ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

The present study investigated language inhibition and cross-language interference as two possible mechanisms of bilingual language control (BLC) that can be affected by Huntington’s disease (HD), a neurodegenerative disease (ND) affecting the striatum. To this aim, the study explored the performance of pre-symptomatic and early-stage HD patients in two experimental tasks meant to elicit cross-language interference and language inhibition, including a Stroop task and a language switching task. The results revealed dissociations between these two mechanisms, indicating that language activation or inhibition is related to HD pathology while cross-language interference is not. Switch costs in HD patients were greater than controls in low-demand control conditions of language switching (longer preparation time), while Stroop effects were similar between the two groups of participants. This result was interpreted as a difficulty in overcoming the excessive inhibition applied to non-target language. The BLC processes related to the striatum and subcortical structures are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
1
Language reconfiguration in bilinguals: a study with Huntington’s disease patients
Calabria Marco1, Jesús Pérez Pérez 2-6, Saúl Martínez-Horta2-4, Andrea Horta-Barba2-4,
Mar Carceller7, Jaime Kulisevsky2-4,6,7 and Costa Albert1,3
1. Center for Brain and Cognition, Pompeu Fabra University, Spain, Barcelona
2. Movement Disorders Unit, Neurology Department, Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau,
Barcelona, Spain
3. Biomedical Research Institute (IIB-Sant Pau), Barcelona, Spain
4. Centro de Investigación en Red-Enfermedades Neurodegenerativas (CIBERNED), Madrid,
Spain
5. Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
6. Autonomous University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
7. Psychiatry Department, Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau, Autonomous University of
Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
8. Universitat Oberta de Catalunya (UOC), Barcelona, Spain
9. ICREA, Institució Catalana de Recerca i Estudis Avançats, Barcelona, Spain
Corresponding author:
Marco Calabria
Center for Brain and Cognition (CBC)
Universitat Pompeu Fabra
C. de Ramon Trias Fargas, 25-27 (Office 24.310)
08005 Barcelona (Spain)
Phone: +34 93 542 2930
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
2
Abstract
The present study investigated language inhibition and cross-language interference as two
possible mechanisms of bilingual language control (BLC) that can be affected by Huntington’s
disease (HD), a neurodegenerative disease (ND) affecting the striatum. To this aim, the study
explored the performance of pre-symptomatic and early-stage HD patients in two experimental
tasks meant to elicit cross-language interference and language inhibition, including a Stroop task
and a language switching task. The results revealed dissociations between these two mechanisms,
indicating that language activation or inhibition is related to HD pathology while cross-language
interference is not. Switch costs in HD patients were greater than controls in low-demand control
conditions of language switching (longer preparation time), while Stroop effects were similar
between the two groups of participants. This result was interpreted as a difficulty in overcoming
the excessive inhibition applied to non-target language. The BLC processes related to the striatum
and subcortical structures are discussed.
Keywords: bilingualism, movement disorders, language control, inhibition, cross-language
interference
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
3
1. Introduction
Interest in research on language and cognition in bilingual speakers with
neurodegenerative diseases (NDs) is increasing. This interest is motivated by the high prevalence
of age-related disorders as a consequence of longer life expectancy. At the same time,
bilingualism and multilingualism are also on the rise, and this situation raises many questions
regarding the interaction between NDs and bilingualism. Research on bilingual language
processing in patients with NDs offers an excellent opportunity to complement data on brain
damage with data on healthy individuals (Calabria, Cattaneo, & Costa, 2017).
One issue explored in this context is the study of the architecture of language control in
bilingual people. Bilingual language control (BLC) is defined as the skill that enables bilinguals
to use a target language while monitoring for potential interference from the language not in use
(Green, 1986, 1998). In the context of NDs, BLC processes have been explored in brain diseases
affecting the executive control (EC) system, such as Parkinson’s disease (PD) (Cattaneo et al.,
2015). The results of this research revealed that basal ganglia degeneration may affect language
control, even before the EC system is significantly impaired, at least for some processes related
to inhibitory control. These results suggest that research on bilingual patients with basal ganglia
degeneration offers an opportunity to gain a greater understanding of BLC processes related to
these brain structures.
The present study explores how the degeneration of the striatum, as a consequence of
Huntington’s disease (HD), affects two processes involving BLC: cross-language interference
and language activation/inhibition. These two processes have been related to striatum structures
(caudate nuclei and putamen) in healthy bilinguals who are required to switch between languages
(Abutalebi et al., 2008; 2013), and these processes are included in the BLC network as crucial
structures for language selection (Abutalebi & Green, 2008, 2016; Calabria, Costa, Green, &
Abutalebi, 2018; Luk, Green, Abutalebi, & Grady, 2011). The study assesses whether these BLC
processes are affected in HD patients as a result of the degeneration of the striatum.
Executive control impairments are present in HD patients from the early onset of disease
and even in pre-symptomatic stages. Some studies have reported EC deficits in switching abilities
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
4
(Aron et al., 2003), attention (Maurage et al., 2017; Thompson et al., 2010), conflict monitoring
and inhibitory control (Baake, Reijntjes, Dumas, Thompson, & Roos, 2017; Lemiere,
Decruyenaere, Evers-Kiebooms, Vandenbussche, & Dom, 2004; Paulsen et al., 2014; Peinemann
et al., 2005). Recent studies have found a clear correlation between EC deficits in HD and the
hypometabolism of the striatum, one of the primary subcortical structures affected in HD patients
(e.g., Georgiou-Karistianis et al., 2013; Montoya, Price, Menear, & Lepage, 2006; Paulsen et al.,
2014; Rosas et al., 2006; Vandenberghe, Demaerel, Dom, & Maes, 2009; Vandervert, 2016).
Also, the degree of degeneration of this brain area predicts the longitudinal clinical outcomes in
HD patients and the cognitive decline over time (Domínguez et al., 2017; Tabrizi et al., 2013).
Essentially, the pathology affecting the striatum leads to an excessive inhibition which is
counterproductive when performing EC control tasks (Aron et al., 2003) and response selection
in particular (Aron et al., 2003; Lawrence, Sahakian, & Robbins, 1998; Smith & Shadmehr, 2000).
While EC deficits have consistently been found in HD patients, currently, no evidence
links these deficits to language control. Huntington’s disease patients have deficits in language
learning (De Diego-Balaguer et al., 2008), verb naming (Kargieman et al., 2014), articulatory
planning for speech (Skodda et al., 2016), and syntactic and sentence processing (Hinzen et al.,
2017; Teichmann, Gaura, et al., 2008; Teichmann, Darcy, Bachoud-Lévi, & Dupoux, 2009;
Teichmann, Dupoux, Cesaro, & Bachoud-Lévi, 2008; Ullman et al., 1997). Given that pathologies
with degeneration in the same subcortical network, such as PD, induce deficits in BLC processes
(Cattaneo et al., 2015), this relationship may suggest that basal ganglia play a role in language
control as well.
1.1. The role of subcortical structures in the BLC network
The striatum, and specifically the caudate nuclei, is described as an essential part of the
BLC network that also includes the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, supplementary motor area,
anterior cingulate cortex and cerebellum (Abutalebi & Green, 2007, 2016; Hervais-Adelman,
Moser-Mercer, & Golestani, 2011; Luk et al., 2011; Pliatsikas & Luk, 2016; Seo, Stocco, & Prat,
2018). According to Abutalebi and Green (2007), the striatum is involved in selecting and
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
5
controlling the activation or inhibition of the language in use. Moreover, some studies have
highlighted that the activation of the prefrontal cortex and striatum is modulated by the linguistic
context in which bilinguals are communicating. For instance, Abutalebi at al. (2008) have found
that the left caudate was activated when participants were required to switch languages (dual-
language context), but not when participants had to switch intra-language (naming pictures as
objects or actions in a single-language context). Similarly, the activation of the left caudate occurs
only in cross-language priming conditions. Crinion et al. (2006) presented sequential word pairs
and instructed bilinguals to judge the pairs’ semantic relatedness. As a key part of the experiment,
the target word was either in the same or a different language. The results revealed an increased
activation of the left head of the caudate when word pairs were in different languages (dual-
language context) but not when they were in the same language (but see also Ali, Green, Kherif,
Devlin, & Price, 2010, for a more general effect of verbal interference and caudate). Moreover,
the crucial role of this area in language control has been demonstrated in lesion and brain
stimulation studies. That is, electrical stimulation of (Wang, Wang, Jiang, Wang, & Wu, 2012)
and brain damage to the left caudate generate pathological language switching and mixing
(Abutalebi, Miozzo, & Cappa, 2000; Aglioti, Beltramello, Girardi, & Fabbro, 1996; Aglioti &
Fabbro, 1993; Ansaldo, Saidi, & Ruiz, 2010; Calabria, Marne, Romero-Pinel, Juncadella, &
Costa, 2014; Fabbro, Skrap, & Aglioti, 2000; Garcia-Caballero et al., 2007; Kong, Abutalebi,
Lam, & Weekes, 2014; Leemann, Laganaro, Schwitter, & Schnider, 2007; Mariën, Abutalebi,
Engelborghs, & De Deyn, 2005).
Caudate nuclei (and frontostriatal circuits) have a direct role in the inhibition of
inappropriate behaviours (Aron, 2011; Casey, Durston, & Fossella, 2001; Rao et al., 2014) and
of non-target language in bilingual production (Abutalebi & Green, 2016; Abutalebi & Green,
2007). Language activation and inhibition are control abilities that are essential to a bilingual’s
ability to switch between languages. At the behavioural level, language inhibition is usually
measured by eliciting language switching costs, which are the increase in naming latencies that
occurs when bilinguals name a picture after switching languages. Evidence suggests that the
language switch cost is greater in bilingual patients with basal ganglia degeneration (as in PD)
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
6
compared to healthy controls, but not when the same patients perform non-linguistic task
switching (Cattaneo et al., 2015). These results suggest that the basal ganglia play a special role
in BLC. Abutalebi and Green (2016) have proposed that the left caudate nucleus is involved in
dual-language contexts and its activation likely reflects the burden associated with language
selection processes. In their revised model of BLC, Abutalebi and Green (2016) described the
caudate nuclei as an access gate that regulates the activations of both the prefrontal cortex and
posterior cortical regions in dual-language contexts.
Given that caudate nuclei are one of the main subcortical structures affected in HD,
deficits in BLC may be expected as well. Regarding EC deficits in HD patients, it has been
proposed that striatum degeneration would lead to excessive inhibition control (Aron et al., 2003)
or impaired response selection (Aron et al., 2003; Lawrence, Sahakian, & Robbins, 1998; Smith
& Shadmehr, 2000). In observing BLC, deficits in language inhibition are expected in HD
patients, as proposed by neural models of BLC (Abutalebi & Green, 2008, 2016).
1.2. The present study
We explore HD patientsperformance in two tasks that reveal cross-language interference
and language activation/inhibition: the Stroop task and the language switching task.
We use Stroop task to assess the integrity of the lexical retrieval system and the efficiency
in preventing cross-language interference in HD patients. Participants are asked to name the text
colour of words written in their dominant-language while ignoring the words’ meaning. In the
within-language condition, the words are presented in the same language in which the response
has to be given (dominant language); in the between-language condition, the words are presented
in the non-dominant language of the participants. The performance of HD patients in this task is
a strong predictor of the progression from the pre-manifestation to the manifest stage of the
disease (Baake, Reijntjes, Dumas, Thompson, & Roos, 2017). Therefore, it is expected that HD
patients would perform worse than healthy controls in both conditions. Additionally, it is
predicted that patients would display a larger Stroop effect (semantic interference) in the between-
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
7
rather than within-language condition as compared to controls. This prediction is based on
evidence that the left caudate is more sensitive to conditions in which bilinguals must manage
two languages (e.g., Abutalebi et al., 2008; Crinion et al., 2006). Hence, considering that HD
patients suffer from damage to this structure, one would expect the between-language condition
to be especially affected.
We use language switching to investigate the integrity of inhibitory control related to
language reconfiguration/reactivation. Specifically, we compare the individual performance on
two versions of the language switching task which differ in one parameter, the cue-stimulus
interval (CSI). This experimental manipulation is introduced to see whether the reconfiguration
of a new language is influenced by the time needed for inhibition to dissipate. In one version of
the task, the cue for the naming language is presented simultaneously with the picture to be named
(short CSI) whereas, in a second version, the cue is presented one second before the actual picture
(long CSI). In comparing these two task designs, previous studies have demonstrated that the
magnitude of the switch costs tends to be larger with short CSIs and is reduced as CSIs increase
(Costa & Santesteban, 2004; Fink & Goldrick, 2015; Ma, Li, & Guo, 2016; Mosca & Clahsen,
2016; Verhoef, Roelofs, & Chwilla, 2009). This reduction is thought to index people’s ability to
overcome inhibition of the non-target language in changing the current language. That is, if people
are given more time between the cue signalling a new naming language and the picture to name
(long CSI), they experience less of a switch cost. This suggests that the inhibition applied to the
previous language has already disappeared and it no longer is able to interfere with production of
the new language.
In undertaking this experiment, our hypothesis is that HD patients will need longer period
of time to override the effect of inhibition from the previous language. This is based on the
evidence that HD patients would have less efficient inhibitory control, as shown in larger switch
costs with long CSIs while performing non-linguistic task switching (Aron, Watkins, et al., 2003).
Similarly, we expect that HD patients will not benefit as much as healthy controls from this longer
preparation time and that, consequentially, they will show similar switch costs for both CSIs.
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
8
In the language switching task, we also explore the so-called mixing costs, an additional
measure of language control abilities (see Cattaneo et al., 2015 for evidence with bilingual PD
patients). This cost is calculated by subtracting the naming latencies of the trials presented in a
block-design language naming condition from those of the repeat trials (in the mixed condition).
This measure is introduced to report a more general and sustained control of BLC (proactive
control, Cattaneo et al., 2015). We expect that, if this type of control is not specifically related
to language activation/inhibition, the magnitude of mixing costs would be the same in HD patients
and healthy controls. Furthermore, HD patients would show a similar decrease in mixing costs as
a function of preparation time when compared with healthy controls (Ma et al., 2015). Therefore,
we predict that proactive control would not be affected in HD patients as not directly related to
basal ganglia activity (e.g., De Pisapia & Braver, 2006).
In the current study, we do not include neuroimaging data as our hypotheses are based on
findings from other studies that the subcortical structures (specifically the striatum) are affected
from the onset of the pre-symptomatic stage of HD (Georgiou-Karistianis et al., 2013; Rosas et
al., 2006; Vandenberghe et al., 2009). Therefore, damage to the striatum in our HD sample is
inferred based on this evidence along with the presence of symptoms (e.g., motor impairments)
that are clinical markers of the dysfunction of this subcortical area.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
Twelve bilingual individuals with positive genetic testing (CAG repeats mean = 42.75 ±
1.5, 41-46) for HD (10 females, mean age = 42.2 ± 5.5, mean education = 13.9 ± 1.9) and fourteen
healthy controls matched for age and education (10 females, mean age = 41.9 ± 4.1, p = 0.87;
mean education = 14.6 ± 1.7, p = 0.31) participated in this study. All individuals with HD were
recruited at the Movement Disorders Unit of the Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau in Barcelona,
diagnosed by an experienced neurologist and evaluated using the Unified Huntington's Disease
Rating Scale (UHDRS; Kieburtz, 1996). Eight of 12 participants were in the pre-symptomatic
stage of the disease (UHDRS score ≤4, Diagnostic confidence level <4). Symptomatic patients
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
9
had a mild degree of motor impairments according to the UHDRS (13.17 ± 18.2). On the total
functional capacity (TFC) scale, HD patients were largely functional (12.33 ± .8, 11-13) with all
falling within early-stage or stage I HD according to Shoulson et al. (1989). Neuropsychiatric
symptoms were also present to a minor extent in the HD group (apathy= 3.8 ± 4.8; depression=
6.0 ± 7.1; irritability= 4.5 ± 6.4) and no patient had any psychotic disorders. As part of the
UHDRS, the Symbol Digit Modality Test, Stroop Colour Word Test and a verbal fluency test
were included to measure cognitive deficits. The scores on cognitive tests were comparable to
those of pre-symptomatic or early-stage HD patients in previous studies (e.g., De Diego-Balaguer
et al., 2008; Hinzen et al., 2017) (Symbol Digit Modality Test = 45.30 ± 15.8, Stroop Colour
Word Test = 42.2 ± 12.77, verbal fluency = 22.40 ± 4.32).
As for the clinical symptoms of the disease, symptomatic patients were significantly
different from pre-symptomatic in their motor score on the UHDRS (p< .01), but not in the
cognitive score (p= .09). Additionally, the degree of neuropsychiatric symptoms (ps > .25) and
disease burden (p= .11) were not found to be statistically different between the two groups.
2.1.1. Linguistic profile
Language history and dominance were determined by means of a questionnaire
administered to participants as well as interviews conducted during the experimental session.
Each participant self-rated their proficiency in two languages Catalan and Spanish on a four-
point scale of their speaking, comprehension, writing and reading abilities (1 = poor, 2 = regular,
3 = good, 4 = perfect) (See Appendix). As illustrated in Table 1, both patients and healthy controls
were highly proficient in all four linguistic domains. Participants were considered to be early
bilinguals as, on average, they had been regularly exposed to their non-dominant language starting
at 6 years old. Age of acquisition was not significantly different between the two languages (p >
.05).
Language use was examined by means of ten questions in which participants were
required to report how frequently they spoke the two languages during their lifetime; scores were
transformed into percentages, with 0% being equivalent to using only Spanish, 100% being
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
10
equivalent to using only Catalan, and 50% being equivalent to balanced use of the two languages.
Both patients and healthy controls reported using Catalan and Spanish in with equal frequency
(57.3% and 56.3% respectively). Given that all participants were early bilinguals and reported
similar proficiency in both languages, no objective criteria were used to define individual
language dominance. The preference expressed by participants when asked which language they
felt more comfortable speaking was used as a proxy. Seven HD patients and seven healthy
controls reported greater dominance in Spanish; the remaining participants reported greater
dominance in Catalan.
INSERT TABLE 1 ABOUT HERE
2.2. Materials and procedure
All participants were tested in the two versions of language switching, one with short and
one with long CSI, as well as in within-language and between-languages versions of a Stroop
task. In most cases, the tasks were administered in one experimental session. When time
constraints posed a challenge, the tasks were run during two sessions a week apart (for 9 out 12
HD patients). All tasks were administered using a laptop (screen 15.6” and resolution of 1280 x
800) and controlled with the DMDX software (Forster & Forster, 2003). Responses were analysed
offline, and naming latencies were measured using the Checkvocal software (Protopapas, 2007).
2.2.1. Language switching
Two sets of eight pictures of objects (with non-cognate names) were selected from
Snodgrass and Vanderwart (1980). The two sets were designed such that different pictures
appeared within the two versions of the language switching task (short vs. long CSI).
In each task, participants were required to name eight pictures in Catalan or in Spanish as
quickly as possible according to a cue (the Catalan or the Spanish flags). The pictures were
presented in single and mixed blocks. In the single blocks, the naming language (Spanish or
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
11
Catalan) was held constant for all trials; in the mixed blocks, participants had to name the pictures
according to the flag cue on the screen. The mixed blocks include two types of trials: repeat trials
in which participants had to name objects in the same language used in the previous trial; and
switch trials in which participants were required to name objects in the language other than the
one used in the previous trial (see Cattaneo et al., 2015).
Each of the two versions of the task included a total of 96 trials in the single block
condition and 96 trials in the mixed block condition (33 Spanish repeat trials, 33 Catalan repeat
trials, 15 Spanish switch trials and 15 Catalan switch trials). The proportions of switch and repeat
trials were 31% and 69%, respectively. Every trial began with a fixation point (a white cross) in
the centre of the screen displayed for 500 ms, followed by a cue (a Spanish or Catalan flag) and
then by the picture for a maximum of 2500 ms. In the short CSI version of the task, the cue and
target appeared at the same time (CSI 0); in the long CSI version, the cue was displayed for 1000
ms (CSI 1000), and then the picture appeared for a maximum of 2500 ms.
2.2.2. Stroop task
Nine adjectives in Spanish and Catalan were selected as distractor words, and three ink
colours were included in each language (green, red and yellow). Participants were required to
name the colour in which the words were printed, and each colour was presented with three types
of distractor words. In the within-language condition, the ink colour was named in the
participant’s dominant language (Spanish or Catalan depending on each participant), and the
distractor word belonged also to the dominant language (see Participants section for the
classification of language dominance). In the between-language version, the distractor words
were in the non-dominant language and the ink colour was named in the participant’s dominant
language.
The three conditions for distractors were as follows: 1) non-colour words printed in
yellow, red or blue, called the “neutral” condition; 2) names of the colours printed with a
congruent ink colour, the congruent condition; and 3) names of the colours printed with a
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
12
different ink colour, the incongruent condition. Green ink was used as a filler and was presented
in combination with the words of the three conditions.
Each trial was organized as follows: a fixation point was presented for 1000 ms, then a
word appeared on the screen for a maximum of 2000 ms. For each version of the task, each word-
colour combination was presented four times for a total of 96 trials (72 experimental and 24
fillers). Each experimental session began with a block of ten practice trials.
3. Statistical analysis
Both naming latencies and accuracy were analysed. Latencies exceeding three standard
deviations above or below a participant’s mean and incorrect responses were excluded from the
analyses. Verbal responses were classified as errors in the following cases: omission, when a
participant was unable to name an object; “lexical, when a participant produced a word that was
not semantically or phonologically related to the target; cross-language intrusion, when a
participant produced the correct word but in the non-requested language; and phonological,
when the participant deleted, substituted or added phonemes to the correct word related to the
picture.
The language switching data was analysed with a repeated measures ANOVA with type
of trial (single, repeat, and switch), language (dominant, non-dominant), and CSI (short, long) as
within-subject factors, and group (HD patients, healthy controls) as between-subject factors.
The Stroop task data was analysed with a repeated measures ANOVA with type of trial
(neutral, congruent and incongruent) and language context (within-language, between-languages)
as within-subject factors, and group (HD patients, healthy controls) as between-subject factors.
Both F1 (by-participants) and F2 (by-items) values are reported within the statistics
performed for the language switching task and the Stroop task.
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
13
4. Results
4.1. Language switching
Naming latencies. The omnibus ANOVA revealed differences between the two versions
of the language switching task as illustrated by a significant main effect of CSI [F1 (1, 24) = 16.16,
p < .001, η2 = .40; F2 (2, 14) = 16.77, p < .01, η2 = .54] and interaction between CSI and type of
trial [F1 (2, 48) = 8.11, p < .01, η2 = .25; F2 (2, 28) = 3.34, p < .05, η2 = .19]. Moreover, the
significant interaction between type of trial and group [F1 (2, 48) = 6.35, p < .01, η2 = .21; F2 (2,
28) = 5.89, p < .05, η2 = .29] suggested different patterns (or magnitude) of switching costs
between patients and healthy controls. To explore these different patterns, each task version was
analysed separately.
4.1.2. Short CSI (0 ms)
Naming latencies. The results revealed a significant main effect of type of trial [F1 (2, 48)
= 61.59, p < .001, η2 = .72; F2 (2, 28) = 47.79, p < .001, η2 = .77]. Naming latencies for three
conditions were significantly different among them (single trials: 912 ms; repeat trials: 1000 ms;
switch trials: 1078 ms; all ps < .05) indicating the presence of both switch and mixing costs (see
Figure 1). The main effect of group was also significant [F1 (1, 24) = 9.01, p < .01, η2 = .27; F2
(1, 14) = 21.44, p < .001, η2 = .60] indicating that HD patients were slower (1121 ms) than healthy
controls (872 ms). No other interactions or main effects were statistically significant.
Accuracy. The results revealed a significant main effect of type of trial [F1 (2, 48) = 6.28,
p < .01, η2 = .21; F2 (2, 28) = 21.44, p < .001, η2 = .60], and post-hoc analyses revealed that
accuracy was lower for switch (95.9%) and repeat (96.5%) trials compared to single trials (99.0%,
p < .05). The main effect of language was significant only in the by-participants analysis [F1 (1,
24) = 4.49, p < .05, η2 = .16; F2 (2, 28) = .84, p = .38] indicating that accuracy was slightly higher
in the dominant (97.8%) than the non-dominant language (96.5%). Finally, HD patients (94.5%)
were less accurate than healthy controls (99.8%) [F1 (1, 24) = 5.56, p < .05, η2 = .18; F2 (1, 14) =
5.42, p < .05, η2 = .28], and their performance was modulated by the type of trial [group x type of
trial interaction: F (2, 48) = 5.72, p < .01, η2 = .19; F2 (2, 28) = 3.25, p = .05, η2 = .19]. Post-hoc
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
14
analyses revealed that HD patients were less accurate in switch (92.2%) than single trials (98.1%,
p < .05) while healthy controls performed with the same level of accuracy in all type of trials.
4.1.3. Long CSI (1000 ms)
Naming Latencies. The results revealed a significant main effect of type of trial [F1 (1,
48) = 16.88, p < .001, η2 = .41; F2 (2, 28) = 21.72, p < .001, η2 = .61], and post-hoc analyses
showed that naming latencies were fastest for single trials (877 ms), slowest for switch trials (959
ms) and trials in the middle for repeat (916 ms) (ps < .05). Moreover, the main effect group [F1
(1, 24) = 10.90, p < .01, η2 = .31; F2 (1, 14) = 4.53, p = .05, η2 = .21] and the interaction between
group and type of trial [F1 (1, 48) = 3.58, p < .05, η2 = .13; F2 (2, 28) = 4.05, p < .05, η2 = .22]
were significant. No other main effects or interactions were statistically significant.
The significant interaction between group and type of trial suggests differences in the
magnitude of switch and mixing costs between patients and healthy controls. To further assess
these potential differences, we used proportional costs to account for group differences in speed
of processing. Proportional switch costs were calculated as the difference between naming
latencies in switch trials and repeat trials (mixed blocks) divided by naming latencies in repeat
trials. Likewise, proportional mixing costs were calculated as the difference between naming
latencies in repeat and single trials divided by naming latencies in single trials. Said proportional
costs were calculated for each participant.
Proportional switch costs were significantly higher in HD patients (6.2%) than healthy
controls (1.7%) [F1 (1, 26) = 5.07, p < .05, η2 = .17; F2 (1, 14) = 5.44, p = .05, η2 = .28].
Additionally, a t-test revealed that this cost was not significantly different from zero in healthy
controls [t (13) = 1.19, p = .26] while in HD patients it was significantly different from zero [t
(11) = 4.83, p = .01]. Mixing costs were not significantly different between the two groups of
participants [healthy control: 4.7%, HD patients: 4.0% [F1 (1, 26) = .04, p = .84; F2 (1, 14) = .01,
p = .94].
Accuracy. The results revealed a significant main effect of type of trial [F1 (2, 48) = 3.11,
p = .05, η2 = .11; F2 (2, 28) = 4.91, p < .05, η2 = .26], and post-hoc analyses demonstrated that
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
15
accuracy was lower for switch (97.5%) and repeat trials (97.6%) than single trials (99.0%, p <
.05). Huntington’s disease patients were also significantly less accurate (96.3%) compared to
healthy controls (99.8%) [F1 (1, 24) = 6.25, p = .02, η2 = .21; F2 (1, 14) = 4.91, p < .05, η2 = .17].
INSERT FIGURE 1 ABOUT HERE
4.2. Stroop task
Naming latencies. The by-participants ANOVA showed a significant main effect of
language context [F1 (1, 24) = 17.55, p < .001, η2 = .42], with slower naming latencies in the
within-language (863 ms) than the between-languages condition (821 ms) (Figure 2). However,
the main effect of language context was not significant in the by-items ANOVA [F2 (1, 10) = .55,
p = .48].
The main effect of type of trial was also significant [F1 (2, 48) = 44.22, p < .001, η2 = .65;
F2 (2, 20) = 24.54, p < .001, η2 = .71]. Post-hoc analysis suggested that naming latencies were
significantly different between the neutral (798 ms) and the incongruent conditions (920 ms, p <
.001) but not between the neutral and the congruent conditions (807 ms) (p = .36).
The main effect of group was also significant [F1 (1, 24) = 6.14, p < .05, η2 = .20; F2 (2,
20) = 12.65, p < .01, η2 = .56], indicating that HD patients were slower overall (921 ms) than
healthy controls (762 ms).
However, the interaction between the main effects of language context and type of trial
was not significant [F1 (2, 48) = .44, p = .65; F2 (2, 20) = .03, p = .97] indicating that the within-
and between-language versions were not different in terms of Stroop effects.
Accuracy. The main effect of type of trial was significant [F1 (2, 48) = 4.81, p < .05, η2 =
.17; F2 (2, 20) = 6.27, p < .01, η2 = .38]. Post-hoc analysis suggested that accuracy was
significantly lower in the incongruent (99.2%) than the neutral condition (99.8%), but no
difference in accuracy was found between the neutral and congruent (99.9%, p = .37) conditions.
No other main effect or interaction resulted significant in the analysis, suggesting that two groups
of participants performed at the same level (HD patients = 99.4%, controls = 99.8%; F1 (1, 24) =
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
16
2.44, p = .14; F2 (1, 10) = 1.37, p = .27) and with no difference between the two versions of the
task.
INSERT FIGURE 2 ABOUT HERE
5. Discussion and conclusion
We studied how two components of BLC (language activation/inhibition and cross-
language interference) are impaired by a disease that affects the integrity of the striatum (HD).
The results revealed a clear dissociation between these two mechanisms: while language
activation/inhibition is affected by the pathology, presumably due to dysfunctions in the striatum,
cross-language interference is not.
To test language activation/inhibition, we explored the modulation of preparation time
for selecting the target language in a language switching task. The measure used for this BLC
mechanism was the language switch cost; specifically, the difference in naming latencies between
switch and repeat trials in a mixed-language condition. This language switch cost is often
interpreted as resulting from the residual activation of the previous language, which interferes
with lexical access in the current language (Philipp, Gade, & Koch, 2007) or reactive inhibition
of the non-target language (Green, 1998). Importantly, this cost is reduced or even disappears
when bilinguals have enough time to prepare the new naming language, as seen when they are
given a longer preparation time (longer CSIs). We hypothesised that, since language activation
and selection involves basal ganglia activity and more specifically striatum (Abutalebi & Green,
2008, 2016; Luk et al., 2011), patients with dysfunctions in these subcortical areas would not
benefit from longer preparation times. Indeed, HD patients did not benefit from preparation time,
suffering the same switch cost in both short or long CSI conditions, unlike healthy controls in
which such cost was reduced as CSI became longer (Costa & Santesteban, 2004; Fink & Goldrick,
2015; Ma et al., 2016; Mosca & Clahsen, 2016; Verhoef et al., 2009).
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
17
Interestingly, however, in terms of switch costs, HD patients did not seem to be impaired
in the language switching task with short CSIs since they showed comparable performance to
healthy controls. This result contrasts with findings from a previous study in which bilingual PD
patients experienced greater costs than healthy controls when switching between languages with
short CSIs, suggesting that basal ganglia play a role in language inhibition (or reactive control
according other EC models such as Braver, 2012). These contrasting observations are likely due
to the fact that most of the patients tested in our study were in a pre-symptomatic stage or very
early stage of the disease, and consequently only subtle changes in cognitive impairment can be
detected. Pre-symptomatic HD patients are usually less impaired that those who are in the
symptomatic stage of the disease for EC, and their deficits can isolated to specific tasks (De
Diego-Balaguer et al., 2008) or EC processes (e.g., Maurage et al., 2017; Tabrizi et al., 2013; You
et al., 2014; for a review see Papp, Kaplan, & Snyder, 2011). In our sample, symptomatic patients
were different from pre-symptomatic patients solely in the manifestation of motor impairments;
we therefore assume that they were equivalent in terms of EC deficits.
Our results also speak to the difference between reactive and proactive control in HD. As
described above, proactive control was measured by mixing costs, defined as the difference in
naming latencies between repeat trials in the mixed language condition and single trials in the
blocked language condition. Proactive control is a flexible control system needed to actively
maintain goal-relevant information in a sustained manner (Cattaneo et al., 2015; Ma et al., 2016).
Bilinguals use this type of control to actively maintain the two languages and monitor them during
the task. We did not find any impairment of this mechanism due to the disease, and no difference
was found in the magnitude of mixing costs between the HD patients and controls. Moreover,
with longer CSIs, the magnitude of the mixing cost decreased in the same way as in healthy
controls, suggesting that proactive language control is not impaired and is a sustained mechanism
of BLC. Therefore, the dissociation between the impaired reactive control and the spared
proactive one suggests that these controls are two distinct mechanisms dependent on qualitatively
dissimilar processes, as Braver suggests for the EC system (2012; 2003). This result is interesting
because it provides evidence for a specificity of mechanisms to subcortical structures such as the
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
18
striatum within the BLC network, as suggested in Abutalebi and Green’s (2016) neural adaptation
hypothesis. According to this revised version of the BLC network, these structures play a role in
language selection (activation and inhibition) in conversation contexts that are more demanding
because bilinguals switch back and forth continuously (dual-language) (see also Yan et al., 2018).
These structures activate and inhibit languages in cooperation with the frontal areas for conflict
resolution and parietal areas for maintaining language representations (see also Abutalebi &
Green, 2008a; Branzi, Della Rosa, Canini, Costa, & Abutalebi, 2016; Seo, Stocco, & Prat, 2018;
Zou, Ding, Abutalebi, Shu, & Peng, 2012). Alternatively, and specifically in relation to EC
deficits in HD patients, it has been proposed that longer switch costs are related to response
suppression at selection level (for a general account on response selection on HD patients see
Lawrence, Sahakian, & Robbins, 1998). For instance, Aron et al. (2003) demonstrated that HD
patients were greatly impaired compared to healthy controls in repeated response relative to
alternated response in task switching. The researchers argued that excessive inhibition applied to
the response that had just been performed impaired the suppression of the response and had a
negative carry-over effect on the selection of the new response. Although we do not have
measures of the brain damage of our patients, we speculate that our results may be interpreted
similarly as due to dysfunction of basal ganglia in HD: the inhibition applied to the language used
just before a switch was so excessive that it persisted even with longer preparation times.
To study the mechanisms of language interference, we used a within- and a between-
language version of the Stroop task. Although patients were overall slower than healthy controls,
they did not suffer more semantic interference in the incongruent condition. In fact, the overall
pattern of results in both conditions were very similar for HD patients and healthy controls. This
observation contrasts with our predictions. One potential explanation of why HD patients do not
seem to have deficits in managing cross-language interference may be because our task only
involved production in one language. It is then possible that the striatum is mostly involved in
resolving cross-language interference in conditions where the two languages are both potential
candidates to be produced as in the case of the language switching task
i
.
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
19
To conclude, our results would suggest that the striatum is a crucial structure in certain
mechanisms involved in BLC. In particular, seeing that HD patients exhibit dysfunction in the
striatum, it appears that said structure participates in the mechanisms that control language
activation/inhibition. What is less clear is the involvement of the striatum in cross-language
interference resolution, at least when only one language is used for production.
However, it must be acknowledged that the current study did not include neuroimaging
data that would make it possible to establish a direct correlation between the damage of the
striatum and language switching deficits in patients. Our hypothesis was based on the findings
from other studies which reveal that the subcortical structures are affected since the start of the
pre-symptomatic stage of HD and they consistently indicated that striatum is the one most
damaged structures in pre-symptomatic patients (for volumetric reductions see Georgiou-
Karistianis et al., 2013; for alterations in diffusion indices of basal ganglia see Rosas et al., 2006;
Vandenberghe et al., 2009). Future neuroimaging studies would help to make a more direct
connection between how damage to this structure alters bilinguals’ abilities to control their two
languages.
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
20
1
Moreover, the activation of the left caudate in monolinguals performing the Stroop task has been reported (Ali et al.,
2010), and this result has been interpreted as reflecting the suppression of verbal interference. It may be the case that
in HD patients, the degeneration of the striatum affects performance in the Stroop task as a deficit of verbal response
inhibition independent of the language used. Indeed, this relationship would explain why this task is sometimes
sensitive to detecting early cognitive deficits in monolingual patients with HD (Baake et al., 2017; Montoya, Price,
Menear, & Lepage, 2006; Peavy et al., 2010; Peinemann et al., 2005; Stout et al., 2011; Tabrizi et al., 2013).
Acknowledgments
The research was supported by grants from the Spanish government (PSI2014-52181-P,
PSI2014-52210-P and PI17/01885), AEI/FEDER and UE (PSI2017-87784-R), Catalan
government (SGR 2009-1521 and 2017SGR268), La Marató de TV3 Foundation (373/C/2014)
and European Research Council under the European Community’s Seventh Framework (FP7/
20072013 Cooperation Grant Agreement 613465-AThEME). MC was supported by the
postdoctoral Ramón y Cajal fellowship (RYC2013-14013). JPP was supported by the Juan Rodés
Program (JR16/00034) and Instituto de Salud Carlos III (Ministerio de Economía, Industria y
Competitividad).
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
21
Figure 1. Naming latencies, proportional switch and mixing costs for the language switching task
as a function of CSI and group of participants
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
22
Figure 2. Naming latencies, proportional semantic and identity effects for the Stroop tasks as a
function of language context and group of participants
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
23
References
Abutalebi, J., Annoni, J. M., Zimine, I., Pegna, A. J., Seghier, M. L., Lee-Jahnke, H.,
Lazeyras, F., Cappa, S. F., & Khateb, A. (2008). Language control and lexical
competition in bilinguals: An event-related fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 18(7), 1496
1505.
Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Gonzaga, A. K. C., Keim, R., Costa, A., & Perani, D.
(2013). The role of the left putamen in multilingual language production. Brain and
Language, 125(3), 307315.
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W. (2007). Bilingual language production: The neurocognition
of language representation and control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20, 242275.
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W. (2008a). Control mechanisms in bilingual language
production: Neural evidence from language switching studies. Language and
Cognitive Processes, 23(4), 557582.
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. W. (2016). Neuroimaging of language control in bilinguals:
neural adaptation and reserve. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(April), 1
10.
Abutalebi, J., Miozzo, M., & Cappa, S. F. (2000). Do subcortical structures control
“language selection” in polyglots? Evidence from pathological language mixing.
Neurocase, 6, 5156.
Aglioti, S., Beltramello, A., Girardi, F., & Fabbro, F. (1996). Neurolinguistic and follow-
up study of an unusual pattern of recovery from bilingual subcortical aphasia. Brain :
A Journal of Neurology, 119 (5), 15511564.
Aglioti, S., & Fabbro, F. (1993). Paradoxical selective recovery in a bilingual aphasic
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
24
following subcortical lesions. Neuroreport, 4(12), 13591362.
Ali, N., Green, D. W., Kherif, F., Devlin, J. T., & Price, C. J. (2010). The Role of the Left
Head of Caudate in Suppressing Irrelevant Words. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
22(10), 23692386.
Ansaldo, A. I., Saidi, L. G., & Ruiz, A. (2010). Model‐driven intervention in bilingual
aphasia: Evidence from a case of pathological language mixing. Aphasiology, 24(2),
309324.
Aron, A. R. (2011). From reactive to proactive and selective control: Developing a richer
model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biological Psychiatry, 69(12), e55-68.
Aron, A. R., Schlaghecken, F., Fletcher, P. C., Bullmore, E. T., Eimer, M., Barker, R.,
Sahakian B. J., Robbins, T. W. (2003). Inhibition of subliminally primed responses is
mediated by the caudate and thalamus: Evidence from functional MRI and
Huntington’s disease. Brain, 126(3), 713723.
Aron, A. R., Watkins, L., Sahakian, B. J., Monsell, S., Barker, R. A., & Robbins, T. W.
(2003). Task-Set Switching Deficits in Early-Stage Huntington’s Disease:
Implications for Basal Ganglia Function. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(5),
629642.
Baake, V., Reijntjes, R. H. A. M., Dumas, E. M., Thompson, J. C., & Roos, R. A. C.
(2017). Cognitive decline in Huntington’s disease expansion gene carriers. Cortex, 95,
5162.
Braver, T. S. (2012). The variable nature of cognitive control: A dual-mechanisms
framework. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 106113.
Braver, T. S., Reynolds, J. R., & Donaldson, D. I. (2003). Neural Mechanisms of
Transient and Sustained Cognitive Control during Task Switching. Neuron, 39(4),
713726.
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
25
Calabria, M., Costa, A., Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2018). Neural basis of bilingual
language control. Ann N Y Acad Sci. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13879.
Calabria, M., Cattaneo, G., & Costa, A. (2017). It is time to project into the future:
“Bilingualism in healthy and pathological aging.” Journal of Neurolinguistics, 43, 1
3.
Calabria, M., Marne, P., Romero-Pinel, L., Juncadella, M., & Costa, A. (2014). Losing
control of your languages: a case study. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 31(3), 266286.
Casey, B. J., Durston, S., & Fossella, J. A. (2001). Evidence for a mechanistic model of
cognitive control. Clinical Neuroscience Research, 1(4), 267282.
Cattaneo, G., Calabria, M., Marne, P., Gironell, A., Abutalebi, J., & Costa, A. (2015).
The role of executive control in bilingual language production: A study with
Parkinson’s disease individuals. Neuropsychologia, 66, 99110.
Costa, A., & Santesteban, M. (2004). Lexical access in bilingual speech production:
evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. J.
Mem.Lang., 50, 491511.
Crinion, J., Turner, R., Grogan, A., Hanakawa, T., Noppeney, U., Devlin, J. T., Aso, T.,
Urayama, S., Fukuyama, H., Stockton, K., Usui, K., Green, D. W., & Price, C. J.
(2006). Language control in the bilingual brain. Science, 312(5779), 15371540.
De Diego-Balaguer, R., Couette, M., Dolbeau, G., Dürr, A., Youssov, K., & Bachoud-
Lévi, A. C. (2008). Striatal degeneration impairs language learning: Evidence from
Huntington’s disease. Brain, 131(11), 28702881.
De Pisapia, N., & Braver, T. S. (2006). A model of dual control mechanisms through
anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions. Neurocomputing, 69(1012),
13221326.
Domínguez D, J. F., Poudel, G., Stout, J. C., Gray, M., Chua, P., Borowsky, B., Egan, G.
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
26
F., & Georgiou-Karistianis, N. (2017). Longitudinal changes in the fronto-striatal
network are associated with executive dysfunction and behavioral dysregulation in
Huntington’s disease: 30 months IMAGE-HD data. Cortex, 92, 139149.
Fabbro, F., Skrap, M., & Aglioti, S. (2000). Pathological switching between languages
after frontal lesions in a bilingual patient. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and
Psychiatry, 68(5), 650652.
Fink, A., & Goldrick, M. (2015). Pervasive benefits of preparation in language switching.
Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 22(3), 808814.
Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: a windows display program with
millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers : A
Journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc, 35(1), 116124.
Garcia-Caballero, A., Garcia-Lado, I., Gonzalez-Hermida, J., Area, R., Recimil, M. J.,
Juncos Rabadan, O., Lamas, S., Ozaita, G., & Jorge, F. J. (2007). Paradoxical recovery
in a bilingual patient with aphasia after right capsuloputaminal infarction. Journal of
Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 78(1), 8991.
Georgiou-Karistianis, N., Gray, M. A., Domínguez D, J. F., Dymowski, A. R., Bohanna,
I., Johnston, L. A., Churchyard, A., Chua, P., Stout, J. C., & Egan, G. F. (2013).
Automated differentiation of pre-diagnosis Huntington’s disease from healthy control
individuals based on quadratic discriminant analysis of the basal ganglia: The IMAGE-
HD study. Neurobiology of Disease, 51, 8292.
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system.
Bilingualism:Language and Cognition, 1, 6781.
Hervais-Adelman, A. G., Moser-Mercer, B., & Golestani, N. (2011). Executive control
of language in the bilingual brain: Integrating the evidence from neuroimaging to
neuropsychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 2: 234.
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
27
Hinzen, W., Rosselló, J., Morey, C., Camara, E., Garcia-Gorro, C., Salvador, R., & de
Diego-Balaguer, R. (2017). A systematic linguistic profile of spontaneous narrative
speech in pre-symptomatic and early stage Huntington’s disease. Cortex, 100, 7183.
Kargieman, L., Herrera, E., Baez, S., García, A. M., Dottori, M., Gelormini, C., Manes,
F., Gershanik, O., & Ibáñez, A. (2014). Motor-language coupling in Huntington’s
disease families. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 6: 122.
Kieburtz, K. (1996). Unified Huntington’s disease rating scale: Reliability and
consistency. Movement Disorders, 11(2), 136142.
Kong, A. P. H., Abutalebi, J., Lam, K. S. Y., & Weekes, B. (2014). Executive and
language control in the multilingual brain. Behavioural Neurology, 2014.
Lawrence, A. D., Sahakian, B. J., & Robbins, T. W. (1998). Cognitive functions and
corticostriatal circuits: Insights from Huntington’s disease. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 2(10), 379388.
Leemann, B., Laganaro, M., Schwitter, V., & Schnider, A. (2007). Paradoxical switching
to a barely-mastered second language by an aphasic patient. Neurocase, 13(3), 209
213.
Lemiere, J., Decruyenaere, M., Evers-Kiebooms, G., Vandenbussche, E., & Dom, R.
(2004). Cognitive changes in patients with Huntington?s disease (HD) and
asymptomatic carriers of the HD mutation. Journal of Neurology, 251(8), 935942.
Luk, G., Green, D. W., Abutalebi, J., & Grady, C. (2011). Cognitive control for language
switching in bilinguals: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging
studies. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27(10), 14791488.
Ma, F., Li, S., & Guo, T. (2016). Reactive and proactive control in bilingual word
production: An investigation of influential factors. Journal of Memory and Language,
86, 3559.
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
28
Mariën, P., Abutalebi, J., Engelborghs, S., & De Deyn, P. P. (2005). Pathophysiology of
language switching and mixing in an early bilingual child with subcortical aphasia.
Neurocase, 11 (6), 385398.
Maurage, P., Heeren, A., Lahaye, M., Jeanjean, A., Guettat, L., Verellen-Dumoulin, C.,
Halkin, S., Billieux, J., & Constant, E. (2017). Attentional impairments in
Huntington’s Disease: A specific deficit for the executive conflict. Neuropsychology,
31(4), 424436.
Montoya, A., Price, B. H., Menear, M., & Lepage, M. (2006). Brain imaging and
cognitive dysfunctions in Huntington’s disease. Journal of Psychiatry and
Neuroscience, 31(1), 2129.
Mosca, M., & Clahsen, H. (2016). Examining language switching in bilinguals: The role
of preparation time. Bilingualism, 19(2), 415424.
Paulsen, J. S., Long, J. D., Ross, C. A., Harrington, D. L., Erwin, C. J., Williams, J. K.,
et al. (2014). Prediction of manifest huntington’s disease with clinical and imaging
measures: A prospective observational study. The Lancet Neurology, 13(12), 1193
1201.
Peavy, G. M., Jacobson, M. W., Goldstein, J. L., Hamilton, J. M., Kane, A., Gamst, A.
C., Lessing, S. L., Lee, J. C., & Corey-Bloom, J. (2010). Cognitive and functional
decline in Huntington’s disease: Dementia criteria revisited. Movement Disorders,
25(9), 11631169.
Peinemann, A., Schuller, S., Pohl, C., Jahn, T., Weindl, A., & Kassubek, J. (2005).
Executive dysfunction in early stages of Huntington’s disease is associated with
striatal and insular atrophy: A neuropsychological and voxel-based morphometric
study. Journal of the Neurological Sciences, 239(1), 1119.
Philipp, A. M., Gade, M., & Koch, I. (2007). Inhibitory processes in language switching:
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
29
Evidence from switching language-defined response sets. European Journal of
Cognitive Psychology, 19(3), 395416.
Pliatsikas, C., & Luk, G. (2016). Executive control in bilinguals: A concise review on
fMRI studies. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 53(9), 16891699.
Protopapas, A. (2007). CheckVocal: a program to facilitate checking the accuracy and
response time of vocal responses from DMDX. Behavior Research Methods, 39(4),
859862.
Rao, J. A., Harrington, D. L., Durgerian, S., Reece, C., Mourany, L., Koenig, K., Lowe,
M. J., Magnotta, V. A., Long, J. D., Johnson, H. J., Paulsen, J. S., & Rao, S. M. (2014).
Disruption of response inhibition circuits in prodromal Huntington disease. Cortex,
58, 72-85.
Rosas, H. D., Tuch, D. S., Hevelone, N. D., Zaleta, A. K., Vangel, M., Hersch, S. M., &
Salat, D. H. (2006). Diffusion tensor imaging in presymptomatic and early
Huntington’s disease: Selective white matter pathology and its relationship to clinical
measures. Movement Disorders, 21(9), 13171325.
Seo, R., Stocco, A., & Prat, C. S. (2018). The bilingual language network: Differential
involvement of anterior cingulate, basal ganglia and prefrontal cortex in preparation,
monitoring, and execution. NeuroImage, 174, 4456.
Shoulson, I., Kurlan, R., Rubin, R. J., Goldblatt, D., Behr, J., & Al., E. (1989). Assessment
of functional capacity in neurodegenerative movement disorders: Huntington’s disease
as a prototype. in Quantification of Neurologic Deficit, T. Munsat (Ed.), Butterworths,
Stoneham, MA., 271-283.
Skodda, S., Grönheit, W., Lukas, C., Bellenberg, B., Von Hein, S. M., Hoffmann, R., &
Saft, C. (2016). Two different phenomena in basic motor speech performance in
premanifest Huntington disease. Neurology, 86(14), 13291335.
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
30
Smith, M. A., & Shadmehr, R. (2000). Error correction and the basal ganglia. Trends in
Cognitive Sciences, 4(10), 367369.
Stout, J. C., Paulsen, J. S., Queller, S., Solomon, A. C., Whitlock, K. B., Campbell, J. C.,
Carlozzi, N., Duff, K., Beglinger, L. J., Langbehn, D. R., Johnson, S. A., Biglan, K.
M., & Aylward, E. H. (2011). Neurocognitive Signs in Prodromal Huntington Disease.
Neuropsychology, 25(1), 114.
Tabrizi, S. J., Scahill, R. I., Owen, G., Durr, A., Leavitt, B. R., Roos, R. A., et al. (2013).
Predictors of phenotypic progression and disease onset in premanifest and early-stage
Huntington’s disease in the TRACK-HD study: Analysis of 36-month observational
data. The Lancet Neurology, 12(7), 637649.
Teichmann, M., Darcy, I., Bachoud-Lévi, A. C., & Dupoux, E. (2009). The role of the
striatum in phonological processing. Evidence from early stages of Huntington’s
disease. Cortex, 45(7), 839849.
Teichmann, M., Dupoux, E., Cesaro, P., & Bachoud-Lévi, A. C. (2008). The role of the
striatum in sentence processing: Evidence from a priming study in early stages of
Huntington’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 46(1), 174185.
Teichmann, M., Gaura, V., Démonet, J. F., Supiot, F., Delliaux, M., Verny, C., Renou,
P., Remy, P., & Bachoud-Lévi, A. C. (2008). Language processing within the striatum:
Evidence from a PET correlation study in Huntington’s disease. Brain, 131(4), 1046
1056.
Thompson, J. C., Poliakoff, E., Sollom, A. C., Howard, E., Craufurd, D., & Snowden, J.
S. (2010). Automaticity and attention in Huntington’s disease: When two hands are
not better than one. Neuropsychologia, 48(1), 171178.
Ullman, M. T., Corkin, S., Coppola, M., Hickok, G., Growdon, J. H., Koroshetz, W. J.,
& Pinker, S. (1997). A Neural Dissociation within Language: Evidence that the Mental
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
31
Dictionary Is Part of Declarative Memory, and that Grammatical Rules Are Processed
by the Procedural System. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(2), 266276.
Vandenberghe, W., Demaerel, P., Dom, R., & Maes, F. (2009). Diffusion-weighted
versus volumetric imaging of the striatum in early symptomatic Huntington disease.
Journal of Neurology, 256(1), 109114.
Vandervert, L. (2016). The prominent role of the cerebellum in the learning, origin and
advancement of culture. Cerebellum & Ataxias, 3(1), 10.
Verhoef, K., Roelofs, A., & Chwilla, D. J. (2009). Role of inhibition in language
switching: Evidence from event-related brain potentials in overt picture naming.
Cognition, 110(1), 84-99.
Wang, X., Wang, Y. Y., Jiang, T., Wang, Y. Z., & Wu, C. X. (2012). Direct evidence of
the left caudate’s role in bilingual control: An intraoperative electrical stimulation
study. Neurocase, 19(5), 462-469.
Yang, J., Ye, J., Wang, R., Zhou, K., & Wu, Y. J. (2018). Bilingual Contexts Modulate
the Inhibitory Control Network. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 395.
You, S. C., Geschwind, M. D., Sha, S. J., Apple, A., Satris, G., Wood, K. A., Possin, K.
L. (2014). Executive functions in premanifest Huntington’s disease. Movement
Disorders, 29(3), 405409.
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
32
Appendix
BILINGUALISM QUESTIONNAIRE
1. Language acquisition and usage
a. Current city of residence
If different from the place of birth, specify when (year) you moved to your current residence
b. Father’s place of birth/Language(s) spoken
c. Mother’s place of birth/Language(s) spoken
d. Catalan Age of acquisition
At what age were you exposed to Catalan?
At what age did you begin to use (speak) Catalan?
e. Spanish Age of acquisition
At what age were you exposed to Spanish?
At what age did you begin to use (speak) Spanish?
f. Where did you learn Catalan? At home At school Other
g. Where did you learn Spanish? At home At school Other
h. Specify in which language (or languages) you are used to speak to:
Father: Mother: Partner:
Sisters/Brothers: Children:
i. Do you speak any other languages besides Catalan and Spanish?
j. What was the age of acquisition for this/these language(s)?
2. Language proficiency
Rate your proficiency in the following aspects of language within Catalan, Spanish, and any other
languages you speak:
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
33
a. Speaking
Catalan
Perfect
Regular
Poor
Spanish
Perfect
Regular
Poor
L3
Perfect
Regular
Poor
L4
Perfect
Regular
Poor
b. Comprehension
Catalan
Perfect
Regular
Poor
Spanish
Perfect
Regular
Poor
L3
Perfect
Regular
Poor
L4
Perfect
Regular
Poor
c. Reading
Catalan
Perfect
Regular
Poor
Spanish
Perfect
Regular
Poor
L3
Perfect
Regular
Poor
L4
Perfect
Regular
Poor
d. Writing
Catalan
Perfect
Regular
Poor
Spanish
Perfect
Regular
Poor
L3
Perfect
Regular
Poor
L4
Perfect
Regular
Poor
e. Which language do you consider to be your dominant language?
Catalan
Spanish
f. In which language do you feel more confortable speaking?
Catalan
Spanish
Both
g. Are you able to distinguish speakers’ regional accents?
Catalan
No
In some cases
Yes, always
Spanish
No
In some cases
Yes, always
In press. Under copyright and the publisher should be contacted for permission to re-use or reprint the material in any form
34
3. Frequency of language use and dominance
In answering the questions below, specify the frequency with which you used Spanish and/or
Catalan during the different periods of your life and according to the contexts described below.
Use the following scale for your responses:
0
Only Spanish
1
Mostly Spanish, rarely Catalan
2
Frequently Spanish with ¼ of the time in Catalan
3
Same time for Spanish and Catalan
4
Frequently Catalan with ¼ of the time in Spanish
5
Mostly Catalan, rarely Spanish
6
Only Catalan
a. As a child, before formal schooling?
only Spanish
only Catalan
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
b. As an adolescent, from 4-5 to 12 years old?
only Spanish
only Catalan
At school
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
At home
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
With friends
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
c. As a teenager, from 13 to 16 years old?
only Spanish
only Catalan
At school
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
At home
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
With friends
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
d. As a young adult, from 17 to 22 years old?
only Spanish
only Catalan
At school/university
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
At home
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
With friends
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
e. In adulthood?
only Spanish
only Catalan
At work
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
At home
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
With friends
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
... To better understand the functional role of basal ganglia in language switching, Calabria et al. (2021b) studied pre-symptomatic patients with HD. Research has shown that this type of neurodegeneration affects caudate function, even in the pre-symptomatic stage of disease (Johnson & Gregory, 2019;Ross et al., 2014). ...
... B. Figure 6. (A) Cerebral atrophy of the bilingual patient with semantic dementia (Calabria et al., 2021a); (B) Semantic interference effect in bilingual patients with post-stroke aphasia (Calabria et al., 2019b) and a bilingual patient with semantic dementia (Calabria et al., 2021b) in the semantic cyclic naming task in bilinguals could be differentiated. During recent years, the small number of studies centered upon bilingual patients with PPA has increased (Calabria et al., 2021b;Costa et al., 2019;Druks & Weekes, 2013;Ellajosyula et al., 2020;Filley et al., 2006;Hernández et al., 2008;Karpathiou et al., 2018;Simões et al., 2009;Zanini et al., 2011), but neuropsychology and psycholinguistics should continue to work together to advance in this promising field of research in the future. ...
... (A) Cerebral atrophy of the bilingual patient with semantic dementia (Calabria et al., 2021a); (B) Semantic interference effect in bilingual patients with post-stroke aphasia (Calabria et al., 2019b) and a bilingual patient with semantic dementia (Calabria et al., 2021b) in the semantic cyclic naming task in bilinguals could be differentiated. During recent years, the small number of studies centered upon bilingual patients with PPA has increased (Calabria et al., 2021b;Costa et al., 2019;Druks & Weekes, 2013;Ellajosyula et al., 2020;Filley et al., 2006;Hernández et al., 2008;Karpathiou et al., 2018;Simões et al., 2009;Zanini et al., 2011), but neuropsychology and psycholinguistics should continue to work together to advance in this promising field of research in the future. All rights reserved Potential directions for this research also include studying the dissociation between comprehension and production in both languages and the comparison of language deficits in single-and dual-language naming conditions. ...
Chapter
The field of neuropsychology can contribute to bilingualism research from a multidisciplinary perspective that ranges from psycholinguistics and brain imaging studies. While the psycholinguistic approach provides the outlook on linguistic processes in experimental study of patients with brain damage, neural models define the underlying brain areas of such processes and help to predict language deficits in said patients. Current neural models of bilingualism do not provide accurate predictions of deficits in bilinguals with brain damage since they have not been tested in a systematic way. However, they do offer a roadmap for the underlying cognitive and linguistic processes of bilingual language control and speech production. In this chapter, I propose how a neurolinguistic approach to bilingualism might be implemented in neuropsychology by including: (a) the application of traditional methods of cognitive (neuro)psychology to the field of bilingualism, such as dissociations, (b) the use of psycholinguistic methods, and (c) how neurodegenerative diseases may be a neuropsychological paradigm in which one can study bilingual language processes.
... However, given that subcortical structures also tend to be primary targets for healthy ageing and also progressive neurodegenerative diseases, an interesting question emerges: do the effects of bilingualism and ageing interact, and in a way that mitigates the effects of ageing [35]? This is in the light of suggestions that bilingualism acts as a form of cognitive reserve in older age and in neurodegenerative diseases such as Huntington's disease [36] and multiple sclerosis [37] (for review, see Ref. [38]). Some corroborating evidence has been presented for the neighbouring region of the hippocampus for healthy ageing bilinguals and bilinguals diagnosed with MCI [39,40], but the literature remains extremely limited as far as the basal ganglia and the cerebellum are concerned. ...
Article
Full-text available
Experience-based neuroplasticity in the healthy brain is a well-documented finding, with functional and structural adaptations in cortical and subcortical structures reported as the brain’s response to cognitively challenging experiences. These experiences include bi-/multilingualism: speaking more than one language entails increased cognitive demands related to language acquisition, processing and control, which affect subcortical structures subserving these processes, including the basal ganglia and the cerebellum. This paper reviews evidence on bilingualism-induced subcortical neuroplasticity at the level of brain structure, function, and metabolism and explores how it interacts with brain decline. As such, it highlights bi-/multilingualism as a test case for studying long-term neuroplastic effects in the brain.
... Detailed language and demographic information data were collected. Language history was assessed using a questionnaire administered to the participants and an interview with the patient and relatives (see Calabria et al., 2021). The data collected included the following measures: Age of acquisition of each language (Catalan and Spanish), self-reported language proficiency, and language use frequency. ...
Article
Full-text available
Bilingualism has been shown to contribute to increased resilience against cognitive aging. One of the key brain structures linked to memory and dementia symptom onset, the hippocampus, has been observed to adapt in response to bilingual experience – at least in healthy individuals. However, in the context of neurodegenerative pathology, it is yet unclear what role previous bilingual experience might have in terms of sustaining integrity of this structure or related behavioral correlates. The present study adds to the limited cohort of research on the effects of bilingualism on neurocognitive outcomes in Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) using structural brain data. We investigate whether bilingual language experience (operationalized as language entropy) results in graded neurocognitive adaptations within a cohort of bilinguals diagnosed with MCI. Results reveal a non-linear effect of bilingual language entropy on hippocampal volume, although they do not predict episodic memory performance, nor age of MCI diagnosis.
... Although the literature directly examining this hypothesis is scarce, recent studies have shown that inhibition (as measured by a crosslingual Stroop task) was better preserved than language skills in bilingual individuals with HD (Calabria et al., 2020b), and also that higher degrees of bilingual use predict better inhibitory abilities in HD patients (Martínez-Horta et al., 2018). Moving from behaviour to effects of bilingualism on brain structure and function in HD specifically, Martínez-Horta et al. (2018) recently reported active bilingualism to be associated with increased GM volume in the right IFG. ...
Article
Bilingualism has been argued to have an impact on cognition and brain structure. Effects have been reported across the lifespan: from healthy children to ageing adults, including clinical (ageing) populations. It has been argued that active bilingualism may significantly contribute to the delaying of the expression of Alzheimer's disease symptoms. If bilingualism plays an ameliorative role against the expression of neurodegeneration in dementia, it is possible that it could have similar effects for other neurodegenerative disorders, including Multiple Sclerosis, Parkinson's and Huntington's Diseases. To date, however, direct relevant evidence remains limited, not least because the necessary scientific motivations for investigating this with greater depth have not yet been fully articulated. Herein, we provide a roadmap that reviews the relevant literatures, highlighting potential links across neurodegenerative disorders and bilingualism more generally.
... Although the literature directly examining this hypothesis is scarce, recent studies have shown that inhibition (as measured by a crosslingual Stroop task) was better preserved than language skills in bilingual individuals with HD (Calabria et al., 2020b), and also that higher degrees of bilingual use predict better inhibitory abilities in HD patients (Martínez-Horta et al., 2018). Moving from behaviour to effects of bilingualism on brain structure and function in HD specifically, Martínez-Horta et al. (2018) recently reported active bilingualism to be associated with increased GM volume in the right IFG. ...
Preprint
Bilingualism has been argued to have an impact on cognition and brain structure. Such effects have been reported in healthy children and young adults, but also in ageing adults, including clinical ageing populations. For example, bilingualism may significantly contribute to the delaying of the expression of Alzheimer’s dementia symptoms. If bilingualism plays an ameliorative role against neurodegeneration, it is possible that it would have similar effects for other neurodegenerative disorders, including Multiple Sclerosis, Parkinson’s and Huntington’s Diseases; however, relevant evidence remains limited. Herein, we provide a focused literature review on the effects of these progressive neurological disorders on cognition and brain structure, examine how the affected functions and brain regions map to those suggested to be impacted by bilingualism, and report the limited evidence of the impact of bilingualism on these conditions. We then examine the value of making links across neurodegenerative disorders and bilingualism, proposing that available evidence warrants claims for bilingualism- related effects more generally, with an eye at future research to fill in gaps in our understanding.
Chapter
Full-text available
Professor Albert Costa (1972-2018) was one of the most influential scholars in the fields of psycholinguistics and bilingualism. This book provides a faithful look at the most relevant lines of research in which he worked during his academic career. Written by some of his close collaborators and friends, the book presents a coherent summary of the most relevant psycholinguistic theories on language processing and bilingualism, including critical reviews to current models of lexical access, the representation of cognate words, neurolinguistic models of bilingualism, cross-linguistic effects in bimodal bilinguals (sign language), prediction processes and linguistic alignment in bilinguals, the influence of foreign-language effects in social cognition and the effects of bilingualism in emotion and decision making processing. This volume is a tribute to Prof. Costa and his work, and is born from a deep love and respect for his way of approaching the science of multilingualism from a psycholinguistic perspective.
Article
Full-text available
The aim of the study is to explore the differential effects of neurodegenerative disorders on cross-language interference and facilitation, by comparing the performance in a bilingual Stroop task of Catalan–Spanish bilinguals with different neurodegenerative disorders [Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI), n = 16; Alzheimer's disease (AD), n = 16; and Parkinson's disease (PD), n = 16)] and healthy controls (n = 14). Interference is the difference in naming latencies between the incongruent (colour words not matching with their ink colour) and control (non-colour words) conditions. Facilitation is the difference between the control and congruent conditions (colour words matching with their ink colour). The results showed that AD and MCI patients, but not PD, showed a larger interference effect than healthy controls. Nevertheless, the patient groups showed the same facilitation effect as healthy controls. The contribution of cortical and subcortical brain areas to cross-language interference suppression is discussed in relation to the type of brain degeneration.
Chapter
Bilingualism is a ubiquitous global phenomenon. Beyond being a language experience, bilingualism also entails a social experience, and it interacts with development and learning, with cognitive and neural consequences across the lifespan. The authors of this volume are world renowned experts across several subdisciplines including linguistics, developmental psychology, and cognitive neuroscience. They bring to light bilingualism’s cognitive, developmental, and neural consequences in children, young adults, and older adults. This book honors Ellen Bialystok, and highlights her profound impact on the field of bilingualism research as a lifelong experience. The chapters are organized into four sections: The first section explores the complexity of the bilingual experience beyond the common characterization of “speaking multiple languages.” The next section showcases Ellen Bialystok’s earlier impact on psychology and education; here the contributors answer the question “how does being bilingual shape children’s development?” The third section explores cognitive and neuroscientific theories describing how language experience modulates cognition, behavior, and brain structures and functions. The final section shifts the focus to the impact of bilingualism on healthy and abnormal aging and asks whether being bilingual can stave off the effects of dementia by conferring a “cognitive reserve.”
Article
Full-text available
Lifelong bilingualism may contribute to cognitive reserve (CR) in neurodegenerative diseases as shown by a delay of the age at symptom onset in bilinguals with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI). However, some studies have failed to show this bilingual advantage, suggesting that it might depend on the type and degree of bilingualism. In the present study, we tested the hypothesis that active bilingualism, defined as the continuous use of the two languages as opposed to second language exposition only, may protect against cognitive decline. Moreover, we investigated whether bilingualism as a CR factor may be explained by an advantage within the executive control (EC) system. To do so, we collected clinical measures (age at onset of cognitive symptoms, age at the first medical visit for cognitive impairments, and age at diagnosis) in patients with MCI and patients with AD with different degrees of language experience and usage of Catalan and Spanish. Additionally, all participants were tested on four EC tasks and one long-term memory recognition task. First, results from multiple regression analyses showed that active bilingualism was a significant predictor of delay in the age at onset for all the clinical measures in MCI, but not AD patients. Second, the effect of active bilingualism was independent of occupation, educational level and job attainment across the individuals’ lifespan. Finally, although we did not find an effect of active bilingualism across all EC tasks, we did find an effect for conflict resolution. These results are discussed in the context of CR hypotheses, suggesting that compensatory mechanisms may play a role in protecting against cognitive decline.
Article
Full-text available
Acquiring and speaking a second language increases demand on the processes of language control for bilingual as compared to monolingual speakers. Language control for bilingual speakers involves the ability to keep the two languages separated to avoid interference and to select one language or the other in a given conversational context. This ability is what we refer with the term “bilingual language control” (BLC). It is now well established that the architecture of this complex system of language control encompasses brain networks involving cortical and subcortical structures, each responsible for different cognitive processes such as goal maintenance, conflict monitoring, interference suppression, and selective response inhibition. Furthermore, advances have been made in determining the overlap between the BLC and the nonlinguistic executive control networks, under the hypothesis that the BLC processes are just an instantiation of a more domain‐general control system. Here, we review the current knowledge about the neural basis of these control systems. Results from brain imaging studies of healthy adults and on the performance of bilingual individuals with brain damage are discussed. Acquiring and speaking a second language (L2) increases demand on the processes of language control for bilingual as compared to monolingual speakers. Language control for bilingual speakers involves the ability to keep the two languages separated to avoid interference and to select one language or the other in a given conversational context. Here, we review the current knowledge about the neural basis of these control systems. Results from brain imaging studies of healthy adults and on the performance of bilingual individuals with brain damage are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
The present functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study investigated influences of language contexts on inhibitory control and the underlying neural processes. Thirty Cantonese–Mandarin–English trilingual speakers, who were highly proficient in Cantonese (L1) and Mandarin (L2), and moderately proficient in English (L3), performed a picture-naming task in three dual-language contexts (L1-L2, L2-L3, and L1-L3). After each of the three naming tasks, participants performed a flanker task, measuring contextual effects on the inhibitory control system. Behavioral results showed a typical flanker effect in the L2-L3 and L1-L3 condition, but not in the L1-L2 condition, which indicates contextual facilitation on inhibitory control performance by the L1-L2 context. Whole brain analysis of the fMRI data acquired during the flanker tasks showed more neural activations in the right prefrontal cortex and subcortical areas in the L2-L3 and L1-L3 condition on one hand as compared to the L1-L2 condition on the other hand, suggesting greater involvement of the cognitive control areas when participants were performing the flanker task in L2-L3 and L1-L3 contexts. Effective connectivity analyses displayed a cortical-subcortical-cerebellar circuitry for inhibitory control in the trilinguals. However, contrary to the right-lateralized network in the L1-L2 condition, functional networks for inhibitory control in the L2-L3 and L1-L3 condition are less integrated and more left-lateralized. These findings provide a novel perspective for investigating the interaction between bilingualism (multilingualism) and inhibitory control by demonstrating instant behavioral effects and neural plasticity as a function of changes in global language contexts.
Article
Full-text available
Cognitive decline accompanying the clinically more salient motor symptoms of Huntington’s disease (HD) has been widely noted and can precede motor symptoms onset. Less clear is how such decline bears on language functions in everyday life, though a small number of experimental studies have revealed difficulties with the application of rule-based aspects of language in early stages of the disease. Here we aimed to determine whether there is a systematic linguistic profile that characterizes spontaneous narrative speech in both pre-manifest and/or early manifest HD, and how it is related to striatal degeneration and neuropsychological profiles. Twenty-eight early-stage patients (19 manifest and 9 gene-carriers in the pre-manifest stage), matched with 28 controls, participated in a story-telling task. Speech was blindly scored by independent raters according to fine-grained linguistic variables distributed over 5 domains for which composite scores were computed (Quantitative, Fluency, Reference, Connectivity, and Concordance). Voxel-based morphometry was used to link specific brain degeneration patterns to loci of linguistic decline. In all of them significant differences were observed between groups. Deficits in Reference and Connectivity were seen in the pre-manifest stage, where no other neuropsychological impairment was detected. Among HD patients, there was a significant positive correlation only between the values in the Quantitative domain and gray matter volume bilaterally in the putamen and pallidum. These results fill the gap of qualitative data of spontaneous narrative speech in HD and reveal that HD is characterized by systematic linguistic impairments leading to dysfluencies and disorganization in core domains of grammatical organization. This includes the referential use of noun phrases and the embedding of clauses, which mediate crucial dimensions of meaning in language in its normal social use. Moreover, such impairment is seen prior to motor symptoms onset and when standardized neuropsychological test profiles are otherwise normal.
Article
Full-text available
Huntington’s disease (HD) is characterized by motor and cognitive impairments including memory, executive, and attentional functions. However, as earlier studies relied on multidetermined attentional tasks, uncertainty still abounds regarding the differential deficit across attentional subcomponents. Likewise, the evolution of these deficits during the successive stages of HD remains unclear. The present study simultaneously explored the three distinct networks of attention (alerting, orienting, executive conflict) in preclinical and clinical HD. 38 HD patients (18 preclinical) and 38 matched healthy controls completed the Attention Network Test, an integrated and theoretically grounded task assessing the integrity of the three attentional networks. Preclinical HD was not characterized by any attentional deficit compared to controls. Conversely, clinical HD was associated with a differential deficit across the three attentional networks, showing preserved performance for alerting and orienting networks but massive and specific impairment for executive conflict network. This indexes an impaired use of executive control to resolve the conflict between task-relevant stimuli and interfering task-irrelevant ones. Clinical HD does not lead to a global attentional deficit but rather to a specific impairment for the executive control of attention. Moreover, the absence of attentional deficits in preclinical HD suggests that these deficits are absent at the initial disease stages.
Article
Full-text available
Neuroimaging studies suggest that the neural network involved in language control may not be specific to bi-/multilingualism but is part of a domain-general executive control system. We report a trilingual case of a Cantonese (L1), English (L2), and Mandarin (L3) speaker, Dr. T, who sustained a brain injury at the age of 77 causing lesions in the left frontal lobe and in the left temporo-parietal areas resulting in fluent aphasia. Dr. T’s executive functions were impaired according to a modified version of the Stroop color-word test and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance was characterized by frequent perseveration errors. Dr. T demonstrated pathological language switching and mixing across her three languages. Code switching in Cantonese was more prominent in discourse production than confrontation naming. Our case suggests that voluntary control of spoken word production in trilingual speakers shares neural substrata in the frontobasal ganglia system with domain-general executive control mechanisms. One prediction is that lesions to such a system would give rise to both pathological switching and impairments of executive functions in trilingual speakers.
Article
Research on the neural bases of bilingual language control has largely overlooked the role of preparatory processes, which are central to cognitive control. Additionally, little is known about how the process involved in global language selection may differ from those involved in the selection of words and morpho-syntactic rules for manipulating them. These processes were examined separately in an fMRI experiment, with an emphasis on understanding how and when general cognitive control regions become activated. Results of region-of-interest analyses on 23 early Spanish-English bilinguals showed that the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) was primarily engaged during the language preparation phase of the task, whereas the left prefrontal (DLPFC) and pre-supplementary motor areas showed increasing activation from preparation to execution. Activation in the basal ganglia (BG), left middle temporal lobe, and right precentral cortical regions did not significantly differ throughout the task. These results suggest that three core cognitive control regions, the ACC, DLPFC, and BG, which have been previously implicated in bilingual language control, engage in distinct neurocognitive processes. Specifically, the results are consistent with the view that the BG "keep track" of the target language in use throughout various levels of language selection, that the ACC is particularly important for top-down target language preparation, and that the left prefrontal cortex is increasingly involved in selection processes from preparation through task execution.
Article
Background: In Huntington's Disease (HD) cognitive decline can occur before unequivocal motor signs become apparent. As cognitive decline often starts early in the course of the disease and has a progressive nature over time, cognition can be regarded as a key target for symptomatic treatment. The specific progressive profile of cognitive decline over time is unknown. Objective: The aim of this study is to quantify the progression of cognitive decline across all HD stages, from pre-motormanifest to advanced HD, and to investigate if CAG length mediates cognitive decline. Methods: In the European REGISTRY study 2669 HD expansion gene carriers underwent annual cognitive assessment. General linear mixed models were used to model the cognitive decline for each cognitive task across all disease stages. Additionally, a model was developed to evaluate the cognitive decline based on CAG length and age rather than disease stage. Results: There was significant cognitive decline on all administered tasks throughout pre-motormanifest (close to estimated disease onset) participants and the subsequent motormanifest participants from stage 1 to stage 4. Performance on the Stroop Word and Stroop Color tests additionally declined significantly across the two pre-motormanifest groups: far and close to estimated disease onset. The evaluation of cognition performance in relation to CAG length and age revealed a more rapid cognitive decline in participants with longer CAG length than participants with shorter CAG length over time. Conclusion: Cognitive performance already shows decline in pre-motormanifest HD gene expansion carriers and gradually worsens to late stage HD. HD gene expansion carriers with certain CAG length have their own cognitive profile, i.e., longer CAG length is associated with more rapid decline.
Article
Neuropsychiatric disturbance−particularly executive dysfunction and behavioral dysregulation−is a common feature of Huntington’s disease (HD), with implications for functional capacity and quality of life. No study to date has ascertained whether longitudinal change in brain activity is associated with neuropsychiatric deficits in HD. We used a set-response-shifting task together with functional magnetic resonance imaging to investigate 30-month longitudinal blood-oxygen level dependent (BOLD) signal changes in the fronto-striatal attentional control network in premanifest and symptomatic HD (pre-HD and symp-HD, respectively), relative to healthy control participants. We also assessed the extent to which changes in the BOLD signal over time were related to neuropsychiatric measures in the domains of executive dysfunction and behavioral dysregulation. Associations were also evaluated with clinical and disease severity. We found no longitudinal BOLD differences between pre-HD and controls over 30 months. In contrast, reduction in BOLD response over time was greater in symp-HD, relative to controls, in task-related areas (e.g., anterior cingulate cortex and striatum) and in regions from the default mode network (e.g., medial prefrontal cortex and posterior cingulate/precuneus). Moreover, when considered across both premanifest and symptomatic stages, longitudinal BOLD signal decline in the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and putamen was associated with executive dysfunction and behavioral dysregulation measures. In addition, longitudinal reduction in BOLD signal, in fronto-striatal and default mode networks, correlated with disease severity. These results suggest that longitudinal change in fronto-striatal and default mode networks may be useful in understanding the biological underpinnings of functional decline in HD. Such findings offer new avenues for targeted treatments in terms of minimizing psychiatric impairment and potentially maximizing cognitive function.