ArticlePublisher preview available

When Neutral is Not Neutral: Neurophysiological Evidence for Reduced Discrimination between Aversive and Non-Aversive Information in Generalized Anxiety Disorder

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the authors.

Abstract and Figures

Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) is characterized by a range of cognitive and affective disruptions, such as pathological worry. There is debate, however, about whether such disruptions are specifically linked to heightened responses to aversive stimuli, or due to overgeneralized threat monitoring leading to deficits in the ability to discriminate between aversive and non-aversive affective information. The present study capitalized on the temporal and functional specificity of scalp-recorded event-related potentials (ERPs) to examine this question by exploring two targeted neurocognitive responses in a group of adults diagnosed with GAD: (1) visual processing of angry (aversive) versus neutral (non-aversive) faces; and (2) response monitoring of incorrect (aversive) versus correct (non-aversive) responses. Electroencephalography was recorded while 15 adults with GAD and 15 age-matched controls viewed angry and neutral faces prior to individual trials of a flanker task. ERPs to faces were the P1, reflecting attention allocation, the early posterior negativity (EPN), reflecting early affective discrimination, and the N170, reflecting face-sensitive visual discrimination. The error-related negativity (ERN) and positivity (Pe) were generated to incorrect and correct responses. Results showed reduced discrimination between aversive and non-aversive faces and responses in the GAD relative to the control group during visual discrimination (N170) and later-emerging error monitoring (Pe). These effects were driven by exaggerated processing of non-aversive faces and responses, suggesting over-generalized threat monitoring. Implications for cognitive-affective models of GAD are discussed.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
Vol.:(0123456789)
1 3
Motivation and Emotion (2019) 43:325–338
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-018-9732-0
ORIGINAL PAPER
When neutral isnotneutral: Neurophysiological evidence forreduced
discrimination betweenaversive andnon-aversive information
ingeneralized anxiety disorder
SamanthaDenefrio1,2· SarahMyruski1· DouglasMennin3· TracyA.Dennis‑Tiwary1,2
Published online: 8 October 2018
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2018
Abstract
Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) is characterized by a range of cognitive and affective disruptions, such as pathological
worry. There is debate, however, about whether such disruptions are specifically linked to heightened responses to aversive
stimuli, or due to overgeneralized threat monitoring leading to deficits in the ability to discriminate between aversive and
non-aversive affective information. The present study capitalized on the temporal and functional specificity of scalp-recorded
event-related potentials (ERPs) to examine this question by exploring two targeted neurocognitive responses in a group of
adults diagnosed with GAD: (1) visual processing of angry (aversive) versus neutral (non-aversive) faces; and (2) response
monitoring of incorrect (aversive) versus correct (non-aversive) responses. Electroencephalography was recorded while 15
adults with GAD and 15 age-matched controls viewed angry and neutral faces prior to individual trials of a flanker task. ERPs
to faces were the P1, reflecting attention allocation, the early posterior negativity(EPN), reflecting early affective discrimi-
nation, and the N170, reflecting face-sensitive visual discrimination. The error-related negativity(ERN) and positivity (Pe)
were generated to incorrect and correct responses. Results showed reduced discrimination between aversive and non-aversive
faces and responses in the GAD relative to the control group during visual discrimination (N170) and later-emerging error
monitoring (Pe). These effects were driven by exaggerated processing of non-aversive faces and responses, suggesting over-
generalized threat monitoring. Implications for cognitive-affective models of GAD are discussed.
Keywords Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD)· Event-related potentials (ERPs)· Emotion· Anxiety
Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) is among the most com-
monly-diagnosed anxiety disorders (Ballenger etal. 2001)
affecting an estimated 5.7% of the adult population in their
lifetime and over 18million individuals in the U.S. alone
(Kessler etal. 2005). Recent theoretical models of GAD
(Mennin etal. 2002, 2005; Salters-Pedneault etal. 2006)
posit that overgeneralized threat monitoring, measured in
terms of reduced ability to differentiate between aversive and
non-aversive affective information, may be a key mechanism
in the emergence and maintenance of GAD symptoms.
Reduced affective discrimination may be one of several,
temporally-distinct stages of disrupted threat processing in
GAD. For example, research on GAD using reaction time
measures documents early-emerging exaggerated atten-
tion to threat-themed relative to neutral stimuli (Amir etal.
2009), whereas research using neuroimaging techniques
such as fMRI documents reduced discrimination between
aversive and neutral images (e.g., enhanced bilateral dorsal
amygdala activation to cues predicting both aversive and
neutral images; Nitschke etal. 2009). These two findings,
one of early exaggerated response to aversive stimuli and
the other of reduced discrimination during more elabora-
tive processing, may reflect methdological differences in the
level of analysis (brain or behavior) and in the time-scale of
measurement. The use of a single measurement technique
that can simultaneously capture multiple stages of response
to affective stimuli is therefore a crucial next step in clarify-
ing the nature of affective discrimination in GAD.
* Tracy A. Dennis-Tiwary
tracy.dennis@hunter.cuny.edu
1 Hunter College, The City University ofNew York,
NewYork, NY, USA
2 The Graduate Center, The City University ofNew York,
NewYork, NY, USA
3 Teachers College, Columbia University, NewYork, NY, USA
Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.
... Some studies have found that, relative to healthy controls, adults diagnosed with GAD show larger-magnitude ERNs following the commission of an error relative to a correct response (Weinberg et al., 2010(Weinberg et al., , 2012(Weinberg et al., , 2015. Yet, other studies document that GAD is associated with broader disruptions in error-related brain activity, including exaggerated processing of correct responses that are non-aversive (Denefrio et al., 2019;Endrass et al., 2010;Xiao et al., 2011). For example, in one study, adults diagnosed with GAD, compared to healthy controls, showed greater magnitude error positivity to non-aversive correct responses, suggesting overgeneralized threat monitoring at a temporal window slightly later than the ERN (Denefrio et al., 2019). ...
... Yet, other studies document that GAD is associated with broader disruptions in error-related brain activity, including exaggerated processing of correct responses that are non-aversive (Denefrio et al., 2019;Endrass et al., 2010;Xiao et al., 2011). For example, in one study, adults diagnosed with GAD, compared to healthy controls, showed greater magnitude error positivity to non-aversive correct responses, suggesting overgeneralized threat monitoring at a temporal window slightly later than the ERN (Denefrio et al., 2019). ...
... The goal of the present study was to test whether the association between GAD symptom severity and ERN was sensitive to individual differences in processing inhibition measured via NQ. To do so, we reanalyzed data from a previously published study examining the ERN in adults diagnosed with GAD (i.e., Denefrio et al., 2019). We quantified NQ to non-aversive (neutral faces) and aversive (angry faces) stimuli presented prior to flanker arrays on each trial of a speeded flanker task, from which ERN to incorrect versus correct responses were generated. ...
Article
Full-text available
Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) has been inconsistently associated with exaggerated threat monitoring measured via the error-related negativity (ERN). This suggests the need to consider whether the link between GAD and ERN is influenced by additional processes, such as cognitive inhibition of non-threat. The current study explored this possibility by employing a novel, trait-like measure of cognitive processing inhibition, neural quenching (NQ). Electroencephalography was recorded while 16 adults diagnosed with GAD and 14 age-matched healthy controls viewed angry and neutral faces prior to individual trials of a flanker task. NQ was generated to aversive (angry) and non-aversive (neutral) facial primes, and the ERN was generated to incorrect and correct responses on the flanker task. We tested the hypothesis that higher GAD symptom severity would be associated with larger-magnitude ERN when NQ to non-aversive was enhanced (higher levels of non-aversive processing inhibition), but with blunted ERN when NQ to non-aversive was also blunted (lower levels of non-aversive processing inhibition). Overall, greater NQ to non-aversive faces was associated with larger-magnitude ERNs. As predicted, higher GAD symptom severity was associated with blunted ERN when accompanied by blunted NQ to non-aversive. Findings suggest that exaggerated threat processing is not uniform in GAD and may depend on individual differences in the ability to inhibit processing of non-aversive and other types of information.
... Other studies hypothesized that distinguishing between safety and threat cues occur during the later stages of processing (Mogg et al., 2000), in which anxious individuals have top-down processing in threat-safety detection (Eysenck et al., 2007). These conflicting findings may reflect methodological differences (Denefrio et al., 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
Detection of safety-threat signals during uncertainty is an important mechanism of developmental anxiety disorder (AD). Although extensive research has focused on the detection of uncertain threat signals in anxious individuals, relatively little attention has been given to the identification of safety signals during uncertainty, which is an important way to relieve anxiety in individuals with AD. To investigate this phenomenon, 16 subjects with high trait anxiety (HTA) and 16 with low trait anxiety (LTA) completed a modified cue-target task in certain and uncertain stimulus blocks. In the uncertain block, the cue was followed by a threat picture or safety picture in 20% of trials, respectively; in the certain block, the cue could be followed by a threat picture or a safety picture on 100% of trials. Behavioral responses and event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded. The ERP results demonstrated that LTA participants exhibited larger P2 amplitudes in the detection of safety cues than of threat cues during the uncertain block, whereas HTA participants showed significant P2 amplitudes between the safety and threat cues during the certain block, impairing the detection of safety stimuli during uncertainty. However, all participants exhibited greater N2 amplitudes following threat cues in certainty or uncertainty conditions. These findings pertaining to the P2 amplitude suggested distinctive attentional biases between HTA and LTA individuals, whereas the N2 amplitude showed association learning in uncertain conditions, compensating for safety-threat detection in HTA individuals.
... Viewing angry faces (N170) N170 is an ERP that is elicited to faces (e.g., angry faces). Previous research has found that the N170 is larger in response to angry faces compared to other emotional expressions (e.g., neutral or happy Denefrio et al., 2019;Hinojosa et al., 2015;Jetha et al., 2013;Kolassa et al., 2009, Kolassa et al., 2007Rossignol et al., 2005). Furthermore, individuals with greater threat sensitivity (or anxiety) tend to have a larger N170 to angry faces than those with less sensitivity to threat or anxiety (Bechor et al., 2019;Kolassa & Miltner, 2006;O'Toole et al., 2013;Wieser et al., 2010). ...
Article
Threat sensitivity is thought to be a precursor for anxiety. Yet it remains unknown whether individuals have consistently high neural activation to different threatening situations. The current study (N = 161, Mage = 11.26, SD = 1.79) used three ERPs from different threat-related events: 1) the P3 to receiving negative feedback; 2) the ERN to making mistakes; 3) the N170 to viewing angry faces. Participants also completed self-report measures of threat sensitivity, impulsivity, and demographics. In a follow-up analysis, we also investigated whether the results replicate when using the difference score for each ERP. Youth with higher self-reported sensitivity to threats and lower self-reported impulsivity had consistently higher neural activation to threatening situations. Males also had consistently higher neural activation to threats compared with females. When using the difference score, we found that youth with higher self-reported threat sensitivity had consistently higher neural activation to threats than nonthreats. Although it is common for youth to have high neural activation during at least one threatening situation (e.g., making mistakes), only ~12% of youth have consistently high neural activation across a variety of different threats. Thus, detecting youth who are sensitive to a variety of different threats may be an important avenue to investigate to identify youth most at risk for the development of anxiety.
... EPN is a negative-going wave observed in the posterior parts of the scalp at around 200-350 ms after stimulus onset. Namely, the negative stimuli have been reported to evoke more negative potentials than neutral ones [72]. Also, in the LPC component, positively valenced stimuli have been reported to evoke more positive potentials than negative ones [69]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The role of emotional factors in maintaining cognitive control is one of the most intriguing issues in understanding emotion-cognition interactions. In the current experiment, we assessed the role of emotional factors (valence, arousal, and subjective significance) in perceptual and conceptual inhibition processes. We operationalised both processes with the classical cognitive paradigms, i.e., the flanker task and the emotional Stroop task merged into a single experimental procedure. The procedure was based on the presentation of emotional words displayed in four different font colours flanked by the same emotional word printed with the same or different font colour. We expected to find distinct effects of both types of interference: earlier for perceptual and later for emotional interference. We also predicted an increased arousal level to disturb inhibitory control effectiveness, while increasing the subjective significance level should improve this process. As we used orthogonal manipulations of emotional factors, our study allowed us for the first time to assess interactions within emotional factors and between types of interference. We found on the behavioural level the main effects of flanker congruency as well as effects of emotionality. On the electrophysiological level, we found effects for EPN, P2, and N450 components of ERPs. The exploratory analysis revealed that effects due to perceptual interference appeared earlier than the effects of emotional interference, but they lasted for an extended period of processing, causing perceptual and emotional interference to partially overlap. Finally, in terms of emotional interference, we showed the effect of subjective significance: the reduction of interference cost in N450 for highly subjective significant stimuli. This study is the first one allowing for the investigation of two different types of interference in a single experiment, and provides insight into the role of emotion in cognitive control.
Article
Objective: Attention bias modification (ABM) aims to decrease anxiety symptom severity through the reduction of threat-related attention bias (AB). Individual differences in treatment response and poor measurement reliability of AB have called its clinical promise into question. The current study examined whether individual differences in anxiety severity at baseline moderated treatment response, and employed both behavioral and neurophysiological metrics of AB. Methods: Participants (N = 99) were randomly assigned to four weeks of ABM or placebo control training (PT). Self-reported anxiety symptom severity, and AB metrics and ERPs generated during the dot probe task were collected at baseline (Time 1), one-week post-intervention (Time 5), and at a three-month follow-up (Time 6). Results: ABM, relative to PT, reduced ERPs indexing attention discrimination (N170) and increased ERPs indexing salience tracking (P3). Increases in P3 were associated with ABM-related reductions in anxiety. Anxiety severity was reduced following ABM, but only among those with higher baseline anxiety symptom severity. Conclusions: ABM effectively reduced symptom severity among those with higher levels of anxiety, and modulated neurophysiological indices of AB. Significance: Results provide evidence for attention-relevant ERPs as outcomes of ABM treatment responsivity and suggest that ABM may be most beneficial for those with more severe anxiety symptoms.
Chapter
WorryWorries is a core feature of GADGeneralized anxiety disorder (GAD). It is defined as a chain of mainly verbal thoughts that are negative and uncontrollable. The chapter discusses seven cognitive conceptualizations of GADGeneralized anxiety disorder (GAD): the Avoidance Model of WorryAvoidance Model of worry (AMW) (Borkovec), the Intolerance of Uncertainty ModelIntolerance Of Uncertainty Model (IoU) (Dugas), the Metacognitive Model (Wells), the Emotion Dysregulation ModelEmotion Dysregulation Model (EDM), the Acceptance-Based ModelAcceptance-Based Model (ABM), the Contrast Avoidance ModelContrast Avoidance Model (CAM), and the Hirsch and Mathews’ cognitive modelHirsch and Mathews’ cognitive model. The chapter surveys the somewhat more limited research on etiological factors of GADGeneralized anxiety disorder (GAD): genetic, childhood pathology, parental attachment, and childhood cognitive deficits. Structural, functional, and connectivity MRIMagnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies are discussed in relation to GADGeneralized anxiety disorder (GAD) and constructs related to GADGeneralized anxiety disorder (GAD) such as intolerance of uncertainty and attentional bias to threatAttentional bias to threats. Electrophysiological event-related potential (ERP) components associated with GADGeneralized anxiety disorder (GAD) symptoms are delineated. Finally, dysregulations in the neuroendocrine system and hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal (HPA) are considered.
Article
Full-text available
Need-satisfying experiences corresponding to students’ psychological needs of autonomy, relatedness, and competence lead to increased academic engagement and well-being. A lack of education-specific basic needs instruments validated in Swedish may inhibit basic needs research in Swedish-speaking student populations. Thus, the present study aimed to adapt the Need Satisfaction and Frustration Scale (NSFS) to the context of education in Sweden and validate this new Swedish NSFS in a sample of Swedish university students (n = 417, 59.7% women, mean age = 23.3 (SD = 4.3) years). Confirmatory factor analysis was used to investigate the dimensionality of NSFS ratings, followed by structural equation modelling of nomological networks of basic needs, academic engagement, life satisfaction, academic burnout, and perceived stress. The NSFS ratings were best described by a three-factor model with methods correction for reversed item bias. Results showed that each need uniquely contributed to perceived stress and academic burnout. Relatedness and competence, but not autonomy, were significant predictors of life satisfaction and academic engagement. The main study contribution was providing a Swedish NSFS to assess autonomy, relatedness, and competence in higher education. The results support the use of the Swedish NSFS as a three-dimensional measure of basic needs in Swedish-speaking student populations.
Article
Background Anxiety disorders are highly prevalent and cause substantial personal, social and economic burden. Altered attentional control has been shown to be present across anxiety disorders and is associated with specific changes in brain activity which can be recorded by electroencephalogram (EEG). These include changes in the EEG markers of error-related negativity (ERN) and correct-response negativity (CRN), both believed to reflect response monitoring and attentional control pathophysiology in anxiety. The aim of this review was to systematically assess the research on ERN and CRN in attentional control in individuals with clinical anxiety and healthy controls, across emotional and non-emotional attentional control. Methods A comprehensive literature search was conducted for studies published prior to October 22nd, 2020. Details of the protocol for this systematic review were registered on PROSPERO (CRD42019144885). Results 66 studies had their data extracted. All 66 studies measured ERN, with 85% finding significantly increased ERN amplitudes associated with clinical anxiety. Only 44 of the extracted studies analysed CRN and only ~20% of these found significant changes in CRN amplitude associated with individuals with clinical anxiety. Limitations There were several anxiety disorders that had either limited literature (i.e. specific phobia, separation anxiety disorder or agoraphobia) or nil literature (i.e. selective mutism) available. No extracted studies included samples of older adults (i.e. aged 60+ years), and only six extracted studies included measures of emotional attentional control. Conclusions Findings indicate the promising utility of ERN of attentional control as a robust, transdiagnostic trait marker of clinical anxiety.
Article
Full-text available
Background: It has been established that the inability to inhibit a response to negative stimuli is the genesis of anxiety. However, the neural substrates of response inhibition to sad faces across explicit and implicit tasks in general anxiety disorder (GAD) patients remain unclear. Methods: Electrophysiological data were recorded when subjects performed two modified emotional go/no-go tasks in which neutral and sad faces were presented: one task was explicit (emotion categorization), and the other task was implicit (gender categorization). Results: In the explicit task, electrophysiological evidence showed decreased amplitudes of no-go/go difference waves at the N2 interval in the GAD group compared to the control group. However, in the implicit task, the amplitudes of no-go/go difference waves at the N2 interval showed a reversed trend. Source localization analysis on no-go/N2 components revealed a decreased current source density (CSD) in the right dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex in GAD individuals relative to controls. In the implicit task, the left superior temporal gyrus and the left inferior parietal lobe showed enhanced activation in GAD individuals and may compensate for the dysfunction of the right dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex. Conclusion: These findings indicated that the processing of response inhibition to socially sad faces in GAD individuals was interrupted in the explicit task. However, this processing was preserved in the implicit task. The neural substrates of response inhibition to sad faces were dissociated between implicit and explicit tasks.
Article
Full-text available
The ability to detect and respond to errors is critical to successful adaptation to a changing environment, and variation in error-related brain activity has been linked to psychopathology. The error-related negativity (ERN), an event-related potential component, represents a unique neural response to errors and is generated in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC). In the present study, we measured the ERN in a sample of individuals with Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD), Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD), Major Depressive Disorder (MDD), or some combination of the 3. Also included were 56 healthy control participants. Consistent with previous research, a diagnosis of GAD, only in the absence of a comorbid diagnosis of depression, was characterized by a larger ERN than controls. No such enhancement was evident in the depressed group, or the comorbid group, suggesting comorbid depression may eliminate the relationship between the ERN and GAD. Across all groups, symptoms of checking were associated with a larger ERN, whereas symptoms of psychomotor retardation were associated with a smaller ERN. The results of the present study indicate that interactions among transdiagnostic dimensions will likely need to be considered in the creation of neurobiologically informed classification schemes. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2014 APA, all rights reserved).
Article
The common approach to the multiplicity problem calls for controlling the familywise error rate (FWER). This approach, though, has faults, and we point out a few. A different approach to problems of multiple significance testing is presented. It calls for controlling the expected proportion of falsely rejected hypotheses — the false discovery rate. This error rate is equivalent to the FWER when all hypotheses are true but is smaller otherwise. Therefore, in problems where the control of the false discovery rate rather than that of the FWER is desired, there is potential for a gain in power. A simple sequential Bonferronitype procedure is proved to control the false discovery rate for independent test statistics, and a simulation study shows that the gain in power is substantial. The use of the new procedure and the appropriateness of the criterion are illustrated with examples.
Article
Informal caregivers (ICs) are integral to care provided to patients facing life-threatening or incurable illnesses. This responsibility causes considerable burden, as approximately one half of ICs report clinically significant symptoms of depression and/or anxiety that persist when left untreated. Psychosocial interventions containing efficacious treatment principles (e.g., cognitive behavior therapy [CBT]) show disappointing results in reducing anxiety and depression in ICs. This may reflect failure of these interventions to specifically target crucial mechanisms underlying the central feature of distress caused by the patient's illness-notably, persever-ative negative thinking (PNT). Emotion Regulation Therapy (ERT) is an efficacious CBT developed to explicitly target mechanisms underlying PNT and the emotional concomitants that arise in response to stressful situations. This open trial was conducted to evaluate the acceptability and initial efficacy of ERT adapted to the experience of cancer ICs (ERT-C). Thirty-one ICs provided informed consent and completed eight weekly individual sessions of ERT-C. Participants completed self-report measures of depression and anxiety symptoms, PNT, emotion regulation deficits, and caregiver burden before and after treatment. ERT-C was well tolerated as indicated by 22 treatment completers and feedback provided in exit interviews. ICs demonstrated reduced depression and anxiety symptoms, PNT, and emotion regulation deficits with moderate to large effect sizes (Hedge's g range: 0.36-0.92). Notably, care-giver burden was not reduced but ICs expressed more ability to confront caregiving-related challenges. Findings offer promising but preliminary support for ERT-C as a conceptual model and treatment modality for distressed cancer ICs.
Book
Event-related potentials (ERPs) have been used for decades to study perception, cognition, emotion, neurological and psychiatric disorders, and lifespan development. ERPs consist of multiple components and reflect a specific neurocognitive process. In the past, there was no single source that could be consulted to learn about all the major ERP components; learning about a single ERP component required reading dozens or even hundreds of separate journal articles and book chapters. The Oxford Handbook of Event-Related Potential Components fills this void with a detailed review of the major ERP components. The book looks at the fundamental nature of ERP components, including essential information about how ERP components are defined and isolated. It explains in detail individual components, such as the N170, P300, and ERN. It further examines groups of related components within specific research domains, such as language, emotion, and memory. Finally, it analyses ERP components in special populations, including children, the elderly, nonhuman primates, and patients with neurological disorders, affective disorders, and schizophrenia.
Article
The efficacy of attention bias modification training (ABMT) for anxiety is debated, in part because individual differences in task engagement and pre-training threat bias impact training efficacy. In the present study, an engaging, gamified ABMT mobile application, or "app," was utilized in 42 (21 females) trait-anxious adults. EEG was recorded during pre- and post-training threat bias assessment to generate scalp-recorded event-related potentials (ERPs) reflecting neurocognitive responses to threat. Following app play (ABMT versus placebo), subjective anxiety and stress responses (observed and self-reported) were measured. ABMT, versus placebo, resulted in improved behavioral performance during the stress task for females, and in potentiation of the N2 ERP to threat for males, suggesting increased attention control. Training groups did not differ in self-reported anxiety. ABMT also resulted in improved performance during the stress task among those evidencing specific pre-training ERP responses: decreased P1, suggesting less attention allocation, but potentiated N170, suggesting enhanced attention selection and discrimination. Differences in behavioral threat bias did not moderate training effects. Results suggest that efficient allocation of attention to threat combined with enhanced discrimination between threat and non-threat may facilitate stress-reduction effects of ABMT. Targeting neurocognitive responses to threat to personalize ABMT and develop more effective methods of treatment delivery, such as gamification, are discussed.
Article
The delineation of specific versus overlapping mechanisms in generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) and major depressive disorder (MDD) could shed light on the integrity of these diagnostic categories. For example, negative emotion generation is one mechanism that may be especially relevant to both disorders. Emotional processing abnormalities were examined among 97 outpatients with GAD or MDD and 25 healthy adults, using the late positive potential (LPP), an event-related potential that is larger for emotional versus neutral stimuli. GAD and MDD were also assessed dimensionally across all participants. Both MDD diagnosis and dimensional depression scores were associated with reduced ΔLPP. When controlling for MDD diagnosis/dimension, both the diagnosis and dimension of GAD were associated with increased ΔLPP. Both MDD and GAD dimensions, but not diagnoses, were associated with increased ΔRT to targets that followed emotional pictures. Therefore, MDD and GAD have distinguishable and opposing features evident in neural measures of emotion processing.