Available via license: CC BY-ND 4.0
Content may be subject to copyright.
1
Title: Hunger for Knowledge: How the Irresistible Lure of Curiosity is 1 Generated in the Brain 2
3
Authors: Johnny King L Lau1,2*, Hiroki Ozono3, Kei Kuratomi4, Asuka Komiya5, Kou 4 Murayama1,2* 5
6
Affiliations: 7
1School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Reading, UK. 8
2Centre for Integrative Neuroscience and Neurodynamics, University of Reading, Reading, UK. 9
3Faculty of Law, Economics and Humanities, Kagoshima University, Kagoshima, Japan. 10
4Faculty of Psychology, Aichi Shukutoku University, Nagakute-city, Japan. 11
5Graduate School of Integrated Arts and Sciences, Hiroshima University, Higashi-Hiroshima 12 city, Japan. 13
*Correspondence to: johnny.lau@reading.ac.uk (Johnny Lau), k.murayama@reading.ac.uk (Kou 14 Murayama) 15
16
17
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
2
Introductory Paragraph 1
Curiosity is often portrayed as a desirable feature of human faculty. For example, a meta-2
analysis revealed that curiosity predicts academic performance above and beyond intelligence1, 3
corroborating findings that curiosity supported long-term consolidation of learning2,3. However, 4
curiosity may come at a cost of strong seductive power that sometimes puts people in a harmful 5
situation. Here, with a set of three behavioural and two neuroimaging experiments including 6
novel stimuli that strongly trigger curiosity (i.e. magic tricks), we examined the psychological 7
and neural mechanisms underlying the irresistible lure of curiosity. We consistently 8
demonstrated that across different samples people were indeed willing to gamble to expose 9
themselves to physical risks (i.e. electric shocks) in order to satisfy their curiosity for trivial 10
knowledge that carries no apparent instrumental values. Also, underlying this seductive power of 11
curiosity is its incentive salience properties, which share common neural mechanisms with 12
extrinsic incentives (i.e. hunger for foods). In particular, the two independent fMRI experiments 13
using different kinds of curiosity-stimulating stimuli found replicable results that acceptance 14
(compared to rejection) of curiosity/incentive-driven gambles was accompanied with an 15
enhanced activity in the striatum. 16
17
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
3
Curiosity is a fundamental part of human motivation that supports an enormous variety of 1
human intellectual behaviours, ranging from early learning in children to scientific discovery 4–6. 2
The critical importance of curiosity in human intellectual behaviour is succinctly expressed in 3
Albert Einstein’s famous quote “I have no special talent. I am only passionately curious.” 4
Moreover, empirical literature has revealed a number of positive outcomes associated with 5
curiosity over the life span 1,7–9. 6
However, in both historic and modern literature, the positive portrayal of curiosity is 7
often compromised by its inherent negative aspect: strong seductive power 10. In Greek 8
mythology, for example, after losing his beloved Eurydice to the underworld, Orpheus 9
convinced the gods to let him take her back to the world of the living on the condition that he 10
would not look back until they had returned. Orpheus could not help but looked back; he 11
succumbed to curiosity and lost Eurydice. This theme, which appears repeatedly in classic 12
ancient anecdotes (e.g., Pandora, Psyche, Eve), illustrates the irresistible power of curiosity, 13
which biases our decision-making despite the knowledge of consequential negative outcomes. 14
Previous literature has indicated that both animals 11–13 and humans 3,14–16 are willing to pay 15
small amounts of costs to satisfy curiosity for the knowledge about a future reward that cannot be 16
changed. For example, people are willing to sacrifice parts of future monetary reward in 17
exchange for immediate information about the outcome of a monetary lottery, even though that 18
information cannot be used to alter lottery outcomes14. However, little research has directly 19
addressed the nature of the strong lure of curiosity for non-instrumental trivial facts, which may 20
overcome even significant risks. 21
Herein, we report a set of behavioural and neuroimaging experiments, showing that i) 22
people are willing to expose themselves to physical risks (i.e. electric shocks) to satisfy their 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
4
curiosity for trivial, non-consequential knowledge (i.e. magic tricks, trivia), and ii) the seductive 1
power of curiosity shares the same motivational processes as extrinsic incentives (i.e. foods). 2
In the initial behavioural experiment, participants (N=17) were presented with short (8 to 3
46s; mean = 22.22s; median = 20s) video clips of magic tricks performed by professional 4
magicians (filmed specifically for this study; Supplementary videos 1-2), followed by a wheel of 5
fortune visually depicting the probability of winning (versus losing) a lottery in each trial (Fig. 6
1a). Participants were then asked to make a decision on whether they would gamble to take the 7
lottery or not. They were told that, if they accept the lottery and win, they can see the secret 8
behind the magic trick after the experiment (with a certain probability; 16.7% - 83.3%; see 9
Methods). The experiment also included food trials, in which participants were presented with 10
food pictures, and they were told that they can eat the food after the experiment if they accept the 11
lottery and win. Critically, for both trials, if participants accepted the lottery and lost, they were 12
told that they would receive electric shock after the experiment. The magnitude of electric shock 13
was calibrated and demonstrated to participants before the experiment. Electric shock was not 14
delivered during the experimental task, as pilot data indicated that expectation of electric shock 15
has more sustained effects on participants’ fear perception than receiving actual electric shock 16
multiple times. 17
[INSERT FIGURE 1] 18
Generalised linear mixed-effects modelling showed that participants were more likely to 19
reject the lottery as the presented outcome probability of receiving electric shock increases 20
[Exp( ) = 2.30 (for curiosity condition) & 2.91 (for food condition), Ps < 0.01]. This suggests 21
that electric shock worked effectively as an aversive stimulus. Importantly, curiosity ratings of 22
magic tricks and desirability ratings of food items both positively predicted the “accept” decision 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
5
above and beyond the outcome probability [Exp ( ) = 2.32 (for magic curiosity) & 2.94 (for food 1
desirability), Ps < 0.01]. In other words, curiosity for non-consequential knowledge (i.e. magic 2
solution), as well as incentive value of foods, can bias decision-making entailing physical risks 3
(Supplementary Fig. 1 and Supplementary Table 1; also see Fig. 1b for results integrated with 4
following fMRI experiments). 5
How does curiosity bias our risky decision-making? There are two possibilities. The first 6
possibility is that curiosity distorts the perception of probability of risks (i.e. when people are 7
curious and in a positive mood, they overestimate the probability of winning the lottery; 17). In a 8
follow-up behavioural experiment, participants (N=29) rated their subjective chance of winning 9
when presented with a lottery, after seeing a curiosity/hunger evoking stimulus (see 10
Supplementary Methods). However, curiosity rating for magic tricks [Exp ( ) = 1.03, P = 0.13] 11
or desirability rating for foods [Exp ( ) = 1.05, P = 0.17] did not predict the participant’s 12
subjective probability of winning [Bayes Factor (BF01) for Magic = 0.00524, BF01 for Food = 13
0.00658; very strongly supportive of the null hypothesis]. 14
Another possibility is that curiosity triggers incentive salience for unknown information18. 15
Expectation of reward often increases incentive salience (or “wanting”) and this likely promotes 16
irrational and risky behaviour through modulating the neurobiological processes in the reward 17
system 19–21. Similar psychological mechanisms may underlie the seductive power of curiosity. 18
Indeed, previous functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) research has revealed that the 19
state of curiosity (e.g. when participants are waiting for the answer to a trivia question) activates 20
the striatum2,3 --- a key brain area for processing incentive salience 22. To test this hypothesis, we 21
conducted a neuroimaging experiment with fMRI using a similar experimental paradigm as in 22
the initial behavioural investigation. If the seductive power of curiosity is driven by increased 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
6
incentive salience, the striatum will be activated when participants accept the lottery23,24. A 2 1
(stimulus type: magic tricks vs. foods) X 2 (decision: accept vs. reject) analysis of variance 2
(ANOVA) (N=31) on a priori anatomical striatum region of interest (ROI) showed that the 3
striatum was indeed activated when participants accepted the lottery in comparison to when they 4
rejected it, regardless of the stimulus type (PFWE < 0.05; Fig. 2a-c). This effect was robustly 5
observed even after controlling for the “win” probabilities (PFWE < 0.05; Supplementary Fig. 2) 6
in the ROI analysis. No significant interaction between stimulus type and decision was observed 7
within the ROI. A whole brain analysis also indicated that the striatum showed extensive 8
activation for accepted trials in comparison to rejected trials among all brain areas 9
(Supplementary Tables 2-3). 10
[INSERT FIGURE 2] 11
We also examined whether the observed striatal activation mediates the relationship 12
between curiosity/food desirability ratings and the acceptance of the lottery on a trial-by-trial 13
basis. A multilevel mediation analysis showed that this mediation effect is statistically significant 14
(Pmediation < 0.05; Fig. 2d), further supporting the idea that the curiosity or desire for food induced 15
by the stimuli prompted people to risk of physical pain by recruiting a common brain network. 16
To further explore the mechanisms underlying curiosity-driven decision that overcomes 17
potential physical risks, we conducted a beta-series correlation functional connectivity analysis 18
using the striatum as the seed [a spherical ROI with a 6 mm-radius was created around the peak 19
voxel of the striatum shown to be activated for the accept (vs. reject) decision in the main fMRI 20
analysis]. This exploratory analysis found weaker connectivity between the left sensorimotor 21
cortex and left striatum, suggesting a decoupling effect (i.e. weaker functional correlation) 22
associated with accepting the lottery (Fig. 3, Supplementary Table 4); however statistical 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
7
analysis only revealed marginal significance (PFWE = 0.056) after cluster-level familywise error 1
correction. The sensorimotor cortex has been implicated in anticipation of aversive signals 2
including pain25,26. Our results may suggest that, when the participant’s curiosity to see the secret 3
of a magic trick overcame the risk of receiving electric shock, there tended to be a dissociation of 4
incentive salience and expected fear of physical shock in the brain. 5
[INSERT FIGURE 3] 6
Magic tricks evoke people’s curiosity through sensory (i.e. visual) input. This type of 7
curiosity can be described as “perceptual curiosity”27. To examine the robustness of our findings 8
and investigate whether they can be generalised to the type of curiosity that is evoked by 9
conceptual ambiguities or uncertainties (called “epistemic curiosity” 27), we conducted a 10
replication fMRI experiment, in which we presented trivia questions (e.g., “What is the only food 11
that never spoils?”) selected from a standardised database 28 (instead of magic tricks) to stimulate 12
curiosity within an independent sample. A 2 (stimulus type: trivia questions vs. foods) X 2 13
(decision: accept vs. reject) ANOVA (N=30) was performed to compare the average beta 14
contrast values within the striatum ROI, created based on the activation map in the first fMRI 15
experiment. In the replication fMRI experiment, striatum was again more activated, regardless of 16
the stimulus type, when participants accepted the lottery to satisfy their curiosity in comparison 17
to when they rejected it (P < 0.05, see Supplementary Fig. 3a). Moreover, the mediation effect 18
was observed (P < 0.05, Supplementary Fig. 3b), and the sensorimotor cortex (based on a 19
functional mask created from the connectivity analysis in the first fMRI experiment) also 20
exhibited functional decoupling with striatal activation (P < 0.05, Supplementary Fig. 4). Finally, 21
to explore other brain areas that are associated with a participant’s decision-making process, a 22
whole-brain conjunction analysis was performed to examine the overlapping brain areas between 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
8
the two fMRI samples for the “accept” (vs. “reject”) decision. In additional to the caudate 1
nucleus, the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex, premotor cortex and thalamus (P < 0.00001, uncor; 2
Fig. 4 & Supplementary Table 2) all exhibited involvement in the decision-making process. 3
[FIGURE 4] 4
Incentive salience is a motivational “hot” feeling, potentially prompting people toward 5
impulsive behaviours29. The effect of incentive salience would be stronger when people make 6
immediate decisions involving personal dilemmas in contrast to when they make predictions 7
about the behaviours of others with whom they are emotionally detached30. To test this 8
possibility, we conducted another experiment in which participants made a prediction about the 9
decision of some hypothetical participants who were depicted to be the subjects to receive actual 10
experimental treatments. In this “prediction” experiment (N = 67), ratings of curiosity and food 11
desirability were still positively associated with the (predicted) decision to accept lottery, but the 12
effect of ratings was smaller when compared with the other experiments requiring actual 13
decision-making (P<0.05, for the interaction between Prediction and Rating; see Methods and 14
Supplementary Table 6). These results provide another compelling evidence for the role of 15
incentive salience in curiosity-driven risky decision. 16
The current study showed that curiosity biases our decision-making by recruiting the 17
same incentive motivation process as extrinsic rewards (e.g. foods). These findings suggest that 18
the seemingly irrational curiosity-based choices that people make can be explained by standard 19
decision-making models, by assuming that knowledge acquisition has inherent (yet rather 20
immediate) rewarding value per se 6,31. A question remains regarding why incentive salience 21
triggered by curiosity is so strong that it drives people into even self-harming behaviour 32–35. 22
Also, previous studies have indicated that curiosity is inherently related to the feeling of anxiety 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
9
and fear10, but our neuroimaging studies did not find clear pattern of activations in the 1
anxiety/fear related areas36,37. A cooperative effort from multiple perspectives such as 2
development6, personality38, computation39, and evolution40, would be required to 3
comprehensively understand the elusive concept of curiosity. 4
5
6
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
10
Methods 1
Participants 2
The study was approved by the research ethics committees of the University of Reading, UK 3
(ethics approval number: UREC 16/36). Apart from the follow-up ‘prediction’ experiment, 4
participants were recruited via mailing lists and SONA, a research participation scheme, at the 5
University. Participants provided informed consent, completed and passed a health and safety 6
screening for receiving electric stimulation to confirm that i) they did not have a cardiac 7
pacemaker (or any other devices that can be affected by electric stimulation); ii) they were free 8
from neurophysiological symptoms or conditions including peripheral vascular disease, 9
vasculitis cryoglobulinemia, lupus, tingling or numbness in hands and/or feet. To maximise 10
participants’ motivation for food consumption during the experiment, they were required not to 11
eat or drink anything (apart from water) within 2 hours prior to the testing session. 12
13
Depending on personal preference, participants were compensated either with course credits at a 14
fixed rate of 1 unit per hour or cash payments for their participation. For behavioural 15
experiments, a fixed rate of £7 per hour was given, while for fMRI experiments the 16
compensation for participation was fixed at £10 per hour. In addition, participants were informed 17
that they may receive extra rewards according to their task performance in the experiment. Each 18
participant took part in only one version of the experiment. 19
20
Initial behavioural experiment: Seventeen healthy participants (5 males) with an average age of 21
23.11 years old (sd= ±5.30) participated. 22
23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
11
fMRI experiment 1 (Curiosity about Magic Tricks): Thirty-two healthy individuals were 1
recruited. One participant accepted every single gamble (100% acceptance on all trials) in the 2
decision-making task and was thus excluded prior to data analysis. The remaining 31 participants 3
(5 males) were on average 19.53 years old (sd= ±1.59) and all right-handed. 4
5
fMRI experiment 2 (Curiosity about Trivia): Thirty-two healthy adults took part in the 6
experiment. One of them accepted every single gamble (100% acceptance on all trials). Another 7
participant had pronounced head movements during MRI scan (> 3mm displacement in a motion 8
direction, see Supplementary Methods for further details). Both were excluded prior to data 9
analysis. A sample of the remaining 30 individuals (6 males) had a mean age of 20.3 years old 10
(sd= ±2.49) and all were right-handed. 11
12
Follow-up behavioural experiment on estimating the subjective outcome probability (FU1): 13
Twenty-nine healthy participants (7 males) with a mean age of 20.17 years old (sd=±3.96) 14
participated in this experiment. 15
16
Follow-up ‘prediction’ experiment (FU2): Sixty-nine individuals were recruited via Prolific 17
Academic (www.prolific.ac; a crowdsourcing platform for research participation) to complete 18
the experiment online and were paid at a fixed rate of £5.2 per hour. Two participants reported 19
that they had taken part in a similar experiment before and were thus excluded prior to data 20
analysis. The remaining 67 participants (42 males) were 27 years old (sd=±5.00) on average. 21
22
23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
12
Materials 1
Food Images: All pictures were colour photographs selected from different sources on the 2
Internet. They had a resolution of at least 512 X 384 pixels and were edited so that the single 3
food item was presented in the centre against a white background using GNU Image 4
Manipulation Program (GIMP) 2, a free open-source graphics editor. A selection of food was 5
chosen based on the following criteria: first, the items would be familiar to participants to avoid 6
hesitation due to uncertainty during decision-making; second, there was a wide variety of items 7
including fruits/vegetables, sweets, snacks and savoury bites (e.g. grapes, salad, chocolate, nuts, 8
sausage roll, etc.), which might accommodate different individual preferences and tastes for food 9
and elicit different levels of desirability in participants. 10
11
Magic trick videos: Magic tricks, performed by three professional magicians including a 12
champion of an international magic competition, were recorded in a TV studio with a 13
professional cameraman using high resolution video cameras. All videos were then edited using 14
Adobe® Premiere Pro CC® (2015) software to a similar monotonic (dark) background, size (720 15
x 404 pixels) and viewing focus. The videos were muted (and subtitles were added in a few 16
videos, when needed). The face of the magician was hidden to avoid potential distraction due to 17
appearance and any facial expressions (see Supplementary videos 1-2). Out of the pool of 166 18
video clips, we selected magic tricks to be used in the current study by ensuring that they (1) 19
include a range of different features (such as the use of cards, sleight of hands, optical illusions) 20
and (2) likely elicit curiosity to different extents (curiosity ratings were obtained in a different 21
pilot study). The initial behavioural experiment included 45 food items and 45 magic trick 22
videos. These videos ranged between 8 – 46 seconds long (mean=22.22; median=20). Due to the 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
13
precious scanning time allowed, the first fMRI experiment (fMRI experiment 1) used only a 1
subset of 36 food items and 36 magic tricks from the initial experiment (length of the videos: 2
range= 8-46 sec; mean=20.61; median=18.5). Then, the two follow-up experiments (FU1 & 3
FU2) made use of the same set of stimuli as fMRI experiment 1. 4
5
Trivia questions: Sixty trivia questions were selected from a publicly-available 244-item 6
database, created by our research group 28 (http://koumurayama.com/resources.php). The 7
selected questions were not obviously culture or age specific. Also, for all questions, the answers 8
were likely to be unknown to the majority of participants. The selection of items corresponded to 9
different trivia categories that might elicit curiosity among individuals to different extents, 10
including art/music, history/geography, movies/TV, nature/animals, science, space, sports, food, 11
as well as other miscellaneous facts. To ensure within-person variability in curiosity evoked 12
across the experiment, half of the chosen questions were picked among those with high mean 13
curiosity scores in the database and the other half among those with low mean scores (rated by a 14
sample of 1498 respondents from a separate online study; for more information, refer to 28). The 15
second fMRI experiment (fMRI experiment 2) used 60 trivia questions, as well as 60 food items. 16
On average, the chosen questions contained 10 (ranging between 6-16) words. 17
18
Procedure 19
Curiosity-driven Decision-Making Task 20
The main task of the study followed similar procedures (although there were slight modifications 21
for the two fMRI and the two follow-up experiments, which are detailed in Supplementary 22
Methods). In brief, each trial started with a central fixation cross and then a brief letter cue (‘M’ 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
14
signified ‘magic trick’; ‘F’ signified ‘food’) to prepare participants for the kind of stimulus they 1
were about to see. This was followed by the stimulus (either a video of a magic trick in the 2
curiosity condition or an image of a food item). Participants then gave a rating to indicate their 3
level of curiosity about the magic trick or level of desirability of the food, on a 7-point scale 4
(1=not at all, 7=very much). In the curiosity trials, they also had to report how confident they 5
were to know about the solution to a magic trick, using also a 7-point scale (1=not at all, 7=very 6
much). (However, because rated confidence was not associated with a participant’s decision and 7
the inclusion of this measure did not change any main results, to make straightforward 8
comparisons between curiosity and food trials, it was not included in the reported analysis here.) 9
After rating the stimulus, participants were then presented with a wheel of fortune (WoF) 10
representing a lottery which visualised their probability of winning/losing in that trial, and were 11
asked to decide whether to gamble or not. Participants were instructed that if they accepted the 12
lottery and won, they would receive a token that might allow them to see the secret behind the 13
magic trick after the experiment. If they gambled and lost, they would get a token that might 14
increase the amount of shock they were to experience at the end of the experiment. Participants 15
could also opt to reject the lottery. At the end of each trial, the outcome of the lottery was 16
presented. Participants were informed that, as a general rule, the more ‘win’ tokens they 17
collected, the more likely they would get more rewards. Similarly, the more ‘loss’ tokens they 18
got, the more likely they would receive more electric shock. 19
20
There were 5 versions of WoF, each displaying a different combination of the probabilities of 21
winning and losing a gamble: i) 16.7% (1/6) win vs. 83.3% (5/6) loss; ii) 33.3% (2/6) win vs. 22
66.7% (4/6) loss; iii) 50% (3/6) win vs. 50% (3/6) loss; iv) 66.7% (4/6) win vs. 33.3% (2/6) loss; 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
15
v) 83.3% (5/6) win vs. 16.7% (1/6) loss. In the experiments, participants were never shown the 1
actual percentages but the relative win-to-loss contrast of probabilities was illustrated visually by 2
the relative sizes of the constituent slices on the WoF (Fig. 1a). To control for the number of 3
‘success’ and ‘loss’ experiences, unbeknownst to the participants, there was an equal chance of 4
winning or losing the lottery across all trials. 5
6
The curiosity and food trials were mixed and shown in a random order. The task was 7
programmed and presented using PsychoPy 41. 8
9
Program on the day of the testing session 10
Participants were asked not to consume any food and drinks (apart from water) within at least 2 11
hours before attending their testing session. This was confirmed with the participants at the 12
beginning of the study, including asking them to indicate when they last ate and had their last 13
meal. Following standard procedures of informed consent and completing corresponding health 14
and safety screening, participants underwent calibration for electric stimulation to identify a 15
maximum (uncomfortable yet non-painful) threshold of electric shock they can endure (see 16
Supplementary Methods for details). Then, they were told about the decision-making task. There 17
was a practice task that used a different set of stimuli (3 curiosity, 3 food) prior to the main task. 18
19
At the end of the experiment, participants filled out a post-experiment questionnaire that asked 20
the extent to which they expected to receive electric shock. In this questionnaire, the majority of 21
participants across the two fMRI and the follow-up behavioural (i.e. FU1) experiments reported 22
prospectively that during the experiment they expected to receive the electric shock - 89% of 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
16
them gave a rating of 3 or above out of 5 (mean=3.67, mode=4), representing their strong belief 1
that the shock would have happened (we do not have this information from the initial 2
behavioural study). After completing the questionnaire, rewards (including solutions to some 3
magic tricks and food items) were delivered. Participants did not actually receive any 4
electroshock. They were told that they were exempt from the shock as determined by a 5
probabilistic equation programmed in the task. 6
7
fMRI acquisition 8
Whole-brain functional and anatomical images were acquired at the Centre for Integrative 9
Neuroscience and Neurodynamics (CINN), University of Reading, UK using a 3.0 Tesla 10
Siemens MAGNETOM Trio scanner with a 32-channel Head Matrix coil. 11
12
fMRI experiment 1 (Curiosity about Magic) 13
Functional images were acquired using a T2*-weighted gradient-echo echo planar imaging (EPI) 14
pulse sequence with 37 axial slices (in-plane resolution of 3 x 3 x 3mm, interslice gap: 0.75mm), 15
interleaved from bottom to top (echo time (TE): 30 ms; repetition time (TR): 2000 ms; flip angle: 16
90˚; field of view (FOV): 1344 x 1344 mm2; in-plane matrix: 64 x 64). A high-resolution T1-17
weighted three-dimensional anatomical image was also collected, using an MPRAGE-gradient 18
sequence with 176 x 1mm slices (in-plane resolution of 1 x 1 x 1 mm; TE: 2.52 ms; TR: 2020 19
ms; Inversion Time (TI):1100 ms; FOV: 250 x 250; flip angle: 9°), enabling optimal localisation 20
of the functional effects. 21
22
fMRI experiment 2 (Curiosity about Trivia) 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
17
Functional images were acquired using the same sequence and parameters as in fMRI 1
experiment 1. A high-resolution T1-weighted three-dimensional anatomical image was collected 2
using an MPRAGE-gradient sequence with 192 x 1 mm slices (TE: 2.29 ms; TR: 2300 ms; 3
TI:900 ms; FOV: 240 x 240; flip angle: 8°). 4
5
fMRI analysis 6
Preprocessing and data analyses of the imaging data were performed using the SPM software 7
(www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/~spm). The preprocessing procedures included spatial realignment of the 8
EPI volumes, co-registration with the structural image, segmentation, group-wise normalisation 9
using DARTEL, and smoothing (see Supplementary Methods for more details). 10
11
Region-of-Interest (ROI) Mask 12
Given past findings and shown in our previous works 42,43, the striatum plays an important role in 13
influencing motivation-driven behaviour. We created an anatomical mask encompassing the 14
bilateral caudates and performed ROI analyses in fMRI experiment 1, using the WFU PickAtlas 15
toolbox in SPM. The resulting anatomical mask included 832 voxels (2.5 resels). 16
17
To gauge the robustness and generalisability of the findings observed in fMRI experiment 1, we 18
created a functional ROI mask based on the functionally activated voxels within the bilateral 19
striatum from the ROI analysis of fMRI experiment 1 (specifically, from the main ‘decision’ 20
effect: Acceptance > Rejection; thresholded at p<0.001, uncorrected; i.e., see fig 2b). This 21
bilateral functionally defined caudate ROI, comprising 230 voxels (0.9 resels), was then used in 22
the fMRI experiment 2 analysis. In particular, we computed the average beta/contrast values 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
18
within this functional mask using MarsBar 44 and conducted a single statistical test to examine 1
whether the results of fMRI experiment 1 were replicated at a significance level of P < 0.05. 2
3
Activation predicting curiosity/incentive-driven decision-making 4
One of the aims of the current study was to test whether the participant’s decision-making was 5
influenced by curiosity (about knowledge) and food attraction in a similar manner via the 6
incentive motivation system. To this aim, the first GLM modelled brain activation depending on 7
the dichotomous decision in the lottery – whether an individual opted to accept or reject the 8
gamble. Four separate regressors were specified for the accepted and rejected trials in curiosity 9
and food categories at the onset of the decision phase of each trial (i.e. when the WoF appeared 10
and the participant had to start indicating a choice). In addition, to account for the brain 11
activation related to stimulus presentation, another 4 regressors were included to model the 12
stimulus presentation phase (for magic trick trials specifically, this was time-locked at the 13
moment of subjective surprise) of the accepted and rejected trials for the two stimulus categories 14
(results not reported). We performed a 2x2 repeated-measures ANOVA with category 15
(curiosity/food) and decision (accepted/rejected) as factors, at the decision phase. We 16
hypothesised a main effect of decision (i.e. accepted > rejected) in the caudate ROI if curiosity 17
and food influenced decision in a similar way via the incentive motivation system. We also 18
examined the interaction effects to probe any potential stimulus-specific effects on decision-19
making. A familywise error-corrected voxel-level significance threshold of P < 0.05 was used 20
within the ROI. 21
22
For additional whole brain analyses to explore activations outside of the defined ROI and in 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
19
other contrasts, we used a familywise error-corrected significance threshold of P < 0.05 at the 1
cluster-level (cluster defining threshold: P < 0.001). 2
3
Controlling for the effect of the probability of outcome (winning/losing) probability 4
To factor out the effect of the presented probability of winning a lottery (or getting electric 5
stimulation), we created another GLM and performed a parametric modulation analysis. In this 6
GLM, the first-level design matrices included two main categorical regressors, modelling the 7
onsets of the decision phase of trials for the two types of stimuli (curiosity, food). For these two 8
regressors, a participant’s decision in the lottery (i.e. ‘accept’ choice coded as +1; ‘reject’ choice 9
coded as -1) was added as a parametric modulator (PM); the outcome probability was added as 10
another PM. Similar to the other GLM, we still included reaction time as a PM to factor out the 11
its potential confounding effect. 12
13
Conjunction analysis 14
To examine whether there were consistent effects in curiosity/incentive-driven decision-making 15
regardless of the stimuli used, we performed a conjunction analysis on the results of the two 16
fMRI studies (which made use of very different curiosity-inducing materials). The conjunction 17
analyses, using a masking function of SPM, began with creating and saving a functional 18
activation mask (image) resulting from a particular SPM contrast of interest (i.e. accepted > 19
rejected trials) in the first fMRI experiment, thresholded at P < 0.001 (uncorrected). This 20
activation image was then used as an inclusion mask for the same contrast in the second fMRI 21
experiment (also thresholded at P < 0.001, uncorrected), resulting in a conjunction probability of 22
P < 0.00001 for any ‘activated’ clusters within the mask. 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
20
Functional Connectivity Analysis 1
Functional connectivity was examined with the beta series correlation method 45 implemented in 2
BASCO toolbox 46. This method allows us to use trial-to-trial variability to characterise dynamic 3
inter-regional interactions. Our a priori ROIs for functional connectivity analyses were the 6mm-4
radius spheres centred on the peak voxels of the left (MNI coordinate: -9, 15, 3) and right (6, 15, 5
3) caudate clusters identified in the main analysis (accepted > rejected gambles) of fMRI 6
experiment 1. 7
8
At the first level of the analysis, a new GLM was constructed, in which BOLD response time-9
locked to the onset of the decision phase of each trial was modelled individually by a separate 10
covariate using a canonical haemodynamic response function. This resulted in different 11
parameter estimates for each trial for each participant. The onsets of stimulus presentation in 12
each trial, and the six motion parameters for each run were also included in this GLM. Next, 13
seed-based correlations were computed voxel-wise for each participant and for each of the 14
experimental conditions of interest. This procedure generated an individual correlation map 15
between each seed region’s beta series and the beta series of all other voxels in the brain 16
separately for each condition of interest, which was normalised using Fisher’s r-to-z 17
transformation. At the second level, the individual correlation maps were taken into random-18
effects t-tests to identify voxels that showed changes in functional connectivity with the ROI 19
seed (based on trial-by-trial variability in parameter estimates) across different conditions. Given 20
the exploratory nature of the analysis, in fMRI experiment 1, the analyses were thresholded at P 21
< 0.005 at the voxel-level (uncorrected) (see Supplementary Table 4 for the results). In 22
particular, a large cluster of voxels were revealed in the left sensorimotor cortex to be less 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
21
correlated (i.e. decoupled) with the caudate (spherical ROI) in accepted relative to rejected 1
gambles (Fig. 3). We then created a functional ROI mask based on these ‘functionally 2
decoupled’ sensorimotor cortex voxels (comprising 105 voxels) to be used in the replication 3
analysis of fMRI experiment 2. 4
5
The functional connectivity analysis of fMRI experiment 2 mainly aimed to evaluate whether the 6
results from fMRI experiment 1 could be replicated, using the same approach as the main GLM 7
analysis. Specifically, with the correlation (functional connectivity) maps from fMRI experiment 8
2, we computed average contrast values (parameter estimates) within the functional sensorimotor 9
cortex ROI using Marsbar toolbox, and performed a single statistical test to examine whether the 10
functional connectivity results from fMRI experiment 1 (focusing on the differences in the 11
average contrast values between ‘accept’ and ‘reject’ choices) could be replicated with the 12
significance level set at P < 0.05. For further exploratory purposes, we performed an additional 13
whole brain analysis of differences in functional connectivity between the ‘accept’ and ‘reject’ 14
decisions in fMRI experiment 2 (see Supplementary Table 5). 15
16
Behavioural analysis 17
Linear mixed effect modelling 18
To examine the main question of whether curiosity and food desirability influence decisions on a 19
trial-by-trial basis at the behavioural level, we performed analysis using a generalised linear 20
mixed effects model (GLMM). In the model (separately for curiosity and food conditions), a 21
participant’s decision (i.e. choice in the lottery, which was either ‘accept’ or ‘reject;) was 22
specified as the dichotomous outcome, in a logistic link function. Rating of curiosity (or 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
22
desirability in the food condition) (group-mean centred) and the presented probability of 1
winning/losing were entered as predictors of the decision. To account for the nested structure of 2
the data, we specified random intercepts and slopes of participants. 3
4
We also combined the data from the initial behavioural experiment, fMRI experiment 1 and 5
fMRI experiment 2 and applied the same GLMM to the combined dataset to synthesise the 6
results (see Supplementary Table 1). We controlled for the effect of the experiment (i.e. from 7
which experiment the data were taken from) as an extra fixed effect. This is equivalent to 8
conducting a fixed-effect meta-analysis across the experiments. Importantly, the ‘experiment’ 9
factor was not significant, indicating no differences in participants’ performance (in terms of 10
acceptance rate) between the three experiments. 11
12
The follow-up behavioural experiment FU1 had a specific aim to investigate whether curiosity 13
and food desirability distort a participant’s subjective probability of winning/losing. The main 14
analysis in this experiment was based on a linear mixed effects model (LMM) (again, separately 15
for curiosity and food conditions) that included the rating of curiosity (or desirability in the food 16
condition) and the presented probability of winning/losing as predictors of subjective probability 17
of winning the lottery. Again, we specified random intercepts and slopes of participants. We 18
compared two models, one including rated curiosity (or desirability in the food condition) and 19
one without, and computed Bayes factors based on the Bayesian Information Criterion from each 20
model to evaluate to the effect of the rating. Conventional cut-offs for interpreting Bayes factors 21
are typically based on the ones suggested by Harold Jeffreys47. 22
23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
23
The follow-up ‘prediction’ experiment FU2 had participants to predict what choices they thought 1
other people would make. To compare and test whether these predictions were different from 2
(i.e. underestimating) the actual decisions made by the ‘real-life’ participants, we performed a 3
GLMM incorporating data from the initial behavioural experiment, the two fMRI experiments, 4
as well as the “prediction” study FU2. In this model, there was a fixed-effect factor ‘prediction’ 5
which specified whether a response was a prediction (+1) or an actual decision (-1), a factor 6
‘rating’ which specified participants’ ratings of curiosity/desirability of food, a 2-way interaction 7
of these factors, as well as a factor ‘category’ and a factor for ‘the presented probability of lottery 8
outcome’ to control for these effects. Importantly, the prediction X rating interaction effect was 9
negative and significant – specifically, the effect of rating was smaller in the ‘prediction’ 10
experiment than in the other actual experiments, suggesting that making a prediction for others 11
underestimates the actual effect of curiosity and food desirability in overcoming fear of physical 12
risks (see Supplementary Table 6). 13
14
Testing of GLMM and LMM was carried out using the package ‘lme4’ in R. 15
16
Mediation analysis 17
To test whether the relationship between rated curiosity/food desirability and the decision to 18
accept a lottery is mediated by the striatal activation observed in the main fMRI analysis, we 19
performed a multilevel-mediation analysis using multilevel structural equation modelling with 20
Mplus 48 (version 7). 21
22
In this analysis, we specified the indirect paths as starting from Rating through Striatal activation 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
24
to Decision (i.e. Rating Striatal activation Decision) as well as the direct path from Rating 1
to Decision, and analysed data of curiosity and food conditions altogether using trials as the unit 2
of analysis. Category of the stimulus (i.e. curiosity or food) and the presented probability of 3
winning were included as covariates in the model. Trial-by-trial average striatal activation was 4
extracted within the ROI masks, 6mm-radius spheres centred on the peak voxels of the left (MNI 5
coordinates: -9, 15, 3) and right (6, 15, 3) caudate clusters (i.e., the cluster of the largest 6
activation) identified previously in the main analysis (accepted > rejected gambles) in fMRI 7
experiment 1. Decision was treated as a binary outcome variable. 8
9
To account for the multilevel nature of the data, we used person-mean centring 49 before 10
parameter estimation and computed cluster robust-standard error to make statistical inference 50. 11
After estimating the model, mediation effect was tested by directly evaluating the distribution of 12
the product confidence limits for the mediation effect using the software PRODCLIN 51. We 13
claimed statistical significance at 5% if the 95% confidence interval computed by PRODCLIN 14
did not include zero. 15
16
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
25
1
2
3
Fig. 1 | The experimental task and behavioural results of motivation-driven decision-4
making. a, Illustration of a typical trial of the experimental task in curiosity and food conditions. 5
In modified versions for the fMRI experiments, (due to time constraints in scanning) the rating 6
phase appeared in only 10% of all trials during the task. b, Behavioural results integrated from 7
the initial behavioural and two fMRI experiments. On the left: Modelled logistic curve illustrates 8
participants’ decisions to accept the lottery at different levels of probability of electric shock in 9
food (blue curve) and curiosity (red curve) conditions. Acceptance rate on y-axis ranges from 0 10
(100% rejection) to 1 (100% acceptance). Each dot represents the average acceptance rate of a 11
participant at that level of shock probability. On the right: Bar chart shows higher average rating 12
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
26
of curiosity (about magic tricks and trivia questions) and food desirability in accepted (blue) 1
compared with rejected (orange) gambles. Each dot represents the average rating per participant, 2
for each condition. Error bars represent ±1 standard error of the mean (SEM). Dots on the graphs 3
are jittered for better visual display. Also see Supplementary Fig. 1 for illustration of the same 4
effects in individual studies separately. 5
6
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
27
1
2
Fig. 2 | Motivation-driven decision-making is mediated by the striatum (fMRI experiment 3
1). a, Analysis of brain activity associated with decision-making was based on whether a 4
participant accepted or rejected the lottery. b, Increased activity within the bilateral caudate 5
nucleus for accepted compared with rejected gambles at the decision phase was significant at 6
P<0.05 with voxel-wise FWE-correction. For display, the results shown here (in red) are 7
thresholded at P<0.001 (uncorrected). c, The bar plot shows the mean parameter estimates (i.e. 8
brain activations) for a 6mm-radius sphere centred at a peak voxel (MNI: 6, 15, 3) of the caudate 9
cluster (indicated by the white crosses in b) across participants. Error bars represent ±1 SEM for 10
each condition. d, In the mediation path diagram, rating (of magic curiosity and food 11
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
28
desirability) predicts the mean activation of the sphere in the caudate nucleus, illustrated by Path 1
i. The connection of caudate activation (the mediator) to decision in the lottery (the outcome), 2
Path ii, was calculated controlling for rating, as is standard practice for mediation models. The 3
direct path is Path iii’, calculated controlling for the mediator. The lines are labelled with 4
exponential values of path coefficients, exp(B), and z values. This model also controlled for the 5
effects of the category of the stimulus and the presented outcome probability in each trial. 6
*significant at P<0.05; **P<0.01. 7
8
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
29
1
Fig. 3 | Functional connectivity of caudate nucleus in fMRI experiment 1. Analysis of whole-2
brain functional connectivity was based on the beta series correlation method using the trial-by-3
trial variability45; the ROI was defined as a 6mm-radius sphere centred at the peak voxel (MNI: -4
9, 15, 3) in the caudate nucleus (left) identified in the main fMRI analysis. This connectivity 5
analysis revealed a significant main effect of decision in the left sensorimotor cortex (SMC), in 6
which activation was less correlated with the caudate ROI in the accepted compared with 7
rejected gambles (thresholded at P<0.005, uncorrected). This is illustrated in 3D rendered and 8
whole brain images in the figure. The bar plot shows the magnitude of correlation (r, converted 9
back from z values) between activities in the left caudate ROI and the left SMC cluster in 10
different trial types. Error bars indicate ±1 SEM. 11
12
13
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
30
1
Fig. 4 |Common effects of motivation-driven decision from the two fMRI experiments. The 2
sagittal multi-slice brain images show the conjunction of activations from the main GLM whole-3
brain analysis of fMRI experiment 1 & 2, each thresholded at the statistical significance of 4
P<0.001 (uncorrected), giving rise to a conjunction probability of P<0.00001. Shown in green, 5
the conjunction effects for the contrast ‘Accepted > Rejected gambles’ (across both stimulus 6
types) are found in the left and right caudate nuclei (in slices x=-8 and x=6 (cluster a) 7
respectively), right thalamus (cluster b), dorsal medial frontal lobe (cluster c), and right premotor 8
cortex (x=48). 9
10
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
31
References 1
1. von Stumm, S., Hell, B. & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. The hungry mind: Intellectual 2
curiosity is the third pillar of academic performance. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6, 574–588 3
(2011). 4
2. Gruber, M. J., Gelman, B. D. & Ranganath, C. States of curiosity modulate hippocampus-5
dependent learning via the dopaminergic circuit. Neuron 84, 486–496 (2014). 6
3. Kang, M. J. et al. The wick in the candle of learning: Epistemic curiosity activates reward 7
circuitry and enhances memory. Psychol. Sci. 20, 963–973 (2009). 8
4. Gottlieb, J., Oudeyer, P.-Y., Lopes, M. & Baranes, A. Information-seeking, curiosity, and 9
attention: computational and neural mechanisms. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 585–593 (2013). 10
5. Jirout, J. & Klahr, D. Children’s scientific curiosity: In search of an operational definition 11
of an elusive concept. Dev. Rev. 32, 125–160 (2012). 12
6. Kidd, C. & Hayden, B. Y. The psychology and neuroscience of curiosity. Neuron 88, 13
449–460 (2015). 14
7. Renninger, K. A. & Hidi, S. The power of interest for motivation and engagement. 15
(Routledge, NY, 2016). 16
8. Sakaki, M., Yagi, A. & Murayama, K. Curiosity in old age: A possible key to achieving 17
adaptive aging. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 88, 106–116 (2018). 18
9. Rozek, C. S., Svoboda, R. C., Harackiewicz, J. M., Hulleman, C. S. & Hyde, J. S. Utility-19
value intervention with parents increases students’ STEM preparation and career pursuit. 20
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 114, 909–914 (2017). 21
10. Loewenstein, G. The psychology of curiosity: A review and reinterpretation. Psychol. 22
Bull. 116, 75–98 (1994). 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
32
11. Blanchard, T. C., Hayden, B. Y. & Bromberg-Martin, E. S. Orbitofrontal cortex uses 1
distinct codes for different choice attributes in decisions motivated by curiosity. Neuron 2
85, 602–614 (2015). 3
12. Daddaoua, N., Lopes, M. & Gottlieb, J. Intrinsically motivated oculomotor exploration 4
guided by uncertainty reduction and conditioned reinforcement in non-human primates. 5
Sci. Rep. 6, 20202 (2016). 6
13. Vasconcelos, M., Monteiro, T. & Kacelnik, A. Irrational choice and the value of 7
information. Sci. Rep. 5, 13874 (2015). 8
14. Bennett, D., Bode, S., Brydevall, M., Warren, H. & Murawski, C. Intrinsic valuation of 9
information in decision making under uncertainty. PLOS Comput. Biol. 12, e1005020 10
(2016). 11
15. Brydevall, M., Bennett, D., Murawski, C. & Bode, S. The neural encoding of information 12
prediction errors during non-instrumental information seeking. Sci. Rep. 8, 6134 (2018). 13
16. Eliaz, K. & Schotter, A. Paying for confidence: An experimental study of the demand for 14
non-instrumental information. Games Econ. Behav. 70, 304–324 (2010). 15
17. Wright, W. F. & Bower, G. H. Mood effects on subjective probability assessment. Organ. 16
Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 52, 276–291 (1992). 17
18. Litman, J. Curiosity and the pleasures of learning: Wanting and liking new information. 18
Cogn. Emot. 19, 793–814 (2005). 19
19. Beckmann, J. S. & Chow, J. J. Isolating the incentive salience of reward-associated 20
stimuli: value, choice, and persistence. Learn. Mem. 22, 116–127 (2015). 21
20. Berridge, K. C. From prediction error to incentive salience: mesolimbic computation of 22
reward motivation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 35, 1124–1143 (2012). 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
33
21. Berridge, K. C. The debate over dopamine’s role in reward: the case for incentive 1
salience. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 191, 391–431 (2007). 2
22. Berridge, K. C., Robinson, T. E. & Aldridge, J. W. Dissecting components of reward: 3
‘liking’, ‘wanting’, and learning. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 9, 65–73 (2009). 4
23. Kringelbach, M. L. & Berridge, K. C. in Recent Developments in Neuroscience Research 5
on Human Motivation (eds. Kim, S., Reeve, J. & Bong, M.) 23–35 (Emerald Group 6
Publishing Limited, Bingley, 2016). doi:10.1108/S0749-742320160000019020 7
24. Delgado, M. R., Schotter, A., Ozbay, E. Y. & Phelps, E. A. Understanding overbidding: 8
Using the neural circuitry of reward to design economic auctions. Science. 321, 1849–9
1852 (2008). 10
25. Hayes, D. J. et al. GABAA receptors predict aversion-related brain responses: An fMRI-11
PET investigation in healthy humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 1438–1450 (2013). 12
26. Whitmarsh, S., Nieuwenhuis, I. L. C., Barendregt, H. P. & Jensen, O. Sensorimotor alpha 13
cctivity is modulated in response to the observation of pain in others. Front. Hum. 14
Neurosci. 5, (2011). 15
27. Berlyne, D. E. A theory of human curiosity. Br. J. Psychol. 45, 180–191 (1954). 16
28. Fastrich, G. M., Kerr, T., Castel, A. D. & Murayama, K. The role of interest in memory 17
for trivia questions: An investigation with a large-scale database. Motiv. Sci. 4, 227–250 18
(2018). 19
29. Loewenstein, G. & Lerner, J. S. in Handbook of Affective Science (eds. Davidson, R., 20
Goldsmith, H. & Scherer, K.) 619–642 (Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2003). 21
30. Gilbert, D. T., Lieberman, M. D., Morewedge, C. K. & Wilson, T. D. The peculiar 22
longevity of things not so bad. Psychol. Sci. 15, 14–19 (2004). 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
34
31. Murayama, K., FitzGibbon, L. & Sakaki, M. Process account of curiosity and interest: A 1
reward learning model of knowledge acquisition (OSF Preprints). Preprint at 2
https://osf.io/hbcz5 (2018). 3
32. Hsee, C. K. & Ruan, B. The Pandora effect: The power and peril of curiosity. Psychol. 4
Sci. 27, 659–666 (2016). 5
33. Wilson, T. D. et al. Just think: The challenges of the disengaged mind. Science. 345, 75–6
77 (2014). 7
34. Kruger, J. & Evans, M. The paradox of Alypius and the pursuit of unwanted information. 8
J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 45, 1173–1179 (2009). 9
35. Oosterwijk, S. Choosing the negative: A behavioral demonstration of morbid curiosity. 10
PLoS One 12, e0178399 (2017). 11
36. Mobbs, D. et al. When fear Is near: Threat imminence elicits prefrontal-periaqueductal 12
gray shifts in humans. Science. 317, 1079–1083 (2007). 13
37. Adolphs, R. The biology of fear. Curr. Biol. 23, R79-93 (2013). 14
38. Dubois, J., Galdi, P., Han, Y., Paul, L. K. & Adolphs, R. Resting-state functional brain 15
connectivity best predicts the personality dimension of openness to experience. Personal. 16
Neurosci. 1, e6 (2018). 17
39. Oudeyer, P.-Y. & Kaplan, F. What is intrinsic motivation? A typology of computational 18
approaches. Front. Neurorobot. 1, 1–14 (2007). 19
40. Pisula, W., Turlejski, K. & Charles, E. P. Comparative psychology as unified psychology: 20
The case of curiosity and other novelty-related behavior. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 17, 224–229 21
(2013). 22
41. Peirce, J. W. Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. Front. Neuroinform. 2, 23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
35
(2009). 1
42. Murayama, K., Matsumoto, M., Izuma, K. & Matsumoto, K. Neural basis of the 2
undermining effect of monetary reward on intrinsic motivation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 107, 3
20911–20916 (2010). 4
43. Murayama, K. et al. How self-determined choice facilitates performance: A Key role of 5
the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 25, 1241–1251 (2015). 6
44. Brett, M., Anton, J.-L., Valabregue, R. & Poline, J.-B. Region of interest analysis using an 7
SPM toolbox. In: 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human 8
Brain. NeruoImage 16, (2002). 9
45. Rissman, J., Gazzaley, A. & D’Esposito, M. Measuring functional connectivity during 10
distinct stages of a cognitive task. Neuroimage 23, 752–763 (2004). 11
46. Göttlich, M., Beyer, F. & Krämer, U. M. BASCO: a toolbox for task-related functional 12
connectivity. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 9, (2015). 13
47. Jeffreys, H. Theory of Probability. (Clarendon Press, Oxford, 1961). 14
48. Muthén, L. K. & Muthén, B. O. Mplus User’s Guide. Seventh Edition. (2012). 15
49. Enders, C. K. & Tofighi, D. Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel 16
models: A new look at an old issue. Psychol. Methods 12, 121–138 (2007). 17
50. McNeish, D., Stapleton, L. M. & Silverman, R. D. On the unnecessary ubiquity of 18
hierarchical linear modeling. Psychol. Methods 22, 114–140 (2017). 19
51. MacKinnon, D. P., Fritz, M. S., Williams, J. & Lockwood, C. M. Distribution of the 20
product confidence limits for the indirect effect: Program PRODCLIN. Behav. Res. 21
Methods 39, 384–389 (2007). 22
23
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;
36
Acknowledgments 1
The study was supported by the Marie Curie Career Integration Grant (CIG630680 to K.M.), 2
JSPS KAKENHI (15H05401; 16H06406 , 18H01102, and 18K18696 to K.M.), F. J. McGuigan 3
Early Career Investigator Prize (to K.M.), and the Leverhulme Trust (RPG-2016-146 and RL-4
2016-030 to K.M.). We are grateful of the magicians (including Shota Irieda and Ohkubo Kohei) 5
for producing magic tricks for our research. We thank E. Daveau for helping with data collection, 6
C. Inaltay, A. Firat, A. Haffey, J. Raw, and G. Fastrich for editing and pilot-testing magic video 7
clips, and C. McNabb for providing useful comments on the drafts of the article. 8
9
Author contributions 10
K.M. conceived the idea; J.K.L.L. and K.M. jointly designed the study; H.O., A.K., and K.K. 11
created experimental materials. J.K.L.L. performed research and analyzed the data; J.K.L.L. and 12
K.M. jointly wrote the paper. All authors provided critical comments. The authors declare no 13
conflicts of interest. 14
15
Supplementary Materials: 16
Supplementary Methods 17
Supplementary Figures 1-4 18
Supplementary Tables 1-6 19
Captions for Supplementary videos 20
.CC-BY-ND 4.0 International licenseIt is made available under a
(which was not peer-reviewed) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity.
The copyright holder for this preprint. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/473975doi: bioRxiv preprint first posted online Nov. 22, 2018;