ArticlePDF Available

What Do Criminal Justice Students Know About Autism? An Exploratory Study Among Future Professionals

Authors:

Abstract

The social and communication impairments and other atypical behaviors among those with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) make this subset of the population particularly vulnerable. These vulnerabilities also present a separate set of concerns when they have contact with the criminal justice system, typically as victims or witness of abuse, as offenders or suspicious persons, or lost or missing persons. Specific measures must be taken to improve communication and to avoid misinterpreting communication impairments and other atypical behaviors as an indication of a lack of cooperation, being under the influence of substances, or of guilt/lack of remorse. Without the benefit of having basic knowledge and understanding of autism, criminal justice system professionals will struggle with meeting the needs of those with ASD. The current study explored the level of autism knowledge and awareness of among a sample of 400 undergraduate criminal justice students and possible future criminal justice professionals. The results demonstrated that the sample of students had moderate knowledge of ASD, which did not appear to increase with time in program. Those with greater exposure to people with ASD had more knowledge and understanding than those who did not. Recommendations and implications are discussed.
1 23
Journal of Police and Criminal
Psychology
The Official Journal of the Society for
Police and Criminal Psychology
ISSN 0882-0783
Volume 34
Number 4
J Police Crim Psych (2019) 34:428-438
DOI 10.1007/s11896-018-9302-0
What Do Criminal Justice Students Know
About Autism? An Exploratory Study
Among Future Professionals
Melanie Clark Mogavero
1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and
all rights are held exclusively by Society
for Police and Criminal Psychology. This e-
offprint is for personal use only and shall not
be self-archived in electronic repositories. If
you wish to self-archive your article, please
use the accepted manuscript version for
posting on your own website. You may
further deposit the accepted manuscript
version in any repository, provided it is only
made publicly available 12 months after
official publication or later and provided
acknowledgement is given to the original
source of publication and a link is inserted
to the published article on Springer's
website. The link must be accompanied by
the following text: "The final publication is
available at link.springer.com”.
What Do Criminal Justice Students Know About Autism? An Exploratory
Study Among Future Professionals
Melanie Clark Mogavero
1
#Society for Police and Criminal Psychology 2018
Abstract
The social and communication impairments and other atypical behaviors among those with autism spectrum disorder (ASD)
make this subset of the population particularly vulnerable. These vulnerabilities also present a separate set of concerns when they
have contact with the criminal justice system, typically as victims or witness of abuse, as offenders or suspicious persons, or lost
or missing persons. Specific measures must be taken to improve communication and to avoid misinterpreting communication
impairments and other atypical behaviors as an indication of a lack of cooperation, being under the influence of substances, or of
guilt/lack of remorse. Without the benefit of having basic knowledge and understanding of autism, criminal justice system
professionals will struggle with meeting the needs of those with ASD. The current study explored the level of autism knowledge
and awareness of among a sample of 400 undergraduate criminal justice students and possible future criminal justice profes-
sionals. The results demonstrated that the sample of students had moderate knowledge of ASD, which did not appear to increase
with time in program. Those with greater exposure to people with ASD had more knowledge and understanding than those who
did not. Recommendations and implications are discussed.
Keywords Autism Spectrum disorder .Aspergers.Police .Criminal justice system
Introduction
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a rapidly increasing con-
dition that affects between 1 and 2% of the population (Center
for Disease Control and Prevention, CDC). Research has in-
dicated that people with ASD or other developmental disabil-
ities will have up to seven times more contacts with the crim-
inal justice system (CJS) during their lifetimes than members
of the general population (Curry et al. 1993;Debbaudt2002).
Individuals with ASD may encounter professionals in CJS for
a variety of reasons and there are many concerns when they
do, particularly with police, as this is typically their initial
contact. The initial experience may positively or negatively
affect any future contact and experiences with policeand other
sectors of the CJS. Unfortunately, research has implied an
overall dissatisfaction with police encounters among the au-
tism community, and some revealed that these contacts were
traumatic (Crane et al. 2016; Edworthy and Hylton 2010;
Rava et al. 2017).
Characteristics of ASD and Prevalence
ASD
1
is a complex neurological disorder characterized in part,
by limited verbal and non-verbal communication and social in-
teraction. Some characteristics of ASD listed in the Diagnostic
and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition
(DSM-5) published by the American Psychiatric Association
(APA) include lack of eye contact, lack of social or emotional
reciprocity, extreme egocentricity, narrow interests, and overly
dependent on routines. Individuals diagnosed with ASD have
varying degrees of social communication and interaction impair-
ments. The severity of onesASDdiagnosisisdeterminedby
one of three levels of required support (support, substantial sup-
port, very substantial support), in two domains (social commu-
nication and restricted/repetitive behaviors) (APA 2013).
1
Due to the diagnostic changes from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders (DSM-IV, APA, 2000) to the DSV-5 (2013) that reclassified
all Pervasive Developmental Disorders under one term, Autism Spectrum
Disorder, the use of ASD in this paper includes Autistic Disorder,
Aspergers Syndrome, and Autism Spectrum Disorder.
*Melanie Clark Mogavero
mmogavero@georgian.edu
1
Department of Criminal Justice, Georgian Court University,
Lakewood, NJ, USA
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11896-018-9302-0
Published online: 21 November 2018
Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology (2019) 34:428438
Author's personal copy
AccordingtotheCDC(http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/
autism/data.html, Retrieved: September 8, 2018), the
prevalence of those with ASD has increased significantly
over the past decade. The most recent statistics published by
CDC researchers note that the rate of those with ASD as 1 in
59, a 30% increase from 2008 (1 in 88), a 60% increase from
2006 (1 in 110), and a 120% increase from 2002 (1 in 150).
The explanations for the rate increase are unknown but likely
the result of the increased autism awareness, changes to
diagnostic methods and expansions in diagnostic criteria,
2
improvements in clinician training in diagnosing ASD,
increased assistance for children in local communities, and/
or increased familial and social acceptance (Haskins and Silva
2006; Hacking 2009). Furthermore, the CDC reports that
ASD is almost five times more common among males (1 in
42) than among females (1 in 189).
Literature Review
Contacts with the Criminal Justice System
Currently, there are no official record-keeping practices nor
official statistics measuring the rate of contacts individuals with
ASD have had with the CJS. Although not an exhaustive list,
such contacts with the CJS include them as victims, witnesses,
suspicious persons/offenders, and lost or missing persons.
Surveys of adolescents and adults with ASD and/or caregivers
revealed 8% (Turcotte et al. 2018), 16% (Tint et al. 2017), to
that as many as 19% (Rava et al. 2017) had contact with police.
Some of the problems noted in these surveys included the of-
ficerslack of awareness and knowledge of ASD; the physical,
emotional, or cognitive needs were not met (e.g., failed to ex-
plain procedures, legal terminology, possible outcomes), and
unnecessary force/violence (Crane et al. 2016; Edworthy and
Hylton 2010;Ravaetal.2017). A survey of police officers in
the UK revealed that they had an average of 20 encounters with
persons with ASD, of which about 36% were victims, 43%
were suspects, 10% were witnesses, and another 13% of other
contacts or situations (Crane et al. 2016). With the rates of ASD
increasing, the number of contacts with police will likely in-
crease, and efforts must be made to reduce any dissatisfaction
or trauma of such interactions.
As Abuse Victims or Witnesses
Due to behavioral characteristics such as social isolation, so-
cial issues, poor communication skills, and other atypical be-
haviors, those with ASD are at greater risk of physical and
sexual abuse (Carlile 2018;Duanetal.2015;Edelson2010;
Hall-Lande et al. 2015; Mandell et al. 2005;Perkinsand
Wolkind 1991). For example, Mandell et al. (2005)reported
that of the 156 children with ASD, 14% were victims of phys-
ical abuse, 12% sexual abuse, and more than 4% had been
both victims of both physical and sexual abuse. An unpub-
lished 2007 report by The Autism Society revealed that
among a survey of 1500 individuals with ASD and their care-
givers, 17% reported physical abuse or assault, 13% reported
being the victim of sexual abuse, 8% reported a sexual assault,
and 8% reported neglect (Autism Society 2006). Hall-Lande
et al. (2015) noted that children with ASD and other
disabilities were overrepresented in the state child protection
system than children without disabilities. Further, Duan et al.
(2015) noted that the severity of abuse not only increased with
age, but also the severity of ones ASD symptomology.
Unfortunately, many abuse cases go unreported and police
follow-up, prosecution, and convictions of the perpetrators are
low, often due to a lack of specialized police training in han-
dling victims with developmental disabilities, and many indi-
viduals with disabilities are overwhelmed by the CJS
(Gammicchia and Johnson n.d.). Victim credibility as a wit-
ness is often challenged due to concerns with memory, sug-
gestibility, and communication (Bruck et al. 2007; Henry et al.
2017; Lindblad and Lainpelto 2011; Maras and Bowler 2010;
Maras et al. 2014;Mattisonetal.2015,2016; McCrory et al.
2007). Some parents reported that police appeared to be im-
patient and lacked empathy when their child had difficulty
remembering events, dates, and times, which sometimes led
to their child becoming too distressed to continue the inter-
view (Edworthy and Hylton 2010).
Surveys of police officers reported that obtaining written
statements or conducting interviews with individuals with
ASD was difficult (Crane et al. 2016). Therefore, when police
must communicate with someone with ASD, certain provisions
must be implemented, beginning with the initial contact,
questioning (to ensure accurate accounts of crimes are obtained),
and having appropriate representation so their rights preserved.
Communication provisions include adequately preparing the
victim of the legal process, being aware of any communication
or reading difficulties, using assistive technology when neces-
sary, eliminating/reducing noise or other visual stimuli that
could be distracting, and also limiting the number of interviews
or reducing the length of interviews (Edworthy and Hylton
2010; Henry et al. 2017; Gammicchia and Johnson n.d.).
As Suspicious Persons and Offenders
Most diagnosed with ASD are law-abiding, and criminal be-
havior among this population has shown to be rare
(Ghaziuddin et al. 1991; Scragg and Shah; Kohn et al. 1998;
Woodbury-Smith et al. 2006;Mouridsen2012). However, the
ASD characteristics likely have a considerable role for the
minority who do commit offenses. Some common offenses
2
See Pervasive Developmental Disorder in the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV, APA, 2000),
J Police Crim Psych (2019) 34:428438 429
Author's personal copy
cited in the literature include assault (Geluk et al. 2012;Allen
et al. 2008; Ray et al. 2004; Barry-Walsh and Mullen 2004;
Murrie et al. 2002; Scragg and Shah 1994), arson (Geluk et al.
2012; Allen et al. 2008; Haskins and Silva 2006;Murrieetal.;
Siponmaa et al. 2001; Scragg and Shah), and sexual offenses
(Kohn et al. 1998; Barry-Walsh and Mullen; Murrie et al.;
Hart-Kerkhoffs et al. 2009; Griffin-Shelley 2010).
There is an overall a lack of information about those with
ASD involved in the CJS (Loynes 2001). Cheely et al. (2011)
measured this prevalence among a sample of 609 youths with
ASD between the age of 1218 and revealed that 32 (5%)
were charged with 103 offenses, of which 26 (27.1%) were
prosecuted. Despite these results, it remains unknown whether
there is an overrepresentation of those with ASD in the juve-
nile or adult CJS. Involvement in CJS for offenders with ASD
can be particularly challenging for those with average or
above average intelligence. They may appear more Bnormal^
and intelligent, where any social and communication deficits
may go unrecognized or misunderstood by others. For exam-
ple, an individuals lack of social or emotional reciprocity or
failure to make eye contact may be interpreted as having a lack
of remorse, indifference, or callousness. Frith (1991) noted
when apprehended by authorities, those with ASD often do
not appear to feel guilt, may not attempt to conceal or excuse
their behavior, and may even bluntly and honestly admit their
behavior. An individual with ASD may laugh, speak loudly,
and have an aloof body language (inherent to ASD), which
could lead many police, prosecutors, and judges and to con-
clude that the individual isguilty and lacks remorse (Debbaudt
2004). Safety for all those involved is also a concern. Specific
de-escalation techniques must also be used, as Edworthy and
Hylton (2010) noted, common restraint methods used by the
police, such as handcuffs, may escalate the situation. If the
individual is unable to communicate, they may become phys-
ical towards the police to prevent restraint or what Chown
(2009)referredtoastheBfright,^Bfight,^or Bflight^response.
Individuals with ASD may be more susceptible to interrog-
ative pressure during formal interviews, referred to as inter-
rogative suggestibility (Gudjohnsson 1984; North et al. 2007).
Interrogative pressure may provide misleading statements, as
standard interrogation techniques utilize deception that may
confuse those with ASD who tend to be Bconcrete-thinkers^
into providing false confessions (Debbaudt 2004). It has also
been suggested that among those with ASD who commit
criminal offenses, the manner in which they are handled in
the CJS may also be due to their ASD characteristics
(Debbaudt 2004). To handle those who enter the CJS fairly
and justly, it is imperative to understand any underlying causes
that may be responsible for or to explain their current behavior
(Crane et al. 2016; Debbaudt 2004). Individuals with ASD
who come into contact with the CJS are often misunderstood,
mistreated, and sometimes receive harsher punishments than
are warranted (Attwood et al. 2014; Crane et al. 2016;
Freckelton 2013; Haskins and Silva 2006; Mayes 2003;
Mogavero 2016; North et al. 2007; Taylor et al. 2009).
As Lost or Missing Persons
An important issue that primarily concerns police officers and
other first responders is elopement. Eloping (leaving a
predetermined area without the permission or supervision of
caregivers) is often reported among parents, teachers, and
caregivers of those with ASD (Anderson et al. 2012;
Barnard-Brak et al. 2016;Langetal.2009;Riceetal.2016;
Solomon and Lawlor 2013). Anderson et al. (2012)revealed
that about 50% of parents/teachers/caregivers reported an
elopement incident, and police were involved in about one-
third of such cases. The consequences of elopement can be
serious and may sometimes result in death (Lowe et al. 2007;
Mouridsen et al. 2008). The high-profile case of Avonte
Oquendo who fled from his school on October 4, 2013 and
was later found deceased months later on January 16, 2014
drew nation-wide attention to elopement among those with
ASD (Hilton 2017). Moreover, Rice et al. (2016)havenoted
that those who eloped tended to have more limitations in so-
cial and communication skills than those who did not wander,
making them particularly vulnerable to harm.
>Police officers may encounter a person with ASD in the
community or be requested to locate a lost or missing person
with ASD. Therefore, it is imperative that police understand
characteristics of ASD, recognize the behaviors, and
understand how to best approach those with ASD to
minimize risk. As Debbaudt (2002) and Debbaudt and
Rothman (2001) noted, when approached by law enforcement,
individuals with ASD may not respond to Bstop^or other
commands or may run/walk away when approached. They
may react negatively to lights, sirens, or canine officers and
may begin to pace, flap their hands, hit themselves, or scream.
To an untrained or uninformed officer, they may mistake some-
one with ASD as someone who is under the influence of alco-
hol or having a psychotic episode (Debbaudt 2002;Debbaudt
and Rothman 2001). When police encounter an unsupervised
individual with ASD, parents often find themselves in a posi-
tion to explain or Beducate^police officers about autism, for
example, Bhe has autism, hesnotverbal^(Solomon and
Lawlor 2013, p. 111). Such precautions can help prevent for-
mal allegation of the use of excessive force. In addition, a
police report or involvement of child protection services may
also be avoided when police are informed and understand the
situation (Solomon and Lawlor 2013).
Knowledge and Awareness of ASD
Research on police officersknowledge and experience with
individuals with ASD is lacking, particularly in the US, but
the little research that has accessed such knowledge in the UK
J Police Crim Psych (2019) 34:428438430
Author's personal copy
has been discouraging. A survey of police officers in the UK
revealed that nearly half (48%) believed that they were knowl-
edgeable about ASD while 20% reported that they had little
knowledge (Crane et al. 2016). Similarly, Chown (2009)mea-
sured the knowledge of ASD among a sample of 120 police
officers in the UK and reported that although more than half
were able to identify the main characteristics of autism, only
half were able to correctly identify Aspergerssyndromeas
being on the autism spectrum. Modell and Mak (2008)sur-
veyed 124 police officers about their knowledge and percep-
tion of autism and revealed that 80% were unable to identify
accurate characteristics of ASD and more than 35% simply
listed, BRain Man
3
^as their response.
According to Debbaudt (2004),a gap remains with both the
skill and training for professionals in dealing with a rapidly
increasing population of individuals diagnosed with ASD.
The survey of police officers by Crane et al. (2016)revealed
that nearly half (48%) believed they were well equipped to
work with individuals with ASD, while 29% believed they
were poorly equipped to work with such individuals. Only
37% of officers surveyed reported that they had received train-
ing on ASD, and of those 28% reported that they were unsat-
isfied with the training (Crane et al. 2016). Chown (2009)
revealed that 62% received no formal training in autism and
Modell and Mak (2008) similarly revealed that 48% received
no training, and 45% indicated they had some training de-
scribed as minimal, basic, or vague. Overall, it appears from
the three studies (two of which were published a decade ago)
that police officers are uninformed and ill-equipped to handle
this vulnerable population during their policing duties and
perhaps more can be done to prepare them prior to entering
their careers. The above study results assessed police officers
in the UK, and therefore, it is unknown whether such results
are generalizable to police officers in the US.
Current Study
The purpose of the current study was to report on results
of an exploratory and descriptive study regarding knowl-
edge of autism among a large and diverse sample of un-
dergraduate CJ students. Few studies have been conduct-
ed to measure the level of ASD knowledge among college
students (e.g., Tipton and Blacher 2014), but none to the
authors knowledge were conducted on this specific stu-
dent population. Are our future CJ professionals knowl-
edgeable about this population that, statistically, they will
encounter at some point during their careers? The author
assessed basic knowledge and understanding of ASD with
regard to behavior and communication characteristics,
misconceptions, and understanding the broad spectrum
of the characteristics of this disorder. CJS professionals
must handle a person with ASD differently when first
approaching, stating commands, and asking questions, es-
pecially during formal interviews, to better ensure that
they handle individuals with ASD properly, respectfully,
and justly.
Four primary questions were addressed: (1) What was the
overall level of knowledge about autism among the respon-
dents? (2) Which specific domains were respondents more or
less knowledgeable? (3) Does knowing someone with ASD
and the relationship increase level of knowledge? (4). Are
primary demographics (gender, race/ethnicity, class year) as-
sociated with the extent of knowledge?
Methods
The author administered the Autism Knowledge Survey-
Revised (AKS-R, Swiezy 2007,2013), the updated ver-
sion of the original by Stone (1987).
4
The AKS-R is a 20-
item measure that assesses a respondents degree of agree-
ment with statements (10 accurate, 10 inaccurate) regard-
ing ASD, including etiology, interventions, diagnosis, and
outcomes. The AKS has been revised several times to
reflect current knowledge informed by research of ASD
(Swiezy et al. 2005;Swiezy2007,2013). Respondents
either agree or disagree to each statement on 6-point
Likert-type scale (fully agree = 1, fully disagree = 6).
Subjects were encouraged to respond to the AKS-R to
the best of their knowledge. If subjects skipped questions
on the AKS-R, those questions were not counted towards
their overall score. The accurate statements were reverse
coded, so that a higher number reflected greater knowl-
edge of autism (highest possible AKS-R score = 120). The
surveys were anonymous, and no names, social security,
or student identification numbers were collected. Each
survey was assigned a number that could not be linked
back to the subject completing the survey.
Subjects for this study were drawn from a purposive
and convenience sample of the student body enrolled in
undergraduate CJ courses at two public and two private
universities in New Jersey and Pennsylvania (one public
and one private in each state). The Institutional Review
Board application was approved from each university. The
four universities varied in student enrollment size (ap-
proximately 2100, 3900, 8200, 8500) and location setting
(rural, suburban, urban). The universities were selected to
obtain a diverse sample of undergraduate CJ students with
3
Rain Main (1988) see https://www.imdb.com/title/tt0095953/awards?ref_=
tt_awd
4
An analysis of the Stones original 1987 survey conducted by Campbell,
Reichle, and Van Bourgondien (1996) revealed that it was unidimensional,
had reasonable internal consistency (alpha = 0.66), stable
reliability, and showed initial reasonable validity.
J Police Crim Psych (2019) 34:428438 431
Author's personal copy
different backgrounds. The courses varied in size and lev-
el (100-level to 400-level) to capture students of different
ages and stages in their academic program. The author
selected students in CJ courses to capture a greater per-
centage of CJ or related program majors, as such pro-
grams are typically designed educate students, in part,
about the CJS (law enforcement, courts, corrections). It
is important to measure the reactions, thoughts, and per-
ceptions of such behaviors of this specific student popu-
lation that will most likely work in some sector of CJS in
the future, prior to entering their careers. The data was
collected in the Spring 2017 through Spring 2018 semes-
ters. Students were not compensated for their participation
and the response rate varied per class and ranged from 84
to 100%.
Results
Descriptive Statistics
All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 25.
Tab le 1displays the characteristics and demographics of
the sample by university. All four academic years were
represented, with slightly fewer first year students partic-
ipating(16.7%),withfourthyearstudentsbeingthemost
represented (32.5%). These percentages allowed a com-
parison by time in academic program, with a sufficient
sample of upper-class students to measure any acquired
knowledge of ASD. The sample was approximately half
male and half female, and racially and ethnically diverse,
with 54.5% white/Caucasian, 15.5% black/African
American, 19% Hispanic, and another 11% identifying
as either Asian, Native American, or bi-racial/multi-racial.
Multivariate Analyses
Tab le 2displays the mean average of the AKS-R scores by
academic year, race, and gender and whether they know
someone with ASD exposure and their relationship to the
person they know with ASD. Participants were asked
whether or not they know someone with ASD, and whether
the person was a family member, and acquaintance (e.g.,
friend, neighbor, co-worker, classmate), or if they knew
someone through your employment (e.g., work at daycare,
school). As demonstrated, the AKS-R scores across aca-
demic year show moderate knowledge of ASD (maximum
score is 120). An analysis of variance (ANOVA) failed to
demonstrate statistically significant differences among the
class years (F= 0.387, p< .818) and a Pearson Chi-square
test failed to demonstrate gender differences (X
2
= 2.2,
p< .704). There were racial differences noted in AKS-R
scores, with Caucasians having higher scores than all other
racial/ethnic groups, and African Americans having lower
scores (F= 10.7, p< .000). However, these differences are
likely attributed to knowing someone with ASD, as more
than two-thirds (67.4%) of Caucasians reported knowing
someone with ASD while only 38.7% of African
Americans reported such.
Tab le 3displays the mean AKS-R scores by the amount
of persons known with ASD and the relationship. An
Table 1 Sample characteristics,
N=400 University
PA Public
n=153
n(%)
PA Private
n=67
n(%)
NJ Public
n=119
n(%)
NJ Private
n=61
n(%)
Tot al
N=400
n(%)
Academic year (n=396)
1 18 (12.4) 13 (31.5) 10 (8.0) 13 (22.4) 54 (16.7)
2 36 (26.3) 21 (22.6) 23 (24.8) 12 (17.9) 92 (24.0)
3 44(28.1) 20(25.8) 29(22.6) 16(23.9) 109(25.7)
4 55(33.2) 13(19.4) 53(41.6) 20(35.8) 141(32.5)
Gender (n=399)
Male 87 (56.9) 37 (55.2) 62 (52.5) 27 (44.3) 213 (53.4)
Female 66 (43.1) 30 (44.8) 55 (46.6) 34 (55.7) 185 (46.4)
Race/ethnicity (n=400)
White/Caucasian 103 (67.3) 52 (77.6) 29 (24.4) 34 (55.7) 218 (54.5)
Black/African Am. 20 (13.1) 1 (1.5) 28 (23.5) 13 (21.3) 62 (15.5)
Hispanic 21 (13.7) 10 (14.9) 33 (27.7) 12 (19.7) 76 (19.0)
Others 9 (5.9) 4 (6.0) 29 (24.4) 2 (3.3) 44 (11.0)
J Police Crim Psych (2019) 34:428438432
Author's personal copy
ANOVA revealed that those who know someone with
ASD and those who know more than one person with
ASD scored significantly higher on the AKS-R than those
who did not (F= 13.1, p< .000). With regard to relation-
ships, an independent ttest revealed that only those who
knew an acquaintance appeared to increase scores on the
AKS-R (t=3.33, p< .001).
Tab le 4displays the results of a bivariate linear re-
gression predicting AKS-R score by the relationship to
the person(s) known with ASD, controlling for the total
number of persons known with ASD. The author con-
ducted a series of bivariate regression analyses to sepa-
rately test the influence of each ASD relationship on the
AKS-R. Only the acquaintance relationship was statisti-
cally significant (B=2.15, p< .001) after controlling for
the total number of persons known with ASD. It ap-
pears that those who knew someone in a social manner,
opposed to family member or in a professional environ-
ment, had more overall knowledge.
Tab le 5displays the results of a ttest of the mean indi-
vidual item scores for the 20 AKS-R items by the
amount of persons known with ASD and the whether
they were an acquaintance. Items 1, 3, 7, 9, 12, 18, and
19 represent misconceptions or lay beliefs regarding the
causes, symptoms, and interventions for ASD. Only
items 1 and 18 demonstrated little difference across
groups. Most incorrectly identified ASD as an emotional
disorder, opposed to development or neurological. Items
3, 4, 5, 7, 10 represent social-affective characteristics,
reflecting the social skills and social engagement fea-
tures of ASD. Of these, there were no significant dif-
ferences across exposure level among item 5, suggesting
that the sample students understood that ASD is differ-
ent than other psychiatric disorders, and among item 10,
suggesting that students are unaware that the delay or
lack of communication is the first concern. More of
those who reported knowing more than one person
agreed that a lack of eye contact is not present with
Table 2 Mean full AKS-R scores by demographics and ASD exposure
(N=400)
Demographics AKS-R scores Know someone
w/ ASD
Academic year n= 399 Mean (SD) N(%)
1(129 credits) 83.2 (9.3) 36 (67.9)
2(3059 credits) 82.6 (8.5) 60 (65.2)
3(6089 credits) 83.7 (7.6) 62 (57.4)
4 (90 + credits) 83.8 (8.8) 87 (62.6)
F=0.387,p< .818
Pearson X
2
=2.2,p<.704
Race ethnicity n=400
White/Caucasian 85.3 (8.1) 147 (67.4)
Black/African Am. 78.9 (8.2) 24 (38.7)
Hispanic 82.2 (7.1) 48 (64.0)
Others 82.3 (8.9) 28 (65.1)
F=10.7,p< .000
Pearson X
2
=21.6p< .001
Gender n=399
Female 83.8 (8.5) 127 (59.6)
Male 83.1 (8.4) 118 (64.5)
ttest t=.915, p< .361
Pearson X
2
=1.1,p<.577
Table 3 Mean full AKS-R scores by amount of ASD exposure (N=
400)
Know someone w/ ASD N= 394 Mean (SD)
No 81.2 (8.3)
182.9(7.3)
2 or more 86.0 (8.7)
F=13.1,p< .000
Know family member N=395
Yes 84. 7 (8. 3)
No 83.1 (8.5)
ttest t=1.45, p< .147
Know through employment N=395
Yes 85. 5 (8. 4)
No 83.1 (8.4)
ttest t=.915, p< .361
Know an acquaintance N=395
Yes 85. 1 (8. 5)
No 82.2 (8.6)
ttest t=3.33, p< .001
Table 4 Bivariate linear regression of AKS-R score by ASD
relationship (N=395)
BStd. E βtsig.
Constant (1) 82.6 .528 156.52 .000
Family Member .704 1.10 .033 .638 .524
Tot al know n numbe r
of persons with ASD
.427 .157 .141 2.71 .007
R
2
=.023,f(4.618) = .010
Constant (1) 2.57 .215 11.94 .000
Employment .800 1.394 .032 .574 .566
Tot al know n numbe r
of persons with ASD
.412 .168 .136 2.45 .015
R
2
=.023,f(4.578) = .011
Constant (1) 81.96 .581 141.19 .000
Acquaintance 2.15 .887 .126 2.43 .016
Total known number of
persons with ASD
.342 .158 .113 2.17 .031
R
2
=.037,f(7.43) = .001
J Police Crim Psych (2019) 34:428438 433
Author's personal copy
Table 5 Mean AKS-R item scores by race/ethnicity and ASD exposure (N= 400)
Survey items Know someone Know 2 or more Know acquaint. Ideal score
YNYNYN
1. Autism is an emotional disorder 3.3 (1.5) 3.1 (1.3) 3.1 (1.5) 3.1 (1.3) 3.2 (1.5) 3.2 (1.4) 6
2. Early intervention can lead to significant gains
in childrens social and communication skills.
1.9** (1.1) 2.1** (1.1) 1.8** (1.1) 2.1** (1.1) 1.9 (1.1) 2.0 (1.1) 1
3. All people with autism display poor eye contact. 4.1 (1.4) 4.0 (1.2) 4.2*** (1.3) 3.9*** (1.3) 4.2** (1.3) 1.4** (1.4) 6
4. People with autism typically perform better when tasks are presented visually
than when tasks are presented verbally.
2.4*** (1.2) 2.7***(1.1) 2.4 (1.2) 2.5 (1.1) 2.5 (1.2) 2.5 (1.1) 1
5. Problems with social relatedness that are present in autism are different from social
problems seen in other psychiatric conditions
2.8 (1.1) 2.9 (0.9) 2.8 (1.2) 2.8 (1.0) 2.8 (1.0) 2.8 (1.2) 1
6. Autism is more frequently diagnosed in males than in females. 2.9** (1.3) 3.3** (1.3) 2.9 (1.4) 3.1 (1.3) 3.0 (1.3) 3.1 (1.4) 1
7. Children with autism do not show attachments, even to parents/caregivers 4.7* (1.3) 4.5* (1.3) 4.8** (1.3) 4.5** (1.3) 4.8* (1.3) 4.5* (1.3) 6
8. Research indicates that sensory integration therapy is an effective treatment for
autism and its symptoms
2.8*** (1.0) 3.1*** (1.0) 2.8 (1.1) 2.9 (1.0) 2.8** (1.0) 3.0** (1.1) 1
9. Children with autism are deliberately uncooperative 4.8 (1.3) 4.8 (1.2) 4.9 (1.3) 4.7 (1.2) 4.9 (1.2) 4.7 (1.3) 6
10. Most parents/caregivers of children with autism report their first
concerns were related to the childssocialbehavior.
2.5 (1.2) 2.6 (1.0) 2.5 (1.1) 2.5 (1.0) 2.6 (1.1) 2.5 (1.0) 6
11. Autism tends to run in families 3.4 (1.3) 3.4 (1.2) 3.3 (1.4) 3.5 (1.2) 3.5 (1.3) 3.4 (1.3) 1
12. We now have treatments that can cure autism. 4.7** (1.4) 4.4** (1.2) 4.8** (1.4) 4.4** (1.3) 4.7** (1.4) 4.4** (1.3) 6
13. People with autism can grow up to live independently. 2.4*** (1.4) 2.8*** (1.4) 2.3** (1.4) 2.7** (1.4) 2.4** (1.4) 2.7** (1.5) 1
14. There is one approach or program that works for all people with autism. 4.8** (1.4) 4.5** (1.5) 4.9** (1.3) 4.6** (1.5) 4.9*** (1.4) 4.5*** (1.5) 6
15. It is important that all people diagnosed with autism receive some form
of special education services at school.
4.8 (1.4) 4.5 (1.5) 2.2 (1.2) 2.2 (1.1) 2.1 (1.1) 2.2 (1.2) 1
16. Autism occurs more commonly among higher socioeconomic and
educational levels.
4.0 (1.3) 4.1 (1.2) 4.1 (1.4) 4.1 (1.2) 4.1 (1.3) 4.1 (1.3) 6
17. Autism can be diagnosed as early as 18 months. 2.7 (1.4) 2.9 (1.2) 2.4*** (1.2) 3.0*** (1.3) 2.7 (1.4) 2.8 (1.3) 1
18. With the proper treatment, most children diagnosed with autism eventually
outgrow the disorder
4.1 (1.5) 4.1 (1.3) 4.3* (1.5) 4.0* (1.4) 4.1 (1.6) 4.1 (1.4) 6
19. People with autism do not show affection, even to parents/caregivers. 5.0** (1.2) 4.7** (1.2) 5.0 (1.3) 4.8 (1.2) 5.0** (1.2) 4.8** (1.3) 6
20. The need for routines and sameness is one of the earliest behavioral features of autism 2.7*** (1.2) 3.1*** (1.1) 2.7 (1.2) 2.9 (1.2) 2.7* (1.2) 2.9* (1.2) 1
1 = fully agree; 2 = mostly agree; 3 = somewhat agree; 4 = somewhat disagree; 5 = mostly disagree; 6 = fully disagree
ttest: ***p< .01, **p< .05, *p<.10
J Police Crim Psych (2019) 34:428438434
Author's personal copy
all persons with ASD. Those who reported knowing
someonewithASDopposedtothosewhodidnotap-
pear to better understand that those with ASD often
communicate better visually than verbally.
Items 2, 8, 12, 13, 14, 15, 17 reflect knowledge
related to early interventions for ASD. Of these, items
13 and 14 demonstrated significant differences across
groups, noting that there is not one approach that works
for all persons with ASD, recognizing the different
needs and levels of independence. Items 6, 11, 16 re-
flect knowledge of causes and performance in ASD.
With the exception of item 6, there were few significant
differences reported among groups.
Discussion
The results demonstrate that overall, CJ studentsknowl-
edge of ASD is lacking or is moderate at best. Those who
reported knowing someone with ASD consistently scored
closer to the ideal score (see Stuart et al. 2008) than those
who did not. The results demonstrate that having expo-
sure to those with ASD is key to better understanding the
complexities of the disorder. The more knowledge and
information one possesses, the better equipped they will
be when having to make critical decisions (Debbaubt
2004). Specific to the AKS-R, those who understand that
although lack of eye contact may be a common character-
istic of ASD, it is not present with all persons, and if
someone gives eye contact while speaking, one must not
assume that they cannot have ASD. Those working in the
CJS must have other communication methods aside from
strictly verbal. Many persons with ASD perform better
when tasks or commands are presented with visual cues,
and as Edworthy and Hylton (2010) noted, a pictorial
toolkit should be present when necessary to help those
with communication difficulties understand questions
and commands.
It is also imperative that those working in the CJS
understand the vast array of needs and simply because
one method worked in the past, does not mean it will
be effective for others they encounter with ASD in the
future. There is no one approach that works for all
persons with ASD, and it is important to recognize dif-
ferent needs. Lastly, it is also important that many indi-
viduals with ASD become independent adults, and sim-
ply because for example, they are driving a vehicle, one
must not assume that cannot have ASD. To better serve
thosewithASDwhocomeintocontactwithanymem-
ber of the CJS, it is imperative they not only be aware
of various communication methods, but also be aware
of and disregard misconceptions about those with ASD.
Limitations
Although the results demonstrate moderate knowledge of
ASD, with increased knowledge and understanding among
those with greater exposure to those with ASD, this study is
not without limitations. The principle limitation would be
the representativeness of the sample, as this was a conve-
nience sample of college students enrolled in CJ classes and
reported to be majoring in CJ or a related field. The subjects
may not be representative of the general population of all CJ
students in the US. The sample also measured reactions of
CJ students or likely future CJ professionals, and not the
knowledge of those who have already entered their careers
in the CJS. The author did not question students if they had
taken any psychology courses where they may have been
exposed to the diagnostic criteria and other characteristics
of ASD, and thus, may have increased their scores on the
AKS-R. The authors also did not control for socio-
economic status; therefore, it is unknown whether this has
an effect on the amount of exposure, resources in the com-
munity, and level of knowledge or views of ASD, in general.
Nonetheless, the large sample of a variety of students at
different universities provided valuable insight to the level
of knowledge they possess of ASD. Future research could
examine this extent of knowledge and perhaps inquire how
CJ professionals react to, and handle those with ASD when
encountering them during their professional duties.
Conclusion and Recommendations
The rate of ASD increasing and the various circumstances
someone with ASD may have contact with the CJS, it is a
matter of when, not if, a CJ professional will encounter
someone with ASD. Are we preparing future CJ professions
to work with specialized populations? In order to better
serve community members with ASD, it is imperative that
CJ professionals are knowledgeable and informed of vari-
ous ways to communicate and interact with those with ASD,
prior to entering their careers. It is imperative that CJ pro-
fessionals understand the broad spectrum of the disorder
and the wide range of characteristics an individual with
ASD can present with so that when police encounter such
an individual, so they can both better identify the persons as
being on the autism spectrum. If they are unable to identify
the ASD, they risk misinterpreting behaviors as a refusal to
comply or cooperate and may perceive failure to cooperate
with questions or commands as deliberate. Due to the char-
acteristics of ASD, during initial contact, those with ASD
may be unable to quickly process questions, repeat what is
stated to them (echolalia), make Brude^statements, have
difficulty understanding common figures of speech, or
avoid eye contact.
J Police Crim Psych (2019) 34:428438 435
Author's personal copy
It is important to approach those with ASD in a quiet,
non-threatening manner, and speak calmly in a moderated
voice (Debbaudt 2004; Debbaudt and Rothman 2001). They
must understand the importance of putting a person with
ASD at ease and use a range of techniques available to
prevent escalation (Chown 2009). Considering that the sam-
ple of students has only moderate knowledge of ASD,
which does not appear to increase with time in the program
(i.e., across academic years), and knowing/exposure to
someone with ASD increases knowledge, the author high-
lights the following points:
&It is recommended that CJ curriculums add course(s) re-
lated to working with special populations, including those
with ASD and other communication disorders/disabilities,
and perhaps be required for those in practitioner-related
tracks or concentrations.
&Students should be encouraged to work, volunteer, or per-
form internships where they would have exposure to those
with ASD and other disabilities, to be better prepared to
serve and protect the entire community, including resi-
dents with disabilities.
&Such valuable knowledge and experience should be
known prior to their hiring and serving the community.
CJ programs can better prepare students to work effective-
ly with various members of the community in various
sectors of the CJS. It is also important to note that not all
law enforcement agencies require a college education, or a
degree in CJ or related field. Therefore, it is also recom-
mended that such education and training be provided in
police academy training and continue among CJ profes-
sionals. Several states
5
have passed legislation that re-
quires police officers and certain first responders to under-
go autism awareness and education training. However,
Debbaudt
6
noted most training programs have no standard
for the content, length, and/or credentials of the person
administering the training, which is what he recommend-
ed be included in Floridas legislation (D. Debbaudt, per-
sonal communication, March 4, 2016).
As mentioned, research on CJ professionals experience with
individuals with ASD and the experiences and satisfaction among
those with ASD with the CJS in the US is lacking. To date, such
research has only been conducted in the UK. More information
about both CJ professionals and the experience community mem-
bers with ASD have had with the CJS in the US is needed in order
to determine how to best serve and meet the needs all community
members and improve overall satisfaction with the CJS.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Exclusivity The author is submitting this manuscript for exclusive con-
sideration as an article in the Journal of Police and Criminal Psychology
and declares the manuscript or parts of the manuscript have not been
published elsewhere.
Conflicts of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of
interest.
Research Involving Human Participants All procedures performed in
studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical
standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with
the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable
ethical standards.
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual
participants included in the study.
PublishersNote Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdic-
tional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
Allen D, Evans C, Hider A, Hawkins S, Peckett H, Morgan H (2008)
Offending behaviour in adults with Asperger syndrome. J Autism
Dev Disord 38:748758
American Psychiatric Association (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edn, (DSM 5). Washington,
DC: APA
Anderson C, Law JK, Daniels A, Rice C, Mandell DS, Hagopian L, Law
PA (2012) Occurrence and family impact of elopement in children
with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 130:870877
Attwood T, Henault I, Dubin N (2014) The autism Spectrum, sexuality,
and the law. Jessica Kingsley Publishers, London
Autism Society (2006) Results of the victims of crime with autism survey
(unpublished)
Barnard-Brak L, Richman DM, Moreno R (2016) Predictors of elopement
exhibited by school-aged children with special health care needs:
towards the development of a screening instrument for elopement. J
Prim Prev 37:543554. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10935-016-0449-0
Barry-Walsh JB, Mullen PE (2004) Forensic aspects of Aspergerssyn-
drome. J Forensic Psychiatry Psychol 15(1):96107
Bruck M, London K, Landa R, Goodman J (2007) Autobiographical
memory and suggestibility in children with autism spectrum disor-
der. Dev Psychopathol 19:7395. https://doi.org/10.1017/
s0954579407070058
Carlile KA (2018) Teaching help-seeking when lost to individuals with
autism spectrum disorder. J Appl Behav Anal 51(2):191206
Cheely CA, Carpenter LA, Letourneau EJ, Nicholas JS, Charles J, King
LB (2011) The prevalence of youth with autism spectrum disorders
in the criminal justice system. J Autism Dev Disord 42:18561862.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1427-2
Chown N (2009) Do you have any difficulties that I may not be aware
of?A study of autism awareness and understanding in the UK
police service. Int J Police Sci Manag 12(2):256273. https://doi.
org/10.1350/ijps.2010.12.2.174
Crane L, Maras KL, Hawken T, Mulcahy S, Memon A (2016) Experiences
of autism spectrum disorder and policing in England and Wales: sur-
veying police and the autism community. J Autism Dev Disord 46:
20282041. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2729-1
5
New Jersey, Indiana, Illinois, California, Massachusetts, Florida.
6
Dennis Debbaudt is a world-renowned author and ASD educator for law
enforcement and emergency
responders. See https://www.debbaudtlegacy.com/
J Police Crim Psych (2019) 34:428438436
Author's personal copy
Curry K, Posluszny M, Kraska S (1993) Training criminal justice personnel
to recognize offenders with disabilities. Office of Special Education
and Rehabilitative Services News In Print, Washington, DC
Debbaudt D (2002) Autism, advocates and law enforcement profes-
sionals. Jessica Kingsley Publishers, London
Debbaudt D (2004) Beyond guilt or innocence. Forum paper, Autism
New Zealand Inc., Worcester, MA. Retrieved May 31, 2018 from
http://autismnz.org.nz/police/Pol_BeyGuilt.pdf
Debbaudt D, Rothman D (2001) Contact with individuals with autism.
FBI Law Enforce Bull 70(4):2024
Duan G, Chen J, Zhang W, Yu B, Jin Y, Wang Y, Yao M (2015) Physical
maltreatment of children with autism in Henan province in China: a
cross-sectional study. Child Abuse Negl 48:140147. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2015.03.018
Edelson G (2010) Sexual abuse of children with autism: factors that
increase risk and interfere with recognition of abuse. Disability
Studies Quarterly 30(1):189200. http://dsqsds.org/article/view/
1058/1228
Edworthy A, Hylton C (2010) Brain-injured children and the police. J
Learn Disabil Offending Behav 1(3):4048
Freckelton I (2013) Autism spectrum disorder: forensic issues and chal-
lenges for mental health professionals and courts. J Appl Res
Intellect Disabil 26(5):420434
Frith U (1991) Autism and Asperger Syndrome. Cambridge University
Press, Cambridge
GammicchiaC, Johnson C (n.d.) The Autism Society Autism information
for advocates, attorneys, and judges. Retrieved September 8, 2018
from https://www.autism-society.org/wp-content/.../Advocates_
Attorneys_and_Judges.pdf
Geluk C, Jansen L, Vermeiren R, Doreleijers T, van Domburgh L, de
Bildt A, Twisk J, Hartman C (2012) Autistic symptoms in childhood
arrestees: longitudinal association with delinquent behavio r. J Child
Psychol Psychiatry 53(2):60167
Ghaziuddin M, Tsai L, Ghaziuddin N (1991) Brief report: Violence in
Asperger syndrome, A critique. J Autism Dev Disord 21(3):349354
Griffin-Shelley E (2010) An Aspergers adolescent sex addict, sex of-
fender: a case study. Sex Addict Compulsivity 17:4664. https://
doi.org/10.1080/10720161003646450
Gudjohnsson GH (1984) A new scale of interrogative suggestibility.
Personal Individ Differ 5:5358
Hacking I (2009) Humans, aliens, and autism. Daedalus 138(3):4459
Hall-Lande J, Hewitt A, Mishra S, Piescher K, LaLiberte T (2015)
Involvement of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in
the child protection system. Focus Autism Other Dev Disabl
30(4):237248. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088357614539834
Hart-Kerkhoffs LA, Jansen LM, Doreleijers TA, Vermeiren R, Minderaa
RB (2009) Autism Spectrum disorder symptoms in juvenile sus-
pects of sex offenses. J Clin Psychiatry 70(2):266272
Haskins BG, Silva JA (2006) Aspergers disorder and criminal behavior:
forensic-psychiatric considerations. J Am Aca Psychiatr Law 34:
374384
Henry LA, Crane L, Nash G, Hobson Z, Kirke-Smith M, Wilcock R
(2017) Verbal, visual, and intermediary support for child witnesses
with autism during investigative interview. J Autism Dev Disord 47:
23482362. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-017-3142-0
Hilton LJ (2017) Avonte's law: autism, aandering, and the racial surveil-
lance of neurological difference. Afr Am Rev 50(2):221235
Kohn Y, Tarek F, Ratzoni G, Apter A (1998) Aggression and sexual
offence in Aspergers syndrome. Isr J Psychiatry Relat Sci 35(4):
293299
Lang R, Rispoli M, Machalicek W, White PJ, Kang S, Pierce N, Mulloy
A, Fragale T, OReilly M, Sigafoos J, Lancioni G (2009) Treatment
of elopement in individuals with developmental disabilities: a sys-
tematic review. Res Dev Disabil 30:670681
Lindblad F, Lainpelto K (2011) Sexual abuse allegations by children with
neuropsychiatric disorders. J Child Sex Abuse 20:182195. https://
doi.org/10.1080/10538712.2011.554339
Lowe K, Allen D, Jones E, Brophy S, Moore K, James W (2007)
Challenging behaviours: prevalence and topographies. J Intellect
Disabil Res 51(8):625636
Loynes F (2001) The Impact of Autism. Report prepared for the All Party
Parliamentary Group on Autism. Published: Autism
Mandell DS,Walrath CM, Manteuffel B, Sgro G, Pinto-Martin JA (2005)
The prevalence and correlates of abuse among children with autism
served in comprehensive community-based mental health settings.
Child Abuse Negl 29:13591372
Maras KL, Bowler DM (2010) The cognitive interview for eyewitnesses
with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 40(11):1350
1360. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0997-8
Maras KL, Mulcahy S, Memon A, Picariello F, Bowler DM (2014)
Evaluating the effectiveness of the self-administered interview for
witnesses with autism spectrum disorder. Appl Cogn Psychol 28(5):
693701. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3055
Mattison M, Dando CJ, Ormerod T (2015) Sketching to remember: epi-
sodic free recall task support for child witnesses and victims with
autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 45(6):17511765.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2335-z
Mattison M, Dando CJ, Ormerod T (2016) Drawing the answers:
sketching to support free and probed recall by child witnesses and
victims with autism spectrum disorder. Autism 22:181194. https://
doi.org/10.1177/1362361316669088
Mayes TA (2003) Persons with autism and criminal justice: core concepts
and leading cases. J Posit Behav Interv 5(2):92100. https://doi.org/
10.1177/10983007030050020401
McCrory E, Henry LA, Happ F (2007) Eyewitness memory and suggest-
ibility in children with Asperger syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry
48:482489. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01715.x.
Modell SJ, Mak S (2008) A preliminary assessment of police officer
knowledge and perceptions of persons with disabilities. Intellectual
and Developmental Disabilities 46:183189
Mogavero MC (2016) Autism, sexual offending, and the criminal justice
system. J Intellect Disabil Offending Behav 7(1):116126
Mouridsen SE (2012) Current status of research on autism spectrum
disorders and offending. Res Autism Spectr Disord 6:7986
Mouridsen SE, Brønnum-Hansen H, Rich B, Isager T (2008) Mortality
and causes of death in autism spectrum disorders: an update. Autism
12(4):403414
Murrie DC, Warren JI, Kristiansson M, Dietz PE (2002) Aspergerssyn-
drome in forensic settings. Int J Forensic Ment Health 1(1):5970
North AS, Russell AJ, Gudjonsson GH (2007) High functioning autism
spectrum disorders: an investigation of psychological vulnerabilities
during interrogative interview. J Forensic Psychiatry Psychol 19(3):
323334
Perkins M, Wolkind S (1991) Aspergers syndrome: who is being
abused? Arch Dis Child 66(6):693695
Ray F, Marks C, Bray-Garretson H (2004) Challenges to treating adoles-
cents with Asperger's syndrome who are sexually abusive. Sex
Addict Compulsivity 11:265285. https://doi.org/10.1080/
10720160490900614
Rava J, Shattuck P, Rast J, Roux A (2017) The prevalence and correlates
of involvement in the criminal justice system among youth on the
autism spectrum. J Autism Dev Disord 37(2):340346. https://doi.
org/10.1007/s10803-016-2958-3
Rice CE, Zablotsky B, Avila RM, Colpe LJ, Schieve LA, Pringle B,
Blumberg SJ (2016) Reported wandering behavior among children
with autism spectrum disorder and/or intellectual disability. J Pediatr
174:232239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2016.03.047
Scragg P, Shah A (1994) Prevalence of Aspergers syndrome in a secure
hospital. Br J Psychiatry 165:679682
J Police Crim Psych (2019) 34:428438 437
Author's personal copy
Siponmaa L, Kristiansson M, Jonson C, Nyden A, Gillberg C (2001)
Juvenile and young adult mentally disordered offenders: the role
of child neuropsychiatric disorders. J Am Aca Psychiatr Law
29(4):420426
Solomon O, Lawlor MC (2013) And I look down and he is gone: narrat-
ing autism, elopement and wandering in Los Angeles. Soc Sci Med
94:106114
Stone WL (1987) Cross-disciplinary perspectives on autism. J Pediatr
Psychol 12(4):615630
Stuart M, Swiezy N and Ashby I (2008) Autism Knowledge Survey:
Trends in Understanding of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Poster pre-
sented at the 2nd annual ABA Autism Conference, February 9,
Atlanta, GA
Swiezy NA (2007) Autism Knowledge Survey-Revised. Christian
Sarkine Autism Treatment Center, HANDS in Autism,
Indianapolis, IN
Swiezy NA (2013) Autism Knowledge Survey-Revised. Christian Sarkine
Autism Treatment Center, HANDS in Autism, Indianapolis, IN
Swiezy NA, Fairbanks J, Stuart M, Ashby I, Ables EM and Stone W.
(2005) Autism Knowledge Survey. Christian Sarkine Autism
Treatment Center, HANDS in Autism
Taylor K, Mesibov G, Debbaudt D (2009) Autism in the criminal justice
system. N Carolina Bar J 14(4):3236
Tint A, Palucka AM, Bradley E, Weiss JA, Lunsky Y (2017) Correlates of
police involvement among adolescents and adults with autism spec-
trum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 47(9):26392647
Tipton LA, Blacher J (2014) Brief report: autism awareness: views from a
campus community. J Autism Dev Disord 44:477483
Turcotte P, Shea LL, Mandell D (2018) School discipline, hospitalization,
and police contact overlap among individuals with autism spectrum
disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 48(3):883891. https://doi.org/10.
1007/s10803-017-3359-y
Woodbury-Smith MR, Clare ICH, Holland AJ, Kearns A (2006) High
functioning autistic spectrum disorders, offending and other law-
breaking: findings from a community sample. J Forensic
Psychiatry Psychol 17(1):108120
J Police Crim Psych (2019) 34:428438438
Author's personal copy
... Therefore, we must also consider that the lack of early diagnoses for these adults may have prohibited therapeutic intervention and support during their childhood. This lack of support would have signi cant safeguarding implications for the young adult with a NDD who has been placed in a juvenile facility (141)(142)(143). Many individuals with NDDs can self-harm and are frequently misunderstood in the youth justice system due to their cognitive differences. ...
... Many individuals with NDDs can self-harm and are frequently misunderstood in the youth justice system due to their cognitive differences. The behavioural characteristics can often be misinterpreted by their peers placing them in yet more vulnerable situations (141)(142)(143). More qualitative research on the experiences of adults with bipolar who were previously Lac would contribute to a better understanding of why some of these adults end up in the criminal justice system (144). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background Looked after children (Lac) continue to attain suboptimal health and social outcomes and have a high prevalence of mental, developmental and behavioural disorders. Limited in research is the exploration of neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs) in this vulnerable population. This review aims to compare prevalence rates of NDDs in children who are looked after (Lac) versus children not looked after (non-Lac) and identify any impacts on Lac diagnosed with a NDD. Methods Articles were identified in PubMed, ASSIA, IBSS, Web of Science, PsychINFO, Scopus, Psych articles, Social Care Online. Studies that provided primary data on the prevalence of a ND for both Lac and non-Lac under the age of < 25 years were included. Two reviewers appraised the articles using the Joanna Briggs Institute critical appraisal tools. Risks ratio and 95% confidence intervals (CI) using the Mantel-Haenszel random-effects model were used to express and estimate the mean effect, Results Of a total 5,611 records, 12 studies met eligibility. Six studies that met inclusion criteria for a meta-analysis highlight how Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) RR = 2.69 [1.44, 5.01] and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) RR = 2.23 [1.63, 3.05] have a higher prevalence in Lac compared to non-Lac. The remaining six studies that compared prevalence of NDDs but were not eligible for the meta-analysis are described as a narrative and highlight the adverse outcomes that effect the Lac with a NDD. Findings related to foster care placement, mental health service usage and medication, physical, emotional abuse and criminal justice involvement are discussed. Conclusion Results show that the prevalence of NDDs is higher in Lac versus non-Lac and propose that Lac with a NDD experience more adverse outcomes compared to their non-Lac peers with a NDD. More research is needed to explore the prevalence of NDDs in both populations and further investigate the adverse outcomes that may affect the Lac with a NDD.
... who has been placed in a juvenile facility(McCarthy et al., 2019, Mogavero, 2019Young et al., 2018). Many individuals with NDDs can self-harm and are frequently misunderstood in the youth justice system due to their cognitive impairments; their behaviours can often be misinterpreted by their peers placing them in yet more vulnerable situations(McCarthy et al., 2019, Mogavero, 2019Young et al., 2018). ...
... who has been placed in a juvenile facility(McCarthy et al., 2019, Mogavero, 2019Young et al., 2018). Many individuals with NDDs can self-harm and are frequently misunderstood in the youth justice system due to their cognitive impairments; their behaviours can often be misinterpreted by their peers placing them in yet more vulnerable situations(McCarthy et al., 2019, Mogavero, 2019Young et al., 2018). ...
... The breadth of recommended training topics for police is vast. It includes identification of autistic traits Hepworth, 2017); co-occurring conditions (Copenhaver & Tewksbury, 2018); challenging myths and misunderstanding about autism (Mogavero, 2019); interview accommodations ; understanding autistic communication and behaviour (Copenhaver & Tewksbury, 2018;Salerno-Ferraro & Schuller, 2020); handling confrontations in an autisticsensitive manner (Copenhaver & Tewksbury, 2018); sensory accommodations ; and more. ...
... Interesting were the results of a recent investigation of the autism knowledge of 400 undergraduate criminal justice students studying at four universities in the United States (Mogavero, 2019). At best, they had only a moderate degree of autism knowledge. ...
Thesis
The past two decades have seen increasing social science and legal interest in the relationship between autism and criminal justice. Most publications have been case study or legal analyses, or focussed on quantitative topics (for example prevalence or offence type). What is noticeable by their relative absence are the voices of autistic people, especially those who have been suspected of or charged with a crime, and for whom legal proceedings occurred in the regular criminal system. This runs counter to the goals of the autism rights movement and critical autism studies which value the experiences and opinions of autistic people, and promote research that will make a positive difference to their lives. This research investigates the lived experience of 10 autistic adults subject to the Aotearoa New Zealand criminal justice system. Through interpretative phenomenological analysis, six superordinate themes were identified. Illustrated through participant quotes, these were: unease with New Zealand Police; overwhelming legal processes; encounters with mental health professionals; the impact on self and others; recommendations from the participants; and adversity across the lifespan. The thesis also identified four factors, power, process, perception and participation, that both influenced how the participants made sense of their CJS experiences and were compelling forces for change.
... Research suggests that between 8 and 20% of autistic individuals and/or their caregivers report a history of LEO contact, with approximately 5% leading to arrest (Rava et al., 2017;Tint et al., 2017;Turcotte et al., 2018), and 20% leading to involuntary psychiatric hospitalization evaluation (Gardner & Campbell, 2020). Contact between autistic people and LEOs is impacted by the social communication and interaction differences that are characteristic of ASD and other repetitive behaviors associated with autism (Mogavero, 2019). Frequent interactions between LEOs and autistic people has led to an increased support for training programs focused on increasing LEOs knowledge of autism and to facilitate positive outcomes from these interactions (Gardner et al., 2019;Railey et al., 2020;Sreckovic et al., 2023;Vega & Peacock, 2023). ...
Article
Full-text available
Prevalence rates of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) across the globe and lifespan suggest that law enforcement officers (LEOs) are likely to interact with autistic people in their professional duties. In developed countries, LEOs have professional encounters with autistic individuals, and research strongly supports LEOs need training to increase their knowledge about autism and confidence interacting with autistic people. The purpose of the present study is to compare three training approaches with LEOs: (a) a training presentation alone (P), (b) a training presentation combined with virtual reality simulation (P + VR), and (c) a training presentation combined with in-vivo simulation (P + SIM). The primary outcomes are LEO knowledge of autism and self-reported confidence in responding to autism calls. The present study included 259 LEOS who completed a survey assessing autism knowledge and confidence in level of skill before and after participating in ASD-specific training for LEOs. Findings support that participant knowledge of autism and confidence in responding significantly increased for all LEOs equally across training conditions. Results support that formalized training in ASD significantly impacts LEO’s knowledge of ASD and confidence responding to calls. As such, standardized training for LEOs is needed to assure appropriate knowledge and confidence when responding to calls involving autistic persons.
... Recent research suggests between 8 % and 20 % of autistic individuals and/or their caregivers have experienced LEO contact, with approximately 5 % leading to arrest (Rava et al., 2017;Tint et al., 2017;Turcotte, Shea et al., 2018), and 20 % leading to involuntary psychiatric hospitalization evaluation (Gardner & Campbell, 2020). The disproportionate contact with LEOs in this population is often attributed to the core impairments characterized by ASD, including deficits with social interactions and communication as well as atypical behaviors (Clark Mogavero, 2019). The rate of individuals diagnosed with autism has continued to increase over time, with recent estimates by the Center for Disease Control (CDC) suggesting that in the United States 2.27 % of children (Maenner et al., 2021) and 2.21 % of adults (Dietz et al., 2020) meet criteria for ASD. ...
Article
Little research exists examining interactions between law enforcement officers (LEOs) and autistic individuals. The present study includes responses from 130 LEOs who participated in autism-specific training and completed surveys assessing professional experiences responding to calls that involved individuals with known autism spectrum disorder (ASD) diagnosis. The purpose of the present study was to determine the types of incidents LEOs respond to involving autistic people, and the level of force used in response to the incident. Analysis revealed four categories captured the majority of reported incidents: disruptive behavior, suspected abuse/neglect, elopement, and noncriminal behavior. The most commonly reported responses by LEOs included providing support and extreme controlling behaviors, with female officers more likely to report utilizing supportive behaviors and less force compared to male counterparts.
... Although there is limited research examining compliance and social vulnerability in relation to bullying in schools (Chandler et al., 2019;Sofronoff et al., 2011), specific factors undergirding criminal victimization remain unclear, and highlight a need for future research. Research has found that knowledge of ASD is lacking among the general population (Brewer et al., 2017;Mogavero, 2019) and increasing ASD awareness could positively impact victimization rates (Morrison et al., 2020;Sasson & Morrison, 2019). The same can be said for decreasing or eliminating sensationalized media portrayals that paint autistic individuals as more violent (Weiss & Fardella, 2018), which put autistic individuals at further risk for a CJS interaction (Farley et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
Autism spectrum disorder prevalence is rising, and as this population enters adulthood, preliminary research has identified high rates of contact with the criminal justice system. Policy and programmatic reform are crucial given reported negative and violent outcomes for autistic individuals when encountering the criminal justice system. Given the size and scope of the entire criminal justice system, identifying priorities and opportunities for change is critical, and must be rooted in evidence-based findings to maximize impact and scalability. This article provides a systematic review of the literature on autism spectrum disorder and criminal justice system intersections, analyzed through a convergent qualitative synthesis. As the extant literature is diverse and employs a variety of study methods, this review allows for an analysis across study types. The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis structure was utilized and captured 89 total articles from six databases. Studies are grouped by the Sequential Intercept Model, which offers a framework for analyzing criminal justice system dimensions, and informs where research at the intersection of autism spectrum disorder and the criminal justice system is most prevalent. Themes were identified at each intercept and described through key study findings to articulate implications and guidance for policy, practice, and future research to promote equitable justice for autistic individuals. Lay abstract The number of people with autism spectrum disorder has increased, and as this population ages, research is showing high rates of contact with the criminal justice system among this group. Social and communication differences that autistic individuals experience can act as a risk factor during these interactions, as shown by public reports of negative and violent encounters between autistic individuals and the law enforcement. There is a clear need for evidence-based strategies to reduce high rates of contact and to improve outcomes when an interaction occurs. This article provides a systematic review of research on autism spectrum disorder and criminal justice system to compile this evidence base. The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis structure was used to identify 89 articles after searching six databases. The Sequential Intercept Model describes the criminal justice system as different stages, or intercepts, that are connected, and the Sequential Intercept Model serves as an overall framework to organize the included articles. Articles were analyzed to identify research themes at each intercept, which offer guidance for policy and program changes that support equitable justice for autistic individuals.
... For incidents where an autism diagnosis was not disclosed, both autistic adults and parent/carers reported that this was due to a fear that disclosure would lead to a negative outcome, based on the perception of the person involved in the interaction that police did not have a good understanding of autism or autistic people. The potential for misinterpretation of behaviour has been highlighted in previous studies, not only from the perspective of autistic people but also from police officers themselves (Mogavero et al. 2019;Railey et al. 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
This study aimed to describe the experiences of autistic people who had interacted with police in Australia in the previous 5 years. Fifty autistic adults and 61 parent/carers completed a questionnaire and 30 participants took part in an interview. Participants were most commonly interacting with police in the context of seeking assistance or as victims of crime. Autistic adults were largely unsatisfied with their interactions and reluctant to disclose their autism. Parent/carers reported significantly higher satisfaction than autistic adults and incidents involving children were rated more highly than those involving adults. Suggestions for improved interactions included increased autism awareness amongst police and use of appropriate accommodations. Areas for future research in relation to the evaluation of police training is discussed.
Poster
Full-text available
Accommodations for Individuals with Autism while Interacting with the Criminal Justice System
Article
Full-text available
Objectives The article addresses the issue of human disappearances in four Eastern European countries – Russia, Belarus, Ukraine, and Lithuania. Although there are significant discrepancies between the four concerning economic welfare and individuals’ mobility, their search procedures are quite similar. Material and methods There have been several studies regarding human disappearances and police search investigations in those countries. However, there have been no comparative reviews of this kind so far. Results The authors also highlighted the areas that require improvement, pointing to some common deficiencies such as a selective approach to human disappearances, the lack of specialized search units within the national police forces, and the somewhat limited use of the modern internet-based media. The issues relating to the identification of unknown human, which is a crucial part of the police search investigations, were addressed as well Conclusions The authors also highlighted the areas that require improvement, pointing to some common deficiencies such as a selective approach to human disappearances, the lack of specialized search units within the national police forces, and the somewhat limited use of the modern internet-based media. The issues relating to the identification of unknown human, which is a crucial part of the police search investigations, were addressed as well
Article
Full-text available
The National Registry of Exonerations tracks cases of individuals who have been wrongly convicted and exonerated since 1989. Their most recent report revealed that 12% of those exonerated gave false confessions. The current study used data from the National Registry of Exonerations and focused on a sample of 2,378 individuals convicted of felonies with a focus on sexual offenses, as such crimes tend to be viewed as particularly heinous with increased consequences for those convicted. After examining various legal and extralegal factors, the results showed that those accused of sexual offenses, particularly sexual murders, had a greater likelihood of giving a false confession than those accused of non‐sexual offenses. The likelihood of falsely confessing was higher among juveniles, those with mental illness and intellectual disabilities, and cases with multiple suspects. Females had a greater odds of falsely confessing to murder than males. Implications and recommendations are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: To investigate salient predictors of self-reported false confessions. The conditions included mild intellectual disability (ID), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), ADHD, conduct disorder (CD), psychiatric symptoms, and compliance. Method: The sample consisted of 386 male Scottish prisoners, who completed a diagnostic ADHD interview, psychometric tests, and a questionnaire about false confessions they had given to police over their lifetime. Results: There was a high rate of reported false confessions (33.4%); the reason for the majority (62.2%) being to “cover up for somebody else.” CD, ADHD, psychological distress and psychiatric symptoms, and compliance were all significant predictors of false confession. When examining the influence of underlying domains of ADHD, it was found that hyperactivity/impulsivity drove the association with false confession after adjusting for coexisting CD. Conclusion: CD and ADHD were the two most powerful predictors of false confessions, with CD mediating some of the effects of ADHD.
Article
Full-text available
Psychological vulnerabilities in police suspects may interfere with the demands of police interrogations, and thereby increase the risk of an unreliable statement, or even a false confession. This study examined: (1) the prevalence of a number of psychological vulnerabilities in police arrestees, and (2) the views of police officers on identifying vulnerable suspects. Both have not been studied previously in the Dutch context. Psychological assessments of a sample of police suspects (N = 149) showed that about 60% rated positive on a mental health screen, and, compared to the general Dutch population, levels of psychopathology, depression, anxiety, stress and interrogative suggestibility were significantly higher. In a second study in police detectives (N = 103), 55% stated that they had not interrogated a vulnerable suspect within the previous 12-month period, and again 55% mentioned that they did not take any special precautions when interrogating vulnerable suspects. Forty-two per cent of police detectives took precautions when interrogating vulnerable suspects, such as consulting their supervisor, a police psychologist, the public prosecutor or a specialised police interrogator. The two studies together indicate that police officers seriously underestimate the base rate of psychological vulnerabilities among suspects. Implications for police interrogation training and supervision are provided.
Article
Full-text available
Deficits in safety skills and communication deficits place individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) at an increased risk of danger. We used a multiple-probe across-participants design to evaluate the effects of video modeling and programming common stimuli to teach low- and high-tech help-seeking responses to children with ASD when lost. Participants acquired answering or making a FaceTime® call and exchanging an identification card in contrived and natural settings. Responses generalized to novel community settings and maintained during a one- and two-week follow-up. Social validity measures showed that the procedures and outcomes of the study were acceptable to indirect and direct consumers, and immediate and extended community members. Implications are that children with ASD can effectively be taught both low- and high-tech help-seeking responses when lost.
Article
False confessions represent a significant problem for the criminal and juvenile justice systems and juveniles may be at particular risk for falsely confessing. In part, this risk may be due to juveniles’ greater likelihood of waiving Miranda rights and, consequently, undergoing interrogation, as well as their heightened suggestibility and greater susceptibility to and compliance with authority figures compared to adults. With data from 260 participants in correctional facilities, this study compared juveniles’ (n = 168) and adults’ (n = 92) self-reported likelihood of false confessions. Younger juveniles reported significantly greater false confession propensity than did adults, especially in response to directive interrogation techniques. Older juveniles did not differ significantly from adults in their self-reported likelihood of false confession. Miranda rights comprehension mediated the relationship between age and false confession likelihood. These findings suggest the need for greater protections of younger juveniles during interrogations.
Article
Although it is known that interrogation tactics can elicit false confessions and interviewer manner may determine the outcome of an interview, the combined effects of questioning technique and interviewer manner on false confessions have not been examined empirically. Following a false accusation of theft, participants were interviewed in one of four questioning conditions (minimisation, repetitive questioning, leading questions, and nonleading questions) in which interviewers adopted a stern or friendly manner. Perceptions of pressure to confess and interviewer behaviours were measured. Significantly more false confessions were elicited using nonleading questions rather than repetitive questioning. More false confessions were elicited in the friendly interviewer condition than in the stern interviewer condition. Neither interviewer manner nor questioning technique had a significant effect on subjective ratings of pressure to confess. The finding that false confessions may be elicited in the absence of coercive tactics may have implications for informing best practices in investigative interviewing.
Book
Provides a comprehensive and up-to-date review of the development of the science behind the psychology of false confessions. Four decades ago, little was known or understood about false confessions and the reasons behind them. So much has changed since then due in part to the diligent work done by Gisli H. Gudjonsson. This eye-opening book by the Icelandic/British clinical forensic psychologist, who in the mid 1970s had worked as detective in Reykjavik, offers a complete and current analysis of how the study of the psychology of false confessions came about, including the relevant theories and empirical/experimental evidence base. It also provides a reflective review of the gradual development of the science and how it can be applied to real life cases. Based on Gudjonsson's personal account of the biggest murder investigations in Iceland's history, as well as other landmark cases, The Psychology of False Confessions: Forty Years of Science and Practice takes readers inside the minds of those who sit on both sides of the interrogation table to examine why confessions to crimes occur even when the confessor is innocent. Presented in three parts, the book covers how the science of studying false confessions emerged and grew to become a regular field of practice. It then goes deep into the investigation of the mid-1970s assumed murders of two men in Iceland and the people held responsible for them. It finishes with an in-depth psychological analysis of the confessions of the six people convicted. Written by an expert extensively involved in the development of the science and its application to real life cases. Covers the most sensational murder cases in Iceland's history. Deep analysis of the 'Reykjavik Confessions' adds crucial evidence to understanding how and why coerced-internalized false confessions occur, and their detrimental and lasting effects on memory. The Psychology of False Confessions: Forty Years of Science and Practice is an important source book for students, academics, criminologists, and clinical, forensic, and social psychologists and psychiatrists.