ResearchPDF Available

Towards a Conceptual Space of Trust in the Social Neuroscience of Consciousness

Authors:

Abstract

In bridging the explanatory gap between cognition, neural substrates and approaches to understanding consciousness, there is a need for modeling concepts and their relations as conceptual spaces. Conceptual spaces help construct a universal state-space of human consciousness. It is argued that the neural substrates responsible for perception of the other's awareness and attention mechanize a social process, that when directed inward as a somatosensory process, presumably realizes consciousness. Therefore striving to model conceptual spaces in the domain of social exchanges, as related to awareness, might pose a means to bridge this gap.
Towards a Conceptual Space of Trust in the Social
Neuroscience of Consciousness
Svenja Pieritz 1,2, Lucas Lorenzo Pena 1,2,
Xerxes D. Arsiwalla 1,2,3 and Paul F.M.J. Verschure 2,3,4
1 SPECSInstitute for Bioengineering Catalonia (IBEC)
2 Universitat Pompeu Fabra (UPF)
3 The Barcelona Institute of Science and Technology (BIST)
4 Institció Catalana de Recerca i Estudis Avançats (ICREA)
{svenja.pieritz01, lucaslorenzo.pena01}@estudiant.upf.edu
[Introduction/Motivation:]
In bridging the explanatory gap between cognition and neural substrates approaches in understanding
consciousness, there is a need in modelling concepts and their relations as conceptual spaces. Conceptual spaces
help construct a universal state-space of human consciousness [1]. It is argued, the neural substrates responsible
for the perception of another's awareness and attention mechanize a social process, that when directed inward as
a somatosensory process, presumably realizes consciousness [2]. Therefore striving to model conceptual spaces
in the domain of social exchanges, as related to awareness, might pose a means to bridge this gap.
In order to survive within a social game theoretic scenario, an agent needs to estimate expectations of another
agent resulting from processing multimodal information. This perspectival expectation assists the agent in
generating mental models of the world and is realized as confidence. While confidence is firstly needed in
deciding and driving behavior, this metric needs to delegate decisions resulting from this a threshold. Here we
argue this delegation threshold as a component of trust beyond risk-reward, or utility assessment [3], [4].
[Methods:]
Table 1: Results of a meta-study of brain regions connected to trust [5][15] showing 5 main functions of brain
circuits activated in trust situations. These findings were clustered into functions supporting of confidence [16]
and valence [17][19], respectively within various subdomains. We categorized substrates responsible for
processing mental models, risk assessment, and outcome rewards as confidence [16]. Substrates related to
emotion and social memory are categorized into valence. In social-science, valence is argued to be linked to trust
while accounting for subjective perception within a social space and being specifically defined as emotional
affinity or aversion towards another [17][19].
After conducting a meta-study of neural substrates related to trust (Table 1), we propose generalizing trust along
dimensions of confidence, and valence (Figure 1). Furthermore, we include accuracy as being an objective
measure of how well one’s mental model describes the world [20].
[Results and Discussion:]
We construct a conceptual space of trust as defined by three dimensions: confidence (C), valence (V), and
accuracy (A), each with their respective gradients: high (+) and low (-). We hypothesize the concept of trust into
five instantiations: Trust, Distrust, Mistrust, Mis-distrust, and Untrust (Figure 1). Trust as willingness to make
oneself vulnerable [21] and subjective probability by which another will perform an action which outcome will
result in positive utility.(+C, +V, +A). Distrust as willingness to not make oneself vulnerable and subjective
probability by which another will perform an action which outcome will result in negative utility [22] (+C, -V,
+A). Mistrust as a measure misplaced trust [22] (+C, +V, -A). Mis-distrust as a measure of misplaced mistrust
(+C, -V, -A). Untrust as unwillingness to make decisions about both vulnerability and to create expectations of
another’s actions (-C, -V, -A).
Figure 1: A Conceptual Space of Trust
Thus, by proposing a concrete definition through this conceptual space, we lay the foundational framework that
might support a cognitive model of trust. We are conducting pilot studies of a modified version of a dyadic trust
game where we introduce emotional priming to influence valence, report accuracy and measure confidence in
order to compare positioning within this conceptual space. In summary, this data might validate among possible
socio-economic models of trust, which can be tested within more complex social ecological validity scenarios. A
cognitive theory of trust might also play a significant role in aiding cognitive architectures to construct mental
models of a socially dynamic world. Distributed Adaptive Control (DAC) is such a cognitive architecture, which
aims at realizing the Who, What, Where, Why and How (H5W) of a social agent [23]. Our model, when placed
in multi-agent interactions driven by DAC and subsequently H5W, might support the variability expressed in
social agent interactions which reside outside of classical game theoretic interactions and yet still be biologically
grounded.
References:
[1] A. Pereira Junior and L. F. Almada, “Conceptual Spaces and Consciousness: Integrating Cognitive and
Affective Processes,” Int. J. Mach. Conscious., vol. 3, no. 1, pp. 127143, 2011.
[2] M. S. A. Graziano and S. Kastner, “Human consciousness and its relationship to social neuroscience: A
novel hypothesis,” Cogn. Neurosci., vol. 2, no. 2, pp. 98113, 2011.
[3] C. Castelfranchi and R. Falcone, “Socio-cognitive theory of trust,” J. Pitt. London Wiley, no. January
2015, pp. 5889, 2005.
[4] D. Lee, “Game theory and neural basis of social decision making,” Nature Neuroscience, vol. 11, no. 4.
NIH Public Access, pp. 404409, Apr-2008.
[5] J. A. Aimone, D. Houser, and B. Weber, “Neural signatures of betrayal aversion: an fMRI study of
trust,” Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci., vol. 281, no. 1782, pp. 2013212720132127, 2014.
[6] M. Visser and C. B. O, “Using Trust for Detecting Deceitful Agents in Artificial Societies,” 2005.
[7] A. Dimoka, “What does the brain tell us about trust and distrust? Evidence from a functional
neuroimaging study,” Mis Q., vol. 34, no. 2, pp. 124, 2010.
[8] A. Javor, “The Biology of Trust: Integrating Evidence From Genetics , Endocrinology , and Functional
Brain Imaging,” vol. 5, no. 2, pp. 6391, 2012.
[9] B. King-Casas, D. Tomlin, C. Anen, C. F. Camerer, S. R. Quartz, and P. R. Montague, “Getting to Know
You: Reputations and Trust in a Two-Person Economic Exchange,” Science (80-. )., vol. 308, no. 5718,
pp. 7883, 2005.
[10] F. Krueger et al., “Neural correlates of trust,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci., vol. 104, no. 50, pp. 2008420089,
2007.
[11] C. Lamm and T. Singer, “Author ’ s personal copy The role of anterior insular cortex in social emotions,”
vol. 214, 1863.
[12] J. K. Rilling, D. A. Gutman, T. R. Zeh, G. Pagnoni, G. S. Berns, and C. D. Kilts, “A neural basis for social
cooperation,” Neuron, vol. 35, no. 2, pp. 395405, 2002.
[13] D. Samson, I. A. Apperly, C. Chiavarino, and G. W. Humphreys, “Left temporoparietal junction is
necessary for representing someone else’s belief,” Nat. Neurosci., vol. 7, no. 5, pp. 499500, 2004.
[14] H. Walter, M. Adenzato, A. Ciaramidaro, I. Enrici, L. Pia, and B. G. Bara, “Understanding Intentions in
Social Interaction: The Role of the Anterior Paracingulate Cortex,” J. Cogn. Neurosci., vol. 16, no. 10,
pp. 18541863, 2004.
[15] M. Watabe, H. Ban, and H. Yamamoto, “Judgments About Others’ Trustworthiness: An fMRI Study,”
Lett. Evol. Behav. Sci., vol. 2, no. 2, pp. 2832, 2011.
[16] B. G. Robbins, “What is Trust? A Multidisciplinary Review, Critique, and Synthesis,” Sociol. Compass,
vol. 10, no. 10, pp. 972–986, 2016.
[17] T. DeWitt, D. T. Nguyen, and R. Marshall, “Exploring customer loyalty following service recovery: The
mediating effects of trust and emotions,” J. Serv. Res., vol. 10, no. 3, pp. 269281, 2008.
[18] J. R. Dunn and M. E. Schweitzer, “Feeling and Believing: The Influence of Emotion on Trust,” J. Pers.
Soc. Psychol., vol. 88, no. 5, pp. 736748, 2005.
[19] G. R. Jones and J. M. George, “The Experience and Evolution of Trust: Implications for Cooperation and
Teamwork Author ( s ): Gareth R . Jones and Jennifer M . George Source: The Academy of
Management Review , Vol . 23 , No . 3 ( Jul ., 1998 ), pp . 531-546 Published by: Academy of,” Acad.
Manag., vol. 23, no. 3, pp. 531546, 2016.
[20] A. W. Kruglanski, “The Psychology of Being „Right“: The Problem of Accuracy in Social Perception and
Cognition,” Psychol. Bull., vol. 106, no. 3, pp. 395409, 1989.
[21] F. D. Schoorman, R. C. Mayer, and J. H. Davis, “An Integrative Model of Organizational Trust: Past ,
Present, and future,” vol. 32, no. 2, pp. 344354, 2007.
[22] S. Marsh and M. R. Dibben, “Trust, Untrust, Distrust and Mistrust An Exploration of the Dark(er) Side,”
Proc. 3rd Int. Conf. iTrust, pp. 1733, 2005.
[23] Verschure, P. F. M. J. (2016). Synthetic consciousness: the distributed adaptive control perspective.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1701), 20150448.
https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0448
ResearchGate has not been able to resolve any citations for this publication.
Article
Full-text available
Understanding the nature of consciousness is one of the grand outstanding scientific challenges. The fundamental methodological problem is how phenomenal first person experience can be accounted for in a third person verifiable form, while the conceptual challenge is to both define its function and physical realization. The distributed adaptive control theory of consciousness (DACtoc) proposes answers to these three challenges. The methodological challenge is answered relative to the hard problem and DACtoc proposes that it can be addressed using a convergent synthetic methodology using the analysis of synthetic biologically grounded agents, or quale parsing. DACtoc hypothesizes that consciousness in both its primary and secondary forms serves the ability to deal with the hidden states of the world and emerged during the Cambrian period, affording stable multi-agent environments to emerge. The process of consciousness is an autonomous virtualization memory, which serializes and unifies the parallel and subconscious simulations of the hidden states of the world that are largely due to other agents and the self with the objective to extract norms. These norms are in turn projected as value onto the parallel simulation and control systems that are driving action. This functional hypothesis is mapped onto the brainstem, midbrain and the thalamo-cortical and cortico-cortical systems and analysed with respect to our understanding of deficits of consciousness. Subsequently, some of the implications and predictions of DACtoc are outlined, in particular, the prediction that normative bootstrapping of conscious agents is predicated on an intentionality prior. In the view advanced here, human consciousness constitutes the ultimate evolutionary transition by allowing agents to become autonomous with respect to their evolutionary priors leading to a post-biological Anthropocene. This article is part of the themed issue ‘The major synthetic evolutionary transitions’.
Article
Full-text available
We investigated how information about others' trust-worthiness affects brain region activation in a functional MRI (fMRI) study. Participants were given statements about a person's behaviors and were asked to judge whether or not the person was trustworthy while undergoing fMRI imaging. Participants read 32 statements , half of which were relevant to making judgments about trustworthiness, and half of which were irrelevant to making judgments about trustworthiness. We found that making trustworthiness judgments when reading relevant statements was associated with differential activation in five regions: the angular gyrus (AG), anterior cingulate (AC), left frontal lobe (LF), right frontal lobe (RF), and putamen/caudate nucleus (PU/CA). Previous study using a highly abstract economic game situation has also shown activation in these regions. These regions are also related to the learning process and to theory-of-mind processing. In addition, we found that people with high or low scores on a general trust scale showed less activation than did people with middle-range scores. These results suggest that we use trial-and-error learning to decide whether to trust others, and that this learning history (represented here as general trust level) influences automatic processing of new trust judgments.
Article
Full-text available
After arguing about the crucial importance of trust for Agents and MAS, we provide a definition of trust both as a mental state and as a social attitude and relation. We present the mental ingredients of trust: its specific beliefs and goals, with special attention to evaluations and expectations. We show the relation between trust and the mental background of delegation. We explain why trust is a bet, and implies some risks, and analyse the more complex forms of social trust, based on a theory of mind and in particular on morality, reputation and disposition, and authority (three party trust). We explain why promises, contracts, authorities can increase our trust by modifying our mental representations. We present a principled quantification of trust, based on its cognitive ingredients, and use this "degree of trust" as the basis for a rational decision to delegate or not to another agent. We analyze the trust in the information sources and its relation with the general notion of trust. We explain when trust is rational, and why it is not an irrational decision by definition. We also criticise the economic and game-theoretic view of trust for underestimating the importance of cognitive ingredients of trust and for reducing it to subjective probability and risk. The paper is intended to contribute both to the conceptual analysis and to the practical use of trust in social theory and MAS.
Article
Full-text available
Decisions are said to be 'risky' when they are made in environments with uncertainty caused by nature. By contrast, a decision is said to be 'trusting' when its outcome depends on the uncertain decisions of another person. A rapidly expanding literature reveals economically important differences between risky and trusting decisions, and further suggests these differences are due to 'betrayal aversion'. While its neural foundations have not been previously illuminated, the prevailing hypothesis is that betrayal aversion stems from a desire to avoid negative emotions that arise from learning one's trust was betrayed. Here, we provide evidence from an fMRI study that supports this hypothesis. In particular, our data indicate that the anterior insula modulates trusting decisions that involve the possibility of betrayal.
Article
Full-text available
Several difficulties are noted with general questions psychologists have been asking about human accuracy, such as whether people are typically accurate or inaccurate, what the boundary conditions for accuracy are, or the general process whereby accuracy may be improved. Instead, a situationally specific approach to accuracy is adopted in which a central role is assigned to the judgmental process. Accordingly, two general paradigms are distinguished addressing accuracy from realistic and phenomenal perspectives. The realist paradigm focuses on Ss' judgments and the degree to which these correspond to an external criterion. The phenomenal paradigm focuses on Ss' internal criterion as well as their perceptions of the target judgment and the judgment-to-criterion correspondence. Research possibilities in each paradigm are noted. It is suggested that attention to judgmental factors may extend accuracy work in previously unexplored directions. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
Trust is among the most important factors in human life, as it pervades almost all domains of society. Although behavioral research has revealed a number of insights into the nature of trust, as well as its antecedents and consequences, an increasing number of scholars have begun to investigate the topic from a biological perspective to gain a deeper understanding. These biological investigations into trust have been carried out on three levels of analysis: genes, endocrinology, and the brain. Based on these three levels, we present a review of the literature on the biology of trust. Moreover, we integrate our findings into a conceptual framework which unifies the three levels of analysis, and we also link the biological levels to trust behavior. The results show that trust behavior is at least moderately genetically predetermined. Moreover, trust behavior is associated with specific hormones, in particular oxytocin, as well as specific brain structures, which are located in the basal ganglia, limbic system, and the frontal cortex. Based on these results, we discuss both methodological and thematic implications. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Conference Paper
Full-text available
There has been a lot of research and development in the field of computational trust in the past decade. Much of it has acknowledged or claimed that trust is a good thing. We think it’s time to look at the other side of the coin and ask the questions why is it good, what alternatives are there, where do they fit, and is our assumption always correct? We examine the need for an addressing of the concepts of Trust, Mistrust, and Distrust, how they interlink and how they affect what goes on around us and within the systems we create. Finally, we introduce the phenomenon of ‘Untrust,’ which resides in the space between trusting and distrusting. We argue that the time is right, given the maturity and breadth of the field of research in trust, to consider how untrust, distrust and mistrust work, why they can be useful in and of themselves, and where they can shine.
Article
Despite decades of interdisciplinary research on trust, the literature remains fragmented and balkanized with little consensus regarding its origins. This review documents how this came to be and attempts to offer a solution. Specifically, it evaluates issues of conceptualization found in the trust literature. I recommend that we move away from varieties of trust — multidimensional conceptualizations of trust — and toward a single trust concept built around four basic elements: actor A’s beliefs, actor B’s trustworthiness, the matter(s) at hand, and unknown outcomes. I finish the article by proposing a synthetic structural-cognitive theoretical framework for investigating the causes and consequences of trust in everyday life.
Article
We demonstrate that incidental emotions (e.g. anger stemming from an argument with your spouse) influence trust in unrelated settings (e.g. the likelihood of trusting a co-worker). Incidental happiness and gratitude increase trust, and incidental anger decreases trust. Other-person control appraisals mediate this relationship, and trustee familiarity moderates this relationship.
Article
A considerable amount of research has examined trust since our 1995 publication. We revisit some of the critical issues that we addressed and provide clarifications and extensions of the topics of levels of analysis, time, control systems, reciprocity, and measurement. We also recognize recent research in new areas of trust, such as affect, emotion, violation and repair, distrust, international and cross-cultural issues, and context-specific models, and we identify promising avenues for future research.