ArticlePDF Available
OPINION
published: 20 September 2018
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01672
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 1September 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 1672
Edited by:
Wenfeng Chen,
Renmin University of China, China
Reviewed by:
Chris N. H. Street,
University of Huddersfield,
United Kingdom
Cristina Scarpazza,
Università degli Studi di Padova, Italy
*Correspondence:
Judee K. Burgoon
judee@email.arizona.edu
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Emotion Science,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received: 21 June 2018
Accepted: 20 August 2018
Published: 20 September 2018
Citation:
Burgoon JK (2018) Microexpressions
Are Not the Best Way to Catch a Liar.
Front. Psychol. 9:1672.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01672
Microexpressions Are Not the Best
Way to Catch a Liar
Judee K. Burgoon*
Center for the Management of Information, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ, United States
Keywords: lying, deception, deception detection, microexpression, emotion, social signals, rigidity
Microexpressions are lauded as a valid and reliable means of catching liars (see Porter and ten
Brinke, 2010). However, there are many reasons to question what I will call microexpression theory
(MET).
For MET to be supported, several propositions must hold true: One, deception produces
internal negative emotional experiences. Two, these internal experiences have associated outward
expressions, including microexpressions. Three, microexpressions are uncontrollable. Four, these
expressions are reliable and valid indicators of deception. Five, microexpressions occur frequently
enough to be detectable. Six, detected microexpressions successfully distinguish truth from
deception. Let me address each of these propositions in turn. I will then offer an alternative theory
accounting for truth versus deception indicators under multiple circumstances.
DOES DECEPTION PRODUCE NEGATIVE EMOTIONAL
EXPERIENCES?
During this discussion, I distinguish among emotional experiences, emotional expressions, and
felt emotions. Emotional experiences are internal events, not measured directly but inferred
from measurable physiological states, and labeled as emotional expressions. Felt emotions are
an individual’s subjective reporting of one’s state. Another important distinction is between
macro- [>0.5 s; see (Yan et al., 2013)] and microexpressions. Most of the research on emotions
during deception examines macro displays rather than fleeting microexpressions (e.g., DePaulo
et al., 2003; Warren et al., 2009; Okubo et al., 2012; ten Brinke et al., 2012). Ability to predict
deception from the former does not equal ability to predict deception from the latter.
So, yes, deception can produce the prototypical negative felt emotions of guilt, shame, sadness,
and fear; but it can also produce positive emotions of relief, enjoyment, pleasure, and what is called
duping delight—pleasure at succeeding with one’s lies (Ekman and Friesen, 1969). Additionally,
prevaricators may experience arousal alone, without a felt emotion, or a blend of emotions,
producing complex displays that range from non-aroused to highly aroused and unpleasant to
pleasant (Burgoon et al., 1989). If stakes are low, liars may not experience any particular emotion,
making them indistinguishable behaviorally from truth-tellers (Hartwig and Bond, 2011).
DO DECEPTION-INSTIGATED EMOTIONAL EXPERIENCES HAVE
ASSOCIATED OUTWARD DISPLAYS?
The Ekman Facial Action Coding System (FACS) specifies particular Action Units (AUs) of
the face that are associated with each emotion. For example, fear entails seven different facial
muscle movements. However, as Barrett (2006) concluded, felt emotions lack a one-to-one
correspondence with emotional expressions; there are no unique and specific somatovisceral
changes that correspond to specific emotional expressions. Put another way, a single felt emotion
can give rise to multiple expressions, and multiple felt emotions can give rise to the same expression.
Burgoon Microexpressions Not Best
For example, felt fear can be displayed as anxiety, anger,
contempt, or surprise. An experiment intending to examine the
coherence between subjective emotions and prototypical AUs
for fear showed participants the iconic scene from the movie
The Shining of axe-wielding Jack Nicholson about to attack his
terrified wife hiding in the bathroom (Fernández-Dols et al.,
1997). Of 14 participants reporting exclusively disgust and 2,
fear, only 1 and 2, respectively, showed the AUs associated with
those felt emotions. Of 11 reporting surprise, none displayed
the prototypical display. This study offered strong demonstration
that felt and expressed emotions are often not aligned. Elsewhere,
both a frustrating task and a delightful one elicited smiling from
participants (Hoque et al., 2012).
Only at the risk of false positives, then, can one infer backward
from observed displays that the sender is lying. Put differently,
deception does not reliably produce negative emotions and
negative emotions do not reliably signal deception.
ARE DECEPTION-INSTIGATED
MICROEXPRESSIONS
UNCONTROLLABLE?
Some are. But they appear to be partial or longer than the
traditional definition of a microexpression. In an investigation
of countermeasures, Hurley and Frank (2011) found that liars
could not completely inhibit eyebrow or lip corner movement
despite instructions to do so during a mock crime interrogation.
In a comprehensive analysis of 78 public pleas for the return of
missing children, deceivers failed to simulate sadness and grief
to the degree truthful pleaders did and they leaked displays of
happiness (ten Brinke et al., 2012). These, however, were full
emotional expressions lasting nearly a second or longer. The
microexpressions did not differ between truth and deception.
The analysis of 1,711 expressions revealed that high-intensity
emotions were harder to conceal than low-intensity ones during
masking. There were no cases of complete microexpressions
(simultaneously present in the upper and lower face), and only
25% of the participants displayed the partial expressions (Porter
et al., 2012).
Other expressions, such as the eyebrow flash or contempt, are
inherently social, voluntary, and controllable (Grammer et al.,
1988). As social signals, they are responsive to context. This is
a central reason why microexpressions are a poor telltale sign of
lying, because they can be masked, minimized, exaggerated, or
neutralized, especially during deception (Ekman, 2009). Masking
involves disguising felt emotions, as in replacing fear with a show
of defiance. Minimizing involves suppressing the intensity of an
expression. Exaggeration entails intensifying an expression, as in
dialing up a display of surprise when accused of a transgression.
Neutralizing entails concealing a felt emotion by eliminating its
outward expression entirely.
This management of facial expressions follows what Ekman
and Friesen (1969) labeled display rules. Cultures designate what
emotions are appropriate, sanctioned, or punished if displayed;
how such displays should appear; and the consequences of
their display. This designation of social signals during deception
has received considerable scholarly attention (Driskell et al.,
2012). Buller and Burgoon (1994, 1997) labeled such regulation
of deception expressions as strategic communication. Their
argument, bolstered by numerous investigations, is that much of
our nonverbal behavioral repertoire is manageable and managed.
More broadly, because facial expressions are part of a
social signal system, they fulfill a variety of functions beyond
simply revealing one’s internal emotional reactions (e.g., Buck,
1991; Chovil, 1991; Buck et al., 1992; Barrett, 2006). During
social interaction, individuals purposely regulate and withhold
expressions of felt emotions, and they enact expressions of
emotions they do not feel. “Emotional expressions, then, can
be used purposely in deception to communicate symbolically
information that has very little to do with the communicators’
felt emotions” (Buller and Burgoon, 1994, p. 387).
HOW RELIABLE AND VALID ARE
MICROEXPRESSIONS AS INDICATORS OF
DECEPTION?
High stakes circumstances may prompt some leakage, though
not necessarily of microexpressions. ten Brinke and Porter (2012)
found that liars pleading for the return of their missing children
displayed more upper face surprise and lower face happiness
than truthful pleaders, making these expressions candidates for
detecting deception. “The ‘grief’ muscles (corrugator supercilii,
depressor anguli oris) were more often contracted in the faces
of genuine than deceptive pleaders. Subtle contraction of the
zygomatic major (masking smiles) and full contraction of the
frontalis (failed attempts to appear sad) muscles were more
commonly identified in the faces of deceptive pleaders” [(ten
Brinke et al., 2012), p. 411].
Contrariwise, Pentland et al. (2015) found during a Concealed
Information Test (CIT) that deceptive (guilty) respondents
exhibited less contempt and more intense smiles than truthful
(innocent) respondents. Contempt and less intense smiling
would be expected of liars, not truth-tellers. Numerous studies
have found that deceivers often show appeasement or fake
smiles that can be mistaken as signals of pleasure, comfort,
or enjoyment. In their experiment comparing cheaters (who
lied) with cooperators (who were truthful), Okubo et al. (2012)
concluded that, “cheater detection based on the processing
of negative facial expressions can be thwarted by a posed or
fake smile, which cheaters put on with higher intensity than
cooperators” (p. 217).
These patterns would result in false positives. Thus, some
expressions like smiling are not uniquely associated with
deception, and some emotional expressions—both micro and
macro—can be associated with either truth or deception.
DO MICROEXPRESSIONS OCCUR WITH
ENOUGH REGULARITY TO BE
DETECTABLE?
False negatives are commonplace. In one of the very few
investigations of microexpression frequency, Porter and
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 2September 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 1672
Burgoon Microexpressions Not Best
ten Brinke (2008) coded 700 high-stakes genuine and
falsified emotional expressions and found only 2% were
microexpressions. Their subsequent analysis of these high-stakes
pleadings found only six instances of microexpressions among
deceivers and slightly more (8) among genuine pleaders (ten
Brinke and Porter, 2012), obviating the role of microexpressions
as sufficiently frequent or exclusive to catch deception. Legions
of law enforcement personnel and airport security Behavioral
Detection Officers have been trained to look for microexpressions
as “telltale” signs of nefarious intent. However, testimony to the
U.S. Congress revealed that only 0.6% out of 61,000 passenger
referrals to law enforcement in 2011 and 2012 resulted in arrests
(U.S. Government Accountability Office, 2013), and a 2017
ACLU report concluded the behavioral observation approach
was based on biased, weak, and junk science (Cushing, 2017).
DO DETECTED MICROEXPRESSIONS
SUCCESSFULLY DISTINGUISH TRUTH
FROM DECEPTION?
Recent empirical results are not encouraging. Porter et al. (2012)
found that untrained observers were unable to distinguish truth
from deception. In particular, they could not distinguish sadness,
fear or disgust—all emotions thought to be associated with
deception. Pentland et al. (2015) found during a CIT that
deceivers’ and truth-tellers smiling intensity was essentially the
same on target questions and only differed on neutral questions.
Between-subjects classification accuracy was below 56%. These
kinds of results led my colleagues and me to seek an alternative
approach.
ALTERNATIVE THEORY: THE RIGIDITY
EFFECT
An alternative hypothesis that offers a reliable and valid
set of indicators is what has been called the rigidity effect
(RE). RE postulates that extemporaneous deception under high
stakes leads to an initial freeze response. In interpersonal
deception theory (IDT), Buller and Burgoon (1996) (Burgoon,
2014, 2015a,b; see also Burgoon et al., 2014) argued that
deceivers attempt to manage their nonverbal behavior and
overall image so as to appear credible (strategic communication)
while simultaneously attempting to control behaviors that are
detrimental to their performance (non-strategic behavior). If
efforts to appear natural, expressive, and relaxed are overridden
by attempts to suppress signs of discomfiture, the overcontrol will
backfire.
Early research focused on gestural and postural activity.
Zuckerman et al. (1981) theorized that contrary to the stereotype
of liars being fidgety and nervous during an interview, liars
may adopt a stiff, wooden posture due to overcontrol. Several
experiments confirmed that deceivers reduced their gestural,
foot, and overall kinesic animation relative to truthtellers (e.g.,
Buller and Aune, 1987; DePaulo et al., 2003; Caso et al.,
2006; Mullin, 2012), suggestive of participants seeking to limit
incriminating behaviors.
As automated measurement advanced, more investigations
pursued dynamic ocular and facial displays of emotion. These,
too, showed the RE pattern of depressed activity. Studies of blink
rates regularly found inhibition of blinking during deception
(Leal and Vrij, 2008). Hurley and Frank (2011) found that
suppressing a given facial emotion during deception resulted in
suppressing all facial expressions. Pentland et al. (2014, 2015,
2016) and Pentland and Zhang (2016) found deceivers reduced
several emotion-relevant face and head movements.
Two experiments (Pentland et al., 2017) tested the RE
directly with videotapes from high-stakes experiments in
which emotional and microexpressions were captured with
the Computer Expression Recognition Toolkit. In the first
experiment, guilty subjects completing a CIT (which controls
for cognitive load) showed far less variance in four deception-
relevant emotions (disgust, fear, sadness, and surprise) than
did innocent subjects when responding to target questions. In
the second experiment, which measured variance in 10 facial
movements, the guilty (deceivers) showed REs on 8 during
presentation of target images. In a separate test (Twyman et al.,
2015), rigidity persisted despite participants being asked to
employ countermeasures to offset it.
CAUSAL MECHANISMS
A question that needs to be resolved is the causal mechanisms
that produce the RE. Does it reflect an involuntary reflex and
defensive reaction associated with flight or fight (Twyman et al.,
2015), a cessation of activity due to too much demand on
cognitive resources and working memory (ten Brinke and Porter,
2012; Sporer, 2016), a temporary orientation response while
absorbing contextual information (Le Poire and Burgoon, 1996),
or intentional behavioral control and impression management
(Grazioli et al., 2006; Lee et al., 2009; Twyman et al., 2011)?
Sporer (2013, 2016) and Twyman et al. (2014) nicely articulates
these alternatives. If the inhibition of movement1is temporary
while the deceiver decides how to respond, it may better reflect
an adaptive response in line with IDT that is only evident if
there is sufficient time for dynamics to be observed (Duran
et al., 2013). (This would make rigidity a misnomer.) As already
noted, immobility also is affected by distressful emotions (e.g.,
fear, anxiety, shame) and grave consequences (e.g., physical
pain, financial loss, imprisonment) and by opportunities and
cognitive resources for preparation. Speculatively, it can be
hypothesized that degree of behavioral inhibition will be positively
related to ones emotional distress and the severity of the stakes
involved and inversely related to opportunity to plan and adapt
responses.
Needed are experiments that tease out these effects and
identify significant moderators. Methods like the cognitive
interview [(Köhnken et al., 1999; Vrij, 2015); but cf. (Levine
et al., 2018)] to induce higher cognitive load may further test
cognitive resources, and the Twyman et al. (2015) experiments
1This is not to be confused with Darwin’s inhibition hypothesis that deceivers
fail to inhibit some emotional aspects—leakage—while also failing to adequately
simulate other aspects of genuine emotional expressions.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 3September 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 1672
Burgoon Microexpressions Not Best
employing various countermeasures may control and test
alternative influences articulated in Burgoon (2015a,b). The
increased complexity despite reduced displacement found by
Duran et al. (2013) also forecasts the importance of dynamic as
well as static facets of emotions.
SUMMARY
The time has come to look beyond fleeting, infrequent and
minuscule emotional expressions to movements themselves and
not to their presence but their absence. Deception produces
positive as well as negative emotional experiences and sometimes
no emotions at all. Felt emotions do not have a one-to-one
correspondence to outward expressions, and microexpressions
are especially rare, leading to false negatives and false positives.
Discerning initial rigidity and temporal patterning of facial
behavior may greatly increase the viability of facial movements
in catching a liar.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and
approved it for publication.
FUNDING
Preparation of this editorial was sponsored, in part, by the Army
Research Office and was accomplished under Grant Number
W911NF-16-1-0342. The views and conclusions contained in this
document are those of the author and should not be interpreted
as representing the official policies, either expressed or implied,
of the Army Research Office or the U.S. Government. The U.S.
Government is authorized to reproduce and distribute reprints
for Government purposes notwithstanding any copyright
notation herein.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The author thanks David Buller for his early collaboration on
deception and emotion, the U. S. Air Force Office of Scientific
Research and the U. S. National Science Foundation for their
support of the foundational work on interpersonal deception
theory, and Jay Nunamaker and the cadre of exceptional graduate
students at the University of Arizona for their contributions
to the research program on automated detection of deception.
Special acknowledgment goes to Nathan Twyman and Steve
Pentland for their elaboration and pursuit of the rigidity effect.
REFERENCES
Barrett, L. F. (2006). Solving the emotion paradox: categorization and
the experience of emotion. Person. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 10, 20–46.
doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_2
Buck, R. (1991). Social factors in facial display and communication: a reply
to Chovil and others. J. Nonverbal Behav. 15, 155–162. doi: 10.1007/BF01
672217
Buck, R., Losow, J. I., Murphy, M. M., and Costanzo, P. (1992). Social facilitation
and inhibition of emotional expression and communication. J. Pers. Soc.
Psychol. 63, 962–968. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.6.962
Buller, D. B., and Aune, R. K. (1987). Nonverbal cues to deception
among intimates, friends, and strangers. J. Nonverbal Behav. 11, 269–290.
doi: 10.1007/BF00987257
Buller, D. B., and Burgoon, J. K. (1994). “Deception: strategic and nonstrategic
communication, in Strategic Interpersonal Communication, eds J. A. Daly and
J. M. Wiemann (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 191–223.
Buller, D. B., and Burgoon, J. K. (1996). Interpersonal deception theory. Commun.
Theor. 6, 203–242. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2885.1996.tb00127.x
Buller, D. B., and Burgoon, J. K. (1997). “Emotional expression in the deception
process, in Communication and Emotion, eds P. A. Andersen and L. K.
Guerrero (Orlando, FL: Academic Press), 381–402.
Burgoon, J. K. (2014). “Interpersonal deception theory, in Encyclopedia of Lying
and Deception, ed T. R. Levine (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 532–536.
Burgoon, J. K. (2015a). “Deception detection accuracy, in International
Encyclopedia of Interpersonal Communication, eds C. R. Berger and M. E. Roloff
(Malden, MA: WileyBlackwell).
Burgoon, J. K. (2015b). When is deceptive message production more effortful
than truth-telling? A baker’s dozen of moderators. Front. Psychol. 6:1965.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01965
Burgoon, J. K., Kelley, D. L., Newton, D. A., and Keeley-Dyreson, M. P. (1989).
The nature of arousal and nonverbal indices. Hum. Commun. Res. 16, 217–255.
doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.1989.tb00210.x
Burgoon, J. K., Proudfoot, J. G., Wilson, D., and Schuetzler, R. (2014).
Patterns of nonverbal behavior associated with truth and deception:
illustrations from three experiments. J. Nonverbal Behav. 38, 325–354.
doi: 10.1007/s10919-014-0181-5
Caso, L., Maricchiolo, F., Bonaiuto, M., Vrij, A., and Mann, S. (2006). The impact
of deception and suspicion on hand gestures. J. Nonverbal Behav. 30, 1–19.
doi: 10.1007/s10919-005-0001-z
Chovil, N. (1991). Social determinants of facial displays. J. Nonverbal Behav. 15,
141–154. doi: 10.1007/BF01672216
Cushing, T. (2017). Confirmed: TSA’s Behavioral Detection Program is Useless,
Biased, and Based on Junk Science. Techdirt. Available online at: (www.techdirt.
com/articles/20170209/10331936675/confirmed-tsas- behavioral-detection-
program-is- useless-biased-based-junk-science.shtml)
DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton,
K., and Cooper, H. (2003). Cues to deception. Psychol. Bull. 129, 74–118.
doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.74
Driskell, J. E., Salas, E., and Driskell, T. (2012). Social indicators of deception. Hum.
Factors 54, 577–588. doi: 10.1177/0018720812446338
Duran, N. D., Dale, R., Kello, C. T., Street, C. N., and Richardson, D. C.
(2013). Exploring the movement dynamics of deception. Front. Psychol. 4:140.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00140
Ekman, P. (2009). Telling Lies: Clues to Deceit in the Marketplace, Politics, and
Marriage (Revised Edition). New York, NY: WW Norton.
Ekman, P., and Friesen, W. V. (1969). Nonverbal leakage and clues to deception.
Psychiatry 32, 88–105. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1969.11023575
Fernández-Dols, J.-M., Sanchez, F., Carrera, P., and Ruiz-Belda, M.-A. (1997).
Are spontaneous expressions and emotions linked? An experimental test of
coherence. J. Nonverbal Behav. 2, 163–177. doi: 10.1023/A:1024917530100
Grammer, K., Schiefenhövel, W., Schleidt, M., Lorenz, B., and Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I.
(1988). Patterns on the face: the eyebrow flash in crosscultural comparison.
Ethology 77, 279–299. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1988.tb00211.x
Grazioli, S., Johnson, P. E., and Jamal, K. (2006). A cognitive approach to fraud
detection. J. Forensic Account. 7, 65–68. doi: 10.2139/ssrn.920222
Hartwig, M., and Bond, C. F. Jr. (2011). Why do lie-catchers fail? A lens
model meta-analysis of human lie judgments. Psychol. Bull. 137, 643–659.
doi: 10.1037/a0023589
Hoque, M. E., McDuff, D. J., and Picard, R. W. (2012). Exploring temporal patterns
in classifying frustrated and delighted smiles. IEEE Trans. Affect. Comput. 3,
323–334. doi: 10.1109/T-AFFC.2012.11
Hurley, C. M., and Frank, M. G. (2011). Executing facial control during deceptive
situations. J. Nonverbal Behav. 35, 119–131. doi: 10.1007/s10919-010-0102-1
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 4September 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 1672
Burgoon Microexpressions Not Best
Köhnken, G., Milne, R., Memon, A., and Bull, R. (1999). The cognitive interview:
a meta-analysis. Psychol. Crime Law 5, 3–27. doi: 10.1080/106831699084
14991
Le Poire, B. A., and Burgoon, J. K. (1996). Usefulness of differentiating arousal
responses within communication theories: orienting response or defensive
arousal within theories of expectancy violation. Commun. Monogr. 63,
208–230. doi: 10.1080/03637759609376390
Leal, S., and Vrij, A. (2008). Blinking during and after lying. J. Nonverbal Behav.
32, 187–194. doi: 10.1007/s10919-008-0051-0
Lee, C.-C., Welker, R. B., and Odom, M. D. (2009). Features of computer-mediated,
text-based messages that support automatable, linguistics-based indicators for
deception detection. J. Inform. Syst. 23, 5–24. doi: 10.2308/jis.2009.23.1.24
Levine, T. R., Blair, J. P., and Carpenter, C. J. (2018). A critical look at
meta-analytic evidence for the cognitive approach to lie detection: a re-
examination of Vrij, Fisher, and Blank (2017). Legal Criminol. Psychol.23, 7–19.
doi: 10.1111/lcrp.12115
Mullin, D. S. (2012). Effects of Deceptive Behavior on Biomechanical Measures of
Standing Posture. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Missouri-Kansas
City.
Okubo, M., Kobayashi, A., and Ishikawa, K. (2012). A fake smile thwarts cheater
detection. J. Nonverbal Behav. 36, 217–225. doi: 10.1007/s10919-012-0134-9
Pentland, S. J., Burgoon, J. K., and Twyman, N. W. (2015). “Face and
head movement analysis using automated feature extraction software, in
Proceedings of the 48th Annual Hawaii International Conference on System
Sciences (Kauai).
Pentland, S. J., Twyman, N. W., and Burgoon, J. K. (2014). “Automated analysis
of guilt and deception from facial affect in a Concealed Information Test, in
Society for Personality and Social Psychology (Austin, TX).
Pentland, S. J., Twyman, N. W., Burgoon, J. K., Nunamaker, J. F., and
Diller, C. B. R. (2017). A video-based screening system for automated risk
assessment using nuanced facial features. J. Manage. Inform. Syst. 34, 970–993.
doi: 10.1080/07421222.2017.1393304
Pentland, S. J., Twyman, N. W., and Burgoon, J. K. (2016). “In search of reliable
facial cues for deception detection, in Proceedings of the 49th Annual Hawaii
International Conference on System Sciences (Kauai).
Pentland, S. J., and Zhang, B. (2016). “Identifying deception using facial
motion capture and analysis, in Paper Presented at Workshop on Information
Technology and Systems (WITS) (Dublin).
Porter, S., and ten Brinke, L. (2008). Reading between the lies: identifying concealed
and falsified emotions in universal facial expressions. Psychol. Sci. 19, 508–514.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02116.x
Porter, S., and ten Brinke, L. (2010). The truth about lies: what works
in detecting high-stakes deception? Legal Criminol. Psychol. 15, 57–75.
doi: 10.1348/135532509X433151
Porter, S., ten Brinke, L., and Wallace, B. (2012). Secrets and lies: involuntary
leakage in deceptive facial expressions as a function of emotional
intensity. J. Nonverbal Behav. 36, 23–37. doi: 10.1007/s10919-011-
0120-7
Sporer, S. (2013). “Bodily communication and deception, in Body Language
Communication: An International Handbook On Multimodality in Human
Interaction, eds C. Müller, A. Cienki, E. Fricke, S. H. Ladewig, D. McNeill, and
S. Tessendorf (Berlin; Boston, MA: de Gruyter Mouton), 1913–1921.
Sporer, S. (2016). Deception and cognitive load: expanding our horizon with a
working memory model. Front. Psychol. 7:420. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00420
ten Brinke, L., and Porter, S. (2012). Cry me a river: identifying the behavioral
consequences of extremely high-stakes interpersonal deception. Law Hum.
Behav. 36, 469–477. doi: 10.1037/h0093929
ten Brinke, L., Porter, S., and Baker, A. (2012). Darwin the detective:
observable facial muscle contractions reveal emotional high-stakes
lies. Evol. Hum. Behav. 33, 411–416. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.
12.003
Twyman, N. W., Elkins, A. C., and Burgoon, J. K. (2011). “A rigidity detection
system for the guilty knowledge test, in Proceedings of the 44th Annual Hawaii
International Conference on System Sciences (CD-ROM) (Koloa: Computer
Society Press).
Twyman, N. W., Elkins, A., Burgoon, J. K., and Nunamaker, J. F. Jr. (2014).
A rigidity detection system for automated credibility assessment. J. Manage.
Inform. Syst. 31, 173–201. doi: 10.2753/MIS0742-1222310108
Twyman, N. W., Proudfoot, J. G., Schuetzler, R. M., Elkins, A. C., and Derrick,
D. C. (2015). Robustness of multiple indicators in automated screening
systems for deception detection. J. Manage. Inform. Syst. 32, 215–245.
doi: 10.1080/07421222.2015.1138569
U.S. Government Accountability Office (2013). Aviation Security: TSA Should
Limit Future Funding for Behavior Detection Activities. Testimony Before the
Subcommittee on Transportation Security; Committee on Homeland Security;
House of Representatives.
Vrij, A. (2015). “A cognitive approach to lie detection, in Deception Detection:
Current Challenges and New Approaches, eds P. A. Granhag, A. Vrij, and and B.
Verschuere (Chichester: Wiley), 205–229.
Warren, G., Schertler, E., and Bull, P. (2009). Detecting deception from
emotional and unemotional cues. J. Nonverbal Behav. 33, 59–69.
doi: 10.1007/s10919-008-0057-7
Yan, W. J., Wu, Q., Liang, J., Chen, Y. H., and Fu, X. (2013 ).How fast are t he leaked
facial expressions: the duration of micro-expressions. J. Nonverbal Behav. 37,
217–230. doi: 10.1007/s10919-013-0159-8
Zuckerman, M., DePaulo, B. M., and Rosenthal, R. (1981). “Verbal
and nonverbal communication of deception, in Advances
in Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 14, ed L. Berkowitz
(New York, NY: Academic Press), 1–59.
Conflict of Interest Statement: The author declares that the research was
conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could
be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Copyright © 2018 Burgoon. This is an open-access article distributed under the
terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution
or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and
the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal
is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or
reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 5September 2018 | Volume 9 | Article 1672
... For example, Porter and ten Brinke (2008) found that only 2% of all the videos they analysed depicted such expressions, and almost half of them were shown by truth tellers rather than liars. Burgoon (2018) also argued that micro-expressions might be ineffective and that it is more likely their absence, rather than their presence, is indicative of lying, yet to the best of our knowledge, this assumption has not been tested directly. ...
... In essence, both truth tellers and liars produced almost no micro-expressions during the interview. This goes against Ekman's assumption (Ekman and Friesen 1969;Ekman 2001), but corroborated other work which found that micro-expressions are ineffective in detecting lies (Burgoon 2018;Porter and ten Brinke 2008). Furthermore, we also explored whether participants showed micro-expressions before they started answering the interviewer. ...
Article
Full-text available
Research shows that lying is a common behaviour, and that verbal cues can be effective for lie detection. However, deception detection is not straightforward as there are several factors at play, such as interpersonal differences and the content of the lie. Consequently, the effectiveness of available cues for deception detection can vary significantly. In a pre-registered study involving 80 participants (a priori sample size analyses were conducted), we instructed participants to either tell the truth or lie about an autobiographical event and an opinion. The participants also completed questionnaires on personality traits and cognitive tasks, resulting in two participant clusters. Surprisingly, when analysing verbal behaviour, truthfulness, cluster memberships, and their interactions were not found to be significant. Only lie content affected verbal cues. Additional, non-pre-registered analyses revealed that liars displayed more micro-expressions than truth tellers, but only when describing their memories and when focusing on the latency time between the investigator's question and the interviewee's answer. The results were interpreted in terms of the experimental design, which encouraged only short answers from the interviewees, leaving limited room for verbal content to be effective.
... When comparing male and female educators, schools often noticed that female professors were more likely to take the lead in class activities. However, the findings are contradicted by Burgoon (2018), who pointed out that male and female instructors do not vary in their methods for the moral growth of children. ...
Article
The study's goal was to investigate current practices promoted by teachers for students' moral development in primary schools. The study was descriptive. The study's population consisted of 3955 teachers from government schools in the district of Multan, Punjab, Pakistan. A sample of 300 (male, 160; female, 140) The elementary public schools in the Multan district were surveyed for their teachers using a simple random sample approach. Descriptive and inferential statistical techniques were used to examine the acquired data. These methods included calculating the mean, standard deviation, percentage, independent variable, and t-test. Overall, the study's results indicate that most teachers are performing well in promoting activities, contributing positively to the moral development of their students. The study recommended that schools should recognize and reward students who demonstrate excellent behavior during their school stay.
... To improve human detection ability, Ekman et al. [38] developed the facial action coding system (FACS), which utilizes all possible visible signals of facial muscle movements, including subtle and microexpressions. This approach depends on human experts to label the AUs related to universal emotions from static images, which can be biased and constrained by a universal framework that overlooks cultural variations [39]. Additionally, the use of static images may not fully capture the complexity and inconsistency of individual subtle-and microexpressions in specific stimuli and contexts, which can hamper detection reliability in related works [22], [40]. ...
Article
Whether an interviewee’s honest and deceptive responses can be detected by the signals of facial expressions in videos has been debated and called to be researched. We developed deep learningmodels enabled by computer vision to extract the temporal patterns of job applicants’ facial expressions and head movements to identify self-reported honest and deceptive impression management (IM) tactics from video frames in real asynchronous video interviews. A 12- to 15-min video was recorded for each of the N = 121 job applicants as they answered five structured behavioral interview questions. Each applicant completed a survey to self-evaluate their trustworthiness on four IM measures. Additionally, a field experiment was conducted to compare the concurrent validity associated with self-reported IMs between our modeling and human interviewers. Human interviewers’ performance in predicting these IMmeasures from another subset of 30 videos was obtained by having N = 30 human interviewers evaluate three recordings. Our models explained 91% and 84% of the variance in honest and deceptive IMs, respectively, and showed a stronger correlation with self-reported IMscores compared to human interviewers.
... Entendiéndose este último factor, las pequeñas señales no verbales que una persona puede dar de manera voluntaria o no, "proporcionando información sobre sus pensamientos, emociones o intenciones" (Burgoon, 2018). Algunos ejemplos de micro gestos incluyen el movimiento de las cejas, la dilatación o contracción de las pupilas, los movimientos de la boca o la tensión en los músculos faciales. ...
Article
Full-text available
El interrogatorio es un método clave para un sistema legal que busca proteger los ideales de justicia y verdad pero que a causa de la implementación de la virtualidad pone en cuestión la utilidad y ética por el medio en que se ejecuta, replanteando cambios acelerados en el derecho tanto prácticos como teóricos. Se cuestiona el método cross examination de los interrogatorios en la obtención de testimonios creíbles, por sus técnicas cuestionadas por la psicología del testimonio. Por lo cual, en el siguiente trabajo a través de un método deductivo-analítico con una metodología cualitativa se examinará, si el método de interrogatorio es conveniente, y si responde a las exigencias de la eficiencia de un entorno digital.
... Detecting lies and deception is not only of personal relevance in day-to-day situations but since many decades an essential part of criminal investigations and trials. Unfortunately, persons seem to be surprisingly tenacious in their assumption to catch liars by means of non-verbal signs (Bogaard and Meijers, 2022) although neither non-verbal communication nor micro expressions as a means for lie detection demonstrate sufficient reliability (DePaulo et al., 2003;Burgoon, 2018;Jordan et al., 2019) and both have been criticized as ineffective (Vrij et al., 2019). The analysis of verbal content however is a more reliable and valid approach to differentiate true from fabricated statements (Volbert and Steller, 2014;Vrij, 2014;Amado et al., 2015Amado et al., , 2016Oberlader et al., 2016Oberlader et al., , 2020. ...
Article
Full-text available
Verbal content analyses to differentiate truthful and fabricated statements, such as the Criteria-Based Content Analysis (CBCA), are used in lie detection research as well as in practice to assess the credibility of statements in criminal court proceedings. Meta-analyses demonstrate validity of verbal content analyses above chance, but the traditional research paradigms usually lack either ecological or internal validity. The authors discuss the usage of immersive virtual reality scenarios to solve this dilemma, as both types of validity can be increased by this approach. In this integrative review of existing literature on the current use of virtual scenarios in forensic and victimology research, the authors extract strengths and limitations for possible VR studies in the context of verbal content analysis. Furthermore, novel ethical challenges involved are summarized and implications for future studies proposed. Overall, we argue in favor of using virtual reality scenarios to validate methods for verbal content analysis, but also urge to consider ethical limitations regarding unwanted short- and long-term aftereffects.
... But the second, and more significant reason, is that there were only a handful of studies that actually coded both macro and micro facial expressions of emotion to determine their relationship to telling lies (e.g., Ekman et al., 1988Ekman et al., , 1990Kennedy & Coe, 1994). The limited number of such studies may account for why there was significant pushback on the utility of micro expressions of emotion as a behavioral clue to deception (Burgoon, 2018), and why scientists would dismiss the leakage of these emotional clues as being "…undefined. The (unspecified) cue could literally be any behavior." ...
Article
Full-text available
Meta-analyses have not shown emotions to be significant predictors of deception. Criticisms of this conclusion argued that individuals must be engaged with each other in higher stake situations for such emotions to manifest, and that these emotions must be evaluated in their verbal context (Frank and Svetieva in J Appl Res Memory Cognit 1:131–133, 10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.04.006, 2012). This study examined behavioral synchrony as a marker of engagement in higher stakes truthful and deceptive interactions, and then compared the differences in facial expressions of fear, contempt, disgust, anger, and sadness not consistent with the verbal content. Forty-eight pairs of participants were randomly assigned to interviewer and interviewee, and the interviewee was assigned to steal either a watch or a ring and to lie about the item they stole, and tell the truth about the other, under conditions of higher stakes of up to 30rewardsforsuccessfuldeception,and30 rewards for successful deception, and 0 plus having to write a 15-min essay for unsuccessful deception. The interviews were coded for expression of emotions using EMFACS (Friesen and Ekman in EMFACS-7; emotional facial action coding system, 1984). Synchrony was demonstrated by the pairs of participants expressing overlapping instances of happiness (AU6 + 12). A 3 (low, moderate, high synchrony) × 2 (truth, lie) mixed-design ANOVA found that negative facial expressions of emotion were a significant predictor of deception, but only when they were not consistent with the verbal content, in the moderate and high synchrony conditions. This finding is consistent with data and theorizing that shows that with higher stakes, or with higher engagement, emotions can be a predictor of deception.
... Mesmo compartilhando a mesma fisiologia, cada ser humano é distinto e responde diferentemente às condições ambientais. Além disso, a popularização do conhecimento científico sobre CNV e sua equivocada generalização são elementos que agravam a difusão de conhecimento pseudocientífico (DENAULT et al., 2020;FISHER, 2020;BURGOON, 2018). ...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Neste artigo, apresentamos uma introdução teórica à comunicação não-verbal (CNV), apresentando suas fontes, funções e aplicações no campo da saúde. A CNV, conhecida por vários termos ao longo do tempo, como "linguagem corporal", ganhou destaque graças a livros de autoajuda sobre linguagem corporal. No entanto, é importante observar que a "linguagem corporal" abrange apenas uma fração da CNV, concentrando-se em comportamentos não verbais observáveis. Não obstante, a CNV tem uma presença constante no cotidiano, muitas vezes passando despercebida, mas suas funções e influências são profundas. Profissionais de saúde devem dominá-la, uma vez que a CNV pode desempenhar um papel crucial na interação com os pacientes, na aquisição e na validação de informação. Destacamos a relevância da CNV para profissionais de saúde, organizando seus aspectos teóricos de acordo com suas funções: (1) contextualização da comunicação, quando a CNV fornece contexto à comunicação, ajudando a compreender o ambiente e as emoções dos pacientes; (2) suplementação à comunicação verbal, uma vez que complementa a comunicação verbal, enfatizando informações importantes e revelando dúvidas, ansiedades e emoções não expressas verbalmente; e (3) regulação de interações: A CNV regula as interações, o fluxo e a duração da conversação, além de influenciar a qualidade da relação terapêutica. Também enfatizamos a auto-observação da CNV por parte dos profissionais de saúde, ajudando-os a compreender suas próprias reações em relação ao ambiente, narrativas e emoções dos pacientes. Além disso, ressaltamos a importância de interpretar a CNV com base em conhecimento confiável, dada a disseminação de informações pseudocientíficas sobre o assunto. Concluímos que a aprendizagem da CNV é valiosa para profissionais de saúde, aprimorando a comunicação e a percepção das emoções, o que, por sua vez, eleva a qualidade do atendimento e da relação terapêutica. Palavras-chave: Comunicação verbal na saúde, Linguagem corporal e saúde.
... When detected properly, micro expressions could provide important information about patients' well-being and about risk assessment that psychotherapists could use for diagnosis and decision making (see, e.g., Ekman & Friesen, 1969;Frank & Svetieva, 2015). However, more recent research posits a more cautious understanding of micro expressions as indicators of (conscious) deception, like attempts to lie (Burgoon, 2018;Jordan et al., 2019;Porter and ten Brinke, 2008;Vrij et al., 2019;Weinberger, 2010;Zloteanu et al., 2021). Nonetheless, beyond deception, micro expressions could have the potential to provide useful information about conflicting, blended or confusing emotions that patients have, but don't fully understand yet, or about repressed or dissociated emotions that could be explored in a safe therapeutic setting (see, e.g., Donovan et al., 2017). ...
Thesis
Full-text available
Researchers, practitioners and legislators agree that it is important to un- derstand which kinds of psychotherapeutic treatments lead to lasting pos- itive changes in patients’ well-being, how those treatments can be admin- istered in efficient ways and how it can be determined which patients would benefit from which treatment. In recent years, there has also been growing interest in those who practice psychotherapy; specifically, in the socio-emotional and interpersonal characteristics and competencies that psychotherapists should possess to provide high quality treatments for a variety of patients, irrespective of psychotherapy approach. This thesis studies one such important psychotherapist competency, namely the abil- ity to recognize non-verbal emotional expressions in others. Psychothera- pists need to be able to help patients experience, understand and express their emotions, and, in this context, it is crucial that they themselves have good socio-emotional competencies, like emotion recognition accuracy. Still, there is surprisingly little research about psychotherapists’ emotion recognition accuracy and about how they could be supported in improving this ability in the course of their education. Study I explores trainee psychotherapists’ emotion recognition accu- racy in the beginning and in the end of theoretical and practical psycho- therapy education, and compares it to a control group of undergraduate students. The results reveal that trainee psychotherapists in the beginning of their education show superior emotion recognition accuracy for multi- modal (audio, video, audio-video) emotional expressions and micro ex- pressions (<200ms) compared to the control group. This suggests that those who choose to become psychotherapists might already possess ele- vated emotion recognition accuracy or might have developed it early on during their studies. However, after one and a half years of education, their multimodal and micro expression emotion recognition accuracy does not improve significantly more than the control groups’ accuracy. This suggests that standard (psychodynamic and cognitive behavioral) psycho- therapy education does not automatically lead to improved emotion recog- nition accuracy, even though the trainees learn how to conduct psycho- therapy and also treat their first patients at the university clinic. Or, that the socio-emotional competencies that develop during the education might not be captured by the standardized computerized emotion recognition ac- curacy tasks used in this study. Nonetheless, this finding might also sug- gest that more explicit training of emotion recognition accuracy is needed. Study II then investigates two newly developed standardized computer- ized emotion recognition accuracy trainings, one for multimodal emotion recognition accuracy and one for micro expression recognition accuracy. The trainings are evaluated in a sample of undergraduate students using a mixed design. The trainings are compared to one another and to an active control training. Both trainings are found to significantly improve the par- ticipants’ emotion recognition accuracy in a one-week posttest. Study III extends those findings using a sample of trainee psychotherapists. Also in the target population, both trainings are found to be effective in the one- week posttest. In addition to that, the multimodal training shows effects for unimodal emotion recognition accuracy for audio-only and video-only stimuli in the one-year follow-up towards the end of psychotherapy edu- cation. This indicates that standardized computerized emotion recognition accuracy training can be used as a tool for improving trainee psychother- apists’ emotion recognition accuracy, even though additional interven- tions might be needed for securing long-term success for all facets of emo- tion recognition. Future research should explore the practical impact of trainee psychotherapists’ emotion recognition accuracy and the training of this ability. The findings of this thesis are, on the one hand, surprising, in so far as psychotherapy education likely does not lead to improvements in trainee psychotherapists’ emotion recognition accuracy. On the other hand, they are encouraging, because they suggest that this ability can be trained with relatively simple and resource-efficient methods. Emotion recognition ac- curacy training could become part of standard or individualized psycho- therapy training, alongside the training of other relevant verbal and non- verbal socio-emotional and interpersonal psychotherapist competencies.
Article
Full-text available
The growth of machine learning and artificial intelligence has made it possible for automatic lie detection systems to emerge. These can be based on a variety of cues, such as facial features. However, there is a lack of knowledge about both the development and the accuracy of such systems. To address this lack, we conducted a review of studies that have investigated automatic lie detection systems by using facial features. Our analysis of twenty-eight eligible studies focused on four main categories: dataset features, facial features used, classifier features and publication features. Overall, the findings showed that automatic lie detection systems rely on diverse technologies, facial features, and measurements. They are mainly based on factual lies, regardless of the stakes involved. On average, these automatic systems were based on a dataset of 52 individuals and achieved an average accuracy ranging from 61.87% to 72.93% in distinguishing between truth-tellers and liars, depending on the types of classifiers used. However, although the leakage hypothesis was the most used explanatory framework, many studies did not provide sufficient theoretical justification for the choice of facial features and their measurements. Bridging the gap between psychology and the computational-engineering field should help to combine theoretical frameworks with technical advancements in this area.
Chapter
The human face can convey information in several ways, including facial displays (i.e., stereotyped skin movements). Facial displays are quick, versatile, and inaccurate signals. According to some academics, they have emotional roots. This idea fueled discussion about facial expressions universality, voluntariness, and recognition. Theories then spread in an attempt to conciliate diverging data, unfortunately also favoring the disclosure of debated notions. Microexpressions and asymmetry of facial displays seem to be the keys to quickly accessing people’s internal states, for example. Nevertheless, they are complex phenomena with several potential determinants, as anatomy, neuroscience, phylogeny, and ontogeny of facial displays suggest.
Article
Full-text available
This study investigates the development of an automated interviewing system that uses facial behavior as an indicator of the risk of given illicit behavior. Traditional facial emotion indicators of risk in semistructured dialogue may have limitations in an automated approach. However, an initial analysis of mock crime interviews suggests that the face may exhibit some form of rigidity during highly structured interviews. An interviewing system design using facial rigidity analysis was implemented and experimentally evaluated, the results of which further reveal that the rigidity is fairly generalized across the face. Whereas existing theory traditionally focuses on leakage of facial expressions, this study provides evidence that neutralization of facial expression may be a valuable alternative for automated interviewing systems. The proof-of-concept system in this study may help human risk assessment move beyond traditional boundaries, into fields such as auditing, emergency room management, and security screening.
Chapter
Full-text available
This chapter discusses emotion and its related components, and reviews how emotion enters the deception process. Emotion is comprised of three interrelated psychological, physiological, and behavioral processes—the subjective experience of emotion, arousal, and overt behavioral expression. Emotional experiences and arousal are internal events, whereas emotional expressions refer to the external behavioral manifestations that are presumed to signal the internal events. Communicative behavior is dynamic, multifunctional, multidimensional, and multimodal. Because deception is goal-directed and deliberate, senders encode strategic behaviors; however, along with purposive actions, senders also exhibit unintentional, often unconscious cues. Deception produces several cognitive and emotional responses, most notably arousal and negative affect. Deception requires more cognitive resources and effort than telling the truth and may therefore produce overt indicators of cognitive arousal. The chapter also discusses the four roles that emotions and emotional expression can play in deceptive exchanges. Emotions can motivate deception. They can be the content of deceptive messages. They can be expressed inadvertently as biological signals of arousal. And they can be expressed independently and intentionally as social code units that bolster the credibility of the deceptive message.
Article
Full-text available
Recently, studies on deception and its detection have increased dramatically. Many of these studies rely on the “cognitive load approach” as the sole explanatory principle to understand deception. These studies have been exclusively on lies about negative actions (usually lies of suspects of [mock] crimes). Instead, we need to re-focus more generally on the cognitive processes involved in generating both lies and truths, not just on manipulations of cognitive load. Using Baddeley’s (2000, 2007, 2012) working memory model, which integrates verbal and visual processes in working memory with retrieval from long-term memory and control of action, not only verbal content cues but also nonverbal, paraverbal, and linguistic cues can be investigated within a single framework. The proposed model considers long-term semantic, episodic and autobiographical memory and their connections with working memory and action. It also incorporates ironic processes of mental control (Wegner, 1994, 2009), the role of scripts and schemata and retrieval cues and retrieval processes. Specific predictions of the model are outlined and support from selective studies is presented. The model is applicable to different types of reports, particularly about lies and truths about complex events, and to different modes of production (oral, hand-written, typed). Predictions regarding several moderator variables and methods to investigate them are proposed.
Article
Purpose This article provides a re‐analysis of Vrij et al .'s (2017, Leg. Crim. Psychol . 22, 1) meta‐analysis of the cognitive approach to lie detection. Vrij et al .'s analyses confounded dependent variables, capitalized on aberrant controls, and used unreliable data to inflate support. Methods Meta‐analysis was used to reanalyse Vrij et al .'s data. Studies of human detection and studies involving statistical classification were analysed separately. Results The advantage offered by the cognitive approach was much smaller than previously claimed. Accuracies in control conditions were unusually low, and the most supportive findings came from the least reliable data. Conclusions Human detection and statistical classification are different. The evidence for the cognitive approach has been overstated.
Chapter
Deception detection accuracy concerns the ability of instruments or humans to distinguish truthful from deceptive communication. Accuracy is often described in terms of signal detection theory (hits, the percentage of actual deceptive utterances that are accurately classified as deceit; misses, the percentage of actual deception that is erroneously misclassified as truthful; false alarms, the percentage of truthful cases erroneously identified as deceptive; and true negatives, the percentage of truthful cases correctly identified as truthful). Whereas the polygraph achieves accuracy near 90% for specific-incident diagnostic examinations, and other instruments return a mix of accuracies, human detection hovers near chance. Multiple meta-analyses estimate that humans accurately classify truth 61% of the time and deception 47% of the time, for an overall average of 54% detection accuracy. False alarms are less frequent than misses due to a robust truth bias. Disputed is whether professionals are more accurate than novices or merely more confident and lie-biased. Training appears to confer some advantage, but its effect depends on such factors as feedback, repeated sessions, computer aids, and training in telltale content, language, or nonverbal behaviors. Factors moderating detection accuracy include base rate, deceiver communication skills, availability of context, extent of exposure, and questioning strategies.
Article
: Research relevant to psychotherapy regarding facial expression and body movement, has shown that the kind of information which can be gleaned from the patients words - information about affects, attitudes, interpersonal styles, psychodynamics - can also be derived from his concomitant nonverbal behavior. The study explores the interaction situation, and considers how within deception interactions differences in neuroanatomy and cultural influences combine to produce specific types of body movements and facial expressions which escape efforts to deceive and emerge as leakage or deception clues.
Article
This study investigates the effectiveness of an automatic system for detection of deception by individuals with the use of multiple indicators of such potential deception. Deception detection research in the information systems discipline has postulated increased accuracy through a new class of screening systems that automatically conduct interviews and track multiple indicators of deception simultaneously. Understanding the robustness of this new class of systems and the limitations of its theoretical improved performance is important for refinement of the conceptual design. The design science proof-of-concept study presented here implemented and evaluated the robustness of these systems for automated screening for deception detection. A large experiment was used to evaluate the effectiveness of a constructed multiple-indicator system, both under normal conditions and with the presence of common types of countermeasures (mental and physical). The results shed light on the relative strength and robustness of various types of deception indicators within this new context. The findings further suggest the possibility of increased accuracy through the measurement of multiple indicators if classification algorithms can compensate for human attempts to counter effectiveness.