ArticlePDF Available

Gut Rhetorics: Toward Experiments in Living with Microbiota

Authors:

Figures

Content may be subject to copyright.
Rhetoric of Health & Medicine
Vol. 1, Nos. 3- 4, pp. 269–295
doi: 10.5744/rhm.2018.1014
© 2018 University of Florida Press
— -1
—0
— +1
Gut Rhetorics: Toward
Experiments in Living
with Microbiota
Jason Kalin and David R. Gruber
When considering the material ecologies of the human body, we must consider
the bodies within— at least ve hundred known species of microbes. We propose
the term gut rhetorics to highlight how our guts have become an environment to
which we are exposed: a biologically active actant contributing to the physiology
and psychology— the rhetorical capacitiesof the human body. Gut rhetorics
incorporatebring into the body and, importantly, into the body of rhetoric— the
hungry horde within human bodies. First, we trace one probiotic formulation across
three scientic studies to show how bodies, aects, and microbes are being “cali-
brated” at the level of experiment. Second, we stress skilled probiotic experimen-
tation and encourage scholars to play amid environments, give attention to
embodiment, and pursue phenomenological inquiry (Gruber, ; Melonçon,
). Gut rhetorics consider bodies, aects, and microbiota as entangled meta-
bolic intra- actions that aect how the world appears to the body and the body to
the world.
: attunement, body, microbes, new materialism, rhetorical ontology
A wave of scientic research and popular culture publicity— government
initiatives (for example, the Human Microbiome Project), TED talks, mag-
azine articles, best- selling books, and new diet trends— has given promi-
nence to the gut, the microbiota and the microbiome, situating them as
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 269 26/10/18 5:32 PM
270
-1—
0—
+1—
integral to the health of the human body. We are, as this research often
points out, only % human; the other % is the microbiota— the bacteria,
fungi, and yeasts that live on or in the body (Hird, ; Yong, ). e
microbiota has been linked to not only gastrointestinal problems, includ-
ing gastroenteritis, nervous stomach, and irritable bowel syndrome, but
also allergies, asthma, chronic inammation, depression, and weight gain
and loss (Yong, ). Justin and Erica Sonnenburg (), two leading
researchers of the microbiota, argue that the microbiota “is directly respon-
sive to diet” (p.); thus, we have “trillions of mouths to feed” (p.).
Homogeneous processed foods, according to Sonnenburg and Sonnenburg,
have been stripped of “microbiota accessible carbohydrates” (p.), leav-
ing the microbiota and our bodies starving, if not to death, then for a dif-
ferent way of life, one more congenial to bodily and microbial appetites.
In his article “An Appetite for Rhetoric,” Nathan Stormer () sug-
gests that rhetoric scholars have long taken the rhetor’s body for granted,
when they have not been ignoring it altogether. Most often, he adds, rhetoric
scholars have assumed the body “to be well- fed or at least not a starving one”
(p.), and in so doing, they have limited rhetoricity to a “stunted aective
potential and responsiveness to the world” (p.). Stormer thus argues that
because “rhetoric is enlivened by bodies that eat” (p.), rhetoric scholars
need to attend to the hungry body and hunger itself as a “material vulnerabil-
ity to the world” (p.). Who among us well- fed academics has not expe-
rienced a feeling of being hangry a portmanteau of hungry and angry— to
describe that irritability accompanying extended hunger that emerges when
a department meeting pushes against the lunch hour? Attending to bodies
that eat enables rhetoric scholars to imagine dierently the “material ecolo-
gies of rhetoric” and “the evolution of rhetorical capacities” (p.).
When considering the material ecologies of the human body, we must
also consider the bodies withinat least ve hundred known species of
microbes, numbering over a hundred trillion and weighing approximately
three to ve pounds (Hird, ; Yong, ). Body, microbiota, and their
appetites are entangled in intestinal folds so that to (mal)nourish the body
is to (mal)nourish microbiota, and vice versa. Attending to the body and its
material ecologies, specically the microbiota, suggests how “the evolution
of rhetorical capacities” may have much to do with the hunger of microbes
and corresponding bodily activities. us, we propose gut rhetorics to
acknowledge that being human is living with microbes and that rhetoric is
enlivened by both bodies that eat and microbes that eat bodies.
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 270 26/10/18 5:32 PM
Kalin and Gruber
271
— -1
—0
— +1
Moreover, we are not simply living with microbes; we are reinventing
what it means to be human by reintroducing microbes— through the inges-
tion of fermented foods, fecal transplants, parasites, and pre- and probiotic
diets— into and as part of our bodies, cultivating some as “gut buddies”
(Lorimer, ) and exterminating others with antibiotics. In short, gut
rhetorics reveal the inseparability of epistemological, ontological, and phe-
nomenological realities. e gut has become a both metaphoric and meta-
bolic environment to which we are exposed: a scientically calibrated
representation, a biologically active actant, and a bodily eshy feeling. e
metaphoric and the metabolic contribute to the physiology and psychology—
the rhetorical capacities— of the human body. Gut rhetorics (re)compose
bodies as metabolizing forces— ingestion, digestion, inammation, fermen-
tation, and excretion— in which bodies are always on the way to becoming
something else yet feeding to maintain a stasis that remains elusive. Gut
rhetorics incorporate— bring into the body and, importantly, into the body
of rhetoric— the hungry horde within human bodies. If we are to under-
stand rhetorical capacities appropriate to the “material ecologies of rheto-
ric” (Stormer, ) and to “ecological thought,” such as vulnerability and
resilience (Stormer & McGreavy, ), we need our guts— literally and
guratively— not only our intestinal folds and their microbial inhabitants
but also our stomach, our appetite for creative experimentation.
We examine the “deliberate reentanglement” (Lorimer, , p.)
of bodies and the microbiota within two epistemological- ontological-
phenomenological formations giving rise to gut rhetorics. We begin by
briey situating our material and methodological commitments within the
ontological turn in rhetoric (Graham, ; Stormer, , Stormer &
McGreavy, ) and S. Scott Graham’s rhetorical- ontological inquiry. We
then trace one probiotic formulation across three scientic studies to show
how bodies, aects, and microbes are being calibrated, that is, sorted, clas-
sied, and justied (Graham, ), at the level of experiment, an “experi-
mental formalization” of the microbiota (Landecker, ). Here, we detail
how gastroenterologists enact microbes ontologically as a probiotic formu-
lation, epistemologically through diagnostic questionnaires, and phenom-
enologically by relying on patients’ self- reported aects and feelings. In a
second formation of gut rhetorics, we stress how similar ontological,
epistemological, and phenomenological entanglements are being enacted at
the level of everyday practices, noting how probiotic foods and consumer-
driven diets advocate other means of living with the microbiota. ese
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 271 26/10/18 5:32 PM
-1—
0—
+1—
Gut Rhetorics
everyday gut rhetorics not only focus on discursive appeals to the gut but
also orient consumers towards the development of skillful living with the
microbiota— a shift from knowing that to knowing how (Ingold, ; Coeck-
elbergh, ), a kind of learning through sensual inquiry in an age when
knowing is uneasy, not at all comprehensive, complex, and ecological (Mel-
onçon, ). In this way, everyday gut rhetorics encourage probiotic
experimentation in ways that resonate with recent methodological consid-
erations in the rhetoric of science and medicine, which encourage scholars
to play amid environments, give attention to embodiment, and pursue phe-
nomenological inquiry (Gruber, ; Melonçon, ). Consequently, we
outline how gut rhetorics can nd room not only for “experimental entan-
glements” (Fitzgerald & Callard, ) but also for “experiments in rheto-
ric” (Gruber, ) to understand how bodies, microbes, and environments
metabolize one another.
Rhetorical Ontology: Entanglement and Inquiry
Gut rhetorics begin with the premise that although the living ontology of
the body is dierent than not only our phenomenological experience as a
body but also our epistemological concepts used to textually inscribe,
describe, prescribe, and proscribe that experience, we cannot simply disre-
gard either bodily experience or metaphoric representation. Put simply, rhet-
oric scholars should be careful not to abandon language, discourse, and
representation in their turn to rhetoric’s ontology (Barnett & Boyle, ;
Stormer, ; Stormer & McGreavy, ). In this way, we follow Gra-
ham () who argues that rhetoric and ontology need each other to think
dierently, if not move beyond, the “two world problem” of “bifurcations
that include the subject/object, culture/nature, and mind/body dichotomies”
(p.). To counter the “two world problem,” Graham develops “rhetorical-
ontological inquiry” as a method/ology that accepts ontological multiplic-
ity and ecological entanglement—i nformed by science and technology
studies and new materialisms, primarily drawing upon Bruno Latour and
Annemarie Mol— while also studying how pain, in his case, is talked about
among diverse discourse communities (for example, medical, psychiatric,
pharmaceutical professionals) and experienced by patients who report
various pain symptoms. Rhetorical- ontological inquiry considers how epis-
temology, ontology, and phenomenology are inseparable from, yet irreduc-
ible to, one another in enacting multiple realities of pain. As such, rhetoric
272
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 272 26/10/18 5:32 PM
Kalin and Gruber
273
— -1
—0
— +1
scholars can no longer be content with constructing text- bodies, or bodies-
as- texts, and must attend to lived, living bodies. We do so here by studying
what Graham calls two “practical regimes of engagement” (p.): rst,
gastroenterological research focusing on probiotic formulations, and second,
everyday embodied encounters with probiotics.
In examining these practical regimes of engagement, what becomes
apparent is the allusiveness of epistemological and phenomenological
accounts alongside the elusiveness of ontological calibration of the micro-
biota. e absence of certain microbes and their ecologies may be just as
consequential as the presence of other microbes. Likewise, the absence of
certain gut feelings, or aects, may be as consequential to the inscription
of a gut bacteria’s agency as the presence of other gut feelings. And the use
of specic psychometric questionnaires in gastroenterological studies inu-
ences how the participants’ feelings are understood and then how gut bac-
teria are ontologically empowered or delimited. us, the following inquiry
illuminates the ways we come to know gut bacteria, which are acting in the
world and have a material say in how “we see ourselves going forward in that
world” (Rickert, , p. xiii) even as they obscure understanding. In short,
microbes and bodies are metaphorically and materially shaped even while
they shape “our thoughts, feelings, and sociabilities” (Lorimer, , p.).
Gut rhetorics, accordingly, compose bodies and microbes as ruminations,
coughing up the capacitation of rhetoric, a kind of chewing, as well as chew-
ing the cud, that is, thinking with/in the gut.
In her study of the gut and depression, Elizabeth Wilson () writes,
“My argument is not that the gut contributes to minded states, but that the
gut is an organ of mind: it ruminates, deliberates, comprehends” (p.). In
like manner, gut rhetorics suggest that rhetoric’s ontology arises with the
millions of missed, missing, and mist microbes dwelling with/in a gut that
is both environmental and irreducible to representation. In other words, the
very capacitation of a rhetoric, such as aective response and conscious feel-
ing, may begin with microbes. As the rhetorical- ontological inquiry in the
next section shows, we have much to learn about how bodies and gut bac-
teria are becoming environmental and to what extent gut bacteria and aect
states, such as stress, anxiety, and depression, may be calibrated through
gastroenterological research and everyday embodied experimentation.
Understanding microbes, bodies, and bodies of rhetoric as “multispe-
cies achievements” (Lorimer, , p.) in living well together— or not
so well (see Haraway, ; Hird, )— oers an opportunity to advance
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 273 26/10/18 5:32 PM
274
-1—
0—
+1—
rhetorical- ontological inquiry. In reecting upon his study of pain medi-
cine, Graham () notes that he enacts an “empirical- discursive” ontol-
ogy, “an iterative series of ocular and inscriptive practices so as to create an
account of doing” (p.). He is an ethnographer, looking at words and
observing practices in situ. Alongside this empirical- discursive praxiogra-
phy, Graham calls for the development of “a more fully interventional form
of inquiry” (p.). We suggest that gut rhetorics may do so by encourag-
ing rhetoric scholars to enact empirical- phenomenological experience, such
an “experiment in rhetoric” (Gruber, ) that relies on (microbial) emo-
tions, in tandem with contextual discourses, to augment critique, to explore
bodily relations, to feel through rhetorics, to sense the persuasive power of
bacterial life, and to invent new possibilities for living with gut bacteria.
Part 1— Rhetorical- Ontological Inquiry:
Experiments in Probiotics and Affects
Among the many competing explanations for depression is the presence and
inuence of microbiota in the gut. e extent to which gut bacteria may
make a dierence in a given situation remains largely unknown. However,
Ali Naseribafrouei and colleagues () situate depression as a “chronic
syndrome with a pathogenesis linked to various genetic, biological, and
environmental factors,” adopting the appropriate hedging as they seek to
pinpoint specic gut bacteria associated with patients diagnosed with major
depression disorder (p.). Although Naseribafrouei etal. focus on the
Oscillibacter type strain of gut bacteria and the negative eects of its valeric
acid output, other studies indicate that a Lactobacillus strain along with other
lactic acid bacteria can regulate neurotransmitters “implicated in the patho-
genesis of anxiety and depression” (Bravo etal., , p.), concluding
that individual strains of bacteria “may have benecial eects in the treat-
ment of depression and anxiety” (p.).
Although it is tempting to suggest that gastrointestinal research is
merely in a nascent phase and that these researchers ultimately will delin-
eate which bacteria inuence which bodily system to explain how humans
might be prone to mood swings or the loss of self- worth associated with
depression, the claim proves tentative when the scientic processes of tex-
tualizing and calibrating the body and microbiota are brought to the surface.
Gut bacteria and symptoms of depression do appear, in some cases, to shift
together (Dinan etal., ; Petra etal., ). In other cases, correlating
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 274 26/10/18 5:32 PM
Kalin and Gruber
275
— -1
—0
— +1
specic types of gut bacteria with patients diagnosed with depression
proves uneven, not statistically signicant, or, put simply, a bit tricky (Diop,
Guillou, & Durand, ). Because depression is diagnosed through patient
evaluations involving patient self- narrative and psychometric questionnaires
(Matza etal., ), the microbiota is composed by a process of textualiza-
tion. Patients must be sorted into dened categories associated with diag-
nostic tools such as the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (Chakraborti
etal., ), the Spielberger State- Trait Anxiety Inventory (Schmidt etal.,
), the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (Gwee etal., ), the Hop-
kins Symptom Checklist, and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale
(Messaoudi etal., ). Patients are grouped in terms of mood disorders,
anxiety disorders, and clinical depression. Sometimes doctors have a say in
the process; other times, clinical researchers grade patients based solely on
demographics and computational factors (Chakraborti etal., ; Schmidt
etal., ). Whatever the case, patients must be institutionally labeled
and correlated with stool or urine samples and examinations of the micro-
biota over time (Dinan etal., ).
is diagnostic process, which primarily constructs bodies- as- texts,
allows for claim- making and legitimizes comparisons to animal studies,
despite obvious dierences in clinical evaluations that determine levels of
anxiety in animals versus humans and despite the diculty of determining
whether or not the feelings of rats are at all the same as those of humans.
We analyze this complicated scientic process across three studies looking
at the eects of one probiotic formulation. Doing so shows how textualiza-
tion, as an epistemic process inscribing ontological multiplicity, leaves gas-
trointestinal scholars and scholars of rhetoric with a tangible dilemma— not
only how to know a body, but also how to live with bodies that eat. Exam-
ining these experimental processes, and how each subsequent study seeks
to build upon the last yet confronts divergence and instability, shows how
lived bodies and microbes continually eace texts, eluding the processes of
textualization brought to bear on them.
Ingesting probiotics, which are complexes of microbes that may have
benecial eects in the human body, has been proposed as one possible
response to bring together the known body and the lived body. For example,
Michaël Messaoudi and colleagues () have attempted to better under-
stand how, when, and why a probiotic formulation (PF)— comprising Lacto-
bacillus helveticus R0052 and Bidobacterium longum R0175— might mitigate
the severity of stress, anxiety, and depression in humans. Messaoudi etal.
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 275 26/10/18 5:32 PM
276
-1—
0—
+1—
turn to several other gastroenterological studies examining the same or
similar Lactobacillus- based PFs to contextualize their claims and compare
outcomes. Exposing disjunctions across the various epistemic resources
brought to bear on the microbiota from these studies presents a “praxiog-
raphy of representation,” which is a way of understanding “not so much
what people say or what texts mean but rather how representational activity
circulates within and contributes to a deeper ecology of practices in which
those acts of representation are embedded” (Graham, , p.). Speci-
cally, examining the psychometric questionnaires used in each study, how
those questionnaires were facilitated, and how they rhetorically and mate-
rially established dierent aect states— namely, “stress,” “anxiety” and/or
“depression”— and how they were compared to stool or urine samples
todraw conclusions about the healthfulness of the PF elucidates what Mol
() calls the prescriptive “epistemic normativity” of the eld (p. ).
However, attending to the gaps and conicts that appear across three inter-
related probiotic studies, starting with Messaoudi etal. (), displays how
dierent psychometric questionnaires compel transformations in key terms,
such as “anxiety,” which help to determine the results. Gastroenterologists
need to contextualize the PF through their colleagues’ studies despite
obvious and important material dierences to secure a space for their work
and to draw conclusions that might push forward the science of the micro-
biota despite diering patient reports. For the rhetoric of health and medi-
cine (RHM), understanding how aect states are forged amid this process
brings insight into how, in gastroenterology, scientic calibration— the
process through which patients, the microbiota, and probiotics are catego-
rized and legitimized (Graham, )— makes bodies legible and remakes
bodies (see Jack, ; Wells, ).
Because questionnaires inform when and which gut bacteria can be des-
ignated helpful or healthy, a rhetorical- ontological inquiry draws out how
the microbiota is presumed to alter, to some measurable degree, feelings of
self- worth, personal isolation, or in/ability to connect with others as reported
by study participants. Finding ways to measure the inuence of food choices
and one’s gut bacteria on sociality and/or self- feeling is essentially the focus
of such research. e work re- materializes and medicalizes dinner plates,
indicating a rhetorical ontology that is material, phenomenological, and dis-
cursive in entwining human and bacterial bodies. Accordingly, we understand
PF studies as premised upon a broader cultural insight: “Food communi-
cates values, history, politics, demography, regionality, and culture” such
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 276 26/10/18 5:32 PM
Kalin and Gruber
277
— -1
—0
— +1
that “what and how much we eat . . . comes with the promise of ever greater
levels of agency” even as food can also “generate identities,” contribute to
exploitation, and physically change our bodies (Young, Eckstein, & Conley,
, p.). In like manner, we propose that the various interrelated pro-
biotic studies attempt dierent ways of “touching” and then securing, if
not universalizing, a link between food and human feeling in the mission
to increase human agency. ey literally forge ontologies in building up
knowledge- making relations between the microbiota and personality,
appropriate sociality, and appropriate diet.
e praxiography of representation we identify proceeds as follows:
. Food choices or habits of eating are conceptualized as socially and
materially entwined with psychological conditions.
. Consequently, psychometric questionnaires are employed by
scientic/ medical communities to determine the kinds of conclusions
that might be made by gastroenterological experimentation examin-
ing dierent strains of gut bacteria.
. Claims about unhealthy and healthy strains are dependent on
thechanges in follow- up responses to participants’ psychometric
questionnaires.
. Consequently, gut bacteria become agentive and salient by being cal-
ibrated to patients’ reported feelings amid dierent habits of eating,
as well as other habits of sleep, habits of work, etc.
. e gut bacteria, then, in the form of PFs can operate as “punctal-
ized actors,” or objects endowed with a particular role for network
building (Law, ). PFs become a “material delegate” conveying a
narrative of healthfulness/unhealthfulness from past research, which
is then purported to do something, such as expand agencies and build
up networks of PFs to sooth gut remedies (Akrich and Latour, ).
e problem is that probiotic formulations hold the promise of “ever
greater levels of agency” and personal satisfaction even while psychometric
questionnaires across the studies conict or talk past each other in seeking
to measure and categorize dierent aect states, and often do so dierently.
Although the psychometric questionnaires are a “materializing force” (Hill,
) in gastroenterological research, generating new scientic realities and
categories for negotiating the body, they are unable to provide stable accounts
of a living body and its hungers. Examining how one study builds claims
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 277 26/10/18 5:32 PM
278
-1—
0—
+1—
by including and occluding various forms of psychometric questionnaires
helps to illuminate just how an epistemological apparatus grapples with phe-
nomenology to inscribe an ontology of the body.
Study #1: MeSSaoudi et al. (2011)
Messaoudi and colleagues () hope to mitigate the severity of the stress,
anxiety, and depression in humans through discovering viable PF treatments.
To measure specic moods that they term “stress, anxiety, and depression,
they construct a study that employs four dierent psychometric measures.
ose include:
Hopkins Symptom Checklist (HSCL- ),
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS),
Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), and
Coping Checklist (CCL).
e rst (HSCL- ) is a - item questionnaire with a ve- point rating
scale evaluating “somatization, obsessive- compulsive, interpersonal sensi-
tivity, anxiety, and depression” with individual questions strictly correlated
to specic aect states (Derogatis etal., ). e second (HADS) uses a
- item questionnaire on a four- point scale with various answers at either
end of that scale (“yes” or “often” or “denitely,” etc. as examples of the posi-
tive end), evaluating anxiety and depression with seven questions each (see
“SVRI”). e third (PSS) uses a ten- item questionnaire with a ve- point
scale ranging from “never” to “very often” and intends to determine stress
levels (Cohen, ). e fourth, and nal, (CCL) is a - item questionnaire
on a “yes/no” scale used to determine “a broad range of behavioural and cog-
nitive responses that may be used to handle stress” and can be understood
as drawing a distinction in degrees between “normals” and “neurotics”
(Rao, Subbakrishna, & Prabhu, ). Messaoudi etal. () administer
each questionnaire following its own diagnosis protocols— some requiring
delivery and interpretation by a trained psychologist or professional and
others self- given and then apply them across two groups of human test
participants, a placebo group and a probiotic group. After four weeks of
Messaoudi etal. () do not entirely clarify whether HSCL and H ADS are administered by
professiona ls, despite recommendations to do so.
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 278 26/10/18 5:32 PM
Kalin and Gruber
279
— -1
—0
— +1
collecting urine samples and giving PF treatment to the test group, they ask
the participants to again complete the four psychometric questionnaires.
In this case, the researchers argue that the tested PF treatment miti-
gates psychological symptoms associated with anxiety and depression:
“e benecial eects of probiotics on anxiety and depression may be
explained by competitive exclusion of deleterious gut pathogens, decreases
in pro- inammatory cytokines and communication with the central ner-
vous system via vagal sensory bres, leading to changes in neurotransmit-
ter levels or function” (Messaoudi etal., , p.). In layman’s terms,
specic strains of microbes ingested seem to force out other bacteria or
overwhelm their negative eects by sending new benecial signals to the
brain.
Messaoudi etal. () are unable to pinpoint how or why microbiota
seem eective in alleviating symptoms of anxiety and depression, compar-
ing the eects measured in their study against other studies, including ones
on aggression in rats (Hanstock, Drew, & Van Kessel, ), the gastroin-
testinal tract of pigs (Estrada etal., ), and the sleep rates of elderly peo-
ple (Yamaura et al., ). In each case, similar bacterial formulations
were used, but the results varied and depended upon what could be known
from the human or nonhuman participants and what kind of epistemic
apparatus enabled the practice. Indeed, in reviewing related studies, they
spend signicant time confronting confounding disjunctions with Diop
etal. (), which reports no statistically signicant eects on anxiety or
psychological symptoms, even though the PF did show clear benets on GI
problems. In contrast, Messaoudi etal. argue for their own positive results
with the necessary hedging: “ese data are preliminary,” and “these results
provide further evidence that gut microora play a role [emphasis added] in
stress, anxiety and depression” (p.); the “role” remains unclear, and the
researchers suggest more research is needed (p.). eir judicious state-
ment of results is proper to scientic inquiry and ethically composed. Dis-
covering conicting results is likely always confounding, but reviewing the
other studies exposes how, as Mol () says, “ontology is not in the given
order of things . . . ontologies are brought into being, sustained, or allowed
to wither away” (p.). e ontology of microbes are made and questioned.
As the following section shows, knowing the microbiota becomes a com-
plex process of integrating prior studies as well as feeling a way through the
practices of other researchers and the reported phenomenological realities
of other participants.
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 279 26/10/18 5:32 PM
280
-1—
0—
+1—
Study #2: diop et al. (2008) & Study #3:
ait- Belgnaoui (2006)
For Messaoudi and colleagues (), the diculty in dealing with Diop
etal. () is not only that those researchers discovered no signicant
eects related to what they termed “psychosocial stress” but that the GI
problems noted in Diop’s participants were strongly correlated to “stress
induced symptoms of colorectal dysfunction” by a previous study conducted
by Afa Ait- Belgnaoui and colleagues (). In that  study, a sepa-
rate, but related, Lactobacillus strain showed the ability to prevent colorec-
tal hypersensitivity (p.). Chronologically, the studies proceed as follows:
Ait- Belgnaoui etal. correlate GI problems to heightened “stress” in rats and
show that a Lactobacillus PF alleviates those GI symptoms for rats, provid-
ing no comment on the psychosocial responses. Subsequently, Diop etal.
argue that those same microbiota strains do help with abdominal pain and
vomiting in humans but show no eect on any adverse psychological symp-
toms as measured by certain psychometric questionnaires administered
before and after PF treatment. Messaoudi etal. then argue for meaningful
correlations between their similar Lactobacillus- based PF and the psycho-
logical symptoms involved with “anxiety” and “depression” but nd noth-
ing in relation to “stress.” us, we are left with a dilemma about how “stress”
could be unaected while “anxiety” shows improvement, and how GI prob-
lems could disappear while the “stress” that purportedly inamed those
problems does not. e inquiry becomes simultaneously semiotic and mate-
rial, about questions and questionnaires working in tandem with bodies
and aects.
Interestingly, Diop etal. () cite the Ait- Belganoui etal. ()
study as partially motivating their work. ey engage in what Charles
Bazerman () calls the construction of “the accomplishment,” encour-
aging “some lines of development” through building on what has been done
while asserting what has not (p.)— in this case, transitioning this PF from
rats to humans. In staging the research gap in their introduction, Diop and
colleagues state, “Recent studies have shown that lactobacilli prevent vis-
ceral pain and stress- induced visceral hypersensitivity in rats [here citing
Ait- Belgnaoui etal. and Kamiya etal., ]. However, there has been, as
of yet, few data available on the eects of probiotics on stress- induced gas-
trointestinal symptoms in humans” (p. ) e nal line indicates why
Messaoudi etal. () would later cite Diop etal.; the eort to correlate
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 280 26/10/18 5:32 PM
Kalin and Gruber
281
— -1
—0
— +1
psychological symptoms to specic Lactobacillus strains could bolster the
power of the PF, thus moving the claim from rat gastroenterology to human
gastroenterology to human psychology. e move makes sense: If stress can
induce GI problems that are alleviated with the PF, then that same PF might
alleviate the related feelings of stress, including anxiety. However, show-
ing aect states changing confronts patients, not to mention a reasonably
large epistemic toolkit, rife with choices.
We call attention to not only the shift from animals to humans and
the shift in the exact Lactobacillus strains being used— despite their genome
similarities and abilities to produce lactic acid (see Salvetti, Torriani, &
Felis, )— but also the structure of the study designs. Diop and col-
leagues (), for instance, deviate signicantly from Messaoudi et al.’s
() study, choosing to construct a specialist questionnaire to test for
“stress induced symptoms.” Indeed, Diop and colleagues build their own
- item questionnaire with the aim to “encompass the following areas: gas-
trointestinal, cardiovascular, sleep disorders, locomotor systems, physical,
emotional and psychological symptoms, intellectual problems, spiritual
symptoms, and social aspects” (p.). Consequently, any direct comparisons
between Diop etal. and Messaoudi etal. confront a dierence in measure-
ment tools, what questions constitute certain aect states, and how those
assessments are administered. Quickly reviewing the questionnaires shows
that “anxiety” in one includes questions about “feeling ‘wound up’” or hav-
ing “‘butteries’ in the stomach” (“SVRI”) while the other asks about the
“feeling of being trapped or caught” and having “shakiness inside” (Access
Counseling Group). Likewise, one questionnaire determining “stress” asks
about “condence handling personal problems” (Cohen, ) while the
others do not (Diop etal., ).
Fast- forwarding to the Messaoudi etal. () study, those researchers
rightly assert, “is discrepancy [in our ndings about stress versus the Diop
etal.  study] may be due to the duration of the period of administra-
tion of the preparation and/or to the use of a dierent questionnaire” (p.).
With this explanation, Messaoudi etal. acknowledge that dierent dura-
tions of ingestion may result in dierent outcomes as the body incorporates
new bacterial formulations with old ones, engaging new environments along
the way. However, the dierence in duration between the two studies is
only eight days (Diop etal., ). If deemed signicant, researchers might
argue for the time it takes for a body to “exclude” and “decrease” pro-
inammatory agents, and if not, then researchers would be left once again
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 281 26/10/18 5:32 PM
282
-1—
0—
+1—
with an unknown. What remains, then, are the dierences in the ques-
tionnaires, the kinds of questions asked, how aect states are conceptual-
ized, and how the calibration process was facilitated.
As with the relations among body, the microbiota and aects like
depression, the centrality of textual diagnostic tools can be hard to detect
unless they stop working and the results are negative. NathanR. Johnson
() makes this clear in his discussion of medical infrastructures: “Work-
ing infrastructure often stays invisible” because when important classica-
tions “function” and “serve audience needs,” then the standards and
algorithms tend to disappear into the background (p.). Precisely
because one of the four psychometric tests used in the study— the PSS
questionnaire— did not show any signicant results in response to the PF,
Messaoudi and colleagues () are compelled to suggest that patient self-
reporting might have been a salient factor. ey state, “Taken together, PF
appears to show a benecial eect on general signs of anxiety and depres-
sion, which did not generalise to the PSS, although all three tests comprise
self- reported measures” (p.). One way to read the statement is to see a
aw in the PSS’s ability to obtain good patient self- reporting; another way
is to see aws in relying on patient self- reporting, thus taking all the results
with a proverbial grain of salt; yet another way to understand the statement
is to see patients as more likely to report shifts in anxiety or depression,
given the questions on those questionnaires or the nature of the diagnosis.
What Messaoudi and colleagues intend remains unclear.
Many scales used for evaluating anxiety and depression do rely upon
self- reported measures, but it is important to note that some should be con-
ducted in formal hospital settings for validity or administered by a trained
professional (see “SVRI”; Snaith, ), while others, such as the PSS, can
be much more widely used and lled out privately. Given that the PSS is
tailored to identify symptoms of stress and intended for general audiences,
Messaoudi etal. () could remark that the PSS is the one scale— of the
four used— presumably most likely to be comparable to the - question sur-
vey created by Diop and colleagues () who specically make claims
about the lack of results tying the probiotic formulation to stress. Perhaps
rightly, Messaoudi etal. focus mostly on legitimizing their positive results
and the eectiveness of the probiotic for alleviating “anxiety” and “depres-
sion” symptoms. Yet, they also, at least for a rhetoric scholar, unintention-
ally highlight the complexity of deriving any results at all: e variable
means of self- reporting, the separate legitimacies of each questionnaire
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 282 26/10/18 5:32 PM
Kalin and Gruber
283
— -1
—0
— +1
across studies, and the dierent test periods for similar but not exactly the
same PFs, all combine to produce diculty and suggest future adjustments
across variables. Figure outlines the divergences.
If microbiota studies are going to solidify claims about the positive
eect of probiotics, they must necessarily rely upon patient accounts because
the medical diagnosis of psychological conditions are largely dependent
upon feelings of guilt, shame, and self- worth as well as the corresponding
narrations about one’s own body as documented across various forms of
interviews and questionnaires (Evrensel & Ceylan, ; Naseribafrouei
etal., ). If microbiota are “mind- altering micro- organisms,” as Cryan
and Dinan () declare, they seem to alter the mind because patients say
so, reporting life changes, while researchers collect the corresponding sam-
ples of gut bacteria. Understanding this complex process illuminates the
value of gut rhetorics to learn how to consider material- discursive infrastruc-
tures composing the gut and to develop a response to the popularization of
microbiota research. Understanding how individual questionnaires ask par-
ticipants about food habits, sleep, paranoia symptoms, work stress, and
lived experience— and ask dierentlyas well as how probiotics studies
forge exigencies by relating their work to previous studies positions gut rhet-
orics as a vital site of rhetorical- ontological inquiry in RHM.
Figure. Study Comparisons Tracing Back Citations from Messaoudi
etal. (). (Created by authors)
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 283 26/10/18 5:32 PM
284
-1—
0—
+1—
e three studies also highlight some confusion in gastroenterology
about best means to delineate, capture, and measure aect states to relate
them to gut bacteria. en again, rhetoric scholars have only started to learn
how to talk about aect, how to see variable aects, how to relate them to
situated encounters, how and when to stage them as universal, and how much
to see them as intimately responsive to interventions. In looking outward to
other elds, we can also look inward to discover a gut rhetoric right inside
ourselves, one that groans for interdisciplinary engagement and epistemo-
logical entanglement (Fitzgerald and Callard, ) to digest what rheto-
ric’s ontologies demand. And we may begin by considering how living with
depression, for example, may call for developing the environmental skill “to
meet with” (see Haraway, ; Hird, ) gut bacteria and their meta-
bolic hungers.
Part 2— Environmental Skill: Experiments in
Living with Probiotics
e three probiotic formulation studies reveal how scientists struggle to
comprehend the body, or enough of a body to make claims about variables
that move in/dependently of the epistemic apparatus and the test partici-
pant’s experience of those variables. is observation is not made to simply
reinforce the need to move beyond representation (Lynch, ). Indeed,
the problem that gut rhetorics confronts is not simply the need to devise
better epistemic modes of data collection, such as textual categorization
and patient self- reports. e problem may not be empirical- discursive, but
empirical- phenomenological. e problem is coming to grips with unfold-
ing, messy, often subtle and sudden entanglements among bodies and envi-
ronments. Gut rhetorics within a rhetorical- ontological inquiry foreground
microbial life— millions of nonhuman bodies, the new discursive construc-
tions of what it means to be healthy with a microbiota, and the inadequacy of
any scientic or epistemic account of how to live with/in this environment.
In pragmatic laboratory terms, Messaoudi and colleagues () dem-
onstrate the lack of conceptual resources for analyzing such complex intra-
actions. ey administer treatments to a placebo group and a probiotic
group, yet they conclude that the reported probiotic eects could result from
the presence of benecial microbiota or the absence of harmful ones, or some
other unknown and strange combination. Similarly, in her analysis of anti-
depression medication and placebos, ElizabethA. Wilson () argues that
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 284 26/10/18 5:32 PM
Kalin and Gruber
285
— -1
—0
— +1
“placebos seem to treat and not treat at the same time” (p.). She even-
tually concludes that the “inert placebos also seem to be able to be activated
by the forces of persuasion” (p.): “If an inert substance can be brought
to action by suggestion, if a nondrug can become a drug under the sway of
a clinician’s care, authority, paternalism, or attention, is there not a muddle
(for my purposes, an appealing muddle) between the actions of body and
mind?” (p.). In brief, body and microbiota are dispersed across “a net-
work of psycho- genetic- institutional- pharmacological action” (p. ).
Accordingly, gut rhetorics must do more than reiterate this observation of
ecological entanglement and co- being; it must intensify relationships and
discover ways to move with the many realities, agencies, and vitalities—
human and nonhumanthat compose a body continuously producing and
enacting its relations. Gut rhetorics must have an appetite for metabolic
intra- actions as integral to explorations not only of bodies and aect but
also of rhetoric’s ontologies.
Although we agree with Wilson () by suggesting that rhetoric
scholars need to establish more conceptual frameworks to understand
material- symbolic intra- actions and to compose new analyses of rhetorical
ontology— thus, our call for gut rhetorics— we also extend beyond the
conceptual to posit another, active dimension to gut rhetorics: experiments
in living with probiotics. Gut rhetorics cannot be limited to showing how
objects and things are “‘always already’ inextricably and irrevocably impli-
cated, intertwined or even imbricated with everything else” (Ingold, ,
p.). Ontological entanglement is an initializing premise, whereas the
practical and practicable challenge becomes how to “ join with the texture
of the world, with its materials and processes. . . . e practice of theory, in
short, must be a modality of habitation— a way of thinking and working
with stu” (Ingold, , p.). If omas Rickert () encourages
rhetoric scholars to adopt “the more holistic terroir . . . earth, sun, vine,
and weather” in thinking ambiently (p. x)— that is, when the entire world
becomes environmental— we must become more capacious still— to include,
in our thinking and living, the hungry horde within that makes and arbi-
trates the terrior. And we must ask how we see ourselves going forward in
that world. How do we act, inhabit, dwell?
To enact an active, living rhetorical- ontological inquiry— that is to
fold performance and phenomenology into such inquiry— rhetoric schol-
ars may need to diverge from the mapping impulse in Latour’s ANT and
start ruminating on dierently responsive modes of inquiry. Tim Ingold’s
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 285 26/10/18 5:32 PM
286
-1—
0—
+1—
() SPIDERSkilled Practice Involves Developmentally Embodied
Responsiveness— oers one avenue. When the spider abides at the center
of its web, it is “all a- quiver,” wherein every movement is a movement of its
attention, which, in turn, is an instance of action (Ingold, , p.). Ingold
suggests that the essence of action occurs in “the close coupling of bodily
movement and perception,” the activity of a skilled practitioner:
e skilled practitioner is one who can continually attune his or her
movements to perturbations in the perceived environment without
ever interrupting the ow of action. But such skill does not come
ready- made. Rather, it develops, as part and parcel of the organism’s
own growth and development in an environment. (, p.)
Skilled activity as moving with the materials of the environment helps to
close the gap between knowledge and action, thereby becoming the moti-
vation for moving forward dierently in the world. Similar to how the spi-
der spins a web from the materials of its body, gut bacteria and intestines fold
into one another, and these intra- actions become a condition of bodily agency
and rhetorical capacity. Continually responding to and experimenting
with materials, attuning to those intra- actions and the environment in
which they emerge means becoming more skilled practitioners.
is impulse to create and engage in bodily experimentation is already
evident within the world of consumer probiotics through everyday embod-
ied encounters. In fact, Dannon’s Activia brand managers seem to wield
experimental life as a strategic marketing campaign. ey are not waiting
to know more about PF treatments; they do not need more or dierent
knowledge and conceptual frameworks. ey are performing a gut rhetoric—
the capacitation of rhetoric— as a skilled activity of experimenting with
probiotics. Dannon’s website for Activia makes not only the entanglements
of guts and the microbiota explicit but also the skilled practice involving
developmentally embodied responsiveness— a phenomenological investiga-
tion of the gut. e website asks, “Gut check. Do you ever feel bloated,
gassy, or uncomfortable with rumbling?” (“Activia Challenge,” ). In
promoting its brand of probiotic yogurt containing “a unique cocktail of 
dierent cultures,” including “billions of bidus bacteria in every  oz. cup,”
Dannon suggests that Activia allows the consumer to “feel the dierence”
(“Activia in numbers,” ). e website explains: “When the intestinal
microora is out of balance, that imbalance may aect overall health. is
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 286 26/10/18 5:32 PM
Kalin and Gruber
287
— -1
—0
— +1
balance can be disturbed during physical or psychological stress, with age,
in menopause, during drug treatment, with an unbalanced diet, and in the
event of acute or chronic intestinal diseases” (Dannon, “What role,” ).
Bloating, gassiness, rumbling, and discomfort are agonistic “meetings- with,”
aective, bodily entanglements among body, microbes, food, nutrients, and
other materials.
As Activia’s list of imbalances implies, bodily responses are not rational
knowledge but more and dierent kinds of entanglements, attunements,
and experiments. In this way, the microbiota does not present a problem to
be solved, but the capacitation of a gut rhetoric to be lived— the very
grounding of thought, cognition, and response to the world. With respect
to analyzing and performing gut rhetorics, then, the living cannot be situ-
ated merely as a coming- together but also as a “rumbling” and a “gassiness”—
the feeling that something is wrong, the wrangle with misalignment and
rupture. Skillful living is a continuous responsiveness to the ow of materi-
als through and with the body. e need to run to the toilet or desire to pull
out one’s hair are the realities, those living concerns, motivating PF research
in gastroenterology as well as experiments in living with probiotics. at
Activia’s marketing discursively encourages people to feel these ruptures
and buy the product to resolve them may, in some ways, introduce skepti-
cism about doing so, but the marketing materials make the overall point—
responsiveness to a body- environment can function as experimental means
for rethinking how we live and how we might try to (re)attune and then see
what happens. In this way, a rhetorical methodology for gut rhetorics de-
cient in ecological concepts that capture microbial inuences must turn to
performative phenomenologies (Melonçon, ) to see what might “reside
at the corner of play and experimentation” (Gruber, , p.).
Skilled engagement, rather than detachment from the microbiota,
pushes toward “thinking and feeling that grow out of one’s engagement with
the environment” (Coeckelberg, , p.). Such thinking and feeling
shifts attention away from theoretical knowledge (knowing that) and to
embodied, responsive practices and habits (know- how) (Coeckelberg, ).
Although everyday gastroenterological experimentation may not precisely
calibrate relationships between probiotic formulations and aect states, our
bodies do seem to know something— gassiness, rumbling, inammation—
indicating that we could try to eat and live dierently: “Rather than rea-
soning about the environment or having feelings about the environment, we
act and think in a relational, environmental way” (Coeckelberg, , p.).
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 287 26/10/18 5:32 PM
288
-1—
0—
+1—
Savoir faire becomes saveur faire: not only the “art of responding to our envi-
ronment, which invites us and challenges us to respond” (p.), but also
“the art of eating well” and “the art of savoring” our relationships with our
gut bacteria and probiotics. Developing experiments in living with probi-
otics oers one possible response “that moves us, makes us care, and makes
us more ‘environmental,’ and thereby lets us, and our environment, our-
ish” (p.). “Rewilding” our guts with probiotics may well be a trope intended
to reinstill some cooked- up sense of “naturalness” to human technoscien-
tic engagements; nevertheless, it is also a multispecies achievement that
has no nal conclusion.
Ingold’s SPIDER and Coeckelberg’s environmental skill oer non-
dualistic, non- modern ways of thinking and being in the world, of living
with bodies that eat. On the one hand, the scientic view of gastroenterol-
ogy treats gut bacteria and probiotics as “things” that may initiate all kinds
of reactions, contributing to health or sickness (Coeckelberg, , p.).
On the other hand, Dannon’s Activia may oer a romantic view of “natu-
ral” and “authentic” foods, which is equally problematic because it presup-
poses an ontological division between the two and risks glorifying the
agentive power of consumer products. In contrast, a phenomenological
approach— the development of environmental skill that hones uneasy feel-
ings to nd new ways to live— proposes an active relation to an environ-
ment. Examining the textual frameworks and epistemological apparatuses
that construct an image of living bodies will not settle how to live with bod-
ies that eat; rather, we need something less rational in composition, not
completely content with conceptual network mapping— gut rhetorics that
examine how the gut is articulated, what it can do socio- culturally for ref-
ormations of brain- body- environment relations, and what it can do within
the experiences of living as environmental beings.
Conclusion: Experiments in Rhetoric
We have much to learn about rhetoric from our microbial entanglements,
and this knowledge should be both conceptual and skillful. e body does
not simply have or not have benecial or harmful microbiota, and micro-
biota do not simply inhabit the body; they make the body inhabitable and
remake the body. Eating feeds both the body and microbiota that nourish
each other. e body and microbiota are entangled and attuned in ways
beyond the epistemological scope of science’s and rhetoric’s textualizations.
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 288 26/10/18 5:32 PM
Kalin and Gruber
289
— -1
—0
— +1
Indeed, the rst material ecology of gut rhetorics— the experimental cali-
bration of probiotic formulations and aect statesreveals a gut rhetoric
that is an entanglement of patient self- feeling, patient self- report, psycho-
logical assessment, urine samples, and testing kits set both with and against
previous ndings involving lab rats, electroshock treatments, mazes, bright
lights, and so on. is rhetorical- ontological inquiry foregrounds what is
absent as much as what is present, what is attuned and not attuned, expos-
ing breakdown and misalignment just as much as ecological connections
and compelling relations. We hope not to make too strong a point here about
rhetoric scholars ambitiously focusing on ecologies or attunements because,
they, too, tap into the enticements of materiality and the formations of a
rhetorical ontology. Even so, with intense attention on new materialist
approaches in rhetoric and repeated calls to examine how bodies, technol-
ogies, places, and things come together (Blackman, ; Johnson, ;
Rickert, ), we believe that gut rhetorics emphasize those messy onto-
logical formations dependent upon incompatibilities— what cannot be
touched or made palatable, or even made known, just as much as what can
be made anew. We suggest that the embarrassing, forceful, disgusting, icky,
often surprising, movements of the gut, the tricky ruminations of micro-
biota, the unexpected aective response, and the suspicion that resides in
rhetoric’s ability to stir sudden anger or unreasonable outbursts hold some-
thing in common— a glimpse into the embodiment, even the very capaci-
tation, of rhetoric.
Consequently, we suggest a second material ecology of gut rhetorics—
the active, embodied, and skillful experiments in living with probiotics, spe-
cically, and the microbiota, generally. is environmental skill reminds
us again, “No one will ever nally exclaim: ‘So, there it is: now, we know
all that a body can do! Let’s call it a day’” (Gregg & Seigworth, , p.).
We will never know what a body can do because, in part, microbes blanket
the surfaces of the world, literally and guratively terraforming and sculpt-
ing bodies. ey are our environment, our bodily materials, a condition of
our agency, and therefore an invitation and challenge to our embodiment.
ey are both our environment and our happening. Microbes have domes-
ticated us, and we attempt to domesticate microbes. Where gut rhetorics
makes its contribution, then, is not necessarily in conrming the need for
a broader move toward entanglements, but in making explicit the suasive
potentials of microbial life and the continuous embodied responsiveness to
environmental exposure: “We are stuck here and we have to act everyday
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 289 26/10/18 5:32 PM
290
-1—
0—
+1—
as we live alongside an innumerable and unknowable number of species”
(Rivers, , p.). Environmental skill requires a “knowing how” that
surfaces “in the skills of perception and capacities of judgment that develop
in the course of direct, practical, and sensuous engagement with the beings
and things with whom, and with which, we share our lives” (Ingold, ,
p.). Experiments in living with probiotics oers one possibility for ongo-
ing skillful, yet agonistic encounters with microbiota.
Finally, experiments with the dense material complexities of gut bac-
teria come at a time when scholars grow hungry for expanded analyses
involving explorations of aect and the body. Gut rhetorics can serve as a
call to investigate how individual strands of bacteria “aect certain conse-
quences in the world” (Stormer, , p.) not only by being “good” and
producing digestive ease but also by being agonistic to the body and opposed
to scientic textualizations. Gut rhetorics goad a challenge to and a project
for rhetoricians of health and medicine: As we continue to explore rhetori-
cal ontologies to “meet- with” all that a body can do, we must produce both
textualized constructions of bodies across assessment tools, medical charts,
consultations, and probiotic consumerism and ontological provocations for
how we might live with the realities of microbiota and feel the directions
our own bodies are taking us as we feed.
Lisa Melonçon () sets out a similar methodological proposal, argu-
ing that rhetoricians of health and medicine should seek to “understand
how the body experiences health and illness individually” as a way to eval-
uate entwined relations. She suggests folding a “mutual dependence on
embodiment and experience into performative phenomenology,” by which
she means a move to spend more time reecting on in situ sensory experi-
ences within the formation of otherwise traditional rhetorical analysis
(p.). David Gruber () oers a similar proposal: Rhetoric scholars see-
ing the potential to build new theory from neuroscience would benet
from working through performative means, “seeking invention and residing
at the corner of play and experimentation” (p.). Here, Gruber situates
experimentation as both literal and creative, stressing the value of arti-
cial test environments as much as uid discovery wherein bodies play in new
environments that might expose new realities of the gut or realize entirely
new conditions for thought and action (p.).
As such, rhetoric scholars of health and medicine might begin to set
up and conduct their own experiment in living with probiotics. Gut rheto-
rics encourage scholars to feel the inamed linings of the gastrointestinal
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 290 26/10/18 5:32 PM
Kalin and Gruber
291
— -1
—0
— +1
tract as a way of beginning to think with the “appealing muddle” (Wilson,
, p. ) of mucilaginous membranes and metabolizing microbiota.
Perhaps more salaciously in our conception, gut rhetorics take up a sickening
feeling as a starting point for conceptualizing rhetoric as a bodily act and a
variable embodied phenomenon that cannot be well ordered or maintained;
indeed, gut rhetorics suggest an approach that investigates what does not
line up or feel quite right, just as we have done with the examination of dis-
junctions across PF studies which were interrelated precisely to try to stage,
legitimize, and secure the science. However, what is important for gut rheto-
rics is that this calibration does not occur seamlessly or symmetrically, is
not conceptualized as a uid series of attunements; rather, every gut rhetoric
appears as an asymmetric wild fermentation— pungent, intoxicating, repel-
lent. Just as the body is not merely a text, and depression not merely a colo-
nization of the “right” or “wrong” bacteria, nor scientic experimentation
merely a material encounter, so must rhetoricians develop environmental
skill to attune to all the ways that a body is composed— from within conver-
gences as well as “exclusions” and “deleterious gut pathogens” (Messaoudi
etal., , p.), all the ways that a body eats and spills its guts.
J K is an assistant professor in the Department of Writing,
Rhetoric, & Discourse at DePaul University. His research interests include
rhetorical theory; visual, material, and digital rhetorics; and rhetorical
methods. He has published in Rhetoric Society Quarterly, Space & Culture,
and Visual Communication Quarterly, among other forums.
DR. G is an assistant professor in the Department of Media, Cog-
nition and Communication at the University of Copenhagen. His research
focuses on neurorhetorics and rhetoric of the body. His forthcoming book is
titled Brain Art and Neuroscience: Neuro- sensuality and Aective Realism.
References
Access Counseling Group. (). SCL- . Retrieved from http: //accesscm .org /wp
- content /uploads / / /SCL -  .pdf
Ait- Belgnaoui, Afa, Han, Weiping, Lamine, Florance, Eutamene, Hélène, Fiora-
monti, Jean, Bueno, Lionel, & eodorou, Vassilia. (). Lactobacillus
farciminis treatment suppresses stress induced visceral hypersensitivity: A
possible action through interaction with epithelial cell cytoskeleton con-
traction. Gut, 55(), – .
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 291 26/10/18 5:32 PM
292
-1—
0—
+1—
Akrich, Madeleine, & Latour, Bruno. (). A summary of a convenient vocabu-
lary for the semiotics of human and nonhuman assemblies. In WiebeE.
Bijker & John Law (Eds.), Shaping technology/building society: Studies in
sociotechnical change (pp.– ). Cambridge, MA: e MIT Press.
Barnett, Scot, & Boyle, Casey. (Eds.). (). Rhetoric, through everyday things. Tus-
caloosa: University Alabama Press.
Bazerman, Charles. (). Shaping written knowledge: e genre and activity of the
experimental article in science. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Blackman, Lisa. (). Immaterial bodies: Aect, embodiment, mediation. London:
SAGE.
Bravo, JavierA., Forsythe, Paul., Chew, MariaaneV., Escaravage, Emily, Savignac,
Hélène M., Dinan, TimothyG., . . . Cryan, JohnF. (). Ingestion of
Lactobacillus strain regulates emotional behavior and central GABA
receptor expression in a mouse via the vagus nerve. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, 108(), – .
Chakraborti, SushilK., Dey, BinayK., Ghosh, N., Chaudhury, A. N., & Mazumder,
D. N. Guha. (). Objective evaluation of psychological abnormality in
irritable bowel syndrome. Indian Journal of Gastroenterology, 15(),  .
Coeckelberg, Mark. (). Environmental skill: Motivation, knowledge, and the pos-
sibility of a non- Romantic environmental ethics. New York: Routledge.
Cohen, Sheldon. (). Perceived stress scale. Menlo Park, CA: Mind Garden, Inc.
Retrieved from http: //www .mindgarden .com /documents /PerceivedStress
Scale .pdf
Cryan, JohnF., & Dinan, TimothyG. (). Mind- altering microorganisms: e
impact of the gut microbiota on brain and behaviour. Nature Reviews Neu-
roscience, 13(), – .
Dannon Inc. (a). Activia challenge. Retrieved from http: //www .activia .us .com
/activia - challenge
Dannon Inc. (b). Activia in numbers. Retrieved from http: //www .activia .us
.com /about - activia /who - we - are /activia - numbers
Dannon Inc. (c). What role does your digestive system play in your overall
health? Retrieved from http: //www .activia .us .com /about - us /digestive
- health
Derogatis, LeonardR., Lipman, RonaldL., Rickels, Karl, Uhlenhuth, E. H., &
Covi, Lino. (). e Hopkins Symptoms Checklist: A self- report
symptom inventory. Behavioral Science, 19(), –  .
Dinan, Timothy. G., Stilling, RomanM., Stanton, Catherine, & Cryan, JohnF.
(). Collective unconscious: How gut microbes shape human behav-
ior. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 63, – .
Diop, Laurent, Guillou, Sonia, & Durand, Henri. (). Probiotic food supple-
ment reduces stress- induced gastrointestinal symptoms in volunteers: A
double- blind, placebo- controlled, randomized trial. Nutrition Research,
28(), – .
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 292 26/10/18 5:32 PM
Kalin and Gruber
293
— -1
—0
— +1
Estrada, Alberto, Drew, MurrayD., & Van Kessel, Andrew. (). Eect of the
dietary supplementation of fructooligosaccharides and Bidobacterium
longum to early- weaned pigs on performance and fecal bacterial popula-
tions. Canadian Journal of Animal Science, 81(), – .
Everensel, Alper, & Ceylan, Mehmet Emin. (). e gut- brain axis: e miss-
ing link in depression. Clinical Psychopharmacology and Neuroscience, 13(),
– .
Fitzgerald, Des, & Callard, Felicity. (). Social science and neuroscience beyond
interdisciplinarity: Experimental entanglements. eory, Culture & Soci-
ety, 32(), – .
Graham, S. Scott. (). e politics of pain medicine: A rhetorical- ontological inquiry.
Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Gregg, Melissa, & Seigworth, Greg. J. (Eds.). (). e aect theory reader. Dur-
ham, NC: Duke University Press.
Gruber, David. R. (). Experiments in rhetoric: Invention and neurorhetorical
play. In Lisa Melonçon & J. Blake Scott (Eds.), Methodologies for the rhet-
oric of health & medicine (pp.– ). New York: Routledge.
Gwee, Kok- Ann, Leong, Y.- L., Graham, Clive, McKendrick, MichaelW., Col-
lins, Stephen. M., Walters, StephenJ., Underwood, James, Read, NickW.
(). e role of psychological and biological factors in postinfective gut
dysfunction. Gut, 44(), – .
Hanstock, TanyaL., Clayton, EdwardH., Li, KongM., & Mallet, PaulE. ().
Anxiety and aggression associated with the fermentation of carbohydrates
in the hindgut of rats. Physiology & Behavior, 82(– ), – .
Haraway, DonnaJ. (). Staying with the trouble: Making kin in the Chthulucene.
Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Hill, Annie. (). Breast cancer’s rhetoricity: Bodily border crisis and bridge to
corporeal solidarity. Review of Communication, 16(), – .
Hird, MyraJ. (). e origins of sociable life: Evolution after science studies. New
York: Palgrave Macmillan.
Ingold, Tim. (). Being alive: Essays on movement, knowledge, and description. New
York: Routledge.
Ingold, Tim. (). e life of lines. New York: Routledge.
Ingold, Tim. (). Surface visions. eory, Culture & Society, 34(– ), – .
Jack, Jordynn. (). A pedagogy of sight: Microscopic vision in Robert Hooke’s
Micrographia. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 95(),   .
Johnson, NathanR. (). Infrastructural methodology: A case in protein as pub-
lic health. In Lisa Melonçon & J. Blake Scott (Eds.), Methodologies for
the rhetoric of health & medicine (pp.– ). New York: Routledge.
Kamiya, Takeshi, Wang, Lu, Forsythe, Paul, Goettsche, Gudrun, Mao, Yukang,
Wang, Yufang, Tougas, Gervais, Bienenstock, JohnJ. (). Inhibitory
eects of Lactobacillus reuteri on visceral pain induced by colorectal dis-
tension in Sprague- Dawley rats. Gut, 55(), – .
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 293 26/10/18 5:32 PM
294
-1—
0—
+1—
Landecker, Hannah. (). Food as exposure: Nutritional epigenetics and the new
metabolism. BioSocieties, 6(), – .
Law, John. (). Notes on the theory of the actor network: Ordering, strategy,
and heterogeneity. Systems Practice, 5(), – .
Lorimer, Jamie. (). Gut buddies: Multispecies studies and the microbiome.
Environmental Humanities, 8(), – .
Lorimer, Jamie. (). Probiotic environmentalities: Rewilding with wolves and
worms. eory, Culture & Society, 34(), – .
Lynch, JohnA. (). Articulating scientic practice: Understanding Dean Ham-
er’s “gay gene” study as overlapping material, social and rhetorical regis-
ters. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 95(),  .
Matza, LouisS., Wyrwich, KathleenW., Phillips, GlennA., Murray, LindseyT.,
Malley, KarenG., & Revicki, DennisA. (). e fatigue associated
with depression questionnaire (FAsD): Responsiveness and responder
denition. Quality of Life Research, 22(), – .
Melonçon, Lisa. (). Bringing the body back through performative phenome-
nology. In Lisa Melonçon & J. Blake Scott (Eds.), Methodologies for the
rhetoric of health & medicine (pp.). New York: Routledge.
Messaoudi, Michaël, Lalonde, Robert, Violle, Nicolas, Javelot, Hervé, Desor,
Didier, Nejdi, Amine, . . . Cazaubiel, Jean- Marc. (). Assessment of
psychotropic- like properties of a probiotic formulation (Lactobacillus hel-
veticus R0052 and Bidobacterium longum R0175) in rats and human sub-
jects. British Journal of Nutrition, 105(), – .
Mol, Annemarie. (). e body multiple: Ontology in medical practice. Durham,
NC: Duke University Press.
Naserifbafrouei, Ali, Hestad, Knut, Avershina, Ekaterina, Sekelja, Monika, Lin-
løkken, Arne, Wilson, Robert, & Rudi, Knut (). Correlation between
the human fecal microbiota and depression. Neurogastroenterology &
Motility, 26(), – .
Petra, AnastasiaI., Panagiotidou, Smaro, Hatziagelaki, Erili, Stewart, JulieM.,
Conti, Pio, & eoharides, eoharisC. (). Gut- microbiota- brain
axis and its eect on neuropsychiatric disorders with suspected immune
dysregulation. Clinical erapeutics, 37(), – .
Rao, Kiran, Subbakrishna, D. K., & Prabhu, G. G. (). Development of a coping
checklist: A preliminary report. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 31(),  – .
Rickert, omas. (). Ambient rhetoric: e attunements of rhetorical being. Pitts-
burgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.
Rivers, NathanielA. (). Deep ambivalence and wild objects: Toward a strange
environmental rhetoric. Rhetoric Society Quarterly, 45(), – .
Salvetti, Elisa, Torriani, Sandra, & Felis, GiovannaE. (). e genus Lactoba-
cillus: A taxonomic update. Probiotics and Antimicrobial Proteins, 4(),
– .
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 294 26/10/18 5:32 PM
Kalin and Gruber
295
— -1
—0
— +1
Schmidt, Kristin, Cowen, PhilipJ., Harmer, CatherineJ., Tzortzis, George, Err-
ington, Stevene, & Burnet, PhilipW. J. (). Prebiotic intake reduces
the waking cortisol response and alters emotional bias in healthy volun-
teers. Psychopharmacology, 232(),  – .
Snaith, R. Philip. (). e hospital anxiety and depression scale. Health and
Quality of Life Outcomes, 1(), – .
Sonnenburg, Justin, Sonnenburg, Erica. (). e good gut: Taking control of your
weight, your mood, and your long- term health. New York: Penguin Books.
Stormer, Nathan. (). An appetite for rhetoric. Philosophy and Rhetoric, 48(),
– .
Stormer, Nathan. (). Rhetoric’s diverse materiality: Polythetic, ontology, and
genealogy. Review of Communication, 16(), – .
Stormer, Nathan., & McGreavy, Bridie (). inking ecologically about rhet-
oric’s ontology: Capacity, vulnerability, and resilience. Philosophy and
Rhetoric, 50(), – .
SVRI. (). Hospital anxiety and depression scale. Sexual Violence Research Ini-
tiative. Retrieved from http: //www .svri .org /sites /default /les /attachments
/ -  -  /HADS .pdf
Wells, Susan. (). Legible bodies: Nineteenth- century women physicians and
the rhetoric of dissection. In Jack Selzer & Sharon Crowley (Eds.), Rhe-
torical bodies (pp.– ). Madison: University of Wisconsin Press.
Wilson, ElizabethA. (). Gut feminism. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Yamamura, Schuhei, Morishima, Hiroko, Kumano- go, Takayuki, Suganuma,
Nakamori, Matsumoto, Hiroshi, Adachi, Hiroyoshi, . . . Takeda,
Masatoshi. (). e eect of Lactobacillus helveticus fermented milk
on sleep and health perception in elderly subjects. European Journal of
Clinical Nutrition, 63(), – .
Yong, Ed. (). I contain multitudes: e microbes within us and a grander view of
life. New York: Harper Collins.
Young, AnnaM., Eckstein, Justin, & Conley, Donovan. (). Rhetorics and
foodways. Communication and Critical/Cultural Studies, 12(), – .
RHM_1-3_04_Kalin_1P.indd 295 26/10/18 5:32 PM
... Following this approach when examining the material-semiotic arrangements of probiotics experimentation, we previously proposed the term gut rhetorics to emphasize how bodies, microbes, and the scientific apparatus being used to study them co-constitute one another (Kalin & Gruber, 2018). In particular, we studied how probiotic experiments are argued for and made viable through asymmetrical relations. ...
... One goal here is to help researchers find new avenues for making more specific health claims by interrelating divergent questionnaires rhetorically-foregrounding the epistemological, ontological, and phenomenological assumptions mutually informing and validating the questionnaires. For instance, rather than relying on quantitative aspects only (EFSA, 2012;Kalin & Gruber, 2018;Shane et al., 2010) and overlooking the social purposes from which questionnaires have emerged as psychometric measures, researchers might adopt experimental resources with similar presumptions and then deploy them as originally designed. Attending to the rhetorical, materialsemiotic qualities of experimental resources elucidates how control and test groups are developed and managed and how affect states are delineated and calibrated. ...
... In this approach, the symbolic intention always, in some way, includes the agency of matter (without intention) and the suasion of matter (affectability). For example, with our term gut rhetorics, we try to demonstrate the inextricable agencies and suasions of probiotic formulations, gut bacteria, and dietary recommendations (Kalin & Gruber, 2018). In this case, we build on the notion of gut rhetorics by looking at the history and purpose of two psychometric questionnaires. ...
Article
Full-text available
Prebiotic and probiotic products continue to experience strong consumer growth while gastroenterological researchers work behind the scenes to understand the effects of specific bacterial strains on study participants’ feelings of mind‐body healthfulness. Correlations are achieved through the use of psychometric questionnaires, which aim to measure affect states like stress, anxiety, and depression. In this paper, we turn to qualitative textual analysis to examine two commonly employed psychometric questionnaires (HAM‐D and HADS) used in studies of the same probiotic formulation. We show how concepts of social deviance and norms of behavior (e.g., sleep patterns, social life, and work productivity) inform such questionnaires, thus inserting normative ethical imperatives into clinical findings. After demonstrating several key disjunctions across these questionnaires, we develop recommendations for gastroenterological researchers. In particular, we recommend that researchers give more focused attention to aligning psychometric questionnaires and more carefully enact their qualitative assessment requirements. In like manner, we recommend that ethical reviewers and science policy reviewers consider how psychometric questionnaires embed social assumptions into clinical trials, which can affect determinations of healthfulness.
... Where it appears, aggression is isolating in these stories-Metis is imprisoned alone, Medea is persecuted for her retaliation-but perhaps we should reconceive antagonism and the biological processes that generate it as fertile grounds for collectivization and visionary reform. The gut microbiome is complex and variable, but if chronic pain alters its composition, then fibromyalgic gut feminism, mētis by another name, might constitute a biological and sociopolitical commons: respectively, a pan-microbiome of diminished F. prausnitzii and butyrate, and of hostility and harm as animating principles of feminist engagement and solidarity (Wilson, 2015, p. 17;Kalin & Gruber, 2018). Wilson (2015) asserts that the destructive, damaging elements of political action can't always be repurposed for productive possibilities, that "feminist politics are most effective . . . ...
Thesis
Full-text available
This project theorizes relationships among discourses around the ailing body, biomedical technologies intended to render visible chronic pain, and the compulsory able-bodymindedness of academic culture and its writing conventions. Through analysis of discursive artifacts, such as imaging reports, pharmaceutical rhetoric, self-monitoring technologies, and academic interchange, I show how techniques and institutions converge to bring the scholar-in-pain under the biomedical and academic gaze. From pain questionnaires to requirements within the academy, institutions and standards seek to quantify pain in ways that minimize chronicity, contingency, and idiosyncrasy to craft acceptable, visible experiences of pain and its embodiment; chronically pained subjects must conform to these understandings in order to be believed. In this project, I analyze and examine my analysis of my pain as both a patient and a scholar to de-individuate the singular experience of chronic pain and recover what is desirable and resistive about the ontology of fibromyalgia. I consider how meanings are made around a queer Eelam Tamil fibromyalgic woman scholar’s bodymind in biomedical and academic settings in order to excavate broader cultural relationships among chronic pain, ocularcentrism, Euro-Western and Tamil sensory hierarchies, and decolonial ways of knowing.
... Nonetheless, while recent contributions to the social studies of microbes have acknowledged the multiplicities of microbes (e.g. Kirksey 2019;Lorimer 2017;Paxson 2012;Helmreich 2009;Jasarevic 2015;Kalin and Gruber 2018), the analyses of many social scientists remain dualistic. Paxson (2008: 17-8) argued that the revival of artisanal cheesemaking in the United States 'provides a window onto social and regulatory negotiations of a hyperhygienic Pasteurian social order (as forwarded by the FDA [Food and Drug Administration]) and a post-Pasteurian microbiopolitics' advocated by raw milk activists. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Collectively authored introduction to 'With Microbes', an open-access edited collection of ethnographic encounters between microbes and humans.
Article
Music is a visceral experience. It allows audiences to feel its message in and through their bodies. In this essay, I bring together the rhetorical concepts of viscerality and harmony to offer a new approach to feminist musical rhetorical criticism. I argue that recording artists ROES and Sia produce rhetorical harmony by performing and inciting viscerality in “Battle Cry,” and that rhetorical harmony exists when connections are made across difference for social change.
Article
Full-text available
This essay conceptualizes and applies a theory of rhetorical debility to new materialist rhetorical studies. Drawing from critical disability studies, rhetorical debility frames the ways that hierarchical human and nonhuman relations can inhibit certain rhetoricities while enabling others under neoliberalism. This theory extends the concept of “rhetorical capacities,” located within a genealogy of new materialist and posthuman thought in rhetorical studies, in response to intersectional critique of new materialism from Indigenous scholars and disability studies. The essay demonstrates rhetorical debility’s applicability to transnational sites of oppression along axes of disability, colonialism, and neoliberalism through a case study analysis of Palestinian protest rhetoric.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter describes an infrastructural approach to rhetoric in health and medicine. An infrastructural approach explores how shared, background resources politicize the knowledge work of health and medicine. The chapter describes the infrastructural approach described provides a set of conceptual tools. It focuses on the rhetorical tradition to perform a rhetorical-historical study of protein classifications. When infrastructural technologies break, they cause massive interruptions in knowledge work. The more foundational and global the infrastructure, the more likely it can cause global, catastrophic problems. Electronic medical records aren't yet globally adopted, and if the software that ran them ceased to work, it would be less of a problem than if all access to the International Classification of Diseases (ICD) disappeared. Infrastructural technologies like the ICD bridge relationships between several groups that have different information needs. Consequently, identifying potential infrastructural breakdowns and underserved groups becomes important for providing equitable access to health and medical care.
Book
Full-text available
Today it is widely recognized that we face urgent and serious environmental problems and we know much about them, yet we do very little. What explains this lack of motivation and change? Why is it so hard to change our lives? This book addresses this question by means of a philosophical inquiry into the conditions of possibility for environmental change. It discusses how we can become more motivated to do environmental good and what kind of knowledge we need for this, and explores the relations between motivation, knowledge, and modernity. After reviewing a broad range of possible philosophical and psychological responses to environmental apathy and inertia, the author argues for moving away from a modern focus on either detached reason and control (Stoicism and Enlightenment reason) or the natural, the sentiments, and the authentic (Romanticism), both of which make possible disengaging and alienating modes of relating to our environment. Instead he develops the notion of environmental skill: a concept that bridges the gap between knowledge and action, re-interprets environmental virtue, and suggests an environmental ethics centered on experience, know-how and skillful engagement with our environment. The author then explores the implications of this ethics for our lives: it changes the way we think about , and deal with, health, food, animals, energy, climate change, politics, and technology.
Article
A probiotic turn is underway in the management of human and environmental health. Modern approaches are being challenged by deliberate interventions that introduce formerly taboo life forms into bodies, homes, cities and the wider countryside. These are guided by concepts drawn from the life sciences, including immunity and resilience. This analysis critically evaluates this turn, drawing on examples of rewilding nature reserves and reworming the human microbiome. It identifies a common ontology of socio-ecological systems marked by anthropogenic absences and tipped across thresholds into less desirable states. It examines the operation of an environmental mode of biopower associated with deliberate efforts to engineer ecologies through the introduction of keystone species. It offers a set of criteria for critically evaluating the degree to which these interventions transform or sustain prevalent forms of late modern biopolitics. The conclusion reflects on the potentials of probiotic environmentalities for hospitable government beyond the Anthropocene.
Article
Responding to Susan Sontag?s groundbreaking text Illness as Metaphor, this article analyzes breast cancer as a figure of entanglement, drawing on Karen Barad?s theory of agential realism. Communication scholars have fruitfully explored discursive constructions of breast cancer, but a material?discursive analysis of the disease, and the significant site it inhabits, provides a more robust account of the constraints and opportunities configuring bodies and social movements. To make the case, I show that agential realism is equipped to grasp breast cancer?s rhetoricity as it destabilizes binary codes of being, including language/matter, subject/object, and human/nonhuman. I then offer the concept of transmaterial intra-actionality to track entanglements of disease and target the political stakes of accounting for human and nonhuman life. I conclude with a call for corporeal solidarity: a posthuman politics that acknowledges connections across and through bodies.
Article
Rhetoric is multiple and mutable in the sense that there is more than one kind of rhetoric and any particular rhetoric is highly adaptable to the point that what qualifies as rhetoric according to scholar A may be unrecognizable as such to scholar B. It is not safe to assume that we can account for rhetoric as a multiplicity or in its mutability. Despite an arsenal of terms to characterize rhetoric, how to talk about it as diverse? This essay first conceptualizes material ?diversity? and presents a borrowed term, polythesis, to give some character to the problem of rhetoric as ontologically one and many. Second, the essay discusses genealogy as an approach that enables the sorting of different rhetorics without producing a fixed taxonomy. Parsing rhetoric?s multiplicity requires mobile discriminations and should be paired with a methodology sensitive to ontological flux. As historical ontologies, genealogies of different rhetorics can produce meaningful distributions while emphasizing impermanence and changeability.
Article
Two experiments were conducted to evaluate the effect of administration of fructooligosaccharides (FOS) and Bifidobacterium longum on growth performance and fecal bacterial populations in pigs weaned at 18 d of age. In exp. 1, two groups of 20 pigs each were fed diets containing 0 or 0.5% FOS for 21 d. On days 1 and 3, pigs receiving FOS were administered an oral dose of 10(10) B. longum cells. During week 1, average daily gain was higher and feed efficiency was improved (P < 0.05) in pigs fed FOS and bifidobacteria relative to the control. On day 7, supplemented pigs had reduced numbers of total anaerobes and clostridia, and increased numbers of bifidobacteria in faeces (P < 0.05). in exp. 2, 80 pigs were divided into four groups of 20 animals each in a 2 x 2 factorial design. Main effects included FOS supplementation (0 or 0.5% of diet) and B. longum supple mentation (0 or 10(7) cells per gram feed). FOS supplementation reduced growth while B. longum supplementation increased growth (P < 0.05). On days 12 and 19, serum insulin-like growth factor I (IGF-I) was reduced with FOS supplementation and increased with B. longum supplementation (P < 0.05). It was concluded that the continuous administration of bifidobacteria provided a beneficial effect on growth parameters studied in this experiment.