ArticlePDF Available

Group size and individual ‘personality’ influence emergence times in hermit crabs

Authors:

Abstract

Many animals benefit from aggregating due to the anti-predator effects associated with living in groups. Hermit crabs are known to form groups, or ‘clusters’, which may occur at sites of high shell availability. Clustering may also have anti-predator benefits, if individuals in larger clusters able to spend less time engaging in defensive behaviours such as hiding in their shells. Here, we test the hypothesis that crabs in larger clusters will emerge faster from their shells after an elicited startle response in the European hermit crab (Pagurus bernhardus). We found that individuals were generally consistent in their emergence times across group sizes (displaying ‘personality’ in relation to emergence time), but that group size influenced emergence time in P. bernhardus. In contrast to the hypothesis, crabs in larger clusters had longer emergence times relative to their own emergence times in smaller clusters. Suggested explanations for this effect include intra-specific competition for the gastropod shells that hermit crabs inhabit, as well as the possible release of chemical cues by crabs in larger clusters.
BioscienceHorizons
.................................................................................................................................................................
.................................................................................................................................................................
.................................................................................................................................................................
Volume 00 2018 10.1093/biohorizons/hzy011
Research article
Group size and individual personalityinuence
emergence times in hermit crabs
Harvey Eliot Broadhurst*and Lesley J. Morrell
School of Environmental Sciences, University of Hull, Yorkshire, HU6 7RX, UK
*Corresponding author: Harvey Eliot Broadhurst, Email: harv.broadhurst@gmail.com
Supervisor: Lesley J. Morrell, School of Environmental Sciences (Biology), Hardy Building, University of Hull, Kingston-upon-Hull HU6 7RX.
Many animals benet from aggregating due to the anti-predator effects associated with living in groups. Hermit crabs are
known to form groups, or clusters, which may occur at sites of high shell availability. Clustering may also have anti-predator
benets, if individuals in larger clusters able to spend less time engaging in defensive behaviours such as hiding in their
shells. Here, we test the hypothesis that crabs in larger clusters will emerge faster from their shells after an elicited startle
response in the European hermit crab (Pagurus bernhardus). We found that individuals were generally consistent in their
emergence times across group sizes (displaying personalityin relation to emergence time), but that group size inuenced
emergence time in P. bernhardus. In contrast to the hypothesis, crabs in larger clusters had longer emergence times relative
to their own emergence times in smaller clusters. Suggested explanations for this effect include intra-specic competition
for the gastropod shells that hermit crabs inhabit, as well as the possible release of chemical cues by crabs in larger clusters.
Key words: hermit crabs, emergence time, personality, group size, behavioural consistency, Pagurus bernhardus
Submitted on 3 March 2017; editorial decision on 29 October 2018
Introduction
Group-living has been observed across a broad range of ani-
mal taxa (Krause and Ruxton, 2002), and group size in par-
ticular has a major inuence on the outcome of predatorprey
interactions, allowing group-living animals to manage their
vulnerability to predation risk (Cresswell and Quinn, 2011).
The major costs associated with group-living, such as higher
rate of attack from predators due to increased conspicuous-
ness, may be offset by anti-predator mechanisms (Uetz et al.,
2002). These mechanisms include the dilution of individual
risk (Foster and Treherne, 1981), the confusion of predators,
reducing attack success (Miller, 1922;Krakauer, 1995),
encounter-dilution (Turner and Pitcher, 1986) and selsh
herd effects (Hamilton, 1971). Grouping individuals also
benet from collective vigilance, with those in larger groups
able to reduce time spent scanning and increase time engaging
in other activities (Pulliam, 1973;Cresswell and Quinn,
2011), which can also allow for cooperative warning, escape
and defence behaviour (Krause and Ruxton, 2002). However,
as group size increases, individuals may also be subjected to
increased competition for resources, which could be a limiting
factor in group size regulation (Grand and Dill, 1999).
Clusteringhas been identied as a behavioural strategy in
several hermit crab (superfamily Paguroidea) species, (Taylor,
1981;Gherardi and Vannini, 1989). Hermit crabs aggregate at
sites of gastropod mortality, possibly to engage in vacancy
chainbehaviour; the sequential distribution of the acquired
gastropod shells that hermit crabs inhabit (Lewis and Rotjan,
2009). When a hermit crab vacates its shell in order to occupy
amoresuitableone,othercrabshavebeenobservedlining up
in order to vacate their own shells in favour of a newly avail-
able one (De Waal, 2005). In P. bernhardus, the structure of
these vacancy chains differs in the presence and absence of
.................................................................................................................................................................
1
© The Author(s) 2018. Published by Oxford University Press.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/),
which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biohorizons/article-abstract/doi/10.1093/biohorizons/hzy011/5253902 by guest on 21 December 2018
predation risk (Briffa and Austin, 2009), but it is not known
whether the size of a cluster affects predation risk. Hermit
crabs in the genus Pagurus do exhibit alarm responses when
exposed to the chemical cue of a crushed conspecic(
Rittschof
et al.,1992
), and therefore clustering may serve an anti-
predator function, with individuals beneting from dilution or
detection effects. However, larger clusters may also carry
increased risk of competition for shells from conspecics.
Hermit crabs employ two major defences when exposed to
potential predation: eeing and refuging within their acquired
gastropod shells (Scarratt and Godin, 1992). If, on detecting a
predator, a crab decides to hide within the shell, then there is
an associated second decision that determines the length of
time wherein the crab will remain hidden before emerging
once again (Briffa and Twyman, 2011). This decision to
emerge is sensitive to the perceived risk of predation (Scarratt
and Godin, 1992). For example, the presence of chemical
cues in the form of efuent from the predatory rock crab,
Cancer productus, has been shown to signicantly reduce
emergence times in hermit crabs; whereas exposure to efuent
from the herbivorous kelp crab, Pugettia productus, showed
no difference from a saltwater control (Rosen, Schwarz and
Palmer, 2009). Withdrawal into a shell is also a response to
competition: individuals are able to defend themselves from
competitors in shell ghts by retreating into their shells to
avoid being forcibly removed (Courtene-Jones and Briffa,
2014), and thus the decision to emerge may also be sensitive
to the risk of competition. Emergence from a startle is also
consistent across individuals, with some showing consistently
longer recovery times, while others show consistently shorter
times (Briffa, Rundle and Fryer, 2008;Briffa and Twyman,
2011;Briffa, Bridger and Biro, 2013;Briffa, 2013).
Rather than responding optimally across every situation
(behavioural plasticity), some individuals are constrained by
consistent differences in behaviour over time or across con-
texts (sometimes known as animal personality;Mathot and
Dingemanse, 2014). Startle responses may therefore be con-
sistent between individuals, forming a component of a behav-
ioural syndrome; which occurs when behaviours are
correlated across multiple behavioural categories (Jandt et al.
2014). One behaviour which is often reported as consistent is
the shyness-boldnessaxis, allowing for the classication of
individuals as somewhere between shyor bold(Wilson
et al., 1994). A bold individual would emerge rapidly from a
startle stimulus, while a shy would not (Briffa, Rundle and
Fryer, 2008), and in P. bernhardus is correlated with each
individuals willingness to engage in riskybehaviour
(Gherardi, Aquiloni and Tricarico, 2012). Behaviour however
is also plastic in response to environmental conditions, and
individuals can adapt their behaviour to the environment
(Pigliucci, 2001). In P. bernhardus, this plasticity is exceeded
by individual consistency in boldness in response to high- and
low-predation risk scenarios (Briffa, Rundle and Fryer,
2008). These between-individual differences over an environ-
mental gradient (context) are termed behavioural reaction
norms(Briffa, Bridger and Biro, 2013).
This study investigates whether P. bernhardus exhibits reac-
tion norm variation across individuals when exposed to differ-
ent degrees of clustering (i.e. different group sizes). By
analyzing the variation in startle responses exhibited by indi-
vidual hermit crabs across several classes of group size, this
study explores whether emergence time is inuenced by cluster-
ing in P. bernhardus. If cluster size in this species is inuenced
by both the anti-predator benets and competition-associated
costs of group-living, we expect to nd a signicant effect of
group size on emergence time. If this species forms clusters as a
response to predation risk, or gains anti-predator benets from
clustering, individuals are predicted to register shorter emer-
gence times in larger groups (where individual risk is lower)
relative to their emergence times in smaller groups (where indi-
vidual risk is higher). Alternatively, if clustering carries
increased risk of competition, we might expect individuals in
larger groups to remain in their shells for longer periods, to
reduce the risk of engaging in shell ghts. Additionally, we pre-
dict that individual hermit crabs show signicant patterns of
individual consistency across different group sizes.
Methods
Data collection
Sixty Pagurus bernhardus were collected from South Bay,
Scarborough, UK (54°1612N0°2325W) in October 2015.
They were transported back to the laboratory at the
University of Hull within 4 h of collection, where they were
kept in a holding tank (1.5-m circular diameter) that con-
tained steadily ltered aerated saltwater at a constant tem-
perature of 11°C. Crabs were given access to a large number
of vacant shells of varying size (primarily common periwin-
kle, Littorina littorea; dog whelk, Nucella lapillus; and at
top shell, Gibbula umbilicalis) and left to acclimatize to their
new surroundings (and occupy a new shell if required) for
72 h. Following acclimatization, 25 crabs were randomly
selected, weighed within their shells and individually num-
bered on the shell using nail varnish, then placed inside plastic
containers (18 ×10 cm; one side meshed for aeration) within
the holding tank to isolate them and prevent shell-swapping
(Gherardi, 2006), a behaviour observed in the holding tank
among unmarked individuals. Crabs were fed twice a week
on chopped mussel purchased from a local supermarket.
Crabs were not sexed as previous studies have found that
individual differences in startle response are independent of
sex (Briffa, Rundle and Fryer, 2008).
Startle response times for each marked individual were
measured in ve different group sizes (1, 2, 5, 10 and 20 indi-
viduals). A group consisted of the marked individual and an
appropriate number of unmarked specimens selected haphaz-
ardly from the holding tank. A circular observation container
(35 cm diameter) was lled to a depth of 10 cm with water
taken from the holding tank. The focal crab and the correct
number of unmarked individuals were placed onto a plate
(22 cm diameter), ensuring that the focal crab was positioned
.................................................................................................................................................................
YYYY Bioscience Horizons Volume 00 2018
.................................................................................................................................................................
2
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biohorizons/article-abstract/doi/10.1093/biohorizons/hzy011/5253902 by guest on 21 December 2018
in an inverted position to ensure it withdrew fully into its shell
before observations began. All crabs were then gently tipped
into the observation container. This method of eliciting a star-
tle response is both successful and non-harmful in determin-
ing response times of P. bernhardus (Briffa, Rundle and
Fryer, 2008;Briffa, 2013).
Latency to emerge from the shell was timed to the nearest
second using a digital stopwatch from the point that the crab
enters the observation container until the point at which its
pereopods made contact with the base of the container
(Briffa, 2013), when it is considered to be fully emerged. After
emergence, the focal crab was returned to its individual con-
tainer, and the remaining crabs to the holding tank. The water
in the observation container was changed between each trial.
Each crab was given a maximum of 3 min to emerge before
the trial was terminated (failure to emerge was recorded in
61/375 trials). Startle responses were induced in each crab
twice a week for seven and a half weeks with each crab pro-
viding up to 15 latencies in total, with 3 at each of the 5 group
sizes to evaluate consistency of emergence time within a con-
text (group size). Crabs were assigned a group size at random
during each data collection session, while ensuring that each
experienced each group size a maximum of three times.
Ethical approval was obtained from the School and
Faculty Ethics Committees before the study began. At the end
of the experiment, the crabs were returned to the shore where
they were collected from.
Statistical analyses
To assess the relationship between group size and the time
that it took individuals to emerge from their shells, linear
mixed-effects models were implemented using the package
nlme(Pinheiro et al., 2015) in R version 3.2.3 (R Core
Team, 2015). Since the nature of the data involved several
group size classes (1, 2, 5, 10 and 20), group size was treated
as a categorical variable during analysis. Group sizes were
compared to a reference level of group size 2, as this was the
group size with the lowest mean emergence time (pairwise
comparisons with all reference levels can be found in
Appendix I). Body size was included as an additional xed
effect, and the identity of the crab was included as a random
effect to account for multiple measures on each individual
both within and between group sizes. Data were log-
transformed to meet the assumptions of normality for statis-
tical analysis, and non-signicant interactions between body
size and group size were removed.
To investigate whether different individuals had predict-
ably different emergence times, an ANCOVA model was used
on data from across all group sizes. This included the emer-
gence time of the crab as the independent variable, the crabs
identity as a dependent variable, and the group size that the
emergence time was obtained from as a covariate. A series of
regression analyses were then used to further assess whether
an individuals emergence time in one group size was a signi-
cant predictor of their respective emergence time in another
group size. A total of 10 linear regressions were used in this
manner, with the mean emergence time of each crab in a given
group size being regressed against their mean emergence time
in another group size, until each group size had been com-
pared against every other condition. To quantify individual
consistency, repeatability was calculated using the intraclass
correlation coefcient (r
IC
); a measure of testretest reliability
(Uher, 2011). This was achieved using the R package ICC
(Wolak, Fairbairn and Paulsen, 2012).
Results
Does group size affect emergence time?
Accounting for individual variation in startle response, emer-
gence time increased with group size (Table 1, Fig. 1). There
was no signicant effect of body size on emergence time
(Table 1). Treatment groups were compared against a group
size of 2 as this was the group with the lowest mean emer-
gence time (Fig. 2). Crabs emerged faster in groups of 10 and
20 than they did in groups of 2 (Table 1). All other pairwise
comparisons can be found in Appendix I.
Table 1. Linear mixed-effects models assessing the relationship between group size, body size and emergence time with individual identity as a
random effect. Signicant P-values are highlighted in bold.
Value SE df tP
Group size as a categorical variable (intercept: group size =2)
(intercept) 2.150 0.493 285
Group size =1 0.225 0.161 285 1.391 0.165
Group size =5 0.231 0.158 285 1.456 0.147
Group size =10 0.400 0.160 285 2.492 0.013
Group size =20 0.426 0.164 285 2.595 0.010
Body size 0.156 0.120 23 1.309 0.234
.................................................................................................................................................................
Bioscience Horizons Volume 00 2018
.................................................................................................................................................................
3
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biohorizons/article-abstract/doi/10.1093/biohorizons/hzy011/5253902 by guest on 21 December 2018
Do hermit crabs exhibit personality?
Emergence times differed between crabs, even when account-
ing for variation caused by the different group sizes (group
size effect, F=4.28, df =1288, P=0.03; individual effect,
F=5.67, df =24 288, P<0.001; Fig. 3). In 8 out of 10 group
size comparisons, the emergence time registered by a crab in
one group was found to be a signicant predictor of how that
crab would respond in other group sizes (Fig. 2; Table 2); this
shows consistency in the behaviour of crabs between treat-
ments. No signicant correlation was observed between emer-
gence times in group sizes of 1 and 10 (t=1.67, P=0.11),
and 5 and 20 (t=1.86, P=0.08).
Across all groups, emergence time was found to be gener-
ally repeatable when measured with the intraclass correlation
coefcient (r
IC
=0.26; Fig. 4). However, considering each
group size alone, signicant repeatability was only found in a
group size of 2 (r
IC
=0.56) and a group size of 20 (r
IC
=
0.51). All other treatment groups returned an r
IC
value which
had a 95% condence interval inclusive of zero (Fig. 4), indic-
ating non-signicant repeatability despite the overall result.
Discussion
The results suggest that both individual consistency and
group size affect emergence time in hermit crabs. Individual
crabs were consistent in their behaviour across group sizes:
those with shorter emergence times (bolderindividuals)
consistently emerging rapidly from their shells, while those
with longer emergence time (shyindividuals) having consist-
ently longer emergence times. The positive relationship
between larger group size and longer emergence times suggest
that competition with conspecics, rather than the anti-
predator benets of grouping, are the key determinant of
emergence decisions. Two related factors may explain this
relationship: direct competition for gastropod shells and
exposure to ghting cuesfrom conspecics.
Competition for gastropod shells is well-documented in
hermit crabs (Elwood and Glass, 1981;Briffa, Elwood and
Dick, 1998;Caven, Clayton and Sweet, 2012). Increased
emergence times in larger groups may be explained by unin-
tentional interference between individuals as they begin to
move and emerge, such as shell-to-shell hitting or knocking.
This type of disturbance would cause an emerging crab to
retreat into their shell (Edmonds and Briffa, 2016), regardless
of group size, but is probabilistically more likely as group
size, and therefore density, increased. A tap to the shell during
emergence could indicate to the crab that the risk of competi-
tion was high, as it may be able to perceive whether the source
of the tap was initiating a ght or not, and, given that crabs
were likely to be in preferred-size shells, would have with-
drawn into the shell in defence (Elwood and Glass, 1981).
Any anti-predator benets potentially gained from cluster-
ing may have been offset by the risk of forced shell-eviction
from a conspecic. Indeed, in the wild, many members of the
genus Pagurus are known to maintain a rather large individ-
ual distanceinstead of aggregating (Hazlett, 1968). Shell
ghts are often initiated by larger crabs and crabs that occupy
poor quality and/or unsuitably small shells (Dowds and
Elwood, 1985), although we found no effect of size on emer-
gence time. As crabs in this study were able to select from
unoccupied shells before experiments began, they may have
had low motivation to initiate a ght and maximal motivation
to retain their shells. As potential competition increases
(increasing numbers of nearby conspecics) motivation to
remain in the shell, defending it against competitions, may
have increased, as shybehaviour is associated with higher
chances of successful shell-defence during a ght (Courtene-
Jones and Briffa, 2014). As motivation to ght in hermit crabs
is dependent on the quality of their shells (Elwood and Briffa,
2001), future studies may therefore benet from examining
how individual emergence behaviour varies when crabs are
housed in shells of varying size, quality and t.
Hermit crabs are also able to use chemical cues in order to
detect conspecics and discriminate shells (Benoit, Peeke and
Chang, 1997), and to distinguish between crabs that have
recently fought and crabs that have not (Briffa and Williams,
2006). Exposure to these ghting cueslengthens the amount
of time a hermit crab spends withdrawn into its shell (Briffa
and Williams, 2006). Therefore, if the unmarked crabs had
been engaged in ghts, the presence of ghting cues in the
water may have increased time spent withdrawn in the shell
Figure 1. Bar graph showing mean responses of all crabs across group
sizes with standard error represented as error bars. Signicant effects
of group size on emergence time are shown between a group size of 2
and 10, and 2 and 20.
.................................................................................................................................................................
YYYY Bioscience Horizons Volume 00 2018
.................................................................................................................................................................
4
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biohorizons/article-abstract/doi/10.1093/biohorizons/hzy011/5253902 by guest on 21 December 2018
in larger groups, where the probability of any one crab having
recently engaged in a ght would be increased, particularly as
crowding is associated with increased aggression (Hazlett,
1968). In the green swordtail (Xiphophorus helleri), eaves-
droppingon ghts reduces a bystanders propensity to
engage in aggressive behaviour with the winning combatant
post-ght (Earley and Dugatkin, 2002).
The degree in which hermit crabs are able to detect discrete
differences in conspecic group size is not known, forming a
potentially enlightening area for future investigation. The sug-
gestion that animals are able to discriminate quantity through
the mental representation of numbers (counting)as opposed
to through non-numerical perceptible variables which differ
with numerosityhas traditionally been restricted to mammalian
Figure 2. Signicant positive correlations between crab emergence times in two group sizes; one larger than the other. Signicance in these
regressions represents predictability of an individuals behaviour across treatments. Each data point represents an individual crabs mean log
emergence time at the specied group size. Group sizes are indicated on the axes of the graphs.
.................................................................................................................................................................
Bioscience Horizons Volume 00 2018
.................................................................................................................................................................
5
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biohorizons/article-abstract/doi/10.1093/biohorizons/hzy011/5253902 by guest on 21 December 2018
models (Agrillo et al., 2009). However, previous studies have
documented counting of conspecics in mosquitosh
(Gambusia holbrooki;Agrillo et al., 2008), as well as the
counting of landmarks in honey bees (Apis mellifera;Chittka
and Geiger, 1995). The relationship between group size and
emergence time discovered in this study therefore presents a
novel opportunity to explore quantity discrimination in a
crustacean model.
In line with previous work (Briffa, Rundle and Fryer, 2008;
Briffa and Twyman, 2011;Briffa, Bridger and Biro, 2013;Briffa,
2013), hermit crabs showed signicant individual consistency in
Figure 3. Box and whisker diagrams for each crab showing variation in emergence time across all treatments. Grey box represents interquartile
range with black line representing the median emergence time; outliers are represented as grey circles.
Table 2. Pairwise comparisons of emergence time in ve different
group sizes. Signicance (indicated by bold) represents predictability
of an individuals behaviour across treatments.
Group size comparison tP nR
2
1 and 2 2.16 0.04 24 0.18
1 and 5 2.66 0.01 25 0.26
1 and 10 1.67 0.11 25 0.11
1 and 20 2.31 0.03 25 0.19
2 and 5 2.75 0.01 24 0.26
2 and 10 3.36 <0.01 24 0.34
2 and 20 3.05 <0.01 24 0.30
5 and 10 2.27 0.03 25 0.18
5 and 20 1.86 0.08 25 0.13
10 and 20 2.71 0.01 25 0.24
Figure 4. Intraclass correlation coefcients for all treatments and each
group size separately, represented by data points and lines indicating
the 95% condence intervals. Grey dashed line represents zero. r
IC
values with intervals not crossing zero are considered signicant (black
data points and lines) whereas intervals crossing zero are considered
non-signicant (grey data points and lines).
.................................................................................................................................................................
YYYY Bioscience Horizons Volume 00 2018
.................................................................................................................................................................
6
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biohorizons/article-abstract/doi/10.1093/biohorizons/hzy011/5253902 by guest on 21 December 2018
behaviour across group sizes, yet adjusted that behaviour in
response to their environment. This supports previous research
which has found that although P. bernhardus modulates its
behaviour to show measureable behavioural plasticity, this effect
is exceeded by the degree of behavioural consistency observed in
this species (Briffa, Rundle and Fryer, 2008). The results pre-
sented here suggest that investment in mechanisms required for
behavioural plasticity and accurate modulation of responses is
relatively low, with behavioural consistency and approximate
modulation of responses favoured instead. Both the costs asso-
ciated with the production and maintenance of sensory and infor-
mation processing systems, and variation in the level of
environmental heterogeneity, have been suggested as factors that
could explain the balance between plasticity and consistency
(Briffa, Rundle and Fryer, 2008). Previous research has remarked
on the limited extent of behavioural plasticity in hermit crabs
(Hazlett, 1995). As consistent individual differences in behaviour
as well as patterns of appropriate adjustment in boldness
across situationshave previously been used to suggest the pres-
ence of animal personalities (Brown, Jones and Braithwaite,
2005;Mowles, Cotton and Briffa, 2012;Rudin and Briffa,
2012), this limited behavioural plasticity may be due to the pres-
ence of personality in hermit crabs, previously reported by Briffa,
Rundle and Fryer (2008).
Further work is needed to elucidate the function of
increased emergence times in larger groups, and the mechan-
isms by which individual crabs determine their emergence
time across group sizes. We suggest that both immediate
threat of competition for shells and the detection of cues from
previous ghts may inuence this decision. Clustering may
still be associated with reduced predation risk, as grouping
carries anti-predator benets across species (Krause and
Ruxton, 2002) although larger groups may be more likely to
attract predators, particularly if predators use movement to
detect their prey. Future studies examining the potential anti-
predator benets of clustering in P. bernhardus may therefore
benet from exploring locale-dependent variation in boldness
between sites with measurable differences in predation risk.
Extended emergence times in hermit crabs have previously
been observed with the presence of predatory cues (Scarratt
and Godin, 1992), and therefore the role of predation risk in
determining emergence behaviour across group sizes would
shed light on whether this factor too plays a role in determin-
ing emergence times. Finally, how hermit crabs determine the
size of the cluster and relative risk is another route for further
research.
Supplementary Data
Supplementary data are available at BIOHOR online.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank aquatic technicians Rose Wilcox and
Alan Smith for their assistance in the collection and
transportation of the hermit crabs that this project required,
as well as for ensuring the crabscare and maintenance during
their brief experience of captivity. Harvey Broadhurst would
also like to thank Adam Bakewell for his invaluable advice
and tutorials on how to use statistical computing programme R.
Author biography
Harvey graduated from the University of Hull in 2016 with a
BSc (Hons) in Zoology. Specializing in evolutionary biology
and behavioural ecology, his most accomplished work ranged
from a eld project on the symbiotic mutualisms of clownsh
and sea anemones, to a presentation on the evolution of the
foot in early hominids. Harvey is currently working within
the technology communications industry, conducting media
relations for startups and high-growth tech companies.
Eventually, he would like to develop a career in science com-
munication, promoting novel research in the eld of zoology.
Statement of responsibility
Designing the studyL.J.M. and H.E.B., conducting experi-
mentsH.E.B., analyzing the dataH.E.B., writing the
manuscriptH.E.B. with feedback and input from L.J.M.,
technical supportsee acknowledgements, conceptual advice
L.J.M.
References
Agrillo, C., Dadda, M., Serena, G. et al. (2008) Do sh count?
Spontaneous discrimination of quantity in female mosquitosh,
Animal Cognition, 11, 495503.
Agrillo, C., Dadda, M., Serena, G. et al. (2009) Use of number by sh,
PLoS One, 4, e4786.
Benoit, M. D., Peeke, H. V. and Chang, E. S. (1997) Use of chemical cues
for shell preference by the hermit crab, Pagurus samuelis,Marine &
Freshwater Behaviour & Phy, 30, 4554.
Briffa, M. (2013) Plastic proteans: reduced predictability in the face of
predation risk in hermit crabs, Biology Letters, 9, 20130592.
Briffa, M. and Austin, M. (2009) Effects of predation threat on the struc-
ture and benets from vacancy chains in the hermit crab Pagurus
bernhardus,Ethology : Formerly Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 115,
10291035.
Briffa, M., Bridger, D. and Biro, P. A. (2013) How does temperature affect
behaviour? Multilevel analysis of plasticity, personality and predict-
ability in hermit crabs, Animal Behaviour, 86, 4754.
Briffa, M., Elwood, R. W. and Dick, J. T. A. (1998) Analysis of repeated sig-
nals during shell ghts in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus,
Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 265,
14671474.
.................................................................................................................................................................
Bioscience Horizons Volume 00 2018
.................................................................................................................................................................
7
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biohorizons/article-abstract/doi/10.1093/biohorizons/hzy011/5253902 by guest on 21 December 2018
Briffa, M., Rundle, S. D. and Fryer, A. (2008) Comparing the strength of
behavioural plasticity and consistency across situations: animal per-
sonalities in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus,Proceedings of the
Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 275, 13051311.
Briffa, M. and Twyman, C. (2011) Do I stand out or blend in?
Conspicuousness awareness and consistent behavioural differ-
ences in hermit crabs, Biology Letters, 7, 330332.
Briffa, M. and Williams, R. (2006) Use of chemical cues during shell
ghts in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus,Behaviour, 143,
12811290.
Brown, C., Jones, F. and Braithwaite, V. (2005) In situ examination of
boldness-shyness traits in the tropical poeciliid, Brachraphis episco-
pi. Animal Behaviour, 70, 10031009.
Caven, D. J., Clayton, R. L. and Sweet, M. J. (2012) A comparative study
of aggression and spatial differences between populations of
Pagurus bernhardus,Journal of Young Investigators, 12, 36.
Chittka, L. and Geiger, K. (1995) Can honey bees count landmarks?
Animal Behaviour, 49, 159164.
Courtene-Jones, W. and Briffa, M. (2014) Boldness and asymmetric con-
tests: role- and outcome-dependent effects of ghting in hermit
crabs, Behavioural Ecology, 25, 10731082.
Cresswell, W. and Quinn, J. L. (2011) Predicting the optimal prey group
size from predator hunting behaviour, Journal of Animal Ecology,
80, 310319.
De Waal, F. B. M. (2005) How animals do business, Scientic American,
292, 7279.
Dowds, B. M. and Elwood, R. W. (1985) Shell wars II: the inuence of
relative size on decisions made during hermit crab shell ghts,
Animal Behaviour, 33, 649656.
Earley, R. L. and Dugatkin, L. A. (2002) Eavesdropping on visual cues in green
swordtail (Xiphophous helleri)ghts: a case for networking, Proceedings
of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 269, 943952.
Edmonds, E. and Briffa, M. (2016) Weak rappers rock more: hermit crabs
assess their own agnostic behaviour, Biology Letters, 12, 20150884.
Elwood, R. W. and Briffa, M. (2001) Information gathering and commu-
nication during agnostic encounters: A case study of hermit crabs,
Advances in the Study of Behaviour, 30, 5397.
Elwood, R. W. and Glass, C. W. (1981) Negotiation or aggression during
shell ghts of the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus,Animal
Behaviour, 29, 12391244.
Foster, W. A. and Treherne, J. E. (1981) Evidence for the dilution effect
in the selsh herd from sh predation on a marine insect, Nature,
293, 466467.
Gherardi, F. (2006) Fighting behavior in hermit crabs: the combined
effect of resource-holding potential and resource value in Pagurus
longicarpus,Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology, 59, 500510.
Gherardi, F., Aquiloni, L. and Tricarico, E. (2012) Behavioral plasticity, behav-
ioral syndromes and animal personality in crustacean decapods: an
imperfect map is better than no map, Current Zoology, 58, 567579.
Gherardi, F. and Vannini, M. (1989) Field observations on activity and
clustering in two intertidal hermit crabs, Clibanarius virescens and
Calcinus laevinmanus (Decapoda, Anomura), Marine & Freshwater
Behaviour & Phy, 14, 145159.
Grand, T. C. and Dill, L. M. (1999) The effect of group size on the for-
aging behaviour of juvenile coho salmon: reduction of predation
risk or increased competition? Animal Behaviour, 58, 443451.
Hamilton, W. D. (1971) Geometry for the selsh herd, Journal of
Theoretical Biology, 31, 295311.
Hazlett, B. A. (1968) Effects of crowding on the agonistic behavior of
the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus,Ecology, 49, 573575.
Hazlett, B. A. (1995) Behavioral plasticity in crustacea: why not more?
Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 29, 5766.
Jandt, J. M., Bengston, S., Pinter-Wollman, N. et al. (2014) Behavioural
syndromes and social insects: personality at multiple levels,
Biological Reviews, 89, 4867.
Krakauer, D. C. (1995) Groups confuse predators by exploiting percep-
tual bottlenecks: a connectionist model of the confusion effect,
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 36, 421429.
Krause, J. and Ruxton, G. D. (2002) Living In Groups, Oxford University
Press, Oxford.
Lewis, S. M. and Rotjan, R. D. (2009) Vacancy chains provide aggregate
benets to Coenobita clypeatus hermit crabs, Ethology : Formerly
Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 115, 356365.
Mathot, K. J. and Dingemanse, N. J. (2014) Plasticity and personality,
Integrative Organismal Biology, 12, 5569.
Miller, R. C. (1922) The signicance of the gregarious habit, Ecology,3,
122126.
Mowles, S. L., Cotton, P. A. and Briffa, M. (2012) Consistent crustaceans:
the identication of stable behavioural syndromes in hermit crabs,
Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 66, 10871094.
Pigliucci, M. (2001) Phenotypic plasticity: beyond nature and nurture,
JHU Press, Maryland.
Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S. and Sarkar, D., R Core Team. (2015)
_nlme: Linear and nonlinear mixed effects models_. R package ver-
sion 3.1122. URL: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme.
Pulliam, H. R. (1973) On the advantages of ocking, Journal of
Theoretical Biology, 38, 419422.
R Core Team. (2015) R: A language and environment for statistical com-
puting. R Foundation for Statistical Computing Vienna, Austria. URL:
https://www.R-project.org.
Rittschof, D., Tsai, D. W., Massey, P. G. et al. (1992) Chemical mediation
of behavior in hermit crabs: alarm and aggregation cues, Journal of
Chemical Ecology, 18, 959984.
.................................................................................................................................................................
YYYY Bioscience Horizons Volume 00 2018
.................................................................................................................................................................
8
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biohorizons/article-abstract/doi/10.1093/biohorizons/hzy011/5253902 by guest on 21 December 2018
Rosen, E., Schwarz, B. and Palmer, A. R. (2009) Smelling the difference:
hermit crab responses to predatory and nonpredatory crabs,
Animal Behaviour, 78, 691695.
Rudin, F. S. and Briffa, M. (2012) Is boldness a resource-holding poten-
tial trait? Fighting prowess and changes in startle response in the
sea anemone Actinia equina,Proceedings of the Royal Society of
London B: Biological Sciences, 279, 19041910.
Scarratt, A. M. and Godin, J. J. (1992) Foraging and antipredator deci-
sions in the hermit crab Pagarus acadianus (Benedict), Journal of
Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 156, 225238.
Taylor, P. R. (1981) Hermit crab tness: The effect of shell condition and
behavioral adaptation environmental resistance, Journal of
Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology, 52, 205218.
Turner, G. F. and Pitcher, T. J. (1986) Attack abatement: a model for
group protection by combined avoidance and dilution, American
Naturalist, 128, 228240.
Uetz, G. W., Boyle, J. A. Y., Hieber, C. S. et al. (2002) Antipredator bene-
ts of group living in colonial web-building spiders: the early warn-
ingeffect, Animal Behaviour, 63, 445452.
Uher, J. (2011) Individual behavioral phenotypes: an integrative meta-
theoretical framework. Why behavioral syndromesare not analogs
of personality, Developmental Psychobiology, 53, 521548.
Wilson,D.S.,Clark,A.B.,Coleman,K.etal.(1994)Shynessandboldnessin
humans and other animals, Trends in Ecology & Evolution,9,442446.
Wolak, M. E., Fairbairn, D. J. and Paulsen, Y. R. (2012) Guidelines for esti-
mating repeatability, Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 3, 129137.
.................................................................................................................................................................
Bioscience Horizons Volume 00 2018
.................................................................................................................................................................
9
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/biohorizons/article-abstract/doi/10.1093/biohorizons/hzy011/5253902 by guest on 21 December 2018
Article
Full-text available
Agonistic behavior is a common behavior among agonistic Chinese mitten crabs (Eriocheir sinensis). Such behavior often leads to limb loss or physical impairment, and significantly affects the survival, growth, and quality of the crabs, and even the yield and economic value for E. sinensis. Agonistic behavior often occurs in agonistic crabs, which is closely related to personality traits and interactive behavior of animals. E. sinensis has personality traits such as boldness, aggression, and exploration as evidenced by the partition-crossing experiment, mirror experiment, and shelter experiment. Agonistic crabs were identified as individuals with boldness, high aggression, and high exploration. The interactive behavior spectrum of E. sinensis was first obtained, consisting of 15 behaviors. This spectrum described and summarized all interactive behaviors of E. sinensis during fights. The interactive behavior characteristics of agonistic crabs were identified as darting, intimidating, grabbing, pushing, stretching, and visitation. These results lay a theoretical basis for in-depth behavioral research on E. sinensis in the future. The technique of identifying agonistic crabs by personality traits and interactive behaviors not only allows for the elimination of agonistic crabs from the aquaculture process and the reduction in negative impacts caused by aggressive crabs, but also allows for the breeding of non-agonistic crabs and the further reduction in the economic losses caused by fighting behaviors.
Article
Full-text available
Boldness of personality is an important theme in animal behavior and has significant ecological and evolutionary consequences. Studies on boldness in crustaceans typically focus on their behavior, while relatively few studies have focused on the formation and maintenance of and change in boldness, such as energy metabolism and neurotransmission. In this study, we measured the boldness of swimming crabs (Portunus trituberculatus) and analyzed the relationship between boldness and oxygen consumption rate, energy concentration, and the relative expression of energy-metabolism-related and 5-HT genes in mRNA. The results showed that boldness remained stable across repeated tests but changed under dangerous conditions. Swimming crabs could be divided into bold and shy individuals. Bold individuals consumed oxygen at a significantly higher rate than shy individuals. Lactate and glucose concentrations in hemolymph were significantly lower in bold individuals than in shy individuals, and mRNA relative expression of Na+/K+-ATPase and 5-HT genes was significantly higher in bold than in shy individuals. Preliminary results indicate that energy metabolism and neurotransmitters may underlie the formation and maintenance of personality characteristics of swimming crabs. Swimming crabs also exhibit behavioral flexibility in order to cope with risks. This may be an adaptation to their complex environments.
Book
Full-text available
Journal of the Graduate Course of the Humboldt-Universität and the Museum für Naturkunde Berlin at at the AWI, List (Sylt)
Article
Full-text available
Fighting animals use a variety of information sources to make strategic decisions. A neglected potential source of information is an individual's own performance during a fight. Surprisingly, this possibility has yet to be incorporated into the large body of theory concerning the evolution of aggressive behaviour. Here, by experimentally dampening the impact of their shell rapping behaviour, we test for the possibility that attacking hermit crabs monitor their own fight performance. Attackers with dampened raps did not show a reduction in the number of raps used. By contrast, they showed an increased frequency of a less intense agonistic behaviour, shell rocking. This change in behaviour, in attackers that are forced to rap weakly, indicates that they assess their own agonistic behaviour.
Article
Full-text available
The hermit crab Pagurus acadianus (Benedict) possesses two major defences against mobile aquatic predators, viz., fleeing and refuging in its gastropod shell. When approached by a potential predator, a crab must choose to either continue its current activity (e.g., feeding), flee or hide in its shell. If a foraging crab flees when threatened, it must secondarily decide whether to carry its food item and, if so, how far to carry it whilst escaping. Alternatively, if the crab takes refuge in its shell, it must then decide how long to remain hidden and when to resume locomotory activity following emergence from the shell. Because these behavioural alternatives have associated costs (e.g., energy expenditure, predation risk, lost foraging opportunity), the crabs' decision as to which behaviour to adopt should be sensitive to the respective cost of each. We tested specific predictions of this general economic hypothesis of behavioural decision making by varying the weight of food items presented to crabs (= lost feeding opportunity cost, energetic cost of carry) and the amount of time the crabs were "handled" by a predator following "capture" (= predation risk). Most crabs tested fled from an approaching lobster predator model. Contrary to expectation, flight initiation distance was unaffected by the mass of the food item available to the foraging crabs. As predicted, however, the distances fleeing crabs carried individual food items varied inversely with the food's weight. Time spent hiding in the shell and latency to resume locomotion following a predator "attack" were relatively unaffected by the size of the food item available. As expected, the crabs' decision to emerge from their shell following a threat appeared sensitive to their perceived risk of predation, as evidence by an observed positive relationship between refuging time (in shell) and the duration of preceding handling of the crab by the predator. These findings are interpreted within the cost-benefit framework of behavioural decision making.
Article
Full-text available
Consistent between-individual differences in behavior have been demonstrated in an array of species from diverse taxa, and variation in boldness may be associated with variation in aggressiveness. However, little is known about how boldness is linked with the ability to win fights (resource holding potential) or about how the experience of fighting may alter subsequent boldness. Animal contests often involve role asymmetries, where the 2 opponents fight in different ways. Here, we investigate boldness before and after fighting in attacking and defending hermit crabs during contests over the ownership of gastropod shells. Although prefight boldness did not influence the chance of winning for attackers, successful defenders had longer startle responses (less bold) than those that gave up. Postfight changes in boldness also differed between roles. For defenders, there was a significant decline in consistency of startle responses after fighting, coupled with outcome-dependent plasticity in mean boldness. Furthermore, postfight boldness in defenders varied with the intensity of agonistic behavior inflicted on them by attackers. In contrast, boldness in attackers was stable across the before-and after-fighting situations. Links between internal state and agonistic behavior are known to vary between roles in asymmetric contests. It now appears that similar role-specific links are present between aggression and animal personality.
Article
Full-text available
When animals are observed on multiple occasions, consistent between-individual differences in behaviour, often referred to as animal personality, may be observed. However, this does not mean that the behaviour of a given individual is readily predictable. While some individuals show low levels of variation around their behavioural mean, others show high levels of variation, and there may be significant between-individual differences in this intraindividual variation ('IIV') in behaviour. While it has been suggested that IIV might reduce susceptibility to predators, little is known about the functions or causation of IIV. We investigated the effects of temperature on the startle response duration of hermit crabs, Pagurus bernhardus. For poikilothermic animals, temperature has a direct and multiplicative influence on metabolic rate, which in turn is expected to influence behaviour because of its effect on energy requirements. At the level of mean startle response durations, the effect of temperature was dependent on treatment order; within treatment orders, individuals showed different reaction norms. In contrast, at the level of IIV, while the presence of significant between-individual differences was dependent on treatment order, there was a clear unidirectional effect in both treatment orders for individuals to be less predictable at higher temperature. Thus, predictability in behaviour appears to vary with a key environmental variable that is known to influence energy requirements and potentially the level of risk that individuals are willing to accept.
Article
Aggregation is a well documented behaviour in a number of animal groups. The “confusion effect” is one mechanism thought to mitigate the success of predators feeding on gregarious prey and hence favour aggregation. An artificial neural network model of prey targeting is developed to explore the advantages prey species might derive through a tendency to group. The network illustrates how an abstract model of the computational mechanisms mediating the perception of prey position is able to show a degradation in performance as group size increases. The relationship between group size and predator confusion has a characteristic decreasing decelerating shape. Prey “oddity” is shown to reduce the impact of the confusion effect, thereby allowing predators to target prey more accurately. Hence shoaling behaviour is most profitable to the prey when prey phenotypes are visually indistinguishable to a predator. Futhermore it is shown that prey “oddity” is relatively more costly in large groups than in small groups and the implications for assortative schooling are discussed. Both the model and the results are intended to make the general point that cognitive constraints will limit the information that a nervous system can process at a number of different levels of neural organization.
Chapter
Behavioral ecology has traditionally used what is known as the optimality approach to study animal behavior. This approach is typically used to predict the optimal expression of a behavioral trait for the average individual in a population given any constraints that may limit the expression of that trait (Davies et al. 2012). For example, in order to minimize the probability of being depredated, individuals can invest in anti-predator vigilance. However , individuals are limited in how much time they can invest in vigilance, because time is a limited resource and individuals must also engage in other time consuming behaviors such as foraging. The trade-off between the costs and benefits of increased time invested in vigilance leads to individuals accruing the maximum net-benefit from vigilance at some intermediate level of expression, and this level of expression is considered optimal. The optimality approach has been successfully used to predict mean behaviors in populations but not variation among individuals. However, individuals often show consistent differences in behavior over time or across contexts, a phenomenon referred to as animal personality. This is represented statistically by a significant proportion (i.e., repeatability, r) of the total variation in behavior occurring between individuals [i.e., V between individuals /(V between individuals + V within individuals)], and suggests that individuals may differ from the optimum or mean behavior at the population level. For example, female Ural owls (Strix uralensis) aggressively defend their nests, and while the intensity of nest defense varies depending on brood size or environmental factors like food availability, an
Article
The effects of increased population density were studied in the hermit crab Pagurus bernhardus. Without periods of adjustment between increments of population increase, the number of aggressive interactions increased rapidly, and the proportion of fights decided by physical contact increased significantly as density was increased. With period of adjustment, there were no marked increases in aggression as density increased.