ArticlePDF Available

The role of emotional dependence in the relationship between attachment and impulsive behavior

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

The emotional dependence has been related with the problems in the impulse control and with the preoccupied attachment style. The objectives of this study are to analyze the relation between the emotional dependence, attachment and the impulsive behavior, as well as, to analyze if these final two can predict the emotional dependence. Furthermore, it analyze if the emotional dependence mediate in the relation between attachment and impulsivity and, finally, it studies the differences of gender in the three mentioned variables. The sample is formed by 1533 teenagers (707 women and 826 men), with ages between 14-18 years old (M = 15.76; SD = 1.25). The results showed of positives relations between the emotional dependence and the parental permission, childhood trauma and impulsive behavior. Moreover, the impulsivity is positively related with the fact to avoid to be alone, asymmetric relation, the need to please others, parental permission and childhood trauma. In the same way, it proves how the parental permission, self-sufficiency and resentment against parents and childhood trauma can predict the emotional dependence. Finally, it observes how the relation between the attachment and the impulsive behavior is mediated with the emotional dependency and it appreciates differences between the three variables in gender basis.
Content may be subject to copyright.
anales de psicología / annals of psychology
2018, vol. 34, nº 3 (october), 438-445
http://dx.doi.org/10.6018/analesps.34.3.313681
© Copyright 2018: Editum. Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de Murcia. Murcia (Spain)
ISSN print edition: 0212-9728. ISSN on line edition (http://revistas.um.es/analesps): 1695-2294.
On line edition License Creative Commons 4.0: BY-NC-ND
- 438 -
The role of emotional dependence in the relationship between attachment
and impulsive behavior
Ana Estévez2, María D. Chávez-Vera1,3, Janire Momeñe2, Leticia Olave1, Daniel Vázquez1, and Itziar Iruarrizaga1
1 Complutense University of Madrid (Spain).
2 University of Deusto (Spain).
3 Technical University of Manabí (Ecuador).
Título: El papel de la dependencia emocional en la relación entre el apego
y la conducta impulsiva.
Resumen: La dependencia emocional se define como una necesidad afec-
tiva extrema que una persona siente hacia su pareja. Se ha relacionado con
el déficit de control de impulsos y con el estilo de apego preocupado. El
presente estudio tiene como objetivos analizar la relación entre la depen-
dencia emocional, el apego y la conducta impulsiva, así como, analizar si es-
tas dos últimas predicen la dependencia emocional. Además, se analiza si la
dependencia emocional media en la relación entre el apego y la impulsivi-
dad y, finalmente, se estudian las diferencias de género en las tres variables
mencionadas. La muestra está formada por 1533 adolescentes (707 mujeres
y 826 hombres), con edades que oscilan entre los 14-18 años (M = 15.76;
DT = 1.25). Los resultados muestran relaciones positivas entre la depen-
dencia emocional y la permisividad parental, traumatismo infantil e impulsi-
vidad. Asimismo, la impulsividad se relaciona de manera positiva con evitar
estar solo, relación asimétrica, necesidad de agradar, permisividad parental y
traumatismo infantil. Igualmente, se comprueba como la permisividad pa-
rental, autosuficiencia y rencor contra los padres y el traumatismo infantil
predicen la dependencia emocional. Finalmente, se observa como la rela-
ción entre el apego y la conducta impulsiva es mediada por la dependencia
emocional y se aprecian diferencias en las tres variables en función del gé-
nero.
Palabras clave: dependencia emocional; apego; conducta impulsiva; ado-
lescentes.
Abstract: The emotional dependence has been related with the problems
in the impulse control and with the preoccupied attachment style. The ob-
jectives of this study are to analyze the relation between the emotional de-
pendence, attachment and the impulsive behavior, as well as, to analyze if
these final two can predict the emotional dependence. Furthermore, it ana-
lyze if the emotional dependence mediate in the relation between attach-
ment and impulsivity and, finally, it studies the differences of gender in the
three mentioned variables. The sample is formed by 1533 teenagers (707
women and 826 men), with ages between 14-18 years old (M = 15.76; SD
= 1.25). The results showed of positives relations between the emotional
dependence and the parental permission, childhood trauma and impulsive
behavior. Moreover, the impulsivity is positively related with the fact to
avoid to be alone, asymmetric relation, the need to please others, parental
permission and childhood trauma. In the same way, it proves how the pa-
rental permission, self-sufficiency and resentment against parents and
childhood trauma can predict the emotional dependence. Finally, it ob-
serves how the relation between the attachment and the impulsive behavior
is mediated with the emotional dependency and it appreciates differences
between the three variables in gender basis.
Key words: emotional dependency; attachment; impulsive behavior; ado-
lescents.
Introduction
Early childhood experiences form the schemes about one-
self and others that will serve for the later development of
personality (Young, 1999). Early affective deficiencies may
be responsible for the etiology of emotional dependence
(Castelló, 2012). Many of the first affective experiences in
people with emotional dependence are negative (Scantam-
burlo, Pitchot & Ansseau, 2013). As a result, relationships
could be used to try to meet unmet emotional needs in
childhood (Lemos & Londoño, 2006; Michelena, 2007;
Skvortsova & Shumskiy, 2014). Emotional dependence is
defined as an extreme affective need that a person feels to-
wards their partner in the course of their different relation-
ships (Castelló, 2005). Their intense fear of rupture contam-
inates the emotional bond, making them vulnerable and
pathological (Del Castillo, Hernández, Romero & Iglesias,
2015). They usually establish relationships as a couple since
adolescence and experience ruptures as catastrophic events
(Castelló, 2012). They are also accompanied by loss of iden-
* Correspondence address [Dirección para correspondencia]:
Itziar Iruarrizaga. Dpto. Psicología Básica II (Procesos Cognitivos). Facultad
de Trabajo Social. Universidad Complutense de Madrid. Campus de Somo-
saguas s/n, 28223 Pozuelo de Alarcón, Madrid (Spain). E-mail: icia-
riru@psi.ucm.es
tity (Schaeffer, 1998), role asymmetry, possessive behaviors,
intense energetic attrition, emotional instability and continu-
ous inner emptiness (Moral & Sirvent, 2008). The fear of
rupture is such that they remain in relationships that cause
them discomfort in order to retain the couple alongside
them, sacrificing their own desires and needs and leading to
deterioration in their quality of life (Cubas, Espinoza, Galli
& Terrones, 2004). However, despite experiencing negative
feelings such as sadness, despair, abrupt emotional changes
and irrepressible temptations, they also perceive very intense
positive feelings such as passion, immense happiness and
reduction of tension (Skvortsova & Shumskiy, 2014).
An important aspect is that the first love relationships
during adolescence are positive because they could influence
the way relationships behave in adulthood (Pradas & Perles,
2012). In this line, the acquisition of emotional dependence
could be explained by inadequate attachment during child-
hood which would remain stable throughout life (Izquierdo
& Gómez-Acosta, 2013). Attachment is considered a basic
necessity throughout the entire life cycle (Gómez-Zapiain,
Ortiz & Gómez-Lope, 2012), influencing the choice of the
couple and the way they act within it (Barroso, 2014; López,
2003). Previous studies have linked emotional dependence
to attachment styles in adulthood by pointing to the pre-
dominance of preoccupied attachment (Momeñe & Estévez,
The role of emotional dependence in the relationship between attachment and impulsive behavior 439
anales de psicología / annals of psychology, 2018, vol. 34, nº 3 (october)
accepted publication; Yárnoz-Yaben, 2010). This style of at-
tachment implies a high anxiety for abandonment, a high de-
sire for closeness, intimacy (Alonso-Arbiol, Shaver &
Yárnoz, 2002), commitment (Feeney & Noller, 1990), jeal-
ousy and obsessive concern (Feeney & Noller, 2001). In this
sense, dependent persons emit behaviours aimed at prevent-
ing the couple's abandonment by employing manipulation,
which could endanger their lives or threaten to inflict harm
(Villegas & Sánchez, 2013). In this line, it has been proved
how the type of attachment influences the functional or dys-
functional expression of anger (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2011),
being the preoccupied attachment, predominant in people
with emotional dependence, prone to experience greater an-
ger and impulsivity (Loinaz, 2011) and inability to regulate it
(Loinaz & Echeburúa, 2012). Consequently, both real or im-
aginary rupture attempts and emotional estrangement (May-
seless, 1991) can disproportionately activate dysfunctional
attachment by precipitating aggressive behaviors (Loinaz &
Echeburua, 2012). In the same way, attachment figures in
childhood play a crucial role in the acquisition of a good ca-
pacity for emotional regulation, being the absence of contact
and lack of care those who predispose to react more impul-
sively and aggressively in adulthood (Barroso, 2014). This is
what happens in emotional dependents where their attach-
ment figures show negligent behavior, seriously neglecting
their obligations, relating in an affectionately poor way and
even providing them with ill-treatment (Castelló, 2005). This
is how they would obtain, therefore, dysfunctional schemes
of insufficient self-control of impulses and emotions and
low tolerance to frustration (Jaller & Lemos, 2009). As can
be seen, impulsivity plays an important role in substance ad-
dictions (Belin, Mar, Dalley, Robins & Everitt, 2008), how-
ever, emotionally dependent people due to their similarity
with addictive pathologies also show deficits in impulse con-
trol (Villegas & Sánchez, 2013), difficulties in emotional reg-
ulation (Momeñe, Jáuregui & Estévez, 2017), and a great
comorbidity with anxious-depressive symptomatology
(Deave & McGraw, 2003). Impulsivity may refer to rapid,
unplanned actions where immediate reinforcement is experi-
enced as a matter of urgency (Moeller, Barrat, Dougerty,
Schmitz & Swann, 2001), showing an inability to delay or in-
hibit a response (Papachristou, Nederkoorn, Havermans,
Van der Horst & Jansen, 2012), actions expressed prema-
turely, risky or inappropriate behaviours that are often unde-
sirable for the situation (Evenden, 1999), leading to behav-
ioural disinhibition (Verdejo-García, Lawrence & Clark,
2008).
Because of this, the objectives of this study have been to
study the relationship between dependence, attachment and
impulsive behavior, as well as to analyze whether the latter
two predict emotional dependence. The differences in the
variables mentioned above are also analyzed according to
gender and, finally, it has been verified whether the average
emotional dependence in the relationship between attach-
ment and impulsivity.
Method
It is a non-experimental, cross-sectional, correlational design
study. The sampling method used is probabilistic with a
stratified-unietapic random sampling type with proportional
affixing; the distribution was made according to weight or
size within the population. In order to stratify, the following
factors were taken into account: the number of educational
units, the number of adolescents per class, urban and rural
parishes, and the distribution of courses by sex. The refer-
ence population is made up of adolescents attending school
in the Fiscal Educational Units of the Urban and Rural Par-
ishes of the Portoviejo Canton in the Province of Manabí in
the Republic of Ecuador, which encompass different socio-
demographic sectors that share common characteristics and
constitute the object of study.
Participants
The sample is composed of 1.533 adolescents with
schooling of both sexes, 53.9% male (826) and 46.1% female
(707), aged between 14 and 18 years (M = 15.76, DT = 1.25)
belonging to twelve Fiscal Educational Units of the different
urban (60%) and rural (40%) socio-demographic sectors of
the Canton Portoviejo of the Province of Manabí of the Re-
public of Ecuador. In order to obtain the sample, Ecuador's
official bodies (National Council for Narcotic and Psycho-
tropic Substances Control, 2005) were considered, and today
it is known as the Technical Secretariat for Drugs (SETED).
The design of the sample and the determination of its size
were defined and calculated following the same criteria as
those used in the Report of the Second National Survey of
Secondary Education Students on Drug Use (2005) of the
Republic of Ecuador conducted by the National Council for
Narcotic and Psychotropic Substances Control. These pa-
rameters used for the calculation, as referred to by the
Council in that report, are those applied to all the studies
carried out on the continent. The target population was the
fiscal educational units of adolescents enrolled in tenth
grade, first, second and third year of high school. The selec-
tion of the educational units was obtained from the database
of the Ministry of Education Zonal Coordination 4 district
13D01, the educational units belong to the canton, parish es-
tablished, institution zone (urban and rural), support related
to the Fiscal Educational Units and representative of differ-
ent socio-demographic zones of the Canton Portoviejo. For
the calculation of the sample size, the confidence level of the
sample and the relationship with the margin of error or vari-
ation that always exists between the results obtained in a
sample and its inference towards the population were con-
sidered. The confidence level used is. 95 with a margin of er-
ror of .015. Due to the sampling characteristics it is consid-
ered a correction factor by design effect to increase sample
size and decrease the variability of observations, this factor
was estimated at 2. Finally, this sample size was increased to
compensate for 10% of possible non-response.
440 Ana Estévez et al.
anales de psicología / annals of psychology, 2018, vol. 34, nº 3 (october)
Each educational unit had the probability of selection di-
rectly proportional to the number of classes in the Tenth
General Basic Education (GBS) and the Unified General
Baccalaureate (BGU) (first, second and third years of Bachil-
lerato). In the educational units with a number of classrooms
greater than the sampling interval, several classes could be
selected. The criteria for the selection of the strata were the
representativeness criteria that correspond to the capital of
Manabí of the Republic of Ecuador; this study will represent
the two parishes (urban and rural) of the different educa-
tional units. The sample consisted of: a) The capital of
Manabí Portoviejo with two parishes: urban and rural, and
b) 12 educational units belonging to the two parishes, as-
suming a total of 1.533 adolescents belonging to the 12 edu-
cational units and parishes.
Procedure
The study was preceded by the provision of informed
consent to the parents and/or guardians of the adolescents
who completed the questionnaires. They were also informed
about the rules of completion, duration and aspects to be
measured, voluntarily conducting the study, confidentiality
and anonymity of the data obtained and telephone and e-
mail addresses of the reference researcher to contact. Since
the data collection has been done in paper format, during
the administration of the questionnaires the researcher
stayed in the classroom with the students until they were all
returned to her. It was optional for teachers to remain in the
classroom. The students collaborating in the research re-
ceived a pencil and certificate of participation as a token of
their appreciation.
This study was carried out following the criteria of the
Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2013).
Instruments
Attachment. CaMir-R (Balluerka, Lacasa, Gorostiaga,
Muela & Pierrehumbert, 2011). This instrument is the short-
ened version of CaMir (Pierrehumbert et al., 1996). Evalu-
ates attachment representations and conception of family
functioning through 32 items.
The internal structure of the questionnaire consists of 7
dimensions, five of which refer to the representations of at-
tachment (security: availability and support of attachment
figures; family concern; parental interference; self-
sufficiency, resentment against parents; and childhood trau-
ma) and two refer to the representations of family structure
(parental authority and parental permission). The scale of
"security: availability and support of attachment figures" re-
fers to one's own perception of having felt and feeling loved
by their attachment figures, as well as being able to trust
them and perceive their availability if needed. The scale of
"family concern" refers to intense separation anxiety and ex-
cessive preoccupation at the present time with attachment
figures. The "parental interference" scale refers to memories
of being overprotected during childhood, being a fearful
child, showing concern about being abandoned. The "paren-
tal authority" scale is the person's positive assessment of
family values of authority and hierarchy, while the "parental
permission" scale shows memories of a lack of limits and pa-
rental guidance during childhood. The scale of "self-
sufficiency and resentment against parents" refers to a sense
of resentment towards loved ones and rejection of feelings
of dependence and emotional reciprocity. Finally, the scale
of "childhood trauma" refers to memories of having experi-
enced lack of availability, violence and threats from attach-
ment figures during childhood. It is answered through a Lik-
ert scale from 1 ("totally disagree") to 5 ("totally agree"). The
internal consistency values in the Spanish adaptation are sat-
isfactory (the value of Alpha in the different subscales is be-
tween .60 and .85). In this study Cronbach's alpha is .90.
Emotional dependence in dating relationships in young people. The
Emotional Dependence Scale in the Bridehood of Youth
and Adolescents (DEN; Urbiola, Estévez & Iraurgi, 2014).
This scale consists of 12 items structured in 4 subscales: (a)
Avoiding being alone, refers to the actions carried out by the
dependent person in order to avoid being alone; (b) Necessi-
ty of exclusivity, refers to the dependent person's need to
know at all times that his or her partner is available only for
him or her; (d) Asymmetric relationship refers to the subor-
dinate and asymmetrical nature of the relationship. It is an-
swered through a Likert scale of 6 points, from 0 ("never")
to 5 ("always"), indicating with which they identify with each
other in their relationships. To complete the questionnaire it
is necessary to have had at least one dating relationship. In
relation to the internal consistency of the DEN, it obtained
a Cronbach alpha of .82 in the validation study, being a value
of reliability considered as high. In this study the value of
Cronbach's alpha is .87.
Impulsivity. Impulsivity scale of BARRAT BIS 11 (Salvo
& Castro, 2013). This scale has been designed to evaluate
impulsivity considering the construct from a multidimen-
sional point of view. It consists of 30 items that are grouped
into three subscales: Cognitive Impulsivity, related to the
restlessness of thinking and quick decision making, Motor
Impulsivity, implies acting in a hasty way without previous
reflection and Unplanned Impulsivity, carrying out actions
without planning towards the future. It is answered by a Lik-
ert scale of 4 response options (0 "rarely or never"; 1 "occa-
sionally"; 3 "often"; 4 "always or almost always"). The inter-
nal consistency values in the Chilean adaptation, used here,
are acceptable (Cronbach's alpha value for the full scale is
.77). In this study Cronbach's alpha is .81.
Statistical analysis
First, bivariate relationships between emotional depend-
ence, attachment and impulsive behavior were analyzed us-
ing Pearson's r. The effect size was interpreted as described
by Cohen (1992) in the following way: values under .20 were
considered small, those around .50 medium and those higher
The role of emotional dependence in the relationship between attachment and impulsive behavior 441
anales de psicología / annals of psychology, 2018, vol. 34, nº 3 (october)
than .80 large. Second, a multi-step multiple regression anal-
ysis was performed to test the predictive role of attachment
and impulsivity over emotional dependence. Third, a media-
tion was conducted to see whether the dependence on the
relationship between attachment and impulsive behavior was
mediated. Finally, the differences in mean differences ac-
cording to gender in emotional dependence, attachment and
impulsive behavior were analyzed.
Results
Correlation between attachment, emotional depend-
ence and impulsivity
First, the correlations between attachment, emotional
dependence and impulsivity were measured (Table 1). The
degree of relationship was determined according to Cohen
(1992) as follows: values below .20 would be small; around
.50 would be medium and above .80 would be large.
The results show small, though significant, positive cor-
relations between emotional dependence and parental per-
mission, childhood trauma and impulsivity. In terms of im-
pulsivity, the results reflect positive relationships with avoid-
ance of loneliness, asymmetric relationship, need to please,
parental permission and childhood trauma. Likewise, un-
planned impulsivity is negatively related to security, family
concern, parental interference and parental authority. Final-
ly, motor impulsivity is positively related to the need for ex-
clusivity and self-sufficiency and resentment against parents.
Table 1. Correlation between attachment, emotional dependence and impulsivity.
1
4
6
7
8
9
12
16
1
--
2
.62**
3
.64**
4
.56**
--
5
.82**
.79**
6
-.09**
-.08**
--
7
-.05*
-.02
.65**
--
8
.02
.05
.45**
.53**
--
9
-.07**
-.06*
.65**
.60**
.52**
--
10
.11**
.07**
.20**
.27**
.41**
.16**
11
.10
.01
.16**
.26**
.37**
.28**
12
0.13**
.12**
-.05*
.09**
.24**
.05
--
13
.06*
.11**
-.18**
-.05*
.00
-.11**
.12*
14
.13**
.14**
-.02
.04
.11**
.02
.15**
15
.13**
.09**
-.27**
-.19**
-.13**
-.25**
.11**
16
.16**
.16**
-.22**
-.10**
-.01
-.16**
.18**
--
Note: **p < .001; *p < .05.
1= Avoid being alone; 2=Necessity of exclusivity; 3=Need to please; 4=Asymmetrical relationship; 5=Emotional dependence; 6=Security; 7=Family con-
cern; 8=Parental interference; 9=Parental authority; 10=Parental permission; 11=Self sufficiency, resentment against parents; 12=Child trauma; 13= Cog-
nitive impulsivity; 14= Motor Impulsivity; 15 = Unplanned Impulsivity; 16 = Total Impulsivity
Regression of attachment and impulsive behavior
on emotional dependence
Secondly, the predictive role of attachment and impul-
sive behavior on emotional dependence was tested (Table 2).
The results obtained in a statistically significant way are
shown below. The results indicate parental permission (R =
.22; = .05; corrected = .04; p = .02), self-sufficiency
and rancor against parents (R = .22; = .05; corrected =
.04; p = .00) and childhood trauma (R = .22; = .05; cor-
rected = .04; p = .00) as predictors of emotional depend-
ence.
Influence of emotional dependence on the relation-
ship between attachment and impulsivity
Thirdly, the relationship between attachment and impul-
sivity is analyzed, being this relationship mediated by emo-
tional dependence.
Attachment is initially associated with impulsivity (Table
3). As can be seen in security, parental authority, parental
permission, self-sufficiency and resentment against parents
and child trauma are statistically significantly associated with
impulsivity.
Table 2. Multiple linear regression of impulsive behavior and attachment to
emotional dependence.
Model
B
β
t
p
Parental permission
.26
.07
2.31
.02*
Self-sufficiency, resentment
against parents
-.26
-.09
-2.89
.00**
Childhood trauma
.22
.10
3.48
.00**
Note: **p < .001; *p < .05.
442 Ana Estévez et al.
anales de psicología / annals of psychology, 2018, vol. 34, nº 3 (october)
Table 3. The predictive role of attachment on impulsivity
Predictor variable
B
β
t
p
Variable criterion
Security
-.28
-.22
-6.01
.00**
Impulsivity
Parental authority
-.29
-.10
-2.70
.01*
Impulsivity
Parental permission
.30
.11
3.91
.00**
Impulsivity
Self-sufficiency, resentment against parents
.27
.12
4.11
.00**
Impulsivity
Childhood trauma
.14
.09
3.11
.00**
Impulsivity
Note: **p < .01; *p < .05
Later, attachment is associated with emotional depend-
ence (Table 4). Parental interference, parental permission,
self-sufficiency and resentment against parents, and child-
hood trauma are statistically significantly associated with
emotional dependence.
Table 4. Attachment as a predictor of emotional dependence
Predictor variable
B
β
t
p
Variable criterion
Parental interference
.21
.07
2.02
.04*
Emotional dependence
Parental permission
.29
.08
2.67
.01*
Emotional dependence
Self-sufficiency, resentment against parents
-.21
-.07
-2.32
.02*
Emotional dependence
Childhood trauma
.25
.11
3.85
.00**
Emotional dependence
Note: **p < .01; *p < .05
Finally, emotional dependence is associated with impul-
sivity by controlling the association between attachment and
impulsivity (Table 5). The results show how safety, parental
authority, parental permission, self-sufficiency and resent-
ment against parents, child trauma and emotional depend-
ence are statistically significantly associated with impulsivity.
As can be seen, the non-standardised coefficient of the
safety variable changes from the first step -.28 to -.27 in the
third step. This means that 3.57% of the variance in the rela-
tionship between security and impulsivity is explained by
emotional dependence. Likewise, the non-standardized coef-
ficient of the variable parental authority is changed from the
first step -.29 to -.28 in the third step. This means that
3.44% of the variance in the relationship between parental
authority and impulsivity is explained by emotional depend-
ence. Similarly, the non-standardised coefficient of the pa-
rental permission variable changes from .30 in the first step
to .28 in the third step. This means that 6.66% of the vari-
ance in the relationship between parental permission and
impulsivity is explained by emotional dependence. Similarly,
the non-standardized coefficient of the variable self-
sufficiency and resentment against parents goes from being
.27 in the first step to .28 in the third step, assuming that -
3.70% of the variance in the relationship between self-
sufficiency and resentment against parents and impulsivity is
explained by emotional dependence. Finally, the non-
standardized coefficient of the variable childhood trauma,
changes from being .14 in the first step to .12 in the third
step, which means that 14.28% of the variance in the rela-
tionship between childhood trauma and impulsivity is ex-
plained by emotional dependence.
Table 5. Mediation analysis of the indirect effect of attachment on impulsive behavior, this relationship being mediated by emotional dependence.
Predictor variable
B
β
t
p
Variable criterion
Security
-.27
-.21
-5.83
.00
Impulsivity
Parental authority
-.28
-.09
-2.62
.01
Impulsivity
Parental leave
.28
.10
3.61
.00
Impulsivity
Self-sufficiency, resentment against parents
.28
.13
4.40
.00
Impulsivity
Childhood trauma
.12
.07
2.67
.01
Impulsivity
Emotional dependence
.08
.11
4.48
.00
Impulsivity
Note: **p < .01; *p < .05
Gender differences in attachment, emotional de-
pendence and impulsivity
Finally, gender differences in attachment, emotional de-
pendence and impulsivity were analyzed (Table 6). The re-
sults obtained in a statistically significant way are shown be-
low. The men scored significantly higher in avoiding being
alone, need to please, asymmetric relationship, emotional
dependence, cognitive impulsivity and unplanned impulsivity
with a small effect size in all of them (avoiding being alone: d
= .25; need to please: d = .27; asymmetrical relationship: d =
.16; emotional dependence: d = .24; cognitive impulsivity: d
= .05; unplanned impulsivity: d = .15). The women scored
significantly higher on safety, family concern and parental
authority, with a small effect size (security: d = .08; family
concern: d = .18; parental authority: d = .15).
The role of emotional dependence in the relationship between attachment and impulsive behavior 443
anales de psicología / annals of psychology, 2018, vol. 34, nº 3 (october)
Table 6. Mean differences in attachment, emotional dependence and impulsivity according to gender
Variables
Men (n= 826)
Woman (n= 707)
t
M
DT
M
DT
Avoid being alone
3.26
3.01
1.81
2.41
10.18**
Need to please
4.46
3.77
2.53
2.95
11.00**
Asymmetric ratio
3.78
3.41
2.70
3.10
6.41*
Emotional dependency
16.83
12.18
11.11
10.16
9.88**
Security
27.63
6.95
28.72
6.24
-3.20*
Family concern
21.60
5.79
23.59
5.08
-7.12**
Parental authority
12.42
3.05
13.29
2.37
-6.10**
Cognitive impulsivity
19.43
2.89
19.15
3.13
1.85*
Unplanned impulsivity
26.06
4.70
24.72
4.28
5.78*
Note: **p < .001; *p < .05.
Discussion
The first objective of this study has been to study the rela-
tionship between emotional dependence, attachment and
impulsive behavior. The results show that as emotional de-
pendency increases, parental permissiveness, childhood
trauma and impulsivity will also increase. These results are in
line with previous studies where emotional dependence has
been positively related to parental permissiveness and child
traumatism (Momeñe & Estévez, accepted in the publica-
tion), thus situating their origin on occasion in the emotional
or physical abuse received by attachment figures during
childhood (Moral & Sirvent, 2009). Regarding the relation-
ship between emotional dependence and impulsivity, previ-
ous studies point to deficits in impulse control in people
with emotional dependence (Villegas & Sánchez, 2013) and
difficulties in emotional regulation (Momeñe et al., 2017).
In this study, impulsivity reflects relationships in the pos-
itive sense of avoiding being alone, asymmetric relationship,
need to please, parental permissiveness and childhood trau-
ma. These results coincide with previous studies where it is
proved how the lack of care and the absence of contact with
attachment figures in childhood predisposes to the devel-
opment of impulsive behaviors (Barroso, 2014). In this vein,
in a study conducted by Patock-Peckham & Morgan-
Lithuania (2006), they found that the permissive parenting
style, on a same-sex parent, was linked to high levels of im-
pulsivity. Likewise, one of the novel results was found in the
relationship between unplanned impulsivity and security,
family concern, parental interference and parental authority,
being this relationship in a negative sense, as well as the rela-
tionship between motor impulsivity and the need for exclu-
sivity and self-sufficiency and resentment against parents,
being in this case, a relationship in a positive sense.
Secondly, the predictive role of attachment and impul-
sive behavior on emotional dependence is analyzed. The re-
sults of this study indicate that parental permission, self-
sufficiency and resentment against parents, and childhood
trauma predict emotional dependence. These results would
go along the lines of previous studies where it is pointed out
how emotionally dependent people experienced in their
childhood by their attachment figures absence of boosters,
aggression (Castelló, 2005) and unsatisfied emotional needs
(Barroso, 2014).
Thirdly, the relationship between attachment and impul-
sive behavior is studied, being this relationship mediated by
emotional dependence. The results obtained show how
emotional dependence acts as a mediator in the relationship
between security and impulsivity, parental authority and im-
pulsivity, parental permission and impulsivity, self-
sufficiency and resentment against parents and impulsivity,
childhood trauma and impulsivity. These results could be
explained by the fact that emotional dependence has been
linked to affective deficits in childhood (Castelló, 2012), as
well as impulsive behavior, with previous studies mentioning
how impulsivity levels vary depending on the parenting style
received by attachment figures (Chapple & Johnson, 2007)
and how low self-control would be the result of attachment
disruptions during childhood (Hayslett-Mccall & Bernard,
2002). Because of this, these are novel results in mentioning
emotional dependence as a mediating variable in the rela-
tionship between attachment and impulsivity.
Finally, the differences in the mean differences of emo-
tional dependence, attachment and impulsive behavior ac-
cording to sex are analyzed. It should be noted that men
scored significantly higher in avoiding being alone, need to
please, asymmetric relationship, emotional dependence,
cognitive impulsivity and unplanned impulsivity, while
women scored significantly higher in safety, family concern
and parental authority. These results are consistent with re-
cent studies where they have found a greater number of
emotionally dependent men to their partners (Urbiola &
Estévez, 2015), which carry with them aspects related to re-
jection of loneliness, the need to please and the asymmetric
relationship (Castelló, 2005). Similarly, previous studies have
found a greater number of impulsive men (Sauceda-García,
Lara-Muñoz & Fócil-Márquez, 2006), despite this, it is men-
tioned how impulsivity varies according to age, with the ado-
lescent stage being the highest rate of impulsive men report-
ing, while at later ages such differences between men and
women would decrease (Escorial & Navas, 2006).
This study is not without limitations. On the one hand,
the design of the study is transversal, making it impossible to
establish causal relationships. In this respect, longitudinal
studies could help to obtain such information. On the other
444 Ana Estévez et al.
anales de psicología / annals of psychology, 2018, vol. 34, nº 3 (october)
hand, people who suffer from emotional dependency show
difficulties in recognizing the problem, which may be due to
the social acceptance of the thoughts and manifestations
that surround it (Cogswell, Alloy, Karpinski & Grant, 2010).
In addition, it is a sample composed solely of adolescents.
In conclusion, due to the scarce research on the relation-
ship between attachment and impulsive behavior, as well as
the mediating role of emotional dependence in this relation-
ship, this study provides novel and highly clinical results for
the intervention and treatment of emotional dependence and
impulsivity. Among many other aspects, it should be noted
that parental permissiveness may be related to more impul-
sive relationships based on emotional dependence on ado-
lescents. Attachment in childhood may be an important fac-
tor for the development of impulsive behaviors in adoles-
cence, such as emotional dependence, which is closely linked
to the establishment of problematic relationships.
References
Alonso-Arbiol, I., Shaver, P. & Yárnoz, S. (2002). Insecure attachment, gen-
der roles, and interpersonal dependency in the Basque Country. Personal
Relationships, 9, 479-490. doi:10.1111/1475-6811.00030
Balluerka, N., Lacasa, F., Gorostiaga, A., Muela, A., & Pierrehumbert, B.
(2011). Versión reducida del cuestionario CaMir (CaMir-R) para la eva-
luación del apego [Short versión of CaMir questionnaire (CaMir-R) to
assess attachment]. Psicothema, 23(3), 486-494.
Barroso, O. (2014). El apego adulto: la relación de los estilos de apego desa-
rrollados en la infancia en la elección y las dinámicas de pareja [Adult at-
tachment: the attachment style developed in childhood has a significant
influence on the processes of mate choice and on the quality of affecti-
ve relationships]. Revista Digital de Medicina Psicosomática y Psicoterapia, 4(1),
1-25.
Belin, D., Mar, A. C., Dalley, J. W., Robbins, T. W. & Everitt, B. J. (2008).
High impulsivity predicts the switch to compulsive cocaine-taking. Sci-
ence, 320(5881), 1352-1355. doi:10.1126/science.1158136
Castelló, J. (2005). Dependencia emocional: características y tratamiento [Emotional
dependence: characteristics and treatment]. Madrid: Alianza.
Castelló, J. (2012). Superación de la dependencia emocional: como impedir que el amor
se convierta en un suplicio [Overcoming emotional dependence: how to prevent love
from becoming a torture]. Málaga: Ediciones Corona Borealis.
Chapple, C. L. & Johnson, K. A. (2007). Gender differences in impulsivity.
Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 5(3), 221-234.
doi:10.1177/1541204007301286
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155-159.
Cogswell, A., Alloy, L., Karpinski, A. & Grant, D. (2010). Assessing de-
pendency using self-Report and indirect measures: examining the signif-
icance of discrepancies. Journal of Personality Assessment, 92, 306-316.
doi:10.1080/00223891.2010.481986
Cubas, D., Espinoza, G., Galli, A. & Terrones, M. (2004). Intervención cog-
nitivo-conductual en un grupo de pacientes mujeres con dependencia
afectiva [Cognitive-behavioral intervention in group of women with af-
fective dependence]. Revista de Psiquiatría y Salud Mental Hermilio Valdi-
zan, 5(2), 81-90.
Deave, T. & McGraw, C. (2003). Domestic violence and women´s emotion-
al dependence on men. Primary Health Care, 13(2), 8.
doi:10.7748/phc.13.2.8.s11
Del Castillo, A., Hernández, M. E., Romero, A. & Iglesias, S. (2015). Violen-
cia en el noviazgo y su relación con la dependencia emocional pasiva en
estudiantes universitarios [Violence in the courtship and their relation-
ship with the passive emotional dependence in university students].
PSICUMEX, 5(1), 4-18.
Escorial, S. & Navas, M. J. (2006). Análisis de la variable género en las esca-
las del EDTC mediante técnicas de funcionamiento diferencial de los
ítems [Analysis of the gender variable in the EDTC using differential
ítem functioning techniques]. Psicothema, 18(2), 319-325.
Evenden, J. L. (1999). Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology, 146(4),
348-361.
Feeney, J. A. & Noller, P. (1990). Attachment Style as a Predictor of Adult
Romantic Relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(2),
281-291. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.281
Feeney, J. A. & Noller, P. (2001). El apego adulto [Adult attachment]. Bilbao:
Desclée de Brouwer.
Gómez-Zapiain, J., Ortiz, M. J. & Gómez-Lope, J. (2012). Capacidad para
aportar y solicitar apoyo emocional en las relaciones de pareja en rela-
ción con los perfiles de apego [Ability to provide and request emotional
support in affective relationships, in accordance with attachment profi-
les]. Anales de Psicología, 28(1), 302-312.
Hayslett-Mccall, K. L. & Bernard, T. J. (2002). Attachment, masculinity, and
self-control: a theory of male crime rates. Theorical Criminology, 6(1), 5-33.
doi:10.1177/136248060200600101
Izquierdo, S. A. & Gómez-Acosta, A. (2013). Dependencia afectiva: aborda-
je desde una perspectiva contextual [Affective dependence: an aproach
of contextual perspective]. Psychologia. Avances de la Disciplina, 7, 81-91.
Jaller, C. & Lemos, M. (2009). Esquemas desadaptativos tempranos en estu-
diantesuniversitarios con dependencia emocional [Early maladaptative
schemas in university students with emotional dependency]. Acta Co-
lombiana de Psicología 12(2), 77-83.
Lemos, M. & Londoño, N. H. (2006). Construcción y validación del cues-
tionario de dependencia emocional en población Colombiana [Design
and validation of the emotional dependence questionnaire in Colom-
bian population]. Acta Colombiana de Psicología, 9(2), 127-140.
López, F. (2003). Apego y relaciones amorosas [Attachment and loving rela-
tionships]. Informació Psicológica, 82, 36-48.
Loinaz, I. (2011). Estudo de tipologías de agressors conjugais em prisoes
[The study of the partner violent men typologies in prison]. Ousar Inte-
grar Revista de Reinsercao Social e Prova, 9, 23-34.
Loinaz, I. & Echeburúa, E. (2012). Apego adulto en agresores de pareja
[Adult attachment in partner-violent men]. Acción Psicológica, 9(1), 33-46.
https://doi.org/10.5944/ap.9.1.435
Mayseless, O. (1991). Adult Attachment Patterns and Courtship Violence.
Family Relations, 40(1), 21-28. doi:10.2307/585654
Michelena, M. (2007). Mujeres malqueridas: atadas a relaciones destructivas y sin fu-
turo [Poorly beloved women: tied to destructive and dead-end relationships]. Ma-
drid: La Esfera de los Libros.
Mikulincer, M. & Shaver, P. R. (2011). Attachment, anger and aggression.
En Shaver, P. R. y Mikulincer, M. (Eds.), Human Aggression and violence.
Causes, manifestations and consequences (pp.241-257). Washington, DC:
American Psychological Association.
Moeller, F. G., Barratt, E. S., Dougherty, D. M., Schmitz, J. M. & Swann, A.
C. (2001). Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. American Journal of Psychiatry,
158(11), 1783-1793. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783
Momeñe, J., Jáuregui, P. & Estévez, A. (2017). El papel predictor del abuso
psicológico y la regulación emocional en la dependencia emocional
[Predictive role of psychological abuse and emotional regulation on
emotional dependency]. Behavioral Psychology/Psicología Conductual, 25(1),
65-78.
Momeñe, J. & Estévez, A. (aceptada publicación). Los estilos de crianza pa-
rentales como predictores del apego adulto, de la dependencia emocio-
nal y del abuso psicológico en las relaciones de pareja adultas [Parental
styles as predictors of adult attachment, emotional dependency and
psychological abuse in adult couple relationships]. Behavioral Psycho-
logy/Psicología Conductual.
Moral, M. V. & Sirvent, C. (2008). Dependencias sentimentales o afectivas:
etiología, clasificación y evaluación [Sentimental or affective dependen-
cies: etiology, classification and evaluation]. Revista Española de Drogode-
pendencias, 33(2), 150-167.
Moral, M. V. & Sirvent, C. (2009). Dependencia afectiva y género: perfil sin-
tomático diferencial en dependientes afectivos españoles [Affective de-
The role of emotional dependence in the relationship between attachment and impulsive behavior 445
anales de psicología / annals of psychology, 2018, vol. 34, nº 3 (october)
pendence and gender: symptomatic profile in Spanish affective depen-
dents]. Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 43, 230-240.
Papachristou, H., Nederkoorn, C., Havermans, R., Van der Horst, M. &
Jansen A. (2012). Can´t stop the craving: the effect of impulsivity on
cue-elicited craving for alcohol in heavy and light social drinkers. Psycho-
pharmacology, 219(2), 511-518. doi:10.1007/s00213-011-2240-5
Patock-Peckham, J. A. & Morgan-López, A. A. (2006). College drinking be-
haviors: mediational links between parenting styles, impulse control,
and alcohol-related outcomes. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 20(2), 117-
125. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.20.2.117
Pierrehumbert, B., Karmaniola, A., Sieye, A., Meister, C., Miljkovitch, R., &
Halfon, O. (1996). Les modèles de relations: Développement d’un auto-
questionnaire d’attachement pour adultes [relationship models: the de-
velopment of an adult self-questionnaire]. Psychiatrie de l’Enfant, 1, 161-
206.
Pradas, E. & Perles, F. (2012). Resolución de conflictos de pareja en adoles-
centes, sexismo y dependencia emocional [Conflict resolution in adoles-
cent couples, sexism and emotional dependence]. Quaderns de Psicología,
14(1), 45-60. doi:10.5565/rev/qpsicologia.1041
Salvo, L., & Castro, A. (2013). Confiabilidad y validez de la escala de impul-
sividad de Barratt (BIS-11) en adolescentes [Reability and validity of Ba-
rrat impulsiveness scale (BIS-11) in adolescents]. Revista chilena de neuro-
psiquiatría, 51(4), 245-254. doi:10.4067/S0717-92272013000400003
Sauceda-García, J. M., Lara-Muñoz, C. & Fócil-Márquez, M. (2006). Violen-
cia autodirigida en la adolescencia: el intento de suicidio [Self-inflicted
violence in adolescence: the suicide attempt]. Medigraphic, 63, 223-231.
Scantamburlo, G., Pitchot, W. & Ansseau, M. (2013). Affective dependency.
Revue medicale de Liege, 68(5-6), 340-347.
Schaeffer, B. (1998). ¿Es amor o es adicción? [Is it love or addiction?]. Barcelona:
Apóstrofe.
Skvortsova, S. N. & Shumskiy, V. B. (2014). Existential-Phenomenological
Analysis of Dependence in Close Interpersonal Relationships. Existen-
zanalyse, 31(1), 4-13.
Urbiola, I. & Estévez, A. (2015). Dependencia emocional y esquemas
desadaptativos tempranos en el noviazgo de adolescentes y jóvenes
[Emotional dependency and early maladaptative schemas in adolescents
and youth dating relationships]. Behavioral Psychology/Psicología Conductual,
23, 571-587.
Urbiola, I., Estévez, A., & Iraurgi, I. (2014). Dependencia emocional en el
noviazgo de jóvenes y adolescentes (DEN): desarrollo y validación de
un instrumento [Emotional dependency on courtship among youth
(EDC): development and validation of an instrument]. Ansiedad y Estrés,
20(2-3), 101-114.
Verdejo-García, A., Lawrence, A. J. & Clark, L. (2008). Impulsivity as a vul-
nerability marker for substance disorders: review of findings from high-
risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neuro-
science Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(4), 777-810.
doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.11.003
Villegas, M. J. & Sánchez, N. (2013). Dependencia emocional en un grupo
de mujeres denunciantes de maltrato de pareja [Emotional dependence
on a group of women reporting partner abuse]. Revista Textos y Sentidos,
7, 10-29.
World Medical Association (2013). Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles
for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA, 310(20), 2191-
2194. doi:10.1001/jama.2013.281053
Yárnoz-Yaben, S. (2010). Attachment style and adjustment to divorce. The
Spanish Journal of Psychology, 13(1), 210-219.
Young, J. (1999). Cognitive therapy for personality disorders: A Schema-Focused Ap-
proach. Sarasota: Proffesional Resource Press.
(Article received: 07-12-2017; revised: 22-01-2018; accepted: 05-02-2018)
... Además, nuestros resultados muestran que a medida que el consumo de cocaína es más regular y aumenta se incrementan a su vez las puntuaciones en las diferentes subescalas del DERS y estos hallazgos concuerdan con los de las investigaciones recientes de Garke et al. (2021) Por otra parte, los adolescentes del grupo experimental y regular de nuestra muestra obtuvieron puntuaciones más altas que los participantes del grupo de no consumo en todas las subescalas del MULTICAGE CAD 4, sobre todo siendo estadísticamente significativas en las subescalas de juego, consumo de otras drogas, compra compulsiva y conducta sexual. Estos hallazgos son compatibles con los hallados en otras investigaciones Estévez et al., 2018;Macía et al., 2023) que refieren la asociación entre conductas adictivas de tipo tóxico con las conductuales como el juego, compra compulsiva, conducta sexual e incluso con la dependencia emocional, considerada por algunos profesionales como un tipo adicción conductual (Villa y Servent, 2008). De hecho, los consumidores del grupo experimental y regular de nuestra muestra puntúan más alto en dependencia emocional en todas las subescalas del DEN respecto a los no consumidores, siendo estadísticamente significativa en el grupo de consumo regular de cocaína para la puntuación total y en las diversas subescalas como evitar estar solo, necesidad de exclusividad, necesidad de agradar y relación asimétrica. ...
... Además, estas diferencias se mantienen e incluso aumentan en algún caso, como en consumo de alcohol, juego, conducta alimentaria, compra compulsiva y conducta sexual a medida que aumenta el consumo de cocaína en el grupo de frecuencia de los últimos 20-30 días. Estos hallazgos que relacionan la asociación entre el consumo de sustancias en este caso de cocaína con otras conductas adictivas y de carácter impulsivo se han obtenido en otras investigaciones Estévez et al., 2018;Macía et al., 2023) y refuerzan la relación entre conductas adictivas de tipo tóxico con las conductuales y lo importante es que nuestro estudio confirma esta asociación en nuestra muestra de adolescentes. Además, en esta línea los adolescentes que no habían consumido mostraron menores puntuaciones en impulsividad y en dependencia emocional. ...
Article
El objetivo de la presente investigación es estudiar las relaciones entre el consumo de cocaína en adolescentes, y la regulación emocional, los esquemas inadaptados tempranos, la dependencia emocional y la impulsividad. Se lleva a cabo un estudio de diseño no experimental y correlacional de corte transversal en una muestra que está compuesta por 1.533 adolescentes escolarizados de ambos sexos, siendo el 53.9% hombres (n= 826) y el 46.1% mujeres (n= 707), con edades comprendidas entre los 14 y 18 años (M= 15.76, DT= 1.25) pertenecientes a doce Unidades Educativas Fiscales del Cantón Portoviejo de la Provincia de Manabí de la República de Ecuador. Los instrumentos de evaluación empleados han sido: MULTICAGE CAD 4 (Pedrero Pérez et al., 2007), Escala de dificultades en la regulación emocional- DERS (Gratz y Romer, 2004; adaptación española realizada por Gómez-Simón et al., 2014), Cuestionario de esquemas, forma reducida-SQ-SF (Young y Brown, 1994; adaptación española realizada por Cid y Torrubia, 2002), la Escala de dependencia emocional en el noviazgo de jóvenes y adolescentes-DEN (Urbiola et al., 2014), la Escala de Impulsividad de BARRATT BIS 11 (Oquendo et al., 2001) y la Encuesta Estatal sobre Uso de Drogas en Estudiantes de Enseñanzas Secundarias -ESTUDES (Plan Nacional sobre Drogas, 2012). Los resultados de este estudio aportan evidencias sobre las relaciones existentes entre las dificultades en la regulación emocional, los esquemas inadaptados tempranos, la dependencia emocional y la impulsividad con el consumo experimental y regular de cocaína en adolescentes. Se han encontrado diferencias entre hombres y mujeres según la frecuencia del consumo de cocaína en el último mes. Se discuten las implicaciones de estos resultados y se plantean retos para futuras investigaciones.
... Research indicates that high ED is associated with domestic violence (Chafla-Quise & Lara-Machado, 2021) and an irrational desire to meet other people's needs (Moyeda et al., 2020). In young people, ED is associated with behavioural impulsivity, childhood trauma, and granting independence from parents (Estévez et al., 2018). The correlation of the phenomenon with distance anxiety and the search for social attention was recorded in students (Lemos et al., 2019). ...
... The consistent links of EI with self-confidence and autonomy support the notion of the phenomenon's impact on student well-being (Toscano-Hermoso et al., 2020). The students' ED manifests itself in social insecurity and is associated with impulsiveness of actions (Estévez et al., 2018). This position was one of the key positions for our research. ...
Article
Full-text available
The aim of the article is to determine the specifics of the relationship between the components of emotional intelligence (EI) and the emotional dependency (ED) reactions in student age. The MSCEIT (The Mayer–Salovey–Caruzo Emotional Intelligence Test) and R. Hirshfield’s Interpersonal Dependence Test were used to achieve the aim and objectives of the study. The article analyses the dynamics and correlation of indicators for educational courses at the bachelor’s level of higher education institutions (HEIs). The prevalence of the medium indicators of the levels of EI and manifestations of ED was found during the studied period. A significant increase in EI indicators was determined between the second and third years of study. A gradual decrease in the ED reaction before the completion of studies in HEIs was recorded. Significant correlations of the ED parameters with the conscious management of emotions, the ability to understand and identify experiences were calculated. Most often, the connections between the EI components were established to manifested with the level of self-confidence. It can be stated that high EI indicators determine a decrease in EI. The research prospects may be the development and verification of the effectiveness of programmes for EI development at student age.
... In Latin American cultures, gender roles clearly influence this predisposition, as women are expected to be more sentimental and affectionate, while men are encouraged to be emotionally detached. However, the findings of the present study contradict this predisposition, with differences in emotional dependence observed primarily among men, differences that were also found by other authors (Urbiola and Estévez, 2015;Estévez et al., 2018). Finally, some studies have not found gender differences in emotional dependence. ...
... These findings corroborate those in previous research, both in similar (Díaz-Mosquera et al., 2024) and in different cultural contexts, in which evidence has been provided of the relationship between emotional dependence and adult attachment. Valle and Moral (2018) found a relationship between emotional dependence and the fearful attachment style (Valle and Moral, 2018) and Estévez et al. (2018) with the preoccupied style; similarly, other research established a relationship between insecure attachment variables and emotional dependence. ...
Article
Full-text available
The purpose of this study is to examine the relationship between attachment styles and emotional dependence in adulthood. Attachment formed in childhood often continues into adulthood, influencing the quality of adult relationships, especially romantic ones. Insecure attachment between children and parents is linked to negative outcomes in adult relationships, such as poor relationship quality and infidelity. Recent societal changes have added new pressures on individuals and their relationships. Emotional dependence has emerged as a disorder rooted in emotional reliance, emphasizing the need to explore the underlying factors of dependent relationships. The study included 1,383 participants aged 25 to 60 from Quito, Ecuador, who completed online questionnaires on relationship styles and emotional dependency. Differences in gender and age were found in both areas, confirming some findings from earlier studies. Additionally, two independent predictive models were identified: preoccupied attachment explained 25% of the emotional dependence, and fearful attachment explained 14%. These results suggest that individuals with low self-confidence and feelings of unworthiness are more likely to develop emotional dependence. These new insights make an important contribution to the field. The study also suggests intervention areas for clinical and real-life settings to improve well-being based on these findings.
... The first study, conducted on a sample of IPV perpetrators and nonviolent men (Buck, Leenaars, Emmelkamp, & Van Marle, 2012), highlighted that insecurely attached batterers scored higher on dependency on partners than securely attached men. Barbarias-García et al. (2024) and Estévez et al. (2018) observed a negative correlation between emotional dependency and security. Further, the authors (2018) found a positive association between emotional dependency and dimensions related to more general childhood insecurity, such as being fearful and overprotected and having a lack of parental limits during childhood. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background and aim The present study stems from the need to deepen knowledge regarding understanding the psychological mechanisms underlying Love Addiction (LA) and investigate the role of attachment style as a theoretical framework. We conducted a systematic review of the literature and a meta-analysis of the investigated topics to achieve this objective. Method Five databases (PsycINFO, PsycARTICLES, MEDLINE, Scopus, and Web of Science) systematically searched for studies that analyzed the relationship between LA and attachment dimensions, published in all years until September 23, 2024. Results Overall, we found 27 studies; of these, we included 15 in the meta-analysis, with 3,628 participants. The analyses were conducted using the test of heterogeneity, effect dispersion, and moderator analysis (gender, age, research continent, quality of the studies, and types of attachment instruments). A significant and positive relationship ( r = 0.39, p < 0 .05) was observed between Love Addiction and anxious attachment, while a significant and negative relationship was found with avoidant attachment ( r = −0.09, p < 0.05). Conclusions The systematic review and meta-analysis provide an important starting point for understanding and operationalizing the construct. The association with attachment styles allows postulating the relational nature of Love Addiction, differentiating it from other addictions. In addition, the association with anxious and avoidant attachment shows promise in understanding Love Addict subtypes.
... Degrees of impulse control may be understood through associations with emotional dependence and a preoccupied attachment style, with emotional dependence serving as a mediator in this relationship. Estévez et al. (2018) highlighted that an increase in impulsivity contributes to a heightened perception of asymmetry in relationships, the need to please others, childhood trauma, and parental permission. From this perspective, impulsivity may play a role in how individuals perceive asymmetry in a relationship, as those with higher impulsive outspokenness are more likely to feel that they receive less support, protection, and initiative from others. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Relating to others and establishing relationships is necessary for optimal human functioning. Perceived relational satisfaction appears to be one of the most important aspects of individuals’ lives, reflecting the extent to which our relational needs are met. This study aimed to test the factor structure, item characteristics, and convergent validity of the Bosnian adaptation of the Relational Needs Satisfaction Scale (RNSS). Method A total of 420 participants (N = 420) completed the Relational Needs Satisfaction Scale (RNSS), the Relationships Questionnaire (RQ), and the Need for Drama Questionnaire (NFD). Descriptive statistics, reliability, and item analysis for the RNSS were conducted. Both exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses were conducted, along with a comparison of four models. The convergent validity of the RNSS was assessed by examining its association with two reference measures and their subscales: RQ and NFD. Results The study showed that the translation was adequate, and the Bosnian version of the RNSS proved to be a reliable measure with mostly adequate item parameters. It confirmed that the RNSS structure can be interpreted as a five-factor model, comprising five dimensions and one higher-order factor, as well as a bi-factor model, where the variance of the items is simultaneously explained by a general factor and the five dimensions to varying degrees. The comparison of models and theory indicated the superiority of the bi-factor model. Conclusion The adaptation of the RNSS Bosnian version demonstrated content validity, adequate measurement accuracy, and appropriate construct validity, as supported by confirmatory factor analysis. Furthermore, this study provides evidence that the translated RNSS is a valid and reliable instrument.
... Therefore, without adequate emotional e-regulation strategies, stressful virtual situations could easily lead girls to lose their self-respect, thereby leading them to become the object of cybervictimisation. It is likewise plausible that boys, who are more impulsive, will be more prone to become victims when trying to escape feelings of loneliness (Estévez et al., 2018). ...
Article
Full-text available
The socialisation of adolescent boys and girls takes place primarily online in virtual environments, where cyberbullying is one of the greatest current social dangers faced by that age group. Socio-emotional e-competencies (i.e., competencies that apply specifically to online environments) are a protective factor against cyberaggression and cybervictimisation, and research has shown that the variables of sex and academic year (age) play a relevant role in that interaction. Although such socio-emotional e-competencies have been shown to play an adaptive role in further types of online behaviour, they have not yet been studied specifically in relation to cyberaggression and cybervictimisation. Our study’s goal was thus to ascertain whether socio-emotional e-competencies have a predictive value for cyberaggression and cybervictimisation, and whether differences can be observed according to sex or academic year. Participants were 945 Spanish adolescents (56.5% female, 43.5% male) ages 12 to 16 (mean age: 13.80, DE = 1.27) enrolled in 13 secondary schools. Data were collected via self-report questionnaires ( European Cyberbullying Intervention Project Questionnaire [ECIP-Q] and Socio-Emotional E-Competencies Questionnaire [e-COM]) . We observed differences according to sex and academic year in cyberbullying behaviour and socio-emotional e-competencies. Moreover, significant relationships can be established between socio-emotional e-competencies, on the one hand, and cyberaggression and cybervictimisation, on the other, with differences among groups as revealed by SEM multigroup analysis. The levels of emotional e-regulation in girls and e-self-control of impulsivity in boys across all academic years explain most instances of cybervictimisation and cyberaggression, especially in younger-aged groups. These findings provide a new focus to help prevent cyberbullying.
... Por ejemplo, hay estudios donde se ha comprobado que la dependencia emocional se da más frecuentemente en las mujeres (González-Jiménez y Hernández-Romera, 2014), lo cual concordaría con la concepción de que los roles subordinados son asignados en mayor medida a las mismas (Ramiro-Sánchez et al., 2018). Por el contrario, en otros se han encontrado niveles más altos de dependencia emocional en hombres y adolescentes varones (Estévez et al., 2018;Marcos et al., 2020;Urbiola y Estévez, 2015) o bien que no existen diferencias atribuibles al género en dependencia emocional (Cuetos, 2017). De acuerdo con Moral y Sirvent (2008), en población clínica se ha comprobado que los varones son más buscadores de sensaciones y se muestran más inflexibles, si bien las mujeres presentan una conciencia de problema distorsionada y emplean más mecanismos de negación y no afrontamiento. ...
Article
Full-text available
RESUMEN Antecedentes/Objetivo: La dependencia emocional es un trastorno que suele pasar desapercibido debido a la creencia social de que no es patológico por su vinculación con la visión idealizada del amor. En este sentido, se diseñó un estudio con el objetivo de conocer la relación entre dependencia emocional, autoengaño y percepción de los mitos del amor romántico. Método: En este estudio participaron 262 personas (113 hombres y 149 mujeres) con edades comprendidas entre los 18 y los 59 años (M = 31.9, DT = 12.37) y con relación de pareja que respondieron a medidas de dependencia, autoengaño patológico (manipulación y mistificación) y mitos sobre el amor idealizado y maltratador. Resultados: Los resultados mostraron una relación positiva entre dependencia emocional, autoengaño y percepción de los mitos del amor romántico. También se han encontrado diferencias en dependencia emocional en función de la edad, pero no del género. Conclusiones: Se concluye que la dependencia emocional se vincula a una visión arquetípica del amor y cursa con mecanismos de autoengaño.
... Posiblemente, este incremento se deba al interés de la comunidad científica por comprender las implicancias en la salud mental de las personas con dependencia emocional y la asociación con variables de relevancia clínica. Así, por ejemplo, se reporta relación con sintomatología ansiosa y depresiva (Lemos et al., 2019;Urbiola et al., 2017), perdón (Beltrán-Morillas et al., 2019), impulsividad (Estévez et al., 2018), intolerancia a la incertidumbre , violencia de pareja (Amor et al., 2022; López-Barreira y Moral-Jiménez, 2020; Martín y Moral, 2019), esquemas y distorsiones cognitivas (Moral-Jiménez y González-Sáez, 2020), conductas adictivas (Macía et al., 2022;Momeñe et al., 2021) y trastornos de la conducta alimentaria (Momeñe et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
La dependencia emocional es un constructo que está generando mayor interés por una parte de la comunidad científica. Por ello, es importante disponer de instrumentos que reúnan propiedades psicométricas adecuadas para su medición. El objetivo del estudio fue analizar las evidencias de validez basado en la estructura interna, validez convergente y medida de confiabilidad del Inventario de Dependencia Emocional (IDE). Participaron 1,401 adultos jóvenes (M =21 años, DE = 2.7; 24.2% varones y 75.8% mujeres), residentes en la ciudad de Lima, Perú. Se usó análisis factorial confirmatorio y medidas de asociación bivariada. Los resultados sugieren que el IDE presenta evidencias de validez de estructura interna para un modelo de siete factores oblicuos; dispone de evidencias de validez basado en la relación con violencia de pareja y satisfacción con la vida; y las dimensiones que la conforman presentan medidas de confiabilidad satisfactorias. Estos hallazgos pueden respaldar el uso del IDE en evaluación e investigación.
Article
Full-text available
Suicide is a major public health concern that has negatively impacted people worldwide socioeconomically, physically, and psychologically. Every instance is a heart‐breaking loss that deeply impacts families, communities and nations, leaving lasting repercussions for those left behind. The aim of the study was to explore precipitating factors of people who experienced a suicide attempt. A qualitative descriptive exploratory design was used with the help of a semi‐structured interview guide to collect data from thirteen (13) suicide attempters' with ages between 20 and 48 years. The data were transcribed verbatim after it had been audio recorded digitally. The data were analyzed using thematic analysis with MAXQDA 20. Information gathered yielded two major themes and six subthemes as the precipitating factors for suicide attempt. The two main themes were (1) Social/family issues, with its subthemes; mistrust, wasted resources and parental pressure (2) Psychological issues with the subthemes; Childhood experiences, emotional breakdown and difficult life pursuits. The study showed that suicide is a very complex problem in every society and there is general ignorance about the triggers, signs and appropriate help seeking strategies. It is therefore vital to create massive public awareness on suicide attempt triggers and symptoms through health education to help save lives.
Article
Full-text available
Antecedentes El uso de las redes sociales se ha desarrollado rápidamente y su uso en los últimos años está aumentado progresivamente, modificado nuestros hábitos y el modo de interacción y comunicación. El uso problemático de redes sociales ha sido relacionado positivamente con otras conductas adictivas, siendo especialmente significativa la dependencia emocional, al tiempo que ésta tiene relación con el apego recibido en la infancia. Objetivos 1) conocer la correlación entre el apego, el uso de redes sociales y la dependencia emocional, 2) estudiar el rol predictivo de la dependencia emocional y el apego sobre el uso de redes sociales, 3) analizar el papel de la dependencia emocional en la relación entre el apego y el uso de redes sociales en jóvenes, y 4) valorar si existen diferencias de sexo. Método La muestra estuvo compuesta por 713 participantes (132 hombres y 581 mujeres) de entre 18 y 35 años de edad (M=21.27; SD: 3.34). Resultados Los resultados mostraron que el estilo de apego y la dependencia emocional predicen el uso de redes sociales. Además, la dependencia emocional media en la relación entre el apego y el uso de redes sociales. Conclusiones Los resultados de este estudio son novedosos y aportan valor clínico.
Article
Full-text available
Abstract: The objective of this research is to identify the characteristics of affective dependency on a group of women who were abused by their partner; who also filled a criminal complaint at the House of Justice in “Villa Santana”, a neighborhood in the city of Pereira. This research also involved eight subjects who reported out some physical or psychological abuse. Case study was the methodological design that was implemented for this research. Results are presented as eight evaluations, which include the implementation of socio-demographic interviews and the application of two questionnaires which measure the emotional dependence and the abuse type respectively. The results to this problem were addressed from a descriptive and explanatory scope. Keywords: Physical abuse. Psychological abuse, emotional dependency
Article
Full-text available
El noviazgo es una etapa donde las personas tienen la oportunidad de conocerse, aprender a compartir, identificar afinidades e, inclusive, detectar indicios de violencia en la relación a largo plazo. Éstos últimos pueden aumentar en frecuencia e intensidad con el paso del tiempo, hasta representar un precedente de la violencia doméstica. Estudios previos refieren que la presencia de ansiedad por separación como indicador de dependencia emocional en las relaciones interpersonales ilustra las variables asociadas al origen de la violencia en el noviazgo. El objetivo del presente estudio fue identificar la relación entre los factores de dependencia emocional y violencia en el noviazgo entre estudiantes universitarios. Participaron 317 estudiantes de las licenciaturas en Medicina, Administración y Derecho de la Universidad Autónoma del Estado de Hidalgo. Se utilizó el Cuestionario de Dependencia Emocional Pasiva y el Cuestionario de Violencia en el Noviazgo, para evaluar las variables mencionadas. Se llevó a cabo un análisis de correlación de Pearson, con el cual se determinaron relaciones positivas, estadísticamente significativas, entre los factores ansiedad por separación y expresiones límite; del cuestionario de dependencia se encontraron relaciones positivas, estadísticamente significativas, con el chantaje, el control y los celos. A partir de estos resultados, se podrán plantear intervenciones cuyo principal objetivo sea desarrollar habilidades sociales que faciliten la independencia y la mejoría en las relaciones de noviazgo.
Article
Full-text available
Resumen El abuso psicológico es la forma de violencia más empleada en las relaciones de pareja. El presente estudio analiza las relaciones afectivas con los padres en la infancia con el fin de explorar cómo han repercutido en la edad adulta a la hora de mantener las relaciones de pareja. En consecuencia, los objetivos de este estudio son analizar la relación entre los estilos de crianza parentales y el apego adulto con la dependencia emocional y el abuso psicológico en las relaciones de pareja, así como, analizar el papel predictivo de los estilos de crianza parentales sobre las variables mencionadas y, finalmente, comprobar si las personas dependientes emocionales presentan esquemas disfuncionales de dependencia. La muestra estuvo compuesta por 269 participantes (219 mujeres y 50 hombres) de entre 18 y 65 años de edad. Los resultados mostraron como los estilos de crianza parentales, sobre todo la privación emocional en la infancia, predeciría la dependencia emocional, la permanencia en relaciones abusivas y el establecimiento de relaciones donde estén presentes la dependencia emocional junto con el abuso psicológico. PALABRAS CLAVE: estilos de crianza, apego, dependencia emocional, abuso psicológico. Abstract Psychological abuse is the most frequent form of violence form relationships. The present study was intended to analyze affective relations with parents in the childhood with the aim of exploring how they affect relationships during adulthood. Thus, the objectives of this study are to analyze the link between parenting styles and attachment with emotional dependency and psychological abuse in relationships, as well as to examine the predictive role of parenting styles in the mentioned variables, and finally to check if emotionally dependent subjects present dysfunctional schemes of dependency. The sample was composed by 269 participants (219 women and 50 men) with ages between 18 and 65. The results show how the parenting styles, above all emotional privation during childhood, will predict emotional dependency, permanence in abusive relationships and the establishment of relationships were emotional dependency with psychological abuse will be present.
Article
Full-text available
La violencia en adolescentes es un tema de especial importancia en la actualidad, además es un tema poco tratado y estudiado, por estos motivos se decidió indagar en algunas de las variables que influyen en el modo de resolver los conflictos de pareja por parte de los adolescentes. El estudio tiene como principal objetivo establecer una relación entre las variables sexismo y dependencia emocional y resolución de conflictos en adolescentes. La muestra está integrada por 608 adolescentes de cinco institutos de la ciudad de Málaga. Los datos del estudio fueron analizados aplicando pruebas paramétricas, índices de correlación y análisis de regresión. Los resultados muestran niveles más favorables en las chicas en el uso de la violencia en las relaciones de pareja, sin embargo se muestran niveles superiores en los chicos en la variable sexismo. También aparecen diferencias estadísticamente significativas entre la dependencia, los conflictos propios y los de pareja. Además se muestra también una asociación positiva entre sexismo y dependencia. Por otra parte, las variables sexismo, dependencia y estrategias de resolución de conflictos en la pareja fueron predictores significativos de las estrategias de resolución de conflictos propias. Finalmente se observa una fuerte asociación entre los conflictos propios y los conflictos de pareja.
Article
Full-text available
Emotional dependency is defined as a need for affection and continuous and excessive contact with the partner. The aim of this study was to analyze the relation between emotional dependency, emotion regulation, and psychological abuse in couple relationships, as well as to analyze differences in these variables regarding gender. The sample comprised 303 participants (232 women and 71 men) aged between 18 and 75 years old (M= 25.93, SD= 8.66). Results showed that women had greater emotion regulation difficulties than men, whereas there were no significant differences amongst men and women in psychological abuse and emotional dependency. Psychological abuse, emotion regulation and emotional dependency were found to correlate. Psychological abuse and difficulties in emotion regulation were predictors of emotional dependency. These results may provide new keys to the incorporation of emotion regulation and emotional dependency in prevention and intervention in psychological abuse. © 2017, Fundacion para el Avance de la Psicologia Clinica Conductual. All rights reserved.
Article
Full-text available
By using the Emotional Dependence Questionnaire (EDQ) (Henderson and Cunningham 1993) this study aimed to examine factors potentially related to women’s emotional dependence on men and how they might contribute to women staying in abusive relationships. Undertaken at a domestic outreach programme, 113 abused women completed the EDQ. Women’s emotional dependence on men was found to be associated with numerous negative factors, including increased depression, anxiety, hostility towards other women, decreased self-esteem, self-efficacy, personal identity, social support and life satisfaction. While the authors had hypothesised that women who had not left their abusers would be more emotionally dependent on men than those women who had, this did not prove to be the case. The results of this study suggest that increasing women’s self-esteem and decreasing their emotional dependence on men may have a positive effect on women’s anxiety, depression and life satisfaction.
Article
Emotional dependency is defined as the extreme need of affection that someone feels towards his/her partner. In childhood and adolescence early maladaptive schemas that could affect dating relationships are formed. Therefore, the aim of this study was to analyze emotional dependency by sex and age and its association with early maladaptive cognitive schemas in youth and adolescents who have maintained at least one dating relationship. Emotional dependency, early maladaptive schemas and variables associated with dating relationships in young adolescents were examined in 1092 participants from different school and colleges. Emotional dependency was found to be significantly higher in boys depending on age. Early maladaptive schemas also showed a significant correlation with Emotional Dependency. These results provide insights into adolescents’ dating relationships and how emotional dependency affects them. © 2015, Fundacion para el Avance de la Psicologia Clinica Conductual. All rights reserved.