ArticlePDF Available

Fake News and the Third-Person Effect: They are More Influenced than Me and You

Authors:

Abstract

Recent research efforts have been invested into undermining the effects of digital disinformation, both on a personal and on a societal level. However, because of the complexity of the phenomena, the actual effects of digital disinformation are still under consideration and, therefore, studies published so far focus on the perceived effects of fake news. Against this backdrop, relying on Davison’s (1983) third-person effect (TPE) theory, this study aims at investigating (1) the way people perceive the effects of fake news and (2) the possible variables predicting different levels of self-other discrepancy perceptions. Based on data gathered from a national representative survey (N=1107) in Romania, main results show that people have the tendency to consider that distant others (i.e., members of the out-group) are more influenced than themselves or the in-group members (i.e., confirming a strong TPE). With reference to TPE predictors, gender and fake news frequency of exposure are the mos significant variables influencing of the intensity of TPE, in the sense that (a) women tend to consider that distant others are more influenced by fake news and (b) the more people perceive they are exposed to fake news, the greatest the TPE. Keywords: Digital disinformation; Fake news; Third-person effect; Predictors of third-person effect.
5
DOI:10.24193/jmr.32.1
Published First Online: 2018/11/05
Fake News and the Third-Person Eect:
They are More Inuenced than Me and You
Assistant Professor Oana ȘTEFĂNIȚĂ, PhD
National University of Political Studies and Public Administration,
Bucharest, Romania
E-mail: oana.stefanita@comunicare.ro
Professor Nicoleta CORBU, PhD
National University of Political Studies and Public Administration,
Bucharest, Romania
E-mail: nicoleta.corbu@comunicare.ro
Assistant Professor Raluca BUTUROIU, PhD
National University of Political Studies and Public Administration,
Bucharest, Romania
E-mail: raluca.buturoiu@comunicare.ro
Abstract. Recent research eorts have been invested into undermining
the eects of digital disinformation, both on a personal and on a soci-
etal level. However, because of the complexity of the phenomena, the ac-
tual eects of digital disinformation are still under consideration and,
therefore, studies published so far focus on the perceived eects of fake
news. Against this backdrop, relying on Davison’s (1983) third-person
eect (TPE) theory, this study aims at investigating (1) the way people
perceive the eects of fake news and (2) the possible variables predicting
dierent levels of self-other discrepancy perceptions. Based on data gath-
ered from a national representative survey (N=1107) in Romania, main
results show that people have the tendency to consider that distant others
(i.e., members of the out-group) are more inuenced than themselves or
the in-group members (i.e., conrming a strong TPE). With reference to
TPE predictors, gender and fake news frequency of exposure are the most
Journal of Media Research,
Vol. 11 Issue 3(32) / 2018, pp. 5-23
6
signicant variables inuencing of the intensity of TPE, in the sense that
(a) women tend to consider that distant others are more inuenced by
fake news and (b) the more people perceive they are exposed to fake news,
the greatest the TPE.
Keywords: Digital disinformation; Fake news; Third-person ef-
fect; Predictors of third-person eect.
Introduction
Within the new media ecosystem, widely dominated by media content easily
swinging between satire and parody, fabrication and manipulation, scholars have
been trying to identify the eects of digital disinformation. Although there are
signicant eorts in this respect, the actual eects of digital disinformation are
still dicult to be measured. The diculties consist in the multiple implications
brought by the variety of forms that digital disinformation might take and the
numerous possible causes leading some people to be more inuenced than others.
In this context, some scholars have tried to determine and analyze if and how
people perceive the eects of digital disinformation. Specically, some recent at-
tempts tackle the perceived eects of fake news in the US context (Calvert 2017;
Jang & Kim 2018). Their approach on the perceived eects of fake news is based
on Davison’s (1983) third-person eect (TPE) theory, stating that people tend to
perceive that others (third persons, out-group members) are more inuenced by
dierent media messages than people themselves or in-group members. This self-
other discrepancy in perception might explain further aspects of people’s behavior
(e.g., the support for censorship).
Against this background, this article deals with the perceived eects of digital
disinformation in a Romanian context, being, to our knowledge, the rst aempt
made so far in this direction. Relying on data gathered from a national representa-
tive survey (N=1107) in Romania, this article aims at oering some insights re-
garding the perceived impact of digital disinformation in Romania, representing
a rst step towards explaining further social aitudes and behaviors. Based on the
TPE theory, the article investigates (1) the perceived eects of fake news among
Romanian people and (2) the predictors leading to various levels of TPE. Particu-
larly, we focus on analyzing people’s perceptions about the eects of fake news on
three levels: on the personal level (self-perception about fake news eects), on the
in-group level (perception about fake news eects on close friends and family),
and on the out-group level (perception about fake news eects on distant others).
At the same time, we aim at identifying the main variables predicting people’s
tendency towards considering others to be more inuenced by fake news than
themselves.
... On one hand, we may believe that everyone, including ourselves, is generally gullible (H 4 below). On the other hand, we may believe that other people are more gullible than we are (H 5 below, see also "third-person effect," Jang and Kim, 2018;Ştefăniţă et al., 2018;Yoo et al., 2022), that is, that others are more easily swayed and manipulated than we are (in particular by false information). The third-person effect stems from a general tendency to downplay one's susceptibility to socially undesirable messages (such as fake news) and overstate one's receptivity to socially desirable messages (such as educational content)-likely for self-enhancement and reputation management considerations (Gunther, 1995;Scharrer and Leone, 2008). ...
... 'A new study suggests fake news might have won Donald Trump the 2016 election'; Blake, 2018). The third-person effect is well established regarding the influence of fake news and misinformation (Corbu et al., 2020;Jang and Kim, 2018;Ştefăniţă et al., 2018;Yoo et al., 2022). For instance, a survey from the Pew Research Center (Barthel et al., 2016) has shown that while 88% of Americans reported that fake news has caused confusion about current events, 84% of them reported being very confident or somewhat confident in their ability to identify fake news: H 4 : Perceived danger of misinformation will be negatively associated with confidence that people in general, friends and family, and themselves, are able to identify misinformation. ...
... In line with previous findings in the literature on the third-person effect of misinformation (Corbu et al., 2020;Jang and Kim, 2018;Ştefăniţă et al., 2018;Yoo et al., 2022), we found clear evidence that people think that others, and in particular distant others, are more vulnerable to misinformation than themselves. In our surveys, 77% of participants believed that people in general were more vulnerable to misinformation than themselves, and only 18% believed that they were more vulnerable to misinformation than people in general. ...
Article
Full-text available
Alarmist narratives about the flow of misinformation and its negative consequences have gained traction in recent years. If these fears are to some extent warranted, the scientific literature suggests that many of them are exaggerated. Why are people so worried about misinformation? In two pre-registered surveys conducted in the United Kingdom ( N study_1 = 300, N study_2 = 300) and replicated in the United States ( N study_1 = 302, N study_2 = 299), we investigated the psychological factors associated with perceived danger of misinformation and how it contributes to the popularity of alarmist narratives on misinformation. We find that the strongest, and most reliable, predictor of perceived danger of misinformation is the third-person effect (i.e. the perception that others are more vulnerable to misinformation than the self) and, in particular, the belief that “distant” others (as opposed to family and friends) are vulnerable to misinformation. The belief that societal problems have simple solutions and clear causes was consistently, but weakly, associated with perceived danger of online misinformation. Other factors, like negative attitudes toward new technologies and higher sensitivity to threats, were inconsistently, and weakly, associated with perceived danger of online misinformation. Finally, we found that participants who report being more worried about misinformation are more willing to like and share alarmist narratives on misinformation. Our findings suggest that fears about misinformation tap into our tendency to view other people as gullible.
... TPE researchers have examined the audience's effects of the media on self and others (Sherrick, 2016; see also Davison, 1983;Perloff, 2002;Ştefăniţă et al., 2018;Sun et al., 2008; and found two separate components of the theory, which are perceptual and behavioural. ...
... Furthermore, earlier researchers on TPP and behavioural components examined their strengths and came up with the antecedents of TPE, such as perceived knowledge (Leone et al., 2006), social distance (Jang et al., 2018), message undesirability (Schreiner et al., 2018), and media attention (Ştefăniţă et al., 2018;Yoo et al., 2022). The above antecedents can yield insight into lowering negative perceptions of TPE, enabling audiences to use effective protective measures against such negative behaviour (Chen et al., 2020;Lo et al., 2015). ...
Article
Full-text available
Third-person effects (TPE) research has increased globally among social science scholars seeking to understand how and why TPE occurs among audiences. This study aims to examine the TPE methodological and theoretical trends in Asia. The study meta-analyzed TPE articles from internationally recognized media and communication-related journals with some Asian roots to understand the research topics, authorship, country/territory, theoretical approaches, methodology, population, sampling, and the available TPE components dominant among TPE researchers in Asia. The results revealed that 38 articles maintain Asia as a firm territory for TPE research. In addition, the analysis also fronted politics and pornography, and sexuality as the dominant research subjects among Asian TPE researchers. The most popular research method among TPE articles in Asia is the survey method, with students as the dominant and reliable respondents sampled randomly. Lastly, the analysis also highlights that both perceptual and behavioral components of TPE are predominantly used in the articles, although Asian TPE articles barely use other theories to explain the phenomenon. The analysis has proved that TPE research in Asia is on the right path.
... In particular, many studies have explored the mediating role of social media in the dissemination of disinformative content and the spread of hate speech (Walker, Mercea, and Bastos 2019;Buchanan and Benson 2019). By contrast, except for a few studies of great interest (Ștefăniță, Corbu, and Buturoiu 2018;Pennycook and Rand 2021), the cognitive biases and psycho-sociological behaviours of citizens that lead to fake news being perceived as truthful have been largely ignored by the academic literature. ...
... Not only do people believe that the media influences the attitudes and behaviours of others more than their own, but they also believe that it influences people outside their own group more than those within it and, in general, those who are distant more than those who are close to them (Ștefăniță, Corbu, and Buturoiu 2018;Talwar et al. 2020), either geographically, culturally or ideologically. Since this theory was formulated, most research has confirmed that such cognitive bias is present in media audiences. ...
Article
This study introduces the concept of “nobody-fools-me perception”, a cognitive bias consisting of overconfidence in one’s own ability to detect disinformation, associated with the belief that one is more immune to false content than almost everyone else. Specifically, it examines the extent to which variables such as age and education determine the conviction that one is able to spot false content, and influence the skills and habits of checking and sharing potentially unverified information on health, a serious problem in the context of the Covid-19 pandemic. Based on two face-to-face focus groups and one online focus group made up of Spanish people between the ages of 25 and 54, this qualitative research study explored the behaviour of regular citizens when assessing the truthfulness of health-related news, and their habits about believing it. The results reveal that younger people tended to distrust the ability of older people to spot false content, and vice versa. They also show that people with a higher educational level were more confident about their own immunity to disinformation. By introducing the concept of “nobody-fools-me perception”, this study contributes to our understanding of how subjective perceptions lead to believing in false news.
... According to Loomba et al. (2021), it seems that individuals' willingness to vaccinate increases when the safety of close others is at stake, rather than when it comes to own health. However, when it comes to various forms of fake news (conspiracy theories included), few studies have concentrated on analyzing its third person effect (TPE) perception (for exceptions, see Corbu et al., 2020;Jang & Kim, 2018;Ștefăniță et al., 2018). Nevertheless, in Romania, people considered their friends and family to be more influenced by fake news than themselves (Ștefăniță et al., 2018), thus having the perception that close others are more vulnerable to conspiracy theories. ...
... This may ultimately lead to increasing levels of vaccine acceptance in their own case and, at the same time, to the perception that other people will display lower levels of vaccine acceptance (by creating a sort of third person perception, that the others are not that well informed/ equipped to deal with this complicated situation). Our findings (validating H3) are in line with recent evidence that suggests a solid TPE among Romanians concerning their own ability to detect fake news or misleading information (Corbu et al., 2020;Ștefăniță et al., 2018). Naturally, this effect is always stronger when people compare their own capacities (whether is fake news detection literacy, general media influence, or vulnerability in front of conspiracies) to those of distant others. ...
Article
Full-text available
The COVID-19 pandemic opened the doors for a corresponding "infodemic", associated with various misleading narratives related to the SARS-CoV-2 virus. As the way to stop the pandemic was unveiled, misleading narratives switched from the disease itself to the vaccine. Nevertheless, a rather scarce corpus of literature has approached the effects of these narratives on the willingness to take a vaccine against COVID-19. This study investigates how exposure to conspiracy narratives versus information that counter these narratives influences people's willingness to get vaccinated. Based on an experimental design, using a sample of Romanian students (N=301), this research shows that exposure to factual information related to COVID-19 vaccines meant to debunk conspiracy theories leads to higher willingness to vaccinate. Furthermore, this study shows that young, educated Romanians consider distant others to be more influenced by conspiracy theories on this topic, and, therefore, more prone to exhibit hesitancy towards COVID-19 vaccination.
... were convinced that other people, not themselves or those like them, were more vulnerable to fake news. More specifically, there was strong evidence of the third-person perception (TPP): People claim that others, particularly distant others (Ştefăniţă et al., 2018), would be more likely to be fooled by fake news than they are (Jang & Kim, 2018;Ştefăniţă et al., 2018). Taken together, it seems apparent that the public has been increasingly worried about the prevalence and the harmful impacts of fake news. ...
... As TPP occurs mostly when the media content is perceived to be socially undesirable (Paul et al., 2000), a growing body of research has documented a TPP of fake news effects. Studies have shown that people tend to presume the harmful impact of fake news as being greater on others than themselves, especially for distant others (Corbu et al., 2020;Ştefăniţă et al., 2018) or for out-group members (Jang & Kim, 2018), and when the fake news is perceived as more socially undesirable (Yang & Horning, 2020). ...
Article
This study examined the third-person perception (TPP) in terms of the influence of fake news through the lens of the Gamson hypothesis – the combination of political trust and political self-efficacy – and how the perception may affect one’s fake news sharing behavior. Data from a national survey (N = 1,024) indicates that Dissidents (low political trust, high political self-efficacy) who are likely to perceive greater exposure to fake news are likely to exhibit stronger TPP of fake news effects than the other Gamson groups. We also found a negative association between TPP and fake news sharing, suggesting that Dissidents who are likely to have stronger TPP are less likely to share fake news than non-Dissidents. Implications for the current political landscape are discussed.
... There are articles confirming the third person effect of fake news both in the perception and in the behaviour in the USA (Jang and Kim, 2018) and in Europe (Ștefăniță, Corbu and Buturoiu, 2018). Because people want to maintain a positive image of themselves, they tend to underestimate media effects on themselves and overestimate the effects on others. ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
When companies decide to promote their workers, one of the options for the latter is training. Based on game theory concepts, this paper derives a threshold that determines the conditions under which a firm can promote an employee; threshold identified after the worker sends a signal that he has finished his college studies. Once the threshold is deducted and from the reports on training and characteristics of the workers such as education, the result shows that companies are willing to promote the worker, as long as the additional benefits for the promotion of the worker are twice as high as the investment made. Although this paper shows a cost-benefit requirement, which could be useful for companies to identify whom to promote, the application is limited as it only considers approximate values rather than data from institutions.
... The fact that the 4 th most disseminated link mentioning NATO not only contains a conspiracy narrative, but was also officially debunked as fake news by the authorities, points to the dangers posed by viralization of disinformation, the speed with which this phenomenon occurs on social media (the link was posted on Facebook on March 13, the authorities shut down the websites on April 23), and also underlines the threat posed by fake news that are not obvious in character, but incorporate pieces of factual information which are modified or framed to "involve negative manipulations designed to deceive" (Corbu et al., 2021, p. 76). This risk is magnified by people's tendency to disregard the impact of fake news, believing that others, and not themselves, are more susceptible to be influenced by fake news (Stefanita et al., 2018). The causes, as well as the consequences, of such behavior, the underlying mechanisms that enable people's tendency to give credit to (and contribute to the spread of) this type of negative content certainly deserve further exploration through sociological research to understand motivations and correlating factors. ...
Article
Full-text available
The Covid-19 infodemic has brought forth an intensification of disinformation and conspiracy narratives, especially in the online environment. Beside the public health implications, the geopolitical aspect is also relevant, since Romania, a member of the EU and NATO, has constantly been under pressure from a multitude of propaganda attempts meant to undermine people’s trust in the West. The main goal of this paper is to explore the type of online information that the Romanian public is exposed to regarding NATO, an institutional actor representative for the Western model, specifically to assess the main communicators, content, and patterns of information dissemination. A dataset of Facebook mentions was created and filtered (n=9705) and visual network analysis was employed to build a NATO issue network. The main clusters of this network were analyzed to reveal that the two largest communities are structured according to a political cleavage, while an anti-Western media actor, Sputnik, controls a separate cluster, but its content infiltrates several others. Qualitative content analysis on a relevance sample of the most frequently distributed content was employed to evaluate both attitudes toward NATO (negative stances were identified in several analyzed documents) and the occurrence of conspiracy narratives (also a significant presence).
Article
Utilizing online surveys of 729 US and 469 Chinese respondents, this study examines the mediated relationships between age and misinformation via fear of missing out (FoMO) and digital literacy in two different cultures. Results suggest that senior citizens are uniquely vulnerable to misinformation as the victims, in that they are less likely to check on suspicious content and that they are also less motivated to share information online in general. In contrast, youngadults have a greater propensity to be the spreaders of misinformation if not made suspicious of the content due to their stronger motivations to share information online. FoMO and digital literacy significantly mediate the relationship between age and motivations to share information and the one between age and reactions to misinformation, respectively. Sociocultural differences vary the intensity of these mediated relationships. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
Health misinformation is a serious problem that can cause confusion and risk-taking behaviors, undermining public health efforts. Fact-checking has been highlighted as an effective tool for coping with the challenge of misinformation. However, few studies have examined the factors influencing individuals’ health fact-checking behaviors. Using the comprehensive model of information seeking, we conducted a two-wave panel survey in China during the COVID-19 pandemic and explored the underlying process that might hinder COVID-19 fact-checking. The results showed that risk perception and worry about COVID-19 triggered a negative COVID-19 information-seeking experience, which reduced COVID-19 fact-checking. Moreover, the propensity to trust COVID-19 misinformation played a moderating role, such that negative information-seeking experience had a stronger negative effect on fact-checking behaviors for people with a greater propensity to trust misinformation. This study demonstrates the dark side of cognitive and affective responses to risks and health information-seeking experiences. The findings offer important implications for future health communication initiatives to effectively promote health fact-checking behaviors.
Article
We extended the influence of presumed media influence model by taking interpersonal communication into account. Our survey (N = 642) results revealed that individuals’ attention to COVID-19 information on social media and their engagement in interpersonal communication about the disease independently and jointly affected presumed others’ attention. The more that individuals engaged in interpersonal communication, the less that their attention to mediated content factored into how they perceived others’ attention to such content. Presumed others’ attention, in turn, was positively associated with presuming that others were influenced by COVID-19 misinformation and the intention to correct, but not censor, misinformation.
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any references for this publication.