Content uploaded by Angela Meadows
Author content
All content in this area was uploaded by Angela Meadows on Mar 04, 2019
Content may be subject to copyright.
This is an Accepted Manuscript of an article published by Taylor & Francis in
Fat Studies on 3 Dec, 2018, available online:
https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/21604851.2019.1550303
Yes, We Can (No, You Can’t): Weight Stigma, Exercise Self-Efficacy, and Active
Fat Identity Development
Angela Meadows & Andrea E Bombak
Title: Yes, we can (No, you can’t): Weight stigma, exercise self-efficacy, and active-fat
identity development
Authors: Angela Meadows1 and Andrea E Bombak2
Affiliations:
1. School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, B15 2TT, UK
2. Department of Sociology, University of New Brunswick, PO Box 4400, Fredericton, New
Brunswick, Canada, E3B 5A3
Email:
AM: drameadows@gmail.com (corresponding author)
AEB: andrea.bombak@unb.ca
Abstract
An inverse relationship has been observed between body mass index and physical activity
levels.
1
In the present paper, we draw from a range of literatures to construct a novel,
theoretical dual-pathway model that identifies direct and indirect impacts of societal weight
stigma on exercise behavior. The direct pathway operates via experiences or threat of stigma
and discrimination, which create traumatic learning experiences and impair the development
of exercise self-efficacy. The indirect pathway impedes engagement in physical activity due
to the absence of positive representations of fat exercisers, and a glut of negative
representations, resulting from societal anti-fat attitudes. Thus, fat people lack role models
from whom they may develop vicarious self-efficacy. Low self-efficacy, in turn, hinders the
development of active fat identities. We review the existing literature for evidence supporting
such a model, identify directions for future research, and briefly consider the implications of
this framework for public health and policy aims.
1
The focus of this paper is on how weight stigma, specifically, impacts on self-efficacy and active identity
development. As such, we have written predominantly about fat people and their exercise behavior. This is not
to suggest that exercise is something that thin people do and fat people don’t, that fat people’s exercise behavior
should be unilaterally targeted in public health policies and health promotion campaigns, or that fat people (or
anyone else) are under any obligation to engage in leisure time physical activity.
Introduction
Higher-weight individuals report experiencing weight stigma in practically every domain of
daily living, including at work, in education, healthcare, and interpersonal relationships (Puhl
& King, 2013). Experience of weight stigma has been linked to avoidance of exercise and
reduced exercise intentions in higher-weight adults (Schvey et al., 2016; Vartanian & Novak,
2011; Vartanian, Pinkus, & Smyth, 2016; Vartanian & Shaprow, 2008) and children (for a
review, see Salvy, de la Haye, Bowker, & Hermans, 2012), and to less frequent engagement
in moderate or strenuous exercise in both higher-weight student (Vartanian & Shaprow,
2008)
2
and community (Jackson & Steptoe, 2017; Vartanian & Novak, 2011) samples.
3
Surprisingly little work has been done to elucidate the mechanisms by which weight stigma
influences physical activity levels in higher-weight individuals. In the present paper, we draw
on insights from self-determination theory, role model theory, and identity theory to devise a
novel, dual-pathway model that links endemic weight stigma with reduced physical activity
via impaired development of exercise self-efficacy and active fat identities. We first consider
the direct impacts of experienced weight stigma on exercise experience, perceived
competence and the development of active fat identity. Secondly, we describe how societal
anti-fat attitudes in general are both manifested and perpetuated via the absence of positive
representations of fat exercisers, and how lack of representation directly and indirectly
impacts on exercise self-efficacy and active identities. We conclude by briefly discussing the
limitations of this model, directions for future research, and potential public health and policy
implications.
2
This study involved a weight-diverse sample (N = 100). Analyses for the subset of participants with a BMI
25 kg/m2 (n = 25) indicated much stronger associations between weight stigma and exercise variables compared
with lower-weight participants.
3
Experienced weight stigma was linked with increased exercise levels in a study of 177 higher-weight women
(Pearl, Puhl, & Dovidio, 2015); however, the measure of experienced stigma in this study comprised only three
broad yes-no items and may not be sensitive enough to capture true levels of stigma experiences, particularly
more subtle and insidious occurrences (Meadows & Higgs, manuscript in preparation).
Exercise self-efficacy and exercise identity
Perceived self-efficacy can be defined as an individual’s beliefs in their ability to achieve
outcomes of interest, and as such, influences, among other things, what behaviors we engage
in, how much effort we invest, the extent of our perseverance in the face of barriers or
hardship, and our actual levels of achievement (Bandura, 1997). Exercise self-efficacy is one
of the most robust psychosocial predictors of engagement in physical activity (Bauman et al.,
2012; S. L. Williams & French, 2011) and has been both cross-sectionally and prospectively
linked with adoption and maintenance of physical activity (McAuley & Blissmer, 2000).
However, within the field of health promotion, much of the work on exercise self-efficacy is
framed in terms of self-regulatory capacity – that is, one’s perceived ability to engage in a
behavior when faced with certain barriers and challenges, such as time constraints, bad
weather, or family commitments (D. M. Williams & Rhodes, 2016). This focus situates non-
participation at the level of personal failings or weaknesses, an approach to health promotion
that tends toward victim-blaming (Crawford, 1977; McLeroy, Bibeau, Steckler, & Glanz,
1988). It also presupposes an underlying a priori desire to participate in physical activity, and
does nothing to address the case where individuals may prefer not to engage in the first place.
Here, we instead focus on a different aspect of exercise self-efficacy – that of task
competence. We propose that both overt and implicit anti-fat attitudes reduce perceived
exercise competence in higher-weight individuals. Awareness and personal experiences of
societal weight stigma have been linked with reduced physical activity competence in
individuals participating in a weight-loss program, and appeared to explain the negative
relationship between BMI and perceived physical activity abilities (Schmalz, 2010). We
suggest two potential mechanistic pathways through which weight stigma lowers exercise
self-efficacy: first, via fat people’s own experiences in the physical activity environment, and
second, via the erasure of positive representations of fat exercise and fat exercisers within the
physical activity environment for fat people to look to as possible selves and inspirational
role models in the domain of exercise. We further argue that this form of low exercise self-
efficacy hinders the development of active-fat identities (Figure 1).
Figure 1. Proposed dual-pathway relationship between weight stigma and physical activity, via impact on exercise self-
efficacy and exercise identity. Solid lines represent positive relationships and dashed lines negative relationships.
An individuals’ self-schema comprises a collection of generalized identities across a range of
domains, each representing one aspect of the self by which we understand who we are. Our
self-schema act as a lens through which we interpret our experiences and the world around
us, but also influence how we interact with the world and behave in a given situation
(Markus, 1977). Different aspects of our self-schema will be more relevant in different
situations, and identity theory posits that the greater the salience of a given identity compared
with other self-categorizations, the more likely behavioral choices will be made in line with
that identity (Stryker & Burke, 2000). People prefer to act in ways that are congruent with
their identities (Stets & Burke, 2000): an action that is congruent with a situationally relevant
identity may be pursued even in the face of difficulty – such difficulties may even reinforce
the importance of that behavior for an individual’s self-concept, whereas the same difficulties
encountered during identity-incongruent behavior is likely to reinforce dis-identification with
the domain (Oyserman, 2015). Indeed, perceived ability has been linked with exercise
identity in both cross-sectional and prospective studies, and a strong exercise identity is, in
turn, linked to more frequent exercise, greater future exercise intentions, and perseverance in
the face of barriers to exercise (for a review, see Rhodes, Kaushal, & Quinlan, 2016).
Pathway 1: Weight stigma and the experience of fat exercisers
Explicit and implicit stigma
The primary means by which self-efficacy is developed is via prior achievement.
Successfully engaging in an activity increases an individual’s belief that they can do so again
(Bandura, 1986). In contrast, repeated lack of success despite effort can result in learned
helplessness – a phenomenon wherein people learn that no matter how hard they try, they
will not succeed, which, unsurprisingly, often results in disengagement from that activity
(Maier & Seligman, 1976). In the absence of perceived ability to achieve a desired outcome,
individuals may have little motivation to engage in an activity (Bandura, 1997).
4
From an
early age, fat people’s experiences of formal exercise may be more likely to resemble this
second pattern of learning. Qualitative studies among adolescents about their experiences
during physical education (PE) classes, report frequent experiences of weight related name-
calling and bullying, being mocked for their perceived or actual lack of skill, and being
laughed at if they fall or are injured, often with implicit or overt acceptance of such behavior
by teachers (W. Li & Rukavina, 2012; Trout & Graber, 2009). Weight-related victimization
in PE classes is associated with lower perceived physical abilities among higher-weight
adolescents, with greater fear of being stigmatized, and with reduced engagement in physical
activity outside the school environment (Maïano et al., 2018). PE teachers often hold
4
Although it is possible that, in some circumstances, an individual may be motivated to try something that they
currently think they are unable to do, in the present context, we refer to the situation where repeated efforts have
resulted in consistently negative outcomes, as described below.
stereotypical negative attitudes toward fat students, expect them to be less fit and healthy than
slimmer students, and have lower performance expectations for them (Greenleaf & Weiller,
2005), especially for female students (Peterson, Puhl, & Luedicke, 2012). Thus, the
intersection of gender and weight compounds the gender-stereotype inequalities already
faced by girls in sports environments (Chalabaev, Sarrazin, Fontayne, Boiché, & Clément-
Guillotin, 2013). These negative assumptions and stereotypical beliefs may well become self-
fulfilling as fat youngsters are implicitly and explicitly taught that their bodies exclude them
from enjoyable and profitable engagement in exercise and sport, and consequently exhibit
reduced self-efficacy and engage less frequently in physical activity (Rice, 2007). These early
experiences may have long-lasting effects. In-depth interviews with adults about their
recollections of school PE classes suggest that, for many, “fat phobia created extremely
difficult situations that demanded constant psychic/emotional work, provided pitiful
opportunities for learning, and numerous alienating and traumatic movement experiences”
(Sykes & McPhail, 2008, p. 68).
The confluence of physical activity with weight-stigmatizing experiences does not end at
graduation. Fitness center employees, including management, reception staff, and fitness
professionals, have been shown to hold negative implicit anti-fat attitudes, despite
considering themselves to be unbiased (Dimmock, Hallett, & Grove, 2009). Witnessing fat
people exercising, that is, engaging in counter-stereotypical behavior, did not ablate these
attitudes. Other studies have demonstrated both strong implicit and explicit anti-fat bias in
current (Robertson & Vohora, 2008) and future fitness professionals (Chambliss, Finley, &
Blair, 2004). Some gyms mock fat people in their advertising (AdWeek, 2010; S. Murphy,
2017; Schlossberg, 2016), and it is not unusual for class instructors to openly stigmatize fat
bodies and use them as a fear-based “motivator” for class participants (Kenen, 1987; Packer,
1989). Other gym members may be another source of weight stigma. In a large UK sample,
frequent exercisers exhibited more explicit anti-fat attitudes in general (Flint et al., 2015), and
in a small qualitative study, focus groups conducted in regular gym-goers demonstrated that
these attitudes were not limited to those fat people who were considered to demonstrate
negative stereotypes of laziness, but extended to fat exercisers, both within a formal gym
setting and those exercising in outdoor settings, with numerous examples of bullying,
harassment, discrimination, and dehumanization recorded (Flint & Reale, 2016). Indeed,
strangers are one of the most common sources of stigma, and being stigmatised in public,
even in the presence of bystanders, is not unusual (Vartanian, Pinkus, & Smyth, 2014). Fat
people report being stared at, photographed, verbally and physically abused by strangers, or
otherwise stigmatized (Puhl & Brownell, 2006), including while exercising (Bombak, 2015;
Ellison, 2009; S. Lewis et al., 2011).
Even in the absence of a stigmatizing incident, most fat people are acutely aware of the
stereotypes that others hold towards them and their devalued status in society (Degher &
Hughes, 1999; Kwan, 2010). This culturally shared knowledge imbues daily life with what
Link and colleagues termed “symbolic interaction stigma” (Link, Wells, Phelan, & Yang,
2015, p. 118), where much psychic effort is devoted to fear, expectation, or anticipation of
others’ reactions, and mental rehearsals of how one would respond if such an interaction did
arise. That is, living as a fat person in a fat-phobic society can lead to feeling stigmatized
even in the absence of overt stigma experiences. Fear of weight-related stigma or anxiety
about others’ weight-based judgments has been shown to be a more potent driver of some
psychological outcomes and health-related behaviors than weight-related self-devaluation per
se (Lillis, Thomas, Levin, & Wing, 2017; Meadows & Higgs, manuscript in preparation). In
one qualitative study, a large proportion of higher-weight participants mentioned,
unprompted, the impact of direct, indirect, and structural stigma on their desire to exercise in
public, and describe their hypervisibility and expectations of ridicule and abuse (S. Lewis et
al., 2011). Even purportedly well-meaning commentary, either in person or on social media,
tends to carry patronizing assumptions about fat people’s motivation for exercise (to lose
weight) and lack of experience and fitness, reinforcing the point that fat bodies are observed,
judged, and somehow considered public property (Chastain, 2014b). Fat exercisers are
deprived of agentic motives. Instead, these exercisers are presumed to be recognizing their
culpability in a fat-phobic culture and trying to atone by taking responsibility for fixing their
burdensome bodies (Monaghan, 2008; Rauscher, Kauer, & Wilson, 2013).
Stereotype threat
Stereotype threat provides another potential barrier to physical activity in fat individuals.
Stereotype threat occurs when a person is in a situation where their behavior is likely to be
judged along stereotyped lines, and they risk reinforcing negative stereotypes about a group
to which they belong (Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002). Engaging in physical activity, by
definition, will produce a physiological response that is likely to include outward signs of
exertion: breathing hard, sweating, flushed face (not to mention a certain amount of
‘bouncing and jiggling’). While these signs are a normal response to a body working hard, it
is a commonplace that they would be associated with the stereotype of lack of fitness in a fat
person (Danielsen, Sundgot-Borgen, & Rugseth, 2016; Sykes & McPhail, 2008). Thus, for
the fat exerciser, simply being observed in the practice of exercising risks confirming the
stereotype that fat people are lazy, inactive, and unhealthy (Bombak, 2015; Puhl, Schwartz,
& Brownell, 2005), with anti-fat attitudes giving rise to the double standard whereby sweaty
thin people are admirable but sweaty fat people are disdained (Flint & Reale, 2016; Zimdars,
2015).
So powerful is the link between a stereotyped identity and attitudes and behaviours in a
stereotype-relevant situation, that activation of a fat identity can induce a threat effect in an
exercise context even when the individual would not be observed exercising. One
experimental study, conducted over the telephone, found that simply asking overweight
women who were already frequent exercisers to give their weight after hearing about a sham
study regarding healthy habits versus health outcomes, thus making their weight salient in a
health context, resulted in reduced exercise (and diet) self-efficacy, which in turn was
associated with lower intentions to engage in exercise, compared with women who were not
asked to provide information about their weight (Seacat & Mickelson, 2009). A Singaporean
study of 140 higher-weight youngsters enrolled in a school-based weight-management
program that used a videogame-based exercise paradigm, found that those in an
experimentally manipulated stereotype threat condition reported more negative attitudes
toward exercise, lower exercise motivation in general, and even lower motivation to engage
in future exergaming activities after completing the running game than those in a non-threat
condition (B. J. Li, Lwin, & Jung, 2014).
Pathway 2: Weight stigma and the erasure of fat exercisers
Active fat role models
Self-efficacy may also be developed via vicarious experience – that is, seeing other people
with whom you can identify engaging in or succeeding at a task (Bandura, 1986). In general,
personal mastery experiences are a more prominent source of self-efficacy than are vicarious
experiences; however, a meta-analysis of studies of physical activity self-efficacy
development found that interventions based upon vicarious experience, including both real-
world and virtual representations of similar others, were more effective at increasing self-
efficacy than many traditionally used approaches (Ashford, Edmunds, & French, 2010).
Until very recently, positive representations of fat exercisers were conspicuous by their
absence. Images of exercisers used in magazines, gyms, and on social media usually display
mesomorphic body types with minimal visible body fat and a high degree of muscularity
(Garvin & Damson, 2008; Kenen, 1987) and content may overtly demonize fat bodies
(Dworkin & Wachs, 2009). When fat people are depicted exercising, the images are often
unflattering at best, and frequently derisory or stigmatizing (e.g., Boe, n.d.; Holloway, n.d.).
Fat people’s exercise behavior is represented as punitive in nature, and serving the sole
purpose of rendering their bodies slimmer and more acceptable (Mocarski & Bissell, 2016;
Sender & Sullivan, 2008). More commonly, representations of fat people in news,
entertainment, and social media are likely to show fat people engaged in sedentary behavior
(Ata & Thompson, 2010; Heuer, McClure, & Puhl, 2011; Puhl, Peterson, DePierre, &
Luedicke, 2013; Yoo & Kim, 2012). These divergent representations reinforce the notion that
only slim bodies are suited to exercise, and that exercise and fatness are incompatible
(Tiggemann & Zaccardo, 2015). Exercising fat bodies are non-normalized and are rendered
intrinsically dis-abled and dys-functional (Mansfield & Rich, 2013).
In addition to reinforcing negative stereotypes about fat bodies, this disparity in
representation serves to deprive fat people of potential active fat role models. Role models
function as representations of the possible; seeing somebody “like us” engaging in an activity
helps us to see ourselves in that context (Morgenroth, Ryan, & Peters, 2015). Possible selves
– being able to imagine a future self that is different from our current self – occur at the
intersection of social cognition, identity, and motivation, and can moderate both who we are
and who we become (Erikson, 2007; Markus & Nurius, 1986). To our knowledge, no
research has addressed the effects of having few positive represenations of fat exercisers.
However, evidence from the wider role-modeling literature supports a negative impact on
self-efficacy and domain identification in under-represented groups (for reviews, see
Dasgupta, 2011; Morgenroth et al., 2015). While these findings remain to be replicated in the
context of fat exercisers, anecdotal evidence suggests that viewing similar fat others simply
engaging in and enjoying active pursuits could serve to counter prevailing stereotypes and
produce a shift in how a non-active fat person views physicality as an option for their own
bodies. For example, a recent UK news story with an image of 41-year old Dawn Nisbet
crossing the finish line in her first parkrun
5
, hot, sweaty, fat, in last place, and with a look of
sheer exhilaration on her face, went viral worldwide and has encouraged hundreds of people
to take up running (Butler, 2017).
Some progress in representation of fat exercisers has occurred in recent years, thanks in large
part to social media, as plus-sized athletic role models in a range of sports and active leisure
pursuits become more well-known (Macadaeg, 2016; Torres, 2016). Yet even as a number of
positive fat role-models emerge, there is a danger that while celebrating fat bodies who
embody the ultimate physical achievement may be inspirational and challenge dominant
negative stereotypes about fat bodies, limiting representation of fat exercisers to only these
ultimate-achievers may create a superhero narrative that could alienate those whose
physicality is unlikely to ever reach such peaks (Fenson, 2017). Indeed, attainable exemplars
appear to be more effective role models than exceptional ones, who may lead to resentment,
feelings of inadequacy, and disengagement from the domain for self-preservation of one’s
self-worth (Han, Kim, Jeong, & Cohen, 2017; Hoyt & Simon, 2011; Lockwood & Kunda,
1997).
Normative erasure
The erasure of fat exercisers occurs not only through the prevailing invisibility of fat role
models, but also via the erasure of fat exercise as a concept in general. Manifestations of
societal anti-fat attitudes create structural barriers for fat exercisers. whether by ommission –
for example, assuming that fat people will not be participating in triathlons and thus
5
parkrun events are free, weekly, 5km timed runs, open to people of any ability.
neglecting to make wetsuits in larger sizes (Chastain, 2014a) or through the lack of
appropriate exercise equipment and clothing for larger bodies (Christel, O’Donnell, &
Bradley, 2016; Greenleaf, Pozolinski, & Kauffung, 2015); or by commission – such as when
fitness-wear companies choose to distance their brand from the ‘taint’ of fatness by refusing
to offer their lines in larger sizes or to market them appropriately when they are available
(Marks, 2013). These exclusions serve to label fat exercise as non-normative (S. T. Lewis &
Van Puymbroeck, 2008), reinforcing the perceived disconnect between fatness and an active
identity. Thus, the hegemonic view of active persons as necessarily thin means there is a lack
of relevant cultural signs or ergonomically-suitable activity available to fat persons interested
in activity (Wathne, 2011).
Further, under-representation of a marginalized group within a particular domain may also
increase the risk of stereotype threat (Cheryan, Plaut, Davies, & Steele, 2009; M. C. Murphy,
Steele, & Gross, 2007; Sekaquaptewa & Thompson, 2002, 2003). Thus, these multiple
erasures of fat exercise and fat exercisers increase the salience of fatness in an exercising
context (Dunlop & Schmader, 2014), which further impedes the development of active
identities among fat individuals.
Limitations, future directions, and policy implications
A number of limitations of the proposed model should be noted. First, the model is founded
on a theoretical synthesis of a number of diverse literatures and a considerable amount of
anecdotal evidence. In the first instance, therefore, it will be necessary to provide empirical
evidence of its validity in the context of physical activity behavior among higher-weight
individuals. Additionally, the majority of research on experienced weight stigma and physical
activity that underpins the development of this model has been conducted in White women,
and it is unclear whether the same mechanisms would operate in other racial or ethnic groups
or in men. Differences in the prevalence and response to weight stigma have been reported
across different racial and ethnic groups (Himmelstein, Puhl, Quinn, & Gorber, 2017;
Meadows & Calogero, 2018). Most forms of weight stigma also occur more frequently in
women, and are prevalent at lower body weights than in men (Andreyeva et al., 2008;
Hatzenbuehler, Keyes, & Hasin, 2009). To date, only one small study with similar
proportions of male and female participant has assessed whether gender influences the impact
of weight stigma on exercise behavior. An Australian study using an ecological experience
sampling technique in 46 higher-weight adults found that in women, but in men, incidents of
weight stigma were associated with reduced positive affect, which in turn predicted lower
motivation to exercise (Vartanian et al., 2016). It is therefore unclear whether gender
moderates the relationship between weight stigma and physical activity outcomes, beyond
that accounted for by the volume of stigma experiences. A consideration of the intersectional
nature of stigma and its relationship with exercise outcomes is beyond the scope of this paper
but is something that should be considered in future studies.
Finally, while the proposed model provides a framework on which future research can be
built, it is not intended to fully elucidate all intermediary mechanisms. For example,
emerging evidence identifies internalized weight stigma as a predictor of exercise enjoyment,
self-efficacy, motivation, and engagement, and a mediator of the relationship between
experienced stigma and physical activity outcomes (Mensinger & Meadows, 2017; Pearl,
Puhl, & Dovidio, 2015). Future development of the model may provide additional targets for
health promotion interventions.
If the theoretical predictions of the model are empirically substantiated, this framework has
implications for public health and education policy. Under current governmental anti-obesity
policies, PE becomes an instrument to deliver policy-mandated ‘solutions’ to the ‘obesity
epidemic’, and success defined in terms of body size rather than fitness, mastery, enjoyment,
or encouragement of physical activity outside of the school environment (Cale & Harris,
2013). Indeed, rather than improving the health of students, these policies promote exclusion
and further entrench prejudicial attitudes (J. Evans & Rich, 2011; O’Dea, 2005). In contrast,
being introduced to alternative ways of understanding fat, wellbeing, movement, and health
outside the boundaries of the ‘obesity as usual’ curriculum (Norman & Petherick, 2016, p.
94) would create more opportunities for positive exercise identities to develop among
children of all sizes. Fat pedagogy is gaining traction (Cameron & Russell, 2016), albeit
slowly, and this movement would be strengthened by availability of data supporting a size-
neutral approach as a means of achieving desired physical activity outcomes.
Within adult physical activity environments,
6
Pickett and Cunningham (2017) describe a
model for developing body-inclusive physical activity spaces, including cultural and
leadership commitment to inclusivity, inclusive language and physical environments, respect
for personal autonomy in providing participants’ agency in their fitness environments, and
importantly, the creation of community. A space in which social identity is created and
reinforced may be essential for fostering an exercise identity for individuals of all sizes
(Stevens et al., 2017). Identifying strongly with an exercise group can strengthen individual
active identities and increase identity salience (M. B. Evans, McLaren, Budziszewski, &
Gilchrist, 2018; Grant, Hogg, & Crano, 2015). While some authors have suggested that
separate spaces for fat bodies may provide less stressful and more supportive environments
for fat exercisers (e.g., Dunlop & Schmader, 2014; Kemp, 2016; cited in Meadows, 2016),
this approach presupposes reduced ability in fat exercisers, continues to create an ‘us and
them’ dichotomy between those who ‘can’ and those who ‘can’t’, othering fat bodies as non-
normative, and does nothing to address the underlying problems of a shaming, non-inclusive
6
Although most of these considerations would apply equally to physical activity environments for youngsters.
environment in more traditional spaces (Cardinal et al., 2015; D’Abundo, 2009; Watkins,
Ebbeck, & S. Levy, 2014). Progressing a shared identity based on embracing empowered
embodiment, rather than a particular body type or shared body modification goals, may be
one method of fostering inclusivity and group identity. Rather than perpetuating a divergent
‘us and them’ distinction, inclusive spaces foster the creation of a new ‘us.’
This model may also have implications for health promotion efforts. At present, it is not
uncommon for positive representations of successful fat bodies to be accused of ‘promoting
obesity’ (Chastain, 2011; Gilbert, 2015). Validation of this model would provide an evidence
base to refute such claims, and to encourage the use of images of fat individuals partaking in
pleasurable physical activity. However, it should be noted that in the current healthist,
neoliberal climate, physical activity may, for some people, present a means of negotiating a
stigmatized identity by demonstrating normative and responsible behavior (Bombak, 2015).
Thus, it is critical that efforts to promote physical activity among dis-identified fat
individuals should employ representations of fat exercisers not to obligate, or even to inspire,
but primarily to render exercising fat bodies as normative. In the longer term, more diverse
representation in this domain may also effect changes in broader societal attitudes and beliefs
about fatness (Burmeister & Carels, 2015; Dunaev, Brochu, & Markey, 2018; Hinman,
Burmeister, Kiefner, Borushok, & Carels, 2015).
Conclusion
We have argued that weight stigma in its many forms serves to impair perceived exercise
competence in fat people, and hinders the development of active fat identities. This state of
affairs essentially deprives many fat individuals of the numerous potential physical and
psychosocial benefits that may be derived from being physically active. Importantly, both
exercise self-efficacy (McAuley & Blissmer, 2000; S. L. Williams & French, 2011) and
exercise identity (Cardinal & Cardinal, 1997; Carraro & Gaudreau, 2010) are dynamic
constructs that are amenable to change. To the extent that people of any weight, size or shape
wish to engage in physical activity, they should be able to do so in a safe non-stigmatizing
space and in a manner that brings them joy and rejuvenation, not stress and shame. It is
therefore imperative to address the systemic and individual biases that result in some fat
people dis-identifying with the exercise domain entirely.
AdWeek. (2010). Gym ad’s recycled fatties joke offends again. Retrieved December 16,
2017, from http://www.adweek.com/creativity/gym-ads-recycled-fatties-joke-offends-
again-13209/
Andreyeva, T., Puhl, R. M., & Brownell, K. D. (2008). Changes in perceived weight
discrimination among Americans, 1995-1996 through 2004-2006. Obesity, 16(5), 1129–
1134. http://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2008.35
Ashford, S., Edmunds, J., & French, D. P. (2010). What is the best way to change self-
efficacy to promote lifestyle and recreational physical activity? A systematic review
with meta-analysis. British Journal of Health Psychology, 15(2), 265–288.
http://doi.org/10.1348/135910709X461752
Ata, R. N., & Thompson, J. K. (2010). Weight bias in the media: A review of recent research.
Obesity Facts, 3(1), 41–46. http://doi.org/10.1159/000276547
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W H Freeman &
Company.
Bauman, A. E., Reis, R. S., Sallis, J. F., Wells, J. C., Loos, R. J., Martin, B. W., … al., et.
(2012). Correlates of physical activity: Why are some people physically active and
others not? The Lancet, 380(9838), 258–271. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-
6736(12)60735-1
Boe, O. (n.d.). Stock image AH1C75. Retrieved January 13, 2018, from
http://www.alamy.com/stock-photo-overweight-woman-riding-a-bike-1121396.html
Bombak, A. E. (2015). Obese persons’ physical activity experiences and motivations across
weight changes: A qualitative exploratory study. BMC Public Health, 15, 1129.
http://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-015-2456-0
Burmeister, J. M., & Carels, R. A. (2015). Weight-related humor in the media: Appreciation,
distaste, and anti-fat attitudes. Stigma and Health, 1(S), 92–107.
http://doi.org/10.1037/2376-6972.1.S.92
Butler, K. (2017). The image of the last-place parkrun finisher that inspired others to get
running. Retrieved January 14, 2018, from
http://www.manchestereveningnews.co.uk/news/greater-manchester-news/image-last-
place-parkrun-finisher-12490437
Cale, L., & Harris, J. (2013). ‘Every child (of every size) matters’ in physical education!
Physical education’s role in childhood obesity. Sport, Education and Society, 18(4),
433–452. http://doi.org/10.1080/13573322.2011.601734
Cameron, E., & Russell, C. (2016). Introduction. In E. Cameron & C. Russell (Eds.), The Fat
Pedagogy Reader: Challenging Weight-Biased Oppression through Critical Education
(pp. 1–12). New York: Routledge.
Cardinal, B. J., & Cardinal, M. K. (1997). Changes in exercise behavior and exercise identity
associated with a 14-week aerobic exercise class - ProQuest. Journal of Sport Behavior,
20(4), 377–386.
Cardinal, B. J., Rogers, K. A., Kuo, B., Locklear, R. L., Comfort, K. E., & Cardinal, M. K.
(2015). Critical discourse analysis of motivational content in commercially available
exercise DVDs: Body capital on display or psychological capital being developed?
Sociology of Sport Journal, 32(4), 452–470. http://doi.org/10.1123/ssj.2014-0157
Carraro, N., & Gaudreau, P. (2010). The role of implementation planning in increasing
physical activity identification. American Journal of Health Behavior, 34(3), 298–308.
http://doi.org/10.5993/AJHB.34.3.5
Chalabaev, A., Sarrazin, P., Fontayne, P., Boiché, J., & Clément-Guillotin, C. (2013). The
influence of sex stereotypes and gender roles on participation and performance in sport
and exercise: Review and future directions. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 14(2),
136–144. http://doi.org/10.1016/J.PSYCHSPORT.2012.10.005
Chambliss, H. O., Finley, C. E., & Blair, S. N. (2004). Attitudes toward obese individuals
among exercise science students. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 36(3),
468–474. http://doi.org/10.1249/01.MSS.0000117115.94062.E4
Chastain, R. (2011). The “promoting obesity” myth. Retrieved December 24, 2017, from
https://danceswithfat.wordpress.com/2011/12/09/the-promoting-obesity-myth/
Chastain, R. (2014a). Gearing up – Plus size workout clothes. Retrieved December 28, 2017,
from https://ironfatblog.wordpress.com/2014/09/27/gearing-up-plus-size-workout-
clothes/
Chastain, R. (2014b). Why it’s not cool to think of fat or disabled people as inspiration.
Retrieved January 12, 2018, from https://archive.li/BZQNm
Cheryan, S., Plaut, V. C., Davies, P. G., & Steele, C. M. (2009). Ambient belonging: How
stereotypical cues impact gender participation in computer science. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 97(6), 1045–1060. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0016239
Christel, D. A., O’Donnell, N. H., & Bradley, L. A. (2016). Coping by crossdressing: An
exploration of exercise clothing for obese heterosexual women. Fashion and Textiles,
3(1), 11. http://doi.org/10.1186/s40691-016-0063-z
Crawford, R. (1977). You are dangerous to your health: The ideology and politics of victim
blaming. International Journal of Health Services, 7(4), 663–680.
http://doi.org/10.2190/YU77-T7B1-EN9X-G0PN
D’Abundo, M. L. (2009). Issues of health, appearance and physical activity in aerobic classes
for women. Sport, Education and Society, 14(3), 301–319.
http://doi.org/10.1080/13573320903037655
Danielsen, K. K., Sundgot-Borgen, J., & Rugseth, G. (2016). Severe Obesity and the
Ambivalence of Attending Physical Activity. Qualitative Health Research, 26(5), 685–
696. http://doi.org/10.1177/1049732315596152
Dasgupta, N. (2011). Ingroup experts and peers as social vaccines who inoculate the self-
concept: The Stereotype Inoculation Model. Psychological Inquiry, 22(4), 231–246.
http://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2011.607313
Degher, D., & Hughes, G. (1999). The adoption and management of “fat” identity. In J. Sobal
& D. Maurer (Eds.), Interpreting weight: The social management of fatness and thinness
(pp. 11–27). New York: Aldine de Gruyter.
Dimmock, J. A., Hallett, B. E., & Grove, R. J. (2009). Attitudes toward overweight
individuals among fitness center employees. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport,
80(3), 641–647. http://doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2009.10599603
Dunaev, J. L., Brochu, P. M., & Markey, C. H. (2018). Imagine that! The effect of
counterstereotypic imagined intergroup contact on weight bias. Health Psychology,
37(1), 81–88. http://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000545
Dunlop, W. L., & Schmader, T. (2014). For the overweight, is proximity to in-shape, normal-
weight exercisers a deterrent or an attractor? An examination of contextual preferences.
International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 21(1), 139–143.
http://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-012-9281-y
Dworkin, S. L., & Wachs, F. L. (2009). Body panic: Gender, health and the selling of fitness.
New York, NY: NYU Press.
Ellison, J. (2009). Not Jane Fonda: Aerobics for fat women only. In E. Rothblum & S.
Solovay (Eds.), The Fat Studies Reader. New York, NY: New York University Press.
Erikson, M. G. (2007). The meaning of the future: Toward a more specific definition of
possible selves. Review of General Psychology, 11(4), 348–358.
http://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.11.4.348
Evans, J., & Rich, E. (2011). Body policies and body pedagogies: Every child matters in
totally pedagogised schools? Journal of Education Policy, 26(3), 361–379.
http://doi.org/10.1080/02680939.2010.500399
Evans, M. B., McLaren, C., Budziszewski, R., & Gilchrist, J. (2018). When a sense of “we”
shapes the sense of “me”: Exploring how groups impact running identity and behavior.
Self and Identity, 1–20. http://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2018.1436084
Fenson, Z. (2017). I’m fat and not fit – so where do I fit in? Retrieved December 21, 2017,
from https://www.self.com/story/fat-and-not-fit
Flint, S. W., Hudson, J., Lavallee, D., James, W., Latner, J., Stunkard, A., … Austin, S.
(2015). UK adults’ implicit and explicit attitudes towards obesity: A cross-sectional
study. BMC Obesity, 2(1), 31. http://doi.org/10.1186/s40608-015-0064-2
Flint, S. W., & Reale, S. (2016). Weight stigma in frequent exercisers: Overt, demeaning and
condescending. Journal of Health Psychology, Epub July 4, 2016.
http://doi.org/10.1177/1359105316656232
Garvin, A. W., & Damson, C. (2008). The effects of idealized fitness images on anxiety,
depression and global mood states in college age males and females. Journal of Health
Psychology, 13(3), 433–437. http://doi.org/10.1177/1359105307088146
Gilbert, L. (2015). Are plus-sized athletes promoting obesity? Retrieved December 24, 2017,
from https://bodypositiveathletes.wordpress.com/2015/02/09/are-plus-sized-athletes-
promoting-obesity/
Grant, F., Hogg, M. A., & Crano, W. D. (2015). Yes, we can: Physical activity and group
identification among healthy adults. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 45(7), 383–
390. http://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12305
Greenleaf, C., Pozolinski, B., & Kauffung, A. (2015). Bias in the online active wear clothing
marketplace. In Proceedings of the 3rd Annual International Weight Stigma Conference.
Reykjavik, Iceland.
Greenleaf, C., & Weiller, K. (2005). Perceptions of youth obesity among physical educators.
Social Psychology of Education, 8(4), 407–423. http://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-005-
0662-9
Han, H., Kim, J., Jeong, C., & Cohen, G. L. (2017). Attainable and relevant moral exemplars
are more effective than extraordinary exemplars in promoting voluntary service
engagement. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 283. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00283
Hatzenbuehler, M. L., Keyes, K. M., & Hasin, D. S. (2009). Associations between perceived
weight discrimination and the prevalence of psychiatric disorders in the general
population. Obesity, 17(11), 2033–2039. http://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2009.131
Heuer, C. A., McClure, K. J., & Puhl, R. M. (2011). Obesity stigma in online news: A visual
content analysis. Journal of Health Communication, 16(9), 976–987.
http://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2011.561915
Himmelstein, M. S., Puhl, R. M., Quinn, D. M., & Gorber, B. (2017). Intersectionality: An
understudied framework for addressing weight stigma. American Journal of Preventive
Medicine, 53(4), 421–431. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.amepre.2017.04.003
Hinman, N. G., Burmeister, J. M., Kiefner, A. E., Borushok, J., & Carels, R. A. (2015).
Stereotypical portrayals of obesity and the expression of impicit weight bias. Body
Image, 12, 32–35. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.09.002
Holloway, Z. (n.d.). Stock photo 170123584. Retrieved January 13, 2013, from
https://www.gettyimages.co.uk/detail/photo/aquaerobics-class-or-water-aerobics-class-
high-res-stock-photography/170123584
Hoyt, C. L., & Simon, S. (2011). Female leaders: Injurious or inspiring role models for
women? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 35(1), 143–157.
http://doi.org/10.1177/0361684310385216
Jackson, S. E., & Steptoe, A. (2017). Association between perceived weight discrimination
and physical activity: A population-based study among English middle-aged and older
adults. BMJ Open, 7(3), e014592. http://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2016-014592
Kenen, R. H. (1987). Double messages, double images: Physical fitness, self-concepts, and
women’s exercise classes. Journal of Physical Education, Recreation & Dance, 58(6),
74–79. http://doi.org/10.1080/07303084.1987.10609581
Kwan, S. (2010). Navigating public spaces: Gender, race, and body privilege in everyday life.
Feminist Formations, 22(2), 144–166. http://doi.org/10.1353/ff.2010.0002
Lewis, S. T., & Van Puymbroeck, M. (2008). Obesity-stigma as a multifaceted constraint to
leisure. Journal of Leisure Research, 40(4), 574–588.
Lewis, S., Thomas, S. L., Blood, R. W., Castle, D. J., Hyde, J., & Komesaroff, P. A. (2011).
How do obese individuals perceive and respond to the different types of obesity stigma
that they encounter in their daily lives? A qualitative study. Social Science & Medicine,
73(9), 1349–1356. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2011.08.021
Li, B. J., Lwin, M. O., & Jung, Y. (2014). Wii, myself, and size: The influence of Proteus
effect and stereotype threat on overweight children’s exercise motivation and behavior
in exergames. Games for Health Journal, 3(1), 40–48.
http://doi.org/10.1089/g4h.2013.0081
Li, W., & Rukavina, P. (2012). The nature, occurring contexts, and psychological
implications of weight-related teasing in urban physical education programs. Research
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 83(2), 308–317.
Lillis, J., Thomas, J. G., Levin, M. E., & Wing, R. R. (2017). Self-stigma and weight loss:
The impact of fear of being stigmatized. Journal of Health Psychology,
135910531773910. http://doi.org/10.1177/1359105317739101
Link, B. G., Wells, J., Phelan, J. C., & Yang, L. (2015). Understanding the importance of
“symbolic interaction stigma”: How expectations about the reactions of others adds to
the burden of mental illness stigma. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 38(2), 117–124.
http://doi.org/10.1037/prj0000142
Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1997). Superstars and me: Predicting the impact of role models
on the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(1), 91–103.
http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.1.91
Macadaeg, K. (2016). The greatest fat athletes of all time. Retrieved December 24, 2017,
from http://www.complex.com/sports/greatest-fat-athletes-all-time/pablo-sandoval
Maïano, C., Lepage, G., Aimé, A., Bayard, C., Dansereau-Trahan, É., Granger, L., … Morin,
A. J. S. (2018). Perceived weight-related victimization and physical activity outcomes
among adolescents with overweight and obesity: Indirect role of perceived physical
abilities and fear of enacted stigma. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 34, 70–78.
http://doi.org/10.1016/J.PSYCHSPORT.2017.08.007
Maier, S. F., & Seligman, M. E. (1976). Learned helplessness: Theory and evidence. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: General, 105(1), 3–46. http://doi.org/10.1037/0096-
3445.105.1.3
Mansfield, L., & Rich, E. (2013). Public health pedagogy, border crossings and physical
activity at every size. Critical Public Health, 23(3), 356–370.
http://doi.org/10.1080/09581596.2013.783685
Marks, H. (2013). Fat shaming or smart marketing? Why Lululemon (and others) don’t sell
plus sizes. Retrieved December 28, 2017, from http://bust.com/general/10300-fat-
shaming-or-smart-marketing-why-lululemon-and-others-dont-sell-plus-sizes.html
Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 35(2), 63–78. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-
3514.35.2.63
Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41(9), 954–969.
http://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.41.9.954
McAuley, E., & Blissmer, B. (2000). Self-efficacy determinants and consequences of
physical activity. Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, 28(2), 85–88.
McLeroy, K. R., Bibeau, D., Steckler, A., & Glanz, K. (1988). An ecological perspective on
health promotion programs. Health Education Quarterly, 15(4), 351–377.
http://doi.org/10.1177/109019818801500401
Meadows, A. (2016). Separate PE classes for fat kids is not the answer. Retrieved from
http://www.huffingtonpost.co.uk/angela-meadows/separate-pe-classes-for-fat-
kids_b_9506028.html
Meadows, A., & Calogero, R. M. (2018). Studies on weight stigma and body image in
higher-weight individuals. In M. Cuzzolaro & S. Fassino (Eds.), Body image, eating,
and weight: A guide to assessment, treatment, and prevention. Cham: Springer
International Publishing. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-90817-5
Mensinger, J. L., & Meadows, A. (2017). Internalized weight stigma mediates and moderates
physical activity outcomes during a healthy living program for women with high body
mass index. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 30, 64–72.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2017.01.010
Mocarski, R., & Bissell, K. (2016). Edutainment’s impact on health promotion: Viewing The
Biggest Loser through the social cognitive theory. Health Promotion Practice, 17(1),
107–115. http://doi.org/10.1177/1524839915613026
Monaghan, L. F. (2008). Men, physical activity, and the obesity discourse: Critical
understandings from a qualitative study. Sociology of Sport Journal, 25(1), 97–129.
http://doi.org/10.1123/ssj.25.1.97
Morgenroth, T., Ryan, M. K., & Peters, K. (2015). The motivational theory of role modeling:
How role models influence role aspirants’ goals. Review of General Psychology, 19(4),
465–483. http://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000059
Murphy, M. C., Steele, C. M., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Signaling threat: How situational cues
affect women in math, science, and engineering settings. Psychological Science, 18(10),
879–885. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01995.x
Murphy, S. (2017). These disturbing, shaming advertisements prove that gyms get body
positivity all wrong. Retrieved December 16, 2017, from
https://hellogiggles.com/lifestyle/body-shaming-gym-advertisements/
Norman, M., & Petherick, L. (2016). The enemy within: Teaching “hard knowledge” about
“soft bodies” in a kinesiology faculty. In E. Cameron & C. Russell (Eds.), The Fat
Pedagogy Reader: Challenging Weight-Biased Oppression through Critical Education
(pp. 91–100). New York: Peter Lang.
O’Dea, J. A. (2005). Prevention of child obesity: “First, do no harm.” Health Education
Research, 20(2), 259–265. http://doi.org/10.1093/her/cyg116
Oyserman, D. (2015). Identity-based motivation. In Emerging Trends in the Social and
Behavioral Sciences (pp. 1–11). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
http://doi.org/10.1002/9781118900772.etrds0171
Packer, J. (1989). The role of stigmatization in fat people’s avoidance of physical exercise.
Women & Therapy, 8(3), 49–63. http://doi.org/10.1300/J015V08N03_07
Pearl, R. L., Puhl, R. M., & Dovidio, J. F. (2015). Differential effects of weight bias
experiences and internalization on exercise among women with overweight and obesity.
Journal of Health Psychology, 20(12), 1626–1632.
http://doi.org/10.1177/1359105313520338
Peterson, J. L., Puhl, R. M., & Luedicke, J. (2012). An experimental assessment of physical
educators’ expectations and attitudes: The importance of student weight and gender.
Journal of School Health, 82(9), 432–440. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1746-
1561.2012.00719.x
Pickett, A. C., & Cunningham, G. B. (2017). Physical activity for every body: A model for
managing weight stigma and creating body-inclusive spaces. Quest, 69(1), 19–36.
http://doi.org/10.1080/00336297.2016.1145129
Puhl, R. M., & Brownell, K. D. (2006). Confronting and coping with weight stigma: An
investigation of overweight and obese adults. Obesity, 14(10), 1802–1815.
http://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2006.208
Puhl, R. M., & King, K. M. (2013). Weight discrimination and bullying. Best Practice &
Research. Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 27(2), 117–127.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.beem.2012.12.002
Puhl, R. M., Peterson, J. L., DePierre, J. A., & Luedicke, J. (2013). Headless, hungry, and
unhealthy: A video content analysis of obese persons portrayed in online news. Journal
of Health Communication, 18(6), 686–702.
http://doi.org/10.1080/10810730.2012.743631
Puhl, R. M., Schwartz, M. B., & Brownell, K. D. (2005). Impact of perceived consensus on
stereotypes about obese people: A new approach for reducing bias. Health Psychology,
24(5), 517–525. http://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.24.5.517
Rauscher, L., Kauer, K., & Wilson, B. D. M. (2013). The healthy body paradox. Gender &
Society, 27(2), 208–230. http://doi.org/10.1177/0891243212472054
Rhodes, R. E., Kaushal, N., & Quinlan, A. (2016). Is physical activity a part of who I am? A
review and meta-analysis of identity, schema and physical activity. Health Psychology
Review, 10(2), 204–225. http://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2016.1143334
Rice, C. (2007). Becoming “the fat girl”: Acquisition of an unfit identity. Women’s Studies
International Forum, 30(2), 158–174. http://doi.org/10.1016/J.WSIF.2007.01.001
Robertson, N., & Vohora, R. (2008). Fitness vs. fatness: Implicit bias towards obesity among
fitness professionals and regular exercisers. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 9(4),
547–557. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2007.06.002
Salvy, S. J., de la Haye, K., Bowker, J. C., & Hermans, R. C. J. (2012). Influence of peers
and friends on children’s and adolescents’ eating and activity behaviors. Physiology &
Behavior, 106(3), 369–378. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.03.022
Schlossberg, M. (2016). A major gym chain is under fire after an actress called out its fat-
shaming ads. Retrieved December 16, 2017, from http://uk.businessinsider.com/golds-
gym-fat-shaming-ad-causes-controversy-2016-8?r=US&IR=T
Schmalz, D. L. (2010). “I feel fat”: Weight-related stigma, body esteem, and BMI as
predictors of perceived competence in physical activity. Obesity Facts, 3(1), 15–21.
http://doi.org/10.1159/000273210
Schvey, N. A., Sbrocco, T., Bakalar, J. L., Ress, R., Barmine, M., Gorlick, J., … Tanofsky-
Kraff, M. (2016). The experience of weight stigma among gym members with
overweight and obesity. Stigma and Health, 2(4), 292–306.
http://doi.org/10.1037/sah0000062
Seacat, J. D., & Mickelson, K. D. (2009). Stereotype threat and the exercise/dietary health
intentions of overweight women. Journal of Health Psychology, 14(4), 556–567.
http://doi.org/10.1177/1359105309103575
Sekaquaptewa, D., & Thompson, M. (2002). The differential effects of solo status on
members of high- and low-status groups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
28(5), 694–707. http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167202288013
Sekaquaptewa, D., & Thompson, M. (2003). Solo status, stereotype threat, and performance
expectancies: Their effects on women’s performance. Journal of Experimental Social
Psychology, 39(1), 68–74. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00508-5
Sender, K., & Sullivan, M. (2008). Epidemics of will, failures of self-esteem: Responding to
fat bodies in The Biggest Loser and What Not to Wear. Continuum: Journal of Media &
Cultural Studies, 22(4), 573–584. http://doi.org/10.1080/10304310802190046
Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). Contending with group image: The
psychology of stereotype and social identity threat. Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology, 34, 379–440. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(02)80009-0
Stets, J. E., & Burke, P. J. (2000). Identity theory and social identity theory. Social
Psychology Quarterly, 63(3), 224–237. http://doi.org/10.2307/2695870
Stevens, M., Rees, T., Coffee, P., Steffens, N. K., Haslam, S. A., & Polman, R. (2017). A
social identity approach to understanding and promoting physical activity. Sports
Medicine, 47(10), 1911–1918. http://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-017-0720-4
Stryker, S., & Burke, P. J. (2000). The past, present, and future of an identity theory. Social
Psychology Quarterly, 63(4), 284–297. http://doi.org/10.2307/2695840
Sykes, H., & McPhail, D. (2008). Unbearable lessons: Contesting fat phobia in physical
education. Sociology of Sport Journal, 25(1), 66–96. http://doi.org/10.1123/ssj.25.1.66
Tiggemann, M., & Zaccardo, M. (2015). “Exercise to be fit, not skinny”: The effect of
fitspiration imagery on women’s body image. Body Image, 15, 61–67.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.06.003
Torres, J. (2016). These 10 kickass athletes are making body-shamers and doubters eat their
words. Retrieved December 24, 2017, from http://www.revelist.com/wellness/plus-size-
athletes/2669/
Trout, J., & Graber, K. C. (2009). Perceptions of overweight students concerning their
experiences in physical education. Journal of Teaching in Physical Education, 28(3),
272–292. http://doi.org/10.1123/jtpe.28.3.272
Vartanian, L. R., & Novak, S. A. (2011). Internalized societal attitudes moderate the impact
of weight stigma on avoidance of exercise. Obesity, 19(4), 757–762.
http://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2010.234
Vartanian, L. R., Pinkus, R., & Smyth, J. M. (2014). The phenomenology of weight stigma in
everyday life. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 3(3), 196–202.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2014.01.003
Vartanian, L. R., Pinkus, R. T., & Smyth, J. M. (2016). Experiences of weight stigma in
everyday life: Implications for health motivation. Stigma and Health.
http://doi.org/10.1037/sah0000077
Vartanian, L. R., & Shaprow, J. G. (2008). Effects of weight stigma on exercise motivation
and behavior: A preliminary investigation among college-aged females. Journal of
Health Psychology, 13(1), 131–138. http://doi.org/10.1177/1359105307084318
Wathne, K. (2011). Movement of large bodies impaired: The double burden of obesity:
Somatic and semiotic issues. Sport, Education and Society, 16(4), 415–429.
http://doi.org/10.1080/13573322.2011.589640
Watkins, P. Lou, Ebbeck, V., & S. Levy, S. (2014). Overcoming weight bias: Promoting
physical activity and psychosocial health. Ethnicity and Inequalities in Health and
Social Care, 7(4), 187–197. http://doi.org/10.1108/EIHSC-11-2013-0043
Williams, D. M., & Rhodes, R. E. (2016). The confounded self-efficacy construct:
Conceptual analysis and recommendations for future research. Health Psychology
Review, 10(2), 113–128. http://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2014.941998
Williams, S. L., & French, D. P. (2011). What are the most effective intervention techniques
for changing physical activity self-efficacy and physical activity behaviour—and are
they the same? Health Education Research, 26(2), 308–322.
http://doi.org/10.1093/her/cyr005
Yoo, J. H., & Kim, J. (2012). Obesity in the new media: A content analysis of obesity videos
on YouTube. Health Communication, 27(1), 86–97.
http://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2011.569003
Zimdars, M. M. (2015). Weight watching: Television, fatness, and the obesity epidemic.
University of Iowa.