ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

People with anxiety disorders tend to make decisions on the basis of avoiding threat rather than obtaining rewards. Despite a robust literature examining approach-avoidance motivation, less is known about goal pursuit. The present study examined the content, motives, consequences, and daily correlates of strivings among adults diagnosed with social anxiety disorder and healthy controls. Participants generated six strivings along with the motives and consequences of their pursuit. Compared with controls, people with social anxiety disorder were less strongly driven by autonomous motives and reported greater difficulty pursuing strivings. Coders analyzed strivings for the presence of 10 themes: achievement, affiliation, avoidance, emotion regulation, generativity, interpersonal, intimacy, power, self-presentation, and self-sufficiency. People with social anxiety disorder constructed more emotion regulation strivings than did controls, but they did not differ across other themes. This research illustrates how studying personality at different levels of analysis (traits, strivings) can yield novel information for understanding anxiety disorders.
Content may be subject to copyright.
https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702618804778
Clinical Psychological Science
2019, Vol. 7(2) 283 –301
© The Author(s) 2018
Article reuse guidelines:
sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/2167702618804778
www.psychologicalscience.org/CPS
ASSOCIATION FOR
PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE
Empirical Article
People with anxiety disorders encounter significant
challenges when pursuing goals. Goal pursuit is most
commonly studied in reference to two relatively inde-
pendent systems that guide motivation and behavior:
the approach system (i.e., behavioral activation) guid-
ing individuals toward potential rewards and the avoid-
ance system (i.e., behavioral inhibition) aiming to
prevent exposure to danger by warning of imminent
threat and signaling the need to escape (Carver &
Scheier, 1990; Strauman, 2017; Watson, Wiese, Vaidya,
& Tellegen, 1999). Approach motivation and avoidance
motivation represent the foundation of several basic
dimensions of personality (Elliot & Thrash, 2002), and
this framework is integral to understanding cognitive-
emotional and behavioral patterns that underlie anxiety
disorders.
An emotion regulation framework can help delineate
why these motivational tendencies engender problem-
atic goal pursuit. Gross’s (1998, 2014, 2015) process
model (and updated extended process model) of emo-
tion regulation suggests that emotion generation typi-
cally occurs in five stages: situation selection, situation
modification, attentional deployment, cognitive change,
and response modulation. At each stage, people deploy
strategies to regulate emotions. People with anxiety
disorders display regulatory deficits throughout this
process, most commonly in the form of maladaptive
avoidance (for theoretical reviews, see Borkovec,
Alcaine, & Behar, 2004; Feldner, Zvolensky, & Leen-
Feldner, 2004; Jazaieri, Morrison, Goldin, & Gross, 2015;
Mennin, Heimberg, Turk, & Fresco, 2005). Avoidance
can be deployed at each stage of emotion regulation;
people with social anxiety disorder (SAD), in particular,
tend to avoid entering anxiety-provoking situations
(e.g., Werner, Goldin, Ball, Heimberg, & Gross, 2011),
engage in safety behaviors to minimize threat and avoid
fully engaging in a social encounter (e.g., averted eye
contact; Plasencia, Alden, & Taylor, 2011), divert atten-
tion inward in fearful social situations (Hope, Gansler,
804778CPXXXX10.1177/2167702618804778Goodman et al.Anxiety Disorders and Strivings
research-article2018
Corresponding Author:
Todd B. Kashdan, Department of Psychology, Mail Stop 3F5, George
Mason University, Fairfax, VA 22030
E-mail: tkashdan@gmu.edu
Personal Strivings to Understand Anxiety
Disorders: Social Anxiety as an Exemplar
Fallon R. Goodman, Todd B. Kashdan, Melissa C. Stiksma,
and Dan V. Blalock
Department of Psychology, George Mason University
Abstract
People with anxiety disorders tend to make decisions on the basis of avoiding threat rather than obtaining rewards.
Despite a robust literature examining approach-avoidance motivation, less is known about goal pursuit. The present
study examined the content, motives, consequences, and daily correlates of strivings among adults diagnosed
with social anxiety disorder and healthy controls. Participants generated six strivings along with the motives and
consequences of their pursuit. Compared with controls, people with social anxiety disorder were less strongly driven
by autonomous motives and reported greater difficulty pursuing strivings. Coders analyzed strivings for the presence of
10 themes: achievement, affiliation, avoidance, emotion regulation, generativity, interpersonal, intimacy, power, self-
presentation, and self-sufficiency. People with social anxiety disorder constructed more emotion regulation strivings
than did controls, but they did not differ across other themes. This research illustrates how studying personality at
different levels of analysis (traits, strivings) can yield novel information for understanding anxiety disorders.
Keywords
strivings, social anxiety, emotion regulation, personality
Received 3/16/18; Revision accepted 7/30/18
284 Goodman et al.
& Heimberg, 1989; Spurr & Stopa, 2002), and try to
control or get rid of anxious thoughts, feelings, and
physical sensations (Kashdan et al., 2013; Kashdan
etal., 2014). One potential explanation for why people
with anxiety disorders frequently deploy avoidance-
based emotion regulation strategies is that they display
high sensitivity to anxiety and other uncomfortable
emotions (Olatunji & Wolitzky-Taylor, 2009). Findings
from self-report and experimental research suggest that
people with anxiety disorders experience considerable
difficulty tolerating uncomfortable internal sensations,
thoughts, or feelings and, in response, exert consider-
able effort to avoid them (for reviews, see Chawla &
Ostafin, 2007; Goodman, Larrazabal, West, & Kashdan,
in press; Kashdan, Barrios, Forsyth, & Steger, 2006). The
energy invested into trying to avoid anxiety puts people
with anxiety disorders into prevention mode, whereby
avoiding threat takes precedence over pursuing impor-
tant goals and seeking rewards (Kashdan, Weeks, &
Savostyanova, 2011; Roemer & Orsillo, 2002).
Taken together, this research suggests that people
with anxiety disorders display maladaptive, avoidance-
based emotion regulation patterns that interfere with
successful, meaningful pursuits. Yet, despite this exten-
sive literature, comparatively less is known about the
actual goals that people with anxiety disorders choose
to pursue and how well they fare when they pursue
them. This is in part because research thus far on goal
pursuit has primarily examined behavioral dispositions
and tendencies (e.g., avoidance) without addressing
additional, higher levels of personality.
McAdams’s (1996) three-tiered model suggests that
personality exists at three levels of analysis. Level 1
consists of personality traits or general behavioral ten-
dencies (e.g., the Big Five traits of Openness to Experi-
ence, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness,
and Neuroticism), Level 2 consists of personal strivings
or life projects that guide a person’s daily behavior
(e.g., striving for strong family relationships), and Level
3 consists of overarching life narratives that are indica-
tive of how a person consolidates his or her life experi-
ences (e.g., pivotal turning points, moments of
redemption). The present study added to a small body
of work integrating Level 2 of personality—idiographic
strivings—into the study of anxiety disorders (Kashdan,
Breen, & Julian, 2010; Rodebaugh, 2007; Rodebaugh &
Shumaker, 2012). Personal strivings are important life
goals (Emmons, 1986, 1989). People devote consider-
able resources (e.g., time, money) toward planning and
pursuing their strivings. Strivings serve as a compass
that guides individuals toward behavior aligned with
core values. Strivings are idiographic and formed within
the context of a person’s unique life experiences, but
they may contain similar themes that allow for com-
parisons between groups of people. For instance, one
person might strive to “be an exceptional uncle to my
three nieces.” A different person might strive to “be
supportive to my wife.” Each striving is unique because
it refers to specific individuals in each person’s life
(nieces vs. wife), but both strivings contain themes
related to developing and maintaining interpersonal
relationships. In this way, strivings contain important
information about personality that is not captured in
assessments of one’s general behavioral tendencies
(e.g., personality traits).
Strivings are relevant to understanding people with
and without various mental health disorders (e.g.,
depression; Emmons & King, 1988). We focused on SAD
for several reasons. SAD can be distinguished from
other anxiety disorders by a lack of approach-oriented
behavior—even in neutral, nonthreatening situations—
and persistently low positive emotions (for a meta-
analysis, see Kashdan, 2007). People with SAD exhibit
positivity deficits, including disqualifying or rejecting
positive feedback and difficulty recalling positive mem-
ories (e.g., Moscovitch, Gavric, Merrifield, Bielak, &
Moscovitch, 2011; Weeks, Menatti, & Howell, 2015; for
a review, see Gilboa-Schechtman, Shachar, & Sahar,
2014) that likely interfere with strivings pursuit. Fortu-
nately, experimental and treatment research has shown
that motivation is malleable. Laboratory interventions
for people with SAD have been successful in increasing
approach behaviors during social interactions, which
in turn facilitate more positive and rewarding experi-
ences (Taylor & Alden, 2011; Taylor & Amir, 2012).
Moreover, many benefits emerge when people with
SAD make productive strides toward important goals.
Experience-sampling research has shown that on days
when people with SAD devoted effort toward a striving
central to their purpose in life, they experienced a boost
in positive emotions, self-esteem, and meaning in life
(Kashdan & McKnight, 2013). For these reasons, we
recruited community adults diagnosed with SAD and a
healthy control group to draw comparisons. We used
a combination of qualitative and quantitative analyses
to examine the consequences (e.g., success, enjoy-
ment), motives, and content of each striving. We also
examined how different content themes and motives
predicted subjective well-being over the course of 14
days. Results offer insight into the factors that put peo-
ple with anxiety disorders at a disadvantage when they
pursue important goals.
Personal Strivings
Strivings pursuit
People with SAD are more likely than healthy individuals
to encounter significant challenges when pursuing goals.
They are characterized by high levels of experiential
Anxiety Disorders and Strivings 285
avoidance or an unwillingness to experience uncomfort-
able thoughts, feelings, and sensations (Kashdan etal.,
2013; Kashdan etal., 2014; Panayiotou, Karekla, & Mete,
2014). An avoidant emotion regulation strategy is prob-
lematic for goal pursuit because important goals often
require a person to overcome discomfort and adversity.
Whereas these obstacles might motivate someone low
in anxiety to work harder to accommodate for setbacks
(e.g., Rothermund & Brandstädter, 2003), someone with
SAD is likely to view these experiences as impediments.
Goal pursuit becomes arduous, unpleasant, and ulti-
mately, less successful (Johnson, Carver, & Fulford, 2010).
Goal pursuit has traditionally been studied as an
intrapersonal process. There is emerging support, how-
ever, for interpersonal processes that influence how
people construct and obtain important goals (Fitzsimons
& Finkel, 2010). People often construct and pursue
goals with other people, and these interpersonal rela-
tionships influence the relative success of goal pursuit.
For instance, if a person is staying up late to work on
a report, a supportive relationship with his or her
spouse (e.g., offering words of encouragement, cooking
dinner) will likely facilitate a more positive influence
than a less supportive relationship (e.g., spouse making
him or her feel guilty for not spending quality time
together). When a person shares a goal with a col-
league, friend, or romantic partner, the goal pursuit is
intensified (Shteynberg & Galinsky, 2011). People with
SAD may lack these social support networks that facili-
tate successful goal pursuit.
Strivings motives
The motives that underlie goal pursuit typically fall on
two continuums: autonomous motivation and con-
trolled motivation (Ryan & Connell, 1989; Shahar,
Henrich, Blatt, Ryan, & Little, 2003; Sheldon & Kasser,
1995). Autonomous motivation is characterized by the
pursuit of a goal because of inherent enjoyment or
congruence with one’s values or interests. Controlled
motivation is characterized by behavior that is governed
by external factors rather than the self; people feel
pressure to pursue a goal because of other people’s
expectations. Importantly, autonomous and controlled
motivations are not opposite ends of a single contin-
uum and can be measured as separate but related
motives (Ratelle, Guay, Vallerand, Larose, & Senécal,
2007). Regarding the source of motivation, people with
SAD disproportionately focus on social evaluation and
attempt to behave in ways that will garnish social
approval. They believe that other people have exces-
sively high social standards and often believe that they
are incapable of meeting them (Alden & Wallace, 1991,
1995; Wallace & Alden, 1991). Thus, they monitor and
alter their behavior in an attempt to meet perceived
expectations that other people hold (Clark & Wells,
1995). When they construct and pursue important goals,
they are likely motivated to please other people and
meet social standards in hopes of avoiding rejection
(forms of controlled motivation).
Strivings content
The types of strivings that individuals pursue offer
insight into their psychological functioning. Research
thus far on strivings and psychopathology is sparse, and
findings are mixed. Some research on anxiety and mood
symptoms suggests that a high percentage of avoidance
and/or a low percentage of approach strivings may be
a predictor of clinically significant symptoms. In one
study, adolescents high in anxiety generated similar fre-
quencies of avoidance goals (but fewer approach goals)
compared with less anxious peers (Dickson & MacLeod,
2004). In another study, adults who previously met diag-
nostic criteria for depression generated more avoidance
goals compared with never-depressed peers, although
the groups did not differ on approach goals (Vergara &
Roberts, 2011). Content analyses can be used to identify
themes in a person’s strivings and elucidate how people
prioritize their time.
Content analyses examining links with anxiety have
focused almost exclusively on approach/avoidance
themes. Although avoidance is a central diagnostic fea-
ture across anxiety disorders, there is heterogeneity in
the frequency and function of avoidance. For example,
there is evidence to suggest that at least a subset of
individuals with SAD engage in approach-oriented and
exploratory behavior (Kashdan & McKnight, 2010), a
profile that deviates from the prototypical inhibited
person with SAD. A host of other themes is potentially
relevant to the phenomenology of anxiety disorders,
and SAD in particular. In the current study, in addition
to analyzing approach/avoidance themes, we broad-
ened the focus to include nine additional themes
(Emmons, 1999; Kashdan etal., 2010) that are relevant
to anxiety disorders. We expand on each below and
describe their particular relevance to SAD.
Strivings may contain themes of power (establishing
impact, control, and/or influence) and achievement
(concern with success, accomplishment). Social rank
systems are organized around hierarchies in which
dominant people are situated at the top and possess
more resources, influence, and power. Evolutionary
theorists posit that all humans use social rank systems,
but people with SAD tend to overuse them and place
themselves lower on social rank systems (Trower &
Gilbert, 1989). To avoid offending more socially domi-
nant peers, they might default to submissive behavior
286 Goodman et al.
(e.g., averted eye contact; Weisman, Aderka, Marom,
Hermesh, & Gilboa-Schechtman, 2011) or abstain from
striving for goals that could signal that they are trying
to usurp more powerful others. This contributes, in
part, to a “fear of doing well,” because striving for a
particular goal might create conflict with other people
and/or unintentionally create higher social standards
(Gilbert, 2001; Weeks, Heimberg, & Rodebaugh, 2008).
Other people might expect more of them in future
situations—standards that people with SAD believe they
cannot meet.
Themes related to one’s self-concept include self-
presentation (making a favorable impression) and self-
sufficiency (not relying on other people, being
autonomous). All individuals with SAD have the func-
tional goal of avoiding evaluation and preventing rejec-
tion. This is perhaps best described in Moscovitch’s
(2009) distillation of the core fear of SAD: “Individuals
with social phobia are uniquely and primarily con-
cerned about characteristics of self that they perceive
as being deficient or contrary to perceived societal
expectations or norms” (p. 3). In an effort to receive
social approval and prevent rejection, people with SAD
frequently search for cues about how other people are
evaluating them (Cougle etal., 2012; Heerey & Kring,
2007). Thus, people with SAD are uniquely concerned
about how they present themselves to other people and
tend to have negatively biased beliefs about their
self-worth.
Strivings may contain content related to emotion
regulation, which involves any attempts to monitor,
manage, and/or change the experience or expression
of emotion. Theorists have proposed that emotion dys-
regulation is central to the development and mainte-
nance of SAD (Kashdan etal., 2011; Turk, Heimberg,
Luterek, Mennin, & Fresco, 2005). In fact, frequency of
experiential avoidance is one of only two characteristics
(the other being low positive affect) that differentiate
people with SAD from healthy adults (Kashdan etal.,
2013). These findings suggest that people with SAD are
more distinguishable by excessive attempts to avoid
their emotional experiences than by the presence of
anxiety or other negative emotions.
Strivings often involve goals related to other people
and may contain themes related to interpersonal rela-
tionships broadly and/or more specific references to
the quality of relationships through affiliation (concern
for or desire to establish, maintain, or repair friend-
ships) or intimacy (commitment to and concern for
personal relationships). People with SAD encounter
significant difficulty establishing, developing, and main-
taining relationships (Alden & Taylor, 2004). They
endorse high levels of functional impairment in nearly
every interpersonal domain—friends (Rodebaugh,
2009), family (Schneier etal., 1994), and romantic rela-
tionships (Sparrevohn & Rapee, 2009). Moreover, when
socially anxious individuals do establish close relation-
ships, they continue to experience interpersonal diffi-
culties, including diminished emotional expression,
self-disclosure, and intimacy (Sparrevohn & Rapee,
2009). These interpersonal deficits likely generalize to
goal pursuit.
Last, strivings may contain content related to genera-
tivity, which involves creating something to make a
lasting contribution, such as contributing to society
(particularly for the betterment of future generations)
and leaving a legacy (McAdams, 1985). Expressions of
generativity involve effort to create or maintain ideas,
things, and relationships that represent a person’s leg-
acy. Generative behaviors (e.g., volunteerism), concerns
(e.g., desire to be generative), and strivings are posi-
tively related to meaning in life and subjective well-
being (Emmons, 2005; Sheldon & Kasser, 2001).
Strivings and Subjective Well-Being
In addition to distinguishing between groups of people,
strivings provide information on broader psychological
functioning. Just as personality traits at Level 1 have
been extensively examined as predictors of subjective
well-being (e.g., Big Five traits; DeNeve & Cooper,
1998; Steel, Schmidt, & Shultz, 2008), researchers can
explore how constructs at higher levels of personality
offer insight into how a person fares in his or her daily
life (e.g., Brunstein, 1993; Diener & Fujita, 1995;
Emmons, 1986). In terms of strivings, meaningful life
goals are generally considered one prerequisite for sub-
jective well-being (for a comprehensive review of pre-
dictors and correlates of subjective well-being, see
Diener, Lucas, & Oishi, 2018). Moreover, research sug-
gests that the extent to which strivings are associated
with subjective well-being depends, in part, on the
content of and motives underlying the striving. For
example, strivings containing themes of intimacy, gen-
erativity, or affiliation have been positively associated
with well-being, whereas power strivings are negatively
associated with well-being (Ackerman, Zuroff, &
Moskowitz, 2000; Emmons, 1991). Similarly, motivation
theorists have long postulated that goals driven by
intrinsic/autonomous motives are typically associated
with greater subjective well-being than those pursued
for extrinsic/controlled motives (e.g., self-determination
theory; Ryan & Deci, 2000). The majority of studies on
strivings and well-being have been conducted with trait
measures of subjective well-being. Trait questionnaires
offer useful information about a person’s general ten-
dencies but do not capture variation across time and
contexts. Experience-sampling measures partially
Anxiety Disorders and Strivings 287
address this limitation by capturing processes as they
unfold in real time, thus minimizing recall biases (Reis &
Gable, 2000). In the present study, participants recorded
state assessments of subjective well-being dimensions
across a 14-day period.
The Present Study
The purpose of this study was to provide a framework
to examine personal strivings among people with anxi-
ety disorders. Using SAD as an exemplar, we examined
idiographic strivings among community adults diag-
nosed with SAD and a healthy control group. We tested
the following hypotheses about the consequences of
pursuing each striving (e.g., success, enjoyment),
motives that underlie pursuit, and content themes. Com-
pared with healthy adults, people with SAD would (a)
report greater difficulty and less effort, enjoyment, and
success in pursuit of their strivings; (b) report less social
support for their goals, encounter more conflict while
pursuing their goals, and be less willing to disclose
details of their goals with other people; (c) endorse
higher levels of controlled motives and lower levels of
autonomous motives; and (d) construct more strivings
related to avoidance, emotion regulation, self-
presentation, and self-sufficiency and fewer strivings
related to affiliation, interpersonal relationships, inti-
macy, power, and achievement. We did not have a priori
hypotheses for themes of generativity and considered
these analyses exploratory. On an exploratory basis, we
examined whether particular striving themes and motives
in conjunction with SAD operate together to predict
three indices of daily well-being: positive affect, nega-
tive affect, and meaning in life. As the first examination
of these moderation models (i.e., the presence of SAD
interacting with striving themes and motives), all tests
and results should be considered exploratory.1
Method
Participants
Participants were 84 community adults recruited from
northern Virginia and the greater Washington, D.C.,
metropolitan area, including 41 individuals diagnosed
with SAD (generalized subtype) and 43 healthy controls
currently absent of psychiatric disorders. Participants
with SAD were excluded if they met criteria for current
psychotic or substance use disorders or criteria only for
the nongeneralized subtype. All participants were
native English speakers. Participants’ mean age was
28.68 years (SD = 8.54), and 61.9% were female. The
sample was ethnically diverse: 54.8% identified as Cau-
casian, 20.2% African American, 9.5% Latino/Hispanic,
4.8% Asian, 1.2% Middle Eastern, and 9.5% other. Par-
ticipants’ self-reported family income varied: 11.90%
reported a family income of less than $15,000, 3.57%
reported $15,000 to $24,999, 17.86% reported $25,000
to $34,999, 14.29% reported $35,000 to $49,999, 17.86%
reported $50,000 to $64,999, 4.76% reported $65,000 to
$79,000, 28.57% reported greater than $80,000, and 1
participant did not report income. The highest educa-
tional degree obtained varied: 46% received a high
school diploma, 29.8% completed some college, 7.2%
associate degree or professional school, 33.3% bache-
lor’s degree, 2.4% some graduate school, 14.3% master’s
degree, 4.8% doctoral degree, and 2 participants did
not report educational status. The two diagnostic groups
did not significantly differ on any aforementioned
demographics (ps = .66–.86). In terms of comorbid con-
ditions, 11 participants in the SAD group met criteria
for a specific phobia, 7 for major depressive disorder,
5 for dysthymia, 5 for posttraumatic stress disorder, 3
for generalized anxiety disorder, 2 for panic disorder,
2 for obsessive-compulsive disorder, and 1 for agora-
phobia. Ten participants were taking psychiatric medi-
cation and 8 received treatment. Self-reported estimated
age of SAD onset varied, from 5 to 18 years of age. Six
participants did not provide end-of-day reports and
were excluded from day-level analyses; these parti-
cipants did not differ from participants who completed
end-of-day reports on diagnoses or demographics
(ps > .10).
Procedure
Participants were recruited from the community using
online advertisements (e.g., listservs) and fliers. Trained
research assistants conducted preliminary phone
screens with prospective participants to streamline
recruitment. After obtaining informed consent, research
assistants conducted semistructured interviews to assess
for symptoms of social anxiety, generalized anxiety,
depression, suicidality, and psychosis. If participants
endorsed evidence suggestive of social anxiety (SAD
group) or endorsed no psychological symptoms
(healthy control group), then research assistants sched-
uled an initial face-to-face assessment. During this
assessment, participants again took part in an informed
consent process and then completed a series of self-
report trait and demographic questionnaires and a striv-
ings assessment (see below for details). Clinical
psychology doctoral students then administered the
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I Disor-
ders (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 2002) to assess
for mood, anxiety, eating, substance use, and psychotic
disorders. To gain further information about social anxi-
ety symptoms, doctoral students administered the SAD
288 Goodman et al.
module of the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule
for DSM–IV: Lifetime Version (Di Nardo, Brown, & Bar-
low, 1994). In addition to meeting the criteria for SAD
from the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM–IV; American Psy-
chiatric Association, 1994), participants in the SAD
group had to endorse fear and avoidance of three or
more social situations (beyond performance settings).
Participants with comorbid disorders were included in
the SAD group if SAD was the primary or most severe
diagnosis. To ensure an accurate diagnosis of SAD, 45
recorded interviews were randomly chosen and rated
by multiple researchers. To determine interrater reli-
ability of diagnoses, two clinical psychology doctoral
students with multiple years of training in diagnostic
assessment served as raters. Each individual watched
videotaped interviews of research participants. Predis-
cussion interrater reliability for generalized SAD diag-
noses had excellent agreement (κ = .87). Any diagnostic
discrepancies were discussed until a consensus was
reached. Participants were then matched to healthy
community participants via targeted advertisements on
age, sex, and ethnicity.
After completing the baseline questionnaires and
diagnostic interview, eligible participants completed a
90-min training session to receive instructions on the
experience-sampling portion of the study. Each partici-
pant received a handheld computer (Palm Pilot X22)
preprogrammed with the Purdue Momentary Assess-
ment Tool (PMAT; Weiss, Beal, Lucy, & MacDermid,
2004). Research assistants explained how to use the
software. Participants completed three types of daily
recordings: self-initiated surveys of social interactions,
random prompts, and end-of-day surveys. For the pres-
ent study, we used only end-of-day data. All entries
were automatically time stamped.
Two days after participants began recording
experience-sampling data, we contacted participants to
troubleshoot and resolve any problems that arose. We
sent multiple emails throughout the 2-week diary data
collection period to emphasize compliance with diary
entries. We intentionally created brief measures to
maintain participant engagement and motivation to
maximize compliance without sacrificing data reliability
or validity (see Nezlek, 2012). Financial compensation
was structured to maximize compliance, such that par-
ticipants could earn up to $50 in bonus compensation
(50 cents for each diary prompt and a $10 bonus for 1
week of uninterrupted completed reports). Participants
earned a minimum of $165 by participating in the study,
thereby earning a maximum payment of $215. This
study was approved by the university institutional
review board and carried out in accordance with the
provisions of the World Medical Association Declaration
of Helsinki.
Measures
Strivings assessment. Participants generated a list of
six strivings using an open-ended format (Emmons,
1999). Strivings were defined as “an objective that you
are typically trying to accomplish or attain” and “goals or
purposes that motivate [you]”). Examples of strivings
were presented, such as “trying to be a good role model
to others” and “trying to avoid feeling inferior to others.”
Participants were informed that strivings could be nega-
tive/avoidance oriented or positive/approach oriented.
After participants generated their strivings, they provided
retrospective reports about their subjective experience
and behavior toward each striving (i.e., consequences)
over the past month on seven dimensions. These dimen-
sions are from Emmons’s (1986) personal strivings assess-
ment and demonstrated adequate validity in a range of
populations including college undergraduates (Coats,
Janoff-Bulman, & Alpert, 1996; Romero, Villar, Luengo, &
Gómez-Fraguela, 2009), athletes (Smith, Ntoumanis, &
Duda, 2007), and individuals diagnosed with anxiety dis-
orders (Kashdan etal., 2010; Kashdan & McKnight, 2013).
Four dimensions were related to intrapersonal goal pur-
suit (difficulty, effort, enjoyment, past success) and three
dimensions were related to interpersonal goal pursuit
(interpersonal conflict, social disclosure, social support).
Participants also rated the degree to which they pur-
sued their strivings for four different motives. Two
motives measured autonomous strivings: internal
importance (“because you believe that it is important”)
and internal desire (“enjoyment or stimulation”). Two
motives measured controlled strivings: external pres-
sure (“because someone else wants you to”) and guilt
(“because you would feel guilty if you didn’t”). These
items are from Sheldon and Kasser’s (1995, 1998)
method of measuring self-concordance of people’s rea-
sons for pursuing their goals. They are rooted in self-
determination theory and self-regulation (Deci & Ryan,
1985, 2000) and broader frameworks of motivation
(e.g., Shahar etal., 2003). These four items represent
the four dimensions outlined in Ryan and Connell’s
perceived locus of causality, a theory of motivation that
outlines reasons that people pursue goals. These scales
have demonstrated to be psychometrically sound in
previous research (Koestner, Lekes, Powers, & Chicoine,
2002; Sheldon & Houser-Marko, 2001). All items
(motives and consequences) were rated on a 7-point
Likert-type scale from 1 (not at all) to 7 (extremely).
Because they are single-item measures, reliability analy-
ses were not conducted.
Daily subjective well-being.2 At the end of each day,
participants answered questions about their day. For pos-
itive and negative affect items, they were instructed, “Use
the scale provided to indicate how well each adjective or
Anxiety Disorders and Strivings 289
phrase describes your mood today.” They provided rat-
ings on 12 adjectives using a Likert-type scale from 1 (very
slightly/not at all) to 5 (extremely). Six items measured
positive affect (content, relaxed, enthusiastic, joyful, proud,
and interested) and six items measured negative affect
(anxious, angry, sluggish, sad, irritable, distressed). These
emotion adjectives reflect items from the PANAS (Watson,
Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) and include each dimension of
high/low positive/negative arousal (circumplex model of
emotions; Barrett, 1998). Daily meaning in life was mea-
sured with two items (“How meaningful did you feel your
life was today?” and “How much did you feel your life had
purpose today?”) rated from 1 (very uncharacteristic of
me) to 7 (very characteristic of me). These items have been
validated in prior daily diary studies (Kashdan & Steger,
2007; Machell, Goodman, & Kashdan, 2015; Steger, Kashdan,
& Oishi, 2008). Reliability estimates for daily measures were
calculated using unconditional three-level models with
items nested within days within people (Nezlek, 2012).
Reliability for positive affect (.64), negative affect (.60),
and meaning in life (.88) was acceptable.
Results
Analytic overview
We examined how people diagnosed with SAD and
healthy controls differed in the consequences, motives,
and content of their strivings. Group differences in striv-
ings consequences and motives were analyzed with
independent-samples t tests. Joint probabilities of endors-
ing specific content in a striving with diagnostic status
were analyzed with chi-square tests. A series of multilevel
models was used for well-being day-level outcomes, in
which days (Level 1) were nested within people (Level
2). For each model, a strivings frequency or motive was
entered grand-mean centered at Level 2 with daily well-
being as the outcome. We examined main effects across
groups and then examined group differences by entering
the SAD diagnostic group (SAD or healthy control)
uncentered as a Level 2 moderator. Significant modera-
tions were probed by examining simple slopes.
Coding
Strivings were categorized by trained coders for the
presence or absence of 10 themes. Nine themes were
derived from Emmons’s (1999) coding manual, and one
theme (emotion regulation) was derived from a prior
study of strivings among military veterans (Kashdan
etal., 2010). For a given category, each striving was
given a score of 1 if the theme was present (e.g., if the
striving indicated a desire for power) and a score of 0
if the theme was absent. Thematic categories were not
mutually exclusive. A single striving could, for example,
contain themes of power, affiliation, and generativity.
See Table 1 for the definition and examples of each
theme.
Prior to completing coder training, the authors of
this article coded a practice data set of 600 strivings
Table 1. Strivings Type, Definition, and Representative Examples
Type Definition Example
Achievement Accomplishing a goal, concern with success or
accomplishment
“Try to be a success in my field”; “Quitting
smoking”
Affiliation Concern for or desire to establish, maintain, or
repair friendships
“Trying to maintain a good group of friends”;
“Trying to keep in touch with friends”
Avoidance Trying not to do something or avoid, prevent, or
get rid of objective of striving
“Trying to avoid feeling inferior to others”; “Try
not to offend others”
Emotion regulation Concern for emotion and reference to monitoring,
managing, or attempting to change it
“Trying to be comfortable at parties”; “Not get
embarrassed so easily”
Generativity Providing for the next generation, creating
something, giving oneself, a lasting contribution
“Spend more time w/ my son”; “To leave a good
mark in life”
Interpersonal An objective or goal focused on others “Trying to help others”; “Let people know I am
angry with them”
Intimacy Commitment and concern for another person,
quality of relationships rather than quantity
“Trying to have a successful marriage”; “Have
meaningful relationships”
Power Establishing, maintaining, or restoring power
(impact, control, influence)
“Seek public office”; “Trying to be more assertive”
Self-presentation Making a favorable impression on others “Role model for others to see”; “Trying to make
others like me”
Self-sufficiency Being an individual, not having to depend on
others
“Work on starting own business”; “Trying to be
more independent”
290 Goodman et al.
from an archival data set not used in the present study.
The authors coded each striving according to the cod-
ing manual (see Appendix) and resolved all discrepan-
cies through discussion until a consensus was reached.
After a consensus data set of coded strivings was final-
ized, coders began training using this practice data set.
Each coder independently read and coded the striv-
ings. Coders worked in quiet locations without distrac-
tion and set a timer to reread the coding manual of the
theme they were coding every 10 min. They were also
unaware of participant details, and none of the strivings
revealed information about diagnostic status. Coders
did not have access to the ratings that participants made
about their strivings. Coding training occurred in
batches of three thematic categories at one time (e.g.,
self-presentation, achievement, and intimacy). Each
batch comprised three practice coding rounds and a
subsequent round of coding the data from the present
study. To determine accuracy, when coders completed
one practice round (approximately 200 individual striv-
ings), their ratings were compared against the master
coded data set to calculate a match percentage. Meet-
ings were held between study authors and coders to
provide feedback (e.g., address common mistakes),
discuss coding issues, and resolve ambiguities. Coders
were required to achieve at least an 80% match by the
third round of coding practice data before coding data
used in the present study.
Each participant (N = 84) generated 6 unique striv-
ings, totaling 504 strivings. Reliability was calculated
between the coders for each of the categories (see
Table 2). The value reported is the average of the three
Cohen’s kappa calculated between two of the three
raters (.73). According to benchmarks in the literature
(Landis & Koch, 1977), seven categories (achievement,
affiliation, avoidance, emotion regulation, generativity,
interpersonal, and intimacy) demonstrated substantial
agreement (.61–.89), and three categories (power, self-
presentation, and self-sufficiency) demonstrated moder-
ate agreement (.45–.52).
Strivings consequences
We examined how people with SAD differed from
healthy controls in consequences of pursuing strivings
(see Table 3). Compared with healthy controls, people
with SAD reported more difficulty (t = −4.11, p < .001)
and less past success (t = 4.49, p < .001). People with
and without SAD did not differ on effort exerted (t =
1.99, p = .05), although results were just below signifi-
cance with trends suggesting that people with SAD
exert less effort. The two diagnostic groups reported
similar levels of pleasure while pursuing strivings (t =
0.62, p = .44).
People with SAD also demonstrated interpersonal
difficulties during their pursuits. They reported greater
interpersonal conflict (t = −3.08, p = .003) and were less
likely to share their strivings with other people (t =
2.30, p = .02). Contrary to the hypotheses, people with
SAD and healthy controls reported similar levels of
social support (t = 1.55, p = .13).
Strivings motives
We examined participants’ motives for pursuing each
striving (see Table 3). Compared with healthy adults,
people with SAD endorsed a greater degree of con-
trolled motivation; they reported more external pres-
sure to pursue their strivings (t = −2.48, p = .02) and
anticipated more guilt if they did not pursue them (t =
Table 2. Strivings Descriptive Statistics and Chi-Square Tests
Coder
reliability
(Cohen’s
kappa)
Frequency (%)
Type
SAD
group
Control
group χ2p
Achievement .61 16.3 19.4 0.83 .36
Affiliation .72 17.9 15.1 0.74 .39
Avoidance .89 12.7 9.9 0.97 .33
Emotion regulation .79 17.1 6.7 12.79 < .001
Generativity .69 8.0 8.7 0.10 .79
Interpersonal .80 26.6 28.6 0.25 .62
Intimacy .68 11.1 15.1 1.70 .19
Power .52 2.8 6.3 3.69 .06
Self-presentation .49 10.7 11.1 0.16 .36
Self-sufficiency .45 4.0 2.0 1.72 .19
Note: Frequencies were calculated within group. SAD = social anxiety disorder.
Anxiety Disorders and Strivings 291
−2.93, p = .004). People with SAD also endorsed a lesser
degree of autonomous motivation than healthy adults;
they reported less internal importance (t = 2.42, p =
.04) and enjoyment (t = 2.66, p = .045) as motivations
for pursuing their strivings.
Strivings content
After coding was finalized, frequency scores were calcu-
lated for each theme for each participant. Using chi-
square tests of independence, we found that diagnostic
status was unrelated to 9 of 10 strivings themes (ps > .06;
see Table 2 for full results). The exception was for emo-
tion regulation; people with SAD generated significantly
more strivings focused on emotion regulation compared
with healthy adults (p < .001). For strivings related to
power, there was a trend to suggest that people with SAD
recorded fewer power strivings than healthy adults, but
this effect failed to reach statistical significance (p = .06).
Strivings predicting daily well-being
We examined how strivings content and motives pre-
dicted daily well-being across a 2-week period. Results
suggest that the type of strivings constructed were
largely unrelated to daily well-being (full results are
presented in Table 4). There were two exceptions:
Greater frequency of interpersonal strivings predicted
greater daily positive affect (b = 1.02, t = 2.14, p < .05),
and frequency of emotion regulation strivings predicted
greater daily negative affect (b = 0.65, t = 2.20, p < .05).
In addition to main effects, we examined group differ-
ences in cross-level effects (i.e., to what extent does the
relationship between strivings content/motives differ for
people with SAD compared with healthy controls?). Simi-
lar to main effects, the two diagnostic groups largely did
not differ (see Table 4). There were four exceptions. SAD
diagnosis moderated the relationship between frequency
of affiliation strivings on daily positive affect (b = 0.91,
t= 2.24, p < .05) and on daily meaning in life (b = 0.68,
t = 2.35, p < .05). Analyses of simple slopes suggest that
for people with SAD, greater frequency of affiliation
strivings was associated with greater daily positive affect
(β = 1.29, z = 3.37, p < .05) and meaning (β = 1.01, z =
2.40, p < .05); for healthy controls, however, greater
frequency of affiliation strivings was unrelated to daily
positive affect (β = −0.53, z = −0.88, p = .38) and mean-
ing in life (β = −0.35, z = −0.88, p = .38). Similarly, SAD
diagnosis moderated the relationship between frequency
of intimacy strivings on daily meaning in life (b = 0.70,
t = 2.06, p < .05) and frequency of interpersonal strivings
on daily positive affect (b = 0.69, t = 2.03, p < .05).
Analysis of simple slopes suggests that greater frequency
of interpersonal strivings was associated with greater
positive affect for people with SAD (β = 1.46, z = 3.34,
p < .001) but not for healthy controls (β = 11, z = 0.22,
p = .83). Similarly, greater frequency of intimacy strivings
was positively associated with meaning in life for people
with SAD, although this was just below statistical signifi-
cance (β = 1.16, z = 1.95, p = .05); greater frequency of
intimacy strivings was unrelated to meaning in life for
healthy controls (β = −0.25, z = −0.74, p = .46).
Table 3. Strivings Consequences and Motives
M (SD)
Item SAD group Control group t d
Subjective consequences
Intrapersonal
Difficulty 5.38 (0.97) 4.50 (0.99) −4.11*** 0.90
Effort made 4.59 (1.02) 4.96 (0.67) 1.99 0.43
Enjoyment 5.90 (1.02) 6.06 (0.76) 0.79 0.17
Past success 3.77 (1.03) 4.65 (0.74) 4.49*** 0.98
Interpersonal
Interpersonal conflict 2.30 (0.96) 1.68 (0.86) −3.08** 0.67
Disclosure to others 4.24 (1.27) 4.92 (1.45) 2.30* 0.50
Social support 3.91 (1.43) 4.40 (1.44) 1.55 0.34
Motives
External pressure 3.45 (1.52) 2.68 (1.31) −2.49* 0.54
Guilt 4.51 (1.46) 3.57 (1.47) −2.93** 0.64
Internal importance 6.07 (0.73) 6.39 (0.49) 2.42* 0.53
Internal desire 5.13 (1.15) 5.71 (0.83) 2.66** 0.58
Note: SAD = social anxiety disorder.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
292
Table 4. Strivings Content and Motives Predicting Daily Well-Being
Positive affect Negative affect Meaning in life
Main effect SAD moderation Main effect SAD moderation Main effect SAD moderation
Item b t b t b t b t b t b t
Strivings type
Achievement −0.10 −0.26 −0.59 −1.63 −0.14 −0.67 −0.02 −0.10 −0.13 −0.37 −0.63 −1.84
Affiliation −0.05 −0.09 0.91 2.24* 0.08 0.23 −0.20 −0.71 0.09 0.25 0.68 2.35*
Avoidance −0.84 −1.81 0.28 0.69 −0.04 −0.13 −0.07 −0.27 −0.12 −0.32 0.42 1.33
Emotion regulation −0.50 −1.09 −0.21 −0.41 0.65 2.20* 0.31 1.18 −0.07 −0.20 −0.08 −0.25
Generativity 0.59 0.81 −0.41 −0.75 0.61 1.31 0.20 0.59 0.25 0.55 −0.49 −1.41
Interpersonal 1.02 2.14* 0.69 2.03* 0.40 1.15 −0.09 −0.32 0.57 1.64 0.27 1.07
Intimacy 1.01 1.93 0.69 1.41 −0.09 −0.26 0.08 0.25 0.69 1.76 0.70 2.06*
Power 1.6 1.56 −0.03 −0.04 −0.50 −1.00 −0.21 −0.47 1.21 1.78 −0.14 −0.27
Self-presentation −0.003 −0.01 −0.30 −0.75 0.36 1.15 0.25 0.97 0.29 0.76 0.05 0.16
Self-sufficiency −0.74 −1.15 0.64 0.96 0.84 1.57 −0.54 −1.13 −0.60 −0.94 1.11 1.98
Strivings motive
External pressure −1.11 −4.07*** 0.41 1.92 0.51 2.72** 0.41 1.92 −0.68 −2.97** 0.13 0.67
Guilt −0.77 −3.1** 0.57 2.27* 0.56 3.88*** 0.19 1.30 0.38 1.70 0.38 1.70
Internal importance 2.02 3.82*** 0.32 0.61 −0.99 −2.92** 0.31 0.86 1.44 3.81*** 0.30 0.79
Internal desire 1.56 5.28*** −0.22 −0.87 −0.72 −2.98** 0.15 0.66 1.21 5.78*** 0.01 0.05
Note: SAD = social anxiety disorder.
*p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
Anxiety Disorders and Strivings 293
In comparison with strivings content, the motives for
why people pursue different strivings were strongly
associated with daily well-being. Specifically, motives
characterized by internal importance and desire were
positively associated with positive affect (b = 2.02, t =
3.82, p < .001; b = 1.56, t = 5.28, p < .001) and meaning
in life (b = 1.44, t = 3.81, p < .001; b = 1.21, t = 5.78,
p < .001) and inversely associated with negative affect
(b = −0.99, t = −2.92, p < .01; b = −0.72, t = −2.99, p <
.01), respectively. In contrast, motives characterized by
external pressure and guilt were inversely associated
with positive affect (b = −1.11, t = −4.07, p < .001; b =
−0.77, t = −3.10, p < .01) and positively associated with
negative affect (b = 0.51, t = 2.72, p < .01; b = 0.56, t =
3.88, p < .001), respectively. External pressure motives
were inversely associated with daily meaning in life
(b = −0.68, t = −2.97, p < .01); guilt motives were unre-
lated to daily meaning in life (b = 0.38, t = 1.70, p =
.09). As for differences between diagnostic groups, SAD
diagnosis moderated the relationship between guilt
motives and positive affect (b = 0.57, t = 2.27, p < .05);
such guilt motives were unrelated to positive affect for
people with SAD (β = 0.42, z = 1.01, p = .31) and
inversely associated with positive affect for healthy con-
trols (β = −0.71, z = −2.61, p < .01).
Discussion
The present study illustrated an idiographic approach
to examining aspects of personality relevant to the pres-
ence of anxiety disorders. We used qualitative and
quantitative analyses to examine the strivings that indi-
viduals with SAD and healthy adults constructed. Peo-
ple with and without SAD could be differentiated by a
single category—emotion regulation. More so than
healthy adults, people with SAD constructed strivings
around attempts to control, manage, or get rid of emo-
tions. Contrary to the hypotheses, people with and
without SAD constructed similar frequencies of strivings
across the other nine thematic categories. This held true
for social themes related to intimacy, affiliation, and
interpersonal relationships. To the extent that people
with SAD constructed strivings with these themes, how-
ever, they also reported increased daily well-being, a
relationship not found in healthy adults. Although peo-
ple with SAD constructed similar strivings to healthy
peers, difficulties arose in the pursuit of their strivings.
People with SAD were relatively less successful, expe-
rienced more difficulty in their goal pursuits, and
encountered greater interpersonal conflict than healthy
adults. Moreover, people with SAD construct strivings
more so out of guilt and external pressure than because
of intrinsic desire or enjoyment. Taken together, results
suggest that people with SAD construct strivings around
attempts to manage their emotions. But more so than
what they strive for, people with SAD differ from healthy
peers in why and how they strive for important goals.
The what, why, and how of goal
pursuit for people with SAD
Compared with healthy adults, people with SAD
endorsed more difficulty and less success pursuing their
strivings. These results fit with findings that people with
SAD demonstrate significant functional impairment
(Aderka et al., 2012; McKnight, Monfort, Kashdan,
Blalock, & Calton, 2016), suggesting that symptoms of
social anxiety interfere with their ability to be success-
ful across work and interpersonal domains. Moreover,
findings of the present study extend prior research
demonstrating that people with SAD tend to have dif-
ficulty achieving adaptive goals in social situations
(Hiemisch, Ehlers, & Westermann, 2002). In addition to
objective impairment, people with SAD also tend to
have overly negative perceptions of their performance.
Thus, they are likely to underestimate how successful
they were at achieving a goal, regardless of their objec-
tive achievement. This cycle maintains social anxiety
and leads to perceptions of poor social performance,
which induces exhaustive avoidance efforts that limit
reward-seeking behavior and reduce positive experi-
ences (Kashdan & Collins, 2010; Kashdan etal., 2011).
Subjective appraisals do not always relate to other,
objective measures of skilled performance (Hopko,
McNeil, Zvolensky, & Eifert, 2001). It is likely that dif-
ferences in goal pursuit are a combination of explicit
impairment and negatively biased subjective appraisals
of performance.
A similar pattern of results emerged for interpersonal
consequences of goal pursuit. In the present study,
people with SAD endorsed greater interpersonal con-
flict and were less likely to share their strivings with
other people. Self-disclosure is generally an adaptive
way to establish positive interactions and build intimacy
with other people (Laurenceau, Barrett, & Pietromon-
aco, 1998). For people with SAD, the idea of revealing
personal details about their life can be daunting. They
worry that if someone sees their “real self, which they
believe is flawed and deficient, other people will view
them poorly and ultimately reject them (Moscovitch,
2009). To ward off negative evaluation, they tend to
spend less time talking in social interactions, avoid
intimate topics, and fail to match other people’s level
of disclosure (Meleshko & Alden, 1993; Papsdorf &
Alden, 1998; Voncken & Dijk, 2013). Interestingly, how-
ever, there were no differences in social support
between individuals with SAD and healthy controls.
Although people with SAD tend to have fewer close
294 Goodman et al.
relationships than nonanxious adults, it is possible that
they are able to obtain social support from the small
number of close others in their life; the number of close
relationships does not necessarily correspond with per-
ceived social support (Cohen & Syme, 1985). Perhaps
the presence of one close friend or partner is sufficient
to feel supported. Nonetheless, this conclusion is spec-
ulative, as quantity and quality of social relationships
were not measured in the present study. Future research
is needed to examine the dynamics of interpersonal
relationships and goal pursuit for people with SAD.
To understand why people construct their goals, we
explored the motives that propelled participants to con-
struct strivings. Compared with healthy adults, people
with SAD were less driven by intrinsic desire and enjoy-
ment (i.e., autonomous motivation) and more driven
by guilt and pressure (i.e., controlled motivation).
These findings fit with theoretical models of self-
presentation that suggest that social anxiety arises out
of a desire to make a favorable impression and please
other people (Schlenker & Leary, 1982). They are moti-
vated to receive social approval and frequently search
for cues about how other people are evaluating them.
If they believe that they are not living up to social
standards, they are likely to feel pressure to pursue
certain goals because they believe that other people
expect them to, not because they inherently want to.
Although people with SAD differed from healthy
adults in their motives for and consequences of pursu-
ing strivings, the types of strivings they constructed
were remarkably similar. People with SAD and healthy
adults endorsed a similar frequency of strivings across
nine of 10 thematic categories (emotion regulation
being the exception). Even in categories related to
interpersonal relationships (i.e., affiliation, intimacy,
and interpersonal), people with SAD did not differ from
their healthy peers. This finding may seem counterintui-
tive given that people with SAD often try to avoid
interpersonal situations that increase the potential for
scrutiny. Yet, simply fearing or avoiding interpersonal
connection does not mean that one lacks desire. One
could hypothesize that because of an inadequate social
network, people with SAD strive for interpersonal con-
nection even more so than nonanxious peers. The
presence of pathology does not rule out basic psycho-
logical human needs; humans have a fundamental
desire to connect with other people (Ryan & Deci,
2000). Results of the present study suggest that even
in the face of excessive and often debilitating fear of
negative evaluation, people with SAD strive for human
connection to the same degree as their nonanxious
peers.
A finding that also runs contrary to study hypotheses
is that people with SAD did not endorse a greater fre-
quency of avoidance strivings than healthy adults. We
offer three explanations for this finding. First, although
the prototypical person with SAD is characterized by
avoidance and inhibited behavior, emerging research
suggests that not all people with SAD engage in avoid-
ance to manage their anxiety (Kachin, Newman, &
Pincus, 2001; Kashdan, Elhai, & Breen, 2008; Kashdan
& Hofmann, 2008). At least a subset of individuals with
SAD, with some estimates as high as approximately 1
in 5, engage in approach-oriented behavior (Kashdan,
McKnight, Richey, & Hofmann, 2009). Rather than avoid
social situations, people in this subgroup may approach
them with aggression or impulsivity. Second, it is pos-
sible that categorization of avoidance did not differenti-
ate the two groups because avoidance was measured
broadly, rather than in social or performance situations
(e.g., Rodebaugh, 2007; Trew & Alden, 2012). Third, it
is possible that the strivings assessment used in the
present study is insufficient to capture the subtle avoid-
ance that people with SAD typically engage in. Partici-
pants were asked to think about broader overarching
life goals, an approach that may overlook everyday
avoidance behaviors. For instance, people with SAD
frequently use safety behaviors to manage or conceal
their anxiety. These behaviors vary from more subtle
behaviors (e.g., averted eye contact) to more conscious
attempts to conceal signs of anxiety (e.g., wearing
excessive makeup to hide blushing). Moreover, the
avoidance central to SAD may be cognitive-emotional
rather than behavioral; people with SAD are character-
ized by experiential avoidance, an unwillingness to
experience negative emotions, thoughts, and feelings
(Kashdan etal., 2013). This internal avoidance, such as
trying to get rid of anxiety-provoking thoughts during
a job interview, is unlikely to be captured with a broad
strivings assessment. Similarly, it is possible that people
with SAD differ on goals explicitly focused on manag-
ing social anxiety and related emotions (e.g., Krans, de
Bree, & Bryant, 2014; Krans etal., 2017).
The only strivings category that differentiated people
with SAD from healthy controls was emotion regulation.
This finding adds to growing literature that suggests that
people with SAD exert considerable effort to try to alter
their emotional experiences (for a review, see Jazaieri
etal., 2015). Beyond daily tendencies to regulate emo-
tions (e.g., Kashdan & Steger, 2006), findings from the
present study suggest that people with SAD explicitly
construct their lives around attempts to regulate their
emotions. Thus, people with SAD demonstrate some
degree of awareness that they want to manage their
emotion and put forth considerable effort to do so. Of
course, the focus on emotion regulation is not inherently
problematic, as some emotion regulation strategies are
adaptive and inversely related to psychopathology
(Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010). However,
when one of someone’s most important goals is to
Anxiety Disorders and Strivings 295
control their emotions, it likely comes at the expense
of pursuing, achieving, and enjoying other, more reward-
ing experiences.
Strivings and subjective well-being
As with personality traits such as the Big Five, strivings
can provide information about subjective well-being.
Results suggest that the motives for pursuing strivings
were associated with all measured types of subjective
well-being, whereas the thematic content of a striving
was less informative. Specifically, autonomous motives
were associated with greater positive affect and mean-
ing in life and less negative affect, whereas controlled
motives were associated with less positive affect and
meaning in life (although not for guilt) and greater
negative affect. This finding fits with a large body of
work on self-determination theory, suggesting that
autonomous goal pursuit is generally more adaptive
than controlled goal pursuit (Deci & Ryan, 2000). As
for content, with only two exceptions (emotion regula-
tion, interpersonal strivings), thematic content of a per-
son’s strivings was unrelated to daily well-being across
diagnostic groups. Interestingly, however, upon consid-
ering group differences (i.e., SAD diagnostic group as
a moderator), results suggest that strivings concerning
other people (interpersonal, intimacy, affiliation) are
associated with daily subjective well-being for people
with SAD but not healthy controls. Writing down goals
about establishing relationships with people might
facilitate more social behavior that moves people closer
to interpersonal goals and reduces social avoidance,
thus increasing subjective well-being. This might be
particularly beneficial for people with SAD, who tend
to have a dearth of positive social experiences. Alter-
natively, it is equally plausible that participants who
had greater subjective well-being at baseline were more
likely to endorse strivings related to developing inter-
personal relationships. Although the study was prospec-
tive in that participants completed strivings assessment
at baseline and then subsequently provided daily
reports of subjective well-being, findings are insufficient
to establish causality (i.e., greater strivings frequency
causing greater well-being). Nonetheless, given that
these analyses were exploratory, these interpretations
should be considered tentative, and future research,
including replication of study findings, is warranted.
Clinical implications
Findings from the present study can be interpreted in
the context of a growing body of work on acceptance-
and mindfulness-based therapies for SAD (Brady &
Whitman, 2012; Dalrymple & Herbert, 2007; Kocovski,
Fleming, Hawley, Huta, & Antony, 2013; Kocovski,
Fleming, & Rector, 2009; Niles etal., 2014). These treat-
ment approaches use techniques that encourage clients
to take values-driven action. Therapists walk clients
through exercises that help them to pursue important
life aims despite the presence of adversity, discomfort,
or pain. During this process, clients work to identify
and clarify what they value (Wilson & Murrell, 2004).
Results of the current study demonstrate that people
with SAD value life aims that are similar to those of
nonanxious adults, but they encounter greater difficulty
and are less successful in their pursuit. One consider-
ation when encouraging clients to think about life aspi-
rations is that although they might readily identify
strivings that appear adaptive (e.g., low preponderance
of avoidance strivings), results from the present study
suggest that they are likely to experience significant
difficulty pursuing them. Clinicians can strategically work
with clients to identify barriers impeding their goals,
develop strategies to overcome them, and ultimately
facilitate values-driven behavior that fosters meaning.
They are also more likely to encounter interpersonal
difficulties while pursuing them. Clinicians can work
with socially anxious clients to identify ways to harness
existing social resources and/or explore ways to build
their support network, possibly in the context of social
exposure. In addition to clinicians offering resources to
be successful, positive social experiences may even
modify the goals that people with SAD construct to be
more adaptive (Trew & Alden, 2015).
Limitations and future directions
Despite using a clinical sample of community adults
diagnosed with SAD and a healthy comparison group,
our findings should be interpreted in light of study
limitations. Subjective assessments of strivings were col-
lected at a single point in time, which prevents us from
drawing conclusions about causal direction. Future
studies can make use of longitudinal methodologies to
test competing hypotheses. For instance, when people
with SAD perceive a lack of social support for their
strivings, do they exert less effort because they do not
feel supported by close others, or alternatively, do they
exert less effort and, as a result, are less likely to recruit
help from close friends? In terms of strivings content,
although we conducted comprehensive coding of 10
previously validated categories, this list is not exhaus-
tive. There are likely additional categories that relate
to the phenomenology of SAD or anxiety disorders
more broadly (e.g., goal specificity; Rodebaugh, 2007).
Additionally, we conceptualized and measured strivings
as overarching life aims outside of a specific context.
Given the dearth of research on goal pursuit for people
296 Goodman et al.
with SAD, there is much to be gained from exploring
goal pursuit across unique situations (e.g., stressful
social situation; Rodebaugh & Shumaker, 2012). Related
to emotion regulation, although theoretical models
emphasize the role of emotion dysregulation in the
phenomenology of SAD (Jazaieri etal., 2015; Kashdan
etal., 2011), most existing research (including the pres-
ent study) has been conducted with self-report ques-
tionnaires. Self-reported strategy use might insufficiently
predict real-world behavior. In a recent study, partici-
pants with SAD reported strong deficits in emotion
regulation on subjective questionnaires, but evidence
for such deficits was less conclusive in psychophysio-
logical measures (i.e., event-related potentials) follow-
ing lab-based tasks (Kivity & Huppert, 2018). It will be
important for future research to simultaneously com-
pare different measurements of emotion regulation
(especially in light of research suggesting that even trait
versus state self-reports of emotion regulation differen-
tially predict emotional experiences in SAD; Blalock,
Kashdan, & Farmer, 2016).
Future research is needed to examine strivings
among people with other anxiety disorders. On the one
hand, there is considerable heterogeneity among anxi-
ety disorders, and examinations of strivings within dif-
ferent clinical samples (e.g., generalized anxiety
disorder) might yield disparate conclusions. To illus-
trate one of several possible future research questions,
do people with other anxiety disorders construct striv-
ings less driven by controlled motives than people with
SAD? SAD is distinct among anxiety disorders in its
central feature of negative evaluation fears (although it
overlaps with related disorders such as body dysmor-
phic disorder; Fang & Hofmann, 2010). People with
SAD are inclined to behave in ways they believe others
want or expect them to act in order to garner social
approval and avoid rejection. This might mean con-
structing goals out of perceived pressure or guilt, as
results of the present study suggest. Given that social
evaluative fears are not characteristic of other anxiety
disorders (although they may still be present), it is pos-
sible that people with other anxiety disorders display
a different profile of motives. On the other hand, anxi-
ety disorders share common underlying features. For
example, emotion dysregulation has been postulated
to be central to the phenomenology of anxiety disor-
ders (e.g., Amstadter, 2008; Cisler & Olatunji, 2012;
Cisler, Olatunji, Feldner, & Forsyth, 2010). Accordingly,
it is possible that, similar to those with SAD, people
with other anxiety disorders will demonstrate a dispro-
portionate number of emotion regulation strivings rela-
tive to healthy controls. There are many questions that
remain unanswered about anxiety disorders, and per-
sonal strivings offer an idiographic approach to under-
standing their complexity.
Conclusion
Our findings illustrate that the diagnosis of SAD does
not necessarily correspond with overtly maladaptive
goals. The strivings that people with SAD construct are
distinguishable by their explicit attempts to regulate
their emotions but largely resemble the strivings of
healthy adults. Had we examined only goal content,
our conclusions would suggest that goal pursuit is rela-
tively normative for people with SAD. By exploring why
individuals construct these goals and how they fare in
their pursuits, we uncovered significant dysfunction
relative to healthy peers. The findings of this study raise
interesting implications for researchers aiming to under-
stand anxiety disorders at higher levels of personality
beyond behavioral tendencies and for clinicians wish-
ing to help clients pursue their most valued life aims.
Action Editor
Erin B. Tone served as action editor for this article.
Author Contributions
T. B. Kashdan developed the study design and collected study
data. All the authors contributed to qualitative coding and
data cleaning. F. R. Goodman analyzed the data and drafted
the manuscript. All the authors provided critical revisions and
approved the final manuscript for submission.
Declaration of Conflicting Interests
The author(s) declared that there were no conflicts of interest
with respect to the authorship or the publication of this
article.
Funding
The Center for the Advancement of Well-Being funded F. R.
Goodman and M. C. Stiksma as research fellows and T. B.
Kashdan as senior scientist. F. R. Goodman was also sup-
ported by a National Research Service Award (F31) predoc-
toral fellowship.
Notes
1. These analyses were included on the basis of the sugges-
tion of an anonymous reviewer and should be considered
exploratory.
2. The term well-being is often used in psychological science
and the general public to capture a range of dimensions of
positive psychological functioning. In this research, the term
subjective well-being is used to refer to Diener and colleagues’
(2018) definition of “an overall evaluation of the quality of a
person’s life from her or his own perspective” (p. 1). Research
on subjective well-being typically incorporates assessments of
both affective and cognitive components, although there is con-
siderable debate as to what constitutes subjective well-being.
For the affective component, measures of the widely accepted
positive and negative affect were used; for the cognitive com-
ponent, a measure of meaning in life was used.
Anxiety Disorders and Strivings 297
References
Ackerman, S., Zuroff, D. C., & Moskowitz, D. S. (2000).
Generativity in midlife and young adults: Links to agency,
communion, and subjective well-being. International
Journal of Aging & Human Development, 50, 17–41.
Aderka, I. M., Hofmann, S. G., Nickerson, A., Hermesh, H.,
Gilboa-Schechtman, E., & Marom, S. (2012). Functional
impairment in social anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety
Disorders, 26, 393–400.
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-
regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-ana-
lytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237.
Alden, L. E., & Taylor, C. T. (2004). Interpersonal processes
in social phobia. Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 857–882.
Alden, L. E., & Wallace, S. T. (1991). Social standards and social
withdrawal. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15, 85–100.
Alden, L. E., & Wallace, S. T. (1995). Social phobia and social
appraisal in successful and unsuccessful social interac-
tions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 497–505.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and sta-
tistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington,
DC: Author.
Amstadter, A. (2008). Emotion regulation and anxiety disor-
ders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 211–221.
Barrett, L. F. (1998). Discrete emotions or dimensions? The
role of valence focus and arousal focus. Cognition &
Emotion, 12, 579–599.
Blalock, D. V., Kashdan, T. B., & Farmer, A. S. (2016). Trait
and daily emotion regulation in social anxiety disorder.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 40, 416–425.
Borkovec, T. D., Alcaine, O., & Behar, E. (2004). Avoidance
theory of worry and generalized anxiety disorder. In R. G.
Heimberg, C. L. Turk, & D. S. Mennin (Eds.), Generalized
anxiety disorder: Advances in research and practice (pp.
77–108). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Brady, V. P., & Whitman, S. M. (2012). An acceptance and
mindfulness-based approach to social phobia: A case
study. Journal of College Counseling, 15, 81–96.
Brunstein, J. C. (1993). Personal goals and subjective well-
being: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 65, 1061–1070.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions
of positive and negative affect: A control-process view.
Psychological Review, 97, 19–35.
Chawla, N., & Ostafin, B. (2007). Experiential avoidance as
a functional dimensional approach to psychopathology:
An empirical review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63,
871–890.
Cisler, J. M., & Olatunji, B. O. (2012). Emotion regulation
and anxiety disorders. Current Psychiatry Reports, 14,
182–187.
Cisler, J. M., Olatunji, B. O., Feldner, M. T., & Forsyth, J. P.
(2010). Emotion regulation and the anxiety disorders:
An integrative review. Journal of Psychopathology and
Behavioral Assessment, 32, 68–82.
Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social
phobia. In R. G. Heimberg, M. R. Liebowitz, D. A. Hope, &
F. R. Schneier (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment,
and treatment (pp. 69–93). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Coats, E. J., Janoff-Bulman, R., & Alpert, N. (1996). Approach
versus avoidance goals: Differences in self-evaluation and
well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
22, 1057–1067.
Cohen, S., & Syme, S. L. (1985). Issues in the study and
application of social support. In S. Cohen & S. L. Syme
(Eds.), Social support and health (pp. 3–22). New York,
NY: Academic Press.
Cougle, J. R., Fitch, K. E., Fincham, F. D., Riccardi, C. J.,
Keough, M. E., & Timpano, K. R. (2012). Excessive reas-
surance seeking and anxiety pathology: Tests of incre-
mental associations and directionality. Journal of Anxiety
Disorders, 26, 117–125.
Dalrymple, K. L., & Herbert, J. D. (2007). Acceptance and
commitment therapy for generalized social anxiety dis-
order: A pilot study. Behavior Modification, 31, 543–568.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). The General Causality
Orientations Scale: Self-determination in personality.
Journal of Research in Personality, 19, 109–134.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of
goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination
of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268.
DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality:
A meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective
well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 197–229.
Dickson, J. M., & MacLeod, A. K. (2004). Approach and avoid-
ance goals and plans: Their relationship to anxiety and
depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 28, 415–432.
Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1995). Resources, personal strivings,
and subjective well-being: A nomothetic and idiographic
approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
68, 926–935.
Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Oishi, S. (2018). Advances and
open questions in the science of subjective well-being.
Collabra: Psychology, 4(1), 15.
Di Nardo, P. A., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1994). Anxiety
Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM–IV: Lifetime Version
(ADIS-IV-L). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Approach-avoidance
motivation in personality: Approach and avoidance tem-
peraments and goals. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 82, 804–818.
Emmons, R. A. (1986). Personal strivings: An approach to per-
sonality and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 51, 1058–1068.
Emmons, R. A. (1989). The personal striving approach to
personality. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Goal concepts in per-
sonality and social psychology (pp. 87–126). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Emmons, R. A. (1991). Personal strivings, daily life events,
and psychological and physical well-being. Journal of
Personality, 59, 453–472.
Emmons, R. A. (1999). The psychology of ultimate concerns:
Motivation and spirituality in personality. New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Emmons, R. A. (2005). Striving for the sacred: Personal goals,
life meaning, and religion. Journal of Social Issues, 61,
731–745.
Emmons, R. A., & King, L. A. (1988). Conflict among per-
sonal strivings: Immediate and long-term implications
298 Goodman et al.
for psychological and physical well-being. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1040–1048.
Fang, A., & Hofmann, S. G. (2010). Relationship between
social anxiety disorder and body dysmorphic disorder.
Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 1040–1048.
Feldner, M. T., Zvolensky, M. J., & Leen-Feldner, E. W. (2004).
A critical review of the empirical literature on coping and
panic disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 123–148.
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W.
(2002). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV–TR Axis I
Disorders, Research Version, Non-patient Edition (SCID-I/
NP). New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric Institute,
Biometrics Research.
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Finkel, E. J. (2010). Interpersonal
influences on self-regulation. Current Directions in
Psychological Science, 19, 101–105.
Gilbert, P. (2001). Evolution and social anxiety: The role
of attraction, social competition, and social hierarchies.
Psychiatric Clinics, 24, 723–751.
Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Shachar, I., & Sahar, Y. (2014).
Positivity impairment as a broad-based feature of social
anxiety. In J. W. Weeks (Ed.), The Wiley Blackwell hand-
book of social anxiety disorder (pp. 409–432). Hoboken,
NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.
Goodman, F. R., Larrazabal, M., West, J., & Kashdan, T. B. (in
press). Experiential avoidance across anxiety disorders.
In B. O. Olatunji (Ed.), Cambridge handbook of anxiety
and related disorders. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
University Press.
Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation:
An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2,
271–299.
Gross, J. J. (2014). Emotion regulation: Conceptual and
empirical foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of
emotion regulation (2nd ed., pp. 3–20). New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and
future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26, 1–26.
Heerey, E. A., & Kring, A. M. (2007). Interpersonal conse-
quences of social anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
116, 125–134.
Hiemisch, A., Ehlers, A., & Westermann, R. (2002). Mindsets
in social anxiety: A new look at selective information pro-
cessing. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
Psychiatry, 33, 103–114.
Hope, D. A., Gansler, D. A., & Heimberg, R. G. (1989).
Attentional focus and causal attributions in social phobia:
Implications from social psychology. Clinical Psychology
Review, 9, 49–60.
Hopko, D. R., McNeil, D. W., Zvolensky, M. J., & Eifert,
G. H. (2001). The relation between anxiety and skill in
performance-based anxiety disorders: A behavioral for-
mulation of social phobia. Behavior Therapy, 32, 185–207.
Jazaieri, H., Morrison, A. S., Goldin, P. R., & Gross, J. J. (2015).
The role of emotion and emotion regulation in social
anxiety disorder. Current Psychiatry Reports, 17, 531–540.
Johnson, S. L., Carver, C. S., & Fulford, D. (2010). Goal dysreg-
ulation in the affective disorders. In A. M. Kring & D. M.
Sloan (Eds.), Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A
transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment (pp.
204–228). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Kachin, K. E., Newman, M. G., & Pincus, A. L. (2001). An
interpersonal problem approach to the division of social
phobia subtypes. Behavior Therapy, 32, 479–501.
Kashdan, T. B. (2007). Social anxiety spectrum and dimin-
ished positive experiences: Theoretical synthesis and
meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 348–365.
Kashdan, T. B., Barrios, V., Forsyth, J. P., & Steger, M. F.
(2006). Experiential avoidance as a generalized psy-
chological vulnerability: Comparisons with coping and
emotion regulation strategies. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 44, 1301–1320.
Kashdan, T. B., Breen, W. E., & Julian, T. (2010). Everyday
strivings in war veterans with posttraumatic stress dis-
order: Suffering from a hyper-focus on avoidance and
emotion regulation. Behavior Therapy, 41, 350–363.
Kashdan, T. B., & Collins, R. L. (2010). Social anxiety and
the experience of positive emotion and anger in every-
day life: An ecological momentary assessment approach.
Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 23, 259–272.
Kashdan, T. B., Elhai, J. D., & Breen, W. E. (2008). Social
anxiety and disinhibition: An analysis of curiosity and
social rank appraisals, approach–avoidance conflicts,
and disruptive risk-taking behavior. Journal of Anxiety
Disorders, 22, 925–939.
Kashdan, T. B., Farmer, A. S., Adams, L. M., Ferssizidis, P.,
McKnight, P. E., & Nezlek, J. B. (2013). Distinguishing
healthy adults from people with social anxiety disorder:
Evidence for the value of experiential avoidance and
positive emotions in everyday social interactions. Journal
of Abnormal Psychology, 122, 645–655.
Kashdan, T. B., Goodman, F. R., Machell, K. A., Kleiman, E. M.,
Monfort, S. S., Ciarrochi, J., & Nezlek, J. B. (2014). A
contextual approach to experiential avoidance and social
anxiety: Evidence from an experimental interaction and
daily interactions of people with social anxiety disorder.
Emotion, 14, 769–781.
Kashdan, T. B., & Hofmann, S. G. (2008). The high-novelty-
seeking, impulsive subtype of generalized social anxiety
disorder. Depression and Anxiety, 25, 535–541.
Kashdan, T. B., & McKnight, P. E. (2010). The darker side
of social anxiety: When aggressive impulsivity prevails
over shy inhibition. Current Directions in Psychological
Science, 19, 47–50.
Kashdan, T. B., & McKnight, P. E. (2013). Commitment to a
purpose in life: An antidote to the suffering by individu-
als with social anxiety disorder. Emotion, 13, 1150–1159.
Kashdan, T. B., McKnight, P. E., Richey, J. A., & Hofmann, S. G.
(2009). When social anxiety disorder co-exists with risk-prone,
approach behavior: Investigating a neglected, meaningful sub-
set of people in the National Comorbidity Survey-Replication.
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 559–568.
Kashdan, T. B., & Steger, M. F. (2006). Expanding the topog-
raphy of social anxiety: An experience-sampling assess-
ment of positive emotions, positive events, and emotion
suppression. Psychological Science, 17, 120–128.
Kashdan, T. B., & Steger, M. F. (2007). Curiosity and path-
ways to well-being and meaning in life: Traits, states,
Anxiety Disorders and Strivings 299
and everyday behaviors. Motivation and Emotion, 31,
159–173.
Kashdan, T. B., Weeks, J. W., & Savostyanova, A. A. (2011).
Whether, how, and when social anxiety shapes positive
experiences and events: A self-regulatory framework and
treatment implications. Clinical Psychology Review, 31,
786–799.
Kivity, Y., & Huppert, J. D. (2018). Are individuals diag-
nosed with social anxiety disorder successful in regulating
their emotions? A mixed-method investigation using self-
report, subjective, and event-related potentials measures.
Journal of Affective Disorders, 236, 298–305.
Kocovski, N. L., Fleming, J. E., Hawley, L. L., Huta, V., &
Antony, M. M. (2013). Mindfulness and acceptance-based
group therapy versus traditional cognitive behavioral
group therapy for social anxiety disorder: A random-
ized controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
51, 889–898.
Kocovski, N. L., Fleming, J. E., & Rector, N. A. (2009).
Mindfulness and acceptance-based group therapy for
social anxiety disorder: An open trial. Cognitive and
Behavioral Practice, 16, 276–289.
Koestner, R., Lekes, N., Powers, T. A., & Chicoine, E. (2002).
Attaining personal goals: Self-concordance plus imple-
mentation intentions equals success. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 83, 231–244.
Krans, J., de Bree, J., & Bryant, R. A. (2014). Autobiographical
memory bias in social anxiety. Memory, 22, 890–897.
Krans, J., Peeters, M., Näring, G., Brown, A. D., de Bree, J.,
& Van Minnen, A. (2017). Examining temporal altera-
tions in social anxiety disorder and posttraumatic stress
disorder: The relation between autobiographical memory,
future goals, and current self-views. Journal of Anxiety
Disorders, 52, 34–42.
Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of
observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33,
159–174.
Laurenceau, J. P., Barrett, L. F., & Pietromonaco, P. R. (1998).
Intimacy as an interpersonal process: The importance of
self-disclosure, partner disclosure, and perceived partner
responsiveness in interpersonal exchanges. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1238–1251.
Machell, K. A., Goodman, F. R., & Kashdan, T. B. (2015).
Experiential avoidance and well-being: A daily diary
analysis. Cognition & Emotion, 29, 351–359.
McAdams, D. P. (1985). Power, intimacy, and the life story.
Homewood, IL: Dorsey.
McAdams, D. P. (1996). Personality, modernity, and the sto-
ried self: A contemporary framework for studying per-
sons. Psychological Inquiry, 7, 295–321.
McKnight, P. E., Monfort, S. S., Kashdan, T. B., Blalock, D. V.,
& Calton, J. M. (2016). Anxiety symptoms and func-
tional impairment: A systematic review of the correlation
between the two measures. Clinical Psychology Review,
45, 115–130.
Meleshko, K. G., & Alden, L. E. (1993). Anxiety and self-
disclosure: Toward a motivational model. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 1000–1009.
Mennin, D. S., Heimberg, R. G., Turk, C. L., & Fresco, D. M.
(2005). Preliminary evidence for an emotion dysregula-
tion model of generalized anxiety disorder. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 43, 1281–1310.
Moscovitch, D. A. (2009). What is the core fear in social
phobia? A new model to facilitate individualized case con-
ceptualization and treatment. Cognitive and Behavioral
Practice, 16, 123–134.
Moscovitch, D. A., Gavric, D. L., Merrifield, C., Bielak, T., &
Moscovitch, M. (2011). Retrieval properties of negative vs.
positive mental images and autobiographical memories in
social anxiety: Outcomes with a new measure. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 49, 505–517.
Nezlek, J. B. (2012). Diary methods for social and personal-
ity psychology. In J. B. Nezlek (Ed.), The SAGE library in
social and personality psychology methods (pp. 29–32).
London, UK: SAGE.
Niles, A. N., Burklund, L. J., Arch, J. J., Lieberman, M. D.,
Saxbe, D., & Craske, M. G. (2014). Cognitive mediators of
treatment for social anxiety disorder: Comparing accep-
tance and commitment therapy and cognitive-behavioral
therapy. Behavior Therapy, 45, 664–677.
Olatunji, B. O., & Wolitzky-Taylor, K. B. (2009). Anxiety sen-
sitivity and the anxiety disorders: A meta-analytic review
and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 974–999.
Panayiotou, G., Karekla, M., & Mete, I. (2014). Dispositional
coping in individuals with anxiety disorder symptomatol-
ogy: Avoidance predicts distress. Journal of Contextual
Behavioral Science, 3, 314–321.
Papsdorf, M., & Alden, L. (1998). Mediators of social rejec-
tion in social anxiety: Similarity, self-disclosure, and overt
signs of anxiety. Journal of Research in Personality, 32,
351–369.
Plasencia, M. L., Alden, L. E., & Taylor, C. T. (2011). Differential
effects of safety behaviour subtypes in social anxiety dis-
order. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 665–675.
Ratelle, C. F., Guay, F., Vallerand, R. J., Larose, S., & Senécal, C.
(2007). Autonomous, controlled, and amotivated types of
academic motivation: A person-oriented analysis. Journal
of Educational Psychology, 99, 734–746.
Reis, H. T., & Gable, S. L. (2000). Event-sampling and other
methods for studying everyday experience. In H. T. Reis
& C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in
social and personality psychology (pp. 190–222). New
York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Rodebaugh, T. L. (2007). The effects of different types of
goal pursuit on experience and performance during a
stressful social task. Behaviour Research and Therapy,
45, 951–963.
Rodebaugh, T. L. (2009). Social phobia and perceived friend-
ship quality. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 872–878.
Rodebaugh, T. L., & Shumaker, E. A. (2012). Avoidance goals
for a specific social situation influence activated negative
and positive affect. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36,
36–46.
Roemer, L., & Orsillo, S. M. (2002). Expanding our con-
ceptualization of and treatment for generalized anxi-
ety disorder: Integrating mindfulness/acceptance-based
300 Goodman et al.
approaches with existing cognitive-behavioral models.
Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 9, 54–68.
Romero, E., Villar, P., Luengo, M. Á., & Gómez-Fraguela, J. A.
(2009). Traits, personal strivings and well-being. Journal
of Research in Personality, 43, 535–546.
Rothermund, K., & Brandstädter, J. (2003). Coping with defi-
cits and losses in later life: From compensatory action
to accommodation. Psychology and Aging, 18, 896–905.
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of cau-
sality and internalization: Examining reasons for act-
ing in two domains. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 57, 749–761.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory
and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social develop-
ment, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
Schlenker, B. R., & Leary, M. R. (1982). Social anxiety and self-
presentation: A conceptualization model. Psychological
Bulletin, 92, 641–669.
Schneier, F. R., Heckelman, L. R., Garfinkel, R., Campeas,
R., Fallon, B. A., Gitow, A., . . . Liebowitz, M. R. (1994).
Functional impairment in social phobia. The Journal of
Clinical Psychiatry, 55, 322–331.
Shahar, G., Henrich, C. C., Blatt, S. J., Ryan, R., & Little, T. D.
(2003). Interpersonal relatedness, self-definition, and their
motivational orientation during adolescence: A theoreti-
cal and empirical integration. Developmental Psychology,
39, 470–483.
Sheldon, K. M., & Houser-Marko, L. (2001). Self-concordance,
goal attainment, and the pursuit of happiness: Can there
be an upward spiral? Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 80, 152–165.
Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1995). Coherence and congru-
ence: Two aspects of personality integration. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 531–543.
Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (1998). Pursuing personal goals:
Skills enable progress, but not all progress is beneficial.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1319–1331.
Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (2001). Getting older, getting bet-
ter? Personal strivings and psychological maturity across
the life span. Developmental Psychology, 37, 491–501.
Shteynberg, G., & Galinsky, A. D. (2011). Implicit coordina-
tion: Sharing goals with similar others intensifies goal
pursuit. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47,
1291–1294.
Smith, A., Ntoumanis, N., & Duda, J. (2007). Goal striving,
goal attainment, and well-being: Adapting and testing the
self-concordance model in sport. Journal of Sport and
Exercise Psychology, 29, 763–782.
Sparrevohn, R. M., & Rapee, R. M. (2009). Self-disclosure,
emotional expression and intimacy within romantic
relationships of people with social phobia. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 47, 1074–1078.
Spurr, J. M., & Stopa, L. (2002). Self-focused attention in social
phobia and social anxiety. Clinical Psychology Review,
22, 947–975.
Steel, P., Schmidt, J., & Shultz, J. (2008). Refining the rela-
tionship between personality and subjective well-being.
Psychological Bulletin, 134, 138–161.
Steger, M. F., Kashdan, T. B., & Oishi, S. (2008). Being good
by doing good: Daily eudaimonic activity and well-being.
Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 22–42.
Strauman, T. J. (2017). Self-regulation and psychopathology:
Toward an integrative translational research paradigm.
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 13, 497–523.
Taylor, C. T., & Alden, L. E. (2011). To see ourselves as others
see us: An experimental integration of the intra and inter-
personal consequences of self-protection in social anxiety
disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120, 129–141.
Taylor, C. T., & Amir, N. (2012). Modifying automatic approach
action tendencies in individuals with elevated social
anxiety symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50,
529–536.
Trew, J. L., & Alden, L. E. (2012). Positive affect predicts
avoidance goals in social interaction anxiety: Testing a
hierarchical model of social goals. Cognitive Behaviour
Therapy, 41, 174–183.
Trew, J. L., & Alden, L. E. (2015). Kindness reduces avoid-
ance goals in socially anxious individuals. Motivation and
Emotion, 39, 892–907.
Trower, P., & Gilbert, P. (1989). New theoretical conceptions
of social anxiety and social phobia. Clinical Psychology
Review, 9, 19–35.
Turk, C. L., Heimberg, R. G., Luterek, J. A., Mennin, D. S., &
Fresco, D. M. (2005). Emotion dysregulation in general-
ized anxiety disorder: A comparison with social anxiety
disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29, 89–106.
Vergara, C., & Roberts, J. E. (2011). Motivation and goal
orientation in vulnerability to depression. Cognition &
Emotion, 25, 1281–1290.
Voncken, M. J., & Dijk, K. F. L. (2013). Socially anxious indi-
viduals get a second chance after being disliked at first
sight: The role of self-disclosure in the development of
likeability in sequential social contact. Cognitive Therapy
and Research, 37, 7–17.
Wallace, S. T., & Alden, L. E. (1991). A comparison of social
standards and perceived ability in anxious and nonanx-
ious men. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15, 237–254.
Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development
and validation of brief measures of positive and nega-
tive affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.
Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The
two general activation systems of affect: Structural find-
ings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiological
evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
76, 820–838.
Weeks, J. W., Heimberg, R. G., & Rodebaugh, T. L. (2008).
The Fear of Positive Evaluation Scale: Assessing a pro-
posed cognitive component of social anxiety. Journal of
Anxiety Disorders, 22, 44–55.
Weeks, J. W., Menatti, A. R., & Howell, A. N. (2015).
Psychometric evaluation of the Concerns of Social Reprisal
Scale: Further explicating the roots of fear of positive
evaluation. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 36, 33–43.
Weisman, O., Aderka, I. M., Marom, S., Hermesh, H., &
Gilboa-Schechtman, E. (2011). Social rank and affiliation
Anxiety Disorders and Strivings 301
in social anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and
Therapy, 49, 399–405.
Weiss, H. M., Beal, D. J., Lucy, S. L., & MacDermid, S. M.
(2004). Constructing EMA studies with PMAT: The Pur-
due Momentary Assessment Tool user’s manual. West
Lafayette, IN: Military Research Institute at Purdue Uni-
versity.
Werner, K. H., Goldin, P. R., Ball, T. M., Heimberg, R. G.,
& Gross, J. J. (2011). Assessing emotion regulation in
social anxiety disorder: The emotion regulation interview.
Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment,
33, 346–354.
Wilson, K. G., & Murrell, A. R. (2004). Values work in accep-
tance and commitment therapy: Setting a course for
behavioral treatment. In S. C. Hayes, V. M. Follette, & M.
Linehan (Eds.), Mindfulness and acceptance: Expanding
the cognitive-behavioral tradition (pp. 120–151). New
York, NY: Guilford Press.
... In a recent study, individuals with SAD were found to have an elevated desire to control, manage or get rid of their emotions compared to individuals without SAD (Goodman et al., 2019). Consistently, individuals with SAD endorsed more explicit emotion control values (i.e., that emotions needed to be controlled) and less emotion malleability beliefs (i.e., the belief that emotions can be modified) compared to individuals without SAD (Goodman, Kashdan et al., 2021). ...
Article
Extant cognitive behavioral models of social anxiety disorder (SAD) have primarily focused on cognitions and behaviors that maintain the disorder. Emotional aspects of SAD have been investigated but have not been sufficiently integrated into current models. To facilitate such integration, we reviewed the literature on emotional constructs (emotional intelligence, emotional knowledge, emotional clarity, emotion differentiation, and emotion regulation), and discrete emotions (anger, shame, embarrassment, loneliness, guilt, pride, and envy) in SAD and social anxiety. We present the studies conducted on these constructs, summarize the main findings, suggest areas for future research, discuss the findings in the context of existing models of SAD and attempt to integrate the findings into these existing models of the disorder. Clinical implications of our findings are also discussed.
... positive reinforcement framework Alcohol use might also be maintained by positive reinforcement via affiliation motives and social rewards. People with SAD desire strong, intimate relationships to the same degree as those without SAD (Goodman et al., 2019) and often enjoy socializing (Goodman et al., 2021). However, people with SAD also tend to have worse quality relationships and less desirable social skills than less anxious peers (see Alden & Taylor, 2010). ...
Article
Full-text available
People with social anxiety disorder (SAD) are at increased risk for alcohol-related problems. Most research exploring social anxiety and alcohol use has examined negative drinking consequences, with less consideration of positive consequences—namely positive social experiences—that may reinforce alcohol use. In this daily diary study, we examined how adults diagnosed with SAD (N = 26) and a psychologically healthy control group (N = 28) experienced positive drinking consequences in naturally occurring drinking episodes during the study period. For 14 consecutive days, participants answered questions about alcohol use, motives for drinking, and positive consequences of drinking. On days when participants drank, those with SAD were more likely than healthy controls to perceive a reduction in anxiety, but the two groups did not differ in their likelihood of experiencing positive social drinking consequences. For both groups, on days when they were more motivated to drink to enhance social experiences (affiliation motives) or cope with distress (coping motives), they were more likely to obtain positive consequences from drinking. Compared to controls, participants with SAD endorsed stronger trait and daily coping motives (anxiety-coping, social anxiety-coping, and depression-coping). Results are discussed in the context of reinforcement mechanisms that may maintain social anxiety and alcohol use.
... Although people with SAD have considerable social evaluation concerns, this does not preclude them from the opportunity to derive benefits when socializing. People with SAD strive to develop and maintain intimate interpersonal relationships to the same degree as healthy adults (Goodman et al., 2019). Social situations may be both anxiety-provoking and rewarding. ...
Article
Full-text available
Judgments about the self compared to internalized standards are central to theoretical frameworks of social anxiety. Yet, empirical research on social comparisons-how people view themselves relative to others-and social anxiety is sparse. This research program examines the nature of everyday social comparisons in the context of social anxiety across 2 experience-sampling studies containing 8,396 unique entries from 273 adults. Hypotheses and analyses were preregistered with the Open Science Foundation (OSF) prior to data analysis. Study 1 was a 3-week daily diary study with undergraduates, and Study 2 was a 2-week ecological momentary assessment (EMA) study with a clinical sample of adults diagnosed with social anxiety disorder (SAD) and a psychologically healthy comparison group. In both studies, social anxiety was associated with less favorable, more unstable social comparisons. In both studies, favorable social comparisons were associated with higher positive affect and lower negative affect and social anxiety. In both studies, social comparisons and momentary affect/social anxiety were more strongly linked in people with elevated trait social anxiety/SAD compared to less socially anxious participants. Participants in Study 2-even those with SAD-made more favorable social comparisons when they were with other people than when alone. Taken together, results suggest that social anxiety is associated with unfavorable, unstable self-views that are linked to compromised well-being. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
... Psychologists have suggested that social interactions facilitate positive emotions because they fulfill a fundamental need to develop close connections with other people (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). There is no evidence that people with SAD are devoid of this desire; to the contrary, people with SAD desire strong, intimate social relationships to the same degree as healthy adults (Goodman, Kashdan, Stiksma, & Blalock, 2019). People with elevated social anxiety may even have a stronger need for belongingness than those with lower social anxiety (e.g., Lavigne, Vallerand, & Crevier-Braud, 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
Quality contact with other people serves as a reliable mood enhancement strategy. We wondered if the emotional benefits of socializing are present even for those with a psychological disorder defined by social distress and avoidance: social anxiety disorder (SAD). We conducted two ecological momentary assessment (EMA) studies and analyzed 7,243 total surveys. In both studies, community adults diagnosed with SAD and healthy controls received five surveys each day for two weeks. Consistent with research on positivity deficits in SAD, between-person analyses in both studies suggest that, on average, participants with SAD reported lower positive and higher negative affect in social and non-social situations than healthy controls. Within-person analyses, however, revealed that in both studies participants with SAD and healthy controls reported higher positive affect when with others than when alone; no differences were found for negative affect. The difference in positive affect between social and nonsocial situations was smaller for participants with SAD in Study 1, suggesting that people with SAD may experience diminished reward responding when socializing. Our results suggest that even those with a mental illness defined by interpersonal distress can and do derive positive emotions from social interactions.
... 随着积极心理学的逐渐兴起,心理学的研究内容就一改过去对焦虑、抑郁等消极情绪状态的过分研 究,主观幸福感作为其研究领域的主题之一越来越被人们所重视。心理学在研究消极方面的文章数量是 积极方面文章数量的 17 倍 (Robitschek & Spering, 2012;Myers & Diener, 1995)。而对大学生的研究大多集 中于一些消极的心理状态,那么研究他们的积极状态就显得更为重要。主观幸福感是个体根据选择的标 准来总体评价生活水平的一种指标 (Ludwigs et al., 2018)。而以往的研究大多集中在人格特质与主观幸福 感的关系,较少研究人格的非特质取向即人格的动机取向。个人奋斗作为动机研究的新取向,是个体当 前正努力去做的事情,而个人奋斗的各因素比人格特质能解释主观幸福感中更多的变异 (Goodman et al., 2019;Emmons, 1991)。主观幸福感的一个前提条件是要有一个明确的奋斗目标并朝着这个方向努力前行 (Chang et al., 2019;Emmons, 1986),即个人奋斗会对主观幸福感起到影响 (Heckhausen et al., 2013)。此外, 自我效能感作为一种认知因素,是指个体确信自己有能力完成某件事进行的评估 (Bandura, 1977)。研究表 明,自我效能感能够影响主观幸福感,且与它存在显著的正相关 (Li et al., 2020;Zheng et al., 2016 ...
... SAD is thought to be not really associated with deficits in social motivation, which can be derived from the fact that socially anxious people seek treatment because they feel unhappy about their relationships with other people. Although direct evidence for the social motivation tendencies of individuals with SAD is sparse, there is an interesting recent study by Goodman et al. (2019) who assessed the personal strivings in 41 adult individuals with this anxiety disorder and 43 healthy controls. Among the list of personal strivings types, four were of a social nature, namely 'affiliation' (i.e., concern for or desire to establish, maintain, or repair friendships), 'interpersonal' (i.e., an objective or goal focused on others), 'intimacy' (i.e., commitment and concern for others, quality of relationships rather than quantity), and 'self-presentation' (i.e., making a favorable impression on others). ...
Article
Full-text available
In current classification systems, selective mutism (SM) is included in the broad anxiety disorders category. Indeed, there is abundant evidence showing that anxiety, and social anxiety in particular, is a prominent feature of SM. In this article, we point out that autism spectrum problems in addition to anxiety problems are sometimes also implicated in SM. To build our case, we summarize evidence showing that SM, social anxiety disorder (SAD), and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are allied clinical conditions and share communalities in the realm of social difficulties. Following this, we address the role of a prototypical class of ASD symptoms, restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests (RRBIs), which are hypothesized to play a special role in the preservation and exacerbation of social difficulties. We then substantiate our point that SM is sometimes more than an anxiety disorder by addressing its special link with ASD in more detail. Finally, we close by noting that the possible involvement of ASD in SM has a number of consequences for clinical practice with regard to its classification, assessment, and treatment of children with SM and highlight a number of directions for future research.
... Theory suggests people high on emotion regulation flexibility are well-attuned to social cues in choosing regulatory strategies (e.g., Bonanno & Burton, 2013). In contrast, people with high social anxiety, who are often socially impaired (e.g., Kashdan & Wenzel, 2005;Rodebaugh et al., 2014), show signs of inflexible emotion regulation-over-relying and placing considerable 6 of 11 -DOORLEY ET AL. value on controlling, avoidance, and concealing their emotions regardless of situational cues (Daniel et al., in press;Dryman & Heimberg, 2018;Goodman, Kashdan, & İmamoğlu, in press;Goodman, Kashdan, Stiksma, & Blalock, 2019;O'Toole, Zachariae, & Mennin, 2017). There is reason to believe psychological flexibility and social functioning covary, but causal links are unclear. ...
Article
Full-text available
Psychological flexibility is the tendency to respond to situations in ways that facilitate valued goal pursuit. Psychological flexibility is particularly useful when challenges arise during goal pursuit that produce distress. In acceptance and commitment therapy, psychological flexibility is considered the pinnacle of emotional health and well-being. A growing body of research demonstrates that psychological flexibility leads to psychological benefits and adaptive behavior change. Yet, much of what we know, or think we know, about psychological flexibility hinges on a single measurement approach using the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire (AAQ and AAQ-II). Research suggests the AAQ-II is highly correlated with distress itself rather than flexible responses to distress. Existing approaches that assess psychological flexibility ignore the context in which flexibility matters most: the pursuit of valued goals. Below, we review theory and research on psychological flexibility, including its associations with healthy functioning, its measurement, and its overlap with related constructs. We discuss how gaps between theory and measurement impede our understanding and review promising evidence for a new measure of psychological flexibility. We provide new research directions in an effort to create a more generalizable foundation of knowledge. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2020;e12566. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/spc3
... Eighty-four adults from northern Virginia and the greater Washington, DC metropolitan area were recruited to participate in the study. These data are drawn from a larger study on daily processes in social anxiety disorder (Blalock et al., 2016;Blalock, Kashdan, & Mc-Knight, 2018;, 2015Goodman, Kashdan, Stiksma, & Blalock, 2019;. Power analyses were conducted to determine target sample size. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study examines relationships between emotion beliefs and emotion regulation strategy use among people with social anxiety disorder (SAD) and a psychologically healthy control group. Using experience-sampling methodology, we tested group differences in 2 types of emotion beliefs (emotion control values and emotion malleability beliefs) and whether emotion beliefs predicted trait and daily use of cognitive reappraisal and emotion suppression. People with SAD endorsed higher emotion control values and lower emotion malleability beliefs than did healthy controls. Across groups, emotion control values were positively associated with suppression (but unrelated to reappraisal), and emotion malleability beliefs were negatively associated with suppression and positively associated with reappraisal. We also addressed 2 exploratory questions related to measurement. First, we examined whether trait and state measures of emotion regulation strategies were related to emotion control values in different ways and found similar associations across measures. Second, we examined whether explicit and implicit measures of emotion control values were related to daily emotion regulation strategy use in different ways-and found that an implicit measure was unrelated to strategy use. Results are discussed in the context of growing research on metaemotions and the measurement of complex features of emotion regulation. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
Social anxiety (SA) and depression are marked by enhanced avoidance motivations (apprehensions) and reduced approach motivations (aspirations). Integrating an approach/avoidance motivational model with the evolutionary-inspired motivational perspective, we examined the associations of SA and depression with apprehensions and aspirations in the domains of social status. In two cross-sectional samples of young adults (N1 = 277; N2 = 256), we found that, whereas apprehensions concerning the loss of social status contributed to both SA and depression, aspirations and apprehensions concerning social status ascendance uniquely contributed only to SA. Additionally, the effects of social-status avoidance motivations on SA were partly accounted for by social-status aspirations: enhanced apprehensions were associated with reduced aspirations, which were associated with more severe SA. Finally, partial support for gender-specific links between social status motivations and SA was obtained. Our results highlight the potential of integrating the motivational frameworks of approach/avoidance and affiliation/social status to understand shared and specific components of SA and depression.
Article
Full-text available
Background Emotion regulation flexibility is a person's tendency to shift their use of emotion regulation strategies in response to contextual demands. A lack of flexibility is thought to underlie affective disorders, yet conceptualizations of “flexibility” vary widely, and few studies have empirically assessed flexibility. In this study, we outline methods for measuring emotion regulation flexibility and then examine evidence for inflexibility in people with a common affective disorder: social anxiety disorder (SAD). Methods Participants were community adults diagnosed with SAD and a psychologically healthycontrol group who completed a 14-day experience-sampling study. Participants recorded their most anxiety-provoking event each day, how they evaluated contextual demands (i.e., perceived controllability, emotional intensity) of these events, and their use of seven emotion regulation strategies to manage anxiety. Hypotheses and analyses were preregistered with the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/s7kqj/). Results Participants with SAD demonstrated some evidence of inflexibility. They used three disengagement strategies (rumination, thought suppression, expressive suppression) more often than controls and did so independently of contextual demands (specifically, perceived controllability). Nonetheless, participants with SAD largely demonstrated similar regulatory patterns as controls, most notably in their use of engagement strategies (acceptance, cognitive, reappraisal, problem-solving). Limitations We measured two of many possible contextual demands, did not compare to a mixed clinical group or other affective disorders (e.g., depression), and did not assess temporal sequences of strategy use. Conclusions People with SAD demonstrate some inflexibility in their use of disengagement regulation strategies.
Article
Full-text available
Subjective well-being (SWB) is an extremely active area of research with about 170,000 articles and books published on the topic in the past 15 years. Methodological and theoretical advances have been notable in this period of time, with the increasing use of longitudinal and experimental designs allowing for a greater understanding of the predictors and outcomes that relate to SWB, along with the process that underlie these associations. In addition, theories about these processes have become more intricate, as findings reveal that many associations with SWB depend on people’s culture and values and the context in which they live. This review provides an overview of many major areas of research, including the measurement of SWB, the demographic and personality-based predictors of SWB, and process-oriented accounts of individual differences in SWB. In addition, because a major new focus in recent years has been the development of national accounts of subjective well-being, we also review attempts to use SWB measures to guide policy decisions.
Article
Full-text available
Background Models of social anxiety emphasize the role of emotion dysregulation, but the nature of these impairments needs clarification. Methods We utilized a mixed-method approach to examine impairments in cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression in social anxiety disorder. Forty nine treatment-seeking individuals diagnosed with social anxiety disorder and 35 healthy controls completed self-reports and a lab-based task of suppression and reappraisal. Unpleasantness ratings and event-related potentials (ERPs) were collected while participants regulated their emotions in response to shame-arousing pictures. ERP analyses focused on the late positive potential, a measure of increased attention to emotional stimuli that is reduced during emotion regulation. Results Participants with social anxiety reported less frequent and effective use of reappraisal and more frequent and effective use of suppression than controls. Counter to most models and our hypotheses, participants with social anxiety were more successful than controls in lab-based reappraisal as measured by unpleasantness ratings, but no differences emerged for ERPs. No differences were found in measures of lab-based suppression. Limitations Use of standardized, and not participant-generated, materials in the lab-based task of emotion regulation may limit the generalizability of the findings. Conclusions Subjective appraisals of self-efficacy and frequency suggest strong impairments in emotion-regulation in social anxiety that are not revealed in the laboratory. Models and treatment protocols should specify the exact nature of emotion dyregulation in social anxiety, highlighting difficulties in implementation of potentially intact emotion regulation abilities.
Article
Full-text available
The self is a multi-faceted and temporally dynamic construct reflecting representations and beliefs about identity in the past, present, and future. Clinical studies have shown that individuals with Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) and Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD) exhibit alterations in self-related processing but these studies have focused primarily on memory. Few studies in PTSD and SAD have examined self-related processing for the present and future, and no studies have directly compared these processes across these two disorders. Individuals diagnosed with PTSD (n = 21), SAD (n = 21), and healthy controls (n = 21) completed cognitive tasks related to the past, present, and future. Disorder congruent temporal alterations were found across both disorders. Further, regression analyses revealed that trauma-related memories were significantly predicted by future goals related to the trauma, whereas social anxiety-related recall was predicted by current socially anxious self-views. Thus, although self-related processing may be common in PTSD and SAD, those aspects of the self most strongly associated with disorder-congruent recall differ by disorder. Self-alterations may be modifiable and developing a better understanding of past, present, and future self-processing might aid in the development of interventions that target these process.
Article
Full-text available
This article presents a general framework in which different manifestations of psychopathology can be conceptualized as dysfunctions in one or more mechanisms of self-regulation, defined as the ongoing process of managing personal goal pursuit in the face of internal, interpersonal, and environmental forces that would derail it. The framework is based on the assertion that self-regulation is a critical locus for the proximal influence on motivation, cognition, emotion, and behavior of more distal factors such as genetics, temperament, socialization history, and neurophysiology. Psychological theories of self-regulation are ideal platforms from which to integrate the study of self-regulation both within and across traditional disciplines. This article has two related goals: to elucidate how the construct of self-regulation provides a unique conceptual platform for the study of psychopathology and to illustrate that platform by presenting our research on depression as an example. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Clinical Psychology Volume 13 is May 7, 2017. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/page/journal/pubdates for revised estimates.
Article
Full-text available
Emotion regulation strategies vary widely in use and effectiveness across psychological diagnostic categories. However, little data exists on (1) the use of these strategies in social anxiety disorder (SAD), and (2) how trait measures compare with actual daily use of emotion regulation strategies. We collected trait and daily assessments of emotion suppression, cognitive reappraisal, and positive and negative emotions from 40 adults with SAD and 39 matched healthy controls. Participants with SAD reported greater trait suppression and less cognitive reappraisal than healthy controls, and exhibited this same pattern of emotion regulation in daily life. Participants overall reported worse emotional experiences when suppressing positive (vs. negative) emotions, and better emotional experiences when reappraising to feel more positive (vs. less negative) emotions. However, SAD participants exhibited greater benefits (specifically increased positive emotions) from reappraising to feel less negative than healthy controls. These findings highlight the importance of positive emotion regulation strategies, particularly for individuals with SAD.
Article
Two studies used the self-concordance model of healthy goal striving (K. M. Sheldon & A. J. Elliot, 1999) to examine the motivational processes by which people can increase their level of well-being during a period of time and then maintain the gain or perhaps increase it even further during the next period of time. In Study I, entering freshmen with self-concordant motivation better attained their 1st-semester goals, which in turn predicted increased adjustment and greater self-concordance for the next semester's goals. Increased self-concordance in turn predicted even better goal attainment during the 2nd semester, which led to further increases in adjustment and to higher levels of ego development by the end of the year. Study 2 replicated the basic model in a 2-week study of short-term goals set in the laboratory. Limits of the model and implications for the question of how (and whether) happiness may be increased are discussed.
Article
The present article includes separate meta-analyses showing that self-concordance and implementation intentions are significantly positively associated with goal progress. Study 1 confirmed the positive relations of both self-concordance and implementation intentions to weekend goal progress. Study 2 confirmed the positive relation of self-concordance with monthly progress on New Year's resolutions but failed to find a direct benefit for implementation intentions. Both studies, however, obtained a significant interaction effect indicating that goal self-concordance and implementation intentions combined synergistically to facilitate goal progress. The article also reports a meta-analysis and results from the 2 studies that demonstrated that goal progress was associated with improved affect over time.
Article
Researchers and clinicians assume a strong, positive correlation between anxiety symptoms and functional impairment. That assumption may be well-justified since diagnostic criteria typically include functional impairment. Still, the relationship remains largely unavailable in any systematic review. Our aim with this paper was to provide empirical evidence for this assumed relationship and to document the observed correlations between anxiety symptom measures and functional impairment measures. Correlations existed for symptoms of six anxiety disorders (Panic Disorder, Agoraphobia, Social Anxiety Disorder, Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder, Generalized Anxiety Disorder, Obsessive Compulsive Disorder) across four functional domains (global, social, occupational, and physical). Overall, the mean of 497 correlations across all disorders and functional domains was modest (r=.34); since the variability between disorders and functional domains tended to be rather large, we explored these correlations further. We presented these results and the potential explanations for unexpected findings along with the clinical and research implications.