9).
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 9 of 23
3.2.5. Post-Meal Appetite Ratings
Immediately post-meal, the normal rate group reported a slightly greater change in fullness (44.9;
SD 14.5) than the slow rate group (39.8; SD 29.2), with a small effect size (0.2). At 120 min post meal,
the slow rate group reported a greater change in fullness (35.2; SD 19.0) than the normal rate group
(21.4; SD 17.8) with a large effect size (0.7), as shown in Figure 3. There were small or no differences in
effect sizes between groups for the other appetite variables, except for nausea for which the normal
rate group reported a greater increase in nausea post-meal than the slow rate group (effect size = 0.7)
(Appendix B, Table A3).
Nutrients 2018, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 9 of 24
3.2.5. Post-Meal Appetite Ratings
Immediately post-meal, the normal rate group reported a slightly greater change in fullness
(44.9; SD 14.5) than the slow rate group (39.8; SD 29.2), with a small effect size (0.2). At 120 min post
meal, the slow rate group reported a greater change in fullness (35.2; SD 19.0) than the normal rate
group (21.4; SD 17.8) with a large effect size (0.7), as shown in Figure 3. There were small or no
differences in effect sizes between groups for the other appetite variables, except for nausea for which
the normal rate group reported a greater increase in nausea post-meal than the slow rate group (effect
size = 0.7) (Appendix, Table B3).
Figure 3. Fullness ratings over time in normal and slow rate groups (error bars = S.E of the mean).
3.2.6. Ad Libitum Consumption
The normal rate group consumed both more cookies and crisps at the ad libitum meal,
consuming on average 144 kcal more (Table 3).
Table 3. Ad libitum energy intake (mean; SD).
Normal Rate Group
Slow Rate
Group
Effect Size
(Cohen’s d)
Lower CI
of d
Upper CI
of d
Cookies Consumed (kcal)
214 (115)
157 (122)
0.5
0.4
1.4
Crisps Consumed (kcal)
232 (117)
185 (136)
0.4
0.5
1.3
Total ad Libitum (kcal)
445 (215)
341 (240)
0.5
0.5
1.4
3.2.7. Blood Results
Glucose values were within the normal range both pre- and post-meal for all participants (Table
B6). Ghrelin suppression was greater in the slow rate group than the normal rate group, with large
effect sizes at 60 and 120 min post-meal (effect size = 0.8; Figure 4a, Table B4). Ghrelin levels at 180
min were correlated with ad-libitum intake (r = 0.59). PYY levels increased more from the baseline in
the normal rate group than the slow rate group, with moderate effect sizes at 30 and 90 min (effect
size = 0.6). (Figure 4b, Table B4).
0
10
20
30
40
50
60
030 60 90 120 150 180
Change in fullness rating (0 to 100)
Time post-meal (min)
Normal Slow
Figure 3. Fullness ratings over time in normal and slow rate groups (error bars = S.E of the mean).
3.2.6. Ad Libitum Consumption
The normal rate group consumed both more cookies and crisps at the ad libitum meal, consuming
on average 144 kcal more (Table 3).
Table 3. Ad libitum energy intake (mean; SD).
Normal Rate
Group
Slow Rate
Group
Effect Size
(Cohen’s d)
Lower CI
of d
Upper CI
of d
Cookies Consumed (kcal) 214 (115) 157 (122) 0.5 0.4 1.4
Crisps Consumed (kcal) 232 (117) 185 (136) 0.4 0.5 1.3
Total ad Libitum (kcal) 445 (215) 341 (240) 0.5 0.5 1.4
3.2.7. Blood Results
Glucose values were within the normal range both pre- and post-meal for all participants
(Table A6). Ghrelin suppression was greater in the slow rate group than the normal rate group,
with large effect sizes at 60 and 120 min post-meal (effect size = 0.8; Figure 4, Table A4). Ghrelin levels
at 180 min were correlated with ad-libitum intake (r= 0.59). PYY levels increased more from the
baseline in the normal rate group than the slow rate group, with moderate effect sizes at 30 and 90 min
(effect size = 0.6). (Figure 5, Table A4).
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 10 of 23
Nutrients 2018, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 10 of 24
Figure 4a. Ghrelin suppression over time in normal and slow rate groups.
Figure 4b. PYY change over time in normal and slow rate groups.
3.2.8. Neuroimaging Results
Whole-brain group analysis examined the areas associated with each trial type compared to rest
in the whole sample (n = 20, due to failure of imaging data acquisition in a single subject), and for
group differences (see Appendix, table B5).
Memory for portion size was associated with increased BOLD signal in temporal, parietal and
occipital cortices, as well as in the putamen. The normal rate group had a greater response in cuneal
cortex (part of the visual system) compared to the slow rate group. Memory for time since eating
(interval trials) was associated with increased BOLD response in the lingual gyrus, intracalcarine
cortex, occipital pole and putamen compared to rest, with no group differences to report. Spatial
memory was associated with an increased BOLD response in the occipital fusiform gyrus, extending
into the superior parietal lobule and occipital cortex, with no group differences to report.
-15
-10
-5
0
5
10
15
20
030 60 90 120 150 180
Change in ghrlein from baseline (pg/ml)
Time post-meal (minutes)
Normal Slow
-5
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
030 60 90 120 150 180
PYY change from baseline (pg/ml)
Time post-meal (min)
Normal Slow
Figure 4. Ghrelin suppression over time in normal and slow rate groups.
Nutrients 2018, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 10 of 24
Figure 4a. Ghrelin suppression over time in normal and slow rate groups.
Figure 4b. PYY change over time in normal and slow rate groups.
3.2.8. Neuroimaging Results
Whole-brain group analysis examined the areas associated with each trial type compared to rest
in the whole sample (n = 20, due to failure of imaging data acquisition in a single subject), and for
group differences (see Appendix, table B5).
Memory for portion size was associated with increased BOLD signal in temporal, parietal and
occipital cortices, as well as in the putamen. The normal rate group had a greater response in cuneal
cortex (part of the visual system) compared to the slow rate group. Memory for time since eating
(interval trials) was associated with increased BOLD response in the lingual gyrus, intracalcarine
cortex, occipital pole and putamen compared to rest, with no group differences to report. Spatial
memory was associated with an increased BOLD response in the occipital fusiform gyrus, extending
into the superior parietal lobule and occipital cortex, with no group differences to report.
-15
-10
-5
0
5
10
15
20
030 60 90 120 150 180
Change in ghrlein from baseline (pg/ml)
Time post-meal (minutes)
Normal Slow
-5
0
5
10
15
20
25
30
35
40
030 60 90 120 150 180
PYY change from baseline (pg/ml)
Time post-meal (min)
Normal Slow
Figure 5. PYY change over time in normal and slow rate groups.
3.2.8. Neuroimaging Results
Whole-brain group analysis examined the areas associated with each trial type compared to rest
in the whole sample (n= 20, due to failure of imaging data acquisition in a single subject), and for
group differences (see Appendix B, Table A5).
Memory for portion size was associated with increased BOLD signal in temporal, parietal and
occipital cortices, as well as in the putamen. The normal rate group had a greater response in cuneal
cortex (part of the visual system) compared to the slow rate group. Memory for time since eating
(interval trials) was associated with increased BOLD response in the lingual gyrus, intracalcarine
cortex, occipital pole and putamen compared to rest, with no group differences to report. Spatial
memory was associated with an increased BOLD response in the occipital fusiform gyrus, extending
into the superior parietal lobule and occipital cortex, with no group differences to report.
When response time (RT) was used to model brain activity (parametric regressors) across the
group, a linear relationship between RT and BOLD signal was found in the middle frontal gyrus during
portion size trials (Appendix B, Table A5). When comparing groups, a cluster in supramarginal gyrus
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 11 of 23
(parietal cortex) showed a greater response in the slow compared to the normal rate group during the
memory for portion size trials. Across the group during spatial trials, the inferior frontal and middle
frontal gyri showed a linear relationship between RT and BOLD signal, with no differences between
groups. There were no clusters to report for interval trials.
The masked analysis showed no difference between the normal and slow rate groups in the BOLD
response for the main effect (modelled with simple EVs) for the portion size, spatial or interval trials.
However, when using parametric regressors, differences between groups were found for the portion
trials in several masks, see Table 4. These regions all show the same pattern, whereby the slope of
the BOLD/RT relationship was steeper in the slow than the normal rate group (example shown in
Figure 6). For spatial trials with RT, the nucleus accumbens showed a greater response in the slow
rate group compared to the normal rate group. There were no clusters to report for interval trials. No
differences were seen in hypothalamus, hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, or frontal pole in any
of the contrasts.
Table 4.
Results of the masked neuroimaging analysis: size and peak co-ordinates of areas of differential
activation between normal and slow rate groups.
Contrast No. of Voxels Peak tValue MNI Co-Ordinates of Peak
x y z
Portion with RT
Normal > Slow
Slow > Normal:
Left OFC 32 5.6 46 22 8
Left OFC 10 4.06 28 18 18
Left Amygdala 37 3.65 20 14 10
Right Insula 12 5.26 38 4 12
Left Striatum (putamen) 108 4.36 24 4 8
Right Striatum (putamen) 83 4.38 30 18 6
Right Precuneus 24 4.05 8 54 50
Precuneus 20 3.21 0 50 44
Angular gyrus 119 3.93 46 54 46
Spatial with RT
Slow > Normal
Nucleus accumbens 20 3.38 12 12 10
NB. No differences survived for simple EVs modelling portion size, interval and spatial trials, or the parametric EVs
for Interval with RT or Spatial with RT Normal > Slow.
Nutrients 2018, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 12 of 24
(a)
(b)
Figure 5. (a) Cluster of voxels in the OFC showing a stronger relationship between the BOLD response
in this region shown and response times for portion size trials in the slow rate group compared to the
normal rate group. This region may show a strong association with memory for eating as linked to
task performance.(b) Mean % BOLD signal change for the analysis extracted for this region of the
OFC for each group (error bars show S.E. mean). The slow group show a steeper BOLD/RT
relationship in the region shown in (a).
4. Discussion
In this study, we sought to assess the feasibility of our novel paradigm that comprised unique
and technical challenging measures. Our objectives addressed the feasibility of several aspects of the
paradigm; all of which were shown to have positive outcomes, and therefore can be used to guide
the design of a future, fully powered study. Moreover, this paradigm was designed to gain a greater
understanding of mechanisms underlying the effect of eating rate on energy intake, both cognitive
and physiological. This combination of measures, tested in this feasibility study, provided
preliminary data to show that slower eating led to a greater feeling of fullness, increased ghrelin
suppression and a more vivid and accurate memory of the meal, yet in the context of reduced
enjoyment and satisfaction from that meal. Importantly, these effects were associated with a 25%
reduction in ad-libitum intake for those who ate their previous fixed meal more slowly. This
corroborates one previous study with a similar design [11], but is contrary to other studies of similar
design [7,8,45], which may in part be due to differing percentage manipulation of eating rate. This is,
therefore a relatively novel effect, as many of the previous studies in this field have measured the
effect of eating rate within a single meal or by comparing different food textures [3,4648].
In terms of feasibility, the study protocol was acceptable for participants and none of the
participants guessed the actual aim of the study, therefore successfully avoiding a potential source of
bias. It proved feasible to combine (i) behavioural measures, such as consuming a test meal, and
regular appetite ratings, with (ii) physiological measures, such as gastrointestinal hormones and
fMRI, in addition to (ii) cognitive measures, such as the memory for recent eating task. The blood
sampling protocol was acceptable to participants and demonstrated that young, adult, healthy
volunteers were prepared to have blood samples taken for research. The two conditions (normal and
slow eating rates) for consuming the meal were acceptable to participants and all participants
finished the test meal within the allocated time. The novel ‘what, where, when’ memory paradigm
utilised to evaluate memory for recent eating was understood by participants and responses were
not at ceiling. Performance on the “what” and “when” memory trials were similar, with a relative
reduced accuracy and longer response times on the ‘where’ memory trials. Trial types may not have
been well matched in terms of difficulty or spatial memory for a meal may not be as relevant as ‘what’
and ‘when’ in relation to memory for a recent eating episode. These issues will be taken forward into
developing the task for future studies.
There are a number of potential limitations of this feasibility study that should be considered in
the design of future, fully powered studies. First, we acknowledge that the small sample size renders
the effect size estimations imprecise and therefore we have been cautious in our interpretation of the
-1.00
-0.50
0.00
0.50
1.00
Normal Slow
BOLD/RT slope (arbitrary
untis)
Figure 6.
(
a
) Cluster of voxels in the OFC showing a stronger relationship between the BOLD response
in this region shown and response times for portion size trials in the slow rate group compared to the
normal rate group. This region may show a strong association with memory for eating as linked to task
performance. (
b
) Mean % BOLD signal change for the analysis extracted for this region of the OFC for
each group (error bars show S.E. mean). The slow group show a steeper BOLD/RT relationship in the
region shown in (a).
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 12 of 23
When the combined masks were analysed, clusters in the OFC (t= 5.6, x=
46, y= 22, z=
8),
putamen (t= 4.38, x= 30, y=
18, z= 6), and insula (t= 5.26, x= 38, y=
4, z= 12) survived correction
for the portion trials with response time modelled using the “food network” mask in the comparison
between the slow and normal eating rate groups. No clusters survived the analysis of the combined
“memory network” mask. With all masks combined, a cluster in the OFC remained (t= 5.6, x=
46,
y= 22, z=8; Figure 6).
4. Discussion
In this study, we sought to assess the feasibility of our novel paradigm that comprised unique
and technical challenging measures. Our objectives addressed the feasibility of several aspects of the
paradigm; all of which were shown to have positive outcomes, and therefore can be used to guide
the design of a future, fully powered study. Moreover, this paradigm was designed to gain a greater
understanding of mechanisms underlying the effect of eating rate on energy intake, both cognitive
and physiological. This combination of measures, tested in this feasibility study, provided preliminary
data to show that slower eating led to a greater feeling of fullness, increased ghrelin suppression and a
more vivid and accurate memory of the meal, yet in the context of reduced enjoyment and satisfaction
from that meal. Importantly, these effects were associated with a 25% reduction in ad-libitum intake
for those who ate their previous fixed meal more slowly. This corroborates one previous study with a
similar design [
11
], but is contrary to other studies of similar design [
7
,
8
,
45
], which may in part be due
to differing percentage manipulation of eating rate. This is, therefore a relatively novel effect, as many
of the previous studies in this field have measured the effect of eating rate within a single meal or by
comparing different food textures [3,4648].
In terms of feasibility, the study protocol was acceptable for participants and none of the
participants guessed the actual aim of the study, therefore successfully avoiding a potential source
of bias. It proved feasible to combine (i) behavioural measures, such as consuming a test meal, and
regular appetite ratings, with (ii) physiological measures, such as gastrointestinal hormones and fMRI,
in addition to (ii) cognitive measures, such as the memory for recent eating task. The blood sampling
protocol was acceptable to participants and demonstrated that young, adult, healthy volunteers were
prepared to have blood samples taken for research. The two conditions (normal and slow eating rates)
for consuming the meal were acceptable to participants and all participants finished the test meal within
the allocated time. The novel ‘what, where, when’ memory paradigm utilised to evaluate memory for
recent eating was understood by participants and responses were not at ceiling. Performance on the
“what” and “when” memory trials were similar, with a relative reduced accuracy and longer response
times on the ‘where’ memory trials. Trial types may not have been well matched in terms of difficulty
or spatial memory for a meal may not be as relevant as ‘what’ and ‘when’ in relation to memory for a
recent eating episode. These issues will be taken forward into developing the task for future studies.
There are a number of potential limitations of this feasibility study that should be considered in
the design of future, fully powered studies. First, we acknowledge that the small sample size renders
the effect size estimations imprecise and therefore we have been cautious in our interpretation of the
preliminary results. This will be borne in mind when calculating the sample size for future larger
studies. A priori, it was decided not to apply null hypothesis significance testing (NHST) to this data
set. While currently this may be unconventional in this field, this study was designed with feasibility
objectives in mind and therefore, in line with CONSORT guidelines for pilot and feasibility studies [
49
],
it would not be valid to perform NHST on the initial data due to the sample size. Secondly, combining
the memory and physiological measures was not without difficulties. A between-subjects design
was employed (as per [10]) to enable memory for recent eating measures to be included and to avoid
possible carry over effects of manipulating the meal within-subjects. However, this design may have
been disadvantageous for the hormonal measures, which can vary between individuals. Importantly,
all factors were measured relative to an individual’s baseline to account for within-session variability.
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 13 of 23
Careful design considerations are necessary to overcome complications of including cognitive and
physiological measures in one design.
Based on our feasibility assessment above, therefore, the recommendations for a future design
would be to first perform a series of power calculations for the main outcome measures to gauge the
level of sensitivity required for each measure and therefore to ascertain the number of participants
required to fully-power those measures. An example power calculation for two brain regions of
interest is provided in Appendix B. The challenge for future studies combining diverse measures will
lie in powering the whole study to the measure requiring the most sensitivity. The memory task would
benefit from some improvements, for example by developing “spatial” trials which enabled a similar
performance to the “what” and “when” trials. We would also recommend consideration as to whether
to employ the same between subject design as this feasibility study, or to utilise a within subject design
to enable potentially more accurate comparison of the two eating rates, whilst minimising the risk of
carry over effects.
The slow rate group reported feeling fuller from 30 min post-meal and persisted for the full study
duration of three hours. This supports our hypothesis and findings of previous studies [
6
,
10
,
50
,
51
],
and provides evidence that was previously lacking regarding the effect of eating rate in a fixed meal on
fullness ratings [
3
]. Although the slow rate group consumed more water than the normal rate group,
previous research has shown that this does not affect fullness or later ad-libitum consumption [
52
];
however this effect cannot be ruled out [46].
The normal rate group reported enjoying and feeling more satisfied by the meal, again supporting
our hypothesis and replicating previous findings [
5
,
11
]. This may explain in part why people consume
more if they eat more quickly, as it is a more enjoyable activity. This is an interesting finding however,
because it could be postulated that savouring one’s food rather than eating it more quickly might be
expected to increase enjoyment and satisfaction. Participants in the slow rate group in the study may
have found it frustrating being instructed to consume their food more slowly, and that undermined
enjoyment, including the enjoyment component of satisfaction. It is possible that there may be an
optimal eating rate for individuals, encompassed within a window of tolerance to change; and reducing
eating rate beyond that, reduces enjoyment and satisfaction from the meal (as seen here with this
experimentally slow condition) which was also suggested by a previous study [
53
]. Exploiting the
boundaries of this tolerance window may be important for the design of eating rate interventions.
On the other hand, tolerance to a slower eating rate might increase with repeated experience, so that
slower eating becomes the norm. This would worthy of investigation in future studies.
Slowing eating rate showed a large effect on ghrelin suppression, which supported our hypothesis
and corroborated the findings of a previous study [
9
]; however, other studies found no effect of
manipulating eating rate on ghrelin suppression [
6
,
7
]. There was a strong correlation between blood
ghrelin concentrations post-meal and ad libitum snack consumption. Accordingly, reduced appetite
stimulation by ghrelin may in part explain why participants who had eaten more slowly consumed
fewer snacks. This is also consistent with the finding that participants who had consumed the meal
more slowly reported feeling fuller subsequently (i.e., ghrelin is effectively a signal for an empty
stomach). The normal rate group showed a greater PYY response than the slow rate group which was
in contrast to what we had hypothesised and findings of previous studies [
6
,
7
,
9
] but in agreement
with another study [
8
]. These differences may in part be due to different methods used to manipulate
eating rate [
8
], different percentage alteration in eating rate and different hormone assays [
3
], and this
is clearly an area where further research is required.
At 3 h post meal, the slow rate group reported that they remembered the meal more vividly than
the normal rate group. This finding was in contrast to previous studies which found no effect of eating
rate on vividness [
10
,
11
], although the latter study showed that vividness of the memory of a meal was
negatively associated with subsequent ad-libitum intake [
11
]. Through the novel memory for recent
eating task, the slow rate group demonstrated more accurate memory for portion size, and responded
more quickly to the portion trials compared to the normal rate group; the latter of which has been
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 14 of 23
previously associated with participants being more confident in their answers and relying on memory
rather than guessing [54,55].
Our fMRI results demonstrate a linear relationship between the BOLD response and time to
respond to portion size trials in several brain regions, whereby a steeper BOLD vs. RT relationship
in the slow rate group might be linked to successful performance on the task. One alternative
interpretation of this finding is that improved performance on the memory for recent eating task
is associated with reward accompanying the sense of answering the trial correctly, and this might
be reflected in activity within reward related brain regions e.g., OFC, amygdala. However, this
explanation sits less well with activity in parietal cortex, which showed the same pattern. Memory for
portion sizes recently consumed may therefore require recruitment of areas such as the OFC, insula and
putamen, as well as precuneus, angular gyrus, supramarginal gyrus and middle frontal and temporal
gyri. Those regions showing a direct relationship with response time have a stronger association with
the memory processes recruited when recalling recent eating episodes. These regions are in keeping
with the areas sub-serving the object recognition task of Kwok et al. [
17
] (closest equivalent to the
portion size task in this study), but not in keeping with the hippocampal activity found associated
with the object task in Cheke et al. [
18
]. The lack of hippocampal activity in response to the current
memory for recent eating task maybe due to the lack of integration required [
18
], as memory elements
(portion size, interval and spatial aspects) were studied separately (as per design in [17]).
The fMRI data also reveals brain regions subserving memory for the spatial environment, or
context, in which the meal was consumed. Superior parietal and fusiform cortices were associated
with the spatial trials, which is in keeping with a similar spatial task in Kwok et al. [
15
]. Across both
groups, a linear relationship between BOLD signal and response time for spatial trials was observed in
the inferior and middle frontal cortices, suggesting that these regions are implicated in memory of
this kind.
In terms of future applications of this work, investigating whether or not these effects of
experimentally manipulating eating rate are also found in childhood would be beneficial, by repeating
this study in both normal weight and obese children, ideally with known genetic variability [
56
].
In view of alternative studies [
6
,
7
] that demonstrated responses for other satiety hormones (such as
glucagon-like peptide 1 and cholecystokinin), it would be informative to incorporate a wider range of
GI hormones into a further study to provide a more complete picture of the endocrine consequences of
slowing eating rate. In order to apply the findings of this research, further work is needed in order
to design effective interventions to manipulate eating rate on a long term basis, through a range of
methods such as environmental modifications [
57
], behavioural training from childhood [
58
] and
modification of food textures [
59
,
60
]. Our findings highlight the need to focus on preserving meal
enjoyment and meal satisfaction when reducing eating rate in behavioural interventions.
5. Conclusions
In conclusion, this study has provided the opportunity to test the feasibility of several aspects of
our eating rate paradigm. The design of future studies can now benefit from the recommendations
made based on this feasibility study. Moreover, the preliminary data highlights some of the neural and
hormonal pathways that may underpin (at least in part) the effect of slower eating rate on reduced
appetite and later eating. The slow rate group reported a greater increase in fullness after the meal
and demonstrated greater ghrelin suppression. In addition, the slow rate group recalled their meal
more vividly and accurately. Our fMRI data provide information about potential underlying neural
responses to a slower eating rate and how this might be related to improved memory for the meal.
The slow rate group also subsequently consumed 25% fewer snacks at an ad libitum meal three hours
later. Therefore, this study provides promising data for the role of manipulating eating rate on
subsequent consumption. However, our data also highlight a potential difficulty for interventions to
slow speed of eating. The slow rate group reported reduced enjoyment and reduced satisfaction after
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 15 of 23
their meal. Thus, calculating and then exploiting an individual’s tolerance to eating rate change may
be important for future interventions.
Author Contributions:
Conceptualization and design: K.H., E.H., D.F., P.R. and J.H.-S.; Formal analysis: K.H.,
E.H., J.B., J.H. and K.B.; Funding acquisition: K.H., E.H., P.R. and J.H.-S.; Data collection: K.H., P.T. and E.H.;
Project administration: K.H. and P.T.; Writing—draft, review & editing: K.H., E.H., D.F., P.R., P.T., J.B., J.H.-S., J.H.
and K.B.
Funding: This research received no external funding.
Acknowledgments:
K.H. was funded by an Elizabeth Blackwell Institute, Bristol, Clinical Primer Award and
J.H.-S., P.R. and E.H. are supported through the National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research
Centre and Unit Funding Scheme. The views expressed in this publication are those of the authors and not
necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care. D.F. and P.R. were funded
by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (Diet and Health Research Industry Club grant
reference: BB/L02554X/1). P.R. also receives support from the European Union Seventh Framework Programme
(FP7/2007–2013) under Grant Agreement 607310 (Nudge-it). We thank Sam Leary (statistician), Aileen Wilson
(radiographer) and staff at CRICBristol for their help, and Amanda Chong for data processing support.
Conflicts of Interest: The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Appendix A. Additional Methodological Details
Appendix A.1. Blood Sample Assays
Total active Ghrelin levels were measured by radioimmunoassay (RIA) according to protocol
recommendations using a standard curve of known concentration of purified 125I-labeled ghrelin
peptide (GHRA-88HK; EMD Millipore Corporation). No plasma dilution was applied when measuring
ghrelin levels. The coefficient of variance (CV) for intra-assay variability for was 5.2% and the CV for
inter-assay variability was 5.5%.
Total PYY levels were measured by radioimmunoassay (RIA) according to protocol
recommendations using a standard curve of known concentration of purified 125I-labeled PYY peptide
(PYYT-66HK; EMD Millipore Corporation). No plasma dilution was applied when measuring PYY
levels. The coefficient of variance (CV) for intra-assay variability was 3.3% and inter-assay variability
was 7.6%.
Blood samples for glucose were collected in sodium citrate bottles and processed the same
day using an enzymatic reference method with hexokinase utilising a UV test. The rate of
NADPH formation during the reaction is directly proportional to the glucose concentration and
is measured photometrically.
Appendix A.2. fMRI Data Acquisition
Functional MR images were acquired in one run using a multiband sequence [
61
,
62
] using the
following parameters: TR = 1000 ms, TE = 30 s, Flip angle = 70
, FOV = 192, number of slices = 42 with
20% gap, interleaved, voxel size 2
×
2
×
2.5 mm, multiband acceleration factor = 3, phase encoding =
A>>P, GRAPPA acceleration factor 2, bandwidth = 1930 Hz/Px. A maximum of 1800 volumes were
sampled over 30 min. Functional data were overlaid on a high-resolution T1-weighted MPRAGE
anatomical image for registration into standard space and functional localisation (TR = 2300 ms;
TE = 4.2 ms
; inversion time = 900 ms; flip angle = 9
; FOV = 224, voxel size = 1
×
1
×
1; 1 mm
isotropic voxels).
Pre-processing and statistical analysis of functional images was performed using FMRIBs Expert
Analysis Tool (FEAT) [
63
]. Standard pre-processing steps were followed: motion correction using
MCFLIRT [
64
], non-brain removal using BET [
65
], spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of
FWHM = 5 mm, mean-based intensity normalisation of all volumes, high-pass temporal filtering (cut
off 50 s). At first level, the extent of movement by each participant was checked. A movement exclusion
criteria of <5 mm over the scan was used; however none of the participants exceeded this. At the first
level, the PNM tool in FSL was used to create regressors to model the effects of physiological noise
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 16 of 23
within the general linear model in FEAT [
66
], temporal derivatives were also included. Registration to
high resolution and standard images (MNI “standard” brain, MNI152) was carried out using FMRIB’s
Linear Image Registration Tool (FLIRT) [
67
] and nonlinear warping (FNIRT; 5 mm warp spacing) [
68
].
Registration was optimised by using field-maps to correct for distortions in the EPI data during
pre-processing in FSL [69].
Appendix A.3. Trial Order for fMRI Task
Figure A1 shows the trial order for fMRI task.
Nutrients 2018, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 16 of 24
Pre-processing and statistical analysis of functional images was performed using FMRIBs Expert
Analysis Tool (FEAT) [63]. Standard pre-processing steps were followed: motion correction using
MCFLIRT [64], non-brain removal using BET [65], spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel of
FWHM = 5 mm, mean-based intensity normalisation of all volumes, high-pass temporal filtering (cut
off 50 s). At first level, the extent of movement by each participant was checked. A movement
exclusion criteria of <5 mm over the scan was used; however none of the participants exceeded this.
At the first level, the PNM tool in FSL was used to create regressors to model the effects of
physiological noise within the general linear model in FEAT [66], temporal derivatives were also
included. Registration to high resolution and standard images (MNI “standard” brain, MNI152) was
carried out using FMRIBs Linear Image Registration Tool (FLIRT) [67] and nonlinear warping
(FNIRT; 5 mm warp spacing) [68]. Registration was optimised by using field-maps to correct for
distortions in the EPI data during pre-processing in FSL [69].
Trial Order for fMRI Task
Figure A1: Trial order sets for fMRI task
Set A: 3, 2, 2, 3, 2, 1, 2, 3, 1, 2, 3, 3, 1, 3, 2, 1, 1, 2, 2, 1, 3, 2, 1, 3, 1, 1, 2, 1, 2, 3, 3, 1, 3, 2, 3, 3, 1, 3, 2, 1, 2,
3, 2, 3, 1, 2, 2, 1, 3, 1, 1, 3, 1, 3, 2, 3, 1, 2, 1, 3, 2, 3, 3, 1, 1, 3, 2, 1, 3, 1, 1, 2, 2, 1, 2, 3, 3, 1, 3, 2, 3, 2, 2, 1, 2,
1, 2, 1, 3, 2, 1, 3, 2, 3, 1, 3, 3, 2, 1, 2, 2, 1, 2, 1, 3, 1, 1, 2, 1, 2, 3, 3, 2, 1, 3, 1, 2, 3, 3, 2, 1, 2, 1, 1, 2, 2, 1, 1, 3,
2, 1, 3, 3, 2, 2, 1, 3, 3, 2, 3, 1, 3, 2, 3, 2, 3, 1, 1, 2, 3.
Set B: 2, 1, 3, 3, 2, 1, 3, 1, 1, 3, 2, 2, 3, 3, 1, 1, 2, 3, 2, 2, 1, 1, 2, 1, 2, 1, 2, 3, 1, 2, 1, 1, 3, 3, 2, 3, 2, 3, 1, 2, 1,
2, 3, 2, 3, 3, 2, 3, 1, 3, 3, 3, 1, 2, 3, 2, 2, 1, 3, 1, 2, 2, 3, 1, 2, 1, 3, 3, 1, 1, 3, 2, 2, 1, 2, 2, 3, 1, 2, 3, 2, 3, 3, 2, 2,
1, 1, 2, 1, 1, 3, 1, 3, 1, 3, 1, 3, 2, 3, 1, 2, 3, 3, 2, 3, 1, 2, 3, 1, 1, 2, 3, 1, 2, 3, 3, 1, 1, 3, 2, 3, 1, 2, 1, 1, 2, 1, 1, 2,
3, 1, 3, 1, 3, 2, 3, 1, 3, 2, 2, 3, 2, 2, 1, 1, 2, 1, 2, 3, 2.
(The numbers represent one of the three trial types (1 = portion; 2 = interval; 3 = spatial))
Appendix B: Additional Results
Table B1. Baseline appetite ratings (mean; SD).
Instruction
Normal Group
Slow Group
Hunger
I feel hungry
19.5 (15.3)
12.7 (9.0)
Fullness
My stomach feels full
71.3 (13.5)
74.7 (15.6)
Thirst
I feel thirsty
57.5 (18.4)
66.7 (13.5)
Desire to eat
How strong is your desire to eat RIGHT NOW?
69.1 (16.3)
72.5 (22.7)
Bloated
I feel bloated
14.0 (18.4)
23.7 (23.5)
Nauseous
I feel nauseous
17.5 (22.2)
39.2 (25.1)
Empty
I feel empty
53.8 (21.4)
57.0 (23.1)
Table B2. Taste tests.
Instructions
Normal
Group
Slow
Group
Effect Size
(Cohen’s d)
Lower
CI of d
Upper
CI of d
Desire to Eat
(pre)
Now look at the food in front of you.
How strong is your desire to eat, that is,
to taste, chew and swallow, the food
RIGHT NOW?
65.7
(19.4)
60.4
(19.4)
0.3
0.6
1.1
Desire to eat
more (post)
How strong is your desire to eat more
of the food RIGHT NOW?
30.5
(28.7)
42.8
(36.9)
0.4
0.5
1.2
Figure A1.
Trial order sets for fMRI task. The numbers represent one of the three trial types (
1 = portion
;
2 = interval; 3 = spatial).
Appendix B. Additional Results
Table A1. Baseline appetite ratings (mean; SD).
Instruction Normal Group Slow Group
Hunger “I feel hungry” 19.5 (15.3) 12.7 (9.0)
Fullness “My stomach feels full” 71.3 (13.5) 74.7 (15.6)
Thirst “I feel thirsty” 57.5 (18.4) 66.7 (13.5)
Desire to eat “How strong is your desire to eat RIGHT NOW?”
69.1 (16.3) 72.5 (22.7)
Bloated “I feel bloated” 14.0 (18.4) 23.7 (23.5)
Nauseous “I feel nauseous” 17.5 (22.2) 39.2 (25.1)
Empty “I feel empty” 53.8 (21.4) 57.0 (23.1)
Table A2. Taste tests.
Instructions Normal
Group Slow Group Effect Size
(Cohen’s d)
Lower
CI of d
Upper
CI of d
Desire to
Eat (pre)
“Now look at the food in front of
you. How strong is your desire to
eat, that is, to taste, chew and
swallow, the food RIGHT NOW?”
65.7 (19.4) 60.4 (19.4) 0.3 0.6 1.1
Desire to eat
more (post)
“How strong is your desire to eat
more of the food RIGHT NOW?” 30.5 (28.7) 42.8 (36.9) 0.4 0.5 1.2
Desire to eat
change NA 35.3 (22.2) 17.6 (49.9) 0.5 0.5 1.3
Pleasantness
pre-meal
“How pleasant does this food taste
in your mouth RIGHT NOW?” 79.5 (12.6) 75.9 (2.8) 0.3 0.5 1.2
Pleasantness
post-meal
“How pleasant does this food taste
in your mouth RIGHT NOW?” 43.7 (6.5) 46.8 (26.2) 0.1 0.7 1.0
Pleasantness
change NA 35.8 (12.6) 29.1 (22.7) 0.4 0.5 1.2
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 17 of 23
Table A3. Changes in mean (s.d.) appetite ratings from baseline immediately pre-meal.
Appetite
Rating
Time Post-
Meal (mins)
Normal
Group Slow Group Effect Size
(Cohen’s d)
Lower CI
of d
Upper CI
of d
Fullness 0 44.9 (14.5) 39.8 (29.2) 0.2 0.6 1.1
30 33.0 (24.6) 46.4 (22.8) 0.6 0.3 1.4
60 31.1 (30.4) 38.4 (25.8) 0.3 0.6 1.1
90 26.2 (16.7) 31.0 (22.4) 0.2 0.6 1.1
120 21.4 (17.8) 35.2 (19.0) 0.7 0.2 1.6
150 23.5 (27.1) 30.3 (29.4) 0.2 0.6 1.1
180 14.2 (10.4) 21.0 (21.1) 0.4 0.5 1.3
Hunger 046.7 (17.8) 38.3 (28.8) 0.4 0.6 1.1
30 46.9 (25.4) 40.4 (26.2) 0.2 0.6 1.1
60 40.3 (24.2) 39.1 (26.6) 0.0 0.8 0.9
90 34.6 (25.5) 38.1 (18.5) 0.2 0.7 1.0
120 30.9 (23.4) 35.8 (19.6) 0.2 0.7 1.1
150 28.9 (18.6) 30.9 (17.4) 0.1 0.8 1.0
180 18.2 (26.1) 21.2 (28.1) 0.1 0.8 1.0
Thirst 0 2.7 (21.2) 19.5 (28.4) 1.0 0.0 1.9
30 3.6 (21.8) 24.8 (30.2) 0.8 0.1 1.7
60 2.2 (15.3) 24.6 (26.9) 1.0 0.1 1.9
90 1.6 (23.0) 23.6 (24.7) 0.9 0.1 1.8
120 2.1 (15.7) 11.6 (25.2) 0.7 0.3 1.5
150 1.2 (16.3) 4.7 (18.0) 0.3 0.5 1.2
180 2.0 (17.6) 5.4 (23.1) 0.4 0.5 1.2
Desire to eat 0
50.22 (20.5)
32.3 (32.2) 0.5 0.4 1.3
30 42.9 (26.0) 37.8 (28.0) 0.2 0.7 1.0
60 41.1 (19.5) 42.4 (23.5) 0.1 0.8 0.9
90 36.5 (24.9) 40.4 (21.1) 0.2 0.7 1.0
120 30.7 (20.5) 28.6 (22.6) 0.1 0.8 1.0
150 24.5 (16.9) 28.5 (24.6) 0.2 0.7 1.1
180 16.9 (21.5) 17.9 (31.8) 0.0 0.8 0.9
Bloated 0 21.6 (25.4) 21.1 (28.3) 0.0 0.8 0.9
30 14.0 (18.3) 14.1 (29.4) 0.0 0.2 0.2
60 11.8 (16.3) 10.4 (27.8) 0.1 0.8 0.9
90 5.9 (20.5) 6.6 (26.8) 0.0 0.8 0.9
120 2.5 (18.1) 7.5 (25.3) 0.2 0.7 1.1
150 6.2 (25.7) 1.3 (25.9) 0.2 0.7 1.1
180 5.5 (13.6) 8.2 (14.5) 0.2 0.7 1.1
Nauseous 0 7.6 (24.8) 9.8 (21.0) 0.8 0.2 1.7
30 1.3 (27.5) 11.7 (26.1) 0.5 0.4 1.3
60 1.7 (16.8) 8.3 (24.2) 0.3 0.6 1.2
90 6.1 (16.0) 12.6 (24.7) 0.3 0.6 1.2
120 5.9 (20.9) 18.0 (25.0) 0.5 0.4 1.4
150 3.5 (29.5) 15.7 (21.3) 0.7 0.2 1.6
180 5.6 (18.6) 5.3 (17.3) 0.6 0.3 1.5
Empty 036.3 (21.3) 36.1 (29.7) 0.1 0.8 1.0
30 37.6 (19.4) 31.0 (33.0) 0.2 0.6 1.1
60 34.6 (18.0) 31.6 (36.1) 0.1 0.8 1.0
90 31.2 (20.2) 32.8 (27.8) 0.1 0.8 0.9
120 24.2 (23.6) 27.9 (28.4) 0.0 0.7 1.0
150 24.6 (18.0) 23.0 (29.9) 0.1 0.8 0.9
180 9.0 (18.5) 9.7 (33.1) 0.0 0.8 0.9
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 18 of 23
Table A4. Change in satiety hormone levels (ghrelin and PYY) from baseline (mean; SD).
Minutes
Post-Meal
Normal
Group Slow Group Effect Size
(Cohen’s d)
Lower CI
of d
Upper CI
of d
Ghrelin
30 5.2 (23.1) 7.7 (10.5) 0.7 0.2 1.6
60 3.2 (11.9) 6.4 (11.0) 0.8 0.1 1.8
90 0.03 (9.9) 6.2 (7.6) 0.7 0.2 1.6
120 3.2 (8.0) 3.8 (9.7) 0.8 0.2 1.7
180 4.7 (11.8) 1.0 (19.1) 0.2 0.7 1.2
PYY
30 26.9 (19.5) 17.4 (12.8) 0.6 0.7 1.2
60 22.4 (13.2) 17.9 (21.7) 0.3 0.7 1.2
90 24.7 (14.8) 14.3 (16.4) 0.7 0.3 1.6
120 34.2 (35.8) 17.6 (25.1) 0.5 0.4 1.4
180 6.7 (35.0) 0.2 (45.9) 0.2 0.7 1.1
Table A5. Whole-brain analysis Results.
Brain Region Cluster
No.
No. of
Voxels
Peak z
Value
MNI Co-
Ordinates of Peak
Portion
trials
x y z
Group mean (n = 20)
Occipital Fusiform Gyrus (10.4%) 5 8690 5.07 50 60 18
Precentral Gyrus (19.2%); Postcentral Gyrus (30.3%) 4 970 4.58 34 34 46
Left Putamen (51.6%) 3 277 4.42 26 8 6
Cingulate Gyrus, posterior division (71.4%) 2 189 4.38 0 30 34
Superior Parietal Lobule (43.9%) 1 94 3.73 32 54 66
Normal > Slow
Cuneal cortex (49.0%) 1 236 4.16 486 32
Slow > Normal 0
Interval
trials
Group mean (n = 20)
Intracalcarine Cortex (10.2%); Lingual Gyrus (14.9%);
Occipital Fusiform Gyrus (12.0%) 4 2713 5.26 8 88 2
Precentral Gyrus (18.1%); Postcentral Gyrus (30.1%) 3 1677 4.91 40 22 54
Juxtapositional Lobule Cortex (formerly
Supplementary Motor Cortex) (43.0%); Paracingulate
Gyrus (17.6%)
2 529 4.14 8 4 58
Left Putamen (92.0%) 1 109 4.13 30 6 2
Normal > Slow 0
Slow > Normal 0
Spatial
trials
Group mean (n = 20)
Peak in Lateral Occipital Cortex, superior division
(15.2%), extends to occipital fusiform gyrus, temporal
fusiform cortex, superior parietal lobule, and lingual
gyrus
1 10554 5.65 34 64 8
Normal > Slow 0
Slow > Normal 0
Portion
RT trials
Group mean (n = 20)
Middle Frontal Gyrus (23.9%); Precentral Gyrus (13.2%)
1 90 3.82 32 2 54
Normal > Slow 0
Slow > Normal
Postcentral Gyrus (41.2%); Supramarginal Gyrus,
anterior division (21.4%) 1 105 3.86 52 28 42
Interval
RT trials
Group mean (n = 20) 0
Normal > Slow 0
Slow > Normal 0
Spatial RT
trials
Group mean (n = 20)
Inferior Frontal Gyrus, pars triangularis (19.3%);
Inferior Frontal Gyrus, pars opercularis (19.4%) 2 298 3.89 50 36 2
Middle frontal gyrus 1 97 3.94 44 20 26
Normal > Slow 0
Slow > Normal 0
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 19 of 23
Table A6. Glucose levels at baseline and 120 min post-meal (mean; SD).
Time of Sample Normal Group Slow Group
Glucose Pre-meal 4.4 (0.2) 4.5 (0.4)
Post-meal (120 min) 5.3 (1.1) 4.8 (0.6)
fMRI Power Calculation
To help inform future studies examining similar processes using fMRI, we ran a power analysis
based on the methods developed by Mumford and Nichols [
70
] and implemented in the fMRIpower
software package (fmripower.org, Austin, TX, USA). This technique estimates the power (for a range
of sample sizes) to detect significant activation within specific regions of interest, with the assumption
that the planned studies will have the same number of runs per subject, runs of the same length,
similar scanner noise characteristics, and data analysis with a comparable model. The mask used here
included: hypothalamus, amygdala, nucleus accumbens, striatum, insula, orbitofrontal cortex, and
hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, angular gyrus frontal pole and precuneus cortex. The effect
sizes have been expressed in standard deviation (sd) units, which is analogous to the Cohens D
measure. The reported “power curves” reflect the power to detect a significant difference between
two groups with an unpaired t-test and corrected p-value of p< 0.05 within: (D1) the hippocampus
and (D2) the hypothalamus. These data reveal that a study performed with approx. 35 subjects would
have at least 80% power to detect an effect of size 0.59 sd units in the hippocampus, whereas in the
hypothalamus, with approx. 65 participants one would have 80% power to detect an effect of size
0.42 sd units.
Nutrients 2018, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 21 of 24
Glucose
Pre-meal
4.4 (0.2)
4.5 (0.4)
Post-meal (120 mins)
5.3 (1.1)
4.8 (0.6)
fMRI Power Calculation
To help inform future studies examining similar processes using fMRI, we ran a power analysis
based on the methods developed by Mumford and Nichols [70] and implemented in the fMRIpower
software package (fmripower.org, Austin, TX, USA). This technique estimates the power (for a range
of sample sizes) to detect significant activation within specific regions of interest, with the assumption
that the planned studies will have the same number of runs per subject, runs of the same length,
similar scanner noise characteristics, and data analysis with a comparable model. The mask used here
included: hypothalamus, amygdala, nucleus accumbens, striatum, insula, orbitofrontal cortex, and
hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, angular gyrus frontal pole and precuneus cortex. The effect
sizes have been expressed in standard deviation (sd) units, which is analogous to the Cohens D
measure. The reported “power curves” reflect the power to detect a significant difference between
two groups with an unpaired t-test and corrected p-value of p < 0.05 within: (D1) the hippocampus
and (D2) the hypothalamus. These data reveal that a study performed with approx. 35 subjects would
have at least 80% power to detect an effect of size 0.59 sd units in the hippocampus, whereas in the
hypothalamus, with approx. 65 participants one would have 80% power to detect an effect of size
0.42 sd units.
Figure B1. Hippocampus Figure B2. Hypothalamus.
References
1. Public Health England. Obesity and Healthy Eating; Public Health England: London, UK. 2015.
2. Ohkuma, T.; Hirakawa, Y.; Nakamura, U.; Kiyohara, Y.; Kitazono, T.; Ninomiya, T. Association between
eating rate and obesity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Obes. (Lond.) 2015, 39, 15891596.
3. Robinson, E.; Almiron-Roig, E.; Rutters, F.; de Graaf, C.; Forde, C.G.; Tudur Smith, C.; Nolan, S.J.; Jebb, S.A.
A systematic review and meta-analysis examining the effect of eating rate on energy intake and hunger.
Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2014, 100, 123151.
4. Shah, M.; Copeland, J.; Dart, L.; Adams-Huet, B.; James, A.; Rhea, D. Slower eating speed lowers energy
intake in normal-weight but not overweight/obese subjects. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2014, 114, 393402.
5. Ferriday, D.; Bosworth, M.L.; Evans, N.R.; Ciborowska, A.; Godinot, N.; Martin, N.; Rogers, P.J.; Brunstrom,
J.M. Are the effects of eating slowly on satiation and satiety anticipated in meal planning? Appetite 2016,
107, 681.
6. Kokkinos, A.; le Roux, C.W.; Alexiadou, K.; Tentolouris, N.; Vincent, R.P.; Kyriaki, D.; Perrea, D.; Ghatei,
M.A.; Bloom, S.R.; Katsilambros, N. Eating slowly increases the postprandial response of the anorexigenic
gut hormones, peptide YY and glucagon-like peptide-1. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2010, 95, 333337.
7. Karl, J.P.; Young, A.J.; Rood, J.C.; Montain, S.J. Independent and combined effects of eating rate and energy
density on energy intake, appetite, and gut hormones. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2013, 21, E244-252.
8. Karl, J.P.; Young, A.J.; Montain, S.J. Eating rate during a fixed-portion meal does not affect postprandial
appetite and gut peptides or energy intake during a subsequent meal. Physiol. Behav. 2011, 102, 524531.
9. Galhardo, J.; Hunt, L.P.; Lightman, S.L.; Sabin, M.A.; Bergh, C.; Sodersten, P.; Shield, J.P. Normalizing
eating behavior reduces body weight and improves gastrointestinal hormonal secretion in obese
adolescents. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2012, 97, E193E201.
Figure A2. Hippocampus
Nutrients 2018, 10, x FOR PEER REVIEW 21 of 24
Glucose
Pre-meal
4.4 (0.2)
4.5 (0.4)
Post-meal (120 mins)
5.3 (1.1)
4.8 (0.6)
fMRI Power Calculation
To help inform future studies examining similar processes using fMRI, we ran a power analysis
based on the methods developed by Mumford and Nichols [70] and implemented in the fMRIpower
software package (fmripower.org, Austin, TX, USA). This technique estimates the power (for a range
of sample sizes) to detect significant activation within specific regions of interest, with the assumption
that the planned studies will have the same number of runs per subject, runs of the same length,
similar scanner noise characteristics, and data analysis with a comparable model. The mask used here
included: hypothalamus, amygdala, nucleus accumbens, striatum, insula, orbitofrontal cortex, and
hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, angular gyrus frontal pole and precuneus cortex. The effect
sizes have been expressed in standard deviation (sd) units, which is analogous to the Cohens D
measure. The reported “power curves” reflect the power to detect a significant difference between
two groups with an unpaired t-test and corrected p-value of p < 0.05 within: (D1) the hippocampus
and (D2) the hypothalamus. These data reveal that a study performed with approx. 35 subjects would
have at least 80% power to detect an effect of size 0.59 sd units in the hippocampus, whereas in the
hypothalamus, with approx. 65 participants one would have 80% power to detect an effect of size
0.42 sd units.
Figure B1. Hippocampus Figure B2. Hypothalamus.
References
1. Public Health England. Obesity and Healthy Eating; Public Health England: London, UK. 2015.
2. Ohkuma, T.; Hirakawa, Y.; Nakamura, U.; Kiyohara, Y.; Kitazono, T.; Ninomiya, T. Association between
eating rate and obesity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Obes. (Lond.) 2015, 39, 15891596.
3. Robinson, E.; Almiron-Roig, E.; Rutters, F.; de Graaf, C.; Forde, C.G.; Tudur Smith, C.; Nolan, S.J.; Jebb, S.A.
A systematic review and meta-analysis examining the effect of eating rate on energy intake and hunger.
Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2014, 100, 123151.
4. Shah, M.; Copeland, J.; Dart, L.; Adams-Huet, B.; James, A.; Rhea, D. Slower eating speed lowers energy
intake in normal-weight but not overweight/obese subjects. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2014, 114, 393402.
5. Ferriday, D.; Bosworth, M.L.; Evans, N.R.; Ciborowska, A.; Godinot, N.; Martin, N.; Rogers, P.J.; Brunstrom,
J.M. Are the effects of eating slowly on satiation and satiety anticipated in meal planning? Appetite 2016,
107, 681.
6. Kokkinos, A.; le Roux, C.W.; Alexiadou, K.; Tentolouris, N.; Vincent, R.P.; Kyriaki, D.; Perrea, D.; Ghatei,
M.A.; Bloom, S.R.; Katsilambros, N. Eating slowly increases the postprandial response of the anorexigenic
gut hormones, peptide YY and glucagon-like peptide-1. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2010, 95, 333337.
7. Karl, J.P.; Young, A.J.; Rood, J.C.; Montain, S.J. Independent and combined effects of eating rate and energy
density on energy intake, appetite, and gut hormones. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2013, 21, E244-252.
8. Karl, J.P.; Young, A.J.; Montain, S.J. Eating rate during a fixed-portion meal does not affect postprandial
appetite and gut peptides or energy intake during a subsequent meal. Physiol. Behav. 2011, 102, 524531.
9. Galhardo, J.; Hunt, L.P.; Lightman, S.L.; Sabin, M.A.; Bergh, C.; Sodersten, P.; Shield, J.P. Normalizing
eating behavior reduces body weight and improves gastrointestinal hormonal secretion in obese
adolescents. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2012, 97, E193E201.
Figure A3. Hypothalamus.
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 20 of 23
References
1. Public Health England. Obesity and Healthy Eating; Public Health England: London, UK, 2015.
2.
Ohkuma, T.; Hirakawa, Y.; Nakamura, U.; Kiyohara, Y.; Kitazono, T.; Ninomiya, T. Association between
eating rate and obesity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Obes. (Lond.)
2015
,39, 1589–1596.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
3.
Robinson, E.; Almiron-Roig, E.; Rutters, F.; de Graaf, C.; Forde, C.G.; Tudur Smith, C.; Nolan, S.J.; Jebb, S.A.
A systematic review and meta-analysis examining the effect of eating rate on energy intake and hunger.
Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2014,100, 123–151. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
4.
Shah, M.; Copeland, J.; Dart, L.; Adams-Huet, B.; James, A.; Rhea, D. Slower eating speed lowers energy
intake in normal-weight but not overweight/obese subjects. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet.
2014
,114, 393–402.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
5.
Ferriday, D.; Bosworth, M.L.; Evans, N.R.; Ciborowska, A.; Godinot, N.; Martin, N.; Rogers, P.J.;
Brunstrom, J.M. Are the effects of eating slowly on satiation and satiety anticipated in meal planning?
Appetite 2016,107, 681. [CrossRef]
6.
Kokkinos, A.; le Roux, C.W.; Alexiadou, K.; Tentolouris, N.; Vincent, R.P.; Kyriaki, D.; Perrea, D.; Ghatei, M.A.;
Bloom, S.R.; Katsilambros, N. Eating slowly increases the postprandial response of the anorexigenic gut
hormones, peptide YY and glucagon-like peptide-1. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab.
2010
,95, 333–337. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
7.
Karl, J.P.; Young, A.J.; Rood, J.C.; Montain, S.J. Independent and combined effects of eating rate and energy
density on energy intake, appetite, and gut hormones. Obesity (Silver Spring)
2013
,21, E244–E252. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
8.
Karl, J.P.; Young, A.J.; Montain, S.J. Eating rate during a fixed-portion meal does not affect postprandial
appetite and gut peptides or energy intake during a subsequent meal. Physiol. Behav.
2011
,102, 524–531.
[CrossRef]
9.
Galhardo, J.; Hunt, L.P.; Lightman, S.L.; Sabin, M.A.; Bergh, C.; Sodersten, P.; Shield, J.P. Normalizing eating
behavior reduces body weight and improves gastrointestinal hormonal secretion in obese adolescents. J. Clin.
Endocrinol. Metab. 2012,97, E193–E201. [CrossRef]
10.
Ferriday, D.; Bosworth, M.L.; Lai, S.; Godinot, N.; Martin, N.; Martin, A.A.; Rogers, P.J.; Brunstrom, J.M.
Effects of eating rate on satiety: A role for episodic memory? Physiol. Behav. 2015,152, 389–396. [CrossRef]
11.
Higgs, S.; Jones, A. Prolonged chewing at lunch decreases later snack intake. Appetite
2013
,62, 91–95.
[CrossRef]
12.
Higgs, S. Cognitive influences on food intake: The effects of manipulating memory for recent eating.
Physiol. Behav. 2008,94, 734–739. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
13.
Brunstrom, J.M.; Burn, J.F.; Sell, N.R.; Collingwood, J.M.; Rogers, P.J.; Wilkinson, L.L.; Hinton, E.C.;
Maynard, O.M.; Ferriday, D. Episodic memory and appetite regulation in humans. PLoS ONE
2012
,7,
e50707. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
14. Higgs, S. Cognitive processing of food rewards. Appetite 2016,104, 10–17. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
15.
Scoville, W.B.; Milner, B. Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. 1957. J. Neuropsychiatry
Clin. Neurosci. 2000,12, 103–113. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
16. Rozin, P.; Dow, S.; Moscovitch, M.; Rajaram, S. What Causes Humans to Begin and End a Meal? A Role for
Memory for What Has Been Eaten, as Evidenced by a Study of Multiple Meal Eating in Amnesic Patients.
Psychol. Sci. 2016. [CrossRef]
17.
Kwok, S.C.; Macaluso, E. Immediate memory for “when, where and what”: Short-delay retrieval using
dynamic naturalistic material. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015,36, 2495–2513. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
18.
Cheke, L.G.; Simons, J.S.; Clayton, N.S. Higher body mass index is associated with episodic memory deficits
in young adults. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. (Hove) 2016,69, 2305–2316. [CrossRef]
19.
Hinton, E.C.; Parkinson, J.A.; Holland, A.J.; Arana, F.S.; Roberts, A.C.; Owen, A.M. Neural contributions to
the motivational control of appetite in humans. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2004,20, 1411–1418. [CrossRef]
20.
Burger, K.S.; Berner, L.A. A functional neuroimaging review of obesity, appetitive hormones and ingestive
behavior. Physiol. Behav. 2014,136, 121–127. [CrossRef]
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 21 of 23
21.
Lawrence, N.S.; Hinton, E.C.; Parkinson, J.A.; Lawrence, A.D. Nucleus accumbens response to food cues
predicts subsequent snack consumption in women and increased body mass index in those with reduced
self-control. Neuroimage 2012,63, 415–422. [CrossRef]
22.
Mehta, S.; Melhorn, S.J.; Smeraglio, A.; Tyagi, V.; Grabowski, T.; Schwartz, M.W.; Schur, E.A. Regional brain
response to visual food cues is a marker of satiety that predicts food choice. Am. J. Clin. Nutr.
2012
,96,
989–999. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
23.
Sun, X.; Veldhuizen, M.G.; Wray, A.E.; de Araujo, I.E.; Sherwin, R.S.; Sinha, R.; Small, D.M. The neural
signature of satiation is associated with ghrelin response and triglyceride metabolism. Physiol. Behav.
2014
,
136, 63–73. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
24.
Van Strien, T.; Frijters, J.E.; Bergers, G.P.; Defares, P.B. The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ)
for assessment of restrained, emotional, and external eating behavior. Int. J. Eat. Disord.
1986
,5, 295–315.
[CrossRef]
25.
Pocock, S.J.; Simon, R. Sequential treatment assignment with balancing for prognostic factors in the controlled
clinical trial. Biometrics 1975,31, 103–115. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
26.
Treasure, T.; MacRae, K.D. Minimisation: The platinum standard for trials? Br. Med. J.
1998
,317, 362.
[CrossRef]
27.
Turton, A.J.; O’Leary, K.; Gabb, J.; Woodward, R.; Gilchrist, I.D. A single blinded randomised controlled pilot
trial of prism adaptation for improving self-care in stroke patients with neglect. Neuropsychol. Rehabil.
2010
,
20, 180–196. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
28.
Rogers, P.J.; Hardman, C.A. Food reward. What it is and how to measure it. Appetite
2015
,90, 1–15. [CrossRef]
29. Rogers, P.J.; Ferriday, D.; Jebb, S.A.; Brunstrom, J.M. Connecting biology with psychology to make sense of
appetite control. Nutr. Bull. 2017,41, 344–352. [CrossRef]
30.
Wilkinson, L.L.; Hinton, E.C.; Fay, S.H.; Ferriday, D.; Rogers, P.J.; Brunstrom, J.M. Computer-based
assessments of expected satiety predict behavioural measures of portion-size selection and food intake.
Appetite 2012,59, 933–938. [CrossRef]
31. Cumming, G. The new statistics: Why and how. Psychol. Sci. 2014,25, 7–29. [CrossRef]
32.
Higgs, S. Manipulations of attention during eating and their effects on later snack intake. Appetite
2015
,92,
287–294. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
33.
Sterne, J.A.; Smith, G.D. Sifting the evidence-what’s wrong with significance tests? Phys. Ther.
2001
,81,
1464–1469. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
34.
Wuensch, K. Using SPSS to Obtain a Confidence Interval for Cohen’s d. Available online: http://core.ecu.
edu/psyc/wuenschk/SPSS/CI-d-SPSS.pdf (accessed on 5 October 2017).
35.
Lancaster, G.A.; Dodd, S.; Williamson, P.R. Design and analysis of pilot studies: Recommendations for good
practice. J. Eval. Clin. Pract. 2004,10, 307–312. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
36.
Cohen, J. CHAPTER 1—The concepts of power analysis. In Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences
(Revised Edition); Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1977.
37.
Woolrich, M.W.; Ripley, B.D.; Brady, M.; Smith, S.M. Temporal autocorrelation in univariate linear modeling
of FMRI data. Neuroimage 2001,14, 1370–1386. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
38.
Buchel, C.; Holmes, A.P.; Rees, G.; Friston, K.J. Characterizing stimulus-response functions using nonlinear
regressors in parametric fMRI experiments. Neuroimage 1998,8, 140–148. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
39.
Eklund, A.; Nichols, T.E.; Knutsson, H. Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated
false-positive rates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2016,113, 7900–7905. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
40.
Winkler, A.M.; Ridgway, G.R.; Webster, M.A.; Smith, S.M.; Nichols, T.E. Permutation inference for the general
linear model. Neuroimage 2014,92, 381–397. [CrossRef]
41.
Heni, M.; Kullmann, S.; Veit, R.; Ketterer, C.; Frank, S.; Machicao, F.; Staiger, H.; Haring, H.U.; Preissl, H.;
Fritsche, A. Variation in the obesity risk gene FTO determines the postprandial cerebral processing of food
stimuli in the prefrontal cortex. Mol. Metab. 2014,3, 109–113. [CrossRef]
42.
Spaniol, J.; Davidson, P.S.; Kim, A.S.; Han, H.; Moscovitch, M.; Grady, C.L. Event-related fMRI studies of
episodic encoding and retrieval: Meta-analyses using activation likelihood estimation. Neuropsychologia
2009
,
47, 1765–1779. [CrossRef]
43. Mai. Atlas of the Human Brain, 3rd ed.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2008.
44.
Smith, S.M.; Nichols, T.E. Threshold-free cluster enhancement: Addressing problems of smoothing, threshold
dependence and localisation in cluster inference. Neuroimage 2009,44, 83–98. [CrossRef]
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 22 of 23
45.
Zhu, Y.; Hsu, W.H.; Hollis, J.H. Increasing the number of masticatory cycles is associated with reduced
appetite and altered postprandial plasma concentrations of gut hormones, insulin and glucose. Br. J. Nutr.
2013,110, 384–390. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
46.
Andrade, A.M.; Kresge, D.L.; Teixeira, P.J.; Baptista, F.; Melanson, K.J. Does eating slowly influence appetite
and energy intake when water intake is controlled? Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act.
2012
,9, 135. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
47.
Zijlstra, N.; Mars, M.; Stafleu, A.; de Graaf, C. The effect of texture differences on satiation in 3 pairs of solid
foods. Appetite 2010,55, 490–497. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
48.
Zijlstra, N.; Mars, M.; de Wijk, R.A.; Westerterp-Plantenga, M.S.; de Graaf, C. The effect of viscosity on ad
libitum food intake. Int. J. Obes. (Lond.) 2008,32, 676–683. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
49.
Eldridge, S.M.; Chan, C.L.; Campbell, M.J.; Bond, C.M.; Hopewell, S.; Thabane, L.; Lancaster, G.A. CONSORT
2010 statement: Extension to randomised pilot and feasibility trials. Pilot Feasib. Stud.
2016
,2, 64. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
50.
Andrade, A.M.; Greene, G.W.; Melanson, K.J. Eating slowly led to decreases in energy intake within meals in
healthy women. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2008,108, 1186–1191. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
51.
Rigamonti, A.E.; Agosti, F.; Compri, E.; Giunta, M.; Marazzi, N.; Muller, E.E.; Cella, S.G.; Sartorio, A.
Anorexigenic postprandial responses of PYY and GLP1 to slow ice cream consumption: Preservation in
obese adolescents, but not in obese adults. Eur. J. Endocrinol. 2013,168, 429–436. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
52.
Rolls, B.J.; Bell, E.A.; Thorwart, M.L. Water incorporated into a food but not served with a food decreases
energy intake in lean women. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1999,70, 448–455. [CrossRef]
53.
Wilkinson, L.; Ferriday, D.B.; Bosworth, M.; Godinot, N.; Martin, N.; Rogers, P.; Brunstrom, J. Keeping Pace
with Your Eating: Visual Feedback Affects Eating Rate in Humans. PLoS ONE 2017. [CrossRef]
54.
Wixted, J.T. Remember/Know judgments in cognitive neuroscience: An illustration of the underrepresented
point of view. Learn. Mem. 2009,16, 406–412. [CrossRef]
55.
Wixted, J.T.; Mickes, L. A continuous dual-process model of remember/know judgments. Psychol. Rev.
2010
,
117, 1025–1054. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
56.
Llewellyn, C.; Wardle, J. Behavioral susceptibility to obesity: Gene-environment interplay in the development
of weight. Physiol. Behav. 2015,152, 494–501. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
57.
Waters, E.; de Silva-Sanigorski, A.; Hall, B.J.; Brown, T.; Campbell, K.J.; Gao, Y.; Armstrong, R.; Prosser, L.;
Summerbell, C.D. Interventions for preventing obesity in children. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev.
2011
.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
58.
Zandian, M.; Bergh, C.; Ioakimidis, I.; Esfandiari, M.; Shield, J.; Lightman, S.; Leon, M.; Sodersten, P. Control
of Body Weight by Eating Behavior in Children. Front. Pediatr. 2015,3, 89. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
59.
Forde, C.G.; van Kuijk, N.; Thaler, T.; de Graaf, C.; Martin, N. Texture and savoury taste influences on food
intake in a realistic hot lunch time meal. Appetite 2013,60, 180–186. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
60.
McCrickerd, K.; Lim, C.M.; Leong, C.; Chia, E.M.; Forde, C.G. Texture-Based Differences in Eating Rate
Reduce the Impact of Increased Energy Density and Large Portions on Meal Size in Adults. J. Nutr.
2017
,
147, 1208–1217. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
61.
Moeller, S.; Yacoub, E.; Olman, C.A.; Auerbach, E.; Strupp, J.; Harel, N.; Ugurbil, K. Multiband multislice
GE-EPI at 7 tesla, with 16-fold acceleration using partial parallel imaging with application to high spatial
and temporal whole-brain fMRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 2010,63, 1144–1153. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
62.
Feinberg, D.A.; Moeller, S.; Smith, S.M.; Auerbach, E.; Ramanna, S.; Gunther, M.; Glasser, M.F.; Miller, K.L.;
Ugurbil, K.; Yacoub, E. Multiplexed echo planar imaging for sub-second whole brain FMRI and fast diffusion
imaging. PLoS ONE 2010,5, e15710. [CrossRef]
63.
Woolrich, M.W.; Jbabdi, S.; Patenaude, B.; Chappell, M.; Makni, S.; Behrens, T.; Beckmann, C.; Jenkinson, M.;
Smith, S.M. Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. Neuroimage 2009,45, S173–S186. [CrossRef]
64.
Jenkinson, M.; Bannister, P.; Brady, M.; Smith, S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear
registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage 2002,17, 825–841. [CrossRef]
65.
Smith, S.M. Fast robust automated brain extraction. Hum. Brain Mapp.
2002
,17, 143–155. [CrossRef]
[PubMed]
66.
Brooks, J.C.; Beckmann, C.F.; Miller, K.L.; Wise, R.G.; Porro, C.A.; Tracey, I.; Jenkinson, M. Physiological
noise modelling for spinal functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Neuroimage
2008
,39, 680–692.
[CrossRef] [PubMed]
Nutrients 2019,11, 50 23 of 23
67.
Jenkinson, M.; Smith, S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images.
Med. Image Anal. 2001,5, 143–156. [CrossRef]
68.
Andersson, J.L.; Jenkinson, M.; Smith, S. Non-linear registration, aka spatial normalisation. FMRIB
2010
,2,
1–21.
69.
Greve, D.N.; Fischl, B. Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration.
Neuroimage 2009,48, 63–72. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
70.
Mumford, J.A.; Nichols, T.E. Power calculation for group fMRI studies accounting for arbitrary design and
temporal autocorrelation. Neuroimage 2008,39, 261–268. [CrossRef]
©
2018 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access
article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution
(CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
... Overall, in laboratory conditions, faster eating rate is associated with the higher energy intake (8). During a slower eating rate condition, individuals reported greater fullness compared with their "normal" eating pace (14,15); notably the normal eating rate group endorsed greater enjoyment and satisfaction from the meal than the slower rate group (14). Eating rate was not associated with hunger ratings at the end of the meal or hours later and did not impact subsequent ad libitum snack intake (8,15). ...
... Overall, in laboratory conditions, faster eating rate is associated with the higher energy intake (8). During a slower eating rate condition, individuals reported greater fullness compared with their "normal" eating pace (14,15); notably the normal eating rate group endorsed greater enjoyment and satisfaction from the meal than the slower rate group (14). Eating rate was not associated with hunger ratings at the end of the meal or hours later and did not impact subsequent ad libitum snack intake (8,15). ...
... One mechanism behind the association may be via an impact on satiety (8). Specifically, eating rate may directly impact satiety hormones: slower eating results in greater ghrelin suppression after meals (14). The rate of eating may effect stomach distention due to the rate of gastric emptying, which may, in turn, influence feelings of satiety. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background A fast rate of eating is associated with a higher risk for obesity but existing studies are limited by reliance on self-report and the consistency of eating rate has not been examined across all meals in a day. The goal of the current analysis was to examine associations between meal duration, rate of eating, and body mass index (BMI) and to assess the variance of meal duration and eating rate across different meals during the day. Methods Using an observational cross-sectional study design, non-smoking participants aged 18–45 years ( N = 29) consumed all meals (breakfast, lunch, and dinner) on a single day in a pseudo free-living environment. Participants were allowed to choose any food and beverages from a University food court and consume their desired amount with no time restrictions. Weighed food records and a log of meal start and end times, to calculate duration, were obtained by a trained research assistant. Spearman's correlations and multiple linear regressions examined associations between BMI and meal duration and rate of eating. Results Participants were 65% male and 48% white. A shorter meal duration was associated with a higher BMI at breakfast but not lunch or dinner, after adjusting for age and sex ( p = 0.03). Faster rate of eating was associated with higher BMI across all meals ( p = 0.04) and higher energy intake for all meals ( p < 0.001). Intra-individual rates of eating were not significantly different across breakfast, lunch, and dinner ( p = 0.96). Conclusion Shorter beakfast and a faster rate of eating across all meals were associated with higher BMI in a pseudo free-living environment. An individual's rate of eating is constant over all meals in a day. These data support weight reduction interventions focusing on the rate of eating at all meals throughout the day and provide evidence for specifically directing attention to breakfast eating behaviors.
... Fast eating is associated with high body weight [8], and interventions to reduce the eating rate seem to enhance weight loss [9]. Eating food quickly may contribute to blunted responses to normal satiety signals, whereby an individual does not respond to gastric distension and gut peptide release, so that normal appetite suppression pathways do not function as expected during fast eating occasions [10,11]. A reduction in the eating rate aimed at reducing portion size and normalizing satiety signaling has been recently studied [10][11][12][13][14][15][16]. ...
... Eating food quickly may contribute to blunted responses to normal satiety signals, whereby an individual does not respond to gastric distension and gut peptide release, so that normal appetite suppression pathways do not function as expected during fast eating occasions [10,11]. A reduction in the eating rate aimed at reducing portion size and normalizing satiety signaling has been recently studied [10][11][12][13][14][15][16]. A study of adolescents aged 9 to 17 years found that an eating rate intervention enhanced weight loss at 12 months compared with usual care (change in BMI z-score of −0.27) [16]. ...
... A study of adolescents aged 9 to 17 years found that an eating rate intervention enhanced weight loss at 12 months compared with usual care (change in BMI z-score of −0.27) [16]. Slowing eating rate can also reduce self-selected portion size with no reduction in postmeal satiety levels among children and teenagers [10,13,16]. A recent review appraised a number of commercial apps targeting appetite regulation [17]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Mobile health (mHealth) platforms have become increasingly popular for delivering health interventions in recent years and particularly in light of the COVID-19 pandemic. Childhood obesity treatment is an area where mHealth interventions may be useful due to the multidisciplinary nature of interventions and the need for long-term care. Many mHealth apps targeting youth exist but the evidence base underpinning the methods for assessing technical usability, user engagement and user satisfaction of such apps with target end-users or among clinical populations is unclear, including for those aimed at paediatric overweight and obesity management. This review aims to examine the current literature and provide an overview of the scientific methods employed to test usability and engagement with mHealth apps in children and adolescents with obesity. A narrative literature review was undertaken following a systematic search. Four academic databases were searched. Inclusion criteria were studies describing the usability of mHealth interventions for childhood obesity treatment. Following the application of inclusion and exclusion criteria, fifty-nine articles were included for full-text review, and seven studies met the criteria for usability and engagement in a clinical paediatric population with obesity. Six apps were tested for usability and one for engagement in childhood obesity treatment. Sample sizes ranged from 6-1120 participants. The included studies reported several heterogenous measurement instruments, data collection approaches, and outcomes. Recommendations for future research include the standardization and validation of instruments to measure usability and engagement within mHealth studies in this population.
... Research shows that slower eating is associated with lower food intake (Robinson et al., 2014a). This may be because it increases orosensory exposure (i.e. the length of time that food is in the mouth) which promotes the release of gut hormones that reduce appetite (Hawton et al., 2019;Krop et al., 2018). Thus, asking people to attend to the sensory properties of their food as they eat could lead to reduced energy intake due to a slowed rate of eating and increased feelings of satiation. ...
... It would also mean that effects could be maximised by focussing on taste and texture rather than sight and smell. Although other types of interventions have been used to try to slow eating rate (Robinson et al., 2014a), these can be associated with reduced pleasure (Hawton et al., 2019). Given that mindful eating may increase food enjoyment (Seguias & Tapper, 2022), it may therefore represent a more acceptable and sustainable way of promoting slowed eating. ...
Article
Full-text available
Mindful eating is increasingly being used to try to promote healthy eating and weight management. However, the term refers to a diverse set of practices that could have quite different effects on behaviour. This narrative review provides a guide to the concept of mindful eating as well as a comprehensive overview of research in the area. This includes the ways in which mindful eating has been operationalised and measured as well as evidence for effects and potential mechanisms of action. The research reviewed suggests that multi‐component mindfulness‐based interventions may be beneficial for disordered eating and weight management, but it is unclear whether these benefits exceed those obtained by alternative treatments. Some studies suggest that specific mindful eating strategies may have immediate effects on eating, but more research is needed to reach any definitive conclusions. These studies also suggest that effects may vary depending on the characteristics of the individual and/or the specific eating context. As such, research may ultimately point towards a more personalised approach to the application of mindful eating in order to maximise benefits. Finally, mindful eating interventions for children represent a relatively new area of research and there is currently insufficient evidence to draw any firm conclusions about their value. To advance both our understanding and effective application of mindful eating, more experimental research with high levels of methodological rigour is needed as well as research that explores underpinning mechanisms of action.
... Many experimental studies have found that eating speed is correlated with obesity risk [10]. In a feasibility study in 21 participants randomly assigned to consume a 600 kcal meal at either a "normal" or "slow" rate (6 versus 24 min), Hawton et al. showed that slow eating could influence satiety, appetite and play a role in hormonal pathways [12]. Taking this evidence into account, an increasing number of studies have examined the interaction between eating speed and metabolic syndrome, diabetes, non-alcoholic fatty liver disease and cardiovascular disease [13][14][15]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Chronotype is a reflection of an individual’s preference for sleeping, eating and activity times over a 24 h period. Based on these circadian preferences, three chronotype categories have been identified: morning (MC) (lark), intermediate (IC) and evening (EC) (owl). Chronotype categories have been reported to influence dietary habits; subjects with EC are more prone to follow unhealthy diets. In order to better characterize the eating habits of subjects with obesity belonging to three different chronotype categories, we investigated eating speed during the three main meals in a population of subjects with overweight/obesity. For this purpose, we included 81 subjects with overweight/obesity (aged 46.38 ± 16.62 years; BMI 31.48 ± 7.30 kg/m2) in a cross-sectional, observational study. Anthropometric parameters and lifestyle habits were studied. Chronotype score was assessed using the Morningness–Eveningness questionnaire (based on their scores, subjects were categorized as MC, IC or EC). To investigate the duration of main meals, a dietary interview by a qualified nutritionist was conducted. Subjects with MC spend significantly more time on lunch than subjects with EC (p = 0.017) and significantly more time on dinner than subjects with IC (p = 0.041). Furthermore, the chronotype score correlated positively with the minutes spent at lunch (p = 0.001) and dinner (p = 0.055, trend toward statistical significance). EC had a fast eating speed and this, in addition to better characterizing the eating habits of this chronotype category, could also contribute to the risk of developing obesity-related cardiometabolic diseases.
... 3 Previous studies have shown that eating rates can impact the overall energy intake of an individual, and a 20% change in eating rate can alter the energy intake by between 10% and 13%, 50 and profiling eating rate may be a useful way to assess the effects that eating rate has on energy intake. 51 People who consume foods at a slower rate tend to have reduced energy intake, 3,52 and people who eat at faster rates have higher energy consumption 37,53 and may be at risk of overconsumption. Furthermore, faster eating rates have been associated with higher body mass 54,55 and risk factors for chronic diseases. ...
Article
Ingestive behaviors (IBs) (eg, bites, chews, oral processing, swallows, pauses) have meaningful roles in enhancing satiety, promoting fullness, and decreasing food consumption, and thus may be an underused strategy for obesity prevention and treatment. Limited IB monitoring research has been conducted because of a lack of accurate automated measurement capabilities outside laboratory settings. Self-report methods are used, but they have questionable validity and reliability. This paper aimed to present a conceptual model in which IB, specifically slow eating, supported by technological advancements, contributes to controlling hedonic and homeostatic processes, providing an opportunity to reduce energy intake, and improve health outcomes.
... This is especially true given their observed importance in the literature as will be discussed. In terms of speed feeding, an association has been reported between the rapid rate of eating, overconsumption, and overweight [26,27]; perhaps because of missing the satiety awareness. This complex process involves neuro-humoral signaling from the stomach to the central nervous system indicating the cue for intake cessation [28,29], which typically takes 20 minutes from the start of a meal. ...
Article
Introduction Maladaptive eating behaviors are emerging as the most significant determinants of obesity with a promising role in intervention. In the absence of a standardized tool to assess eating variations, an Eating Error Score (EES) tool was devised which comprised five zones for evaluating the severity of obesogenic behaviors as well as the specific area(s) with the highest susceptibility. This pilot study was aimed at evaluating the effectiveness of the EES in quantitating the eating behavior errors associated with excess weight and identifying the most affected zones. Methods The EES questionnaire was designed to explore potential disturbances in five zones of eating behavior related to the impetus to eat (Munger), meal choices and attentiveness to cravings (Impulsive), consumption speed (Speed feeding), cues to stop ingestion (Indulgent) and the social aspect of eating (Relationship). The questionnaire was conducted on adults with varying body mass index (BMI) attending governmental outpatient clinics. The correlation between EES and BMI was determined through Pearson Coefficient. Results A total of 204 participants completed the EES questionnaire. There were 72 males and 132 females with a mean BMI of 27.63 ± 6.16 kg/m2 and with nearly equal distribution between normal weight (37.2%), overweight (32.4%), and obese (29.4%) individuals. Nearly 75% of our cohort had a moderate total EES, and the remainder was equally distributed between the mild and severe ranges. A weak but significant correlation was observed between total EES and BMI (r=0.275, p<0.001) suggesting increasing obesogenic styles in participants with excess weight. In addition, a similar weak but significant correlation was noted between Body Mass Index and the Munger and Impulsive zones (r=0.266 and 0.258 and p<0.001, respectively) suggesting more severe maladaptive eating behaviors in these areas. No correlation was found with the Speed feeding, Indulgent, and Relationship zones. Conclusion The EES may be a useful tool for assessing the extent of maladaptive eating behaviors, which predispose individuals to weight gain and sabotage their weight loss efforts. Undoubtedly, the utility of the tool needs to be corroborated in large population studies. Further, identifying the specific operant zones may show promise as many of these habits are potentially modifiable and can be targeted for weight control, most notably those associated with the Munger and Impulsive zones.
... Independently of the mechanism postulated in literature, many experimental studies have found that speed eating correlates to obesity risk [10][11][12]38]. Hawton et al. demonstrated that slower eating might affect fullness, appetite and have a role in hormonal pathways [39]. The "eating speed" mechanism has a strong relation with mastication, which may also affect energy intake and body weight, increasing the food surface to facilitate digestion, satiety and hormone response. ...
Article
Full-text available
Eating speed (ES) as a dietary behaviour has become a widely discussed factor for weight management and obesity. This study analysed the relationship between ES and anthropometric indicators of obesity, including BMI and waist circumference (WC) in adults. A search conducted of PubMed, Web of Science, Science Direct and Scopus found six longitudinal studies and fifteen cross-sectional studies published for further analysis. A quality assessment was performed with the MINORS checklist. Eight studies were included in the meta-analysis and almost all reviewed studies showed that ES was associated with BMI, and non-fast eaters had significantly lower BMI than fast eaters. Therefore, it was assumed that slowing down the ES may be an effective strategy for weight management and lowering obesity risk. There was also an association between WC and ES. Assessment of eating speed can be included in nutrition surveys to analyse obesity risk. More broadly, research is also needed to establish a validated and standardised methodology to determine eating speed. Further research needs to examine the links between eating speed, obesity, ethnicity, sex, food culture and chronic diseases.
Article
Slow eating is associated with lower body mass index (BMI), enhanced satiety, and reduced food intake in laboratory settings. This study developed and tested a 5-week slow-eating intervention, delivered either through individual or small group weekly meetings, in women with overweight and obesity. Women (n = 65; 20.5 ± 3.6 years; BMI 31.3 ± 2.7 kg/m2) were assigned to experimental or parallel non-treatment control. Main outcomes, measured pre- and post-intervention, included eating rate, meal duration, and energy intake during a standardized meal served on a universal eating monitor. Exploratory outcomes included Weight Related Eating Questionnaire (WREQ), Intuitive Eating Scale (IES), and Mindful Eating Questionnaire (MEQ) scores. All women in the experimental group underwent the same slow-eating intervention, but half had individual sessions while the other half had small group sessions. No differences were seen for any outcomes between session modalities, so experimental data were pooled (n = 25) and compared to control data (n = 25). Time-by-group interactions showed reduced eating rate (F(1,48df) = 13.04, η2 = 0.214, p = .001) and increased meal duration (F(1,48df) = 7.949, η2 = 0.142, p = .007) in the experimental group compared to the control group but change in energy intake was not significant (F(1,48df) = 3.298, η2 = 0.064, p = .076). Experimental within-group changes for WREQ subscale scores External Cues (t(23) = 3.779, p = .001) and Emotional Eating (t(23) = 2.282, p = .032) decreased over time, along with increased total and summary IES (t(23) = 2.6330, p = .015) and MEQ (t(23) = 2.663, p = .014) scores. Promising findings of reduced eating rate, increased meal duration, and improved WREQ, IES, and MEQ scores should be followed up in larger more diverse samples for longer durations.
Article
Because a relationship has been reported between masticatory behavior, obesity, and postprandial blood glucose, it is recommended to chew well and take a longer time to eat. The purpose of this study was to examine the possibility of changing masticatory behavior using a small ear-hung wearable chewing counter, which can monitor masticatory behavior without disturbing daily meals. In total, 235 healthy volunteers participated in a 4-wk randomized controlled trial and were divided into 3 groups. All participants were instructed about the importance of mastication at the first visit. During the intervention, group B used the chewing counter without an algorithm during each meal (notification of the number of chews after meal), and group C used the chewing counter with a masticatory behavior change algorithm (setting a target value and displaying the number of chews in real time). Group A was set as the control group. The number of chews and the meal time when consuming 1 rice ball (100 g) were measured before and after the intervention using the chewing counter, and the rate of change in these values was evaluated. Participants also provided a subjective evaluation of their changes in masticatory behavior. The number of chews and the meal time of 1 rice ball increased significantly in groups B and C compared with before the intervention, and the rate of change was significantly higher in group C than in group A and group B. In addition, the subjective evaluation of the change in the number of chews was highest in group C. Self-monitoring of masticatory behavior by providing a target value and the degree of achievement for the number of chews using a wearable chewing counter with a behavioral change algorithm could promote effective change in masticatory behavior and lead to an increased number of chews. (Trial ID: UMIN000034476)
Article
Background: Childhood obesity places a major burden on global public health. We aimed to identify and characterize potential factors, both individually and jointly, in association with overweight and obesity in Chinese preschool-aged children. Methods: We cross-sectionally recruited 9501 preschool-aged children from 30 kindergartens in Beijing and Tangshan. Overweight and obesity are defined according to the World Health Organization (WHO), International Obesity Task Force (IOTF), and China criteria. Results: After multivariable adjustment, eating speed, sleep duration, birthweight, and paternal body mass index (BMI) were consistently and significantly associated with childhood overweight and obesity under three growth criteria at a significance level of 5%. Additional fast food intake frequency, maternal BMI, gestational weight gain (GWG) and maternal pre-pregnancy BMI were significant factors for overweight (WHO criteria) and obesity (both IOTF and China criteria). Importantly, there were significant interactions between parental obesity and eating speed for childhood obesity. Finally, for practical reasons, risk nomogram models were constructed for childhood overweight and obesity based on significant factors under each criterion, with good prediction accuracy. Conclusion: Our findings indicated a synergistic association of lifestyle, fetal and neonatal, and family-related factors with the risk of experiencing overweight and obesity among preschool-aged children.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Large portions and high dietary energy density promote overconsumption at meal times. This could be reduced by eating slowly. Objective: Two studies investigated whether texture-based reductions in eating rate and oral processing moderate consumption at breakfast in combination with variations in energy density and portion size. Methods: Adults attended 4 breakfast sessions (2 × 2 repeated-measures design) to consume rice porridge, combining a 45% reduction in eating rate [thin porridge (140 g/min) compared with thick porridge (77 g/min)] with a 77% increase in energy density (0.57 compared with 1.01 kcal/g) in study 1 [n = 61; aged 21–48 y; body mass index (BMI; in kg/m²): 16–29] and a 50% increase in portion size (100% compared with 150%) in study 2 (n = 53; aged 21–42 y; BMI: 16–29). Oral processing behaviors were coded by using webcams. Porridge intake was measured alongside changes in rated appetite. Results: Increases in energy density and portion size led to increases of 80% and 13% in energy intake at breakfast, respectively (P < 0.001), but only portion size increased the weight of food consumed (13%). The thicker porridges were consumed at a slower rate and led to 11–13% reductions in food weight and energy intake compared with the thin versions (P < 0.001). Combined, the least energy was consumed when the thick “slow” porridge was served with a lower energy density or smaller portion (P < 0.05). Although intake was reduced for the thick porridges, they were expected to be more filling than the thin versions and experienced as equally satiating postconsumption. Conclusions: Adults eat in response to external features of the food environment. An opportunity exists to use a combination of energy-density dilution, smaller portions, and natural variations in food texture to design meals that promote reductions in energy intake while maintaining satiety.
Article
Full-text available
Eating more than is required to maintain bodyweight is weakly resisted physiologically, as appetite does not closely track body energy balance. What does limit energy intake is the capacity of the gut to accommodate and process what is eaten. As the gut empties, we are ready to eat again. We typically refer to this absence of fullness as ‘hunger’, but in this state, even when it is prolonged (e.g. by missing one or two meals), our mental and physical performance is not compromised because body energy stores are mobilised to sustain energy supply to our brain and muscles. We illustrate this by discussing research on the effects of missing breakfast. Contrary to conventional wisdom, it appears that missing breakfast leads to a reduction in total daily energy intake and does not impair cognitive function (in adequately nourished individuals). The problem with missing a meal or eating smaller meals, however, is that we miss out on (some of) the pleasure of eating (food reward). In current studies, we are investigating how to offset the reduced reward value of smaller food portions, by, for example, altering flavour intensity, food variety and unit size, in order to maintain overall meal satisfaction and thereby reduce or eliminate subsequent compensatory eating.
Article
Full-text available
The Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) statement is a guideline designed to improve the transparency and quality of the reporting of randomised controlled trials (RCTs). In this article we present an extension to that statement for randomised pilot and feasibility trials conducted in advance of a future definitive RCT. The checklist applies to any randomised study in which a future definitive RCT, or part of it, is conducted on a smaller scale, regardless of its design (eg, cluster, factorial, crossover) or the terms used by authors to describe the study (eg, pilot, feasibility, trial, study). The extension does not directly apply to internal pilot studies built into the design of a main trial, non-randomised pilot and feasibility studies, or phase II studies, but these studies all have some similarities to randomised pilot and feasibility studies and so many of the principles might also apply. The development of the extension was motivated by the growing number of studies described as feasibility or pilot studies and by research that has identified weaknesses in their reporting and conduct. We followed recommended good practice to develop the extension, including carrying out a Delphi survey, holding a consensus meeting and research team meetings, and piloting the checklist. The aims and objectives of pilot and feasibility randomised studies differ from those of other randomised trials. Consequently, although much of the information to be reported in these trials is similar to those in randomised controlled trials (RCTs) assessing effectiveness and efficacy, there are some key differences in the type of information and in the appropriate interpretation of standard CONSORT reporting items. We have retained some of the original CONSORT statement items, but most have been adapted, some removed, and new items added. The new items cover how participants were identified and consent obtained; if applicable, the prespecified criteria used to judge whether or how to proceed with a future definitive RCT; if relevant, other important unintended consequences; implications for progression from pilot to future definitive RCT, including any proposed amendments; and ethical approval or approval by a research review committee confirmed with a reference number. This article includes the 26 item checklist, a separate checklist for the abstract, a template for a CONSORT flowchart for these studies, and an explanation of the changes made and supporting examples. We believe that routine use of this proposed extension to the CONSORT statement will result in improve
Article
Full-text available
The Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) statement is a guideline designed to improve the transparency and quality of the reporting of randomised controlled trials (RCTs). In this article we present an extension to that statement for randomised pilot and feasibility trials conducted in advance of a future definitive RCT. The checklist applies to any randomised study in which a future definitive RCT, or part of it, is conducted on a smaller scale, regardless of its design (eg, cluster, factorial, crossover) or the terms used by authors to describe the study (eg, pilot, feasibility, trial, study). The extension does not directly apply to internal pilot studies built into the design of a main trial, non-randomised pilot and feasibility studies, or phase II studies, but these studies all have some similarities to randomised pilot and feasibility studies and so many of the principles might also apply. The development of the extension was motivated by the growing number of studies described as feasibility or pilot studies and by research that has identified weaknesses in their reporting and conduct. We followed recommended good practice to develop the extension, including carrying out a Delphi survey, holding a consensus meeting and research team meetings, and piloting the checklist. The aims and objectives of pilot and feasibility randomised studies differ from those of other randomised trials. Consequently, although much of the information to be reported in these trials is similar to those in randomised controlled trials (RCTs) assessing effectiveness and efficacy, there are some key differences in the type of information and in the appropriate interpretation of standard CONSORT reporting items. We have retained some of the original CONSORT statement items, but most have been adapted, some removed, and new items added. The new items cover how participants were identified and consent obtained; if applicable, the prespecified criteria used to judge whether or how to proceed with a future definitive RCT; if relevant, other important unintended consequences; implications for progression from pilot to future definitive RCT, including any proposed amendments; and ethical approval or approval by a research review committee confirmed with a reference number. This article includes the 26 item checklist, a separate checklist for the abstract, a template for a CONSORT flowchart for these studies, and an explanation of the changes made and supporting examples. We believe that routine use of this proposed extension to the CONSORT statement will result in improvements in the reporting of pilot trials. Editor’s note: In order to encourage its wide dissemination this article is freely accessible on the BMJ and Pilot and Feasibility Studies journal websites. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:10.1186/s40814-016-0105-8) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Article
Full-text available
Significance Functional MRI (fMRI) is 25 years old, yet surprisingly its most common statistical methods have not been validated using real data. Here, we used resting-state fMRI data from 499 healthy controls to conduct 3 million task group analyses. Using this null data with different experimental designs, we estimate the incidence of significant results. In theory, we should find 5% false positives (for a significance threshold of 5%), but instead we found that the most common software packages for fMRI analysis (SPM, FSL, AFNI) can result in false-positive rates of up to 70%. These results question the validity of a number of fMRI studies and may have a large impact on the interpretation of weakly significant neuroimaging results.
Article
Full-text available
Deliberately eating at a slower pace promotes satiation and eating quickly has been associated with a higher body mass index. Therefore, understanding factors that affect eating rate should be given high priority. Eating rate is affected by the physical/textural properties of a food, by motivational state, and by portion size and palatability. This study explored the prospect that eating rate is also influenced by a hitherto unexplored cognitive process that uses ongoing perceptual estimates of the volume of food remaining in a container to adjust intake during a meal. A 2 (amount seen; 300ml or 500ml) x 2 (amount eaten; 300ml or 500ml) between-subjects design was employed (10 participants in each condition). In two 'congruent' conditions, the same amount was seen at the outset and then subsequently consumed (300ml or 500ml). To dissociate visual feedback of portion size and actual amount consumed, food was covertly added or removed from a bowl using a peristaltic pump. This created two additional 'incongruent' conditions, in which 300ml was seen but 500ml was eaten or vice versa. We repeated these conditions using a savoury soup and a sweet dessert. Eating rate (ml per second) was assessed during lunch. After lunch we assessed fullness over a 60-minute period. In the congruent conditions, eating rate was unaffected by the actual volume of food that was consumed (300ml or 500ml). By contrast, we observed a marked difference across the incongruent conditions. Specifically, participants who saw 300ml but actually consumed 500ml ate at a faster rate than participants who saw 500ml but actually consumed 300ml. Participants were unaware that their portion size had been manipulated. Nevertheless, when it disappeared faster or slower than anticipated they adjusted their rate of eating accordingly. This suggests that the control of eating rate involves visual feedback and is not a simple reflexive response to orosensory stimulation.