ResearchPDF Available

Hard Times and Religion: Do Religious People Stay Happier in Times of Crisis?

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Numerous studies have concluded that religious people are more likely to be happier in life. In this paper I ask whether people who consider themselves to be spiritual are more likely to report higher subjective life satisfaction in times of an economic crisis than those who are less spiritual. Using data from European Social Survey (Rounds 1-8), I find that on an average people who are more spiritual have a higher life satisfaction in times of crisis than people who report lower level of spirituality.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Hard Times and Religion: Do Religious People Stay
Happier in Times of Crisis?
Shafin Shabir
30 June 2018
Numerous studies have concluded that religious people are more likely to
be happier in life. In this paper I ask whether people who consider themselves
to be spiritual are more likely to report higher subjective life satisfaction in
times of an economic crisis than those who are less spiritual. Using data
from European Social Survey (Rounds 1-8), I find that on an average people
who are more spiritual have a higher life satisfaction in times of crisis than
people who report lower level of spirituality.
1 Introduction
Religion is probably as old as the human civilisation. One could easily
say that religion to this day remains a very strong institution, withstanding
all the ’atrocities’ that it committed or that it had to face. My aim here is
not do discuss or analyse the institutional part of the religion. Rather I am
more interested in its spiritual aspects and the way it influences a person’s
subjective well-being.
In the post-modern era, which happens to be my period of interest, the
idea of religion and spirituality as different ideas has gained popularity.1
A lot of people consider themselves spiritual but at the same time do not
want to be associated with a particular religion. Approximately 30% of peo-
ple who said that they do not identify themselves to a particular religious
denomination consider themselves to be spiritual.2This disinclination to-
wards an institutionalized form if religion may be due to various reasons
Barcelona Graduate School of Economics (shafin.shabir@barceloonagse.eu)
1For an overview see: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Spiritual_but_not_religious#
cite_note-FOOTNOTEMercandante2014-1
2This has been taken from the ESS data(2002-2016) where being spiritual is measured on a
scale from 0-10 with 0 being not at all spiritual to 10 being the most spiritual. For the purpose
of this statistic we coded spiritual with a score 5.
1
some of them are mentioned in a book titled Spiritual but not religious: a
call to religious revolution in America written by Erlandson (2000).
But why do people become spiritual despite not wanting to follow a re-
ligion? One of the reasons might be the positive impact of spiritulity on
life satisfaction (For a review of the literature see Swinyard et al. (2001)).
Ferriss (2002) also finds a positive correlation between religiousness and the
quality of life and a negative impact with the indicators of stress. Taking
this as my motivation, I am interested to know whether spiritual people
remain happier in times of crisis than those who are not spiritual. I use
the European crises as an external shock to analyse the impact of spiritual
people on their life satisfaction. My results suggest that people who con-
sider themselves as spiritual remain happier than others even in times of
crisis.
In the following section I give a brief introduction of the literature on
the topic. In section 3, I discuss the datasets that I have used in the paper
which is followed by empirical specification and results in section 4. I end
with a conclusion in section 5.
2 Literature Review
A lot of work has been done in the subjective measure of well being.
Usually the surveys designed to measure well being ask the question: How
satisfied are you with life? and the respondent has to choose on a scale of
0-103where 0 means not at all satisfied and 1 means completely satisfied.
The basic assumptions that we need to make when doing an analysis using
this variable are that the measures can be compared and individuals can
value their satisfaction on a numeric scale. The assumption of ordinality
or cardinality is not required in such an analyses(Ferrer-I-Carbonell and
Frijters, 2004). Fleurbaey and Schwandt (2015) find that a significant
portion of people try to maximize their subjective well-being. However,
they also discover the ’paradox of happiness’- those who try to maximize
their subjective well being have a lower level of satisfaction then those who
maximize some other goals. One of the reason for this paradox might be
due to the inability of these people to actually reach a maximum level
3It can also be from 0-7 or any other number depending on the questionnaire
2
of satisfaction while the latter by selecting objective goals might become
comparatively happier on achieving such a goal.
The literature on the relationship between religion and subjective well
being is also abundant. We have already mentioned some texts in the In-
troduction of this paper which give a brief review of the entire literature on
religion and subjective well-being. Additionally, Helliwell (2003) using the
World Values Survey finds a positive spillover effects of weekly church at-
tendance and the well being of others at the national level. This essentially
can be viewed as a positive externality of being religious.4
The fact that spiritual people are more satisfied in life, raises an interest-
ing question, which to the knowledge of the author has not been attempted
to answer- Is the satisfaction of spiritual people ’robust’ to shocks in the
external environment, i.e., is the subjective well being of these people not
as much affected by the shocks as those who consider themselves as being
non-spiritual.
3 Data
I am using data from the European Social Survey for all the waves be-
tween 2002 to 2016. The ESS data is collected with two year intervals
which means we have eight rounds of the survey results. I merged all
the data and our required variables to create a big dataset. I only kept the
variables which very available for all the rounds and were related our depen-
dent variable (life satisfaction). This list of variables includes religiousness
(spirituality), marital status, education (self, father and mother), age and
gender.
Subjective well-being or life satisfaction is measured using the variable
called happiness. In the survey the following question is asked: Taking
all things together, how happy would you say you are?. The respondents
are asked to choose on a scale from zero to 10 where the former means
”Extemely unhappy” and the latter means ”Extremely Happy”. Figure 1
summaries the variable for the waves of the ESS data. The distribution
4The estimates are not causal
3
in 2002 is more titled towards the happy side and in the consecutive years
remains similar (more or less).
Figure 1: Figure 1
Our another variable of interest is the variable which measures spiritu-
ality. The question asked to the respondent is: Regardless of whether you
belong to a particular religion, how religious would you say you are?. The
respondents are asked to choose on a scale from 0 to 10 where 0 means
”Not at all religious” and 10 means ”Very Religious”. Since this question
is asked to people regardless of whether they follow a particular religion
or not, I have taken this variable to mean as the self reported measure of
spirituality. Table 1 gives a summary statistic of this variable from 2002-
2016. It reports the percentage distribution of people within the categories
of this variable for a particular religion. For example, in 2004 around 7%
of the people reported that they are very religious while 12% of the people
said that they are very religious.
Summary statistics of education(self, father and mother) and legal mari-
tal status is provided in the appendix of this paper. I have harmonized the
data5as per the first round (2002) and use statistical weights in reporting
all the Summary Tables and also in the regression.
5For example marital status had only 5 categories in 2002 but in 2008 it had 8 categories.
So I bought all the other years to the same level as in 2002. I did a similar coding for the
education variable.
4
Table 1: Summary Statistics- How Religious Are You?
year
2002 2004 2006 2008 2010 2012 2014 2016 Total
% % % % % % % % %
Not at all religious 13.1 11.9 14.6 12.7 14.2 14.5 19.0 17.4 14.5
1 5.0 4.9 5.8 5.4 5.7 5.4 5.9 5.7 5.5
2 7.1 6.5 7.9 6.8 7.6 7.1 7.2 7.6 7.2
3 8.1 7.8 8.6 8.2 8.9 8.2 7.9 8.8 8.3
4 6.6 6.8 7.2 6.6 6.9 6.5 6.7 6.5 6.7
5 17.3 18.3 18.6 17.9 18.6 17.0 14.9 15.8 17.4
6 10.7 10.2 9.2 9.7 9.8 10.4 9.3 9.8 9.9
7 11.8 11.4 10.5 11.8 11.4 10.9 10.4 10.7 11.2
8 10.1 10.4 9.1 10.5 8.8 10.1 9.1 8.9 9.7
9 4.4 4.8 3.4 4.3 3.7 4.0 3.8 3.7 4.0
Very religious 5.8 6.8 5.0 6.0 4.3 6.0 5.7 4.9 5.6
Total 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0
4 Empirical Specifications and Discussions
4.1 Regressions
My analysis relies on the following specifications:
happyijt =β0+α1crisisyeart+α2rlgdgrij t +λcrisisyeartrldgrijt +β1Xit +it
(1)
where
happyijt = Measures the subjective well-being of individual i in coun-
try j at year t
crisisyeart= Is a dummy for the crisis year (2010 and 2012 waves
corresponding to the crisis)
rlgdgrij t = Measures the spirituality of individual i in country j at
year t
X0
ijt controls for education (self, father and mother), age, gender and
legal marital status
In the given equation our variable of interest is λ. We can interpret the
coefficient in the above given equation as an average difference between
life satisfaction and happiness levels of those with marginally higher level
of spirituality in a crisis year against the marginal happiness of spiritual
people in non-crisis year.
5
Despite controlling for all the stuff, using a panel data allows us to con-
trol for more heterogeneity. We can control for country fixed and year fixed
effects. This will control for all the time invariant region specific or time
specific effects and therefore our estimates will be closer to the ’real esti-
mates’ for the population. We do this in the following specification where ηt
and ηjcontrol for the time fixed and the country fixed effects respectively.
happyijt =β0+ηt+ηj+α1rlgdgrijt +λcrisisyeartrldgrijt +β1Xit +it (2)
Before presenting the results a note of caution. The estimates from these
equation cannot be treated as causal in nature. The equation even after
controlling for fixed effects suffers from the problems of omitted variable
bias. It might be the case people’s personal behaviour is such that they
are less prone to external shocks and therefore might stay happier even in
times of crisis. Since we cannot control for individual types, this violates
the assumption of the exogeniety of the error terms. We will see this
effect in our regression equation via our variable of interest. Therefore, the
estimates from our regression will be biased upwards.
4.2 Results and Discussion
The results from our specification are shown in Table 2. On an average
people report lower satisfaction levels in the crisis year which is intuitive
as people are negatively affected by the crisis which should reflect on their
subjective well-being. I also find that on an average spiritual people are
more happier than the less spiritual people. It is important to mention
here that we are using a normal OLS and not ordinal logit as in the case of
subjective well-being both provide similar results (Ferrer-I-Carbonell and
Frijters, 2004).
As expected, λhas a positive sign and the coefficient is statistically
significant. The coefficient is probably biased upwards as argued in the
previous subsection. Moreover, adding country fixed effects and year fixed
effects decreases the magnitude of the coefficient which is further supports
the idea that the value is biased upwards.
But why does a positive coefficient make sense? There are a couple of
reasons that I can think of. Firstly, people might have started becoming
6
Table 2: Results
(1) (2)
happy happy
Subjective Well Being
Crisis Year -0.115∗∗∗
(0.0278)
Spirituality 0.0272∗∗∗ 0.0602∗∗∗
(0.00275) (0.00274)
Crisis Year*Sprituality 0.0248∗∗∗ 0.0108
(0.00508) (0.00494)
Gender -0.0296-0.0409∗∗
(0.0137) (0.0129)
Calculated Age -0.00290∗∗∗ -0.0109∗∗∗
(0.000519) (0.000504)
Legal Marital Status
Married (Base Category)
Separated -0.968∗∗∗ -1.052∗∗∗
(0.0650) (0.0619)
Divorced -0.830∗∗∗ -0.815∗∗∗
(0.0273) (0.0256)
Widowed -1.086∗∗∗ -0.784∗∗∗
(0.0302) (0.0289)
Never Married -0.130∗∗∗ -0.375∗∗∗
(0.0186) (0.0178)
Highest Level of Education
Less than lower secondary education (Base Category)
Lower secondary education completed 0.129∗∗∗ 0.0643
(0.0288) (0.0277)
Upper secondary education completed -0.115∗∗∗ 0.00602
(0.0197) (0.0189)
Post-secondary non-tertiary education 0.255∗∗∗ 0.0533
(0.0284) (0.0269)
Tertiary education completed -0.155∗∗∗ 0.106∗∗∗
(0.0290) (0.0274)
Father’s Education YES YES
Mother’s Education YES YES
Year Fixed Effects No YES
Country Fixed Effects No YES
Number of Observations 285153 285153
R20.041 0.155
Robust standard errors in parentheses
p < 0.05, ∗∗ p < 0.01, ∗∗∗ p < 0.001 7
more spiritual after a shock in their personal life history to protect their
subjective well being in the future from such external shocks. In other
words, people might have self selected themselves to becoming spiritual in
order to escape the hardships of this world. Therefore, the people who
believe that they are more spiritual will by construction be happier.
Secondly, the entire philosophy behind spirituality is looking inwards and
protecting yourself from external desires. Therefore, an external shock like
an Economic Crisis will have lesser impact on these people compared to
others who are not spiritual.
More education is positively related to happiness. But this sign and
estimate can also be because of the income a person. It can be the case that
more educated people are more likely to be employed and therefore might
earn higher income. And, according to a study conducted by Frijters et al.
(2004) income and life satisfaction are positively correlated. Therefore, the
part of the magnitude of the coefficient that we see in Table 2 can be via
the income channel.
The base category for legal marital status is ’Married’ as shown in Table
2. Compared to Married, every other category has lower subjective life
satisfaction. This can be because of lesser number of observations for ’Sep-
arated’, ’Divorced’ and ’Widowed’. But what the data suggests that on an
average in Europe, married people generally report higher life satisfaction
as compared to ’Separated’, ’Divorced’ ’Widowed’ and ’Never Married’.
Once again the estimates from the estimation are not causal in nature.
Apart from this, I also do a difference in difference type of estimation
where I consider a person to be spiritual if her reported subjective happiness
is greater than 6. The results from this estimation are given in Table 3.
The results are similar to Table 2. In this case λcan be interpreted in the
following way: the average difference between the subjective life satisfaction
of the spiritual people and the non-spiritual people after the crisis year
minus the average difference between the life satisfaction between spiritual
and non-spiritual before the crisis is positive. This essentially means that
the life satisfaction of spiritual people is higher than non-spiritual people
even during a crisis year.
8
Table 3: Results: Difference in Difference Type of Estimation
(1) (2)
happy happy
Subjective Well Being
Crisis Year -0.0519∗∗ 0.264∗∗∗
(0.0183) (0.0264)
Spirituality 0.242∗∗∗ 0.363∗∗∗
(0.0168) (0.0163)
Crisis Year*Sprituality 0.164∗∗∗ 0.0792
(0.0327) (0.0313)
Gender -0.0308-0.0562∗∗∗
(0.0136) (0.0128)
Calculated Age -0.00292∗∗∗ -0.0107∗∗∗
(0.000517) (0.000503)
Legal Marital Status
Married (Base Category)
Separated -0.961∗∗∗ -1.057∗∗∗
(0.0647) (0.0616)
Divorced -0.831∗∗∗ -0.827∗∗∗
(0.0272) (0.0255)
Widowed -1.092∗∗∗ -0.791∗∗∗
(0.0301) (0.0288)
Never Married -0.132∗∗∗ -0.384∗∗∗
(0.0185) (0.0177)
Highest Level of Education
Less than lower secondary education (Base Category)
Lower secondary education completed 0.129∗∗∗ 0.0599
(0.0287) (0.0277)
Upper secondary education completed -0.115∗∗∗ 0.00174
(0.0196) (0.0189)
Post-secondary non-tertiary education 0.249∗∗∗ 0.0467
(0.0285) (0.0272)
Tertiary education completed -0.154∗∗∗ 0.105∗∗∗
(0.0291) (0.0277)
Father’s Education YES YES
Mother’s Education YES YES
Year Fixed Effects No YES
Country Fixed Effects No YES
Number of Observations 287329 287329
R20.042 0.154
Robust standard errors in parentheses
p < 0.05, ∗∗ p < 0.01, ∗∗∗ p < 0.001 9
5 Conclusion
In this paper, I find that spiritual people are more likely to report higher
life satisfaction in times of an economic crisis than the group of people who
do not consider themselves spiritual. I argue that the results, though not
causal, are intuitive and logical. I use a difference in difference type of
approach to answer this problem.
However, in order to correctly identify the problem a survey that would
track the individuals across time would be very useful. In such a survey we
could also control for time invariant individual specific effects which will
bring the estimates closer to the ’actual’ estimate.
10
References
Sven E. Erlandson. Spiritual but not religious: a call to religious revolution
in America. Writers Showcase, 2000.
Ada Ferrer-I-Carbonell and Paul Frijters. How important is methodology
for the estimates of the determinants of happiness?*. The Economic
Journal, 114(497):641–659, 2004.
A L Ferriss. Religion and the quality of life. Journal of Happiness Studies,
3(3):199–215, Sep 2002. URL https://link.springer.com/article/
10.1023/A:1020684404438.
Marc Fleurbaey and Hannes Schwandt. Do people seek to maximize their
subjective well-being –and fail? Working paper IZA DP No. 9450, 2015.
Paul Frijters, John P. Haisken-DeNew, and Michael A. Shields. Money
does matter! evidence from increasing real income and life satisfaction
in east germany following reunification. American Economic Review, 94
(3):730–740, June 2004. doi: 10.1257/0002828041464551. URL http:
//www.aeaweb.org/articles?id=10.1257/0002828041464551.
John F Helliwell. How’s life? combining individual and national vari-
ables to explain subjective well-being. Economic Modelling, 20(2):331 –
360, 2003. ISSN 0264-9993. URL âĂćttp://www.sciencedirect.com/
science/article/pii/S0264999302000573. Henry Special Issue.
European Social Survey. Ess data rounds 1-8. Norwegian Centre for Re-
search Data, Norway, 2002-2016.
W R Swinyard, A K Kau, and H Y Phua. Happiness, materialism, and re-
ligious experience in the us and singapore. Journal of Happiness Studies,
2:13–32, 2001.
11
Appendix
Table 4: Summary Statistics- Highest level of education
year
2002 2004 2006 2008 2010 2012 2014 2016 Total
% % % % % % % % %
Less than lower secondary education (ISCED 0-1) 17.2 21.8 12.1 14.4 6.5 5.9 7.4 5.3 11.4
Lower secondary education completed (ISCED 2) 21.9 18.2 16.3 16.0 15.2 15.7 17.6 12.9 16.6
Upper secondary education completed (ISCED 3) 37.7 35.0 36.6 34.0 37.2 35.0 36.6 33.1 35.6
Post-secondary non-tertiary education completed (ISCED 4) 2.5 2.6 2.5 3.1 3.8 4.0 5.6 4.2 3.4
Tertiary education completed (ISCED 5-6) 20.7 22.4 32.6 32.5 37.4 39.4 32.7 44.5 33.0
Total 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0
Table 5: Summary Statistics- Father’s Highest level of education?
year
2002 2004 2006 2008 2010 2012 2014 2016 Total
% % % % % % % % %
Less than lower secondary education (ISCED 0-1) 39.9 37.6 30.4 38.0 24.2 25.0 26.5 20.7 30.7
Lower secondary education completed (ISCED 2) 16.8 20.7 21.2 15.6 19.5 19.7 18.5 18.2 18.7
Upper secondary education completed (ISCED 3) 28.9 26.1 26.5 24.5 29.2 27.7 31.6 29.6 27.7
Post-secondary non-tertiary education completed (ISCED 4) 0.9 1.0 1.0 2.2 2.4 2.7 3.3 2.0 1.9
Tertiary education completed (ISCED 5-6) 13.5 14.6 20.8 19.7 24.7 24.9 20.1 29.5 21.0
Total 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0
Table 6: Summary Statistics- Mother’s highest level of education?
year
2002 2004 2006 2008 2010 2012 2014 2016 Total
% % % % % % % % %
Less than lower secondary education (ISCED 0-1) 44.7 41.9 32.8 41.1 26.7 27.8 30.0 22.1 33.7
Lower secondary education completed (ISCED 2) 25.4 27.3 26.1 19.0 23.8 22.4 23.6 21.1 23.4
Upper secondary education completed (ISCED 3) 22.5 21.4 22.7 21.6 25.2 25.9 29.5 28.0 24.2
Post-secondary non-tertiary education completed (ISCED 4) 0.9 0.9 0.9 1.8 2.2 2.5 3.2 2.2 1.8
Tertiary education completed (ISCED 5-6) 6.4 8.6 17.5 16.6 22.1 21.4 13.8 26.5 16.9
Total 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0
12
Table 7: Summary Statistics- Marital Status
year
2002 2004 2006 2008 2010 2012 2014 2016 Total
% % % % % % % % %
Married 58.6 58.1 57.4 58.0 55.2 55.2 55.9 54.0 56.5
Separated 1.7 1.3 1.3 1.3 0.5 0.5 0.4 0.4 0.9
Divorced 5.2 5.8 7.2 6.9 9.0 8.7 8.3 9.5 7.7
Widowed 7.0 7.4 8.6 8.1 7.8 7.4 6.0 6.9 7.5
Never married 27.6 27.4 25.5 25.7 27.5 28.2 29.3 29.3 27.4
Total 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0
13
... Another benefit of religion is the psychological comfort it offers especially in times when immigrants are faced with hard times in a new land (Mariya & Chiswick, 2011). The mere belief that there is a God who is in control of every situation that's the foundation of Christian faith tends to gain ground in hard times than when people are not faced with any troubles in life (Naik, 2018). Since the immigrants in their countries of origin have no much social problems, the degree of European Journal of Philosophy, Culture and Religious Studies ISSN 2520-0445 (Online) Vol.4, ...
... Religious freedom in most host countries has contributed to high degree of immigrant religiosity. In home countries, immigrant religiosity maintains at a stagnant or even waning state mainly because religious views are not put to taste given that the cultural structures available complement religious traditions (Naik, 2018). When immigrants are in host countries, things are different because in the new land the cultural practices perceived to be as normal are absent. ...
Article
This study aimed to point out the differences between the religiosity of immigrants and natives and how they hinder or facilitate immigrant social integration into the host society. The study took a multi-national perspective as the basis for analyzing religious views within Europe whereby both the natives and immigrants in European countries are evaluated and explanations for individual groups’ integration trajectories are emphasized. With respect to a thorough scholarly analysis, this study found out that the religiosity of immigrants tends to be high than that of natives in most European countries. It also noted that the initial religiosity of immigrants drops with an increase in the duration of time the individual immigrant stays in the host country. This study makes available knowledge about religious differences in a cross-cultural perspective and strives to help sociologists in outlining the differences in order to help in studying behavior patterns in different cultural settings.
Article
Full-text available
The western happiness scale measures those material aspects, so it will not be objective when applied in low-income countries. Furthermore, Countries with high happiness ratings, which may ignore the importance of spirituality, have significantly higher rates of depression and suicide. Islam is the highest source of spirituality for its adherents. They believe happiness cannot only be measured by the material aspect and ignore the spiritual part. This study aims to develop a happiness scale that measures Muslim spiritual-material aspects. We used a development research approach to design an instrument based on the thoughts of Abdullah bin Abbas, test its validity, and estimate its reliability. The authors found Seven sub-scales and thirty-one valid items for Muslim spiritual happiness. The current instrument of happiness can be used to measure the spiritual happiness of Muslims. The authors expected that these findings encourage other researchers to improve the current version and develop a better and more valid instrument.
Article
The contemporary Finland is more culturally diverse than previous years thanks to increased international migration. A large number of immigrants entering Finland today are religious in one way or another. This article is a case study of religious beliefs and experiences of protestant Christian immigrants in Finland with the aim of finding out the personal feelings of immigrants towards the Finnish society. A comparative analysis of Protestant Christian immigrants’ experiences in both the host country and country of origin was taken as a foundation for determining effects of the immigrants’ religious beliefs in Finland. This study takes a cross-cultural perspective in analyzing and evaluating the religious beliefs of immigrants. Even though religion is credited to have an upper hand in facilitating social integration of immigrants into host societies, this study found out that religious beliefs and experiences of protestant Christian immigrants in Finland can be integrating or alienating, depending on the degree of religiosity of the immigrants. An awareness of this observation as discussed in this article goes a long way in providing options to choose from in navigating the common problems faced by Protestant Christian immigrants in Finland.
Article
Full-text available
In this paper we investigate how life satisfaction (or happiness) is affected by a substantial increase in real income. Our context is East Germany in the decade following reunification, and we implement a new fixed-effect estimator for ordinal life satisfaction and develop a decomposition approach that accounts for new entrants and panel attrition. Using data from the German Socio-Economic Panel we find that average life satisfaction in East Germany increased by around 20% between 1991 and 2001, leading to a clear convergence with West Germany. Importantly, increased real household incomes in East Germany accounted for around 35-40% of this increase, which corresponds to the economists' view that money surely matters.
Article
In a new survey we ask respondents, after a standard Subjective Well‐Being (SWB) question, if they can think of changes in their lives that would improve their SWB score. If the SWB score is just one argument among others in the respondents’ goals in life, they should easily find ways to improve it, at the expense of other dimensions they care about. Our results suggest that close to 90% of the respondents actually seek to maximize their SWB. The life satisfaction question appears the best contender as the “maximand” in the contest, before the ladder‐of‐life question and felt happiness. Among the other goals that people pursue and for which they are willing to sacrifice some of their SWB, the prominent appear to be about their relatives and about their future self.
Article
This paper attempts to explain international trends and differences in subjective well-being over the final fifth of the twentieth century. This is done in several stages. First there is a brief review of some reasons for giving a central role to subjective measures of well-being. This is followed by sections containing a survey of earlier empirical studies, a description of the main variables used in this study, a report of results and tests, discussion of the links among social capital, education and well-being, and concluding comments. The main innovation of the paper, relative to earlier studies of subjective well-being, lies in its use of large international samples of individual respondents, thus permitting the simultaneous identification of individual-level and societal-level determinants of well-being. This is particularly useful in identifying direct and indirect linkages between social capital and well-being.
Article
Many empirical studies are ambiguous about whether good formal institutions are conducive to subjective well-being or not. Possibly, this ambiguity is caused by cross-section models that do not account for unobserved cultural and institutional effects. Using the World Value Survey 1980-2005, this paper supports a positive relation in a country panel framework that accounts for unobserved, time-invariant country heterogeneity. This study also shows that using supra-national region dummies (by geography or language) in a country-random effects model appears to be a sufficient substitution for omitted country fixed effects.
Article
This study examines relationships between three central human concepts. From national probability samples of adults in the United States and in Singapore, we measured happiness, materialism (operationalized as the three sub-scales of possession-defined success, acquisition centrality, and acquisition as the pursuit of happiness), and religious experience (using three scales measuring intrinsic religiosity, extrinsic religiosity, and religion as quest). It was expected and found that happiness is negatively related to overall materialism in both the US and in Singapore, although happiness" relationship with the three materialism sub-scales was mixed. We observed that adults in Singapore are less happy and more materialistic than those in the US It was expected that happiness would have a positive relationship with intrinsic religiosity and extrinsic religiosity, but a negative relationship with religion as quest. Though our results largely support these hypotheses, they produce some unexpected differences between the two countries and across the three religiosity dimensions. We conclude that happiness is not associated with people's material accumulation but with their perceived inner world. And happy people see their religion not so much as something they “do” as what they “are”.
Article
Psychologists and sociologists usually interpret happiness scores as cardinal and comparable across respondents, and thus run OLS regressions on happiness and changes in happiness. Economists usually assume only ordinality and have mainly used ordered latent response models, thereby not taking satisfactory account of fixed individual traits. We address this problem by developing a conditional estimator for the fixed-effect ordered logit model. We find that assuming ordinality or cardinality of happiness scores makes little difference, whilst allowing for fixed-effects does change results substantially. We call for more research into the determinants of the personality traits making up these fixed-effects. Copyright 2004 Royal Economic Society.
Spiritual but not religious: a call to religious revolution in America
  • Sven E Erlandson
Sven E. Erlandson. Spiritual but not religious: a call to religious revolution in America. Writers Showcase, 2000.