ArticlePDF Available

Behavioral and cortisol responses of shelter dogs to a cognitive bias test after olfactory enrichment with essential oils

Authors:
  • Odisee University College

Abstract and Figures

A shelter environment tends to present different types of stressors dogs need to cope with. Recent work has shown that olfactory enrichment with essential oils might be able to modify the affective states of certain species (dogs, cats, horses, zoo animals…). In these studies, the welfare measurements included physiological indicators, such as corticosteroid levels, and/or behaviors related to chronic stress. The olfactory effects of 9 essential oils (Cananga od-orata,Cistus ladaniferus, Citrus aurantium, Cupressus sempervirens, Juniperus communis var. montana, Lavandula an-gustifolia, Laurus nobilis, Litsea citrata, Pelargonium graveolens) and a blend of these oils were explored on a cognitive bias test, cortisol levels and the behaviors of 110 shelter dogs (n = 10 dogs within each group). Olfactory enrichment with the blend resulted in a reduced latency to the ambiguous cue, indicating a more optimistic bias and improved welfare. The results of this study suggest that olfactory enrichment with essential oils can have specific effects on the affective states and behaviors of shelter dogs, and could therefore be useful for shelter management. In addition, as not all of the essential oils tested individually were effective, more research should be conducted to better understand the effects of each individual essential oils on dogs.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Behavioral and cortisol responses of shelter dogs
to a cognitive bias test after olfactory enrichment
with essential oils
Stefania Uccheddu1, Chiara Mariti2, Adinda Sannen3, Hilde Vervaecke3,
Heidi Arnout3,4, Jara Gutierrez Rufo2, Angelo Gazzano2, Anouck Haverbeke1,*
1 Vet Ethology, Leemveldstraat44, 3090 Overijse, Belgium
2 Department of Veterinary Sciences, University of Pisa, Viale delle Piagge 2, 56124 Pisa, Italy
3 Odisee University College, Agro- & Biotechnology, Ethology & Animal Welfare, Hospitaalstraat 23,
9100 Sint-Niklaas, Belgium
4 University of Antwerp, Departmentof Engineering Management, Prinsstraat 13, 2000 Antwerp, Belgium
Abstract: A shelter environment tends to present dierent types of stressors dogs need to cope with. Recent work
has shown that olfactory enrichment with essential oils might be able to modify the aective states of certain species
(dogs, cats, horses, zoo animals…). In these studies, the welfare measurements included physiological indicators, such
as corticosteroid levels, and/or behaviors related to chronic stress. e olfactory eects of 9 essential oils (Cananga od-
orata,Cistus ladaniferus, Citrus aurantium, Cupressus sempervirens, Juniperus communis var. montana, Lavandula an-
gustifolia, Laurus nobilis, Litsea citrata, Pelargonium graveolens) and a blend of these oils were explored on a cognitive
bias test, cortisol levels and the behaviors of 110 shelter dogs (n = 10 dogs within each group). Olfactory enrichment
with the blend resulted in a reduced latency to the ambiguous cue, indicating a more optimistic bias and improved
welfare. e results of this study suggest that olfactory enrichment with essential oils can have specic eects on the
aective states and behaviors of shelter dogs, and could therefore be useful for shelter management. In addition, as not
all of the essential oils tested individually were eective, more research should be conducted to better understand the
eects of each individual essential oils on dogs.
Key Words: behavior; cognitive bias; cortisol; dogs; essential oils; olfactory enrichment; shelter; welfare.
* Corresponding Author: anouck.haverbeke@vetethology.be
Introduction
A shelter environment tends to present dierent types of stressors dogs need to cope with: social
stressors (reduced intraspecic and/or interspecic social contacts), environmental stressors
(restraint for medical procedures, separation from a caretaker or handler) or psychogenic stressors
(separation anxiety, use of aversive training methods by a previous owner/lack of ethological
knowledge in caretakers). Moreover, stressors are known to cause activation of metabolic and
endocrine responses in sheltered animals (Titulaer et al., 2013).
Recent work has shown that essential oils might be able to modify the aective states of
certain species (dogs, cats, horses and zoo animals: Wells, 2004; Graham et al., 2005; Ferguson
et al., 2013; Wells & Egli, 2015; Binks et al., 2018). In these studies, the welfare measurements
included physiological indicators, such as corticosteroid levels (Beerda et al., 1998) or behaviors
related to chronic stress, such as repetitive behaviors, nosing, paw-liing, increased locomotion,
displacement behavior or excessive drinking (Beerda et al., 1998; Haverbeke et al., 2008).
However, interpretation of these indicators can be dicult (Titulaer et al., 2013). erefore
the detection of a cognitive bias might be a complementary solution. A recent and innovative
approach utilizes the inuence of aective states on the interpretation of current experience.
e resulting aect-induced cognitive biases can be measured (Mendl et al., 2009) through
cognitive bias tests as indicators of the animal’s psychological well-being (Mendl et al., 2009; Paul
et al., 2005). A cognitive bias test in this context refers to the propensity of a subject to show
Dog Behavior, 2-2018, pp. 1-14
doi 10.4454/db.v4i2.87
Submitted, 07/29/2018
Accepted, 09/12/2018
behavior indicating the anticipation of either relatively positive or relatively negative outcomes in
response to aectively ambiguous stimuli (Mendl et al., 2009). Changes in cognitive bias reect an
individual’s experience of positive and negative events and thus its aective valence and welfare
(Mendl et al., 2010). e eects of environmental enrichment have been already tested through
cognitive bias test in dierent species such as rats (Brydges et al., 2011), pigs (Douglas et al., 2012)
and European starling (Bateson & Matheson, 2007).
Several studies have found correlations between cognitive biases and aective states in a wide
range of species, including mammals (Mendl et al., 2009; Doyle et al., 2010) and birds (Matheson
et al., 2008; Salmeto et al., 2011). e aim of the current study was to assess whether olfactory
enrichment through essential oils inuences the aective states of sheltered dogs. To do that, the
possible eects of 9 dierent of essential oils (Cananga odorata, Cistus ladaniferus, Citrus auran-
tium, Cupressus sempervirens, Juniperus communis var. montana, Lavandula angustifolia, Laurus
nobilis, Litsea citrata, Pelargonium graveolens) and a blend of these oils on a cognitive bias test,
cortisol levels and behavior of 110 shelter dogs were explored.
Materials and Methods
Participants
One hundred ten dogs ranging in age from 1 to 10 years, of both sexes, and of either pure or
mixed breed, were enrolled in the study and randomly allocated to one of 11 dierent groups
(Table 1). e dogs lived in groups of three in kennels with an indoor section measuring 1.5
meters x1.5 meters and an outdoor run measuring 1.5 meters x 2 meters, joined by a metal door
operated by sta. Water was available ad libitum, and food was provided twice per day, at 8 am
and 6 pm.
Dogs were selected based on the following criteria: (a) success at the training phase, (b) no
previous diagnosis of anxiety or aggressive behavior, (c) some socialization prerequisites, such as
the ability to deal with people without fear, (d) the veterinarians agreement and (e) ability to walk
on leash (f) good medical health.
Table 1. Description of the study protocol.
Group Number
of dogs
Pre-test
training Cognitive test 1 Exposure to collar
for 3 hours Cognitive test 2
1 10 Ye s
Before exposure to collar
and aer collection of
saliva at T0
No addition
(control group)
Aer exposure to collar and aer
collection of saliva at T1
2 10 Ye s
Before exposure to collar
and aer collection of
saliva at T0
e blend Aer exposure to collar and aer
collection of saliva at T1
3 10 Ye s
Before exposure to collar
and aer collection of
saliva at T0
Litsea citrata Aer exposure to collar and aer
collection of saliva at T1
4 10 Ye s
Before exposure to collar
and aer collection of
saliva at T0
Cupressus
sempervirens
Aer exposure to collar and aer
collection of saliva at T1
5 10 Ye s
Before exposure to collar
and aer collection of
saliva at T0
Citrus aurantium Aer exposure to collar and aer
collection of saliva at T1
6 10 Ye s
Before exposure to collar
and aer collection of
saliva at T0
Pelargonium
graveolens
Aer exposure to collar and aer
collection of saliva at T1
2
3
7 10 Ye s
Before exposure to collar
and aer collection of
saliva at T0
Lavandula
angustifolia
Aer exposure to collar and aer
collection of saliva at T1
8 10 Ye s
Before exposure to collar
and aer collection of
saliva at T0
Cananga odorata Aer exposure to collar and aer
collection of saliva at T1
9 10 Ye s
Before exposure to collar
and aer collection of
saliva at T0
Juniperus
communis var.
Montana
Aer exposure to collar and aer
collection of saliva at T1
10 10 Ye s
Before exposure to collar
and aer collection of
saliva at T0
Cistus ladaniferus Aer exposure to collar and aer
collection of saliva at T1
11 10 Ye s
Before exposure to collar
and aer collection of
saliva at T0
Laurus nobilis Aer exposure to collar and aer
collection of saliva at T1
Olfactory enrichment
A blend of 9 essential oils (Arhomani, Belgium) and each separate oil of the blend (Flora s.r.l.,
Pisa, Italy) were tested, for a total of 10 treatments and a control group (Table 1). Essential oils were
diused through a cotton collar worn by the dogs for 3 hours before starting the second cognitive
test procedure (see below). e collar, just before being applied to the dog, had 1 drop of an indi-
vidual oil or of the blend added to it. e control group, as the experimental groups, wore a cotton
collar for 3 hours but without any oils or other addition.
During this part of the experiment, the dogs stayed in the pen where they routinely spent time.
Dogs were allowed to mix in the same pen only if they were allocated to the same essential oil
group. In order to avoid odour contamination, there was a distance of 500m between the dierent
pens.
Test protocol
Cognitive test
All dogs of the 11 groups were subjected to two cognitive bias (CB) tests (modied from
(Mendl et al., 2010), one prior to (CB 1) and one aer essential oil exposure (CB 2). CB 2 was
performed 3 hours aer T0. To avoid more stress caused by a dierent routine in the shelter,
we could not control for order eects. All the dogs were tested on the second cognitive bias test
following olfactory enrichment.e protocol of (Mendl et al., 2010) and (Owczarczak-Garstecka
& Burman, 2016) was modied in this study based on a pilot study we carried out, in which we
observed that dogs in the shelter were unable to maintain attention during the original cognitive
test as proposed in (Mendl et al., 2010) and (Owczarczak-Garstecka & Burman, 2016). For this
reason, we used a shortened version. During the training session, all dogs received a minimum
of 8 training trials instead of 15. During the test, we used just one ambiguous location instead of
three ambiguous locations. e test phase involved 6 trials (instead of the 32 proposed by (Mendl
et al., 2010).
In addition, during the pilot study, we realized that shelter dogs were much more interested
in humans than in food, so we changed the original protocol by having the researcher behind the
camera rather than behind the bowl put on the ground, in order to avoid the dog choosing that
bowl for its closeness with a person (the researcher).
Training and cognitive tests were performed with each dog enrolled in the experiment
individually led to a test area (6 meters x 6 meters) within the shelter, the same for all sessions and
all dogs. e setting is described in gure 1. e bowl was placed at one of three predetermined
locations (two during the training) 4 meters in front of the dog’s xed starting position. e
4
latency to reach the bowl, was dened as the time elapsed between release from the lead and the
dog putting its head into the bowl, or touching the rim of the bowl with its nose (Mendl et al.,
2010). CB tests were video recorded and then analysed as described below.
Figure 1. Experimental setting.
Training
Dogs were rst trained to associate a certain location with a reward. During the training, the
distance between the two bowls (Positive and Negative) was 1meter.When the bowl was placed at
the ‘positive’ location (P) on one side of the test area, it contained food, and when it was placed
at the ‘negative’ location (N) on the opposite side of the test area, it was empty. Two visually
identical bowls were used for rewarded (P) and non-rewarded (N) locations, and both bowls had
a piece of food taped to their bottom sides that were inaccessible to the dogs to control for odour
cues. Training was complete when the dogs reached a pre-set criterion, that is, when the dog ran
to the positive location faster than to the negative one twice consecutively.
Each training session started aer a 10-minute period of habituation with the researchers in the
experimental area (Figure 1). e dog was put on a lead and held by one of the researchers behind
a barrier, while the other researcher stood at the far end of the room and baited (or did not bait,
depending on trial type) a food bowl with 50 gr of commercial dog food. e dog was released
to approach the bowl. Each dog received at least 8 training trials conducted so that no location
was repeated more than twice. Each training session started with two positive (rewarded) trials
to encourage participation, followed by two negative (non-rewarded) trials. e remaining trials
were randomly assigned to be rewarded or non-rewarded. e latency to reach the bowl, dened
as the time elapsed between release from the lead and the dog putting its head into the bowl or
touching the rim of the bowl with its nose, was recorded for each trial using a stopwatch. e
maximum time allowed per trial was 30 seconds. If the dog did not reach the food bowl within
that time, the maximum time was scored.
5
Test
When the training was completed, the test started. Each dog was presented a food bowl in
three locations, positive (P), negative (N) and intermediate (M).e Middle bowl was located
between Positive and Negative bowl. e distance between Positive (or Negative) and Middle
Bowl was 50 cm.e bowl was presented in each location twice (P1, M1, N1 and P2, M2, N2) but
in dierent order. e accessible food was only present in the positive location (P). Negative (N)
and Intermediate (M) locations remained empty but with olfactory control cues. All the tests were
videorecorded. Since in each CB the locations were tested twice, we used mean values for each
location in CB 1 and in CB 2 in further analyses.
Behavioral observations
e observations of dog behaviors were carried out on the videos recorded during the whole
test. Each dog was observed using a continuous sampling method.
e behavioral analysis was conducted using the ethogram reported in Table 2 and 3 (Haverbeke
et al., 2008). Depending on the type of behavior, either the duration (in seconds) or the number
of occurrences was recorded.
Table 2. Behaviors scored in terms of number of occurrences.
Behavior Description
Oral behaviors:
Yawning Mouth open to apparent fullest extent while eyes are closed
Non-directed licking
Snout licking
Tongue out, the tip of the tongue is briey extended
Part of the tongue is shown and moved along the upper lip
No oral behaviors
Paw liing Fore paw lied into a position of approximately 45°
Urinatingsquat Urinating by squatting while keeping both hind limbs on cage oor
Urinating, limbraised Urinating while raising one hind limb
Defecating Excreting the contents of the bowels
Table 3. Behaviors analysed in terms of duration (seconds).
Behaviors
Repetitive or stereotypicbehavior
Pacing Immediately repeating a path just taken and continuing in the repetition in circles, in a
gure eight pattern or fence/wall-line running
Circling Continuous walking in short circles, apparently chasing its tail or hind limbs
Other behaviors Manipulating environment (Stereotypic interactions with elements from the
environment, such as digging (scratching the oor with the forepaws in a way that is
similar to how dogs dig holes), oor licking (licking the oor with the tongue)), Auto
grooming
Notseen Unable to determine behavior of the dog owing to darkness or the position of the dog
Miscellaneousoralbehaviors
Barking loud, rough noise
Roaring loud, deep sound
Growling low, rough sound
Whining long, high sound
Yelping sudden, short, high sound
Panting Increased frequency of inhalation and exhalation oen in combination with the opening
of the mouth
Teethclapping Making short loud noise by hitting teeth together
6
Notseen Unable to determine the behavior of the dog owing to darkness or the position of the
dog
Locomotive states
Prone, head down Trunk of body on oor, chin or side of head in contact with the oor, paws or limbs
Prone, head up Trunk of body on the oor, no part of the head in contact with the paws
Sitting Only hindquarters and front paws in contact with the oor
Standing Upright with at least three paws in contact with the oor without any walk
Walking Takes at least one step, shiing body position
Highly active Any motion across oor faster than a walk, including trotting and jumping
Changing from one state of locomotion to another
Notseen Unable to determine behavior of dog owing to darkness or the position of the dog
Postures
High e breed specic posture as shown by dogs under neutral conditions, but in addition
the tail is positioned higher or the position of the head is elevated, and the ears are
pointed forwards, or the animal is standing extremely erect
Neutral e breed posture shown by dogs under neutral conditions
Halow Two or more of the following three features are displayed: a lowered position of the tail
(compared to the neutral posture), a backward position of the ears and bent legs
Low e position of the tail is lowered, the ears are positioned backwards, and the legs are
bent
Verylow Low posture, but now the tail is curled forward between the hind legs
Notseen Unable to determine the behavior of the dog owing to darkness or the position of the
dog
Cortisol
Saliva samples for the assessment of plasma cortisol concentrations were collected, at the same
time in the day, before the addition of the oils at T0 (to identify the basal cortisol levels) and at T1,
i.e. aer 3 hours exposure to the collar for all groups, including the control one. Collection was
always carried out before the cognitive bias tests at T0 and T1. Saliva samples were collected using
Salivette Cortisol code blue(Sarstedt, Nümbrecht, Germany) and stored at-20 °C until they were
further processed using a commercial ELISA kit (Diametra, Milano, Italy).
Statistical analysis
e statistical analysis was performed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 22.0
(Armonk, NY: IBM Corp). For each of the oils under study, the dierence in the variables
measured before and aer exposure was tested using a Wilcoxon signed-rank test. is paired
dierence test was used because each subject is measured twice, resulting inpairsof observations.
is reduces the eect of confounders like individual dierences (e.g. in pace length or in interest
in food) between dogs.e test statistics (sum of positive ranks) as well as the two-sided p-values
are reported in the results below. P values ≤ 0.05 were deemed statistically signicant.
We additionally tested for T0 as well as T1whether the dogs’ responses during the cognitive
bias tests were appropriate (i.e. dogs were slower to approach the ‘negative’ location N when
compared to the ‘positive’ location P) by using a one-sided paired t-test comparing latency to
approach N versus latency to approach P. Statistical p values ≤ 0.05 were deemed statistically
signicant.
7
Results
Cognitive test
We explored the dogs’ latency to approach P and N, just to make sure dogs’ response to the CB
test 1 (before exposure) and CB test 2 (aer exposure) was appropriate (i.e. animals were slower
to approach N than P). e results are reported in Table 4.
Table 4. Statistical results of the comparison between latency to reach positive and negative locations be-
fore exposure and after exposure to essential oils (CB 1: Cognitive test before exposure; CB 2: Cognitive test
after exposure).
Mean (seconds) N Standard. Deviation Standard Error Mean
CB 1 Latency before
exposure P location 19.32 110 17.01 1.62
Latency before
exposure N location 24.18 110 27.28 2.60
CB 2 Latency aer
exposure P location 2.64 110 0.48 0.05
Latency aer
exposure N location 13.81 110 13.53 1.29
e analysis revealed a significant eect of the blend “e blend” in reducing the latency
to reach the intermediate position (test statistic=3; n=10; p=0.039). We also observed a trend
towards reducing the latency to reach the intermediate position (test statistic=5; n=10; p=0.078)
for Litsea citrata oil (Table 5).
Table 5. Latency (mean ± Standard Deviation in seconds before and after 3 hours of exposure) and cortisol
values (mean ± Standard Deviation in ng/ml before and after 3 hours of exposure) to each essential oil or
after 3 hours without any exposure in the control group (P < 0.05, *).
Before
exposure
Aer 3 hours
of exposure
(T1)
Statistical
results
Before
exposure
Aer 3 hours
of exposure
(T1)
Statistical
results
Latency value
(seconds)
Cortisol value
(ng/ml)
Control group
(no exposure) 20.60±11.00 15.95±10.98 P=0.38 2.406 ± 0.30* 1.762 ± 0.435 P=0.03*
Cananga odorata 18.65±7.84 16.92±9.35 P=0.84 1.923 ± 0.70 1.512 ± 0.111 P=0.08
Cistus
ladaniferus 18.77±11.78 14.98±11.93 P=0.54 1.538 ± 0.22 1.424 ± 0.132 P=0.18
Citrus aurantium 17.22±12.48 11.21±11.98 P=0.35 1.642 ± 0.21 1.507 ± 0.196 P=0.43
Cupres
sussempervirens 24.47±7.14 18.46±11.32 P=0.19 1.766 ± 0.58 2.175 ± 0.424 P=0.12
Juniperus
communis var.
Montana
21.93±9.30 14.06±13.71 P=0.20 1.397 ± 0.30 1.497 ± 0.364 P=0.74
Laurus nobilis 20.80 ±11.35 15.45±9.59 P=0.10 1.082 ± 0.45 1.435 ± 0.198 P=0.14
Lavandula
angustifolia 22.19±9.60 16.70±14.08 P=0.29 1.821 ± 0.39*1.549 ± 0.245 P=0.03*
Litsea citrate 21.97±9.34 14.70±10.39 P=0.078 1.467 ± 0.30 1.919 ± 0.313 P=0.078
Pelargonium
graveolens 20.74±9.58 15.48±10.70 P=0.10 1.287 ± 0.33 1.596 ± 0.504 P=0.10
e blend 23.83±9.80 13.46±11.28 P=0.039* 1.557 ± 0.49 1.316 ± 0.119 P=0.25
8
Behavioral observations
Only the olfactory enrichment with Laurus nobilis induced a signicantly longer duration of
high posture among these dogs (test statistic=26.5; n=10; p=0.047).
e analysis revealed non-signicant trends for dierent oils: Cananga odorata reduced the
“nosing” time (test statistic=9; n=10; p=0.064), Citrus aurantium (test statistic=46; n=10; p=0.064)
and Cupressus sempervirens (test statistic=39; n=10; p=0.055) increased the time spent in “tail
wagging”, and Pelargonium graveolens (test statistic=3; n=10; p=0.078) reduced the time spent
in “non-oral stress behaviors” (circling, pacing, manipulation of environment, autogrooming).
Cortisol
Olfactory enrichment with Lavandula angustifolia induced a signicant reduction in saliva
cortisol levels (test statistic=3; n=8; p=0.039). A similar signicant reduction was also found in the
control group (test statistic= 0; n=6; p=0.031) (Table 5).
Discussion
Cognitive test
In the present study, authors applied a cognitive test to evaluate the eectiveness of olfactory
enrichment with essential oils in reducing the level of stress in sheltered dogs. Olfactory enrichment
with the blend of oils resulted in a reduced latency to the ambiguous cue in the cognitive test,
indicating a more optimistic bias and, consequently, an improved welfare (Mendl et al., 2010).
ese results provide support for the idea that the interactions between compounds oen result in
biological activity that is greater than the activity of the isolated compounds(Galindo et al., 2010).
Many domestic dogs are kept in rescue and rehoming shelters which are frequently stressful
and impoverished environments. Dog’s welfare is oen compromised within these environments
and there is a need to determine new practical and eective methods to improve the welfare
of these kenneled dogs (Binks et al., 2018).e development of objective methods to assess the
aective states of non-human animals is a crucial step in improving animal welfare (e.g. Dawkins,
2008.). Mendl (Mendl et al., 2009) enumerated several potential advantages of the cognitive bias
test, including the ability to make a priori predictions for dierent species: mammals (Mendl
et al., 2009; Doyle et al., 2010), birds (Matheson et al., 2008; Salmeto et al., 2011) and insects
(Salmeto et al., 2011). Douglas et al. (2012) support the hypothesis that an enriched environment
induces a more optimistic cognitive bias indicative of a more positive aective state and better
welfare in pigs.
Negative eects from inadequate environmental manipulations have been investigated by
several researchers. Environmental manipulations chosen to induce negative eect produce
pessimistic cognitive biases in animals’ responses to ambiguous stimuli (reviewed in (Mendl et
al., 2009)). Rats show pessimistic responses when housed in impoverished cages but switch to
optimistic responses when moved to enriched cages (Brydges et al., 2011).
To our knowledge, this is the rst time that a cognitive bias test has been applied to assess the
eect of olfactory enrichment with essential oils. Although this is a rst study on this topic and
the number of dogs tested in each experimental group was relatively low, it is remarkable that
the statistical analysis revealed some signicant dierences. In particular, the results regarding
the blend of oils are in line with previous studies that reported improved optimism through
environmental enrichment (Douglas et al., 2012) in pigs.
However, we should take into account that the medium latency for reaching the positive location
in the cognitive bias test 2 (P2- aer exposure) is signicantly lower than the medium latency
9
for reaching the positive location in test 1 (P1- before exposure). So although the dogs respond
appropriate to each CB-test (i.e. approaching N signicantly slower than P), they approach both
P and N signicantly faster during CB2 than during CB1, which might suggest some eagerness to
participate in the test.
Behavioral observations
e results of the present study indicate that olfactory enrichment with Laurus nobilis induced
high posture among dogs. In volatiles, Laurus nobilis has been reported to improve vigilance
performance in a discrimination task (Matsubara et al., 2011), which could be interpreted
positively as increased self-condence (Fatjó et al., 2007) or alternatively as a heightened alertness
due to a negative state. In humans, a high individual variability in response to olfactory exposure
to 1,8-cineol (major component of Laurus nobilis), jasmine absolute ether, linalyl acetate and
peppermint essential oil has been observed on vigilance (Heuberger & Ilmberger, 2010). In the
present study, dog’s high posture aer olfactory exposure with Laurus nobilisis not accompanied
by other signs of alertness and can therefore be interpreted as a sign of improved self-condence
in line with Haverbeke et al. (2008).
Some essential oils had a slight eect on behavior. Olfactory enrichment with Cananga odorata
reduced the “nosing” time. is could indicate a decrease of stress among the dogs (Beerda et al.,
1998). In line with these interpretations, Hongratanaworakit and Buchbauer (2004) showed that
in humans Cananga odorata decreases blood pressure and pulse rate and increases subjective
attentiveness and alertness. Olfactory enrichment with Citrus aurantium and Cupressus
sempervirens increased the time spent in “tail wagging”. Tail wagging can be seen in the interactive
social context or to facilitate interaction and could have ambivalent interpretations going from an
increase to a decrease in condence in dogs (Gasci et al., 2005). In the present study, the exposure
of dogs to Citrus aurantium and Cupressus sempervirens are not accompanied by other changes.
erefore it is likely that in this study tail wagging is a sign of relaxation. is is in line with
previous results that have demonstrated anti-anxiety eects of both Citrus aurantium (in humans
(Akhlaghi et al., 2011; Carvalho-Freitas et al., 2002; De Moraes Pultrini et al., 2006) and rats (Leite
et al., 2008) and Cupressus sempervirens(in humans Bouguenoun et al., 2006).
Olfactory enrichment with Pelargonium graveolens reduced the time spent in “non-oral
stress behaviors” (circling, pacing, manipulation of environment, autogrooming). is observed
reduction of stress behaviors (Haverbeke et al., 2008) is in line with the ndings of Rashidi Fakari
et al., 2015, who observed an anxiolytic and sedative eect of Pelargonium graveolens in humans.
Cortisol
e observed reduction in saliva cortisol with Lavandula angustifoliais in line with Atsumi
& Tonosaki who have observed a decrease of salivary cortisol level on humans aer smelling
lavender essential oil (Atsumi & Tonosaki, 2007). In addition, a previous study using olfactory
enrichment with Lavandula angustifolia on sheltered dogs showed a change in dogs’ activities
(resting time) suggestive of relaxation (Graham et al., 2005).
We also observed a reduction of cortisol levels in the control group. is nding is in line with
previous research in dogs (Shiverdecker et al., 2013; Cobb et al., 2016). One possible explanation
is that, the mere application of a cognitive test can result in a stress relieving factor, being a sort
of cognitive enrichment for sheltered dogs. However, this does not explain why the cognitive test
with essential oil exposure had no eect on cortisol levels except in the Lavandula angustifolia
group. Another explanation is that essential oils (except Lavandula angustifolia) has increased
neophobia (i.e. the fear of novelty, which can be sometimes observed in captive animals that
have received little or no previous novel sensory stimulation (Mason et al., 1991) as observed in
Goeldi’s monkeys exposed to peppermint oil (Boon, 2003) and in a young tiger exposed to catnip
10
(Todd, 2015). However, as dogs belong to a species who tends to be very neophilic (Kaulfuss
& Mills, 2008) this explanation might probably not be considered for the canine species.As the
interpretation remains open, further studies are required in order to demonstrate through a more
detailed and rigorous analysis the eects of Lavandula angustifolia essential oil on cortisol levels
versus the eects of the other essential oils.
Although saliva collection was carried out at dierent times of the day, it is unlikely that
the dierences we observed were inuenced by this. In fact, previous research has not found
a circadian rhythm in the HPA (Hypothalamic Pituitary Adrenal) activity of dogs: neither in
laboratory dogs at 30 minutes intervals over a period of 28 hours (Takahashi et al., 1981) nor
at 20 minutes intervals over a period of 25 hours (Kemppainen & Sartin, 1994), nor in working
dogs exposed to defense training and trailing tasks at 90-180 minutes intervals over a period of 24
hours (Kolevska et al., 2003).
e saliva cortisol collected aer T1 could not have reected an earlier emotional state (pre-
olfactory enrichment), because cortisol concentrations rise approximately 20 minutes aer a dog
encounters a stressor (Vincent & Michell, 1992). Moreover, previous authors (Kirschbaum &
Hellhammer, 2000) have shown that changes in plasma and salivary cortisol levels are closely
synchronized: aer injections of cortisol, salivary levels increase within 1 minute and peak
concentrations in blood are seen 2-3 minutes later in saliva.
Some methodological limitations have been encountered during this study. Firstly, we used
a short version of the cognitive bias test because the sheltered dogs were not able to perform
the longer test (author’s observations in an unpublished, pilot study). eir limited performance
might be due to the fact that these dogs were not accustomed to be involved in cognitive activities
in their actual environment (presence of physical and social stimuli).
Secondly, being a study carried out in the eld and not in a laboratory setting, many factors
could not be controlled. For instance, there is a potential risk of olfactory confounding eects.
However, in order to reduce the risk of crossed stimulation among dierent essential oils groups,
a distance of 500 meters from one pen to another was set. Further, in a shelter environment the
quantity of olfactory stimuli is high and similar for all dogs. us a possible eect of odours other
than essential oils should be equally distributed for all dogs, which is not the case in the present
study. Lastly, even if we might consider any olfactory confounding eect, the main olfactory
eect should still remain the one obtained by the tested essential oil as it is the most proximate
odour from the dog’s nose. In order to conrm our results, further research should investigate the
maximum or minimum distance necessary to create an olfactory eect with essential oils.
irdly, the ndings should be interpreted with caution because it is possible that the dogs’
behavior was inuenced by a learning eect and a decreased interest because the cognitive bias
test was repeated twice. However, each time that a cognitive bias test is being used, information
processing, including attention, learning, memory and decision-making is being addressed
(Mendl et al., 2009).
Fourthly, in our study all the dogs were tested on the second cognitive bias test following
olfactory enrichment. Unfortunately, we could not control for any order eects, because a dierent
protocol would have caused changes in the shelter routine and therefore additional stress for the
shelter dogs and the sta. Nevertheless, as the order was the same for the tested dogs, the results
of a potential order eect should be the same for all groups. e dierent ndings observed in sub
samples suggest that essential oils have dierent eects: this could be a combination of essential
oils’ stimulation and repetition of the test. Further research should investigate the eects of
single essential oils in dierent conditions.e tendencies or signicant decreases that are found
in dierent behaviors in various groups could be caused by an increase in condence the dogs
experienced in the second CB (they were familiar with the CB and might have been eager to
11
participate and enjoy human contact or the enrichment).If a design would be applied in which
50% of the dogs start with essential oils (group 1) and 50% of the dogs without essential oils
(group 2), it is not possible to conduct the control-CB within the same day as the essential oil-
CB for the rst group. It is quite likely that aer 3 hours of exposure to essential oils, an eect of
essential oils would still be present during the control-CB. Conducting the control-CB at another
day would generate a confounding eect of day.
Lastly, we should take into account that Galindo (Galindo et al., 2010) armed that eects
of essential oils can vary considerably depending on the dosage. In our study, we used the same
dosage for each oil. Further studies will need to focus on the eects which obtained by diusing
dierent concentrations of essential oils.
Conclusions
ese preliminary results suggest that olfactory enrichment with essential oils can inuence the
aective states and behaviors of shelter dogs. More research is needed to understand the impact of
each individual essential oil and its eect on dog’s welfare, considering possible factors aecting
their inuence, including individual factors or dierent concentrations of the essential oils.
Acknowledgments
We would rstly like to thank AHVMA for having supported this research. anks to Giorgia
Ascheri and the sta of the shelter Shardana (Sardinia, Italy) for their help during data collection.
We appreciate the contribution of Stijn Schoelynck during the data analysis.
Author Contributions
e idea for the paper was conceived by Haverbeke A. and Uccheddu S. e experimental
protocol was designed by Uccheddu S., Haverbeke A. and Mariti C. e data were statistically
analysed by Arnouts H. and Sannen A. and discussed by all authors. e videos were analysed by
Gutierrez Rufo J. e cortisol concentration in the saliva was analysed by Mariti C. and Gazzano
A.. e paper was written by Uccheddu S. and Haverbeke A. and discussed by all authors.
Conicts of Interest
ere could be a potential conict of interest because Haverbeke A. has selected the composition
of the oils of the blend. e funding sponsors had no role in the design of the study; in the
collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, and in the decision
to publish the results.
References
Akhlaghi M., Shabanian G., Raeian-Kopaei M., Parvin N., Saadat M., Akhlaghi M. Flor de Citrus au-
rantium e ansiedade pré-operatória. Rev. Bras. Anestesiol. 2011; 61: 707-712. https://doi.org/10.1590/
S0034-70942011000600002.
Atsumi T., Tonosaki K. Smelling lavender and rosemary increases free radical scavenging activity and
decreases cortisol level in saliva. Psychiatry Res. 2007; 150: 89-96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psy-
chres.2005.12.012.
Bateson M., Matheson S. Performance on a categorisation task suggests that removal of environmental
enrichment induces “pessimism” in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Anim. Welf. 2007;
16: 33-36.
12
Beerda B., Schilder M.B., van Hoo J.A.R.A., de Vries H.W., Mol J.A., Behavioural, saliva cortisol and
heart rate responses to dierent types of stimuli in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1998; 58: 365-381.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(97)00145-7.
Binks J., Taylor S., Wills A., Montrose V.T. The behavioural effects of olfactory stimulation on
dogs at a rescue shelter. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2018; 202: 69-76. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.AP-
PLANIM.2018.01.009.
Boon M. 2003. Goeldi’s monkeys (Callimico goeldii): olfactory enrichment to stimulate natural behaviour
and greater activity. In: Proc. 5th Annu. Symp. Zoo Res. Marwell Zool. Park. Winchester, UK, 7-8th
July 2003; 212-224.
Bouguenoun I., De Pauw-Gillet M.-C., Bensakhri Z., Baiwir D., Mazzucchelli G., De Pauw E., Bendjed-
dou D. Eet inammatoire des extraits de Cupressus sempervirens planté dans l’Est de l’Algérie: du
modèle murin à la stimulation des cellules BEAS-2B par l’allergène majeur Cup s1. Rev. Fr. Allergol.
2016; 56: 10-19. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.REVAL.2015.09.005.
Brydges N.M., Leach M., Nicol K., Wright R., Bateson M. Environmental enrichment induces optimistic
cognitive bias in rats. Anim. Behav. 2011; 81: 169-175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.09.030.
Carvalho-Freitas M.R.R., Costa M. Anxiolytic and sedative eects of extracts and essential oil from Citrus
aurantium L. Biol Pharm Bull. 2002; 25; 1629-1633. https://doi.org/10.1248/bpb.25.
Cobb M.L., Iskandarani K., Chinchilli V.M., Dreschel N.A. A systematic review and meta-analysis of
saliveray cortisol measurement in domestic canines. Domest. Anim. Endocrinol. 2016; 57: 31-42.
https://doi:10.1016/J.DOMANIEND.2016.04.003.
Dawkins M.S., e Science of Animal Suering. Ethology. 2008; 114: 937-945. https://doi.org/10.1111
/j.1439-0310.2008.01557.
de Moraes Pultrini A., Galindo L.A., Costa M. Eects of the essential oils from Citrus aurantium L.in ex-
perimental anxiety models in mice. Life Sciences 2006; 78: 1720-1725.
Douglas C., Bateson M., Walsh C., Bédué A., Edwards S.A., Environmental enrichment induces optimis-
tic cognitive biases in pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2012; 139: 65-73. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.AP-
PLANIM.2012.02.018.
Doyle R.E., Fisher A.D., Hinch G.N., Boissy A., Lee C. Release from restraint generates a positive
judgement bias in sheep. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010; 122: 28-34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ap-
planim.2009.11.003.
Fatjó J., Feddersen-Petersen D., Ruiz de la Torre J.L., Amat M., Mets M., Braus B., Manteca X. Ambivalent
signals during agonistic interactions in a captive wolf pack. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007; 105: 274-283.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.11.009.
Gácsi M., Zsef J., Csányi T.V., Gyori B., Si M., Fia Z., Kubinyi V.E. A Species-Specic Dierences and
Similarities in the Behavior of Hand-Raised Dog and Wolf Pups in Social Situations with Humans.
2005; 47: 111-122 https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.04(20082).
Ferguson C.D., Kleinman H.F., Browining J. Eect of Lavender aromatherapy on acute-stressed horses. J
Equine Vet Sci. 2013; 1:67-69.
Galindo L.A., de Moraes Pultrini A., Costa M. Biological eects of Ocimum gratissimum L. are due to
synergic action among multiple compounds present in essential oil. J. Nat. Med. 2010; 64: 436-441.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11418-010-0429-2.
Graham L., Wells D.L., Hepper P.G. e inuence of visual stimulation on the behaviour of dogs housed
in a rescue shelter. Anim. Welf. 2005; 14: 143-148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2004.08.024.
Haverbeke A., Diederich C., Depiereux E., Giroy J.M. Cortisol and behavioral responses of working
dogs to environmental challenges. Physiol. Behav. 2008; 93: 59-67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.phys-
beh.2007.07.014.
Heuberger E., Ilmberger J. e inuence of essential oils on human vigilance. Nat. Prod. Commun. 2010;
5: 1441-1446.
Hongratanaworakit T., Buchbauer C. Evaluation of the harmonizing eect of ylang-ylang oil on humans
aer inhalation. Planta Med. 2004; 70: 632-636. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-2004-827186.
Kaulfuß P., Mills D.M. Neophilia in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and its implication for studies of dog
cognition. Animal Cognition 2008; 11: 553-556. https:// doi: 10.1007/s10071-007-0128-x.
Kirschbaum C., Hellhammer D.H. 2000. Salivary cortisol. In: Fink, G. (Ed.), 2000. Encyclopedia of Stress,
Vol. 3. Academic Press, New York, NY, pp. 379-383.
13
Kolevska J., Bruncklik V., Svodoba M. Circadian rhythm of cortisol secretion in dogs of dierent daily ac-
tivities. Acta Vet Brno. 2003; 72: 599-605, https://doi.org/10.2754/avb200372040599.
Kemppainen R.J.. Sartin J.L. Evidence for episodic but not circadian activity in plasma concentrations of
adrenocorticotrophin, cortisol and thyroxin in dogs. J. Endocrinol.1984; 103: 219-226.
Leite M.P., Fassin J., Baziloni E.M.F., Almeida R.N., Mattei R., Leite J.R. Behavioral eects of essential
oil of Citrus aurantium L. inhalation in rats. Brazilian J. Pharmacogn. 2008; 18: 661-666. https://doi.
org/10.1590/S0102-695X2008000500003.
Mason G. J. Stereotypies: a critical review. Anim. Behav. 1991; 41: 1015-1037.
Matheson S.M., Asher L., Bateson M. Larger, enriched cages are associated with “optimistic” response
biases in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008; 109: 374-383.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2007.03.007.
Matsubara E., Fukagawa M., Okamoto T., Fukuda A., Hayashi C., Ohnuki K., Shimizu K., Kondo R. Vola-
tiles emitted from the leaves of Laurus nobilis L. improve vigilance performance in visual discrimina-
tion task. Biomed. Res. 2011; 32: 19-28. https://doi.org/10.2220/biomedres.32.19.
Mendl M., Brooks J., Basse C., Burman O., Paul E., Blackwell E., Casey R. Dogs showing separation-
related behaviour exhibit a ‘pessimistic’ cognitive bias. Curr. Biol. 2010; 20: 839-840. https://doi.
org/10.1016/J.CUB.2010.08.030.
Mendl M., Burman O.H.P., Parker R.M.A., Paul E.S. Cognitive bias as an indicator of animal emotion and
welfare: Emerging evidence and underlying mechanisms. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009; 118: 161-181.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.02.023.
Owczarczak-Garstecka S.C., Burman O.H.P. Can Sleep and Resting Behaviours Be Used as Indica-
tors of Welfare in Shelter Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris)? PLoS One. 2016; 11: e0163620. https://doi.
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0163620.
Paul E.S., Harding E.J., Mendl M. Measuring emotional processes in animals: e utility of a cognitive ap-
proach. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2005; 29: 469-491. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.01.002.
Rashidi Fakari F., Tabatabaeichehr M., Kamali H., Naseri M. Eect of Inhalation of Aroma of Geranium
Essence on Anxiety and Physiological Parameters during First Stage of Labor in Nulliparous Women: a
Randomized Clinical Trial. J. caring Sci. 2015; 4: 135-41. https://doi.org/10.15171/jcs.2015.014.
Salmeto A.L., Hymel K.A., Carpenter E.C., Brilot B.O., Bateson M., Sufka K.J. Cognitive bias in
the chick anxiety-depression model. Brain Res. 2011; 1373: 124-130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
brainres.2010.12.007.
Shiverdecker M.D., Schiml P.A., Hennessy M.B. Human interaction moderates plasma cortisol and
behavioral responses of dogs to shelter housing. Physiol. Behav. 2013; 109: 75-79. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.12.002.
Takahashi T., Ebihara S., Nakamura Y., Takahashi K. A Model of Human Sleep-Related Growth Hor-
mone Secretion in Dogs: Eects of 3, 6, and 12 Hours of Forced Wakefulness on Plasma Growth
Hormone, Cortisol, and Sleep Stages. Endocrinology. 1981; 109: 262-272, https://doi.org/10.1210/
endo-109-1-262.
Titulaer M., Blackwell E.J., Mendl M., Casey R.A. Cross sectional study comparing behavioural, cognitive
and physiological indicators of welfare between short and long term kennelled domestic dogs. Appl.
Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013; 147: 149-158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2013.05.001.
Todd N.B. Inheritance of the catnip response in domestic cats. J. Hered. 2015; 54-56.
Vincent I.C., Michell A.R. Comparison of cortisol concentrations in saliva and plasma of dogs. Res. Vet.
Sci. 1992; 53: 342-345.
Wells D.L., Egli, J.M. e inuence of olfactory enrichment on the behaviour of captive black-foot-
ed cats, Felis nigripes. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015; 85: 107-119. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.AP-
PLANIM.2003.08.013.
Wells D. L. A review of environmental enrichment for kennelled dogs, Canis familiaris. Appl. Anim. Be-
hav. Sci. 2004; 85: 307-317, doi:10.1016/J.APPLANIM.2003.11.005.
14
Risposte comportamentali e del cortisolo di cani di canile sottoposti ad un “Cognitive bias test”
dopo arricchimento olfattivo con olii essenziali
Stefania Uccheddu1, Chiara Mariti2, Adinda Sannen3, Hilde Vervaecke3, Heidi Arnout3,4,
Jara Gutierrez Rufo2, Angelo Gazzano2, Anouck Haverbeke1
1 Vet Ethology, Leemveldstraat44, 3090 Overijse, Belgium
2 Department of Veterinary Sciences, University of Pisa, Viale delle Piagge 2, 56124 Pisa, Italy
3 Odisee University College, Agro- & Biotechnology, Ethology & Animal Welfare, Hospitaalstraat 23,
9100 Sint-Niklaas, Belgium
4 University of Antwerp, Departmentof Engineering Management, Prinsstraat 13, 2000 Antwerp, Belgium
Sintesi
L’ambiente di canile comporta per gli animali diverse forme di stress a cui i cani devono adattarsi. Recenti ricerche
hanno dimostrato che l’arricchimento con olii essenziali potrebbe essere in grado di modicare lo stato emozionale di
certe specie animali (cani, gatti, animali di zoo..). In questi studi la valutazione del welfare includeva indicatori siologici,
come ad esempio le concentrazioni di corticosteroidi e/o comportamenti correlati allo stress cronico.
L’eetto olfattorio di 9 olii essenziali (Cananga odorata, Cistus ladaniferus, Citrus aurantium, Cupressus sempervirens,
Juniperus communis var. montana, Lavandula angustifolia, Laurus nobilis, Litsea citrata, Pelargonium graveolens) e di una
miscela di questi olii è stato valutato sui risultati di un “Cognitive bias test”, sui livelli di cortisolo e sul comportamento
di 110 cani di canile (n= 10 cani per ogni gruppo).
L’arricchimento olfattivo con la miscela di olii ha ridotto la latenza della scelta dello stimolo ambiguo, indicando un
pregiudizio ottimistico ed un miglioramento del welfare.
I risultati di questo studio suggeriscono che l’arricchimento olfattivo con olii essenziali può avere un eetto specico
sullo stato emozionale e sul comportamento dei cani di canile e potrebbe perciò essere utile nel management di queste
strutture.
Inoltre, poiché non tutti gli olii testati singolarmente si sono dimostrati ecaci, ulteriori ricerche dovrebbero essere
eettuate per comprendere meglio gli eetti dei singoli olii sul cane.
... Additionally, olfactory enrichment has shown to help increase activity and species-specific behaviours (e.g., [105,106]), which, in turn, could potentially provide a positive knock-on effect influencing the attitudes of the public towards zoo-housed endangered species and the frequency of zoo visits. Studies on domestic animals in shelters have shown that olfactory enrichment not only decreases stress but can help to increase positive behaviours and improve socialization (e.g., [107,108]). As scents often elicit behavioural and physiological responses, it is important to explore the use of olfactory enrichment to promote potential beneficial effects on reproductive success [17]. ...
... The effects of olfactory enrichment have been tested by a number of studies on domestic, farm, laboratory and zoo-housed species (e.g., [111,112]). For instance, olfactory enrichment has been studied in both dog (Canis lupus familiaris) and cat (Felis catus) rescue shelters as a tool to reduce stress and encourage desirable behaviours (e.g., [108,110,112]). Rescue shelters are often stressful environments, and it is therefore important to find ways to improve the welfare of individuals. A study [108] exposed domestic dogs to a variety of essential oils and blends of oils and found that some scents increased frequencies of behavioural indicators of relaxation while others decreased behavioural indicators of stress such as pacing and over-grooming. ...
... Rescue shelters are often stressful environments, and it is therefore important to find ways to improve the welfare of individuals. A study [108] exposed domestic dogs to a variety of essential oils and blends of oils and found that some scents increased frequencies of behavioural indicators of relaxation while others decreased behavioural indicators of stress such as pacing and over-grooming. Similarly, another study [110] found that the introduction of cloths scented with vanilla, coconut and ginger significantly decreased vocalizations associated with stress in dogs housed in a rescue centre. ...
Article
Full-text available
Simple Summary In the face of a global biodiversity decline, zoos worldwide play important roles in conservation via efforts such as providing breeding programmes and reintroductions into the wild. Zoo populations are important as a buffer against extinction, but substantial differences between the wild and zoo environments can lead to health issues. These problems, in turn, can impact the reproductive success of individuals. Consequently, some primate species have reduced breeding success when housed in zoos compared to their wild counterparts. To prevent the onset of behavioural, physiological, and cognitive negative effects and to continually improve the welfare of their animals, zoos widely implement different types of environmental enrichment. Enrichment can take many forms, but sensory enrichments are less studied. Scent enrichments are less utilized despite multiple research studies showing that they may affect positively the well-being of zoo-housed animals, including non-human primates. Despite being traditionally considered to have a poor sense of smell, more evidence is emerging to suggest that olfaction is important in primates. This review article therefore focuses on scent-based enrichment and the specifics of primate olfaction. Abstract Zoos worldwide play an important role in both in situ and ex situ conservation via efforts such as providing breeding programmes and reintroductions into the wild. Zoo populations are crucial as a buffer against extinction. However, a mismatch between the wild and zoo environments can lead to psychological as well as physiological health issues, such as stress, boredom, diabetes, and obesity. These problems, in turn, can impact the reproductive success of individuals. Consequently, some primate species have reduced breeding success when housed in zoos compared to their wild counterparts. To prevent the onset of behavioural, physiological, and cognitive negative effects and to continually improve the welfare of their animals, zoos widely implement different types of environmental enrichment. There are many forms enrichment can take, such as feeding, puzzles and training, but sensory enrichments, including implementing the use of scents, are currently understudied. Scent enrichments are less utilized despite multiple research studies showing that they may have positive effects on welfare for zoo-housed animal species, including non-human primates. Despite being traditionally considered to be microsmatic, various lines of evidence suggest that olfaction plays a larger role in primates than previously thought. This review therefore focuses on scent-based enrichment and the specifics of captive primates.
... In literature, Essential Oils (EOs) have been already applied as a treatment for travel-induced stress in other domestic species and horses [4,5]. Particularly Lavender EO (Lavandula angustifolia) is recognized for its anxiolytic effects, already been studied in humans and animals [6][7][8][9][10]. ...
... There are other EOs that appear to be interesting as well for their proven anxiolytic effects in humans and shelter dogs [6][7][8][9][10]. In line with previous literature, a recent study on olfactory enrichment in shelter dogs confi rmed a signifi cant anxiolytic effect of the following 5 EOs: Ylang-ylang (Cananga odorata), ...
... This is in line with previous research, suggesting that a blend Table 2: Mean and SD values of the heart rates before (HR1) and after (HR2) the stress event for each treatment. Treatment 1 (control, no olfactory exposure to EOs), Treatment 2 (olfactory exposure to 30 drops of Lavandula angustifolia EO) and Treatment 3 (olfactory exposure to 30 drops of the blend of EOs). of anxiolytic EOs might have more effect than the single use of Lavender EO [8]. These results suggest that this blend of EOs might help rabbits to recover homeostasis quicker after a stressful event. ...
Article
Full-text available
Rabbits can experience stress during transport. This study explores the effects of Essential Oils (EOs) on the heart rate of rabbits during transport. Rabbits were submitted to 3 different treatments: no olfactory exposure to EOs (treatment 1, control); olfactory exposure to 30 drops of Lavandula angustifolia EO (treatment 2); and olfactory exposure to 30 drops of a blend of 5 EOs (treatment 3, Cananga odorata, Citrus aurantium, Cupressus sempervirens, Lavandula angustifolia, Litsea citrata EOs) in a randomized controlled crossover study design. Treatment 1 and Treatment 2 did not induce a significant change in rabbits’ heart rates, however, treatment 3 did induce a significant decrease in rabbits’ heart rates after transport. These results suggest that olfactory exposure to this blend of 5 EOs decreased the rabbit’s heart rate after transport. More research is needed to further evaluate the effects of olfactory enrichment with EOs in rabbits during transport.
... Stress-response hormones include cortisol and corticosterone. Cortisol is widely considered in studies on large laboratory animals such as beagles [23,24], whereas for smaller animals, such as rodents, corticosterone is the more important glucocorticoid in responding to stress exposure [19]. ...
... Studies have been carried out on the effectiveness of E.E [24][25][26][27] at preventing oxidative injury and restoring cholinergic neurotransmission in cognitively impaired aged rats [25], as well as in alleviating the behavioral changes in a mouse model of post-traumatic stress disorder [28]. In addition, studies regarding the relationship between housing conditions, such as single housing or grouped housing, and behavioral phenotypes have been conducted [29]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Animal welfare is recognized as essential for the coexistence of humans and animals. Considering the increased demand and interest in animal welfare, many methods for improving animal welfare are being devised, but which method reduces animal stress has not been scientifically verified. Therefore, reducing animal stress by providing a proper breeding environment and environmental enrichment can be the basis for animal study. In this study, stress levels were assessed based on the mouse-breeding environment. We considered that the higher the body weight and the lower the corticosterone concentration, the lower the stress. According to the results, animals in the individual ventilation cages were determined to have lower serum cortisol concentrations, while the body weight of the animals was increased when in individual ventilation cages compared with individual isolated cages and when providing environmental enrichment compared with group breeding or not providing environmental enrichment. The results provide appropriate guidelines for improving laboratory animal welfare.
... Notwithstanding, non-verbal paradigms are the only available tool to assess judgement biases in animals, therefore various protocols have been developed and adapted for several animal species (e.g. rats Harding et al. 2004;Rygula et al. 2013Rygula et al. , 2015Hales et al. 2014Hales et al. , 2016, starlings (Matheson et al. 2008), bees (Bateson et al. 2011), sheep (Doyle et al. 2010;Verbeek et al. 2014b, a), macaques (Bethell et al. 2012), pigs (Douglas et al. 2012), horses (Hintze et al. 2018), calves (Neave et al. 2013)), including the domestic dog, Canis familiaris (Mendl et al. 2010a;Wells et al. 2017;Uccheddu et al. 2018; Barnard et al. 2018;Duranton and Horowitz 2019). Despite the large number of studies that have used JBTs to evaluate the welfare and the affective state in animals, doubts remain about its reliability: results are not always in line with predictions, with some studies even leading to null results or to opposite findings to those expected (Iigaya et al. 2016;Roelofs et al. 2016;Raoult et al. 2017). ...
... Finally, we detected a potentially influence of the presence of a researcher, as highlighted also by others (Müller et al. 2012;Kis et al. 2015;Uccheddu et al. 2018): the median latency to reach the M location was extremely short, potentially because dogs were interested in the researcher who stood behind the bowl placed in M, rather than in the bowl itself. ...
Article
Full-text available
The judgement bias test represents one of the most applied tools to evaluate animals’ optimistic/pessimistic attitude and to infer their emotional and welfare state accordingly. The judgement bias test (JBT) has been used several times with dogs (Canis familiaris), in most cases using a spatial test that evaluates the dog’s attitude towards a bowl placed in ambiguous positions (located between two unambiguous trained positions associated with opposite outcomes). Results are contrasting and methodological and statistical caveats emerged: dogs struggled to learn the association between unambiguous positions and their outcomes, they hardly discriminated between adjacent locations and they might be influenced by researchers. Therefore, we propose a novel paradigm, aimed at easing the learning process and at achieving more reliable measures. Improvements of the novel paradigm are the increased difference between payoffs of trained locations, the reduction of the number of trials and of their length and the removal of the potential influence of researchers. Results showed that 98% of dogs reached the learning criterion and that their learning appeared more stable: dogs behaved differently between the two trained stimuli and the variability of responses towards these stimuli was lower than the one towards ambiguous stimuli. Behavioural analyses confirmed that dogs fully learned outcomes associated with trained stimuli and that they were hesitant towards ambiguous stimuli. Furthermore, dogs managed to successfully discriminate between each pair of adjacent locations. These results suggest that this protocol is a promising tool to assess judgement biases in dogs and to evaluate their affective state.
... The effects of scent-based enrichment programmes have been tested by a few studies on domestic, farm, laboratory, and zoo-housed animals (e.g., [14,15]). For example, olfactory enrichment has been studied in both dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) and cats (Felis catus) housed in rescue shelters as a way to reduce stress and encourage species-specific behaviours (e.g., [8,16]). Specifically, a study of domestic cats [17] found that almost all subjects showed a positive response, including increased play and decreased stress behaviours, when exposed to scented plants, whereas catnip significantly increased play behaviours. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objects and semiochemicals may be used as enrichment in zoos. Domestic cats release Fraction 3 of Facial Pheromone (F3) by rubbing the muzzle to convey relational and territorial information. We aimed to evaluate whether and how the introduction of novel objects and scent stimuli could change the behaviour of one group (N = 5 subjects) of adult European wildcats (Felis silvestris silvestris) hosted at Parco Natura Viva-Garda Zoological Park, Italy. We assessed the behavioural changes following the introduction of novel objects (blocks and rags) and scent (synthetic F3) via observations over four experimental conditions (baseline, rags, F3 rags, blocks) using continuous focal animal sampling. Our results showed that no behavioural differences were found between the different conditions and the baseline, except for the condition with blocks when significantly less exploration was observed. Between conditions, wildcats performed significantly less individual explorative, affiliative, and agonistic behaviours, but more individual inactivity, when exposed to rags after F3 administration. Our findings suggest that the enrichment programme did not substantively affect the behaviour of the zoo-housed wildcats. However, the behavioural differences recorded between conditions suggest that, while novel objects introduced as visual stimuli (blocks) do not affect the wildcat behaviour, novel manipulative objects (rags) might impact their behaviour. Moreover, the changes in affiliative and agonistic behaviours displayed during the condition with exposure to rags sprayed with F3 suggest that such semiochemical could play an appeasement role within this study group.
... ;Binks et al. 2018;Hermiston et al. 2018;Uccheddu et al. 2018;Haverbeke et al. 2019).Graham et al. (2005a) investigated lavender, chamomile, rosemary, peppermint along with a no-odor control, each diffused in front of and behind the dogs' kennels for four hours a day over five consecutive days. Exposure to both lavender and chamomile led to increases in dogs' resting and decreases in movement and ...
Chapter
Dogs experience a variety of stressors within the shelter that could negatively impact their welfare. The use of enrichment interventions that provide social interaction, either with a human or canine; object enrichment; and sensory stimulation (auditory, olfactory, or visual) is necessary for dogs living in animal shelters, along with the assessment of engagement and determination of benefits. There are a wide range of sampling and measurement techniques for monitoring enrichment usage and its behavioral effects, and such efforts are only worthwhile if the data being collected are used. Data‐informed decisions about which enrichment types are provided, on both the shelter‐wide and individual dog levels, must be consistently re‐evaluated based on the current population of dogs and can allow shelters to most usefully employ their resources and best serve the dogs in their care.
... Cortisol is largely used to evaluate animal stress (Ogi et al., 2020;Mariti et al., 2020a;Uccheddu et al., 2018). Using this hormone as a measure of physiological stress, a research study found significant reduction in salivary cortisol in healthcare providers after as little as a 5-minute interaction with an unfamiliar therapy dog (Barker et al., 2005). ...
Article
Full-text available
... The in uence of human gender on behaviour has been understudied in companion dogs [38,39] and, so far, has never been accounted as a potential confounding factor in eld tests aimed to assess age-related behavioural differences. Finally, cognitive tests designed to measure positive affective states have replicability issues and may not be reliable in ageing dogs, due to the learning required: for example, studies based on the cognitive bias test, a test for mood based on discrimination choices, showed that older dogs may struggle to learn the discrimination and therefore it may not be possible to test them [4,40]. Clinicians still need standardised testing for positive emotions in senior animals. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
The prolonged lifespan of companion dogs has resulted in an increased occurrence of behavioural and physical challenges linked to old age. The development of behavioural tests for identifying and monitoring age-related differences has begun. However, standardised testing requires validation. The present study aimed to assess external validity, interobserver reliability, and test-retest reliability of an indoor test battery for the rapid assessment of age-related behavioural differences in dogs. Two experimenters tested young and old dogs on a first occasion and after two weeks. Our results found external validity for two subtests out of six. On both test occasions, old dogs committed more errors than young dogs in a memory test and showed more object avoidance when encountering a novel object. Interobserver reliability and test-retest reliability was high. We conclude that the Memory and Novel object tests are valid and reliable for monitoring age-related memory performance and object neophobic differences in dogs.
Article
Full-text available
The prolonged lifespan of companion dogs has resulted in increased behavioural and physical challenges linked to old age. The development of behavioural tests to identify and monitor age‑related differences has begun. However, standardised testing requires validation. The present study aimed to assess external validity, interobserver reliability, and test–retest reliability of an indoor test battery for the rapid assessment of age‑related behavioural differences in dogs. Two experimenters tested young dogs (N = 20, mean age ± SD = 2.7 ± 0.4 years) and old dogs (N = 18, mean age ± SD = 11.8 ± 1.3 years) in the test battery once and then again after two weeks. Our results found external validity for two subtests out of six. On both test occasions, old dogs committed more errors than young dogs in a memory subtest and showed more object avoidance when encountering a novel object. Interobserver reliability and test–retest reliability was high. We conclude that the Memory and Novel object subtests are valid and reliable for monitoring age‑related memory performance and object neophobic differences in dogs.
Preprint
Full-text available
Laboratory animals are raised in a fixed space during the study period and are environmentally bound. Laboratory animal may be under stress on the constrained environment, which changes physiological indicators, affecting the reproducibility and accuracy of animal study. Therefore, reducing animal stress by providing proper breeding environment and environmental enrichment can be the basis for animal study. In this study, the stress level was assessed according to the mouse breeding environment. According to the results of the experiment, it was determined that the individual ventilation cage had less cortisol concentration in serum and body weight increased in the individual ventilation cage than individual isolated cage, when providing environmental enrichment rather than group breeding or not providing environmental enrichment. The results will provide appropriate guidelines for laboratory animal welfare.
Article
Full-text available
Previous research on humans and animals suggests that the analysis of sleep patterns may reliably inform us about welfare status, but little research of this kind has been carried out for non-human animals in an applied context. This study explored the use of sleep and resting behaviour as indicators of welfare by describing the activity patterns of dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) housed in rescue shelters, and comparing their sleep patterns to other behavioural and cognitive measures of welfare. Sleep and activity patterns were observed over five non-consecutive days in a population of 15 dogs. Subsequently, the characteristics of sleep and resting behaviour were described and the impact of activity on patterns of sleep and resting behaviour analysed. Shelter dogs slept for 2.8% of the day, 14.3% less than previously reported and experienced less sleep fragmentation at night (32 sleep bouts). There were no statistically significant relationships between behaviours exhibited during the day and sleep behaviour. A higher proportion of daytime resting behaviour was significantly associated with a positive judgement bias, less repetitive behaviour and increased time spent coded as ‘relaxed’ across days by shelter staff. These results suggest that, in the context of a busy shelter environment, the ability to rest more during the day could be a sign of improved welfare. Considering the non-linear relationship between sleep and welfare in humans, the relationship between sleep and behavioural indicators of welfare, including judgement bias, in shelter dogs may be more complex than this study could detect.
Article
Full-text available
Anxiety increases significantly during labor, especially among nulliparous women. Such anxiety may affect the progress of labor and physiological parameters. The use of essential oils of aromatic plants, or aromatherapy, is a non-invasive procedure that can decrease childbirth anxiety. This study examined the effect of inhalation of the aroma of geranium essential oil on the level of anxiety and physiological parameters of nulliparous women in the first stage of labor. In study, was carried out on 100 nulliparous women admitted to Bent al-Hoda Hospital in the city of Bojnord in North Khorasan province of Iran during 2012-2013. The women were randomly assigned to two groups of equal size, one experimental group (geranium essential oil) and one control (placebo) group. Anxiety levels were measured using Spielberger' questionnaire before and after intervention. Physiological parameters (systolic and diastolic blood pressure, respiratory rate, pulse rate) were also measured before and after intervention in both groups. Data analysis was conducted using the x2 test, paired t-test, Mann-Whitney U test, and Wilcox on test on SPSS 11.5. The mean anxiety score decreased significantly after inhalation of the aroma of geranium essential oil. There was also a significant decrease in diastolic blood pressure. Aroma of essential oil of geraniums can effectively reduce anxiety during labor and can be recommended as a non-invasive anti-anxiety aid during childbirth.
Article
Full-text available
JUSTIFICATIVA E OBJETIVOS: Reduzir a ansiedade é muito importante antes da operação. A visita no pré-operatório e a utilização de pré-medicação são os métodos mais populares para se atingir esse objetivo, mas o papel da pré-medicação ansiolítica permanece incerto e os efeitos colaterais no pós-operatório podem partir de uma pré-medicação de rotina. Citrus aurantium é usado como medicina alternativa em alguns países para tratar a ansiedade. Recentemente, o papel ansiolítico dessa planta medicinal foi estabelecido em um estudo realizado em modelo animal. O objetivo deste estudo foi avaliar o efeito ansiolítico da flor de Citrus aurantium sobre a ansiedade pré-operatória. MÉTODOS: Foram estudados 60 pacientes ASA I submetidos a uma pequena cirurgia. Em um desenho randomizado e duplo-cego, dois grupos de 30 pacientes receberam uma das seguintes MPA oral duas horas antes da indução da anestesia: 1) Citrus aurantium destilado 1 mL.kg-1 (Grupo C); 2) solução salina 1 mL.kg-1 como placebo (Grupo P). A ansiedade foi medida antes e após pré-medicação com o Inventário de Ansiedade Traço-Estado (IDATE) e a Escala de Ansiedade e Informação Pré-Operatória de Amsterdam (APAIS) antes da operação. RESULTADOS: Após pré-medicação, tanto o IDATE quanto as escalas APAIS estavam diminuídos no Grupo C (p < 0,05), embora não tenham apresentado alterações significativas no Grupo P. CONCLUSÕES:Citrus aurantium pode mostrar-se eficaz na redução da ansiedade pré-operatória em cirurgias de pequeno porte.
Article
Full-text available
Improving the quality of life of captive animals is dependent on developing valid measures of how animals feel about their lives. It has recently been suggested that biases in information processing may offer a novel means of understanding animal emotions. Anxious and depressed people tend to interpret ambiguous information negatively. We explored the proposal that such cognitive biases also exist in non-human animals and could therefore be used as novel measures of animal welfare. We used a novel cognitive bias task based on a learnt taste aversion to determine whether birds deprived of environmental enrichment show biases in their classification of ambiguous signals. We hypothesised that starlings in enriched cages should be more likely to classify ambiguous signals as being associated with a positive outcome than starlings housed in standard, unenriched cages. Starlings were trained on a go/no-go procedure to discriminate between two visual stimuli (cardboard lids of white and dark grey) associated with outcomes of a different value (palatable and unpalatable mealworms hidden underneath). Individual birds' responses to unreinforced, intermediate stimuli (various shades of grey between white and dark grey) were subsequently examined while each bird was housed sequentially in both standard and enriched cages. The probability of a bird classifying an ambiguous pale grey lid as hiding a palatable mealworm was lower in standard cages than enriched cages, but this difference was found only in birds that received enriched cages first. Our results can be interpreted as showing a pessimistic bias in birds that have recently experienced a decline in environmental quality. These findings support the use of cognitive bias-based tasks as a novel, non-invasive technique for assessing welfare in non-human animals.
Article
Salivary cortisol is widely used as an indicator of stress and welfare in canine research. However, much remains unclear about the basic features of this hormone marker in domestic dogs. This systematic review and meta-analysis aimed to determine a reference range for cortisol concentration in the saliva of dogs, and examine population effects and experimental considerations relating to salivary cortisol concentrations. A systematic review of literature databases and conference proceedings from 1992-2012 identified 61 peer-reviewed studies utilizing domestic dog salivary cortisol. Researchers were contacted via email and 31 raw data sets representing a total of 5,153 samples from 1,205 dogs were shared. Meta-analysis provided a cortisol concentration range of 0 – 33.79 μg/dL (mean 0.45 μg/dL, SEM 0.13). Significant effects (P < 0.05) were found for: sex and neuter status, age, regular living environment, time in environment prior to testing, testing environment, owner presence during testing, and collection media. Significant effects were not found for: dog breed, body weight, dog type, coat color, assay type, exercise, eating, or use of salivary stimulant. Care should be taken when using cortisol studies for dogs at a group or population level as there is a large amount of intra- and inter-individual variability and external variables could influence salivary cortisol concentration. This analysis highlights the importance of carefully controlling experimental design in order to compare samples within and between individual dogs, as well as establishing and using best practices for saliva collection. Caution should be exercised in comparing different studies, as the results could be a reflection of a plethora of factors.
Article
Cupressus sempervirens est l’une des espèces les plus répandues en Algérie ayant une capacité allergénique très élevée. Nous avons étudié, d’abord, comment l’instillation nasale des différentes fractions de l’extrait pollinique de cette espèce peut moduler la réponse immunitaire chez les souris, et, ensuite, la stimulation des cellules épithéliales bronchiques humaines (BEAS-2B) par l’allergène majeur, identifié par spectrométrie de masse et purifié par chromatographie échangeuse d’ions. La sensibilisation des souris a révélé, d’une part, une diminution du nombre de neutrophiles et une augmentation des éosinophiles, et, d’autre part, une augmentation significative du total cellulaire dans le liquide du lavage bronchoalvéolaire. L’étude histologique des poumons a montré une présence d’infiltrat inflammatoire et d’œdème péri-bronchique. La spectrométrie de masse nous a permis de mettre en évidence la présence de l’allergène majeur Cup s 1 dans toutes les fractions avec une présence spécifique du Cup s 3 et Cup a 1 dans la fraction 4. Cet allergène avait le potentiel pour stimuler la libération de l’IL-8 et l’IL-6 par la ligné cellulaire, BEAS-2B, in vitro d’une manière dose-dépendante.
Article
A study was designed to quantify ambivalent behaviour during social aggressive interactions in wolves. Agonistic interactions in a group of six European captive wolves, consisting of three males and three females, were analyzed for bared teeth, body posture and the position of ears, tongue, lips, legs and tail. The behavioural elements in each of these categories were assumed to be neutral or to signal dominance or submission. Wolves were considered to show ambivalence when dominant and submissive signs were observed simultaneously. More than 200 aggressive interactions were videotaped and parts of them were analyzed frame by frame. Results indicated that male wolves showed high levels of ambivalence (48%) and that this behavioural trait is not linked to a particular social status. Regarding specific body signals, tail position was the most reliable indicator of status, whereas bared teeth was not linked to a particular position in a dominance relationship. The possible application of these results to understanding aggression problems in dogs is briefly discussed.
Article
Domestic dogs can be housed in a variety of confined conditions, including kennels, shelters and laboratories. Concern over the well-being of dogs housed in human care has prompted much research in recent years into the enrichment of environments for kennelled dogs. This paper highlights the findings and recommendations arising from this work. Two types of general enrichment method are discussed, namely animate (i.e. enrichment through the provision of social contacts with conspecifics and humans) and inanimate (i.e. enrichment through the provision of toys, cage furniture, auditory and olfactory stimulation). The benefits and, where relevant, possible disadvantages, to these various types of enrichment method are highlighted throughout.