ArticlePDF Available

Meditation experience predicts negative reinforcement learning and is associated with attenuated FRN amplitude

Authors:

Abstract

Focused attention meditation (FAM) practices are cognitive control exercises where meditators learn to maintain focus and attention in the face of distracting stimuli. Previous studies have shown that FAM is both activating and causing plastic changes to the mesolimbic dopamine system and some of its target structures, particularly the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and striatum. Feedback-based learning also depends on these systems and is known to be modulated by tonic dopamine levels. Capitalizing on previous findings that FAM practices seem to cause dopamine release, the present study shows that FAM experience predicts learning from negative feedback on a probabilistic selection task. Furthermore, meditators exhibited attenuated feedback-related negativity (FRN) as compared with nonmeditators and this effect scales with meditation experience. Given that reinforcement learning and FRN are modulated by dopamine levels, a possible explanation for our findings is that FAM practice causes persistent increases in tonic dopamine levels which scale with amount of practice, thus altering feedback processing.
Meditation experience predicts negative reinforcement learning and is
associated with attenuated FRN amplitude
Paul Knytl
1
&Bertram Opitz
1
#The Author(s) 2018
Abstract
Focused attention meditation (FAM) practices are cognitive control exercises where meditators learn to maintain focus and
attention in the face of distracting stimuli. Previous studies have shown that FAM is both activating and causing plastic changes
to the mesolimbic dopamine system and some of its target structures, particularly the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and
striatum. Feedback-based learning also depends on these systems and is known to be modulated by tonic dopamine levels.
Capitalizing on previous findings that FAM practices seem to cause dopamine release, the present study shows that FAM
experience predicts learning from negative feedback on a probabilistic selection task. Furthermore, meditators exhibited atten-
uated feedback-related negativity (FRN) as compared with nonmeditators and this effect scales with meditation experience.
Given that reinforcement learning and FRN are modulated by dopamine levels, a possible explanation for our findings is that
FAM practice causes persistent increases in tonic dopamine levels which scale with amount of practice, thus altering feedback
processing.
Keywords Feedback-learning bias .Feedback-related negativity .FRN .Reinforcement learning .Meditation .Dopamine .
ACC .Striatum
Since the turn of the century meditation has gone from relative
obscurity in Western academia to explosive growth in interest
and research. Only around 60 academic papers were published
on the topic of mindfulness in 2003; by 2013 that number
jumped to 600 (Shonin, Van Gordon, & Griffiths, 2013).
The appeal is understandable; claimed benefits of meditation
range from reduced stress (Kabat-Zinn, 2003) to improved
immune function (Davidson et al., 2003), from improved at-
tention and lower anxiety (Tang et al., 2007) to a novel treat-
ment for depression (Teasdale et al., 2000), and has even been
recommended as a possible way to manage symptoms of psy-
chosis (Shonin, Van Gordon, & Griffiths, 2014).
While some researchers have been pressing forward ex-
ploring the possible applications of meditation, others have
been trying to understand what exactly is occurring in the
nervous system during and after meditation, what systems
are involved, and what the long-term effects of practice might
be. In part due to methodological issues, inconsistent
operationalization, the relative lack of longitudinal studies,
and the infancy of the field, the precise mechanisms and
long- term effects of various meditation styles are still not
entirely clear (Cahn & Polich, 2006;Hölzeletal.,2011;
Tang, Hölzel, & Posner, 2015; Vago & Silbersweig, 2012).
There has been recognition that effective study of these
practices requires precise operationalization of the concept.
The term meditation refers to a diverse group of cognitive
practices which share some similarities and some fundamental
differences (Lutz, Slagter, Dunne, & Davidson, 2008). To the
uninitiated, the matter is further confused by the casual use of
relevant terms by both the public and academia. For example,
the term mindfulness can refer to a type of meditation, a trait,
and a state of mind (Vago & Silbersweig, 2012). To address
this, a framework has been introduced which categorizes med-
itative practices into three groups based on their primary cog-
nitive strategy: focused attention meditation (FAM), open-
monitoring meditation (OMM), and loving-kindness medita-
tion (LKM; Hölzel et al., 2011; Lutz et al., 2008; Vago &
Silbersweig, 2012). Of particular interest in the present study
is FAM. FAM is central to many meditative traditions, such as
Buddhist samatha and vipassana meditation and their deriva-
tive secular mindfulness practices and clinical interventions
such as mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR),
*Bertram Opitz
b.opitz@surrey.ac.uk
1
School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, Surrey GU2
7XH, UK
Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-00665-0
mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT), and other
practices such as transcendental meditation (Harvey, 2015;
Lutz et al., 2008; Vago & Silbersweig, 2012). FAM is charac-
terized by the establishment, monitoring, and maintenance of
attention on a chosen sensory object, such as the sensation of
breathing (Lutz et al., 2008).
What is striking about FAM is that the cognitive processes
invoked during practice bear close resemblance to the process-
es that the brains mesencephalic dopamine system and its
target areas are thought to perform. For instance, others have
hypothesized that the continual establishment, monitoring,
and reestablishment of attention on an object of meditation
during FAM should elicit activity in those brain areas already
associated with conflict monitoring and sustained attention,
such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and the an-
terior cingulate cortex (ACC; Lutz et al., 2008). An early
review of 12 neuroimaging studies of meditation found nu-
merous brain areas active during meditation, such as the stri-
atum, hippocampus, thalamus, along with the ACC and dlPFC
(Cahn & Polich, 2006). As this review predates Lutz et al.s
(2008) operationalization, it does not directly specify what
kind of meditation may be involved (FAM, OMM, LKM,
etc.), even including some studies of Christian prayer. This
inclusion ofa wide range of practices involved in the reviewed
studies may account for the diverse brain areas reportedly
active in meditators. Studies looking only at practices having
a clear FA component (e.g., Buddhist and secular mindful-
ness) consistently report brain areas involved in attention
and conflict monitoring, including the dlPFC and particularly
the ACC, to be reliably active in meditators (for a review, see
Tang et al., 2015).
There is also evidence of morphological differences and
changes in plasticity in FAM practitioners. A recent anatomical
likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analysis of 21 morphometric
brain imaging studies revealed higher grey and white matter
density in the ACCs of meditators compared with
nonmeditators (Fox et al., 2014). Neuroplastic changes may
also happen relatively quickly after beginning FAM training;
one intervention with an FAM component resulted in higher
connectivity between the ACC and the brain stem after only 11
hours of practice (Tang et al., 2010). These findings suggest
that meditation not only activates areas vital to attention but can
also quickly induce neuroplastic growth in these brain regions.
Neuroimaging studies have also revealed that the striatum,
a core component of the dopamine system, is active during
meditation. In one study, [
11
C]raclopride, a radio ligand that
binds competitively with dopamine D2 receptors, had been
used to measure participantsdopamine tone in the striatum,
first while they listened to speech with their eyes closed and
then while they were actively meditating (Kjaer et al., 2002).
Compared with the speech state, the meditation state was as-
sociated with a 65% increase in endogenous dopamine re-
lease. In another investigation, fMRI was used to carry out a
case study of an experienced Buddhist meditator while he
used various meditative techniques including FAM (Hagerty
et al., 2013). The authors reported that this participant
displayed increased activity in the ACC and in the striatum
during the FAM phase. Both of these studies suggest that
practicing FAM activates the dopaminergic system in the cor-
tex and basal ganglia.
The dopaminergic system has been shown to play an im-
portant role in reward, learning, error correction, motor con-
trol, and attention (Schultz, 2002). More precisely, phasic
(fast) bursts of dopamine act as a learning signal which serve
to orient organisms to novel or unexpected stimuli and en-
codes the difference between the expected and actual outcome
of an action, a reward prediction error (RPE), in the form of
either a spike in dopamine if the event was rewarding, or a dip
if the event was not (Schultz, 2002,2013,2016). A seminal
study demonstrated that dysfunctional dopamine levels ex-
plain some perplexing cognitive differences between people
with Parkinsons disease (PD), where dopamine production is
severely reduced, and age and IQ matched controls (Frank,
Seeberger, & OReilly, 2004). While off their dopaminergic
medication, patients suffering from PD performed worse than
controls on a probabilistic selection task (PST)but only
when they received positive feedback. When they received
negative feedback they outperformed controls. When placed
on L-DOPA medication, a dopamine precursor which in-
creases tonic dopamine levels, the pattern reversed with PD
patients outperforming controls on positive feedback-learning
but underperforming on negative feedback-learning (Frank
et al., 2004). Since a person can be said to have a feedback-
learning bias (FLB) if they learn more effectively from either
positive or negative feedback, this study demonstrated that up
or down regulating tonic dopamine levels respectively creates
a positive or negative feedback-learning bias.
The interaction between phasic dopamine bursts and per-
sistent tonic dopamine levels provides a possible reason for
this phenomenon. FLB has been explained using a neuro-
computational model whereby an up-regulated or down-
regulated tonic dopamine signal respectively enhanced or
inhibited sensitivity to a particular type of feedback, thus cre-
ating a FLB (Frank, 2005). In this model, depressed tonic
dopamine levels, such as those found in PD patients off med-
ication, effectively increased the amplitude of phasic dopa-
mine bursts required to activate the Bgo^pathway in the basal
ganglia. At the same time, a smaller magnitude of phasic dip is
required to trigger the threshold that leads to negative feed-
back learning, resulting in a negative FLB. Increasing tonic
dopamine seems to have the opposite effect, amplifying the
effect of phasic bursts, inhibiting the Bno-go^pathway, and
thus leading to a positive feedback-learning bias (Frank,
2005).
This phasic dopaminergic activity is widely believed to be
the cause of one component of the event-related potential
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
(ERP) known as feedback-related negativity (FRN). The FRN
is typically calculated as the difference between negative-
feedback and positive-feedback trial waveforms and is char-
acterized by a frontocentral scalp distribution which peaks
about 250 ms after feedback presentation (San Martín, 2012;
Walsh & Anderson, 2012). The highly influential reinforce-
ment learning (RL) theory of the FRN holds that phasic dips in
dopaminergic activity (negative RPEs) disinhibit the ACC,
which in turn generates the FRN (Holroyd & Coles, 2002).
Hierarchical reinforcement learning (HRL) builds on the suc-
cess of RL theory by breaking complex tasks into simpler sets
of subroutines. This helps to manage the computational de-
mand of real-world learning and decision-making scenarios,
ensuring they are still tractable using the RL algorithm
(Botvinick, 2012). The RL theory of the FRN has also been
updated to leverage the insights gained from HRL (Holroyd &
Yeun g , 2012). This account elaborates on the role of the ACC,
proposing that it is responsible for learning the values of, and
maintaining engagement in, HRL subroutines. It draws on a
number of more recent studies to argue that the ERN/FRN are
in fact a function of an underlying component, sensitive to
reward, termed the reward positivity (RewP), which reflects
a positive, as opposed to negative, RPE. Although there has
been some debate over whether the FRN reflects an underly-
ing RPE, surprise, or response conflict, what remains un-
changed and what is of paramount importance in the present
study, is the abundance of evidence which supports the theory
that the FRN is generated when the ACC responds to phasic
dopaminergic inputs fromthe midbrain and basal ganglia (San
Martín, 2012; Walsh & Anderson, 2012)
Given the tonicphasic interaction in the dopamine system,
it is not surprising that the FRN has also been linked to tonic
dopamine levels. In one study, participants who displayed a
positive FLB on the PST also presented a lower amplitude
FRN than participants who were negatively biased (Frank,
Woroch, & Curran, 2005). Other researchers have shown that
people who are MET homozygous for the val158met COMT
polymorphism, which elevates tonic dopamine levels, also
present an attenuated FRN (Marco-Pallarés et al., 2009), and
that dopamine agonists likewise reduce FRN amplitude
(Santesso et al., 2009). These studies suggest that higher tonic
dopamine levels result in not only a more positive feedback-
learning bias but also a reduction in FRN amplitude.
Due to the overlapping systems involved in reinforcement
learning and FAM, we hypothesized that FAM might induce a
positive FLB and attenuated FRN by upregulating tonic do-
pamine levels. To investigate whether, compared with
nonmeditators, the amount of FAM experience a person has
would predict a more positive FLB and attenuated FRN, we
carried out a study using a version of the probabilistic selec-
tion task (PST) employed by Frank et al. (2005). In this task
participants were required to learn to choose the more reward-
ing of two symbols via trial and error. Positive and negative
feedback was provided on a probabilistic basis, with some
symbols being on average more rewarding than others.
Therefore, when choosing a more rewarding symbol, partici-
pants were more likely to receive positive feedback than they
would from a less rewarding one. We measured FLB and
recorded their FRN while they completed this task. We rea-
soned that if FAM is causing long-term increases to tonic
dopamine levels, meditators should exhibit characteristics of
positively biased feedback learners on the PSTas well as have
an attenuated FRN. We predicted that, if this effect is caused
by FAM practice, both the FLB and the FRN effects should
scale with FAM experience.
Method
Participants
Thirty-five people were recruited from the local student pop-
ulation, Buddhist groups, and the general public. In keeping
with the FAM/OMM/LKM framework, the 23 meditators
among our participants consisted of Buddhist (N= 9), secular
(N= 12), and Qi Gong (N= 2) FAM practitioners. Since the
measures we used are sensitive to tonic dopamine levels, vol-
unteers were prescreened using a self-report questionnaire for
current episodes of major depression or anxiety, and mono-
amine oxidase inhibitors or recreational drug use.
The participants were grouped into a nonmeditator group
(N= 12, mean age = 34.6 years, SD = 14.8 years, range: 22
60, six female), a novice meditator group (N= 12, mean age =
38.6 years, SD =14.3years,range:1859, 10 female) and an
experienced meditator group (N= 11, mean age = 38.2 years,
SD = 13.7 years, range: 2356, six female) based on a median
split
1
on their self-reported meditation experience. We chose
to use years of experience as our grouping variable in keeping
with other studies (Kang et al., 2013). Participants were con-
sidered experienced if they meditated for more than 30 mi-
nutes a week for at least 4 years; this criterion reflects a me-
dian split of our data based on years of experience with the
added proviso that meditators practiced on a weekly basis for
a reasonable amount of time. Mean age was not significantly
different between groups F(2, 32) < 1. A self-report
prescreening questionnaire revealed that, except for one am-
bidextrous participant, all were right-handed. Everyone was a
volunteer, and no compensation was provided beyond reim-
bursement of travel costs.
1
Our findings are relatively robust to alternative quantifications of FAM ex-
perience. See the Appendix for details and discussion.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
Apparatus
Participants sat in a dimly lit, electrically shielded, sound at-
tenuated booth. On a desk in front of them was a 17-in. LCD
monitor used to display stimuli at a screen resolution of 1280
× 1024 at 60 Hz. The screen was approximately 1 meter from
the subjects face at eye level. Responses were recorded via
keyboard. Besides the EEG amplifier and screen, all EMR
sources were located outside the booth, and DC lighting was
used.
Design and procedure
This experiment used a between-subjects design examining
the effect of three levels of FAM experience (control, novice,
experienced) on two feedback learning measures: feedback-
learning bias (FLB) and FRN amplitude (FRN).
We employed a version of the probabilistic selection task
(PST) procedure used by Frank et al. (2005). During the EEG
recording, participants were shown pairs of stimuli consisting
of Japanese hiragana characters. Following the procedure
from cited above, after a short practice block, stimuli were
presented in three blocks each containing a training and test
phase. Each block used one of three stimuli sets whose pre-
sentation order was randomized across participants.
In the training phase, participants were instructed to learn
via trial-and-error to select the hiragana symbol most likely to
provide positive feedback from each of three pairs (represent-
ed here using roman letters: AB, CD, EF). Pairs were present-
ed in random order, and the side that the superior symbol
appeared on was pseudorandomized such that symbols ap-
peared equally often on either side of the screen. If no selec-
tion was made within 1,000 ms, the message Bno response
detected^was displayed. Participants were provided with pos-
itive or negative feedback to guide their learning; however, as
summarized in Table 1, this feedback was probabilistic. For
example, choosing Ain the AB pair would result in positive
feedback (+10 points, green colour, Arial, size matched to the
hiragana characters) 80% of the time, whereas choosing B
would result in positive feedback 20% of the time. In order
to maximize their score in each training block, participants
should have learned to choose Aover B,Cover D,andEover
F. Note that this could be accomplished by either learning to
select the winners (A, C, E), avoid the losers (B, D, F), or both.
We enforced the same training criterion as Frank et al. (2005);
after 60 trials, performance levels were checked (65% Ain the
AB pair, 60% Cin CD, and 50% Ein EF), and if the criterion
was met, participants were released to the test phase. If the
criterion was not met, participants would continue up to a
maximum of 120 trials.
During the test phase, participants were told to use their gut
feeling to choose the superior symbol from the pairs present-
ed, and this time no feedback would be provided. During this
phase novel combinations (e.g., AC or BD) were presented to
participants. This was done in order to assess which feedback
type was most effective in teaching them and how accurate
their learning was. Since Amost reliably provided positive
feedback and Bmost reliably provided negative feedback,
participants should have learned to choose Awhenever it
was presented in a novel pair while avoiding Bwhenever it
was presented in a novel pair. Our paradigm differed from the
procedure in Frank et al. (2005) in two ways. First, we used
points (+10 or 10 points for positive or negative feedback,
respectively) instead of smiley faces, and Xicons as the feed-
back graphics, as preexperimental paradigm testing revealed
higher task engagement when a points system was used.
Second, the number of pairings in the test phase containing
Aor Bwere increased by one, while all other pairings were
reduced by one. This increased reliability without increasing
task duration.
After participants had finished the task, they were provided
with facilities to clean themselves up. They were then
debriefed regarding the full nature and purpose of the
experiment.
Data recording and analysis
Behavioural data
The PST was created and run using the E-Prime 2.0 stimulus
presentation suite. Responses from the training and test phase
were recorded via keyboard. The mean accuracy scores across
all pairs and blocks involving Aand Bwere calculated. The
FLB score is the mean difference, as a percentage, between
correct Choose Aand Avoid Bresponses. The AB trial was
excluded from this calculation since the preferred strategy
(Choose Aor Avoid B)usedtoansweritcannotbedissociated.
Electrophysiological data
During the behavioural task, a 32-channel continuous EEG
recording of each participant was made using Ag/AgCl elec-
trodes. Electrodes were affixed to an appropriately sized EEG
cap (EASYCAP GmbH) worn by each participant. The
Table 1 Summary of the probability a participant would receive
positive feedback when selecting a given character
Character Probability of receiving positive feedback
A 0.80
B 0.20
C 0.70
D 0.30
E 0.60
F 0.40
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
extended 1020 electrode layout (Klem, Otto, Jasper, & Elger,
1999) was employed. Signals from two additional electrodes
placed on the participants left and right mastoid were record-
ed. All electrodes recorded relative to a common average ref-
erence. Horizontal and vertical electrooculograms were record-
ed with electrodes located above and below the participants
left eye, and on the outer canthi of both eyes. Interelectrode
impedances were kept below 10 kΩ. All channels were ampli-
fied with a band pass from DC to 70 Hz and A/D converted
with 16-bit resolution at a rate of 500 Hz.
ERPs were calculated using the off-line data processing
suite included in the BrainAnalyzer2 program (Brain
Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany). Data preprocessing
consisted of a digital band-pass filter from 0.3 Hz to 30 Hz
(3 dB cut-off) to eliminate low-frequency signal drifts and
high-frequency artefacts. Eye-movement artefacts were elim-
inated using an automated independent components approach
as implemented in BrainAnalyzer2. Automatic artefact rejec-
tion (gradient criterion: voltage variation of more than 75 μV
in two subsequent time points, amplitude criterion: any volt-
age exceeding ± 100 μV and low activity criterion: 0.5 μV/50
ms) was applied to all channels to mark segments contaminat-
ed by additional artefacts. These recording epochs were ex-
cluded from further analysis. Recordings were segmented
with epochs ranging from 100 to +800 ms with respect to
the onset of the feedback. Artefact-free segments from the
training phases were averaged separately for each participant
to form 4 ERPs by feedback type and valence (true positive,
true negative, false positive, false negative). The 100-ms prior
to feedback onset served as the baseline. These averages were
digitally re-referenced to average mastoid activity.
We created the FRN difference wave by subtracting the
true positive ERP from true negative ERP for each participant
at the FCz electrode (e.g., Pfabigan, Alexopoulos, Bauer, &
Sailer, 2011). We excluded false feedback trials from the anal-
ysis as recent work by Ernst and Steinhauser (2018)has
shown that the FRN is attenuated in unreliable (i.e., false)
feedback trials. As participants were informed in the present
study that stimuli behaviour would be reliable mostbut not
allof the time, we improved signal-to-noise ratio by exclud-
ing false feedback trials. The proportion of true feedback trials
was constant at 70% across all participants.
Since persons exhibiting a positive bias on behavioural
measures may have delayed FRNs (Frank et al., 2005), auto-
matic peak detection was employed to pick the most negative
value of the difference wave within an epoch ranging between
220 and 320 ms postfeedback. This epoch was selected as it
should encapsulate peak FRN activity, approximately 265 ms
poststimulus (Gehring & Willoughby, 2002; San Martín,
2012; Walsh & Anderson, 2012). The mean amplitude in a
window ±50 ms of this peak was exported for analysis. To
assess whether any change in the FRN across groups was
driven by the ERP response to positive or negative feedback,
the mean amplitude of the same time window around the FRN
peak was also analysed for each condition specific ERP
separately.
Results
Behavioural data
Average training-phase performance across all blocks and
groups was 64%, indicating learning occurred. A 3 × 3
(Block × Group) GreenhouseGeisser corrected (ɛ= .75)
mixed-design ANOVA was conducted revealing no signifi-
cant effect of block, F(1.50, 47.81) = 0.91, p=.38,group,
F(2, 32) = 0.69, p=.51,oranyinteractionF(2.99, 47.81) =
0.56, p= .65, on training phase performance. There were no
significant group differences for the ratio of true positive to
true negative feedback received, F(2, 32) = 1.39, p=.26,or
the ratio of total positive to total negative feedback received,
F(2, 32) = 1.11, p= .34. Likewise, no group differences in the
total number of trials performed F(2, 32) = 0.27, p=.76,were
observed. In addition, overall test-phase performance did not
differ between groups F(2, 32) = 0.05, p=.95.
The primary measure of interest from the behavioural data
was feedback-learning bias (FLB). This quantity measures
whether participants are biased towards learning Choose A
or Avoid Bbehaviour. The value is computed by subtracting
correct test-phase performance on Avoid Btrials from Choose
Atrials. AB trial performance is excluded from this calcula-
tion, as it is not possible to disentangle which strategy (Choose
A, or Avoid B) was used to learn the correct response. Thus,
when FLB has a positive value, participants learned better
from positive feedback; likewise, a negative value indicates
negative feedback was more effective in training a participant.
The nonmeditators had the most negative FLB (M=0.11,
SE = 0.03). Meditators were positively biased, with the novice
group (M=0.00,SE = 0.04) being less positively biased than
the experienced group (M=0.05,SE = 0.06). An independent
ANOVA was performed to determine the effect of meditation
experience on FLB. As hypothesized, there was an effect of
meditation experience on FLB, F(2, 32) = 3.41, p=.045,ƞ
p
2
=
.18. Planned contrasts revealed that meditators were signifi-
cantly more positively biased than were nonmeditators, t(32)
=2.51,p=.017,r= .41. However, no significant difference
between the novice and experienced meditation groups, t(32)
=0.80,p=.43,r= .14, was found.
We added participant age to the model to determine if this
had an effect on FLB. The ANOVA revealed that while there
was still a significant effect of meditation experience on FLB,
F(2, 31) = 3.42, p=.045,ƞ
p
2
= .19, there was no significant
effect of age, F(1, 31) = 0.22, p=.64,ƞ
p
2
=.007.
A post hoc analysis was carried out in order to determine
whether this shift in feedback processing was driven by an
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
increase or decrease in Choose Aor Avoid Bperformance.
Approach and avoid performance is summarized by group in
Fig. 1. The control group had the lowest Choose Atest per-
formance (M=0.53, SE = 0.04), followed by the novice group
(M=0.59, SE = 0.03), while the experienced meditators had
the best performance (M=0.60, SE = 0.03). In terms of Avoid
Bperformance, the control group had the highest performance
(M=0.64, SE = 0.03), followed by the novice group (M=
0.59, SE = 0.03), while the experienced meditators performed
worst (M=0.55, SE = 0.04). A 2 × 3 (Approach/Avoid ×
Meditation Group) mixed-design ANOVA was performed to
determine whether there was an interaction between response
behaviour type and meditation experience on participant test
performance. There was no main effect of behaviour type,
F(1, 32) = 0.50, p= .49, or meditation experience, F(2, 32)
=0.05,p= .95; however, there was a significant interaction
between behaviour type and meditation experience on test
performance F(2, 32) = 3.41, p=.045,ƞ
p
2
=.18.Basedon
our hypothesis that meditation may cause these effects, we
conducted a post hoc linear regression to examine whether
meditation experience independently predicts Choose Aand
Avoi d Bperformance. Mediation experience did not signifi-
cantly predict Choose Aperformance, F(1, 33) = 1.06, p=.31,
R
2
= .03 (see Fig. 2). However, meditation experience did
predict Avoid Bperformance, F(1, 33) = 5.23, p=.029,R
2
= .14 (see Fig. 3), indicating that as meditation experience
increases, participants do not learn as well from negative feed-
back trials.
Overall, these results indicate that feedback-learning bias
becomes more positive with meditation experience and that
this cannot be explained by age. However, despite an upward
trend, the fact that novice and experienced meditators were not
significantly different on FLB is not supportive of our hypoth-
esis that there would be a significant increase of this effect
with meditation experience. Post hoc analysis revealed a sig-
nificant interaction between approach and avoid performance
and meditation experience, but despite trend-level evidence
suggestive of equal contribution of changes in both Choose
Aand Avoid Bperformance, our sample lacked the power to
further explicate the interaction at the group level.
EEG data
For each participant, the main EEG measure was the scalp
voltage at FCz, averaged across a 100-ms window centred
on the peak of the FRN difference wave between 220 and
320 ms postfeedback. The difference wave was created by
subtracting the true positive-feedback-locked waveform from
the true negative-feedback-locked waveform at FCz.
The nonmeditation group had the highest amplitude FRN
(M=4.07 μV, SE =0.57μV; see Fig. 2a), peaking at 279 ms
poststimulus, followed by the novice group (M=2.70 μV,
SE =0.44μV; see Fig. 2b), peaking at 272 ms poststimulus,
and finally the experienced group (M=1.14 μV, SE =0.27
μV; see Fig. 2c), peaking at 263 ms poststimulus. The FRN
was significantly different from zero for all groups:
nonmeditators, t(11) = 7.09, p< .001; novice meditators,
t(11) = 6.09, p< .001; experienced meditators, t(10) =
4.19, p= .002. An independent ANOVA was performed to
examine the effect of meditation experience on FRN ampli-
tude. As expected, analysis of these data revealed that there
was a main effect of meditation experience on FRN ampli-
tude, F(2, 32) = 10.18, p<.001,ƞ
p
2
= .39 (see Fig. 3). The
timing and frontocentral scalp distribution (see Figs. 4and 5)
of these ERPs is characteristic of the classic FRN (Baker &
Holroyd, 2011; Walsh & Anderson, 2012). There was no ef-
fect of meditation experience on FRN latency, F(32) < 1.
Fig. 1 Choose Aand Avoid Bperformance by group and feedback type. Error bars represent SE
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
Planned contrasts revealed that there was a significant dif-
ference between nonmeditators and meditators, t(32) = 3.88,
p<.001,r= .57, and also between novice and experienced
meditators, t(32) = 2.40, p=.022,r= .39. We added partic-
ipant age to the model to determine if this had an effect on the
FRN. The ANOVA revealed that while there was still a sig-
nificant effect of meditation experience on FRN amplitude,
F(2, 31) = 9.87, p<.001,ƞ
p
2
= .39, there was no significant
effect of age, F(1, 31) = 2.92, p=.10,ƞ
p
2
= .09. These results
indicate that the FRN attenuates as meditation experience in-
creases, and this effect is not explained by participant age. It is
also worth noting that although weaker, when false feedback
trials are included in our analysis, the direction of our results
remain unchanged.
As with the FLB data, we decomposed the difference wave
by feedback type to determine whether the group differences
in FRN were driven by an increase or decrease in either the
positive-feedback-elicited or negative-feedback-elicited
waveforms. The condition specific voltage at the FRN differ-
ence wave peak is summarized by feedback type for each
group in Fig. 6and Table 2.A2×3(FeedbackTyp
Meditation Group) mixed-design ANOVA was conducted
and revealed a main effect of feedback type, F(1, 32) =
84.04, p<.001,ƞ
p
2
= 0.72, confirming that negative feedback
elicited a more negative-going waveform than positive feed-
back did in all groups. While there was no main effect of
meditation experience on FRN voltage, F(2, 32) = 0.44, p=
.65, there was a significant feedback type by meditation group
interaction, F(2, 32) = 10.18, p<.001,ƞ
p
2
=.39,indicating
that meditation experience differently affects positive-
feedback-elicited and negative-feedback-elicited voltage.
Post hoc independent ANOVA was performed on the
Fig. 2 Choose Aaccuracy as a function of years of meditation experience.
Fig. 3 Av oid Baccuracy as a function of years of meditation experience
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
Fig. 4 Feedback-locked FRN difference wave and condition specific
ERPs at FCz elicited by true feedback trials for (a) the nonmeditation
group, (b) the novice group, and (c) the experienced group. Following
EEG convention, up is negative. Feedback stimulus occurs at zero ms.
Negative-feedback-elicited waveform in blue, positive-feedback elicited
waveform in red. Note the difference wave attenuates between 220 and
320 ms as meditation experience increases. Also shown, scalp distribu-
tion of the difference wave for each group. Peak activity has the
frontomedial distribution and timing characteristic of the FRN. (Colour
figure online)
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
condition specific ERPs to attempt to explicate the significant
interaction. Neither the positive feedback ERP, F(2, 32) =
0.05, p=.95,ƞ
p
2
< .01, nor the negative feedback ERP, F(2,
32) = 1.07, p=.36,ƞ
p
2
= .06, revealed a significant effect of
meditation experience on ERP voltage. Thus, despite a possi-
ble trend-level effect of meditation experience on negative
feedback voltage, our sample lacks the power to explain the
interaction further.
As with the FLB, to investigate whether meditation expe-
rience predicts FRN amplitude, a linear regression was con-
ducted (see Fig. 7and Table 3). This analysis reveals a trend of
reduced FRN amplitude as a function of increased meditation
experience. Meditation experience did not significantly pre-
dict condition-specific ERP amplitudes, suggesting that this
effect is driven by changes in both positive-feedback and
negative-feedback processing.
Discussion
The aim of this study was to determine whether or not there
was an association between FAM experience and behavioural
and electrophysiological measures of reinforcement learning.
As predicted, the behavioural data revealed that meditators
were more positively biased feedback learners. These differ-
ences appear to be driven primarily by differences in negative
rather than positive feedback processing. The ERP data re-
vealed that the amplitude of the FRN was significantly smaller
Fig. 5 FRN amplitude at FCz as a function of FAM experience; error bars are SEM. All three groups differed significantly from one another, indicating
that FRN amplitude decreases with meditation experience. *p.05. **p.01. ***p.001
Fig. 6 Condition-specific (true positive and negative feedback) elicited voltages, 50 ms either side of the difference wave peak at FCz, grouped by
meditation experience. Error bars are SEM
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
in meditators than in controls, and that these effects scale with
meditation experience. Furthermore, these group differences
cannot be explained by age, disease, or medication.
One potential explanation for these data is that striatal do-
pamine signalling varies as a function of meditation experi-
ence. First, PET has revealed that in experienced meditators,
the act of meditating is associated with a significant increase in
tonic dopamine levels in the striatum (Kjaer et al., 2002).
Second, others using the probabilistic selection task (PST)
reported a shift towards a positive FLB after using l-dopa to
increase dopamine levels in Parkinsonspatients(Franketal.,
2004). Despite some controversy surrounding the reliability of
the PST (Baker, Stockwell, & Holroyd, 2013) and its sensi-
tivity to the pharmacological manipulation of dopamine levels
in the brain in Parkinsons patients (Grogan et al., 2017), there
are a large number of studies demonstrating a trend of in-
creased dopamine levels being associated with a morepositive
feedback-learning bias (FLB) and relatively lower dopamine
levels being associated with more negative FLB (Cox et al.,
2015; Frank & Hutchison, 2009; Frank & Kong, 2008; Frank
&OReilly, 2006; Frank et al., 2004; Klein et al., 2007;
Lighthall, Gorlick, Schoeke, Frank, & Mather, 2013;
Smittenaar et al., 2012; Voon et al., 2010). Third, a recent
PETstudy of healthy individuals has demonstrated that striatal
D1 and D2 signalling respectively predicts learning from pos-
itive and negative outcomes on the PST (Cox et al., 2015). D1
receptors have low affinity and therefore respond more to
phasic DA activity and less to tonic dopamine activity. D2
receptors, meanwhile, have high affinity and so are more
strongly influenced by changes in tonic DA (Frank, 2005).
Since phasic signalling is largely driven by D1 receptor activ-
ity which is correlated with Choose A,notAvoidB,perfor-
mance on the PST (Cox et al., 2015), in the present study if
differencesinphasicsignallingweresolelyormostlyrespon-
sible for the observed differences in feedback processing, we
would expect to see a stronger correlation between meditation
experience and variation in positive feedback processing than
in negative feedback processing. However, the opposite ap-
pears to be the case: meditation experience seems to affect
only negative-feedback learning. This suggests that the effect
is driven by differences in striatal D2 signallingnot D1 sig-
nalling. Since D2 signalling is sensitive to changes in tonic
dopamine or D2 receptor availability, our findings could indi-
cate that one or both of these may increase as meditation
experience increases. In other words, since tonic DA in the
striatum increases during meditation and relatively higher ton-
ic DA manifests as poorer Avoid Bperformance on the PST,
our behavioural data could reflect persistent increases in
striatal tonic DA or D2 receptor availability as a function of
meditation experience.
Table 2 Condition-specific ERP voltages
Condition Group Voltage (μV) SE
Negative feedback Nonmeditators 0.95 1.63
Novice meditators 2.68 1.17
Experienced meditators 3.65 1.04
Positive feedback Nonmeditators 5.02 1.75
Novice meditators 5.38 1.17
Experienced meditators 4.79 0.94
Fig. 7 Linear regression models of the effect of meditation experience on FRN amplitude at FCz
Table 3 Summary of linear regression fit to condition-specific and dif-
ference wave FRN voltages at FCz at difference wave peak
Feedback type Fpr
Negativepositive 4.03 .053 .33
Positive .08 .78 .05
Negative .29 .60 .09
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
This interpretation of our behavioural data is supported by
our ERP data. There are a number of competing theories on
FRN/ERN generation (San Martín, 2012; Walsh & Anderson,
2012); however, the highly influential reinforcement learning
theory of the FRN links the FRN to the dopaminergic process-
ing of feedback. The theory holds that errors and negative
feedback result in dips in phasic dopaminergic activity, which
disinhibits the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and generates
the negative-going condition-specific component of the FRN,
while positive feedback causes bursts of phasic dopaminergic
activity, which inhibit the ACC and results in the more
positive-going condition-specific component of the FRN
(Holroyd & Coles, 2002; San Martín, 2012;Walsh&
Anderson, 2012). Due to the tonic/phasic DA interaction in
the basal ganglia (Frank, 2005), tonic dopamine levels affect
this process. Higher levels bias toward the Bgo^pathway and
inhibition ofthe ACC, thus attenuated FRN. Lower levels bias
toward the Bno-go^pathway and disinhibition of the ACC,
leading to a larger FRN. For instance, Frank et al. (2005)
found that, similar to our data, healthy participants who were
positively biased (go) feedback learners on the PST also
displayed an attenuated FRN compared with negatively bi-
ased (no-go) learners. Another study measuring the effect of
a Gene × Feedback-Valence interaction on FRN generation
during a gambling task reported a lower amplitude FRN in
participants whose tonic DA levels were presumably higher
due to having the MET/MET allele of the val158met COMT
polymorphism (Marco-Pallarés et al., 2009). This pattern is
also seen in a study which found that at baseline MET homo-
zygotes had a lower amplitude FRN as compared with VAL
carriers, and this pattern reversed when participants were giv-
en sulpiride, a D2 antagonist (Mueller et al., 2014). Taken
together, these findings demonstrate that an attenuated FRN
is observed in healthy individuals who have relatively higher
tonic dopamine levels. In the present study, since FRN ampli-
tude decreased as meditation experience increased, and at a
trend level, at least, this appears to be driven mainly by differ-
ences in negative-feedback processing, our data may be indic-
ative of meditators having higher tonic dopamine than do
nonmeditators.
The possibility that meditation could elevate tonic dopa-
mine over time is intriguing. Here, we would like to postulate
a potential mechanism for future investigation: It may be that
the continuous and sustained application of top-down atten-
tion to (and conflict monitoring of) internal and external sen-
sory inputs during FAM practice results in the activation and
maintenance of a feedback loop between the dlPFC, ACC,
and basal ganglia. Activation of this feedback loop could ac-
count for the release of dopamine and activity in dopaminergic
structures reported during various meditation practices
(Hagerty et al., 2013; Kjaer et al., 2002). Over time, medita-
torsrepeated activation of this loop would result in Hebbian
potentiation of the circuit, which could account for the
increased density in the ACC (Fox et al., 2014; Tang et al.,
2015) and the increased connectivity between the ACC and
striatum that has been reported in recent structural imaging
studies of FAM (Tang et al., 2010). The resulting increase in
dopaminergic synapses would then result in higher tonic do-
pamine levels. These elevated tonic dopamine levels could
partially account for the improved mood reported by medita-
tors (Brown & Ryan, 2003;Davidsonetal.,2003;Hagerty
et al., 2013; Singleton et al., 2014). The effect of disrupting
this dopaminergic corticolimbic-striatal circuitry has been
demonstrated in a rat model where chronic exposure to dopa-
mine receptor antagonists resulted in reductions in ACC vol-
ume over time (Vernon et al., 2014). From this view, our
findings fit well with these previous studies and the notion
of meditation potentiating an attention-activated dopaminer-
gic corticolimbic-striatal feedback loop.
Despite the agreement of our data with the above outlined
view, they are, however, in apparent contrast with the results
of a study demonstrating that trait mindfulness did not predict
the FRN (Teper & Inzlicht, 2014). It is important to note that
their study correlated the FRN to trait mindfulness (as mea-
sured by the Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale; PMS) instead of
FAM experience. Although practices with a FAM component
have been shown to increase trait mindfulness, trait mindful-
ness alone does not necessarily imply any FAM experience
the PMS measures self-reported present-moment awareness
and acceptance which may be influenced by FAM, but is also
argued to be a naturally occurring trait (Cardaciotto, Herbert,
Forman, Moitra, & Farrow, 2008). Using trait mindfulness to
infer FAM experience would be a conflation of trait/state/prac-
tice, as mentioned in the introduction.
In a similar vein, a recent study reported evidence for lower
striatal dopamine signalling in experienced meditators (Kruis,
Slagter, Bachhuber, Davidson, & Lutz, 2016). They recorded
the spontaneous eye-blink rate (sEBR) of 27 experienced
meditators and 118 nonmeditators at rest and found that
experienced meditators compared with nonmeditators had a
significantly lower sEBR, which is associated with low striatal
dopamine. At first glance this suggests that we should expect
meditators to have low tonic dopamine, contrary to our view.
However, in an earlier study Slagter, Georgopoulou, and
Frank (2015) found that sEBR is inversely correlated with
Avoi d Bperformance on the PST, suggesting that low levels
of dopamine are associated with high Avoid Bperformance,
and, vice versa, high tonic dopamine is associated with poorer
Avo i d Blearning, which is consistent with our findings.
Crucially, however, sEBR in both studies is recorded at rest,
not under the task-related cognitive load of the PST. Kruis
et al. (2016) argue that low sEBR at rest in meditators may
be indicative of increased stability (requiring lower levels of
dopamine),while under cognitive load (i.e., task performance)
this would imply more cognitive flexibility related to higher
dopamine levels. Our data support this possibility; however,
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
our paradigm did not provide the resting state needed for
us to examine sEBR in our sample. Although further work
is needed to investigate whether this rest/load hypothesis
can explain the apparent discrepancy between our find-
ings and those of Kruis et al., these studies provide evi-
dence that striatal dopamine signalling may be altered in
experienced meditators.
Despite the our data demonstrating a relationship between
total amount of mediation practice and differences in
reinforcement learning and feedback processing, further
research is needed to determine whether these differences
are caused by meditation. Given the literature demonstrating
neuroplastic changes in the dopamine system with meditation
practice, and that meditators need to practice to become
proficient, we find the idea that meditation is responsible for
these changes most credible. However, since we did not
manipulate meditation experience in the current study, we
cannot rule this out as an alternative explanation. Indeed,
Kruis et al. (2016)failedtofindaneffectofan8-weekmed-
itation intervention on sEBR in a sample of 36 naïve partici-
pants. They also failed to find a relationship between medita-
tion experience in experienced meditators and sEBR. This
could be seen as evidence contrary to our hypothesis that
meditation causes differences in striatal dopamine signalling.
It may be that preexisting differences in striatal dopamine lead
meditators to self-select for meditation practice. However, this
would not explain the relationship in the present study be-
tween total amount of practice time and Avoid Bperformance
or FRN amplitude. It is possible that their intervention was not
long enough, a point Kruis et al. raise, and that their experi-
enced meditators had too much experience (minimum experi-
ence in Kruis et al. was 1,439 hours). For comparison, our
sample ranged from 4 to 4,004 hours of practice, with a me-
dian of 260 hours. It may be that our data captures a critical
cross-section in meditation-induced plasticity missed in Kruis
et al.s sample. Another possibility is that type of meditation
mayalsoplayarole.Mindfulness-based stress reduction
(Kabat-Zinn, 1990), used in the Kruis et al. study, combines
both focused attention (FA) and open monitoring meditation
(OM) styles (Lutz et al., 2008), as well as yoga. The emphasis
on single pointed attention in FA meditation (e.g., samatha,
jhana meditation practices) may be more strongly associated
with classically dopaminergic phenomenological effects of
intense pleasure (e.g., Hagerty et al., 2013).
Finally, despite a growing body of evidence implicating
alterations in dopaminergic activity in meditators, it is possible
that our data could be explained by differences in other neu-
rotransmitter systems. For example, while dopamine is be-
lieved to play a key role in FRN generation, increased levels
of norepinephrine have been shown to amplify the ERN while
serotonin may also have a modulatory effect (Jocham &
Ullsperger, 2009). Serotonin has also been shown to influence
the processing of reward value (Seymour, Daw, Roiser,
Dayan, & Dolan, 2012).
Thus there is a clear need to address these questions in
future work which manipulates both striatal dopamine and
(preferably focused attention) meditation experience in a me-
dium to long-term randomized controlled intervention.
Determining whether meditation practice elevates tonic dopa-
mine levels is crucial to our understanding of a widespread
behaviour, how it may interact with disease and drugs, and
presents the possibility of potentially low-cost behavioural
interventions which could be used as an adjunct to pharmaco-
logical treatment of certain dopaminergic disorders.
In closing, the present study is, to the best of our knowl-
edge, the first study to demonstrate that reinforcement learn-
ing and FRN amplitude vary as a function of total meditation
experience. To explain these findings in the context of the
literature, we posit a theory that meditation causes increases
in tonic dopamine levels (or D2 receptor availability) in the
striatum. We then consider alternate explanations, and thus
reveal the need for further study to determine whether we
might increase our tonic dopamine levels by learning to pay
better attention.
Author contributions statements P.K. conceived and designed the exper-
iment, collected and analysed the data, and wrote the manuscript. B.O.
conceived and designed the experiment, coded the task, supervised the
project, and wrote the manuscript.
Appendix: Reported effects are robust
to alternate quantifications of FAM
experience
Since retrospective self-reported mediation experience can
vary in intensity that is not entirely captured by the number
of years of practice alone, we investigated the effect of two
other quantifications of meditation experience on our results.
The first was to multiply participants years of experience with
their self-reported weekly meditation frequency and duration
to get an estimate of their total hours of experience. Our third
method calculates meditation experience, in hours, in the last
6 months, assuming self-report frequency and duration was
constant. In all cases, a median split on meditation experience
was used to separate participants into the novice and experi-
enced meditators categories. The results of these different ap-
proaches can be seen in Table 4. Ultimately there is no perfect
way to retrospectively assess the amount or quality of medi-
tation experience, and this is a limitation of this type of study.
However, it is worth noting that for both measures total years
of experience accounted for the most variance in the data, and
also that the effects reported were relatively robust to how
meditation experience was quantified.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
OpenAccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduc-
tion in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original au-
thor(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and
indicate if changes were made.
References
Baker, T. E., & Holroyd, C. B. (2011). Dissociated roles of the anterior
cingulate cortex in rewardand conflict processing as revealed by the
feedback error-related negativity and N200. Biological Psychology,
87(1), 2534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.01.010
Baker, T. E., Stockwell, T., & Holroyd, C. B. (2013). Constraints on
decision making: Implications from genetics, personality, and addic-
tion. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience,13(3), 417
436. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-013-0164-8
Botvinick, M. M. (2012). Hierarchical reinforcement learning and deci-
sion making. Current Opinion in Neurobiology,22(6), 956962.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.05.008
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present:
Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,84(4), 822848. https://doi.
org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
Cahn, B. R., & Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP,
and neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin,132(2), 180211.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.180
Cardaciotto, L., Herbert, J. D., Forman, E. M., Moitra, E., & Farrow, V.
(2008). The assessment of present-moment awareness and accep-
tance: The Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale. Assessment,15(2), 204
223. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191107311467
Cox, S. M. L., Frank, M. J., Larcher, K., Fellows, L. K., Clark, C. A.,
Leyton, M., & Dagher, A. (2015). Striatal D1 and D2 signaling
differentially predict learning from positive and negative outcomes.
NeuroImage,109,95101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.
2014.12.070
Davidson, R. J., Kabat-Zinn, J., Schumacher, J., Rosenkranz, M., Muller,
D., Santorelli, S. F., ... Sheridan, J. F. (2003). Alterations in brain and
immune function produced by mindfulness meditation.
Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(4), 564570. https://doi.org/10.1097/
01.PSY.0000077505.67574.E3
Ernst, B., & Steinhauser, M. (2018). Effects of feedback reliability on
feedback-related brain activity: A feedback valuation account.
Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience,18(3), 596608.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-0591-7
Fox, K. C. R., Nijeboer, S., Dixon, M. L., Floman, J. L., Ellamil, M.,
Rumak,S.P.,...Christoff,K.(2014).Ismeditationassociatedwith
altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of
morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners.
Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 43, 4873. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.016
Frank, M. J. (2005). Dynamic dopamine modulation in the basal ganglia: A
neurocomputational account of cognitive deficits in medicated and
nonmedicated parkinsonism. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
17(1), 5172. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929052880093
Frank, M. J., & Hutchison, K. (2009). Genetic contributions to avoidance-
based decisions: Striatal D2 receptor polymorphisms. Neuroscience,
164(1), 131140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.04.048
Frank, M. J., & Kong, L. (2008). Learning to avoid in older age.
Psychology and Aging,23(2), 392398. https://doi.org/10.1037/
0882-7974.23.2.392
Frank, M. J., & OReilly, R. C. (2006). A mechanistic account of striatal
dopamine function in human cognition: Psychopharmacological
studies with cabergoline and haloperidol. Behavioral
Neuroscience,120(3), 497517. https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-
7044.120.3.497
Frank, M. J., Seeberger, L., & OReilly, R. C. (2004). By carrot or by
stick: Cognitive reinforcement learning in Parkinsonism. Science,
New Series,306(5703), 19401943.
Frank, M. J., Woroch, B. S., & Curran, T. (2005). Error-related negativity
predicts reinforcement learning and conflict biases. Neuron,47(4),
495501. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2005.06.020
Gehring, W. J., & Willoughby, A. R. (2002). The medial frontal cortex
and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science,
295(5563), 22792282. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1066893
Grogan, J. P., Tsivos, D., Smith, L., Knight, B. E., Bogacz, R., Whone, A.,
& Coulthard, E. J. (2017). Effects of dopamine on reinforcement
learning and consolidation in Parkinsonsdisease.ELife, 6.https://
doi.org/10.7554/eLife.26801
Hagerty, M. R., Isaacs, J., Brasington, L., Shupe, L., Fetz, E. E., &
Cramer, S. C. (2013). Case study of ecstatic meditation: fMRI and
EEG evidence of self-stimulating a reward system. Neural
Plasticity,2013,112. https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/653572
Harvey, P. (2015). Mindfulness in Theravāda samatha and vipassanā
meditations, and in secular mindfulness. In E. Shonin, W. Van
Gordon, & N. Singh (Eds.), Buddhist foundations of mindfulness
(pp. 115137). Cham, Switzerland: Springer. https://doi.org/10.
1007/978-3-319-18591-0_7
Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. H. (2002). The neural basis of human
error processing: Reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-
related negativity. Psychological Review,109(4), 679709. https://
doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.109.4.679
Holroyd, C. B., & Yeung, N. (2012). Motivation of extended behaviors
by anterior cingulate cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,16(2),
122128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.008
Hölzel, B. K., Lazar, S.W., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z., Vago, D. R., &
Ott, U. (2011). How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing
Table 4 Summary of the effect of different quantifications of meditation experience
Grouping variable NBias FRN amplitude
Control Novice Experienced Fpƞ
2
Fp ƞ
2
Total years 12 12 11 3.50 .042 0.18 10.18 <.001 0.39
Total hours 12 12 11 3.14 .06 0.16 8.05 .001 0.34
Last 6 Months 12 11 12 3.14 .06 0.16 7.45 .002 0.32
PublishersNote Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to juris-
dictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective.
Perspectives on Psychological Science,6(6), 537559. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1745691611419671
Jocham, G., & Ullsperger, M. (2009). Neuropharmacology of perfor-
mance monitoring. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,33(1),
4860. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.011
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your
body and mind to face stress, pain and illness. New York, NY:
Delacorte.
Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past,
present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice,
10(2), 144156. https://doi.org/10.1093/clipsy/bpg016
Kang, D. H., Jo, H. J., Jung, W. H., Kim, S. H., Jung, Y. H., Choi, C. H., ...
Kwon, J. S. (2013). The effect of meditation on brain structure:
Cortical thickness mapping and diffusion tensor imaging. Social
Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(1), 2733. https://doi.org/
10.1093/scan/nss056
Kjaer, T. W., Bertelsen, C., Piccini, P., Brooks, D., Alving, J., & Lou, H.
C. (2002). Increased dopamine tone during meditation-induced
change of consciousness. Cognitive Brain Research,13(2), 255
259. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00106-9
Klein, T. A., Neumann, J., Reuter, M., Hennig, J., von Cramon, D. Y., &
Ullsperger, M. (2007). Genetically determined differences in learn-
ing from errors. Science,318(5856), 16421645. https://doi.org/10.
1126/science.1145044
Klem, G. H., Otto, H., Jasper, H. H., & Elger, C. (1999). The ten-twenty
electrode system of the International Federation. In G. Deuschl & A.
Eisen (Eds.), Recommendations for the practice of clinical neuro-
physiology: Guidelines of the International Federation of Clinical
Physiology. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier Science B.V.
Kruis, A., Slagter, H. A., Bachhuber, D. R. W., Davidson, R. J., & Lutz,
A. (2016). Effects of meditation practice on spontaneous eyeblink
rate: Meditation and spontaneous eyeblinks. Psychophysiology,
53(5), 749758. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12619
Lighthall, N. R., Gorlick, M. A., Schoeke, A., Frank, M. J., & Mather, M.
(2013). Stress modulates reinforcement learning in younger and
older adults. Psychology and Aging,28(1), 3546. https://doi.org/
10.1037/a0029823
Lutz, A., Slagter, H. A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Attention
regulation and monitoring in meditation. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences,12(4), 163169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.01.
005
Marco-Pallarés, J., Cucurell, D., Cunillera, T., Krämer, U. M., Càmara,
E., Nager, W., ... Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2009). Genetic variability
in the dopamine system (dopamine receptor D4, Catechol-O-
Methyltransferase) modulates neurophysiological responses to gains
and losses. Biological Psychiatry,66(2), 154161. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.01.006
Mueller, E. M., Burgdorf, C., Chavanon, M.-L., Schweiger, D.,
Hennig, J., Wacker, J., & Stemmler, G. (2014). The COMT
Val158Met polymorphism regulates the effect of a dopamine
antagonist on the feedback-related negativity: Dopamine, feed-
back negativity, and COMT Val158Met. Psychophysiology,
51(8), 805809. https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12226
Pfabigan, D. M., Alexopoulos, J., Bauer, H., & Sailer, U. (2011).
Manipulation of feedback expectancy and valence induces negative
and positivereward prediction error signals manifest in event-related
brain potentials: FRN, P300, and prediction errors signals.
Psychophysiology,48(5), 656664. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-
8986.2010.01136.x
San Martín, R. (2012). Event-related potential studies of outcome pro-
cessing and feedback-guided learning. Frontiers in Human
Neuroscience, 6.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00304
Santesso,D.L.,Evins,A.E.,Frank,M.J.,Schetter,E.C.,Bogdan,R.,&
Pizzagalli, D. A. (2009). Single dose of a dopamine agonist impairs
reinforcement learning in humans: Evidence from event-related
potentials and computational modeling of striatal-cortical function.
Human Brain Mapping,30(7), 19631976. https://doi.org/10.1002/
hbm.20642
Schultz, W. (2002). Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron,
36(2), 241263. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00967-4
Schultz, W. (2013). Updating dopamine reward signals. Current Opinion in
Neurobiology,23(2), 229238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.
11.012
Schultz, W. (2016). Dopamine reward prediction-error signalling: A two-
component response. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,17(3), 183
195. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2015.26
Seymour, B., Daw, N. D., Roiser, J. P., Dayan, P., & Dolan, R. (2012).
Serotonin selectively modulates reward value in human decision-
making. Journal of Neuroscience,32(17), 58335842. https://doi.
org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0053-12.2012
Shonin, E., Van Gordon, W., & Griffiths, M. D. (2013). Mindfulness-
based interventions: Towards mindful clinical integration.
Frontiers in Psychology, 4. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00194
Shonin, E., Van Gordon, W., & Griffiths, M. D. (2014). Do mindfulness-
based therapies have a role in the treatment of psychosis? Australian
and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,48(2), 124127. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0004867413512688
Singleton,O.,Hölzel,B.K.,Vangel,M.,Brach,N.,Carmody,J.,
& Lazar, S. W. (2014). Change in brainstem gray matter con-
centration following a mindfulness-based intervention is cor-
related with improvement in psychological well-being.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8.https://doi.org/10.3389/
fnhum.2014.00033
Slagter, H. A., Georgopoulou, K., & Frank, M. J. (2015). Spontaneous
eye blink rate predicts learning from negative, but not positive, out-
comes. Neuropsychologia,71,126132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
neuropsychologia.2015.03.028
Smittenaar, P., Chase, H. W., Aarts, E., Nusselein, B., Bloem, B.
R., & Cools, R. (2012). Decomposing effects of dopaminer-
gic medication in Parkinsons disease on probabilistic action
selectionLearning or performance?: Dopamine and value-
based choice. European Journal of Neuroscience,35(7),
11441151. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08043.x
Tang,Y.,Lu,Q.,Geng,X.,Stein,E.A.,Yang,Y.,&Posner,M.
(2010). Short-term meditation induces white matter changes in
the anterior cingulate. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences of the United States of America,107(35), 15649
15652.
Tang, Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., ...Posner, M. (2007).
Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regula-
tion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United
States of America, 104(43), 1715217156.
Tang, Y.-Y., Hölzel, B. K., & Posner, M. I. (2015). The neuroscience of
mindfulness meditation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience,16(4), 213
225. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3916
Teasdale,J.D.,Segal,Z.V.,Williams,J.M.G.,Ridgeway,V.A.,
Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/
recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive
therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,68(4),
615623. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-006X.68.4.615
Teper, R., & Inzlicht, M. (2014). Mindful acceptance dampens
neuroaffective reactions to external and rewarding performance
feedback. Emotion,14(1), 105114. https://doi.org/10.1037/
a0034296
Vago, D. R., & Silbersweig, D. A. (2012). Self-awareness, self-regula-
tion, and self-transcendence (S-ART): A framework for understand-
ing the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Frontiers in
Human Neuroscience, 6.https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.
00296
Vernon, A. C., Crum, W. R., Lerch, J. P., Chege, W., Natesan, S., Modo,
M., ... Kapur, S. (2014). Reduced cortical volume and elevated
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
astrocyte density in rats chronically treated with antipsychotic
drugsLinking magnetic resonance imaging findings to cellular
pathology. Biological Psychiatry, 75(12), 982990. https://doi.org/
10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.012
Voon, V., Pessiglione, M., Brezing, C., Gallea, C., Fernandez, H. H.,
Dolan, R. J., & Hallett, M. (2010). Mechanisms underlying
dopamine-mediated reward bias in compulsive behaviors.
Neuron,65(1), 135142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.
2009.12.027
Walsh, M. M., & Anderson, J. R. (2012). Learning from experience:
Event-related potential correlates of reward processing, neural adap-
tation, and behavioral choice. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
Reviews,36(8), 18701884. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.
2012.05.008
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
... On close inspection, the relationship between mindfulness and reward-based learning is complex, with processing influenced both by the specific requirements of the task at hand, situational constraints, and people's levels of meditative expertise (e.g., Kirk & Montague, 2015;Kirk et al., 2019;Knytl & Opitz, 2019). Indeed, one intriguing possibility is that, at different dosages, mindfulness meditation may have the capacity either to enhance or dampen the signal value of reward prediction errors. ...
... As extensive meditative experience is associated with increased tonic dopamine levels (Kjaer et al., 2002), it is possible that the signal value of reward prediction errors is reduced for expert (vs. novice) mindfulness-based practitioners (Knytl & Opitz, 2019). In the current investigation, a brief period of mindfulness meditation accelerated the rate of learning following positive prediction errors. ...
Article
Full-text available
Recent research has demonstrated that mindfulness-based meditation facilitates basic aspects of cognition, including memory and attention. Further developing this line of inquiry, here we considered the possibility that similar effects may extend to another core psychological process—instrumental learning. To explore this matter, in combination with a probabilistic selection task, computational modelling (i.e., reinforcement drift diffusion model analysis) was adopted to establish whether and how brief mindfulness-based meditation influences learning under conditions of uncertainty (i.e., choices based on the perceived likelihood of positive and negative outcomes). Three effects were observed. Compared with performance in the control condition (i.e., no meditation), mindfulness-based meditation (1) accelerated the rate of learning following positive prediction errors; (2) elicited a preference for the exploration (vs. exploitation) of choice selections; and (3) increased response caution. Collectively, these findings elucidate the pathways through which brief meditative experiences impact learning and decision-making, with implications for interventions designed to debias aspects of social-cognitive functioning using mindfulness-based meditation.
... This suggests that mindfulness has an effect on long-term tonic levels. The direct link between dopamine levels and ADHD and mindfulness practice still needs to be explored, but contemporary research studies offer promising suggestions (Knytl & Opitz, 2019). ...
Article
This article provides a computational analysis of the behavioural effects of ADHD and its underlying decision making and epigenetic factors including the DNA methylation in relation to the ST3GAL3 gene, which has been found to play an important role in ADHD. It includes a systematic literature review combined with an adaptive dynamical system modelling approach to model and simulate the adaptive mental and behavioural dynamics in an integrative manner. As individuals with ADHD are prone to consuming digital media in an unhealthy manner, the behavioural effects of ADHD are considered in the context of digital media use. This is examined in relation to the associated neurological impairments in self-regulation as well as reward processing underlying decision-making. To illustrate this interplay, it is modelled by a higher-order adaptive dynamical system modelling approach. In addition, this article discusses both traditional (pharmacological) and non-traditional (mindfulness) interventions to improve quality of life and reduce impairments associated with ADHD and applied in the context of problematic digital media use.
... We also recorded subjects' electroencephalographic signals and based on ERP indices used in the field and their functional relevance 13,22,23 , the ERN, FRN, and P300 were used as primary neural indices. The ERN and FRN are two classical ERP components associated with signaling of the mesolimbic dopamine system 24,25 and have been widely used in previous reinforcement learning studies 13,23,26 . Attention is one of the most important factors influencing learning performance [27][28][29] and effects of OT on attentional processing have also been reported in previous studies [30][31][32] . ...
Article
Full-text available
Outcomes of past decisions profoundly shape our behavior. However, choice-outcome associations can become volatile and adaption to such changes is of importance. The present study combines pharmaco-electroencephalography with computational modeling to examine whether intranasal oxytocin can modulate reinforcement learning under a volatile vs. a stable association. Results show that oxytocin increases choice accuracy independent of learning context, which is paralleled by a larger N2pc and a smaller P300. Model-based analyses reveal that while oxytocin promotes learning by accelerating value update of outcomes in the volatile context, in the stable context it does so by improving choice consistency. These findings suggest that oxytocin’s facilitatory effects on learning may be exerted via improving early attentional selection and late neural processing efficiency, although at the computational level oxytocin’s actions are highly adaptive between learning contexts. Our findings provide proof of concept for oxytocin’s therapeutic potential in mental disorders with adaptive learning dysfunction.
... Second, an experienced Buddhist meditator has shown specific increased activity in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and in the striatum during FA compared with other meditative techniques (Hagerty et al. 2013). Third, Knytl and Opitz (2019) have found that FA meditators, as compared with nonmeditators, exhibited attenuated feedback-related negativity, an electrophysiological component known to be affected by dopamine levels in the striatum and ACC. Hence, the studies outlined above imply a crucial activation of the nigro-striatal dopamine pathway when practicing FA meditation. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objectives: Goal-direct actions requires and integrating processing of stimuli and responses, which is why close stimulus-response bindings have to be created. However, the strength of these bindings can be modified. The metacontrol state model (MSM) hypothesizes that this can be achieved through mindfulness meditation. Yet, the cognitive processes underlying possible effects of meditation on S-R bindings remain unexplored. Methods: We examined the effects of a brief bout of focused attention (FA) meditation on S-R bindings using a standard event-file task measuring S-R bindings. This was done in a within-subject (crossover) design, where each participant (novice to meditation) was examined at two separate appointments (with and without meditation before the task). Results: We found that 15-minutes of a single bout of FA meditation was enough to restrict the retrieval of S-R bindings to relevant information as indexed by decreased partial overlap costs. Conclusion: These findings support the MSM framework suggesting that FA meditation induces a top-down biasing of processes toward cognitive persistence. Importantly, however, the effects of FA meditation were only evident when there was prior experience with the task. This shows close similarities to effects in pharmacological and brain stimulation studies and suggests that FA meditation modulates gain control principles in information processing. Moreover, effects of FA meditation were restricted in its duration since FA meditation modulated the retrieval of S-R bindings only in the early phases of the event file task. In novices, effects of short-term FA meditation are thus relative fragile and only induce some finer adjustments in processing strategy.
... The ERPs were pre-processed as described in Knytl and Opitz (2019) using BrainAnalyzer2 (Brain Products GmbH, Gilching, Germany). Data pre-processing consisted of a digital band-pass filter from 0.5 Hz to 30 Hz (À8 dB cut-off) to eliminate lowfrequency signal drifts and high-frequency artifacts. ...
Article
Full-text available
Anxiety is one of the most prevalent mental health problems; it is known to impede cognitive functioning. It is believed to alter preferences for feedback-based learning in anxious and non-anxious learners. Thus, the present study measured feedback processing in adults ( N = 30) with and without anxiety symptoms using a probabilistic learning task. Event-related potential (ERP) measures were used to assess how the bias for either positive or negative feedback learning is reflected by the feedback-related negativity component (FRN), an ERP extracted from the electroencephalogram. Anxious individuals, identified by means of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire, showed a diminished FRN and increased accuracy after negative compared to positive feedback. Non-anxious individuals exhibited the reversed pattern with better learning from positive feedback, highlighting their preference for positive feedback. Our ERP results imply that impairments with feedback-based learning in anxious individuals are due to alterations in the mesolimbic dopaminergic system. Our finding that anxious individuals seem to favor negative as opposed to positive feedback has important implications for teacher–student feedback communication.
Chapter
The present paper has been investigated the effect of meditation, also known as Vipassana meditation on mental health. To understand these ends and means, practice of Vipassana meditation and how Vipassana meditation helps us to live a noble and blissful life. In addition, the present paper has also tried to find out the importance of mental health for the individual and the society as a whole. The social ripple effect showing the impact of our actions has been described to explore the unseen effects of our speech and actions. Our world become peaceful to enjoy and live happily. That’s why change should start from every individual. If the jungle has dried up and we want to revive it, we have to water each and every tree in that jungle. If we want world peace, we must learn to be peaceful ourselves. Finally, this work has been compared the Buddhist philosophy and Western psychology to see how the two fields are similar or different with respect to the concept of mental health. Both the development of the ideal healthy person and the ways in which individuals deviate from it has been culturally defined. Buddhist psychology argues that the root causes of suffering can be permanently rooted out, while Western psychology states that they are immutable, rigid and irreversible. From this point of view, Vipassana meditation’s ability to calm the human mind, its ability to transform its turbulence into peace with better energy, makes it an important mental health remedy and an excellent tool for potential human development to achieve higher mental health standards, mainly an attempt has been made to throw light on how attitudes are formed.
Article
Full-text available
Adaptive decision making relies on learning from feedback. Because feedback sometimes can be misleading, optimal learning requires that knowledge about the feedback’s reliability be utilized to adjust feedback processing. Although previous research has shown that feedback reliability indeed influences feedback processing, the underlying mechanisms through which this is accomplished remain unclear. Here we propose that feedback processing is adjusted by the adaptive, top-down valuation of feedback. We assume that unreliable feedback is devalued relative to reliable feedback, thus reducing the reward prediction errors that underlie feedback-related brain activity and learning. A crucial prediction of this account is that the effects of feedback reliability are susceptible to contrast effects. That is, the effects of feedback reliability should be enhanced when both reliable and unreliable feedback are experienced within the same context, as compared to when only one level of feedback reliability is experienced. To evaluate this prediction, we measured the event-related potentials elicited by feedback in two experiments in which feedback reliability was varied either within or between blocks. We found that the fronto-central valence effect, a correlate of reward prediction errors during reinforcement learning, was reduced for unreliable feedback. But this result was obtained only when feedback reliability was varied within blocks, thus indicating a contrast effect. This suggests that the adaptive valuation of feedback is one mechanism underlying the effects of feedback reliability on feedback processing.
Article
Full-text available
Emerging evidence suggests that dopamine may modulate learning and memory with important implications for understanding the neurobiology of memory and future therapeutic targeting. An influential hypothesis posits that dopamine biases reinforcement learning. More recent data also suggest an influence during both consolidation and retrieval. Eighteen Parkinson’s disease patients learned through feedback ON or OFF medication, with memory tested 24 hr later ON or OFF medication (4 conditions, within-subjects design with matched healthy control group). Patients OFF medication during learning decreased in memory accuracy over the following 24 hr. In contrast to previous studies, however, dopaminergic medication during learning and testing did not affect expression of positive or negative reinforcement. Two further experiments were run without the 24 hr delay, but they too failed to reproduce effects of dopaminergic medication on reinforcement learning. While supportive of a dopaminergic role in consolidation, this study failed to replicate previous findings on reinforcement learning.
Article
Full-text available
Meditation can be defined as a form of mental training that aims to improve an individual's core psychological capacities, such as attentional and emotional self-regulation. Meditation encompasses a family of complex practices that include mindfulness meditation, mantra meditation, yoga, tai chi and chi gong 1. Of these practices , mindfulness meditation — often described as non-judgemental attention to present-moment experiences (BOX 1) — has received most attention in neuroscience research over the past two decades 2–8. Although meditation research is in its infancy, a number of studies have investigated changes in brain activation (at rest and during specific tasks) that are associated with the practice of, or that follow, training in mindfulness meditation. These studies have reported changes in multiple aspects of mental function in beginner and advanced meditators, healthy individuals and patient populations 9–14. In this Review, we consider the current state of research on mindfulness meditation. We discuss the methodological challenges that the field faces and point to several shortcomings in existing studies. Taking into account some important theoretical considerations, we then discuss behavioural and neuroscientific findings in light of what we think are the core components of meditation practice: attention control, emotion regulation and self-awareness (BOX 1). Within this framework, we describe research that has revealed changes in behaviour, brain activity and brain structure following mindfulness meditation training. We discuss what has been learned so far from this research and suggest new research strategies for the field. We focus here on mindfulness meditation practices and have excluded studies on other types of meditation. However, it is important to note that other styles of meditation may operate via distinct neural mechanisms
Article
Environmental stimuli and objects, including rewards, are often processed sequentially in the brain. Recent work suggests that the phasic dopamine reward prediction-error response follows a similar sequential pattern. An initial brief, unselective and highly sensitive increase in activity unspecifically detects a wide range of environmental stimuli, then quickly evolves into the main response component, which reflects subjective reward value and utility. This temporal evolution allows the dopamine reward prediction-error signal to optimally combine speed and accuracy.
Article
A rapidly growing body of research suggests that meditation can change brain and cognitive functioning. Yet little is known about the neurochemical mechanisms underlying meditation-related changes in cognition. Here, we investigated the effects of meditation on spontaneous eyeblink rates (sEBR), a noninvasive peripheral correlate of striatal dopamine activity. Previous studies have shown a relationship between sEBR and cognitive functions such as mind wandering, cognitive flexibility, and attention-functions that are also affected by meditation. We therefore expected that long-term meditation practice would alter eyeblink activity. To test this, we recorded baseline sEBR and intereyeblink intervals (IEBI) in long-term meditators (LTM) and meditation-naive participants (MNP). We found that LTM not only blinked less frequently, but also showed a different eyeblink pattern than MNP. This pattern had good to high degree of consistency over three time points. Moreover, we examined the effects of an 8-week course of mindfulness-based stress reduction on sEBR and IEBI, compared to an active control group and a waitlist control group. No effect of short-term meditation practice was found. Finally, we investigated whether different types of meditation differentially alter eyeblink activity by measuring sEBR and IEBI after a full day of two kinds of meditation practices in the LTM. No effect of meditation type was found. Taken together, these findings may suggest either that individual difference in dopaminergic neurotransmission is a self-selection factor for meditation practice, or that long-term, but not short-term meditation practice induces stable changes in baseline striatal dopaminergic functioning.
Chapter
This chapter explores the nature of sati/mindfulness and analyzes its different aspects: an equanimous “bare attention” supported by wise and probing attention, protective awareness, clear recollection that calls up helpful qualities, and contemplative recollections. The roles of mindfulness in calm (samatha) and insight (vipassanā) meditations are explored, emphasizing that it is equally important in both of these, though used in different ways. Both are ways of practicing the satipaṭṭhānas, or “applications of mindfulness,” though in samatha, mindfulness works with strong concentration (samādhi) to develop lucid trances (jhāna) that in turn can be a good basis for vipassanā. The nature of “secular” mindfulness is analyzed and compared to the fuller forms of it in the kinds of Buddhism explored in this chapter. The chapter ends with a discussion of how mindfulness is seen in Theravāda and Sarvāstivāda Abhidhamma, as this raises issues of its natural complements, and whether or not it is a quality unique to certain states of mind.
Article
The authors present a unified account of 2 neural systems concerned with the development and expression of adaptive behaviors: a mesencephalic dopamine system for reinforcement learning and a "generic" error-processing system associated with the anterior cingulate cortex. The existence of the error-processing system has been inferred from the error-related negativity (ERN), a component. of the event-related brain potential elicited when human participants commit errors in reaction-time tasks. The authors propose that the ERN is generated when a negative reinforcement learning signal is conveyed to the anterior cingulate cortex via the mesencephalic dopamine system and that this signal is used by the anterior cingulate cortex to modify performance on the task at hand. They provide support for this proposal using both computational modeling and psychophysiological experimentation.