ArticlePDF Available

When your resume is (not) turning you down: Modelling ethnic bias in resume screening

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Resume screening is the first hurdle applicants typically face when they apply for a job. Despite the many empirical studies showing bias at the resume‐screening stage, fairness at this funnelling stage has not been reviewed systematically. In this paper, a three‐stage model of biased resume screening is presented. We first discuss relevant theoretical perspectives (e.g., job market signalling and impression formation theories) to explain why resume screening is vulnerable to biased decision‐making and ethnic discrimination in particular. On the basis of the best available evidence, we consider contingencies of ethnic discrimination in the applicant, the decision‐maker, and the broader context (e.g., organisation), as well as the effectiveness of interventions that might counter ethnic bias in resume screening. The paper ends with a critical agenda for further research and practice.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
REVIEW PAPER
When your resume is (not) turning you down:
Modelling ethnic bias in resume screening
Eva Derous
1
|Ann Marie Ryan
2
1
Department of Personnel Management,
Work and Organizational Psychology, Ghent
University, Ghent, Belgium
2
Michigan State University, East Lansing,
Michigan, USA
Correspondence
Eva Derous, Department of Personnel
Management, Work and Organizational
Psychology, Ghent University, Henri
Dunantlaan 2, Ghent 9000, Belgium.
Email: eva.derous@ugent.be
Abstract
Resume screening is the first hurdle applicants typically face
when they apply for a job. Despite the many empirical
studies showing bias at the resumescreening stage, fairness
at this funnelling stage has not been reviewed systematically.
In this paper, a threestage model of biased resume screening
is presented. We first discuss relevant theoretical perspec-
tives (e.g., job market signalling and impression formation
theories) to explain why resume screening is vulnerable to
biased decisionmaking and ethnic discrimination in particu-
lar. On the basis of the best available evidence, we consider
contingencies of ethnic discrimination in the applicant, the
decisionmaker, and the broader context (e.g., organisation),
as well as the effectiveness of interventions that might
counter ethnic bias in resume screening. The paper ends with
a critical agenda for further research and practice.
KEYWORDS
discrimination, diversity, ethnicity, recruitment, resume screening
1|INTRODUCTION
Despite decades of legislation and HR professionals' commitment to equal opportunities, ethnic minority members
still suffer a weaker labour market position compared with equally qualified majorities (Shen, Chanda, D'Netto, &
Monga, 2009). Human capital factors may explain some of the differences in hiring outcomes, but discrimination
has also been put forward as a possible explanation (Derous & Ryan, 2018; Hoque & Noon, 1999). Ethnic minorities,
for instance, still need to complete 50% more applications to get invited for a job interview when compared with
equally qualified ethnic majorities (Zschirnt & Ruedin, 2016).
The review paper is based on a keynote held by the first author at the 18th Conference of the European Association for Work and
Organizational Psychology, May 18, 2017, Dublin, Ireland.
Received: 9 December 2017 Revised: 19 September 2018 Accepted: 21 September 2018
DOI: 10.1111/1748-8583.12217
Hum Resour Manag J. 2019;29:113130. © 2018 John Wiley & Sons Ltdwileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/hrmj 113
Resume screening, the first hurdle applicants typically face seems particularly vulnerable to hiring discrimination.
Fairness in resume screening, however, is less well investigated when compared with the number of studies on the
adverse impact of personnel selection tests (Outtz, 2010). This is remarkable for several reasons. First, resumes are
worldwide one of the most frequently used screening tools. Over 98% of NorthAmerican companies use resume
screening as the first selection hurdle (Piotrowski & Armstrong, 2006). Second, the quantity and quality of the supply
of applicants sets limit on what subsequent HRM practices can achieve (Newman & Lyon, 2009; Thorndike, 1949).
Finally, the influential resourcebased view of the firm (Barney, 2001) spawned several decades of research to illustrate
that HRM practices are important sources of sustainable competitive advantage.Although the HRM literature has inves-
tigated recruitment within this framework, the lack of focus on this critical entry gateof resume screening is surprising.
This review focuses on resume screening. Resumes
1
are applicantgenerated, annotated career summaries of job
qualifications. HR professionals are expected to screen resumes in an objective and fair way based on applicants' job
relevant characteristics (like work experiences or educational credentials). Resume screening, however, might be
unfair when resulting in differential treatment discrimination or differential effect discrimination (National Research
Council, 2004). Differential treatment discrimination arises when applicants are treated in an unequal way based
on characteristics related to their group membership (like screening out applicants based on ethnicsounding names
as appearing on resumes). Differential effect discrimination results when applicants are treated in an unequal way
based on inadequately justified, nonjobrelated factors that covary with minority characteristics.
The central aim of this paper is to review literature on ethnic bias in the resumescreening phase so as to inform
HRM practice around this critical point of organisational entry. Below, we present a threestage model that integrates
theoretical perspectives to explain why resume screening is vulnerable to biased decisionmaking and ethnic discrimi-
nation (Section 2). This section is followed by research findings on contingencies of discriminatory resume screening
and focuses on factors situated at three different levels (i.e., applicant information in resumes, the decisionmaker,
and the broader resumescreening context) that may moderate biased decisionmaking against ethnic minorities
(Section 3). Whereas these first two sections consider microlevel processes, we follow with a critical reflection upon
several practical HRM interventions to avert ethnic discrimination in resume screening that are also situated at different
levels of our model (i.e., the screening tool, the decisionmaker, and the resumescreening context; Section 4). We
conclude this review with a discussion of limitations, future research opportunities, and implications for HR practice.
2|BIASED RESUME SCREENING
Despite the abundant literature on hiring discrimination, little research has considered why resume screening may be
prone to biased decisionmaking. Integrating assumptions from job market signalling and impression formation theo-
ries, we present a threestage model on biased decisionmaking in resume screening (see Figure 1, Part A). This model
states that when nonjobrelated, stigmatising applicant information is presented in resumes and jobrelated,
personalised information is rather limited (Stage 1: Applicant information in resumes), decisionmakers might engage
in categorisation/Type 1 processing (Stage 2: Impression formation), which increases the risk of biased applicant
impressions/ratings and discriminatory decisionmaking andhencemay undermine workforce diversity (Stage 3:
Resumescreening outcomes). Below, we explain each of the stages in more detail.
2.1 |Stage 1: Applicant information in resumes
The first stage represents the building block of decisionmaking in resume screening, namely, applicant information in
resumes, and is based on job market signalling theory. According to this theory (Spence, 1974), hiring managers and
job seekers have partly conflicting interests and will communicate and interpret signals of the other party's unknown
characteristics (like applicants' competencies or the organisation's culture) to obtain the biggest gains (like getting the
best employees on board or getting hired). Typically, signalling theory in selection considers the cues job seekers use
114 DEROUS AND RYAN
to make inferences about prospective employers (Carter & Highhouse, 2014). However HR professionals also look
for signals of applicants' job qualifications, like work experiences in resumes. Besides jobrelated information,
resumes might also signal nonjobrelated information, like applicants' social group status, through both explicit and
implicit cues. Applicants' skill sets on resumes, for instance, are explicit/observable signals of applicants' job qualifi-
cations thatat the same timemay also reveal information about applicants' chronological age in an implicit/subtle
way (Abrams, Swift, & Drury, 2016). Similarly, certain extracurricular activities on resumes can subtly signal nonjob
related, stigmatising information like applicants' ethnic background (Dovidio & Gaertner, 2000), which might affect
recruiters' information processing.
2.2 |Stage 2: Impression formation
The second stage of the model builds on impression formation theories and represents the way applicant information
is further processed by decisionmakers. Impression formation theories (like the continuum model; Fiske, Lin, &
Neuberg, 1999) specifically explain how signals of applicants' group status affect HR professionals' decisionmaking.
When only a limited amount of personalised information is available (like a onepage resume), individuals will auto-
matically engage in categorisation that in turn may activate group stereotypes. Whereas these processes are auto-
matic or unconscious (i.e., Type 1 processing; Kahneman, 2003), resume screening can also involve high levels of
conscious involvement, such as assessments of the congruence of applicants' characteristics with job and organisa-
tion characteristics (i.e., fit; KristofBrown, 2000). When more personalised information becomes available, recruiters
might engage in attributebased processing instead of categorybased processing of applicant information (i.e., Type
2 processing; Kahneman, 2003). The point, however, is that Type 1 processing readily occurs during resume
screening because of the limited amount of applicant information and, hence, may colour resumescreening outcomes.
2.3 |Stage 3: Resumescreening outcomes
The third stage of the model focuses on the outcomes that result from the information processing stage during
resume screening. Specifically, Type 1 processes may affect decisionmakers' first impressions/ratings and trigger
FIGURE 1 Model of biased resume screening [Colour figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]
DEROUS AND RYAN 115
discriminatory decisionmaking. Perceptions of similarity, for instance, may automatically induce interpersonal
attraction (Byrne, 1961) and explain why recruiters/organisations tend to attract, select, and retain applicants
that are similar to job incumbents (Schneider, 1987). Such cognitive processes may lead towards homogeneous
workforces and may undermine organisational diversity. As we will delineate more in discussing interventions,
when more personalised information becomes available about an applicant, HR professionals should be better
able to monitor Type 1 processing, which may result in a more fully informed and unbiased decision about the
applicant.
The threestage model illustrates why resume screening is vulnerable to biased decisionmaking (see Figure 1,
Part A),but not why ethnic discrimination occurs. Several socialeconomic and psychological theories may further
clarify why HRM systems may lead to such discrimination. Typically, socialeconomic theories stress macrolevel fac-
tors, like resource availability, institutional ideologies, industry culture, and local labour market practices (Almeida,
Fernando, & Sheridan, 2012; Béret, Mendez, Paraponaris, & RichezBattesti, 2003; Blair, Culkin, & Randle, 2003;
Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). Socialpsychological theories discuss microlevel factors; among which are demographic dis-
similarity to others (Goldberg, 2005), individuals' need to protect their ingroup and selfidentity (Tajfel & Turner,
1979), stereotypes and prejudiced attitudes (McConahay, Hardee, & Batts, 1981), and personality and dispositions
that trigger prejudice (Altemeyer, 1981; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999). Table 1 describes and illustrates some of the most
cited theories on ethnic discrimination in hiring.
3|CONTINGENCIES OF BIASED RESUME SCREENING
Resume screening may be more prone to ethnic discrimination when applicants' ethnic minority status is cued in
resumes and jobrelated, personalised information is limited (Stage 1). Decisionmakers, however, may not be equally
affected by applicant information in resumes (Stage 2). Whether they engage in categorisation or move to more
individualised information processing and decisionmaking (Stage 3) might depend on several contingencies that acti-
vate or inhibit categorisation (Kulik, Roberson, & Perry, 2007) and that are situated in the applicant (ethnic markers,
qualifications), the decisionmaker (beliefs, attitudes, experience), and the broader screening context (screening task,
job, organisation, society). Below, we review contingencies that are discussed in the literature and that may moderate
impression formation and resumescreening outcomes (see Figure 1, Part B).
3.1 |Applicant
3.1.1 |Ethnicity cues
Applicants' ethnicsounding names are one of the most investigated and explicit ethnic markers on resumes. In their
seminal correspondence audit study,
2
Bertrand and Mullainathan (2004) showed that resumes with AfricanAmerican
sounding names received 50% less of a chance of a positive callback compared with those with Whitesounding
names. Ethnic name discrimination in resume screening has been reported around the world (see Zschirnt & Ruedin,
2016, for a metaanalysis), and diverse lab studies also showed applicants with ethnicsounding names to be least
liked and hired (e.g., Cotton, O'Neill, & Griffin, 2008). Yet resumes may even include more explicit ethnic markers, like
cues about one's appearance in pictures attached to resumes. Research, for instance, shows an overall preference for
light over darkskinned applicants, even among darker skinned recruiters (Harrison & Thomas, 2009). Because visual
cues are immediately available and more rapidly processed than verbal and behavioural cues, visual markers of one's
ethnic background (like skin tone) might trigger social categorisation more than ethnicsounding names
(Weichselbaumer, 2017). Although it is common to ask applicants for their picture in some countries (Belgium), in
others, it is not (like the Netherlands). Nevertheless, trends towards prescreening via social media (SHRM, 2016)
make ethnic markers (like skin tone, ethnic attire, and even speech) more salient in early screening stages and there-
fore potential for bias should be considered.
116 DEROUS AND RYAN
TABLE 1 Socioeconomic and psychological theories on ethnic discrimination in hiring
Theories (domain/level/description) Illustration
Economy
(macrolevel)
Tastebased
discrimination theory
Majority workers/employers avoid interacting with minorities
because of tastebased factors instead of ascribed
productivity (preferencebased hiring).
Hiring managers set different groupbased thresholds of job
irrelevant characteristics (like ethnic accent), leading to lower
ability estimations of minority members (i.e., tastebased
discrimination). Statistical discrimination occurs when
observed group differentials in productivity are mistakenly
attributed to group identity (Neilson & Ying, 2016)
Statistical discrimination
theory
Majority workers/employers disavow minority individuals
because they consider minority groups as a whole less
productive from an economic point of view.
Sociology/social
psychology
(macrolevel and
microlevel)
Structural discrimination
theory
Societies share ideologies that justify groupbased
inequalities and formally/informally embedded
societal/organisational processes support such ideologies.
Hiring based on employee recommendations instead of
advertisements leads to differential effect discrimination if
employees systematically recommend applicants that are
similar to themselves (Waldinger & Lichter, 2003).
Group position theories,
realistic group conflict,
and competition
theories
Social groups are in competition over valued resources and
perceived threats from loss of resources results in
discrimination.
Socialeconomic threat (like recession) fostered hiring
discrimination against ethnic female applicants (King, Knight,
& Hebl, 2010).
Psychology
(microlevel)
Relational demography
theory
Majority members compare their demographic characteristics
to minority members. Perceived dissimilarity leads to
negative attitudes and behaviour towards minority
members.
Race similarity effects were observed on candidates' overall
interview assessments and job offer decisions by White
recruiters (Goldberg, 2005)
Social categorisation and
social identity theory
Majorities' need for and protection of a positive identity (self/
group) might instigate ingroup favouritism, which may
lead to discrimination against minorities
Resumes of highly ethnicidentified applicants received lower
job suitability ratings than equally qualified but less ethnically
identified counterparts (Derous, Nguyen, & Ryan, 2009).
Stereotype content
models
Stereotypes lead to discrimination. Stereotypes are
majorities' individual beliefs (cognitive schema and
expectations) about group characteristics that are applied
to minority and that are typically (but not necessarily)
negative in nature. Minorities may be aware of majorities'
stereotypical beliefs about them (i.e., metastereotypes)
and even integrate these ideas into their selfconcept (i.e.,
selfstereotyping).
Determining who gets hired for which job type depends on
applicants' match to the job stereotype, like applicants from
stereotypically warm groups hired more for social jobs
(Cuddy, Glick, & Beninger, 2011). (Negative) Meta
stereotypes might withhold qualified minorities from
applying for certain jobs (Wille & Derous, 2017).
Theories on oldfashioned
racism, modern racism,
and motivation to
respond without
prejudice
Racial/ethnic prejudice are racial/ethnic majorities'
predominantly negative attitudes towards racial/ethnic
minorities that might result in either blatant or more subtle
forms of discrimination against these minorities or
suppress them.
Modern racism affected hiring discrimination when a legitimate
authority figure provided a businessrelated justification for
discrimination (Brief, Dietz, Cohen, Pugh, & Vaslow, 2000).
Theories on personality
(authoritarianism and
social dominance
orientation)
Personality underlying majorities' worldviews affecting
ethnic/racial prejudice reactions towards minorities are
rightwing authoritarianism (conformity to tradition,
submission to powerful leaders, and political orientation),
and social dominance orientation (endorsement of group
hierarchy)
Majority of the members high in social dominance orientation
(SDO) were less likely to select a potential team member
from lowstatus groups (like racial minority candidates) than
those low in SDO (Umphress, Simmons, Boswell, &
Triana, 2008).
DEROUS AND RYAN 117
Although studies typically focus on explicit markers on resumes, resumes also contain more implicit cues to one's
ethnicity, such as applicants' affiliations with sociocultural groups, that may trigger biased information processing.
Cole, Rubin, Feild, and Giles (2007) illustrated that although HR professionals believed work experience to be the
strongest influence on ratings of applicants' employability, ratings were mostly affected by affiliations as mentioned
on resumes. Moreover, multiple ethnic cues may also interact and increase category salience such that resumes of
highly ethnically identified applicants (e.g., with ethnicsounding name and affiliations) might receive lower employ-
ability ratings due to increased outgroup status (Derous et al., 2009; Kang, DeCelles, Tilcsik, & Jun, 2016).
3.1.2 |Qualifications
Jobrelated cues on resumes typically temper categorisation. Contemporary models of discrimination (Dovidio &
Gaertner, 2000) suggest less discrimination if applicants either clearly possess the requested job qualifications or
do not possess them at all. However, when qualifications are moderate, a more ambiguous situation is created in
which discriminatory hiring decisions could be justified. For example, Almeida et al. (2012) noted that a lack of
recognition of experience and credentials gained overseas as well as concerns about language skills affected the
employment outcomes of professional immigrants. Hence, ethnic discrimination may occur more when decisions
can be rationalised based on some other factors than applicants' ethnicity (Brief et al., 2000).
3.2 |Decisionmaker
Studies on ethnic bias in resume screening have somewhat disregarded individual differences in decisionmakers' per-
sonality, beliefs about others (worldviews/stereotypes), prejudiced attitudes, and recruiting experience. This may be
explained by the longstanding tradition of audit studies in which decisionmakers' dispositions are typically not
accessible (for an exception, see Rooth, 2010). Studies that measure individual differences are predominantly con-
ducted in the lab and include beliefs in societal group hierarchies, like social dominance orientation (Sidanius& Pratto,
1999), prejudiced attitudes like modern racism (McConahay et al., 1981), and motivation to respond without preju-
dice (Plant & Devine, 1998, 2009). Although findings are sometimes mixed, negative beliefs/attitudes towards others
typically result in larger ethnic bias (Derous & Ryan, 2018). Indeed, although blatant discrimination is still reported,
research also shows that recruiters may not act upon their prejudice in resumescreening situations where bias would
be very obvious (e.g., if directly attributable to themselves) or when there is an external reason not to react in a
biased way (e.g., because of organisational policies; Brief et al., 2000).
Because individuals have become more sensitive to politically correct standards to disavow discrimination
(Dovidio & Gaertner, 2000), researchers directed their attention to implicit prejudice. Rooth (2010), for instance,
showed that Swedish recruiters were less likely to invite ArabMuslim minority applicants when they had more
negative, implicit attitudes towards ArabMuslims (as measured with implicit association tests; Greenwald, Banaji,
& Nosek, 2015
3
). There is, nevertheless, an ongoing debate about the validity of implicit attitude measures and
whether any relation with behaviour can be expected and established outside the lab (Oswald, Mitchell, Blanton,
Jaccard, & Tetlock, 2015).
Finally, there is also a debate about the role of decisionmakers' expertise. Predominantly discussed in interviewing
studies, some show more experienced recruiters to be less biased towards stigmatised applicants, whereas others show
more bias. De Meijer, Born, van Zielst, and van der Molen (2007), for instance, found that experienced recruiters used
more irrelevant information when judging ethnic minorities, perhaps because they feel overconfident and hence,
engage in Type 1 processing.
3.3 |Context
Contextual cues, like job and organisation characteristics as well as the way decisionmakers perform resume
screening tasks, also affect impression formation and resumescreening outcomes.
118 DEROUS AND RYAN
3.3.1 |Job characteristics
Contextual factors studied most are job stereotypes. Stereotypes not only exist for people but also for jobs, and
these may orient HR professionals towards viewing applicants as more or less suitable for certain jobs (i.e., cognitive
matching model; Trope & Liberman, 1993). For instance, King, Mendoza, Madera, Hebl, and Knight (2006) showed
that the effect of applicants' ethnic names on overall resume evaluation was not significant when applicants'
suitability for highversus lowstatus jobs was controlled for, suggesting job stereotypes affected resumescreening
outcomes. Audit studies (Carlsson & Rooth, 2008) further demonstrated lower callback ratios for ethnic minorities
who applied for occupations with higher external client contact (restaurant workers and shop sales assistants). Yet
mixed findings are also reported (Booth, Leigh, & Varganova, 2012; Derous, Ryan, & Serlie, 2015; Weichselbaumer,
2017), perhaps because matching effects may depend on a mixture of contextual cues (Goldberg, Finkelstein, Perry,
& Konrad, 2004). For instance, Dietz, Joshi, Esses, Hamilton, and Gabarrot (2015) showed that bias against qualified
immigrants was mitigated when the fit with the clientele was emphasised. Derous, Pepermans, and Ryan (2017) fur-
ther showed that discriminatory resume screening of the same applicant with varying skin tone (dark vs. light)
depended on the particular combination of several job and industry characteristics.
3.3.2 |Organisation/task characteristics
Aside from some studies that considered the demographic diversity of organisations and their clients (Almeida,
Fernando, Hannif, & Dharmage, 2015), surprisingly little research considers the role of other organisational character-
istics (like size and policies) and screening task characteristics (like time and financial pressure) on the way HR profes-
sionals screen resumes (Almeida et al., 2012). However, the HRM literature has established that line managers do not
fully engage in HRM duties because of time pressures and prioritising operational over HR tasks (McGovern, Gratton,
HopeHailey, Stiles, & Truss, 1997; Woodrow & Guest, 2014). Such time and motivational constraints likely
contribute to using categorisation to move through resume screening more quickly.
In sum, screening out competent people during the resumescreening stage because of nonjobrelated applicant
factors, decisionmakers' dispositions, and contextual factors is worrisome and costly, especially when labour markets
are tight and talented workers are hard to find. Hence, effective interventions are much needed to avert discrimina-
tory resume screening.
4|INTERVENTIONS TO AVERT BIASED RESUME SCREENING
Understanding contingencies may help both researchers and practitioners evaluating selection practices as well as
interventions to mitigate biased decisionmaking. Based on the best available evidence, this section critically dis-
cusses interventions to avert discriminatory resume screening that are situated at the level of the screening tool,
the decisionmaker, and the resumescreening context (see Figure 1, Part C).
4.1 |Screening tool
At the level of the screening tool, three different types of interventions are discussed, namely, anonymisation,
personalisation, and standardisation.
4.1.1 |Anonymisation
Anonymous application procedures such as blind auditions (Goldin & Rouse, 2000), blind interviewing (Buijsrogge,
Derous, & Duyck, 2016), anonymous resume screening (Åslund & Skans, 2012), or whitened
4
resumes (Kang
et al., 2016) aim to combat illegal discrimination by blotting or concealing personal identifiers. Although blind
auditions and interviewing have been found to be effective, studies on anonymous resume screening have shown
positive (Åslund & Skans, 2012; Kang et al., 2016), null, or even negative effects (Behaghel, Crépon, & Le Barbanchon,
DEROUS AND RYAN 119
2015; Hiscox et al., 2017; Krause, Rinne, & Zimmermann, 2012). The French government, therefore, decided to aban-
don the idea of making anonymous resume screening mandatory in the recruitment procedures of their public
employment services (Behaghel et al., 2015).
Why may anonymous resume screening fail? Both signalling and impression formation theory can help us under-
stand unintended side effects of anonymous resume screening. Aside from very explicit markers, resumes might also
contain more implicit cues, such as extracurricular activities, that might signal in a subtle way applicants' ethnic
minority status (Dovidio & Gaertner, 2000). Further, with anonymous resume screening, resumes are
decontextualised and depersonalised. As a result, HR professionals have less possibility to understand and attenuate
negative signals (e.g., from gaps in resumes or lower qualifications; Behaghel et al., 2015) and therefore mightpar-
adoxicallyengage in categorisation.
4.1.2 |Personalisation
Instead of altering or removing information, applicants could provide more personalised information, for instance, by
means of video resumes or social network sites. Video resumes are short videotaped messages of 12 min in which
an applicant presents himself/herself to potential employers. Much like paper resumes, video resumes present candi-
date information in an asynchronous way (one can view the resume information at any time, at any place). However,
they differ from paper resumes in that they provide more and different cues and allow applicants to show relevant com-
petencies. Interestingly, although ethnic minority applicants consider video resumes as more fair than paperandpencil
resumes, HR managers report concerns as more nonjobrelevant information (like physical attractiveness) is included
(Hiemstra & Derous, 2015).
Applicants may also provide more personalised information through social media as individuals increasingly
include links to their social network sites on their resumes (SHRM, 2016). HR professionals may use cybervetting
(i.e., the screening of social media sites like Facebook and LinkedIn) to extract information from applicants to inform
personnel decisions (Berkelaar & Buzzanell, 2014). About 44% of HR managers believe candidates' public social
network sites to be good sources for assessing potential (SHRM, 2016). Indeed, social network information might
provide more and different types of personalised information (like interests, values, and interactions with other users)
that reflect more typical behaviours than resumes do. Therefore, these sources might have incremental validity
beyond traditional screening tools.
However, findings are inconclusive about the validity of social network information. Kluemper, Rosen, and
Mossholder (2012) found that personality traits could be reliably assessed via Facebook profiles and were predictive
of future work behaviour beyond applicants' selfrated personality and intelligence scores. Yet Van Iddekinge,
Lanivich, Roth, and Junco (2016) showed that across a broad array of KSAOs, ratings of applicants' Facebook pages
did not predict job performance (i.e., supervisor ratings, turnover intentions, and actual turnover). Moreover, HR
professionals tended to favour White and female applicants when they screened applicants' Facebook information,
resulting in adverse impact. Furthermore, the availability of job irrelevant information may impair the overall validity
of unstandardised social media despite the fact that typical performance might be reflected in these media.
4.1.3 |Standardisation
Given that standardisation of selection procedures reduces the chance of judgmental biases in both recruiters and
applicants (Highhouse, Doverspike, & Guion, 2015), structured application forms might also be considered. This allows
organisations to score applicants' competencies and background information in a more objective way than with
applicantgenerated resumes that lack uniformity. Standardised application forms may also provide applicants fewer
possibilities to use impression formation tactics and faking than applicantgenerated resumes (Derous & Ryan,
2018). Equally, more structured, jobrelated social network sites like LinkedIn might make these sources less
vulnerable to biased decisionmaking than less structured media and at the same time increase their validity.
Corroborating this, van de Ven, Bogaert, Serlie, Brandt, and Denissen (2017) recently showed accurate personality
120 DEROUS AND RYAN
estimates based on LinkedIn profiles. However, the effectiveness of screening tools will also depend on
decisionmaker's characteristics and the way the decisionmaker uses the tool.
4.2 |Decisionmaker
This section considers the feasibility and effectiveness of four different types of interventions focused on the deci-
sionmaker, whether in HR or line management: selecting out prejudiced raters, offering training, holding raters
accountable, and replacing human decisionmakers with algorithms.
4.2.1 |Selection
Selecting out prejudiced raters seems obvious given effects of raters' particular worldviews (like social dominance ori-
entation) and prejudiced attitudes on judgments (i.e., theories on modern racism and authoritarian personality; see
Table 1). This intervention, however, might not be feasible as those chosen to screen resumes might do so because
of their technical expertise or hiring authority (Brief et al., 2000). Indeed, globally, HRM responsibilities related to
selection are increasing the responsibility of line managers rather than HR professionals (Brewster, Brookes, & Gollan,
2015). Furthermore, explicit prejudice measures may be susceptible to socially desirable responding, and their predic-
tive validity in the context of choosing resume screeners still needs to be demonstrated. The same applies to other
measures of individual predispositions, like social dominance orientation. Therefore, other interventions like training
are considered.
4.2.2 |Training
Recruiters could be trained to increase awareness about judgmental biases in resume screening. Dietz et al. (2015),
for instance, demonstrated how developing a common identity across groups may be a basis for inclusive HRM strat-
egies and reduce hiring discrimination against high skilled immigrants, for example, when a fit with a diverse clientele
is emphasised (i.e., common ingroup identity model; Gaertner & Dovidio, 2000). Building on social psychological the-
ories on categorisation, stereotyping, and motivation to respond without prejudice (see Table 1), Devine, Forscher,
Austin, and Cox (2012) further showed evidence for a multifaceted implicit prejudice habitbreaking intervention
that lasted 8 weeks and included different elements such as contact, perspective taking, stereotype replacement
(i.e., reconsideration of actions and thoughts to replace biased response), counterstereotypical imaging (i.e., imagin-
ing examples of outgroup members who counter commonly held stereotypes), and individuating (i.e., considering
outgroup members as individuals instead of stereotyped group members). However, these interventions are typically
developed for and tested in educational settings and not yet in corporate contexts, like resume screening.
4.2.3 |Accountability
Holding recruiters accountable for their decisions could also hold them from acting in prejudiced ways. However,
Self, Mitchell, Mellers, Tetlock, and Hildreth (2015) showed that type of accountability instruction matters. Holding
people accountable for certain outcomes, like an increase in the representation of minority applicants to face legal
or other pressures (i.e., identityconscious accountability), resulted in more prominority bias and less qualified
applicants than when recruiters were held accountable for making fair selection decisions based on jobrelevant con-
siderations (i.e., identityblind accountability).
Panel recruitment in which a team instead of a single rater screens resumes may be another avenue to increase fair-
ness. Followingpredictions from contemporarytheories on prejudice (seeTable 1), the presence of significant others (like
colleagues) might externally motivate recruiters to respond without prejudice andhenceto avoid being perceived as
discriminatory and/or to avoid repercussions (Plant & Devine, 1998, 2009). Ethnically mixed screening panels might
even lead to less biased decisionmaking. When recruiters work in ethnically mixed screening panels, they might get
to know each other and, as a consequence, might move from social categorisation (Type 1 processing) to
individualisation (Type 2 processing) (Fiske et al., 1999; Kahneman, 2003). Building further on predictions from the social
DEROUS AND RYAN 121
identity theory, ethnic minority and majority recruiters might even develop a common ingroup identity, which also
reduces the chance on biased decisionmaking (i.e., common ingroup identity model; see Gaertner & Dovidio, 2000).
In general, HRM research has clearly established that HR departments play a key role in enabling line managers
to successfully implement effective HR practices (Trullen, Stirpe, Bonache, & Valverde, 2016). Creating accountability
as well as providing recognition for unbiased hiring can be an important lever in ensuring effective resume screening.
4.2.4 |Algorithms
Instead of screening, training, and making decisionmakers accountable for fair screening,one could also replace
human decisionmakers by automated resume readers or algorithms. This idea is not new: In the 1970s, both the
Pentagon and IBM already replaced human decisionmakers by algorithms to narrow down the large piles of resumes
(O'Neil, 2016). Automated resume readers may boost efficiency by saving time, money, and energy. The French cos-
metic company L'Oréal, for instance, developed an algorithm to measure cultural fit based on only three openended
questions candidates answered on their mobile phone, which released recruiters from the timeconsuming procedure
of screening many resumes.
Proponents argue that algorithms may be more accurate and predictive than human decisionmakers (Danieli,
Hillis, & Luca, 2016). Although professionals still prefer holistic information processing (Kuncel, Klieger, & Ones,
2014), Kuncel, Klieger, Connelly, and Ones (2013) showed that mechanical data combination methods resulted in
more than 50% improvement in the prediction of work and academic criteria when compared with more holistic, intu-
itive methods. Other researchers further showed that algorithms can rate applicants' accomplishment narratives as
reliably as human raters (Campion, Campion, Campion, & Reider, 2016), can predict applicants' personality traits
and social/communication skills reasonably well from nonverbal cues extracted from video resumes (Nguyen &
GaticaPerez, 2016) or from Facebook likes (Youyou, Kosinski, & Stillwell, 2015), and can even predict which candi-
dates would most likely become involved in shooting or be accused of abuse as police officers (Chalfin et al., 2016).
Still, opponents remain cautious about the overall validity and fairness of automated resumescreening tools: If
people have the ability to identify how algorithms work, they might beat them too through strategic behaviour (like
drafting resumes to fit the system). Although some biases like friendship bias (Nguyen, 2006) might be countered,
automated resume screening might still be vulnerable to impression management and even faking behaviour (Waung,
McAuslan, DiMambro, & Mięgoć, 2017) as it might be as difficult for algorithms as human decisionmakers to filter
this out. Moreover, when algorithms are built upon human decisionmakers' subtly prejudiced rules, they might be
even more precise and persistent in discriminatory decisionmaking than any human decisionmaker. For instance,
Saint George's Hospital Medical School of SouthLondon was found guilty of discrimination in its admission policy
because their automated resume reader used nonjobrelated criteria (like misspellings), which were correlated with
applicants' ethnic group membership (Lowry & MacPherson, 1988).
4.3 |Context
In addition to interventions in the resumescreening tool and with decisionmakers, organisations as well as society at
large could develop policies and procedures to record discriminatory screening practices, to monitor recruitment
messages/sources and to guarantee competencebased assessments through discriminationfree employment
arrangements.
4.3.1 |Recording
Organisations could use different techniques, like correspondence audits (see earlier) and mystery shopping tests, to
measure and record hiring discrimination at the organisational and industry level. Mystery shopping involves a con-
federate who makes checks against specified criteria in order to get insight into system delivery. The selfregulating
body of recruitment offices in Flanders, for instance, had fictitious commissioning clients deliberately ask discrimina-
tory questions to recruitment offices in order to uncover discriminatory intentions (Federgon, 2013). Similar research
122 DEROUS AND RYAN
has asked subsidised cleaning companies to send out only native, Belgian cleaners to potential employers. Whereas
correspondence audits register actual discrimination, mystery shopping only capture one's intention to act in a dis-
criminatory way. Hence, one point of debate is whether mystery shopping might be used in a punitive rather than
a selfmonitoring way. Also, discriminatory intentions might reflect many different underlying, biasing processes that
are typically not directly measured with these tools (e.g., preferences, beliefs about economic productivity and com-
petitiveness, and social dominance; see Table 1). Another point of discussion is who may administer such tests,
whether to encourage HR managers and CEOs to organise audits and mystery shopping themselves or to consider
using qualified research institutes and/or governmental bodies.
4.3.2 |Targeted recruitment
Organisations may also attract more minority job seekers through targeted recruitment strategies like diversity state-
ments and the portrayal of minority employees in job advertisements. These targeted recruitment strategies build on
the social identity theory: Applicants who perceive the best fit with their social/individual identity may feel most
attracted to the organisation and may apply. Hence, by increasing the number of ethnic minority applicants that
apply, targeted recruitment strategies may be a way to avert adverse impact and to increase fairness in assessment.
Though, because effects of such targeted recruitment strategies on the reduction of adverse impact are rather mixed
(Avery & McKay, 2006), researchers turned their attention towards qualificationbased targeted recruitment strategies,
aimed to attract highly qualified ethnic minorities. Newman and Lyon (2009) indeed showed that job postings
designed to attract highly qualified ethnic minorities (e.g., requiring applicants high in conscientiousness) resulted
in less adverse impact. However and although promising, qualificationbased targeted recruitment strategies still
tend to disregard stereotypical ideas applicants might have about job qualifications/requirements. Indeed, applicants
too might have ideas about the stereotypical beliefs outgroup members hold about ingroup members (i.e., meta
stereotypes; Vorauer, Main, & O'Connell, 1998), and they may even integrate such metastereotypes into their
own selfconcept (selfstereotyping). Building further on stereotype content models, Wille and Derous (2017)
showed that organisations should be cautious about sprinkling job ads with requirements that (minority) candidates
hold negative metastereotypes about, particularly if those requirements are communicated in dispositional ways
(like This company is looking for applicants who are high in integrity). Such job ads might discourage (highly
qualified) minority candidates to apply instead of attracting them.
Besides recruitment messages, organisations may also consider their recruitment sources as some might be less
frequently consulted/used by minority than by majority job seekers. For instance, video resumes are potentially dis-
criminatory against minority groups who may have less tech access (i.e., differential effect discrimination; Heathfield,
2016). Remarkably, bias might even be encoded in algorithms of search engines (Hajian, Bonchi, & Castillo, 2016).
Sweeney (2013) showed that algorithms for public record websites were more likely to imply criminal activities (like
arrest records) with searches for Blacksounding names than Whitesounding names. Finally, labour market interme-
diaries (temporary work agencies and public employment services) can play a role in assuming some level of recruit-
ment and selection functions for hiring organisations (Bonet, Cappelli, & Hamori, 2013). However, Ingold and
Valizade (2017) demonstrated that although intermediaries may increase likelihood of hiring from disadvantaged
groups, employer selective hiring criteria still led to lower employability of marginalised groups.
4.3.3 |Employment (economic/societal)
Finally, more radical interventions consider the rethinking of employment relations at the economic/societal level to
reduce hiring discrimination by promoting open,accessible labour markets. One way to realise this is through new
types of employment arrangements. eLancing
5
(Aguinis & Lawal, 2013) might address this call: Employers' evaluation
of eLancers based on their past assignments resembles work sample tests that are known to be valid predictors of
future work performances. Furthermore, hiring for eLancingassignments may be blind, so that freelancers' ethnicity
does not affect decisionmaking.
DEROUS AND RYAN 123
Open Badges ecosystems are another way to create more accessible, discriminationfree labour markets. The open
badges ecosystem (https://openbadges.org), originally launched by Mozilla, encompasses a method for packaging
information about one's individual accomplishments, skills, qualities, or interests in portable image files as a digital
badge that subsequently can be displayed via job seekers' social media platforms and consulted by potential
employers. The system's infrastructure ensures that badges are reliably issued by institutions and endorsed within
the open badges ecosystem (e.g., as approved by the Department of Education or other reliable institutions). Through
open badges backpacks, applicants might provide potential employers with very personalised, timely, job relevant,
and certified/objective information about their competencies during the initial screening stage, which in turn might
help countering social categorisation and hiring discrimination. Indeed, according to impression formation theories
(e.g., Fiske et al., 1999), the more personalised information a recruiter/HR professional receives about an applicant
in the early screening stage (e.g., through information in open badges), the more she/he might engage inType 2 pro-
cessing (individuating) and move away from Type 1 processing (social categorisation).
Technological developments (like Open Badges) not only offer alternatives for discriminatory resume screening
but also redesign HR practices fundamentally. Whereas traditionally, companies attract, screen, and select applicants
by presenting job requirements/offers, through technological developments like open badges, the power nexus shifts
to the applicant, who will attract, screen, and even select companies/jobs by showing their competencies (i.e., com-
panies bidding for applicants).
5|DISCUSSION
Diversity in organisations can be effectively managed through HRM practices. Remarkably, despite societal debates
about fair hiring (Feintzeig, 2016), fairness of HR tools like resume screening has received less research attention,
especially when compared with the extensive literature on other selection tools, like the job interview. Resume
screening, however, is worldwide one of the most frequently used screening tools that determines the quantity, qual-
ity, and diversity of applicant pools. We aimed to address this gap by formulating a model of biased decisionmaking
in resume screening (Figure 1) that includes contingencies of resume screening as well as interventions to avert dis-
criminatory screening,all related to relevant theories and empirical findings on ethnic discrimination. It further allows
to identify mixed findings and literature gaps. Hence, the model might steer further research on discriminatory
resume screening as well as interventions to avert this. Below, we summarise the most important opportunities for
further research, followed by implications for practitioners.
5.1 |Research opportunities
5.1.1 |Applicant information and screening tool
At the heart of the model (Figure 1, Part A) is a cognitive mechanism of biased decisionmaking that we based on
assumptions from job market signalling and impression formation theory. Research could further investigate
microlevel processes of impression formation in a bottomup way, for example, by tracing decisionmakers' attention
to both nonjobrelated/jobrelated and implicit/explicit information, by investigating whether attention paid to
different resume cues differently affects categorisation/individualisation, and by investigating their effect on
resumescreening outcomes (see for a similar approach on interview bias: Buijsrogge et al., 2016). These findings
might also provide useful information regarding the effectiveness (validity) of anonymisation versus personalisation
of resumes. Related, more empirical studies are needed on the effectiveness of structuring applicant information
to avert Type 1 processing in decisionmakers. Further and as already mentioned, we considered ethnic discrimina-
tion. Systematic reviews of effects of other stigmatising cues in resumes (and combined effects) are also needed,
given the paucity of reviews on judgmental biases in resume screening and the necessity to generalise findings to dif-
ferent stigmatised groups.
124 DEROUS AND RYAN
5.1.2 |Decisionmaker
Surprisingly little research considers individual difference variables that make decisionmakers vulnerable to biased decision
making in the resumescreening stage. Hence, more research is needed on reliable/valid methods of choosing resume
screeners for unbiased decisionmaking (like measures of prejudiced attitudes), on training programs that might change
recruiter bias in resumescreening contexts, and on the usefulness of ethnically mixed screening panels. Also, researchers
have only started evaluating the way algorithms are developed and validated. As human decisionmakers are already being
replaced by algorithms in organisations, algorithms should be compared with humans regarding reliability/validity of
decisions, levels of adverse impact, vulnerability to impression management (including faking), and perceived fairness.
5.1.3 |Context
Not only individual difference variables but also contextual variables may increase the likelihood of Type 1 processing
and trigger biased decisionmaking in resume screening. Future research could consider moderating effect of
microlevel factors like job and resumescreening task characteristics (like available time and other taskrelated pres-
sures). For instance, some recruitment sources do not reach potentially qualified candidates from ethnic minority
communities or might discourage qualified applicants to apply. However, also mesolevel factors (like organisational
diversity policies) and macrolevel factors (like labour market situation, work arrangements, politics, and cultural
habits) might affect recruitment practices and should be further considered. For instance, affirmative action plans
cannot be realised with anonymous resume screening as one needs to be aware of social category membership. As
regards macrolevel factors, one could further investigate whether effectiveness of Open Badges depends on the kind
of information included in badges (like cognitive test performance).
5.2 |Practical implications
Organisations may keep track on how decisionmakers evaluate applicants and could setup specific training pro-
grams in which recruiters are informed about judgmental mechanisms and biases (e.g., induced by cultural differ-
ences) as well as potential effective interventions to avert biases, such as the use of qualificationbased targeted
recruitment and competencebased screening tools like structured application forms. However, targeted recruitment
initiatives as well as more technologydriven applications (like automated resumescreening tools) should always be
critically evaluated to assure they are valid and free from bias, regardless of whether they are developed outside or
inside one's organisation. Relatedly, organisations should keep up to date about, setup, and communicate their pol-
icies on cybervetting so that both recruiters and applicants are fully aware of the kind of information that might be
evaluated online. Finally, more attention should be paid to recruiters/decisionmakers' working conditions. Stress
levels (due to time or any other taskrelated pressure) should be reduced as these might increase the risk of Type
1 processes and biased decisionmaking in resume screening.
Ethnic minority applicants and career counsellors canbenefit too from literature insights. For instance, applicants may
be informed about explicit/implicit cues to both jobrelated and nonjobrelated information on their resumes as well as
about organisational context factors (like client preferences). Career counsellors might also help applicants to properly
interpret job requirements and check critically whether applicants' qualifications are not too ambiguously presented but
clearly match the job requirements to minimise riskson discrimination. Finally, social media profiles may be kept up to date
and best include professional information only. In general, applicants might consider recruitment devices that allow for
more competencebased, individualised screening (like badges, structured competency lists, or perhaps video resumes).
6|CONCLUSION
In conclusion, despite the widespread use of resume screening as well as the plethora of studies on ethnic discrim-
ination in hiring, a model on biased decisionmaking in resume screening that integrates findings was still lacking.
DEROUS AND RYAN 125
One of the strengths of this paper is that we addressed this literature gap by highlighting an underlying mechanism of
biased decisionmaking, contingencies that might moderate bias, and interventions that might avert judgmental bias
in resume screening. This review not only revealed several interesting insights but also showed that there is still much
to be discovered. Specifically, we discussed biased resume screening in the context of ethnic discrimination without
considering other stigmatising factors than ethnic makers (or intersectional effects). Further, we considered biased
resume screening from the HR professional/organisation perspective rather than the applicant/job seeker perspec-
tive. Applicants' perceived discrimination, however, may be as important as actual discrimination. Third, this review
focused on operational HR processes to manage organisational diversity rather than tactical and strategical HR pro-
cesses that also play a role (Shen et al., 2009). As we are among the first to summarise and integrate literature on
biased decisionmaking in resume screening, still more aspects can be looked at to build an even more comprehensive
model. Finally, latest technologydriven tools/systems (like algorithms and Open Badges) reflect not only the
changing nature of our labour market and talent acquisition/management in HRM but also the potential to counter
bias in early screening stages, if carefully thought through, developed, and implemented by HR professionals.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
ENDNOTES
1
Some disciplines (medicine, education, and academia) expect extensive curriculum vitae (CVs) that offer a complete career
history with detailed information on professional activities. This review focuses on resumes, which are a more abridged
career summary; however, much of the research reviewed may be applicable to CV screening.
2
Employment audit studies investigate labour market outcomes of applicants who are equally qualified for a job but differ in
nonjobrelated characteristics, like ethnic background. In correspondence audit studies, pairs of matched resumes are sent
to the same employer and the type and number of callbacks are registered.
3
Implicit association tests are reaction time measures in which respondents are asked to match concepts (Arabsounding
names) to attributes (good/bad). The speed with which respondents do so is considered to reflect implicit attitudes
towards certain minorities.
4
Whitened resumes are ones where identifying information is concealed or blotted, for example, by using one's middle
name instead of first name if the former is more raceneutral or by removing words referring to racial group membership
(like [Black] students' association).
5
eLancing websites are crowdsourcing internet marketplaces where employers place assignments (e.g., software develop-
ment and translations) that freelancers can bid for. Work is completed on an asneeded basis and freelancers are
evaluated on the quality of their previous assignments.
ORCID
Eva Derous http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7874-5836
REFERENCES
Abrams, D., Swift, H. J., & Drury, L. (2016). Old and unemployable? How agebased stereotypes affect willingness to hire job
candidates. Journal of Social Issues,72(1), 105121. https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12158
Aguinis, H., & Lawal, S. O. (2013). eLancing: A review and research agenda for bridging the sciencepractice gap. Human
Resource Management Review,23(1), 617. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.hrmr.2012.06.003
Almeida, S., Fernando, M., Hannif, Z., & Dharmage, S. C. (2015). Fitting the mould: The role of employer perceptions in immi-
grant recruitment decisionmaking. International Journal of Human Resource Management,26(22), 28112832. https://doi.
org/10.1080/09585192.2014.1003087
Almeida, S., Fernando, M., & Sheridan, A. (2012). Revealing the screening: Organisational factors influencing the recruitment
of immigrant professionals. International Journal of Human Resource Management,23(9), 19501965. https://doi.org/
10.1080/09585192.2011.616527
Altemeyer, B. (1981). Enemies of freedom: Understanding right wing authoritarianism. Winnipeg, CA: JosseyBass.
126 DEROUS AND RYAN
Åslund, O., & Skans, O. N. (2012). Do anonymous job application procedures level the playing field? ILR Review,65(1),
82107. https://doi.org/10.1177/001979391206500105
Avery, D. R., & McKay, P. F. (2006). Target practice: An organizational impression management approach to attracting minor-
ity and female job applicants. Personnel Psychology,59(1), 157187. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.17446570.2006.00807.x
Barney, J. (2001). Resourcebased theories of competitive advantage: A tenyear retrospective on the resourcebased view.
Journal of Management,27(6), 643650. https://doi.org/10.1016/S01492063(01)001155
Behaghel, L., Crépon, B., & Le Barbanchon, T. (2015). Unintended effects of anonymous résumés. American Economic Journal:
Applied Economics,7(3), 127. https://doi.org/10.1257/app.20140185
Béret, P., Mendez, A., Paraponaris, C., & RichezBattesti, N. (2003). R&D personnel and human resource management in mul-
tinational companies: Between homogenization and differentiation. International Journal of Human Resource Management,
14(3), 449468. https://doi.org/10.1080/0958519022000031843
Berkelaar, B. L., & Buzzanell, P. M. (2014). Cybervetting, personenvironment fit, and personnel selection. Journal of Applied
Communication Research,42(4), 456476. https://doi.org/10.1080/00909882.2014.954595
Bertrand, M., & Mullainathan, S. (2004). Are Emily and Greg more employable than Lakisha and Jamal? A field experiment on
labor market discrimination. American Economic Review,94(4), 9911013. https://doi.org/10.1257/0002828042002561
Blair, H., Culkin, N., & Randle, K. (2003). From London to Los Angeles: A comparison of local labour market processes in the
US and UK film industries. International Journal of Human Resource Management,14(4), 619633. https://doi.org/
10.1080/0958519032000057619
Bonet, R., Cappelli, P., & Hamori, M. (2013). Labor market intermediaries and the new paradigm for human resources. Acad-
emy of Management Annals,7(1), 341392. https://doi.org/10.5465/19416520.2013.774213
Booth, A. L., Leigh, A., & Varganova, E. (2012). Does ethnic discrimination vary across minority groups? Oxford Bulletin of Eco-
nomics and Statistics,74(4), 547573. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.14680084.2011.00664.x
Brewster, C., Brookes, M., & Gollan, P. J. (2015). The institutional antecedents of the assignment of HRM responsibilities to
line managers. Human Resource Management,54(4), 577597. https://doi.org/10.1002/hrm.21632
Brief, A., Dietz, J., Cohen, R., Pugh, S., & Vaslow, J. (2000). Just doing business: Modern racism and obedience to authority as
explanations for employment discrimination. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,81(1), 7297.
https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.1999.2867
Buijsrogge, A., Derous, E., & Duyck, W. (2016). Often biased but rarely in doubt: How initial reactions to stigmatized
applicants affect interviewer confidence. Human Performance,29(4), 275290. https://doi.org/10.1080/
08959285.2016.1165225
Byrne, D. (1961). Interpersonal attraction and attitude similarity. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,62(3), 713715.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0044721
Campion, M. C., Campion, M. A., Campion, E. D., & Reider, M. H. (2016). Initial investigation into computer scoring of
candidate essays for personnel selection. Journal of Applied Psychology,101(7), 958975. https://doi.org/10.1037/
apl0000108
Carlsson, M., & Rooth, D.O. (2008). Is it your foreign name or foreign qualifications? IZA Discussion Paper No. 3810.
Retrieved from http://ftp.iza.org/dp3810.pdf
Carter, N. T., & Highhouse, S. (2014). You will be known by the company you keep: Understanding the social identity
concerns of job seekers. In K. Y. T. Yu, & D. M. Cable (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of recruitment (pp. 454462). New York:
Oxford University Press.
Chalfin, A., Danieli, O., Hillis, A., Jelveh, Z., Luca, M., Ludwig, J., & Mullainathan, S. (2016). Productivity and selection of
human capital with machine learning. American Economic Review,106(5), 124127. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.
p20161029
Cole, M., Rubin, R., Feild, H., & Giles, W. (2007). Recruiters' perceptions and use of applicant resume information. Applied
Psychology: An International Review,56, 319343.
Cotton, J. L., O'Neill, B. S., & Griffin, A. (2008). The name game: Affective and hiring reactions to first names. Journal of
Managerial Psychology,23,1839. https://doi.org/10.1108/02683940810849648
Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Beninger, A. (2011). The dynamics of warmth and competence judgments, and their outcomes in
organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior,31,7398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.riob.2011.10.004
Danieli, O., Hillis, A., & Luca, M. (2016). How to hire with algorithms. Harvard Business Review, October. Retrieved from
https://hbr.org/2016/10/how-to-hire-with-algorithms
De Meijer, L. A. L., Born, PH. M., van Zielst, J., & van der Molen, H. T. (2007). Analyzing judgments of ethnically diverse appli-
cants during personnel selection. International Journal of Selection and Assessment,15(2), 139152. https://doi.org/
10.1111/j.14682389.2007.00376.x
DEROUS AND RYAN 127
Derous, E., Nguyen, H.H., & Ryan, A. M. (2009). Hiring discrimination against Arab minorities. Human Performance,22(4),
297320. https://doi.org/10.1080/08959280903120261
Derous, E., Pepermans, R., & Ryan, A. M. (2017). Ethnic discrimination during résumé screening: Interactive effects of appli-
cants' ethnic salience with job context. Human Relations,70(7), 860882. https://doi.org/10.1177/0018726716676537
Derous, E., & Ryan, A. M. (2018). By any other name. In U. Klehe, & E. A. J. van Hooft (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of
job loss and job search (pp. 501522). Oxford: Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/
9780199764921.013.017
Derous, E., Ryan, A. M., & Serlie, A. W. (2015). Double jeopardy upon resumé screening. Personnel Psychology,68(3),
659696. https://doi.org/10.1111/peps.12078
Devine, P. G., Forscher, P. S., Austin, A. J., & Cox, W. T. L. (2012). Longterm reduction in implicit race bias: A prejudice habit
breaking intervention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,48(6), 12671278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
jesp.2012.06.003
Dietz, J., Joshi, C., Esses, V. M., Hamilton, L. K., & Gabarrot, F. (2015). The skill paradox: Explaining and reducing employment
discrimination against skilled immigrants. International Journal of Human Resource Management,26(10), 13181334.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09585192.2014.990398
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2000). Aversive racism and selection decisions: 1989 and 1999. Psychological Science,11(4),
315319. https://doi.org/10.1111/14679280.00262
Federgon. (2013). Jaarverslag 2012. Brussels, Belgium: Federgon.
Feintzeig, R. (2016). The boss doesn't want your resume. The Wall Street Journal, 5 January. Retrieved from https://www.wsj.
com/articles/thebossdoesntwantyourresume1452025908
Fiske, S. T., Lin, M., & Neuberg, S. (1999). The continuum model, ten years later. In S. Chaiken, & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process
theories in social psychology (pp. 231254). New York: Guilford Press.
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2000). Reducing intergroup bias: The common ingroup identity model. Philadelphia, PA:
Psychology Press.
Goldberg, C. B. (2005). Relational demography and similarityattraction in interview assessments and subsequent offer deci-
sions. Group & Organization Management,30(6), 597624. https://doi.org/10.1177/1059601104267661
Goldberg, C. B., Finkelstein, L. M., Perry, E. L., & Konrad, A. M. (2004). Job and industry fit: The effects of age and gender
matches on career progress outcomes. Journal of Organizational Behavior,25(7), 807829. https://doi.org/10.1002/
job.269
Goldin, C., & Rouse, C. (2000). Orchestrating impartiality: The impact of blindauditions on female musicians. American Eco-
nomic Review,90, 715741. Retrieved from www.jstor.org/stable/117305
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2015). Statistically small effects of the Implicit Association Test can have soci-
etally large effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,108(4), 553561. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000016
Hajian, S., Bonchi, F., & Castillo, C. (2016). Algorithmic bias. In Proceedings of the 22nd ACM SIGKDD International Confer-
ence on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining KDD'16 (pp. 21252126). New York: ACM Press.
Harrison, M. S., & Thomas, K. M. (2009). The hidden prejudice in selection. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,39(1),
134168. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.15591816.2008.00433.x
Heathfield, S. M. (2016). Do employers want video resumes? Retrieved from https://www.thebalance.com/videoresumes
1918979
Hiemstra, A. M. F., & Derous, E. (2015). Video résumés portrayed: Findings and challenges. In I. Nikolaou, & J. K. Oostrom
(Eds.), Employee recruitment, selection, and assessment: Contemporary issues for theory and practice (pp. 4460). Hove,
UK: Psychological Press.
Highhouse, S., Doverspike, D., & Guion, R. (2015). Essentials of personnel assessment and selection. New York: Routledge.
Hiscox, M. J., Oliver, T., Ridgway, M., ArcosHolzinger, L., Warren, A., & Willis, A. (2017). Going blind to see more clearly:
Unconscious bias in Australian Public Service shortlisting processes. Retrieved from https://pmc.gov.au/sites/default/
files/publications/BETAreportgoingblindtoseemore clearly.pdf
Hoque, K., & Noon, M. (1999). Racial discrimination in speculative applications. Human Resource Management Journal,9(3),
7182. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.17488583.1999.tb00204.x
Ingold, J., & Valizade, D. (2017). Employers' recruitment of disadvantaged groups. Human Resource Management Journal,
27(4), 530547. https://doi.org/10.1111/17488583.12154
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist,58(9),
697720. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003066X.58.9.697
128 DEROUS AND RYAN
Kang, S. K., DeCelles, K. A., Tilcsik, A., & Jun, S. (2016). Whitened résumés. Administrative Science Quarterly,61(3), 469502.
https://doi.org/10.1177/0001839216639577
King, E. B., Knight, J. L., & Hebl, M. R. (2010). The influence of economic conditions on aspects of stigmatization. Journal of
Social Issues,66(3), 446460. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-560.2010.01655.x
King, E. B., Mendoza, S. A., Madera, J. M., Hebl, M. R., & Knight, J. L. (2006). What's in a name? A multiracial investigation of
the role of occupational stereotypes in selection decisions. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,36(5), 11451159.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.00219029.2006.00035.x
Kluemper, D. H., Rosen, P. A., & Mossholder, K. W. (2012). Social networking websites, personality ratings, and the
organizational context. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,42(5), 11431172. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559
1816.2011.00881.x
Krause, A., Rinne, U., & Zimmermann, K. F. (2012). Anonymous job applications of fresh Ph.D. economists. Economics Letters,
117(2), 441444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.econlet.2012.06.029
KristofBrown, A. L. (2000). Perceived applicant fit: Distinguishing between recruiters' perceptions of personjob and person
organization fit. Personnel Psychology,53(3), 643671. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.17446570.2000.tb00217.x
Kulik, C. T., Roberson, L., & Perry, E. L. (2007). The multiplecategory problem: Category activation and inhibition in the hiring
process. Academy of Management Review,32(2), 529548. https://doi.org/10.5465/AMR.2007.24351855
Kuncel, N. R., Klieger, D. M., Connelly, B. S., & Ones, D. S. (2013). Mechanical versus clinical data combination in selection
and admissions decisions: A metaanalysis. Journal of Applied Psychology,98(6), 10601072. https://doi.org/10.1037/
a0034156
Kuncel, N. R., Klieger, D. M., & Ones, D. S. (2014). In hiring, algorithms beat instinct. Harvard Business Review, May. https://
hbr.org/2014/05/in-hiring-algorithms-beat-instinct
Lowry, S., & MacPherson, G. (1988). A blot on the profession. British Medical Journal,296(6623), 657658.
McConahay, J. B., Hardee, B. B., & Batts, V. (1981). Has racism declined in America? Journal of Conflict Resolution,25(4),
563579. https://doi.org/10.1177/002200278102500401
McGovern, P., Gratton, L., HopeHailey, V., Stiles, P., & Truss, C. (1997). Human resource management on the line? Human
Resource Management Journal,7(4), 1229. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.17488583.1997.tb00286.x
National Research Council (2004). Measuring racial discrimination. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
Neilson, W., & Ying, S. (2016). From tastebased to statistical discrimination. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization,
129, 116128. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2016.06.001
Newman, D. A., & Lyon, J. S. (2009). Recruitment efforts to reduce adverse impact: Targeted recruiting for personality, cog-
nitive ability, and diversity. Journal of Applied Psychology,94(2), 298317. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013472
Nguyen, L. S., & GaticaPerez, D. (2016). Hirability in the wild: Analysis of online conversational video resumes. IEEE Trans-
actions on Multimedia,18(7), 14221437. https://doi.org/10.1109/TMM.2016.2557058
Nguyen, N. T. (2006). Recruiters' use of friendship in resume assessment: When qualification does not always make the cut.
Applied H.R.M Research,11(1), 114.
O'Neil, C. (2016). Weapons of math destruction: How big data increases inequality and threatens democracy. New York: Crown.
Oswald, F. L., Mitchell, G., Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., & Tetlock, P. E. (2015). Using the IAT to predict ethnic and racial discrim-
ination: Small effect sizes of unknown societal significance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,108(4), 562571.
https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000023
Outtz, J. L. (2010). Adverse impact: Implications for organizational staffing and high stakes selection. New York: Routledge.
Piotrowski, C., & Armstrong, T. (2006). Current recruitment and selection practices: A national survey of Fortune 1000 firms.
North American Journal of Psychology,8(3), 489496. Retrieved from http://org8220renner.alliant.wikispaces.net/file/
view/Selection_recrutiment_fortune+1000.pdf/33002629/Selection_recrutiment_fortune+1000.pdf
Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (1998). Internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology,75(3), 811832. https://doi.org/10.1037/00223514.75.3.811
Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (2009). The active role of prejudice: Unpacking the intentions guiding control efforts. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,96(3), 640652. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012960
Rooth, D. O. (2010). Automatic associations and discrimination in hiring: Real world evidence. Labour Economics,17,
523534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.labeco.2009.04.005
Schneider, B. (1987). The people make the place. Personnel Psychology,40(3), 437453. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744
6570.1987.tb00609.x
DEROUS AND RYAN 129
Self, W. T., Mitchell, G., Mellers, B. A., Tetlock, P. E., & Hildreth, J. A. D. (2015). Balancing fairness and efficiency: The impact
of identityblind and identityconscious accountability on applicant screening. PLOS One,10(12), e0145208. https://doi.
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0145208
Shen, J., Chanda, A., D'Netto, B., & Monga, M. (2009). Managing diversity through human resource management: An inter-
national perspective and conceptual framework. International Journal of Human Resource Management,20(2), 235251.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09585190802670516
SHRM. (2016). Using social media for talent acquisition: Recruitment and screening. Alexandria, VA: Society for Human
Resource Management.
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social hierarchy and oppression. New York:
Cambridge University Press.
Spence, A. M. (1974). Market signaling: Informational transfer in hiring and related screening processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Sweeney, L. (2013). Discrimination in online ad delivery. Queue,11(3), 10. https://doi.org/10.1145/2460276.2460278
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. Austin, & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psy-
chology of intergroup relations (pp. 3347). Monterey, CA: BrooksCole.
Thorndike, R. L. (1949). Personnel selection: Test and measurement techniques. New York: John Wiley & Sons.
Trope, Y., & Liberman, A. (1993). The use of trait conceptions to identify other people's behavior and to draw inferences
about their personalities. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin,19(5), 553562. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0146167293195007
Trullen, J., Stirpe, L., Bonache, J., & Valverde, M. (2016). The HR department's contribution to line managers' effective imple-
mentation of HR practices. Human Resource Management Journal,26(4), 449470. https://doi.org/10.1111/1748
8583.12116
Umphress, E. E., Simmons, A. L., Boswell, W. R., & Triana, M. (2008). Managing discrimination in selection: The influence of
directives from an authority and social dominance orientation. Journal of Applied Psychology,93(5), 982993. https://doi.
org/10.1037/00219010.93.5.982
van de Ven, N., Bogaert, A., Serlie, A., Brandt, M. J., & Denissen, J. J. A. (2017). Personality perception based on LinkedIn
profiles. Journal of Managerial Psychology,32(6), 418429. https://doi.org/10.1108/JMP0720160220
Van Iddekinge, C. H., Lanivich, S. E., Roth, P. L., & Junco, E. (2016). Social media for selection? Validity and adverse impact
potential of a Facebookbased assessment. Journal of Management,42(7), 18111835. https://doi.org/10.1177/
0149206313515524
Vorauer, J. D., Main, K. J., & O'Connell, G. B. (1998). How do individuals expect to be viewed by members of lower status
groups? Content and implications of metastereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,75, 917937.
Waldinger, R., & Lichter, M. I. (2003). How the other half works: Immigrants and the social organization of labor. Berkeley, CA:
University of California Press.
Waung, M., McAuslan, P., DiMambro, J. M., & Mięgoć, N. (2017). Impression management use in resumes and cover letters.
Journal of Business and Psychology,32(6), 727746. https://doi.org/10.1007/s1086901694709
Weichselbaumer, D. (2017). Discrimination against migrant job applicants in Austria: An experimental study. German
Economic Review,18(2), 237265. https://doi.org/10.1111/geer.12104
Wille, L., & Derous, E. (2017). Getting the words right: When wording of job ads affects ethnic minorities' application deci-
sions. Management Communication Quarterly,31(4), 533558. https://doi.org/10.1177/0893318917699885
Woodrow, C., & Guest, D. E. (2014). When good HR gets bad results: Exploring the challenge of HR implementation in the
case of workplace bullying. Human Resource Management Journal,24(1), 3856. https://doi.org/10.1111/1748
8583.12021
Youyou, W., Kosinski, M., & Stillwell, D. (2015). Computerbased personality judgments are more accurate than those made
by humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,112(4), 10361040. https://doi.org/10.1073/
pnas.1418680112
Zschirnt, E., & Ruedin, D. (2016). Ethnic discrimination in hiring decisions: A metaanalysis of correspondence tests 1990
2015. Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies,42(7), 11151134. https://doi.org/10.10800/
133693183X.20105.11303279
How to cite this article: Derous E, Ryan AM. When your resume is (not) turning you down: Modelling ethnic
bias in resume screening. Hum Resour Manag J. 2019;29:113130. https://doi.org/10.1111/17488583.12217
130 DEROUS AND RYAN
... The resume screening situation is no different, as our age, name, and other cues are present simultaneously and available to the perceiver. Inviting an individual for an interview is a decision that has to be made relatively quickly and based on limited information, which is a type of situation described as ideal for relying on the social categorization process and allowing for bias to affect one's judgments (Derous & Ryan, 2019). ...
... An applicant aims to pass this stage successfully and advance in the hiring process by being invited for an interview or other form of assessment. It is not surprising then that the resume screening stage has become a topic of research that has caught the attention of scholars in economics, sociology, and psychology, among other disciplines (see Bertrand & Duflo, 2017;Derous & Ryan, 2019;Rivera, 2020). In many countries worldwide, discrimination on diverse grounds has been identified in this stage of hiring (see for a review of field experiments: Bertrand & Duflo, 2017;Granberg, 2022;Neumark, 2018;Riach & Rich, 2002;Rich, 2014;Zschirnt & Ruedin, 2016). ...
... According to Derous & Ryan (2019), although resume screening is a very common way of assessing applicants, the fairness of this process has not been researched as extensively as, for example, the adverse impact of personnel selection tests. Derous and Ryan (2019) defined differential treatment as discriminatory behavior where equally qualified applicants are treated differentially based on some characteristics related to minority demographic group memberships that are not job-related or justified. ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Hiring discrimination research has predominantly focused on labor market outcomes based on one or two group memberships, most commonly ethnicity and gender. The main aim of the doctoral dissertation is to explore warmth and competence perceptions associated with multiple demographic group membership and how they may affect evaluations and behavior in a workplace and hiring context. Study I relies on the stereotype content model (SCM, Fiske et al., 2002) to explore how employees perceive intersections of demographic groups on universal dimensions of social perception (warmth and competence). Namely, the intersection of ethnicity (Arab/Swedish), gender (male/female), age (30-year-old/55-year-old), and sexual orientation (gay/heterosexual). Results show that when stigmatized group categories are added to one another, this does not necessarily produce additive negative effects. Study II addressed perceptions of 22 common occupations in Sweden (e.g., Mechanic, Surgeon). Results show that warmth and competence can account for perceptions of the pre-selected occupations with a five-cluster solution found most descriptive of the data. Study III uses a modernized version of the “lost letter” technique (Milgram et al., 1965) and experimentally tests how ethnic discrimination is affected by signals of gender and sexual orientation. We sent out 6 654 emails as a job opportunity follow-up which ostensibly reached the wrong recipient, and measured the number of returned emails notifying the sender of the mistake. We find evidence of ethnic discrimination, with Arabs receiving fewer replies than Swedes. No discrimination based on sexual orientation or intersectional effects were found. Study IV had the main aim of testing whether matching stereotype content of job position and intersected group membership (gender and sexual orientation) in terms of warmth and competence affects the job suitability evaluations of job seekers. The results show no evidence of matching effects operating, as the gay job seekers, regardless of job position or gender, receive higher job suitability ratings than their straight counterparts. Further research should address whether there is an ongoing positive attitude change towards gay groups and whether the positive perceptions affect behavioral outcomes. I conclude that intersecting categories generates important knowledge on the perception and treatment of groups at work.
... high in education, job skills or immigration rank), including second-generation immigrants, can be perceived as "ingrouplike" and similar to the rater even when they do not belong to the same group (Grigoryan, 2020). Multiple social cues embedded in the r esum e can induce nuanced impressions of candidates' overall similarity with the rater (Grigoryan, 2020) and higher interpersonal attraction, which may influence hiring decisions (Derous and Ryan, 2019). ...
... Furthermore, respondents' demographics were included in the analysis, which addresses recent calls (e.g. Derous and Ryan, 2019; to examine how raters' characteristics and attributes can influence r esum e screening and recruitment decisions. ...
Article
Purpose Numerous studies have shown that minority workers are disadvantaged in the labour market due to stereotypes and discrimination. However, published research on résumé screening has overlooked the effects of multiple social categorisations pertaining to candidates' gender, education and origin. This study addresses this gap and examines whether the gender, the level of education and the national origin cues mentioned in the résumé affect the perceived employability of candidates. Design/methodology/approach This study employs an experimental between-subjects factorial design in that 12 résumés varying in gender, education and national origin were rated by 373 Portuguese working adults. Findings The results documented a gender premium as women were favoured in interpersonal and job skills but not in job suitability, and an education premium, since higher educated candidates were preferred despite their gender and origin. No meaningful interactions for gender × education × national origin were observed, which suggests that ingroup favouritism and outgroup discrimination in résumé screening can be averted. Originality/value The findings endorse a multidimensional view of perceived employability by investigating candidates' skills and job suitability from the viewpoint of the decision-makers, which extends our understanding of résumé-screening discrimination. This is critical to prevent hiring discrimination at an earlier career stage, which can increase youth employment and enhance the integration in the labour market of local minorities such as women, inexperienced workers and second-generation immigrants.
... Anonymising personal information by removing explicit minority status markers before parsing might help reduce bias, as found in human recruiters (Åslund & Skans, 2012;Derous & Ryan, 2019;Lacroux & Martin-Lacroux, 2019). Nonetheless, LLMs could still pick up on implicit markers in applicant profiles (e.g. ...
Preprint
Large language models offer significant potential for optimising professional activities, such as streamlining personnel selection procedures. However, concerns exist about these models perpetuating systemic biases embedded into their pre-training data. This study explores whether ChatGPT, a chatbot producing human-like responses to language tasks, displays ethnic or gender bias in job applicant screening. Using a correspondence audit approach, I simulated a CV screening task with 34,560 vacancy–CV combinations in which I instructed the chatbot to rate fictitious applicant profiles only differing in names, signalling ethnic and gender identity. Comparing ratings of Arab, Asian, Black American, Central African, Dutch, Eastern European, Hispanic, Turkish, and White American male and female applicants, I show that ethnic and gender identity influence ChatGPT's evaluations. The ethnic bias appears to arise partly from the prompts' language and partly from ethnic identity cues in applicants' names. Although ChatGPT produces no overall gender bias, I find some evidence for a gender-ethnicity interaction effect. These findings underscore the importance of addressing systemic bias in language model-driven applications to ensure equitable treatment across demographic groups. Practitioners aspiring to adopt these tools should practice caution, given the adverse impact they can (re)produce, especially when using them for selection decisions involving humans.
... Para mejorar la eficacia del proceso de selección, para generar un impacto positivo en los solicitantes, para mitigar los sesgos y discriminación en la selección, a continuación proporciono tres recomendaciones generales: Primero, dado los efectos perjudiciales de formular preguntas personales en la selección (reacción negativa, sesgo y discriminación) se sugiere un proceso ciego a determinados criterios, es decir, un procedimiento de selección donde los tomadores de decisiones ignoren las características personales de los solicitantes (Derous & Ryan, 2019). Segundo, en lugar de formular preguntas personales, se insta a utilizar preguntas comportamentales, situacionales o una combinación de ambos en un formato estructurado y basado en el análisis de puesto, dado su comprobada validez, confiabilidad y reducción de impacto adverso contra grupos protegidos (Huffcutt & Youngcourt, 2012;Taylor & Small, 2002). ...
Article
Full-text available
The selection of personnel has proven to be an invaluable procedure for organizations. However, in practice employers often (and prefer) to use the selection methods of scarce and questionable evidence. In the Bolivian context, the use of personal questions related to age, marital status, sex, race, pregnancy, among others, seems to be quite widespread. Although the usefulness that justifies its inclusion remains unproven. In this article, I analyze the relevance and support of personal questions based on their contribution to three purposes of personnel selection: validity to predict job performance, recruitment, and discrimination. Finally, I provide some suggestions: (1) to improve the efficiency of the selection processes, and (2) to amend and strengthen the laws against discrimination in the selection of Bolivian labor law.
... Derous, E., & Ryan, A. M. (2019). When your resume is (not) turning you down: Modelling ethnic bias in resume screening. ...
... Resume screening is an essential part of the recruitment process that involves reviewing resumes and identifying potential candidates who possess the required qualifications and skills for a job. However, this can be a time-consuming and daunting task, especially for large organizations that receive hundreds or thousands of resumes for a single position (Derous & Ryan, 2018). To address this challenge, many companies are turning to AI-powered resume screening tools to automate the process and save time (Vedapradha et al., 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: The aim of this study is to contribute to the understanding of the power of artificial intelligence (AI) in recruitment and to highlight the opportunities and challenges associated with its use. Theoretical framework: This paper provides a comprehensive analytical review of current AI-based recruitment strategies, drawing on both academic research and industry reports. Design/methodology/approach: The paper critically evaluates the potential benefits and drawbacks of using AI in recruitment and assesses the effectiveness of various AI-based recruitment strategies. Findings: The results indicate that AI-based recruitment strategies such as resume screening, candidate matching, video interviewing, chatbots, predictive analytics, gamification, virtual reality assessments, and social media screening offer significant potential benefits for organizations, including improved efficiency, cost savings, and better-quality hires. However, the use of AI in recruitment also raises ethical and legal concerns, including the potential for algorithmic bias and discrimination. Research, Practical & Social implications: The study concludes by emphasizing the need for further research and development to ensure that AI-based recruitment strategies are effective, unbiased, and aligned with ethical and legal standards. Originality/value: The value of the study lies in its comprehensive exploration of AI in recruitment, synthesizing insights from academic and industry perspectives, and assessing the balance of potential benefits against ethical and legal concerns.
Chapter
More than half the population today lives in urban areas. The UN predicts with 80% confidence that the global population will get to between 9.6 billion and 12.3 billion people within the 21st century. Population increase leads to more people in the city. More people in the city translates to new challenges that need addressing. When addressing the new challenges, cities evolve by improving the efficiency of services. Eventually, cities change in both structure and composition. In helping show how the cities have changed, the authors utilized the industrial revolution theory which occurred in stages from the first industrial revolution to Industry 5.0. Just like the industrial revolution, cities evolve in stages with the latter stage using the success of the former stage as building blocks. Smart cities which are characterized by progressive city plans and state-of-the-art infrastructure act as a building block for cognitive cities which are characterized by the ability to have connectivity, and common data architecture for people to share and drive innovation.
Chapter
Workplaces have become increasingly diverse as accessibility, acceptance, migration, and societal expectation have grown – but does this mean that everyone has an equal opportunity to have their skills and qualifications recognised, access to meaningful employment, and career progression opportunities? Through better recognising how experience may influence decision making during recruitment, this study will support the development of tools, techniques, practices, and policies that reduce discrimination, enhance innovation, and ensure workplaces are both welcoming and benefitting from what diversity offers. Using data gathered through a questionnaire, this exploratory study conducted within the Aotearoa New Zealand social service sector, has considered the impacts of work/life experiences and demography on decision making during a recruitment process – focused on characteristics of diversity. Our research shows links between experience and decision making, noting people with more experience of diversity appeared less likely to be influenced by diverse characteristics when making recruitment decisions. It also highlighted the apparent impact of diversity-based training on perceptions of diversity, and where recruitment bias was evident toward people with disability and those who have gathered their experience or qualifications overseas.KeywordsRecruitmentDiversityDecision makingExperienceAotearoa New Zealand
Article
Many professions necessitate a graduate‐level education, and research conducted by graduate students is integral in many fields, particularly those in the life science programs like ecology and environmental sciences. However, practices for recruiting and selecting graduate students are inconsistent among and within institutions. Although some institutions, departments, or faculty members hire graduate students through open and competitive graduate student hiring processes, graduates are frequently selected through inconsistent processes that limit the pool of applicants and do not maximize the potential for increasing workforce diversity. Here, we review and evaluate six approaches to graduate recruitment processes common in ecology and environmental science degree programs in the US to determine which approaches, or combinations of approaches, could increase equity in career development opportunities, promote workforce diversity, and provide clear justifications to funding bodies. We compiled our list of recruitment methods through informal interviews with recruiters, administrators, faculty, and graduate students in ecology, natural resources, and environmental sciences. We determined that three of the six approaches examined were most effective in supporting equitable graduate student hiring practices, and three were not. While life science fields were the primary focus of this review, our approach to evaluating graduate recruitment methods is widely applicable across disciplines where graduate students conduct research. Practical Takeaways There are six main ways graduate students are recruited in the life sciences: competitive recruitment, candidate pool, student‐driven, contact applicants, current employees, and preselected candidates. Three of the common recruitment methods for graduate students are more competitive (e.g., publicly announced, with clear instructions, a review committee or panel). Open and competitive graduate student hiring processes could increase equity in career development opportunities and promote workforce diversity.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose Job-related social networking websites (e.g. LinkedIn) are often used in the recruitment process because the profiles contain valuable information such as education level and work experience. The purpose of this paper is to investigate whether people can accurately infer a profile owner’s self-rated personality traits based on the profile on a job-related social networking site. Design/methodology/approach In two studies, raters inferred personality traits (the Big Five and self-presentation) from LinkedIn profiles (total n=275). The authors related those inferences to self-rated personality by the profile owner to test if the inferences were accurate. Findings Using information gained from a LinkedIn profile allowed for better inferences of extraversion and self-presentation of the profile owner (r’s of 0.24-0.29). Practical implications When using a LinkedIn profile to estimate trait extraversion or self-presentation, one becomes 1.5 times as likely to actually select the person with higher trait extraversion compared to the person with lower trait extraversion. Originality/value Although prior research tested whether profiles of social networking sites (such as Facebook) can be used to accurately infer self-rated personality, this was not yet tested for job-related social networking sites (such as LinkedIn). The results indicate that profiles at job-related social networks, in spite of containing only relatively standardized information, “leak” information about the owner’s personality.
Article
Full-text available
This article draws on an original comparative survey of employers in the UK and Denmark to analyse the role of active labour market programmes (ALMPs) in employers' recruitment of disadvantaged groups. Using the framework of Bonet et al. to conceptualise agencies delivering ALMPs as labour market intermediaries (LMIs), the effect of ALMPs on employers' recruitment was tested against organisational factors involving firm size and selection criteria. Although ALMPs marginally increased employers' probability of recruiting the long-term unemployed in both countries and lone parents in Denmark, their effect was negligible compared with firm size and employers' selection criteria. While ALMP agencies have the potential to increase employers' recruitment of disadvantaged groups, this is constrained when they act as basic ‘information provider’ LMIs. ALMP agencies' inability to act effectively as ‘matchmaker’ LMIs leads to a failure to overcome rigid intra-organisational barriers to such recruitment.
Article
Full-text available
Systematic research considering job context as affecting ethnic discrimination in hiring is limited. Building on contemporary literature on social categorization and cognitive matching, the interactive effect of context characteristics (client contact; industry status) and person characteristics (i.e. ethnic cues: Maghreb/Arab vs Flemish-sounding name; dark vs light skin tone) were investigated using an experimental field study among 424 white majority HR professionals. Findings showed that equally qualified applicants with a dark skin tone received lower job suitability ratings than applicants with a light skin tone, particularly when they were screened for high client contact/low industry status positions and low client contact/high industry status positions. It is concluded that some ethnic cues (such as skin tone) may be more salient compared with other cues and that job context may influence the salience of ethnic cues and steer hiring discrimination in subtle ways. Implications of these findings for hiring discrimination research and organizations are discussed.
Chapter
This chapter discusses perspectives and empirical findings on ethnic discrimination during the resume screening phase. First, the labor market position of ethnic minorities is discussed and two prominent hypotheses on the disadvantaged labor market position of ethnic minorities are presented, namely the human capital versus hiring discrimination hypothesis. Second, several theoretical perspectives are discussed that illustrate why resume screening might be vulnerable to biased decision making. Third, we turn our attention to influences on hiring discrimination that reside in the applicant, the job/organization, and recruiter. Finally, this chapter ends with a critical reflection on some practical recommendations (such as anonymous resume screening) and future research directions on hiring discrimination during resume screening, including new technologies such as video resumes.
Article
Targeted recruitment strategies aim to communicate job vacancies to specific groups of job seekers that organizations wish to attract, such as qualified ethnic minorities. Typically, these strategies do not consider how person requirements are communicated in job advertisements and how job seekers from different ethnic groups react to such requirements. Two field experiments among actual job seekers investigated whether the type of required trait and the wording of traits affected ethnic minorities’ job attraction (Study 1, N = 140) and qualified ethnic minorities’ and majorities’ decision to apply (Study 2, N = 130). Findings show that ethnic minority job seekers were less attracted to job ads targeting a trait they have negative meta-stereotypes about. Wording of traits did not moderate this effect. However, ethnic minority job seekers who were qualified for a negatively meta-stereotyped trait decided not to apply when that trait was worded in a dispositional (vs. a behavioral) way.
Article
Greenwald, Banaji and Nosek (2015) present a reanalysis of the meta-analysis by Oswald, Mitchell, Blanton, Jaccard and Tetlock (2013) that examined the effect sizes of Implicit Association Tests designed to predict racial and ethnic discrimination. We discuss points of agreement and disagreement with respect to methods used to synthesize the IAT studies, and we correct an error by Greenwald et al. that obscures a key contribution of our meta-analysis. In the end, all of the meta-analyses converge on the conclusion that, across diverse methods of coding and analyzing the data, IAT scores are not good predictors of ethnic or racial discrimination and explain, at most, small fractions of the variance in discriminatory behavior in controlled laboratory settings. The thought experiments presented by Greenwald et al. go well beyond the lab to claim systematic IAT effects in noisy real-world settings, but these hypothetical exercises depend crucially on untested and arguably untenable assumptions.