ArticlePDF Available

The nature of the association between number line and mathematical performance: An international twin study

Wiley
British Journal of Educational Psychology
Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Background The number line task assesses the ability to estimate numerical magnitudes. People vary greatly in this ability, and this variability has been previously associated with mathematical skills. However, the sources of individual differences in number line estimation and its association with mathematics are not fully understood. Aims This large‐scale genetically sensitive study uses a twin design to estimate the magnitude of the effects of genes and environments on: (1) individual variation in number line estimation and (2) the covariation of number line estimation with mathematics. Samples We used over 3,000 8‐ to 16‐year‐old twins from the United States, Canada, the United Kingdom, and Russia, and a sample of 1,456 8‐ to 18‐year‐old singleton Russian students. Methods Twins were assessed on: (1) estimation of numerical magnitudes using a number line task and (2) two mathematics components: fluency and problem‐solving. Results Results suggest that environments largely drive individual differences in number line estimation. Both genes and environments contribute to different extents to the number line estimation and mathematics correlation, depending on the sample and mathematics component. Conclusions Taken together, the results suggest that in more heterogeneous school settings, environments may be more important in driving variation in number line estimation and its association with mathematics, whereas in more homogeneous school settings, genetic effects drive the covariation between number line estimation and mathematics. These results are discussed in the light of development and educational settings.
Content may be subject to copyright.
British Journal of Educational Psychology (2018)
©2018 The British Psychological Society
www.wileyonlinelibrary.com
The nature of the association between number line
and mathematical performance: An international
twin study
Maria Grazia Tosto
1a
, Gabrielle Garon-Carrier
2a
, Susan Gross
3
,
Stephen A. Petrill
4
, Sergey Malykh
1,5
, Karim Malki
6
, Sara A. Hart
7
,
Lee Thompson
3
, Rezhaw L. Karadaghi
6
, Nikita Yakovlev
1
,
Tatiana Tikhomirova
5
, John E. Opfer
4
, Mich
ele M. M. Mazzocco
8
,
Ginette Dionne
2
, Mara Brendgen
9
, Frank Vitaro
10
,
Richard E. Tremblay
1,10,11
, Michel Boivin
1,2
and Yulia Kovas
1,6,12
*
1
Laboratory for Cognitive Investigations and Behavioral Genetics, Department of
Psychology, Institute of Genetic, Neurobiological, and Social Foundations of Child
Development, Tomsk State University, Tomsk, Oblast, Russia
2
School of Psychology, Universit
e Laval, Qu
ebec City, Qu
ebec, Canada
3
Department of Psychological Sciences, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland,
Ohio, USA
4
Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, Ohio, USA
5
Psychological Institute, Russian Academy of Education, Moscow, Russia
6
MRC Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry,
Psychology& Neuroscience, King’s College London, UK
7
Department of Psychology, Florida Center for Reading Research, The Florida State
University, Tallahassee, Florida, USA
8
Institute of Child Development, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis,Minnesota, USA
9
Department of Psychology, School of Psychology, Universit
eduQu
ebec
a Montr
eal,
Qu
ebec, Canada
10
Department of Psychoeducation, Department of Pediatrics and Psychology,
Universit
e de Montr
eal, Qu
ebec, Canada
11
School of Public Health, Physiotherapy and Sports Science, University College
Dublin, Belfield, Dublin 4, Ireland
12
Department of Psychology, University of London, UK
Background. The number line task assesses the ability to estimate numerical
magnitudes. People vary greatly in this ability, and this variability has been previously
*Correspondence should be addressed to Yulia Kovas, Department of Psychology Goldsmiths University of London, LondonSE14
6NW, UK (email: y.kovas@gold.ac.uk).
a
Joint first authors.
DOI:10.1111/bjep.12259
1
associated with mathematical skills. However, the sources of individual differences in
number line estimation and its association with mathematics are not fully understood.
Aims. This large-scale genetically sensitive study uses a twin design to estimate the
magnitude of the effects of genes and environments on: (1) individual variation in number
line estimation and (2) the covariation of number line estimation with mathematics.
Samples. We used over 3,000 8- to 16-year-old twins from the United States, Canada,
the United Kingdom, and Russia, and a sample of 1,456 8- to 18-year-old singleton Russian
students.
Methods. Twins were assessed on: (1) estimation of numerical magnitudes using a
number line task and (2) two mathematics components: fluency and problem-solving.
Results. Results suggest that environments largely drive individual differences in
number line estimation. Both genes and environments contribute to different extents to
the number line estimation and mathematics correlation, depending on the sample and
mathematics component.
Conclusions. Taken together, the results suggest that in more heterogeneous school
settings, environments may be more important in driving variation in number line estimation
and its association with mathematics, whereas in more homogeneous school settings, genetic
effects drive the covariation between number line estimation and mathematics. These results
are discussed in the light of development and educational settings.
Numerical competencies, such as the awareness of quantities and the understanding of
numerical magnitudes, are considered vital precursors of counting skills (Gelman &
Gallistel, 1978), formal arithmetic (Wynn, 1992) and for the development of advanced
mathematical abilities (Dehaene, 1997). One task often used to assess individuals’
numerical magnitude representation is the number line task (Siegler & Opfer, 2003).
Trials of the task usually show a horizontal line representing a range of numerical values,
indicated by the numbers marking the edges of the line. Participants are asked to estimate
the position of target numerals, within the range, along the line. Scores indicate
participants’ accuracy based on how close their estimation is to the correct number
position. The mainstream literature suggests that numerical thinking underlying this task
relates to how individuals mentally represent quantities, how these representations are
tagged by number symbols, and how numbers are related to each other. Recent empirical
evidence suggests that number line estimation tasks entail judgements of proportions
(Barth & Paladino, 2011; Slusser & Barth, 2017).
Irrespectively of the processes involved in number line estimation, greater accuracy in
this task predicts greater achievement in mathematics (Booth & Siegler, 2008; Fuchs et al.,
2010; Geary, 2011; Sasanguie, G
obel, Moll, Smets, & Reynvoet, 2013; Vukovic et al., 2014).
This finding has been reliably replicated across samples in different countries, Sweden
(Tr
aff, 2013), China (Muldoon, Simms, Towse, Menzies, & Yue, 2011; Siegler & Mu, 2008),
the Amazonian tribe of Munduruku (Pica, Lemer, Izard, & Dehaene, 2004), Denmark
(Sasanguie et al., 2013), and the United States (Booth & Siegler, 2008), and across ages
ranging from preschool (4- to 5-year-olds) (Kolkman, Kroesbergen, & Leseman, 2013) to
school-age children (~12-year-olds; Lyons, Price, Vaessen, Blomert, & Ansari, 2014).
Indeed, accuracy in estimation of numerical magnitude improves with age (Laski &
Siegler, 2007; Siegler & Opfer, 2003) and children are less accurate than adults in number
line task performance (Siegler & Opfer, 2003). For example, representation of numbers 1
10 is generally more accurate among 6- to 7-year-olds than representation of numbers 1
100 in the same children (Laski & Siegler, 2007). This evidence suggests the involvement
of age maturation processes in the development of number line estimation skills. Further,
2Maria Grazia Tosto et al.
a male advantage in number line estimation was found in previous studies (Gunderson,
Ramirez, Beilock, & Levine, 2012; LeFevre et al., 2010; Thompson & Opfer, 2008),
indicating that sex differences may also be a contributing factor in such variations.
Experience and practice with numbers have been found to play a role in number line
estimation accuracy (Moeller, Pixner, Kaufmann, & Nuerk, 2009; Pica et al., 2004).
Modest differences in number line performance were also detected in some cross-
cultural studies. For example, 7-year-old Italian students showed better performance in
number line estimation as they committed on average less error (17.78) than their age-
matched Austrian, German-speaking peers (21.06) (Helmreich et al., 2011). Chinese 5- to
6-year-old children showed a superior number line performance compared to age-
matched children from the United States (Siegler & Mu, 2008). Conversely, no differences
in number line performance were observed between Chinese and Scottish 4- to 6-year-old
children, although the two samples differed in mathematical performance (Muldoon
et al., 2011). Considering that the mentioned studies used relatively small samples, and
more likely not representative of their populations, some of the observed cross-cultural
differences in number line performance may stem from sample bias. However, differences
may also stem from environmental differences as social context/culture (e.g., educational
norms, social constructs, or linguistic features) have also been found associated with
number line performance (Ito & Hatta, 2004; Ramscar, Dye, Popick, & O’Donnell-
McCarthy, 2011; Shaki & Fischer, 2008; Wagner, Kimura, Cheung, & Barner, 2015).
Beyond environmental differences that may underlie some of the observed cross-
cultural differences in number line performance, average differences in the frequency of
particular genetic variants across populations may contribute to the observed differences
in number line across cultures. Indeed, genes and cultures are not independent as they are
likely to co-evolve (geneculture co-evolution, Laland, Odling-Smee, & Myles, 2010).
Genes and culture are two interacting forms of inheritance, with offspring acquiring both
a genetic and a cultural legacy from their ancestors. Genetic propensities expressed
throughout development influence what cultural organisms learn, while culturally
transmitted information expressed in behaviour and artefacts spreads through popula-
tions and may influence how genes are selected and expressed (Laland et al., 2010).
Research so far suggests that both genetic and environmental mechanisms play a role
in the development of number line estimation skills. In order to disentangle the
contribution of genetic and environmental effects, genetically sensitive studies are
required. In this study, we use a twin design with samples from four countries to estimate
the relative genetic and environmental contribution to number line task performance.
Understanding the contribution of genes and environment on number line estimation
performance is particularly relevant because of its association with mathematics. The few
genetically sensitive studies that investigated the sources of variation in different aspects
of mathematical abilities suggest the influences of genetic (heritability) and environmen-
tal factors. For example, one study conducted with 10-year-old US twins found moderate
heritability for math calculation (35%) and math fluency (34%), and slightly higher
heritability for applied problems (41%) and quantitative concepts (49%) (Hart, Petrill, &
Thompson, 2010; Hart, Petrill, Thompson, & Plomin, 2009). Similarly, a study conducted
with 10-year-old UK twins assessed on three mathematical subtests, understanding of
algebraic, understanding of non-numerical processes, and computational knowledge,
found heritability estimates between 32% and 45%, non-shared environmental influences
between 42% and 48%, and almost non-existent shared environmental contribution
(Kovas, Haworth, Petrill, & Plomin, 2007). These studies also suggested similar aetiology
among those mathematic components (Hart et al., 2009, 2010), indicated by large genetic
Number line and mathematics 3
overlaps, that is largely the same genes being involved across school achievement and test
assessing understanding of algebraic and understanding of non-numerical processes and
computational knowledge (Kovas, Haworth, Dale, et al., 2007).
Although the nature of the association between mathematics and number line
estimation is unclear, previous research suggests that common genetic factors are mainly
responsible for the covariation between mathematics and other abilities. For example,
genes in common between reading and mathematics explain between 57% and 98% of
their observed association (e.g., Haworth et al., 2009; Thompson, Detterman, & Plomin,
1991), while common genetic factors explain ~70% of the covariation between general
intelligence and mathematics (67%, Kovas, Harlaar, Petrill, & Plomin, 2005; 73%,
Trzaskowski et al., 2013), and ~60% between spatial abilities and mathematics (Tosto,
Hanscombe, et al., 2014).
The present study
The current study uses a genetically informative twin design to explore: (1) the relative
contribution of genetic and environmental factors to individual differences in number line
estimation skills. Given the phenotypic sex differences in number line, we also investigate
the contribution of genetic and environmental effects on male and female performance in
number line as well as mathematics; and (2) the extent to which genetic and
environmental factors drive the covariation between number line estimation and
mathematics assessed with two components: fluency and problem-solving. Using twins
from different countries will allow to uncover the effects of genetic and environmental
factors on the measures in different culturaleducational settings. A sample of singleton
students was also included to further understand the generalizability of the developmental
pattern observed in number line estimation, from twins to the general population
(Plomin, DeFries, Knopik, & Neiderheiser, 2013).
Methods
Participants
The four twin samples are drawn from four ongoing longitudinal twin studies in the
United States (US), Canada (CA), the United Kingdom (UK), and Russia (RU). From
each study, the samples are selected at the age of the data collection of number line
and mathematics as follows: 492 English-speaking twins (246 pairs) from the US-based
‘Western Reserve Reading and Math Project’ (WRRMP; Hart et al., 2009) with age
range between age 8 and 15 years (M=12.27, SD =1.20); 674 (mostly French-
speaking) twins (337 pairs) from the Canadian ‘Quebec Newborn Twin Study’ (QNTS;
Boivin et al., 2013) at age 15 (M=15.17; SD =0.29); 5,100 English-speaking twins
(2,550 pairs) from the UK representative ‘Twins Early Development Study’ (TEDS;
Tosto et al., 2017) at age 16 (M=16.48; SD =0.27); 178 Russian-speaking twin pairs
from the ‘Russian School Twin Registry’ (RSTR; Kovas et al., 2013) at age 16
(M=16.44; SD =0.91); and 1,456 Russian singletons with age ranging from 7.51 to
18.85 years (M=12.30; SD =3.16).
Given the wide age range of the Russian singletons and the observed age-related
developmental changes in number line performance, the singletons’ results are presented
on the sample divided by age in two groups: younger (age <15.99; M=11.42; SD =2.59)
and older (age >16; M=17.14; SD =0.74). This makes the age range of the two
4Maria Grazia Tosto et al.
singletons’ groups closer to the twin samples’ age range. Further details of samples and
their recruitment can be found in Supplementary Online Material.
Measures and procedures
The number line and mathematics tests were embedded in large cognitive and
behavioural test batteries administered to participants as part of the data collections in
the longitudinal studies. The UK, Canadian, and Russian twins completed their
assessment online. US twins were assessed in person and completed the tests in pen
and paper format. No mathematical data were available for the Russian singletons, but
they completed the same online number line task as UK, Canadian, and Russian twins.
Number line estimation task
Number Line estimation task was used to assess estimation of numerical magnitudes. The
version used was adapted from a description in Opfer and Siegler (2007).
Mathematical abilities
Data on mathematical ability were collected in two domains: fluency and problem-solving.
Problem Verification Task (Murphy & Mazzocco, 2008) and Understanding Numbers
test (NferNelson Publishing Co. Ltd, 1994, 1999, 2001) were used to measure,
respectively, fluency and problem-solving in the UK, Canadian, and Russian twins. US
twins’ fluency and problem-solving skills were assessed with Fluency and Applied
Problems, both subtests from WoodcockJohnson III Tests of Achievement (WJ-III,
McGrew, Dailey, & Schrank, 2007; Woodcock, McGrew, & Mather, 2001). Details on the
measures and their reliability can be found in Supplementary Online Material.
Analyses
The twin method compares monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) within-pair twins’
correlations (intraclass correlations) to estimate the contributions of genes (h
2
, heritability),
shared (c
2
), and non-shared (e
2
) environments to individual differences (univariate models)
and of sex differences in traits (univariate sex-limitation models). The method also
allows to estimate the contribution of genes and environments to traits’ covariation
(multivariate genetic models). Details of the twin method can be found in Supplementary
Online Material.
Results
Prior to analyses, a log 10 transformation was applied to Number Line estimation to
correct for non-normality. To control for the contribution of age and sex to the twin
correlations (Eaves, Eysenck, & Martin, 1989), twin analyses were conducted on age
and sex residualized and standardized variables (mean of 0 and standard deviation of
1.0). As analyses were conducted in each sample separately, we treated outliers in
accordance with data preparation practices of each study for consistency with other
publications reporting on the variables analysed in the current study (see
Supplementary Online Material for details of previous publications using the same
measures). Scores outside 3 standard deviations were excluded as outliers from
Number line and mathematics 5
analyses of UK twins and Russian singletons; data were winsorized to 3 standard
deviations in US and Russian twins and winsorized to the 97th percentile for the
Canadian twins.
The correlation between Problem Verification (fluency) and Understanding Numbers
(problem-solving) in CA, the UK and RU were respectively: r=.57, 95% confidence
interval (CI) (.49; .64); r=.67 (.63; .68); and r=.64 (.53; .72). In the US twins, the
correlation between WJ-III subtests Fluency and Applied Problems was r=.47 (.37; .57).
Therefore, fluency and problem-solving measures were combined into a single score in
each sample (Mathematics Composite) by averaging the standardized means. Results are
reported for the single measures and the Mathematics Composite.
Phenotypic analyses
The correlation between Number Line and the Mathematics Composite was strikingly
similar in the four samples (average correlation =.43, range =.40 and .45). When
the correlation of Number Line was carried out with the mathematics components of
fluency and problem-solving respectively, some differences emerged across the samples.
The three twin samples that used the same tests (CA, the UK, and RU in Figure 1) showed
very similar correlations between Number Line estimation and Problem Verification
(fluency) (average correlation =.42, range =.38 and .44) and between Number
Line estimation and Understanding Numbers (problem-solving) (average .37, range
.36 and .38). The 95% CIs were largely overlapping, suggesting that the correlations
may not be significantly different in these three samples. In the US twins, the correlation
WRRMP
QNTS
TEDS
RSTR
Twin samples
Fluency Problem-solving Mathematics composite
enilrebmunhtiwserusaemscita
meh
tamnoitalerroC
–.10
–.15
–.20
–.25
–.30
–.35
–.40
–.45
–.50
–.55
–.60
–.20
–.44
–.38
–.43
–.49
–.3 7 –.38
–.36
–.4 5
–.40
–.42
–.45
Figure 1. Phenotypic correlations. Magnitudes of the phenotypic correlation between Number Line
and the mathematics measures are marked on the Y-axis. The whiskers represent their 95% confidence
intervals (CIs). Mathematics measures are marked on the X-axis: fluency (Problem Verification in the
United Kingdom, Canada and Russia; Fluency WJ-III in the United States), problem-solving (Understand-
ing Numbers in the United Kingdom, Canada and Russia; Applied Problems WJ-III in the United States),
and Mathematics Composite. See Table S7 for the exact values of correlations and CIs.
6Maria Grazia Tosto et al.
between Number Line estimation and Fluency (WJ-III) was weaker (.20) and the
correlation between Number Line estimation and Applied Problems (problem-solving;
WJ-III) was stronger (.49) compared to the correlation of Number Line and the same
mathematical domains in the other three samples. Summary of phenotypic correlations
between number line estimation and mathematics measures is presented in Figure 1 and
Table S7.
Descriptive statistics for each sample are presented in Table S1. The oldest samples
were the most accurate in Number Line estimation, and the youngest samples were the
least accurate. Number Line estimation scores were smaller (less error, hence more
accurate) in the older groups (Canadian, UK and Russian twins, and older Russian
singletons) than in the two youngest groups (US twins and the younger Russian
singletons). In the UK sample, the largest and the most age-homogeneous, participants
were the most accurate on average. The median of the Number Line task in the six groups
of twins and singletons shows a pattern consistent with the previously reported increase
estimation accuracy with age (Table S1).
Males were on average more accurate than females in Number Line and mathematics
measures, except for the Russian twins where females performed slightly better on the
mathematical tests. However, these sex differences were negligible, ranging between
0.0% and 3.1% of variance across samples.
Twin analyses
Univariate models
Twin genetic analyses were conducted only in the United States, Canada, and the United
Kingdom because the Russian sample is smaller than the others and currently
underpowered for these analyses. Given the differences in age and sample size, these
analyses were conducted separately in each sample.
MZ intraclass twin correlations (ICCs) were greater than DZ correlations for all measures,
suggesting some genetic influences on Number Line estimation and mathematics measures
(see Figure 2 and Table S3). Heritability estimates for Number Line estimation were modest:
.16 in the United States; .35 in Canada; .27 in the United Kingdom. The contribution of shared
environmental factors was: .34 in United States; .08 in Canada; and virtuallyzero in the United
Kingdom. Non-shared environmental factors were the most important component in all
samples: .50 in the United States; .57 in Canada; and .73 in the United Kingdom. Variance in
the mathematics measures was explained by genetic, non-shared, and shared environmental
factors in the United States, whereas in Canada and the United Kingdom, variance in
mathematics was largely explained by genetic and non-shared environmental factors. For
example, heritability of the Mathematics Composite was .46 in the United States, .59 in
Canada, and .64 in the United Kingdom; shared environmental sources of variance were
significant only in the United States (.40); non-shared environments were .14 in United States,
.41 in Canada, and .31 in United Kingdom (see Table S4).
Univariate sex-limitation models were fitted only to UK data, as the sample has the
adequate size to be divided into 5 sex-by-zygosity groups required for these analyses (Eley,
2005). ICCs for the 5 sex-by-zygosity groups are presented in Table S3. Sex-limitation
model-fitting analyses revealed no qualitative or quantitative sex differences in Number
Line estimation, Problem Verification (fluency), and Mathematics Composite (Table S5).
This suggests that, for these measures, the same genetic and environmental factors
explain individual variation to the same extent in males and females. For Understanding
Number line and mathematics 7
Numbers (problem-solving), small quantitative sex differences were detected, indicating
significantly higher heritability for females. It should be noted that aetiological sex
differences might not necessarily give rise to phenotypic sex differences (Kovas,
Haworth, Dale, et al., 2007). Small but significant differences in variances between males
and females were also observed for this measure (see footnote Table S5).
Multivariate models
In Canada and the United Kingdom, the cross-trait cross-twin correlations were greater in
MZ than DZ on all measures, suggesting genetic effects in the covariation of all measures.
In the United States, the MZ cross-trait correlations were equal or smaller than the DZ
correlations, suggesting greater environmental contribution in the covariation of the
measures (Table S6). Figure 3 depicts the extent to which genetic and environmental
factors account for the phenotypic (observed) correlations of Number Line estimation
with fluency and problem-solving measures and Mathematics Composite in the three
samples. For example, 82% of the phenotypic correlation between Number Line
estimation and Problem Verification (fluency) in the UK sample (r=.38) is explained by
common genetic factors. Non-shared environmental factors explain the remaining 18% of
the correlation.
In the US sample, a significant genetic correlation was found between Number Line
estimation and Applied Problems (problem-solving), while the association between
Number Line and the other two mathematics variables (fluency and composite) was
explained by common shared and non-shared environmental factors (see Table S7).
Conversely, genetic influences were mainly responsible for the covariation between
Number Line estimation and mathematics in Canada and the United Kingdom (Figure 3);
in these samples, almost all shared environmental correlations were non-significant while
all genetic correlations were significant (Table S7).
Figure 2. Univariate heritability estimates. The bars represent the variance of each measure
decomposed in genetic (h
2
, red portion), shared (c
2
, green portion), and non-shared environmental
(e
2
, blue portion) variances. The first block of bars represents heritability and environmental estimates for
number Line Estimation in the United States, Canada, and the United Kingdom, respectively. The second
block shows heritability for fluency (Fluency WJ-III in the United States, Problem Verification in Canada
and the United Kingdom). The third block shows heritability for the problem-solving component (Applied
Problems WJ-III in the United States, Understanding Numbers test in Canada and the United Kingdom).
The last block shows heritability for the Mathematics Composite. All the univariate heritability estimates
are statistically significant; details can be found in Table S4.
8Maria Grazia Tosto et al.
Discussion
This study investigated the genetic and environmental sources of individual differences in
estimation of numerical magnitudes on a number line task, and of the covariation between
number line estimation and mathematics measures of fluency and problem-solving. It
further explored genetic and environmental contribution to sex differences in estimation
of numerical magnitudes and in mathematics.
The results showed that sources of individual differences in Number Line estimation
and mathematics measures differed across populations. We found larger contribution of
shared environmental factors in the United States and greater influences of non-shared
environmental factors in Canada and the United Kingdom. However, the nature of
individual differences in Number Line was the same for males and females, suggesting that
Figure 3. Bivariate heritability estimates. The length of each bar equals the magnitude of the phenotypic
correlation between Number Line estimation and the mathematics variables. The portions in each bar
represent the contribution of genetic (h
2
-bivariate, red), shared environmental (c
2
-bivariate, green), and
non-shared environmental (e
2
-bivariate, blue) influences to the phenotypic correlation between Number
Line and the relevant mathematics measure. The bars are organized in three blocks showing genetic,
shared, and non-shared environmental contribution to the phenotypic correlation between: Number
Line and fluency (Fluency WJ-III in United States, Problem Verification in Canada and the United
Kingdom), Number Line and problem-solving (Applied Problems in the United States, Understanding
Numbers in Canada and the United Kingdom), and Number Line and the Mathematics Composites. The
asterisk (*) indicates when the estimate of the bivariate heritability is non-significant. Estimates of the
bivariate heritability depicted in this figure can be found in Table S7.
Number line and mathematics 9
any sex differences observed in mathematical ability (e.g., Reilly, Neumann, & Andrews,
2015) are unlikely to be related to Number Line estimation skills. The covariation between
Number Line estimation and mathematical performance was largely driven by shared
environmental component in the United States but was mainly driven by genetic factors in
Canada and the United Kingdom.
Data from all countries supported the typical developmental trend whereby younger
students were less accurate than older ones in number line performance. The same
patterns of results observed in twins were replicated in non-twin participants. This
suggests that results derived from twins in this study can be extended to the general
population.
Which factors contribute to individual differences in number line?
The genetic analyses conducted in the samples from the United Kingdom, Canada and the
United States showed that individual differences in number line estimation are largely
driven by individual-specific environments and are only modestly associated with genetic
factors. As reported by previous studies, estimation of numerical magnitudes on a number
line improves with practice, feedback, or relevant experiences (Siegler & Booth, 2004;
Siegler & Mu, 2008). Although some of these factors may be thought of as shared
environments (e.g., quality and quantity of feedback provided by the teacher), they may
act as individual-specific environments by interacting with individual characteristics.
Examples of such interaction may include perceptions and motivation associated with
engaging and practicing number line estimation skills.
Developmental factors may also be responsible for individual variation in number line
estimation. Discrepancies in the magnitude of genetic effects in Number Line
performance of the younger US twins and the genetic effects of the relatively older
Canadian and UK twins may stem from biological or maturational changes across
development. For example, previous research suggests that heritability of general
intelligence increases with age (Davis, Haworth, & Plomin, 2009; Plomin & Deary, 2015).
Some indication of developmental effects was provided by exploratory analyses
suggesting that age, rather than country or culture per se, is the main factor explaining
mean differences in Number Line estimation across samples (details in Table S2).
However, our results as to the effects of development are merely suggestive, hindered by
the differences in sample sizes and other limitations.
Discrepancies in the environmental estimates of number line for the US twins
compared to the Canadian and UK twins may stem from homogeneous school
environments existing in Canada (Quebec) and the United Kingdom. In these countries,
the Government sets both the educational levels and school curricula resulting in a unified
and standardized system across the whole territory. In addition, teachers in both countries
undergo regular standardized training. At the time of this data collection, the US Federal
Government set only the most basic educational standard levels; more specific school
policies, details of public school curricula, and teaching practices were set through local
school boards. The different school policies may give rise to more homogeneous school
environments in the United Kingdom and Canada compared to the United States. In
homogeneous environments, genetic influences may be more significant in driving
individual differences in a trait compared to environmental influences. Lower genetic
influences on cognitive abilities and achievement have been reported in US twin studies
compared to non-US twin studies (Australian and Western European samples). Such
discrepancies are explained by geneenvironment interaction mechanisms whereby
10 Maria Grazia Tosto et al.
genetic effects may be suppressed in conditions of socio-economic inequality (Tucker-
Drob & Bates, 2015). A similar mechanism, related to cross-cultural differences, might also
explain why the mathematical measures showed lower heritability estimates and higher
shared environmental component in the United States than in Canada and the United
Kingdom.
Despite some differences, heritability of Number Line estimation was overall modest in all
samples. Number line estimation skills are thought to be developmentally more basic than
computational skills or more advanced mathematics; number line estimation is often
categorized as core or domain-specific numerical skills (Fuchs et al., 2010). However, the
importance of a basic skill does not mean that individual differences in this skill are genetically
driven. Another measure of basic numerical skills, non-symbolic estimation, has shown
modest influences of genetic factors in normal populations (~30%) with most of the variance
explained by non-shared environmental factors (Lukowski et al., 2017; Tosto, Petrill, et al.,
2014). More genetically sensitive and cross-cultural research at different ages is needed to
investigate possible developmental or maturational changes and the role of homogeneity/
heterogeneity of the environment in the development of number line estimation. For
mathematics, the results of this study are consistent with previous findings, suggesting that
individual differences in mathematics are driven by genetic and environmental factors to
various extents, depending on the mathematics components and sample characteristics.
Which factors explain the covariation between number line and mathematics?
In the United Kingdom and Canada, the covariation between number line estimation and
mathematics was largely driven by genetic factors (85% on average) with the remaining
portion of the phenotypic correlation driven by shared and non-shared environment. In
the United States, the covariation between number line estimation and mathematics was
more strongly driven by shared environmental factors (63% on average) compared to
genetic factors (27% on average). In all the samples, non-shared, individual-specific
environments had small or non-significant influence.
It is possible that the different pattern of association between accuracy in Number Line
estimation and mathematics reflects cultural differences across Canada, the United
Kingdom, and the United States. In the presence of heterogeneous environments such as
varying school curricula (in the US sample), environmental rather than genetic factors are
the driving force shaping up individual differences in number line estimation and of its co-
variation with mathematics. In the presence of more homogeneous common environ-
ments (school), genetic factors (of modest influence) drive variations in number line
estimation in addition to the individual-specific environments (in Canada and United
Kingdom). It needs to be noted that the observed correlation between mathematics and
number line estimation was overall small to modest in all samples. Thus, even if the
genetic and environmental factors contributing to the association were completely
overlapping, Number Line and mathematics remain largely different domains.
Are there sex differences in number line and in mathematics?
Mean sex differences were negligible in all twin samples for all measures, suggesting that
the observed sex differences in mathematical ability are unlikely to be related to number
line estimation. The proportion of genetic and environmental contributions to variation in
Number Line estimation, the Mathematics Composite, and fluency (Problem Verification)
was also the same for males and females.
Number line and mathematics 11
Only the mathematical component of problem-solving (Understanding Numbers)
showed small aetiological quantitative and variance sex differences in the United
Kingdom. This suggests that, in this mathematical component, the same genes and
environments drive individual differences in boys and girls at age 16, but the magnitude of
their effect is different for males and females. The test Understanding Numbers is designed
according to UK school curricula and can be considered a good index of mathematical
achievement. Previous studies in the UK twins have consistently reported small mean
male advantage in mathematics, but no aetiological sex differences (Kovas, Haworth,
Dale, et al., 2007). However, age 16 marks the first time that aetiological sex differences
are detected in mathematics school achievement (General Certificate of Secondary
Education mathematics exams) in this sample (Shakeshaft et al., 2013).
Limitations and conclusion
Diversity in size, age, and schooling guarded against a formal comparison of the UK, US,
and Canadian samples on genetic analyses. One alternative would have been using a
subset of the UK sample to match in size the other two; however, any criteria for carrying
out the matching (e.g., on the basis of socio-economic status, IQ, verbal, non-verbal
ability) would have introduced different confounding elements on the causes of
discrepancies or similarities across the samples. Further, matching would have resulted
in sample-size reduction and we preferred to carry out analyses on the largest number of
participants available to provide reliable heritability estimates (Martin, Eaves, Kearsey, &
Davies, 1978; Martin, Eaves, & Kendler, 1994).
Some discrepancies in results may have stemmed from measurement differences.
For example, the questions of the two problem-solving tests, Understanding Numbers
and Applied Problems, are designed to assess similar cognitive abilities and similar
mathematical domains, but the items are different in the two tests. In the US sample,
all tests were administered in person; in particular, the pen and paper Number Line
test had a very strict administration protocol. This may have generated more accurate
measurements compared to the online tests. Some evidence in support of accuracy
can be found in the US heritability estimates where all the measures showed the
lowest non-shared environment, which in twin model-fitting includes measurement
error. However, US non-shared environment for pen and paper administered Number
Line was very similar to that of the Canadian Number Line test administered online. In
addition, the Number Line tests yielded adequate testretest reliability and internal
validity in all samples, although the latter varies widely across samples (from .63 to
.95). Thus, the discrepancies in the number line results are unlikely to be affected by
differences in administration mode.
As our study could control for age only to some extent, future studies should include age-
homogeneous twin sample from different countries in order to provide further support to
the explanatory role of age in the observed cross-cultural differences. Future research is also
needed to explore the role of other cultural factors such as the reading/writing systems in
number line estimation (Shaki & Fischer, 2008). In our study, all samples had the same left-
to-right writing direction and therefore were not suited to investigate these effects.
These limitations notwithstanding, this study is the first genetically sensitive
investigation into number line estimation skills and mathematics that use data from
twins from different countries. Although the results are not ready to be translated into the
real-world practice, they provide novel insights into the aetiology of individual differences
in number line performance and its covariation with mathematical ability.
12 Maria Grazia Tosto et al.
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by Grant from the Russian Science Foundation (project # 14-48-
00043) to Tomsk State University; by the Russian Science Foundation (project 15-18-30055); by
a Programme Grant (G0901245; previously G0500079) from the U.K. Medical Research
Council (MRC); by Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Development
(HD059215; HD038075), and (HD075460); by the Qu
ebec Ministry of Health, Fonds
Qu
eb
ecois de la Recherche sur la Soci
et
e et la Culture; the Fonds de la Recherche en Sant
e
du Qu
ebec; the Social Science and Humanities Research Council of Canada; the National Health
Research Development Program; the Canadian Institutes for Health Research; and Sainte-
Justine Hospital’s Research Center. Michel Boivin is supported by the Canada Research Chair
(Tier 1) programme.
References
Barth, H. C., & Paladino, A. M. (2011). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence against a
representational shift. Developmental Science,14(1), 125135. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-
7687.2010.00962.x
Boivin, M., Brendgen, M., Dionne, G., Dubois, L., P
erusse, D., Robaey, P., ... Vitaro, F. (2013). The
Quebec Newborn Twin Study: 15 years later. Twin Research and Human Genetics,16,6469.
https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2012.129
Boker, S., Neale, M., Maes, H., Wilde, M., Spiegel, M., Brick, T., ...Fox, J. (2011). OpenMx: An open
source extended structural equation modeling framework. Psychometrika,76, 306317.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-010-9200-6
Booth, J. L., & Siegler, R. S. (2008). Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic
learning. Child Development,79, 10161031. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.
01173.x
Cicchini, G. M., Anobile, G., & Burr, D. C. (2014). Compressive mapping of number to space reflects
dynamic encoding mechanisms, not static logarithmic transform. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,111, 78677872. https://doi.org/10.
1073/pnas.1402785111
Davis, O. S., Haworth, C. M., & Plomin, R. (2009). Dramatic increase in heritability of cognitive
development from early to middle childhood an 8-year longitudinal study of 8,700 Pairs of twins.
Psychological Science,20, 13011308. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02433.x
Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.
Eaves, L. J., Eysenck, H., & Martin, N. G. (1989). Genes, culture, and personality: An empirical
approach. London, UK: Academic Press.
Eley, T. C. (2005). Sex-limitation models. In B. J. Everitt & D. Howell (Eds.), Encyclopedia of
behavioural statistics. West Sussex, UK: Wiley.
Forget-Dubois, N., P
erusse, D., Turecki, G., Girard, A., Billette, J.-M., Rouleau, G., ... Tremblay, R.
(2003). Diagnosing zygosity in infant twins: Physical similarity, genotyping, and chorionicity.
Twin Research,6, 479485. https://doi.org/10.1375/136905203322686464
Fuchs, L. S., Geary, D. C., Compton, D. L., Fuchs, D., Hamlett, C. L., & Bryant, J. D. (2010). The
contributions of numerosity and domain-general abilities to school readiness. Child
Development,81, 15201533. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01489.x
Geary, D. C. (2011). Cognitive predictors of achievement growth in mathematics: A 5-year
longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology,47, 15391552. https://doi.org/10.1037/
a0025510
Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. R. (1978). The child’s understanding of number (1986th ed.).
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Number line and mathematics 13
Goldsmith, H. H. (1991). A zygosity questionnaire for young twins: A research note. Behavior
Genetics,21, 257269. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01065819
Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., Beilock, S. L., & Levine, S. C. (2012). The relation between spatial skill
and early number knowledge: The role of the linear number line. Developmental Psychology,
48, 12291241. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027433
Hart, S. A., Logan, J. A., Thompson, L., Kovas, Y., McLoughlin, G., & Petrill, S. A. (2016). A latent
profile analysis of math achievement, numerosity, and math anxiety in twins. Journal of
Educational Psychology,108, 181193. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000045
Hart, S. A., Petrill, S. A., & Thompson, L. A. (2010). A factorial analysis of timed and untimed measures
of mathematics and reading abilities in school aged twins. Learning and Individual Differences,
20,6369. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2009.10.004
Hart, S. A., Petrill, S. A., Thompson, L. A., & Plomin, R. (2009). The ABCs of math: A genetic analysis of
mathematics and its links with reading ability and general cognitive ability. Journal of
Educational Psychology,101, 388. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015115
Haworth, C. M. A., Davis, O. S. P., & Plomin, R. (2013). Twins Early Development Study (TEDS): A
genetically sensitive investigation of cognitive and behavioral development from childhood to
young adulthood. Twin Research and Human Genetics,16, 117125. https://doi.org/10.1017/
thg.2012.91
Haworth, C. M., Kovas, Y., Harlaar, N., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Petrill, S. A., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R.
(2009). Generalist genes and learning disabilities: A multivariate genetic analysis of low
performance in reading, mathematics, language and general cognitive ability in a sample of 8000
12-year-old twins. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,50, 13181325. https://doi.org/
10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02114.x
Helmreich, I., Zuber, J., Pixner, S., Kaufmann, L., Nuerk, H. C., & Moeller, K. (2011). Language effects
on children’s non-verbal number line estimations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,42,
598613. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022111406026
Ito, Y., & Hatta, T. (2004). Spatial structure of quantitative representation of numbers: Evidence
from the SNARC effect. Memory & Cognition,32, 662673. https://doi.org/10.3758/
BF03195857
Kolkman, M. E., Kroesbergen, E. H., & Leseman, P. P. (2013). Early numerical development and the
role of non-symbolic and symbolic skills. Learning and Instruction,25,95103. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.12.001
Kovas, Y., Galajinsky, E. V., Plomin, R., Boivin, M., Petrill, S. A., Lemelin, J.-P., ...Malykh, S. B. (2013).
The Russian School Twin Registry (RSTR): Project progress. Twin Research and Human
Genetics,16, 126133. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2012.133
Kovas, Y., Garon-Carrier, G., Boivin, M., Petrill, S. A., Plomin, R., Malykh, S. B., ... Brendgen, M.
(2015). Why children differ in motivation to learn: Insights from over 13,000 twins from 6
countries. Personality and Individual Differences,80,5163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.
2015.02.006
Kovas, Y., Harlaar, N., Petrill, S. A., & Plomin, R. (2005). Generalist genes and mathematics in 7-year-
old twins. Intelligence,33, 473489. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2005.05.002
Kovas, Y., Haworth, C. M. A., Dale, P. S., Plomin, R., Weinberg, R. A., Thomson, J. M., & Fischer, K. W.
(2007). The genetic and environmental origins of learning abilities and disabilities in the early
school years. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,72,1160.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.2007.00439.x
Kovas, Y., Haworth, C. M. A., Petrill, S. A., & Plomin, R. (2007). Mathematical ability of 10-year-
old boys and girls: Genetic and environmental etiology of typical and low performance.
Journal of Learning Disabilities,40, 554567. https://doi.org/10.1177/002221940704000
60601
Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., & Myles, S. (2010). How culture shaped the human genome: Bringing
genetics and the human sciences together. Nature Reviews Genetics,11, 137148. https://doi.
org/10.1038/nrg2734
14 Maria Grazia Tosto et al.
Laski, E. V., & Siegler, R. S. (2007). Is 27 a big number? Correlational and causal connections among
numerical categorization, number line estimation, and numerical magnitude comparison. Child
Development,78, 17231743. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01087.x
LeFevre, J. A., Fast, L., Skwarchuk, S. L., Smith-Chant, B. L., Bisanz, J., Kamawar, D., & Penner-Wilger,
M. (2010). Pathways to mathematics: Longitudinal predictors of performance. Child
Development,81, 17531767. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01508.x
Lukowski, S. L., Rosenberg-Lee, M., Thompson, L. A., Hart, S. A., Willcutt, E. G., Olson, R. K., .. .
Pennington, B. (2017). Approximate number sense shares etiological overlap with mathematics
and general cognitive ability. Intelligence,65,6774. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2017.08.
005
Lukowski, S. L., Soden, B., Hart, S. A., Thompson, L. A., Kovas, Y., & Petrill, S. A. (2014). Etiological
distinction of working memory components in relation to mathematics. Intelligence,47,5462.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.09.001
Lyons, I. M., Price, G. R., Vaessen, A., Blomert, L., & Ansari, D. (2014). Numerical predictors of
arithmetic success in grades 16. Developmental Science,17, 714726. https://doi.org/10.
1111/desc.12152
Martin, N. G., Eaves, L. J., Kearsey, M. J., & Davies, P. (1978). The power of the classical twin study.
Heredity,40(1), 97116. https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.1978.10
Martin, N. G., Eaves, L. J., & Kendler, K. S. (1994). The power of the classical twin study to resolve
variation in threshold traits. Behavior Genetics,24, 239258. https://doi.org/10.1007/
BF01067191
McGrew, K. S., Dailey, D. E. H., & Schrank, F. A. (2007). Woodcock-Johnson iii/Woodcock-Johnson
iii normative update score differences: What the user can expect and why (Woodcock-Johnson
iii assessment service bulletin no. 9). Rolling Meadows, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Moeller, K., Pixner, S., Kaufmann, L., & Nuerk, H. C. (2009). Children’s early mental number line:
Logarithmic or decomposed linear? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,103, 503515.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.006
Muldoon, K., Simms, V., Towse, J., Menzies, V., & Yue, G. (2011). Cross-cultural comparisons of 5-
year-olds’ estimating and mathematical ability. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology,42, 669
681. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022111406035
Murphy, M. M., & Mazzocco, M. M. (2008). Mathematics learning disabilities in girls with fragile X or
Turner syndrome during late elementary school. Journal of Learning Disabilities,41,2946.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219407311038
NferNelson Publishing Co. Ltd (1994). Maths 514 Series. London, UK: Author.
NferNelson Publishing Co. Ltd (1999). Maths 514 Series. London, UK: Author.
NferNelson Publishing Co. Ltd (2001). Maths 514 Series. London, UK: Author.
Opfer, J. E. (2003). Analyzing the number-line task: A tutorial. http://www.psy.cmu.edu/siegler/
SiegOpfer03Tut.pdf
Opfer, J. E., & Siegler, R. S. (2007). Representational change and children’s numerical estimation.
Cognitive Psychology,55, 169195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.09.002
Petrill, S. A., Deater-Deckard, K., Thompson, L. A., DeThorne, L. S., & Schatschneider, C. (2006).
Reading skills in early readers: Genetic and shared environmental influences. Journal of
Learning Disabilities,39,4855. https://doi.org/10.1177/00222194060390010501
Pica, P., Lemer, C., Izard, V., & Dehaene, S. (2004). Exact and approximate arithmetic in an
Amazonian indigene group. Science,306, 499503. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1102085
Plomin, R., & Deary, I. J. (2015). Genetics and intelligence differences: Five special findings.
Molecular Psychiatry,20,98108. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2014.105
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., Knopik, V. S., & Neiderheiser, J. (2013). Behavioral genetics (6th ed.).
New York, NY: Worth Publishers.
Price, T. S., Freeman, B., Craig, I., Petrill, S. A., Ebersole, L., & Plomin, R. (2000). Infant zygosity can
be assigned by parental report questionnaire data. Twin Research and Human Genetics,3, 129
133. https://doi.org/10.1375/twin.3.3.129
Number line and mathematics 15
Ramscar, M., Dye, M., Popick, H. M., & O’Donnell-McCarthy, F. (2011). The enigma of number: Why
children find the meanings of even small number words hard to learn and how we can help them
do better. PLoS ONE,6, e22501. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0022501
Reilly, D., Neumann, D. L., & Andrews, G. (2015). Sex differences in mathematics and science
achievement: A meta-analysis of National Assessment of Educational Progress assessments.
Journal of Educational Psychology,107(3), 645. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000012
Sasanguie, D., G
obel, S. M., Moll, K., Smets, K., & Reynvoet, B. (2013). Approximate number sense,
symbolic number processing, or numberspace mappings: What underlies mathematics
achievement? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,114, 418431. https://doi.org/10.
1016/j.jecp.2012.10.012
Shakeshaft, N. G., Trzaskowski, M., McMillan, A., Rimfeld, K., Krapohl, E., Haworth, C. M. A., ...
Plomin, R. (2013). Strong genetic influence on a UK nationwide test of educational achievement
at the end of compulsory education at age 16. PLoS ONE,8, e80341. https://doi.org/10.1371/
journal.pone.0080341
Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). Reading space into numbersa cross-linguistic comparison of the
SNARC effect. Cognition,108, 590599. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.04.001
Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. (1979). Intraclass correlations: Uses in assessing rater reliability.
Psychological Bulletin,86, 420428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420
Siegler, R. S., & Booth, J. L. (2004). Development of numerical estimation in young children. Child
Development,75, 428444. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00684.x
Siegler, R. S., & Mu, Y. (2008). Chinese children excel on novel mathematics problems even before
elementary school. Psychological Science,19, 759763. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.
2008.02153.x
Siegler, R. S., & Opfer, J. E. (2003). The development of numerical estimation evidence for multiple
representations of numerical quantity. Psychological Science,14, 237250. https://doi.org/10.
1111/1467-9280.02438
Slusser, E., & Barth, H. (2017). Intuitive proportion judgment in number-line estimation: Converging
evidence from multiple tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,162, 181198.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.04.010
Thompson, L. A., Detterman, D. K., & Plomin, R. (1991). Associations between cognitive abilities
and scholastic achievement: Genetic overlap but environmental differences. Psychological
Science,2, 158165. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1991.tb00124.x
Thompson, C. A., & Opfer, J. E. (2008). Costs and benefits of representational change: Effects of
context on age and sex differences in symbolic magnitude estimation. Journal of Experimental
Child Psychology,101,2051. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.02.003
Tikhomirova, T., Kuzmina, Y., & Malykh, S. (2018). Does symbolic and non-symbolic estimation
ability predict mathematical achievement across primary school years?. In ITM Web of
Conferences (Vol. 18, p. 04006). EDP Sciences.
Tosto, M. G., Hanscombe, K. B., Haworth, C., Davis, O. S., Petrill, S. A., Dale, P. S., ... Kovas, Y.
(2014). Why do spatial abilities predict mathematical performance? Developmental Science,17,
462470. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12138
Tosto, M. G., Petrill, S. A., Halberda, J., Trzaskowski, M., Tikhomirova, T. N., Bogdanova, O. Y., ...
Plomin, R. (2014). Why do we differ in number sense? Evidence from a genetically sensitive
investigation. Intelligence,43,3546. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.12.007
Tosto, M. G., Petrill, S. A., Malykh, S., Malki, K., Haworth, C. M. A., Mazzocco, M. M. M., ...Kovas, Y.
(2017). Number sense and mathematics: Which, when and how? Developmental Psychology,
53, 19241939. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000331
Tr
aff, U. (2013). The contribution of general cognitive abilities and number abilities to different
aspects of mathematics in children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,116, 139156.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.04.007
Trzaskowski, M., Davis, O. S., DeFries, J. C., Yang, J., Visscher, P. M., & Plomin, R. (2013). DNA
evidence for strong genome-wide pleiotropy of cognitive and learning abilities. Behavior
Genetics,43, 267273. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-013-9594-x
16 Maria Grazia Tosto et al.
Tucker-Drob, E. M., & Bates, T. C. (2015). Large cross-national differences in gene9socioeconomic
status interaction on intelligence. Psychological Science,27, 138149. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0956797615612727
Vukovic, R. K., Fuchs, L. S., Geary, D. C., Jordan, N. C., Gersten, R., & Siegler, R. S. (2014). Sources of
individual differences in children’s understanding of fractions. Child Development,85, 1461
1476. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12218
Wagner, K., Kimura, K., Cheung, P., & Barner, D. (2015). Why is number word learning hard?
Evidence from bilingual learners. Cognitive Psychology,83,121. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.c
ogpsych.2015.08.006
Wang, Z., Hart, S. A., Kovas, Y., Lukowski, S., Soden, B., Thompson, L. A., .. .Petrill, S. A. (2014). Who
is afraid of math? Two sources of genetic variance for mathematical anxiety. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry,55, 10561064. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12224
Wang, Z., Lukowski, S. L., Hart, S. A., Lyons, I. M., Thompson, L. A., Kovas, Y., ...Petrill, S. A. (2015).
Is math anxiety always bad for math learning: The role of math motivation. Psychological
Science,26, 18631876. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615602471
Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson III tests of achievement.
Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature,358, 749750. https://doi.
org/10.1038/358749a0
Received 2 April 2018; revised version received 29 October 2018
Supporting Information
The following supporting information may be found in the online edition of the article:
Table S1. Means and standard deviation on all measures for each sample.
Table S2. ANOVAs comparison of Number Line estimation and Mathematics means in
groups with restricted age range.
Table S3. Intraclass correlation (ICC). Twins similarity in Number Line estimation and
mathematics.
Table S4. Heritability estimates from twin model-fitting for Number Line estimation
and mathematics measures.
Table S5. Sex limitation model fitting and parameter estimates for males and females
separately (UK TEDS sample only).
Table S6. Cross-trait twin correlations between Number Line estimation and
mathematics measures.
Table S7. Genetic and environmental correlations; bivariate heritabilities; phenotypic
correlations.
Table S8. Bivariate model fitting between Number Line (NL) and mathematics
measures.
Figure S1. Phenotypic correlations between Lambda scores and the absolute mean
error scores of Number Line estimation task for the twin samples.
Figure S2. Phenotypic correlations between Lambda scores and the absolute mean
error scores of Number Line estimation task for the Russian singletons.
`
Number line and mathematics 17
... The present research evaluates the extent to which gender differences arise in confidence on number-line estimation, a task which taps the fundamental ability to estimate numerical magnitude (and is predictive of future math achievement; e.g., Bailey et al. 2014;Siegler 2006, 2008;Fazio et al. 2014;Fuchs et al. 2010;Geary 2011;Schneider et al. 2018;Siegler 2016;Siegler et al. 2011Siegler et al. , 2012Siegler and Thompson 2014;Tosto et al. 2018). In this task, participants are (1) asked to estimate where a provided number (e.g., 25) falls on a horizontal number line and (2) asked to judge their confidence in their estimatean example is presented in Fig. 1. ...
... As compared to girls/women, why did boys/men make more precise number-line estimates? Although considerable debate exists over the causes of such gender differences when they are observed (e.g., Hyde 2014), we imagine that psychological (e.g., differences in math attitudes; Sidney et al. 2019), social (e.g., differences in early spatial experiences, such as exposure to spatial language, media, and toys; Caldera et al. 1989;Doyle et al. 2012;Pruden and Levine 2017; or gendered stereotypes about math and spatial ability, McGlone and Aronson 2006; Moè and Pazzaglia 2006), and possibly even biological factors (e.g., sexual dimorphism in the parietal cortex; Goldstein et al. 2001) could contribute to the gender differences observed in the numberline estimation task (as is the case for performance; e.g., Tosto et al. 2018). We leave it to future research to evaluate the contribution of these factors to the present gender differences observed. ...
Article
Full-text available
Prior research has found gender differences in spatial tasks in which men perform better, and are more confident, than women. Do gender differences also occur in people’s confidence as they perform number-line estimation, a common spatial-numeric task predictive of math achievement? To investigate this question, we analyzed outcomes from six studies (N = 758 girls/women and boys/men with over 20,000 observations; grades 1–5 and adults) that involved a similar method: Participants estimated where a provided number (e.g., ¾, 37) was located on a bounded number line (e.g., 0–1; 0–100), then judged their confidence in that estimate. Boys/men were more precise (g = .52) and more confident (g = .30) in their estimates than were girls/women. Linear mixed model analyses of the trial-level data revealed that girls’/women’s estimates had about 31% more error than did boys’/men’s estimates, and even when controlling for precision, girls/women were about 7% less confident in their estimates than were boys/men. These outcomes should encourage researchers to consider gender differences for studies on math cognition and provide pathways for future research to address potential mechanisms underlying the present gender gaps.
... The present research evaluates the extent to which gender differences arise in confidence on number-line estimation, a task which taps the fundamental ability to estimate numerical magnitude (and is predictive of future math achievement; e.g., Bailey et al. 2014;Siegler 2006, 2008;Fazio et al. 2014;Fuchs et al. 2010;Geary 2011;Schneider et al. 2018;Siegler 2016;Siegler et al. 2011Siegler et al. , 2012Siegler and Thompson 2014;Tosto et al. 2018). In this task, participants are (1) asked to estimate where a provided number (e.g., 25) falls on a horizontal number line and (2) asked to judge their confidence in their estimatean example is presented in Fig. 1. ...
... As compared to girls/women, why did boys/men make more precise number-line estimates? Although considerable debate exists over the causes of such gender differences when they are observed (e.g., Hyde 2014), we imagine that psychological (e.g., differences in math attitudes; Sidney et al. 2019), social (e.g., differences in early spatial experiences, such as exposure to spatial language, media, and toys; Caldera et al. 1989;Doyle et al. 2012;Pruden and Levine 2017; or gendered stereotypes about math and spatial ability, McGlone and Aronson 2006; Moè and Pazzaglia 2006), and possibly even biological factors (e.g., sexual dimorphism in the parietal cortex; Goldstein et al. 2001) could contribute to the gender differences observed in the numberline estimation task (as is the case for performance; e.g., Tosto et al. 2018). We leave it to future research to evaluate the contribution of these factors to the present gender differences observed. ...
Article
Full-text available
Prior research has found gender differences in spatial tasks in which men perform better, and are more confident, than women. Do gender differences also occur in people’s confidence as they perform number-line estimation, a common spatial-numeric task predictive of math achievement? To investigate this question, we analyzed outcomes from six studies (N = 758 girls/women and boys/men with over 20,000 observations; grades 1-5 and adults) that involved a similar method: Participants estimated where a provided number (e.g., ¾, 37) was located on a bounded number line (e.g., 0-1; 0-100), then judged their confidence in that estimate. Boys/men were more precise (g = .52) and more confident (g = .30) in their estimates than were girls/women. Linear mixed model analyses of the trial-level data revealed that girls’/women’s estimates had about 31% more error than did boys’/men’s estimates, and even when controlling for precision, girls/women were about 7% less confident in their estimates than were boys/men. These outcomes should encourage researchers to consider gender differences for studies on math cognition and provide pathways for future research to address potential mechanisms underlying the present gender gaps.
... In contrast, in more homogeneous school settings, genetic effects drive the covariation between basic numerical cognition and mathematics. (Tosto et al., 2018). ...
... Cuando la investigación se realiza en contextos internacionales o zonas con contextos escolares heterogéneos, el ambiente parece ser más importante para generar variación en las habilidades numéricas y su asociación con las matemáticas. En contraste, en contextos escolares más homogéneos, los efectos genéticos generan la co-variación entre la cognición numérica básica y las matemáticas (Tosto et al., 2018). ...
... In a meta-analysis of research data from 45 countries, cross-cultural di erences were associated, rst, with the e ectiveness of the functioning of the national education system at the level of r = 0.25 at p < 0.001 and the socioeconomic status of states at the level of r = 0.16 at p < 0.001 (Brouwers, Van de Vijver, Van Hemert, 2009). It was shown that in a less heterogeneous more e ective educational environment the contribution of cognitive abilities to the formation of individual di erences in academic success increases (Tosto et al., 2019;Tucker-Drob, & Bates, 2016). Additionally, greater school subject orientation of the national education system, such as towards mathematics, can inuence the educational achievements of schoolchildren in this subject (Paik et al., 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: The cognitive predictors of academic achievement are associated both with basic cognitive abilities such as the information processing speed, number sense and visuospatial working memory, as well as with general ability including nonverbal intelligence. However, the ratio between cognitive development and school achievement can depend on sociocultural conditions. Objective: The results of a cross-cultural analysis of the relationship between cognitive development and academic achievement during primary education are presented. The analysis was conducted sampling schoolchildren from Russia and Kyrgyzstan, two countries that have a similar organization of the national education system but differ in the level of socioeconomic development. Design: The study involved 732 schoolchildren aged 7.7 to 11.8 years studying in Russia and Kyrgyzstan. Information processing speed, visuospatial working memory, and number sense were assessed using each of "Choice Reaction Time," "Corsi Block-Tapping Test," and "Number Sense" computerized tests. Results: According to the results, empirical data in both samples show that a model where in information processing speed signifies basic cognitive ability is a key predictor of nonverbal intelligence, working memory, and number sense, and each of these may contribute to individual differences in academic achievement. Notwithstanding the universality of this model, cross-cultural differences were seen to engender a reduction of schoolchildren's academic achievements, given possible impacts of less favorable educational conditions. Conclusion: In the relationship between cognitive abilities and academic success at the primary school education, there are both similarities and differences between schoolchildren studying in Russia and Kyrgyzstan.
Chapter
In this revised and updated edition of Hunt's classic textbook, Human Intelligence, two research experts explain how key scientific studies have revealed exciting information about what intelligence is, where it comes from, why there are individual differences, and what the prospects are for enhancing it. The topics are chosen based on the weight of evidence, allowing readers to evaluate what ideas and theories the data support. Topics include IQ testing, mental processes, brain imaging, genetics, population differences, sex, aging, and likely prospects for enhancing intelligence based on current scientific evidence. Readers will confront ethical issues raised by research data and learn how scientists pursue answers to basic and socially relevant questions about why intelligence is important in everyday life. Many of the answers will be surprising and stimulate readers to think constructively about their own views.
Article
Background: Number line accuracy (NL accuracy) shows improvement over the course of a school education. However, there are practically no cross-country longitudinal studies of NL accuracy over the whole course of elementary school. Aims: This study investigated the developmental trajectories of NL accuracy and its types across the elementary school years in two countries-Russia and Kyrgyzstan. Sample(s): The analyses were carried out on the data collected from the sample of 508 schoolchildren at Grades 1, 2, 3 and 4 (aged 6.4-11.9 years) from Russia and Kyrgyzstan, who were surveyed as part of the 'Cross-cultural Longitudinal Analysis of Student Success' project. Methods: The participants were administered the 'Number Line' computerized test task and a paper-and-pencil 'Standard Progressive Matrices' test at the end of each academic year. Results: During the course of the elementary school education, NL accuracy increases nonlinearly in both samples from Grade 1 to Grade 4, with a pronounced increase in the rate of improvement from the first to the second year. Cross-country differences in NL accuracy were observed during each year of schooling as well as in the growth of NL accuracy. The development of NL accuracy is described by a model with two developmental types: (1) 'high start and growth' (93% of the pooled sample) and (2) 'low start and no growth' (7%). Conclusions: Both NL accuracy and the rate of its growth during elementary school depend on educational conditions. Cross-country differences in the distribution of schoolchildren by these two developmental types were statistically insignificant.
Article
The number line estimation task is an often-used measure of numerical magnitude understanding. The task also correlates substantially with broader measures of mathematical achievement. This raises the question of whether the task would be a useful component of mathematical achievement tests and instruments to diagnose dyscalculia or mathematical giftedness and whether a stand-alone version of the task can serve as a short screener for mathematical achievement. Previous studies on the relation between number line estimation accuracy and broader mathematical achievement were limited in that they used relatively small nonrepresentative samples and usually did not account for potentially confounding variables. To close this research gap, we report findings from a population-level study with nearly all Luxembourgish ninth-graders (N = 6484). We used multilevel regressions to test how a standardized mathematical achievement test relates to the accuracy in number line estimation on bounded number lines with whole numbers and fractions. We also investigated how these relations were moderated by classroom characteristics, person characteristics, and trial characteristics. Mathematical achievement and number line estimation accuracy were associated even after controlling for potentially confounding variables. Subpopulations of students showed meaningful differences in estimation accuracy, which can serve as benchmarks in future studies. Compared with the number line estimation task with whole numbers, the number line estimation task with fractions was more strongly related to mathematical achievement in students across the entire mathematical achievement spectrum. These results show that the number line estimation task is a valid and useful tool for diagnosing and monitoring mathematical achievement.
Article
Full-text available
The article presents the results of a longitudinal study of the association between number sense and success in learning mathematics in primary school. We analysed the data of 133 schoolchildren on two aspects of number sense related to the symbolic and non-symbolic magnitude estimation abilities and academic success in mathematics in third and fourth grade. The average age of schoolchildren during the first assessment was 9.82 ± 0.30; during the second assessment – 10.82 ± 0.30. For the analysis of interrelations, the cross-lagged method was used. It was shown that the reciprocal model best describes the data suggesting cross-lagged associations between number sense and the success in learning mathematics at primary school age. The results of the longitudinal analysis revealed differences in the relationship between the success in learning mathematics with the two aspects of number sense: academic success in third grade only predicted the indicator of number sense associated with the symbolic magnitude estimation ability in fourth grade. The differences in the age dynamics of the two aspects of number sense in primary school are also revealed: the indicator of number sense associated with the non-symbolic magnitude estimation ability was the most stable over time.
Article
Full-text available
Individual differences in number sense correlate with mathematical ability and performance, although the presence and strength of this relationship differs across studies. Inconsistencies in the literature may stem from heterogeneity of number sense and mathematical ability constructs. Sample characteristics may also play a role as changes in the relationship between number sense and mathematics may differ across development and cultural contexts. In this study, 4,984 16-year-old students were assessed on estimation ability, one aspect of number sense. Estimation was measured using 2 different tasks: number line and dot-comparison. Using cognitive and achievement data previously collected from these students at ages 7, 9, 10, 12, and 14, the study explored for which of the measures and when in development these links are observed, and how strong these links are and how much these links are moderated by other cognitive abilities. The 2 number sense measures correlated modestly with each other (r = .22), but moderately with mathematics at age 16. Both measures were also associated with earlier mathematics; but this association was uneven across development and was moderated by other cognitive abilities. (PsycINFO Database Record
Article
Full-text available
A core hypothesis in developmental theory predicts that genetic influences on intelligence and academic achievement are suppressed under conditions of socioeconomic privation and more fully realized under conditions of socioeconomic advantage: a Gene × Childhood Socioeconomic Status (SES) interaction. Tests of this hypothesis have produced apparently inconsistent results. We performed a meta-analysis of tests of Gene × SES interaction on intelligence and academic-achievement test scores, allowing for stratification by nation (United States vs. non-United States), and we conducted rigorous tests for publication bias and between-studies heterogeneity. In U.S. studies, we found clear support for moderately sized Gene × SES effects. In studies from Western Europe and Australia, where social policies ensure more uniform access to high-quality education and health care, Gene × SES effects were zero or reversed.
Article
Full-text available
Underperformance in math is a problem with increasing prevalence, complex etiology, and severe repercussions. This study examined the etiological heterogeneity of math performance in a sample of 264 pairs of 12-year-old twins assessed on measures of math achievement, numerosity, and math anxiety. Latent profile analysis indicated 5 groupings of individuals representing different patterns of math achievement, numerosity, and math anxiety, coupled with differing degrees of familial transmission. These results suggest that there may be distinct profiles of math achievement, numerosity, and anxiety, particularly for students who struggle in math. (PsycINFO Database Record
Article
Full-text available
Gender gaps in the development of mathematical and scientific literacy have important implications for the general public's understanding of scientific issues and for the underrepresentation of women in science, technology, engineering, and math. We subjected data from the National Assessment of Educational Progress to a meta-analysis to examine whether there were sex differences in mathematics and science achievement for students in the United States across the period 1990-2011. Results show that there were small but stable mean sex differences favoring males in mathematics and science across the past 2 decades, with an effect size of d = .10 and .13, respectively, for students in 12th grade. Furthermore, there were large sex differences in high achievers, with males being overrepresented by a factor of over 2:1 at the upper right of the ability distribution for both mathematics and science. Further efforts are called for to reach equity in mathematics and science educational outcomes for all students.
Article
Full-text available
Little is known about why people differ in their levels of academic motivation. This study explored the etiology of individual differences in enjoyment and self-perceived ability for several school subjects in nearly 13,000 twins aged 9–16 from 6 countries. The results showed a striking consistency across ages, school subjects, and cultures. Contrary to common belief, enjoyment of learning and children’s perceptions of their competence were no less heritable than cognitive ability. Genetic factors explained approximately 40% of the variance and all of the observed twins’ similarity in academic motivation. Shared environmental factors, such as home or classroom, did not contribute to the twin’s similarity in academic motivation. Environmental influences stemmed entirely from individual specific experiences.
Article
Approximate number sense (ANS), the ability to rapidly and accurately compare quantities presented non-symbolically, has been proposed as a precursor to mathematics skills. Earlier work reported low heritability of approximate number sense, which was interpreted as evidence that approximate number sense acts as a fitness trait. However, viewing ANS as a fitness trait is discordant with findings suggesting that individual differences in approximate number sense acuity correlate with mathematical performance, a trait with moderate genetic effects. Importantly, the shared etiology of approximate number sense, mathematics, and general cognitive ability has remained unexamined. Thus, the etiology of approximate number sense and its overlap with math and general cognitive ability was assessed in the current study with two independent twin samples (N = 451 pairs). Results suggested that ANS acuity had moderate but significant additive genetic influences. ANS also had overlap with generalist genetic mechanisms accounting for variance and covariance in mathematics and general cognitive ability. Furthermore, ANS may have genetic factors unique to covariance with mathematics beyond overlap with general cognitive ability. Evidence across both samples was consistent with the proposal that the etiology of approximate number sense functions similar to that of mathematics and general cognitive skills.
Article
How children’s understanding of numerical magnitudes changes over the course of development remains a key question in the study of numerical cognition. In an ongoing debate about the source of developmental change, some argue that children maintain and access different mental representations of number, with evidence coming largely from common number-line estimation tasks. In contrast, others argue that a theoretical framework based on psychophysical models of proportion estimation accounts for typical performance on these tasks. The present study explores children’s (n=71) and adults’ (n=27) performance on two number-line tasks, both the “number to position” or NP task and the inverse “position to number” or PN task. Estimates on both tasks are consistent with the predictions of the proportion estimation account and do not support the hypothesis that a fundamental shift in mental representations underlies developmental change in numerical estimation and, in turn, mathematical ability. Converging evidence across tasks also calls into question the utility of bounded number-line tasks as an evaluation of mental representations of number.
Article
The linear relations between math anxiety and math cognition have been frequently studied. However, the relations between anxiety and performance on complex cognitive tasks have been repeatedly demonstrated to follow a curvilinear fashion. In the current studies, we aimed to address the lack of attention given to the possibility of such complex interplay between emotion and cognition in the math-learning literature by exploring the relations among math anxiety, math motivation, and math cognition. In two samples-young adolescent twins and adult college students-results showed inverted-U relations between math anxiety and math performance in participants with high intrinsic math motivation and modest negative associations between math anxiety and math performance in participants with low intrinsic math motivation. However, this pattern was not observed in tasks assessing participants' nonsymbolic and symbolic number-estimation ability. These findings may help advance the understanding of mathematics-learning processes and provide important insights for treatment programs that target improving mathematics-learning experiences and mathematical skills.