ArticlePublisher preview available

Pairing New Words With Unfamiliar Objects: Comparing Children With and Without Cochlear Implants

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research
Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the author.

Abstract and Figures

Purpose This study investigates differences between preschool children with cochlear implants and age-matched children with normal hearing during an initial stage in word learning to evaluate whether they (a) match novel words to unfamiliar objects and (b) solicit information about unfamiliar objects during play. Method Twelve preschool children with cochlear implants and 12 children with normal hearing matched for age completed 2 experimental tasks. In the 1st task, children were asked to point to a picture that matched either a known word or a novel word. In the 2nd task, children were presented with unfamiliar objects during play and were given the opportunity to ask questions about those objects. Results In Task 1, children with cochlear implants paired novel words with unfamiliar pictures in fewer trials than children with normal hearing. In Task 2, children with cochlear implants were less likely to solicit information about new objects than children with normal hearing. Performance on the 1st task, but not the 2nd, significantly correlated with expressive vocabulary standard scores of children with cochlear implants. Conclusion This study provides preliminary evidence that children with cochlear implants approach mapping novel words to and soliciting information about unfamiliar objects differently than children with normal hearing.
This content is subject to copyright. Terms and conditions apply.
JSLHR
Research Article
Pairing New Words With Unfamiliar
Objects: Comparing Children With
and Without Cochlear Implants
Emily Lund
a
Purpose: This study investigates differences between
preschool children with cochlear implants and age-matched
children with normal hearing during an initial stage in word
learning to evaluate whether they (a) match novel words to
unfamiliar objects and (b) solicit information about unfamiliar
objects during play.
Method: Twelve preschool children with cochlear implants
and12childrenwithnormalhearingmatchedforage
completed 2 experimental tasks. In the 1st task, children
were asked to point to a picture that matched either a
known word or a novel word. In the 2nd task, children were
presented with unfamiliar objects during play and were
given the opportunity to ask questions about those objects.
Results: In Task 1, children with cochlear implants
paired novel words with unfamiliar pictures in fewer
trials than children with normal hearing. In Task 2,
children with cochlear implants were less likely to
solicit information about new objects than children
with normal hearing. Performance on the 1st task, but
not the 2nd, significantly correlated with expressive
vocabulary standard scores of children with cochlear
implants.
Conclusion: This study provides preliminary evidence
that children with cochlear implants approach mapping
novel words to and soliciting information about unfamiliar
objects differently than children with normal hearing.
Despite improvements in technology, particularly
relative to speech perception outcomes in the
last decade (Dunn et al., 2014), vocabulary
outcomes for children with cochlear implants lag behind
those of children with normal hearing (Lund, 2016). This
is, perhaps, not surprising. Although children are receiv-
ing cochlear implants at increasingly young ages, there is
growing consensus that language-learning opportunities
missed during the first year of life likely contribute to
later linguistic delays (e.g., Carlson et al., 2018; Houston
& Miyamoto, 2010; Levine, Strother-Garcia, Golinkoff, &
Hirsh-Pasek, 2016). During the first 12 months of infancy,
children with normal hearing develop strategies for parsing
a stream of speech into words and mapping those words
onto the world (Bloom, 2000; Levine et al., 2016). The pur-
pose of this study is to explore the consequences of audi-
tory deprivation on an initial step in the word-learning
process: figuring out how to match a new word with a
referent.
Word Learning in Children With Cochlear Implants
Most studies of spoken word learning in children
with cochlear implants find that these children do not learn
as efficiently as their peers with normal hearing. In studies of
children between the ages of 1 and 3 years, Houston, Stewart,
Moberly, Hollich, and Miyamoto (2012; N=48),Lundand
Schuele (2017; N= 18), and Robertson, von Hapsburg, and
Hay (2017; N= 32) all used different word-learning tasks and
found that children with cochlear implants tended to
learn fewer words than children with normal hearing.
Houston, Carter, Pisoni, Kirk, and Ying (2005; N= 48)
and Walker and McGregor (2013; N= 48) used rapid
word-learning tasks to teach novel names for objects to
preschool-aged children with cochlear implants and age-
matched children with normal hearing. Both studies found
that children with cochlear implants learned fewer words
than children with normal hearing. This lag in learning
appears to continue for older children. Davidson, Geers,
and Nicholas (2014) determined that 6- to 12-year-old
children who had good audibility with a cochlear implant
(n= 46) outperformed children with poor audibility with
a
Davies School of Communication Sciences & Disorders, Texas
Christian University, Fort Worth
Correspondence to Emily Lund: e.lund@tcu.edu
Editor-in-Chief: Sean Redmond
Editor: Lizbeth Finestack
Received December 19, 2017
Revision received February 7, 2018
Accepted May 25, 2018
https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-17-0467
Disclosure: The author has declared that no competing interests existed at the time
of publication.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research Vol. 61 23252336 September 2018 Copyright © 2018 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 2325
... Triggering refers to the process of recognizing that a word is new and must be learned (Hoover et al., 2010). Children with cochlear implants struggle to pair novel words to nameless pictures in disambiguation tasks, likely reflecting difficulty with triggering (Lederberg et al., 2000;Lund, 2018). The process of configuration involves recognizing and mapping a phonological and semantic representation of a new word to its referent (Leach & Samuel, 2007). ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose This study compares responses of children who are deaf and hard of hearing (DHH) who use spoken language with responses of children who have typical hearing on a repeated word association task to evaluate lexical–semantic organization. Method This study included 109 participants in early kindergarten or who had completed first grade. The younger group included 30 children with typical hearing, 22 with hearing aids, and 21 with cochlear implants. The older group included 16 children with typical hearing, nine with hearing aids, and 11 with cochlear implants. Children were asked to give a word associated with 24 stimuli words. Responses were coded according to their relation to the target. Results An analysis of variance revealed that older children, regardless of hearing status, produced more semantically related responses to prompts than younger children. Children in the younger DHH group differed from children with typical hearing in their production of non–semantically related responses: They produced errored responses at higher rates. Conclusion This preliminary data may indicate an early deficit in recognition of semantic relations between words for children who are DHH and provides a basis for continued longitudinal study of changes in lexical–semantic organization.
... Despite the adoption of universal newborn hearing screening programs, advances in hearing technology, and increased access to spoken-language services, many children, even those with cochlear implants, continue to develop smaller, less flexible lexicons than their age-matched peers (Davidson et al., 2013;Nittrouer et al., 2018;Nott et al., 2009). Recent research suggests that this difference may be related to delays in developing disambiguation, or assignment of novel words to unfamiliar objects, at least for children with cochlear implants (Lund, 2018b). ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose This mixed-methods study aimed to examine the conversation techniques used by parents of young children with hearing loss (HL) during dinnertime at home. Parents' usage rates of open- and closed-ended language elicitation, reformulation, imitation, directives, and explicit vocabulary instruction were examined in relation to children's receptive vocabulary and basic-concepts skills. Method Twenty-minute dinnertime segments were extracted from naturalistic, daylong recordings of 37 preschoolers with HL who used listening and spoken language. The segments were hand-coded for parents' use of conversation techniques. Children's receptive vocabulary and basic concepts were assessed using standardized measures. Results Parents' use of conversation techniques varied widely, with closed-ended elicitation and directives used most frequently during dinner. Explicit vocabulary instruction was correlated with general receptive vocabulary and basic-concepts skills. Thematic analysis of the conversations revealed common themes, including concrete topics and sibling speakers. In addition, parents who used many techniques often introduced abstract conversation topics; electronic media was present in all conversations with few techniques. Conclusions Parents of preschoolers with HL may benefit from specific coaching to elicit language and introduce new vocabulary during home routines. These techniques may help develop their children's receptive language.
Article
The purpose of this study was to compare three intervention conditions in virtual rapid word learning instruction for young children who are deaf and hard of hearing (DHH): traditional, general physical activity, and semantic richness physical activity. We focused on an initial step in learning a new word: mapping a word form to a referent. Fourteen children who are DHH and 14 children with typical hearing (TH) participated in a pseudoword-learning virtual session in each condition, and receptive and expressive knowledge of targeted pseudowords was assessed. Of interest was the impact of different types of physical activity on rapid learning of word-like forms. There was an interaction of group and condition for expressive production of pseudowords. Children who were DHH learned to produce more pseudowords in the general physical activity condition, whereas children with TH learned to produce more pseudowords in the semantic richness physical activity condition. There were no effects of group or condition on receptive learning of pseudowords. Children who are DHH can rapidly map word-like forms to referents via teleintervention, and physical activity may interact with word learning differently for children who are DHH and children with TH.
Article
Full-text available
Presents a spreading-activation theory of human semantic processing, which can be applied to a wide range of recent experimental results. The theory is based on M. R. Quillian's (1967) theory of semantic memory search and semantic preparation, or priming. In conjunction with this, several misconceptions concerning Quillian's theory are discussed. A number of additional assumptions are proposed for his theory to apply it to recent experiments. The present paper shows how the extended theory can account for results of several production experiments by E. F. Loftus, J. F. Juola and R. C. Atkinson's (1971) multiple-category experiment, C. Conrad's (1972) sentence-verification experiments, and several categorization experiments on the effect of semantic relatedness and typicality by K. J. Holyoak and A. L. Glass (1975), L. J. Rips et al (1973), and E. Rosch (1973). The paper also provides a critique of the Rips et al model for categorization judgments. (44 ref)
Article
Full-text available
Purpose The purpose of this study is to evaluate the effects of short-term parent training on maternal use of language stimulation strategies and vocabulary scores in children with hearing loss. Method Six mother–child dyads participated in the multiple-baseline study. During baseline and maintenance, children engaged in a business-as-usual model of clinician-only therapy. During intervention, mothers and children participated in parent training focused on transparent labeling and linguistic mapping strategies. Parent strategy use was measured via weekly play-based probe assessments. Child vocabulary growth was measured via parent report. Results A relation between parent training and use of transparent labeling was established for all mothers, and a relation between parent training and use of linguistic mapping was established for 3 of 6 mothers. Child vocabulary growth rate increased from baseline to intervention in 4 of 6 children. Conclusions Short-term parent training can change parent behavior. However, parents may not maintain these skills without support. Further research is needed to characterize the extent to which short-term training can make long-term changes in parent and child outcomes.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: To examine practice variance of cochlear implant candidacy assessment and off-label indications across centers in the United States. Methods: Cross-sectional survey of the American Neurotology Society (ANS). Results: A total of 81 surveys were returned from ANS members who report regular involvement in cochlear implant care. Overall there was a broad distribution in age and clinical experience, with most respondents reporting ACGME accreditation in neurotology and employment at an academic center. The annual volume of cochlear implant surgeries varied considerably across centers.Seventy-eight percent of respondents performed cochlear implantation for at least one of the following indications within the last 2 years: profound hearing loss in children less than 12 months of age (35, 43%), children with asymmetrical hearing loss where at least one ear was better than performance cutoff for age (25, 31%), adults with asymmetrical hearing where at least one ear was better than the performance cutoff for adult criteria (49, 61%), single-sided deafness (37, 46%), and ipsilateral vestibular schwannoma (28, 35%). Centers with a higher annual implant volume more frequently performed off-label implantation in all queried populations (all, p≤0.001), and performed surgery on infants with congenital deafness at a younger age (p = 0.013), compared with centers with lower surgical volume.When surveyed regarding speech perception testing practices for adult candidacy assessment, 75 (100%) respondents who answered this question reported routine use of AzBio sentences, 42 (56%) CNC word scores, and 26 (35%) HINT testing; only 7 (9%) reported using BKB-SIN testing and 6 (8%) reported using CUNY scores. Fifty-one (68%) reported routine use of speech-in-noise testing to determine adult cochlear implant candidacy, 21 (28%) reported selective use only when patient scores were borderline in quiet, and 3 (4%) reported that their center does not currently use testing in noise for candidacy determination. Nineteen (26%) solely used +10 dB signal-to-noise ratio (SNR), 12 (16%) solely used +5 dB SNR, and 41 (55%) used both +10 and +5 dB SNR. Overall, 19% (N = 14) only perform unilateral implantation in the Medicare population, while 81% (N = 58) consider bilateral implantation. Conclusion: Significant variation in cochlear implant candidacy assessment and off-label implantation exists across centers and providers in the United States resulting in healthcare inequities. The high percentage of surgeons performing implantations for off-label or nontraditional indications reflects the overly restrictive and dated status of current implant guidelines. With greater adoption of more difficult speech perception testing in noise, careful clinical judgment is needed to maintain a favorable risk-benefit balance for prospective implant candidates.
Article
Full-text available
This study sought to evaluate the effects of synchronous and asynchronous auditory-visual cues on the word-learning performance of children with cochlear implants and children with normal hearing matched for chronological age. Children with cochlear implants (n = 9) who had worn the implant for less than one year and children matched for chronological age (n = 9) participated in rapid word-learning trials. Children with cochlear implants did not learn words in either the synchronous or asynchronous condition (U = 49.5, p = .99; d = 0.05). Children with normal hearing learned more words in the synchronous rather than asynchronous condition (U = 78.5, p = .04; d = 0.95). These findings represent a first step toward determining how task-level factors influence the lexical outcomes of children with cochlear implants.
Book
Primary Test of Nonverbal Intelligence - Primary Nonverbally formatted test of conceptual reasoning for young children.
Book
Words, Thoughts, and Theories articulates and defends the "theory theory" of cognitive and semantic development, the idea that infants and young children, like scientists, learn about the world by forming and revising theories, a view of the origins of knowledge and meaning that has broad implications for cognitive science. Gopnik and Meltzoff interweave philosophical arguments and empirical data from their own and other's research. Both the philosophy and the psychology, the arguments and the data, address the same fundamental epistemological question: How do we come to understand the world around us? Recently, the theory theory has led to much interesting research. However, this is the first book to look at the theory in extensive detail and to systematically contrast it with other theories. It is also the first to apply the theory to infancy and early childhood, to use the theory to provide a framework for understanding semantic development, and to demonstrate that language acquisition influences theory change in children.The authors show that children just beginning to talk are engaged in profound restructurings of several domains of knowledge. These restructurings are similar to theory changes in science, and they influence children's early semantic development, since children's cognitive concerns shape and motivate their use of very early words. But, in addition, children pay attention to the language they hear around them and this too reshapes their cognition, and causes them to reorganize their theories. Bradford Books imprint
Article
Objectives: Relatively little is known about how young children with hearing impairment (HI) learn novel words in infant- and adult-directed speech (ADS). Infant-directed speech (IDS) supports word learning in typically developing infants relative to ADS. This study examined how children with normal hearing (NH) and children with HI learn novel words in IDS and ADS. It was predicted that IDS would support novel word learning in both groups of children. In addition, children with HI were expected to be less proficient word learners as compared with their NH peers. Design: A looking-while-listening paradigm was used to measure novel word learning in 16 children with sensorineural HI (age range 23.2 to 42.1 months) who wore either bilateral hearing aids (n = 10) or bilateral cochlear implants (n = 6) and 16 children with NH (age range 23.1 to 42.1 months) who were matched for gender, chronological age, and maternal education level. Two measures of word learning were assessed (accuracy and reaction time). Each child participated in two experiments approximately 1 week apart, one in IDS and one in ADS. Results: Both groups successfully learned the novel words in both speech type conditions, as evidenced by children looking at the correct picture significantly above chance. As a group, children with NH outperformed children with HI in the novel word learning task; however, there were no significant differences between performance on IDS versus ADS. More fine-grained time course analyses revealed that children with HI, and particularly children who use hearing aids, had more difficulty learning novel words in ADS, compared with children with NH. Conclusions: The pattern of results observed in the children with HI suggests that they may need extended support from clinicians and caregivers, through the use of IDS, during novel word learning. Future research should continue to focus on understanding the factors (e.g., device type and use, age of intervention, audibility, acoustic characteristics of input, etc.) that may influence word learning in children with HI in both IDS and ADS.
Chapter
Despite positive trends in the literature associated with early implantation, we cannot assume that children with cochlear implants (CIs) learn just like their hearing peers, or even just like each other. Schools that include children with CIs must consider a range of factors that influence academic and social outcomes, and balance each child’s need for challenge and support. Considerations include parent education and emotional support, language and literacy development, social emotional learning, motor and sensory development, working memory and other cognitive skills, classroom acoustics and modifications, individualized support services, classroom supports, and other ecological inputs.
Article
Despite poor vocabulary outcomes for children with hearing loss, few studies have evaluated the effectiveness of specific vocabulary teaching methods on vocabulary learning for this group. The authors compared three vocabulary instruction conditions with preschool children with hearing loss: (a) explicit, direct instruction; (b) follow-in labeling; and (c) incidental exposure using an adapted alternating-treatments single-subject experimental design. Nine preschool children with hearing loss participated in the study across 6 weeks, with intervention implemented by 4 teachers of the deaf. Visual analysis of the results of the data indicated that participants learned the most words in the explicit, direct instruction condition and the fewest words in the incidental exposure condition. These results are not consistent with some recommendations currently made to educators. The authors discuss implications for practice, including emphasis on direct instruction.