PreprintPDF Available

Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation: Time for Expansion and Clarification

Motivation Science
Authors:
Preprints and early-stage research may not have been peer reviewed yet.

Abstract

The goal of this paper is to provide a long overdue clarification and upgrade to what has been called the intrinsic-extrinsic dichotomy in the realm of motivation. We argue that the concept of intrinsic motivation should be limited to refer to the pleasure gained from an activity, divorced from any further elements. It means liking the doing. The term has been confounded with a different type of motivation, which is properly labeled achievement motivation and which refers to competition against some standard of excellence (subconscious or conscious). Achievement motivation means wanting to do well. One can like doing something and not care about how well
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation: Time for Expansion and Clarification
Edwin A. Locke
University of Maryland
Kaspar Schattke
Université du Québec à Montréal
Author Note
Edwin A. Locke, Dean’s Professor of Motivation and Leadership, Robert H. Smith
School of Business, University of Maryland, College Park, USA; Kaspar Schattke, Assistant
Professor in Work and Organizational Psychology, Department of Psychology, Université du
Québec à Montréal, Canada.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kaspar Schattke,
Université du Québec à Montréal, Département de psychologie, CP 8888, Succ. Centre-Ville,
Montréal, Québec, H3C 3P8, Canada, Email: schattke.kaspar_philipp@uqam.ca. Tel: +1-514-
987-3000 ext. 5387.
© 2018, American Psychological Association. This paper is not the copy of record
and may not exactly replicate the final, authoritative version of the article. Please do not
copy or cite without authors' permission. The final article will be available, upon
publication, via its DOI: 10.1037/mot0000116.
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 2
Abstract
The goal of this paper is to provide a long overdue clarification and upgrade to what has been
called the intrinsic-extrinsic dichotomy in the realm of motivation. We argue that the concept of
intrinsic motivation should be limited to refer to the pleasure gained from an activity, divorced
from any further elements. It means liking the doing. The term has been confounded with a
different type of motivation, which is properly labeled achievement motivation and which refers
to competition against some standard of excellence (subconscious or conscious). Achievement
motivation means wanting to do well. One can like doing something and not care about how well
one does it. Conversely, one can strive to do well even if one does not like an activity. The third
type of motivation, known as extrinsic, has been focused heavily on the effect of withdrawing a
monetary incentive. We argue that this focus is far too narrow and that extrinsic motivation should
be generally defined as doing something as a means to an end. It means doing something now in
order to get something later. Nor should money be regarding as “controlling” since people
routinely make many choices in the realm of money. We suggest a research program on the three
types of motivation, their possible interrelationships, and their outcomes. We hope to spark a
discussion and invite comments on this paper.
Keywords: intrinsic motivation; extrinsic motivation; achievement motivation; work motivation;
satisfaction;
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 3
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation: Time for Expansion and Clarification
Going as far back as 1918 (Woodworth, 1918 and noted by Rheinberg & Engeser, 2018) it has
been generally assumed that there are two basic types of motivation: intrinsic and extrinsic. In
this conceptual article we argue that the traditional dichotomy is seriously inadequate particularly
with regard to applications to work and organizational psychology. We recommend a trichotomy,
which would include achievement motivation as well as clearer definitions of intrinsic and
extrinsic motivation. Both the expansion and clarifications have applied implications and
applications for future research. Our ultimate goal is to motivate a discussion and a research
program about the differences and the similarities of the three types of motivation and their
possible interrelationships.
The Concept of Motivation
We start by suggesting that the terms intrinsic and extrinsic themselves are somewhat
problematic when applied to humans. Intrinsic conventionally means inside the entity and
extrinsic means outside the entity or object. This makes sense at the physical (including
inanimate) level, but a human consciousness is always interacting with the environment
(observing, evaluating, choosing) and thus factors pertaining to motivation have both an internal
and an external aspect. Consequently, Schultheiss and colleagues (2009, p. 268) have called
motivation a natural linchpin between the person and the situation”, between internal and
external.
Motivation at the internal level means having desires or aversions. It means wanting or
fearing. One cannot be motivated if one is an empty organism as the behaviorists used to imply.
Motivation orients, energizes, and selects behavior towards anticipated goal states (Heckhausen
& Heckhausen, 2018; McClelland, 1987; Rheinberg & Vollmeyer, 2018). Thus, one can say that
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 4
all motivation involves people wanting to get or avoid something. Ordinarily we want or fear
something in the environment, although some desires are psychological such as wanting to
reduce anxiety. The ultimate source of the values that cause these wants are varied but there must
be an internal locus in some form to motivate action, even as external conditions play a role.
Consider an extreme case of situational influence: a person confronts you on the street with a gun
and demands your money. This represents obvious, external coercion but a normal person will
give over the money because they want safety. Of course, one may choose values out of
conformity but passively chosen values can still motivate action. Values differ in importance
within and between people and related to this, commitment to any given values varies along a
continuum. (Motivation, as we all know and will be noted later, can be conscious and/or
subconscious.)
Selected Concepts Related to Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation
Aristotle. Aristotle identified a distinction in his Nicomachean Ethics between pleasure
as an essential element of an activity and pleasure that stems from outside the activity (see
Schneider, 1996). Presumably he was referring to what we would call intrinsic and extrinsic
motivation though he did not use those terms.
Woodworth. As noted, Woodworth (1918) first used the terms intrinsic and extrinsic in
psychology. The former referred to an “activity running by its own drive” (p. 70) and the latter to
an activity “driven by some extrinsic motive” (p. 70). He postulated that an extrinsic motive
would focus attention on the reward and distract attention from and absorption in the activity,
making intrinsic motivation less likely. He also argued that motivation for an activity might
change during its pursuit in the sense that one might start for extrinsic reasons but find the main
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 5
pleasure in its pursuit later (for an overview see Rheinberg & Engeser, 2018). Woodworth did
not include achievement motivation in the discussion.
Bühler. Bühler (1922) differentiated pleasure from doing or creating something
(“Funktionslust” and “Schaffenslust”) and the pleasure of having completed an activity or by
gaining satisfaction from the result (“Endlust” or “Befriedigungslust”). The first implies intrinsic
motivation and the second implies achievement motivation but Bühler did not use these terms.
Drive Reduction Theories. Classical drive-reduction theories (e.g., Freud, 1952; Hull,
1943; Spence, 1956, 1960) argued that people were driven to reduce the discrepancy between a
desired (goal) state and their current (goal) state, which were energized by a general drive or
need deprivation. The reduction of this drive was perceived as pleasurable (for an overview see
Heckhausen, 2018). The modern version of this is control theory (see Carver & Scheier, 1981).
However, the primary goal of action cannot be simply to remove discrepancies between wants
and reality as such, which logically would have to include abandoning goals. Life and thriving
demand that (at least some) goals are actually attained. Therefore, discrepancy reduction can
only be a correlate of goal directed action, not its primary cause (cf. Bandura & Locke, 2003).
Moreover, focusing on discrepancy-reduction is misguided in another way: it implies that all
motivation is negative, that is, aimed psychologically at reducing pain or discomfort. Of course,
some motivation is negative, but it cannot be the whole story since obviously people must and do
work for positives (Bandura & Locke, 2003).
Self-Determination Theory. Deci and Ryan’s (2000) self-determination theory (SDT)
conceptualizes intrinsic motivation as inside the person but not as an interaction with an activity.
Furthermore, SDT explicitly confounds intrinsic motivation (enjoying the task) and achievement
motivation (pursuing goals and challenges), when describing intrinsic motivation as “the inherent
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 6
tendency to seek out novelty and challenges, to extend and exercise one's capacities, to explore,
and to learn” (Ryan & Deci, 2000, p. 70). To add further confusion, extrinsic motivation can be
inside or outside the self, depending on the degree of self-determination by which goals are
sought. Self-determined goals reflect ones value systems (integrated regulation) or one finds
them important (identified regulation). Less self-determined goals are chosen because one feels
an inner pressure to seek them (introjected regulation) or because they are based on external
demands (external regulation). The more self-determined one’s goals are, the more they satisfy
the three basic human needs for competence, relatedness, and autonomy. The theory includes
elements of determinism. Volition (autonomy) is viewed as a need, whereas we view it as an
axiom (Locke, 2018). Nor do we view money incentives as controlling as SDT often does. We
argue that people have the power, by thinking, to choose their own goals and that external
demands or incentives, in line with our previous definition of motivation, must work through the
value standards that one has chosen or accepted.
Means-Ends Fusion Theory. Kruglanski and colleagues (2018) introduced a theory not
based on human needs in contrast to SDT: the Means-Ends Fusion Theory (MEF). The theory
accepts the intrinsic-extrinsic dichotomy but posits that intrinsic motivation is a result of the
perceived fusion of the activity (means) to achieve a goal (end) with the goal itself. The authors
point out that any activity can be experienced as intrinsically or extrinsically motivated
depending on how similar the activity and the goal are perceived. They posit a continuum from
extrinsic (i.e., means and ends are completely separate) to intrinsic motivation (i.e., means and
ends are completely fused) rather than two qualitatively different types of motivation. For
example, the activity of running is perceived as very close to a fitness goal because running “is
subjectively experienced as (the attainment) of fitness” and therefore intrinsically motivated
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 7
(Kruglanski et al., 2018, p.167). But what this actually implies is that intrinsic and extrinsic
motivation can work together. The authors also claim that “the attainment of any goal is
satisfying by definition” (Kruglanski et al., 2018, p. 171). There is merit to this (e.g., goal setting
theory; Locke & Latham, 1990) but there are exceptions. Goals that do not integrate with one’s
underlying values and subconscious motive dispositions may lead to less emotional well being
(Brunstein, Schultheiss, & Grässmann, 1998), less life-satisfaction (Hofer, Busch, Bond, Kärtner,
Kiessling, & Law, 2010), and even psychosomatic symptoms (Baumann, Kaschel, & Kuhl,
2005).
The 3-C Model. This model is based on McClelland’s (1985) Motive Disposition
Theory. Kehr (2004) proposed that intrinsic motivation is enhanced when there is a
correspondence between implicit (unconscious) and explicit (conscious) motives. In other words,
when people enjoy what they find important at both levels, they will be intrinsically motivated,
which makes the pursuit of the activity less effortful because no volitional regulation (i.e., self-
regulation, will-power) is needed (Kehr, Strasser, & Paulus, 2018). Intrinsically motivated here
means motivated by ones implicit motives. The motivation type is actually classified in terms of
individual differences in the strength of three motives for achievement, power, and affiliation.
Each of the above models make some contribution in its own way but none of them make
a clear distinction between the three types of motivation which we propose. We argue that
intrinsic, achievement, and extrinsic motivation are three distinguishable forms of motivation.
There are particularly relevant at work, although we believe they are also relevant to other life
domains.
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 8
Intrinsic Motivation
We suggest confining the meaning of intrinsic motivation to liking or wanting an activity
for its own sake divorced from any specific outcome level. The enjoyment is in the doing; the
doing can range from passive to active. An example of the more passive form lies in the field of
art (defined broadly to encompass all of its forms, e.g., music, books, TV, movies, shows etc.)
where the pleasure is from contemplating or experiencing the art work. (For an explanation of
the critical, motivational role of art in life, see Rand, 1969). On the more active side, intrinsic
motivation can encompass hobbies, for example walking, cycling, gardening, cooking, games,
stamp collecting, etc. At work, active intrinsic motivation involves liking a certain type of work
or specific tasks, for example selling, analyzing, inventing, computing, programming,
coordinating, managing, experimenting, problem solving, or acting. It means liking a specific
type of job content, for example banking, sports, restaurant management, machining, repairing,
construction, law, medicine, teaching and subcategories within each (teaching math, etc.).
Traditionally, these would be called interests (which some researchers distinguish from intrinsic
motivation, e.g. Krapp, 1999). Although that term may seem emotionally mild, interests can
range from mild to passionate.
People can prefer different aspects of any activity and likings may come and go over
time. The goal in intrinsic motivation is pleasure and enjoyment from the action or experience. In
some form the activity makes one feel good, relaxed, happy, contented, and joyful. If activities
have a further goal than just the pleasure of engaging in them, then they are not purely intrinsic.
(We will talk about mixed motivation later).
We believe that it is an error to assume that simply because people like doing something
that they will necessarily attain a high level of skill at it. Pure intrinsic motivation involves doing
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 9
things for fun and the goal is not to improve as such. For example, the first author plays tennis
with other players his age. All of them have obvious weaknesses in their games. Not one of them
takes lessons in order to improve. For them the fun is in the playing and the exercise. They
definitely prefer winning but that is not their primary. While doing the same thing a lot may
improve skill up to a point, skill building and improvement is not the essence of intrinsic
motivation it is not the reason why one engages in the activity. To get to a high level of skill at
anything requires another form of motivation, achievement motivation, which unfortunately has
been almost universally confounded with intrinsic motivation (see Pinder’s 1998 discussion of
this literature).
Achievement Motivation
McClelland pioneered research on achievement motivation in the 1950s (McClelland,
Atkinson, Clark, & Lowell, 1953). He viewed its essence as competition with a standard, as a
striving for excellence in some form. The authors agree with this formulation. The issue is not the
doing or enjoyment of the activity as such but doing well and/or doing better than before, in
other words improvement. Note, however, that improvement, if it occurs, may be enjoyed but the
activity that leads to the improvement may or may not be. For example, a person may work hard
to succeed in the law profession but not really enjoy being a lawyer. Achievement motivation
can be conceptualized on different levels.
Dispositions and Traits. For McClelland, the achievement motive was subconscious
(implicit) and measured, as noted earlier, by projective tests, especially the Thematic
Apperception Test, which involved guided story telling based on pictures, which are then coded
for achievement imagery (e.g., Pang, 2010). He viewed it as most relevant to entrepreneurship
and it is in fact related to entrepreneurial occupational choice and performance (Collins, Hanges,
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 10
& Locke, 2004), though there are exceptions (Locke, 2015). Collins and colleagues also found
that conscious self-reported achievement motivation, based on trait scales, had the same effect.
However, the conscious and subconscious measures seem unrelated (no covariance, mediation or
moderation, Köllner & Schultheiss, 2014; Spangler, 1992).
Subconscious or implicit need for achievement has been found to be related to other types
of actions (e.g., Brunstein & Maier 2005; Spangler, 1992; Wegner & Teubel, 2014), particularly
after initial failure (Schattke, Taylor, Brandstätter, & Kehr, 2014). It may be related to
performance in professional service firms (cf. Spangler, Tikhomirov, Sotak, & Palrecha, 2014).
Quasi-traits. In recent years a quasi-trait approach has been proposed that includes what
are called learning and performance orientations (reviewed by Morisano, 2013). Both are
achievement related. Quasi traits are general but may also be framed in terms of specific
situations (e.g., learning in school). In a learning or mastery orientation (LO) the focus is on
acquiring new knowledge and skills, whereas performance orientation (PO) stresses doing well
on the task in some way.
There has been considerable confusion regarding PO because it has been measured in
quite different ways (Morisano, 2013) such as focus on success in order to impress others,
competition, or simply doing well, and the different conceptualizations can lead to very different
results. For example, impress through success goals can lead to setting very easy goals, which
would lead to low performance according to goal setting theory (Locke & Latham, 1990, 2013).
However, when the focus is on simply doing well, the combination of learning and performance
orientations seems to lead to the best performance, especially when the measures focus on an
“approach” rather than an “avoid” orientation, that is, seeking success rather than avoiding
failure (Morisano, 2013).
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 11
States. Conscious, state achievement motivation is task and situationally specific and is
most logically related to goal setting (Locke & Latham, 1990, 2013). In goal setting theory,
performance attainment is based on the type and level of task-specific goal that is set in addition
to various moderators (task knowledge, commitment, feedback). Performance is best if the
standard is specific and challenging and there is high commitment, relevant knowledge or skill,
and feedback showing progress in relation to the standard. Assigned goals work best if they are
accepted or committed to. This is most critical when goals are hard (Klein, Cooper, & Manahan,
2013). Satisfaction comes from goal success or the perception of progress toward one’s goal, not
just from taking action (Locke & Latham, 1990). Specific goals can be set for learning as a
means of improving task performance, in combination with performance goals, through
acquiring new or upgraded skills (Masuda, Locke, & Williams, 2015; Seijts, Latham, &
Woodwark, 2013). At a deeper level, performance improvement and goal achievement based on
one’s own efforts is a source of pride (Mento, Locke, & Klein, 1992). Achievement satisfaction
and pride, of course, give pleasure but the source is not just from action but action that leads to
attainment.
Morisano and colleagues (2010) found, contrary to the usual goal assumptions, that if
college students were made to reflect at length on their life goals in writing, their college
performance improved even if their list of goals did not include academics. This finding clearly
requires additional research studies.
Goals and goal orientations may be aroused through subconscious priming (Chen &
Latham, 2014; Latham, Brcic & Steinhauer, 2017). They work like conscious motivation but
regulate action separately (Friedman, 2013). However, there is mounting evidence that priming
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 12
effects have some conscious mediators. Latham and colleagues (2017) found that primed goal
difficulty was partly mediated by consciously, self-set goals.
Self-efficacy (Bandura, 1997), which is also a state theory, pertains to the confidence that
one can execute a course of action to bring about certain performance outcomes. Self-efficacy
plays critical and multiple roles in achievement related action. Aside from main effects, it affects
self-set goal level and goal commitment, it is affected by assigned goals, affects the response to
feedback, and leads to the use of better task strategies (Locke & Latham, 1990, 2013).
A note about flow (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990, 1996). The essential elements of flow appear
to be the experience, based on feedback from the current task that indicates smoothly running,
goal-directed progress on a valued activity and during which one loses track of time. The goal
must be clear so that one is not focused on other non-goal actions and one must feel up to the
challenge rather than being preoccupied with self-doubt. Csikszentmihalyi states that flow
appears particularly (but not exclusively) during autotelic activities, which people pursue for
the sake of doing it. In this sense, flow resembles intrinsic motivation and, consequently, Kehr
(2004, p.490) called flow “a special case of intrinsic motivation”, which also means that not
every intrinsically motivated or autotelic activity automatically leads to flow. Baumann and
Scheffer (2011) also see flow as a “state of intrinsic motivation in which people get fully
immersed in difficult tasks for the sake of the activity itself” (p. 267). In addition, these authors
proposed a “flow motive” as the disposition to seek flow in the achievement domain but others
have also discussed flow in contexts that are not achievement-related (Schiepe-Tiska & Engeser,
2012). Despite its clear relationship to intrinsic motivation, it is important to note that the
experience of flow shares many characteristics of achievement motivation such as clear goals
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 13
and goal progress, challenge, perceived skills, and feedback. These elements are all part of goal
setting theory (Locke & Latham, 1990) and also self-efficacy theory (Bandura, 1997).
Flow is a pleasant experience that does not occur all the time because in life everything
does not always go smoothly. Sometimes one fails, feels stymied, needs help, needs a new plan,
and has to persist in the face of failure, which all break the flow. All his requires self-efficacy,
goal commitment (Bandura, 1997; Locke & Latham, 1990, 2013), and often hard thinking. Flow
should be more likely to occur as one becomes more and more skilled on a task that one
performs repeatedly so long as boredom does not set in.
Extrinsic Motivation
There have been many confusions regarding extrinsic motivation. For example, it has
been tied into the demonization of money on the grounds that it causes problems because of the
capitalist system, (e.g., see Kasser, Cohn, Kanner & Ryan, 2007, and Locke’s, 2007, rebuttal).
Ironically, both Communists (Marxists), who are atheists, and religionists condemn money
making. The Communists claim that making a profit is evil (the exploitation theory) and
Christians claim that love of money is the root of all evil. (An exception was Calvinism, which
claimed that making money was proof of God’s grace).
Money has also been viewed (e.g., by Deci, 1975 and others) as controlling, an extension
of the idea of environmental determinism endemic to behaviorism. However, money cannot
control action unless one values it in a given circumstance in some way and the means of gaining
it. Money (specifically paper money) is only inanimate matter; it is a tool of exchange.
Consequently, it is not the money per se that motivates people but the value significance of the
money because nothing can motivate an empty organism. Further, people have the capacity to
choose their values (Locke, 2018b) and therefore money can have many different meanings for
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 14
people, which can be beneficial or detrimental for people’s psychological health (Thibault
Landry, Kindlein, Trépanier, Forest, Zigarmi, Houson, & Brodbeck, 2016). For example, money
can be seen as a means of gaining material goods, as a status symbol, as a means of supporting
one’s loved ones, as providing security for the future, as allowing freedom of choice in one’s
actions, as a backup in case of emergencies, as a way to relieve self-doubt and more (Srivastava,
Locke, & Bartol, 2001).
In psychology, the main focus has long been on what happens when money is given for
performing a task and is then withdrawn, aside from the fact there are a many moderators of this
phenomenon (e.g., Cerasoli, Nicklin, & Ford, 2014). (There have been bitter, conflicting meta-
analyses of this literature, which will not be discussed here, e.g. Eisenberger & Cameron, 1996;
Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999; Cameron, Banko, & Pierce, 2001; Cerasoli et al., 2014).
Obviously, in a real work setting you do not get paid and then have it suddenly withdrawn.
Failing to attain a bonus can be discouraging but this does not eliminate one’s salary unless one
quits the job. Employees do not work for nothing. Not getting paid would lead to a very high
turnover rate! The fundamental issue, as some have acknowledged (Thibault Landry, Gagné,
Forest, Guerrero, Séguin, & Papachristopoulos, 2017) is how to design effective incentive
systems, which is a very complex matter (Weibel, Wiemann, & Osterloh, 2014).
We believe that research on the motivational effects of money has been far too narrowly
focused, not only with respect to the issue of money itself, but with respect to the issue of
extrinsic motivation in general. So what would be the best way to define extrinsic motivation?
We would define extrinsic motivation as involving means-ends relationships; it is doing
something in order to get some future value (or avoid some future disvalue). Because humans
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 15
need to live long range, acting for the future is not only not inferior to other forms of motivation,
as is sometimes suggested, but is essential to happiness and survival (e.g., see Locke, 2018a).
A critical point needs emphasis here: extrinsic does not mean outside the person but
rather outside the task; it pertains to what value a chosen activity can lead to (Rheinberg &
Engeser, 2018). Of course, achieving goals can lead to future benefits but the core of
achievement motivation is the pleasure one gets from task performance, improvement, or
mastery.
Because so much human action, to varying degrees, is taken as a means to an end, it
encompasses any number of action-outcome possibilities (i.e., instrumentality) relationships.
These include, for example, choice of food and drink (what makes your body function well and
free of damage), physical activity (exercise), health management, (e.g., brushing one’s teeth,
getting checkups, taking medication), psychological counseling (increased self-esteem, lowered
depression and anxiety), education (gaining knowledge and skill), saving and investment
(financial security), developing friendships and romantic relationships, having and raising
children, pursuing a career, etc.
We argue that money itself needs to be seen in a much broader framework than worrying
simply about whether, under some circumstances, it is harmful to intrinsic motivation. It is said
that money cannot buy happiness, but to an extent it can (Diener & Biswas-Diener, 2008; Smith,
2003). The former found that income, up to a certain level, was related to life happiness both
within and between cultures. Smith points out, that humans need material goods to survive and
money is one’s means of getting them. Further, money buys time, which means that one can
avoid things one likes doing less and spend more time doing things one likes more. Money
supports autonomy in the sense that it allows more choices in life. Money expands options.
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 16
Money makes life less precarious. Money provides resources for creating wealth through trade.
Honest wealth creation is a spiritual process in the sense that it requires the best use of one’s
rational faculty (Locke, 2008).
Srivastava and colleagues (2001) found in two studies that there were many self-reported
motives for seeking money. Subjective well-being was associated with wanting money for:
security, family support, getting just compensation for efforts, pride, leisure activities, starting
one’s own business, and supporting favorite charities. These “positive motives as a group were
related positively to subjective well-being among both student and entrepreneurial samples. Two
“negative” motives, social comparisons and alleviation of self-doubt as a group were negatively
related to subjective well-being in both samples. The rated importance of money was not directly
related to subjective well-being but was correlated with negative motives. Thus, it matters what
you seek money for and why you seek it. (See, for example, Rand’s famous money speech, e.g.,
“[Honestly earned] money is the product of virtue, but it will not give you virtue and will not
redeem your vices”; Rand, 1957, p. 384).
The money issue does not end there. In life, including career selection, job hunting and
job selection (when the job could possibly be gotten) or is offered, there are virtually always
trade-offs between money and other values, for examples, location, commuting, housing,
opportunities for learning and self-development, job security, the organization’s reputation,
company culture, family considerations (spouse’s career, children’s schooling) travel, life style,
saving, stress, hours of work, etc. It is grossly misleading to say that one is controlled by the
money offeredpeople make choices that involve money and other values based on their
personal value hierarchy virtually every day and have the power to determine and change that
hierarchy. Therefore, controlled by money” can only mean that it is very near the top of one’s
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 17
value hierarchy and unaffected by other values or moral principles. In reality, monetary values
can always be weighed against non-monetary values (e.g., self-esteem, moral character, romantic
love). Furthermore, character can affect one’s ability to earn money. To the degree that one is
pro: reason, honest, independent in thinking, productive in work, and pro-justice, one’s earning
potential is enhanced (Locke, 2008).
Table 1. Summary of the Trichotomy of Motivation
Intrinsic
Motivation
Achievement
Motivation
Extrinsic
Motivation
Short
Definition
Liking or wanting an
activity for its own
sake.
Recurrent concern for
a standard of
excellence.
Doing something in
order to get some
future value.
Core aspect
Enjoyment
Achievement
Outcomes
Related goals
Pleasure form the
experience
Meeting a standard,
improvement, skill
building
Attainment of valued
outcomes
Locus of
incentive
Inside the activity,
the pursuit of action
In the improvement,
in the challenge
Outside the activity,
in the consequences
Affective
reaction
Happiness during the
pursuit of action
Pride in achievement,
success, or progress
Satisfaction with
outcome
Example of a
salesman
Enjoying interacting
with others.
Improving one’s
sales’ rate.
Valuing the good
salary.
Example of
fencing
Enjoyment of one’s
movement, elegance,
and exercise.
Precise execution of
moves. Challenging
opponent.
Training to win a
bout or a tournament.
Example of
learning a
foreign
language
Enjoyment of
learning. Having fun
at expressing oneself
differently.
Improving one’s skill
level. Personal
development. Liking
the challenge.
Learning for a new
job. Relate better to
others. Requirement
for a study program.
Applications
in
Organizations
Reflect likes and
dislikes. Affective
task visualization.
Match task with
interests.
Clear performance
standards. Give
feedback. Encourage
Learning from
mistakes.
Make goals
meaningful. Link
goals to company
vision. Recognize
merits.
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 18
Extrinsic motivation, when it leads people to think, plan, and act long range, should help
to prevent or lower stress and put one on the road to life happiness insofar as that is in one’s
control. Of course, there are caveats. If one’s value choices are based on impressing others or
overcoming self-doubt by showing off (Locke & Kenner, 2016), one has surrendered control of
one’s life and may experience a life of not so quiet desperation. Table 1 summarizes our
trichotomy of motivation.
We end this section with a trick question: which of the three types of motivation is best?
We think the most likely answer is: an integration among all three tied into one’s idealized life
style. What could be better than loving the work (and other things) that you are doing, doing it
well by a rational, personally relevant standard, and gaining long term life benefits as a result of
your efforts and choices? Although the three types can be identified and discussed separately and
can vary independently, they can relate to one another in various ways. Below we suggest
possible relationships.
Interrelationships between Types
Mutual facilitation. Intrinsic motivation does not guarantee high achievement but it may
contribute to it. People who like an activity are usually anxious to spend more time on it and that
may facilitate some skill development. Achievement motivation, at the same time, may facilitate
intrinsic motivation because people tend to like things they are good at. Bandura (1997) has
noted that higher self-efficacy can lead to higher intrinsic motivation. Achievement motivation
can tie into extrinsic motivation. By propelling people to perform at a high level, it increases the
chances that one will be successful in some form, which may help them make money and gain its
associated rewards and benefits. Circling back, money can provide leisure to purse activities that
one loves but which do not bring income (e.g., travel). Completing the circle, intrinsically
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 19
motivated activities can provide ideas for turning the activity into a money-making business
which then involves achievement.
Compensation. Sometime people do not have and perhaps cannot get careers or jobs that
they love, or even like. Nevertheless, they can try to do a good job (achieve some excellence)
and take pride in it. It is often said that many or most lawyers hate their careers. But they can get
some satisfaction (happiness) and pride in being good lawyers. Further, they can make enough
money to allow them to pursue other ends in their leisure time. Thus, achievement and extrinsic
motivation can compensate to a degree for the lack of intrinsic motivation. Going in the other
direction, people can accept less achievement and lower salaries to give them more time to
pursue things they love doing, for example the lawyer who loves painting. The fun of painting
can help make up for the lack of pleasure in the other realms.
Conflict. The compensation issue implies that people can be in conflict about their wants.
They may want a high salary plus lots of free time but cannot get both. The usual reason for such
conflicts is conflicting value hierarchies; they want A and B but cannot have both and do not
want to give up either. This can be caused by family obligations: they need money for the
children’s education but can only get it by working 70 hours a week and giving up painting.
Conflict resolutions of this type require hard (creative) thinking and communication with
significant others who are involved, directly or indirectly. It usually means rethinking one’s
priorities. The existence of possible interrelationships between the three types of motivation is
shown in Figure 1.
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 20
Figure 1 Possible interrelationships between the different types of motivation.
Practical Applications for Organizations
How, specifically, could organizations apply these concepts?
Intrinsic Motivation. Look for employees who like or think they would like the type of
work available in your organization. Explain vividly the main tasks involved in the job. Give
work samples. Find out what previous experiences they have had that are similar to the open job.
Encourage affect-focused visualization of task and goal pursuits related to the intended job (Job
& Brandstätter, 2009). Place them in a job that matches their interests. Give them variety in order
to let new interests develop. Encourage them to grow the job around themselves. Make them
aware of new task opportunities. Encourage them to reflect on their likes and dislikes.
Achievement motivation. Make clear that there are standards of performance. Make
those transparent. Provide sufficient means to attain them. Support the required skill
development. Goals can be self-set (within limits) and/or be assigned. Fun and games in
Intrinsic Motivation
Liking or wanting an
activity for its own sake
Achievement Motivation
Competition with a
standard of excellence
Extrinsic Motivation
Doing something in order
to get some future value
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 21
organizations have to lead to results. Provide objective feedback about attainments or progress.
Encourage learning from mistakes. Goals in real life settings should be challenging but not
impossible (see Locke & Latham, 1990, Appendix D guidelines). The definition of challenge
should reflect business conditions, employee skills, and organizational resources. Stretch goals
do not have to be fully reached, but can be assigned to stimulate creative thinking (Kerr &
Lepelley, 2013). The appropriate time span depends on the context. Proximal goals can be tied to
distal (longer term) goals. Group (mutually shared) goals are effective (Kramer, Thayer & Salas,
2013) especially if they motivate knowledge exchange and intellectual stimulation. Build self-
and team-efficacy through training. Use learning goals when employees need to acquire new
skills (Seijts et al., 2013), though, it may be possible to combine them with performance goals
(Masuda et al., 2015).
Extrinsic Motivation. Give employees opportunities for self-development. Help them
upgrade their skills. Allow autonomy commensurate with trust in skill and ethics. Make goals as
meaningful as possible and provide a rationale for imposed goals (Güntert, 2015). Emphasize a
higher purpose by relating goals to the vision of the organization (Rawolle, Kehr, & Glaser,
2008). In addition to market sensitive base pay, recognize merit based on fair and objective
judgements (e.g., see Bock, 2015). Facilitate career progress. Encourage and support retirement
and life planning. Try to help coordinate the job with family responsibilities (e.g., flexible
schedules, work at home policies).
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 22
A Recommended Research Program
Frequently when presenting a conceptual paper, scholars are urged to formulate a theory
consisting of propositions deduced from other theories. We have chosen not to follow this
tradition. The first author has been a long-time fan of the inductive method, which means
building theory from accumulated evidence rather than building a theory in advance (cf. Locke,
2007; Locke & Latham, 1990). In our view, premature theorizing cuts off the discovery process.
In science there is an enormous amount of trial and error; if one knew what was going to be
discovered, no experiments would be needed at all. Trial and error can be guided by a
hypothesis, but a hypothesis does not necessarily have to be guided by a wider theory. There can
even be studies done with no hypotheses. The impetus can be: “Let’s see what will happen if I do
this.” In science, we need to try stuff in order to identify conditions and processes. Moreover,
trying will lead to new and creative ideas, albeit inevitably also to some failures, which should be
viewed as learning experiences.
Our view of replication is that exact replication of single studies, though useful, is
insufficient by itself (Locke, 2015). If you replicate one study 100 times, you still have only one
study. To be able to generalize across tasks, subjects, procedures, measures, and time you need
deliberate replication with variation (e.g., see Locke & Latham, 1990). This allows the discovery
of moderators (interactions) and causal mediators.
We recommend a research program on the three types of motivation described. It would start
simply with question asking. Here are some suggestions:
What are some concrete examples of the different type of motivation based on people’s
personal experiences? What are some intrinsically pleasurable activities that you have
experienced? What are examples of times you have attained excellence or succeeded in
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 23
reaching performance goals? What things do you do mainly for future benefits? What
types of attainments are most important to you?
Could questionnaires be constructed to measure people’s experiences in each category?
Should the questions be general or tied to specific life domains (e.g., work, family)?
Should both types of scales be developed?
Should there be attempts to use projective as well as conscious, self-report measures in
these realms? If so, which types?
How do scores on each type of scale correlate with various outcomes (e.g., life, family,
financial, career attainments, self-esteem and satisfaction)?
Does it matter how people weight the three types of motivation? Are there individual
differences?
How can or do the three types of motivation combine? Is the best job, career, or life when
one gets pleasure from all three types or are there exceptions?
Do people experience conflicts between the types? If so, why? How do people try to
reconcile them? How does that work?
Do some types compensate for failings in other types? How do people process this? Are
their different profiles that lead to the same outcome (e.g., life satisfaction)?
Have we overlooked other influential types of motivation (e.g., power lust)?
This is by no means a complete list. We do not suggest an a priori theory as we do not
want or can anticipate the research people will come up with. This is what inductive research is
all about (Locke, 2007).
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 24
Conclusion
Intrinsic motivation has often been inappropriately glorified, seen as somehow superior
(even morally superior) whereas extrinsic motivation, specifically wanting to make money, has
often been inappropriately demonized in the popular and scientific literature. Furthermore, the
debate about intrinsic and extrinsic motivation has almost entirely ignored a third, crucial type,
namely achievement motivation, which has often been confused with intrinsic motivation. The
concepts have also suffered from definitional problems. We suggest that all three types of
motivation are independent, yet related concepts (all are sources of pleasure) that can mutually
facilitate, compensate, or be in conflict with one another. We have proposed some guiding
questions for a future research program and we hope that this will spark the discussion about the
three types of motivation, their differences and communalities. In the end, a personally satisfying
combination of intrinsic, achievement, and extrinsic motivation, we predict, is optimal for a
fulfilled and happy life.
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 25
References
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.
Bandura, A., & Locke, E. A. (2003). Negative self-efficacy and goal effects revisited. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 88(1), 8799. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.88.1.87
Baumann, N., Kaschel, R., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Striving for unwanted goals: Stress-dependent
discrepancies between explicit and implicit achievement motives reduce subjective well-
being and increase psychosomatic symptoms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
89(5), 781799. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.5.781
Baumann, N., & Scheffer, D. (2011). Seeking flow in the achievement domain: The achievement
flow motive behind flow experience. Motivation and Emotion, 35(3), 267284.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-010-9195-4
Bock, L. (2015). Work rules. New York: Twelve (Grand Central).
Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Grässmann, R. (1998). Personal goals and emotional well-
being: The moderating role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 75, 494508. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.2.494
Brunstein, J. C., & Maier, G. W. (2005). Implicit and self-attributed motives to achieve: Two
separate but interacting needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(2), 205
222. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.2.205
Bühler, K. (1922). Die geistige Entwicklung des Kindes [Mental development of the child] (3rd
ed.). Jena, Germany: Fischer.
Cameron, J., Banko, K. M., & Pierce, W. D. (2001). Pervasive negative effects of rewards on
intrinsic motivation: The myth continues. The Behavior Analyst, 24(1), 144.
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03392017
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 26
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and Self-Regulation: A Control-Theory
Approach to Human Behavior. New York: Springer.
Cerasoli, C. P., Nicklin, J. M., & Ford, M. T. (2014). Intrinsic motivation and extrinsic
incentives jointly predict performance: a 40-year meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,
140(4), 9801008. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035661
Chen, X., & Latham, G. P. (2014). The effect of priming learning vs. performance goals on a
complex task. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 125(2), 8897.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2014.06.004
Collins, C. J., Hanges, P. J., & Locke, E. A. (2004). The Relationship of Achievement
Motivation to Entrepreneurial Behavior: A Meta-Analysis. Human Performance, 17(1), 95
117. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327043HUP1701_5
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow. New York: Harper Collins.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity. New York: Harper Collins.
Deci, E. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments
examining the effects of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin,
125(6), 627668. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.627
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The 'what' and 'why' of goal pursuits: Human needs and the
self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11(4), 227268.
https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01
Diener, E. & Biswas-Diener, R. (2008) Happiness. Malden, MA.: Blackwell.
Eisenberger, R., & Cameron, J. (1996). Detrimental effects of reward: Reality or myth?
American Psychologist, 51(11), 11531166. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.51.11.1153
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 27
Freud, S. (1952). Triebe und Triebschicksale [Drives and drive destiny] (GW, Bd. X, 1915).
Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Fischer.
Friedman, S. (2013). Priming subconscious goals. In E. Locke & G, Latham (Eds.), New
developments in goal setting and task performance. New York: Routledge.
Güntert, S. T. (2015). The impact of work design, autonomy support, and strategy on employee
outcomes: A differentiated perspective on self-determination at work. Motivation and
Emotion, 39(1), 7487. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-014-9412-7
Heckhausen, H. (2018). Historical Trends in Motivation Research. In J. Heckhausen & H.
Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (3rd ed., 15-65). Berlin: Springer.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-65094-4_2
Heckhausen, J., & Heckhausen, H. (Eds.). (2018). Motivation and action (3rd ed.). Berlin:
Springer.
Hofer, J., Busch, H., Bond, M. H., Kärtner, J., Kiessling, F., & Law, R. (2010). Is self-
determined functioning a universal prerequisite for motive goal congruence? Examining the
domain of achievement in three cultures. Journal of Personality, 78(2), 747780.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2010.00632.x
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Job, V., & Brandstätter, V. (2009). Get a taste of your goals: Promoting motive-goal congruence
through affect-focus goal fantasy. Journal of Personality, 77(5), 15271560.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00591.x
Kasser, T., Cohn, S., Kanner, A. D., & Ryan, R. M. (2007). Some Costs of American Corporate
Capitalism: A Psychological Exploration of Value and Goal Conflicts. Psychological
Inquiry, 18(1), 122. https://doi.org/10.1080/10478400701386579
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 28
Kehr, H. M. (2004). Integrating implicit motives, explicit motives, and perceived abilities: The
compensatory model of work motivation und volition. Academy of Management Review,
29(3), 479499. https://doi.org/10.2307/20159055
Kehr, H. M., Strasser, M., & Paulus, A. (2018). Motivation and Volition in the Workplace. In J.
Heckhausen & H. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (3rd ed., 819-852). Berlin:
Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-65094-4_19
Kerr, S. & Lepelley, D. (2013). Stretch goals: Risks, and best practices. In E. A. Locke & G. P.
Latham (Eds.), New development in goal setting and task performance. New York:
Routledge.
Klein, H. J., Cooper, J. T. & Monahan, C. (2013). Goal commitment. In E. A. Locke & G. P.
Latham (Eds.), New developments in goal setting and task performance. New York:
Routledge.
Krapp, A. (1999). Intrinsische Lernmotivation und Interesse: Forschungsansätze und
konzeptuelle Überlegungen [Intrinsic motivation to learn and interest: Research Approaches
and Conceptual Considerations]. Zeitschrift für Pädagogik, 45, 387406.
Köllner, M. G., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2014). Meta-analytic evidence of low convergence
between implicit and explicit measures of the needs for achievement, affiliation, and power.
Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 826. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00826
Kramer, W. S., Thayer, A. L. & Salas, E. (2013). Goal setting in teams. In E. A. Locke & G. P.
Latham (Eds.), New developments in goal setting and task performance. New York:
Routledge.
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 29
Kruglanski, A. W., Fishbach, A., Woolley, K., Bélanger, J. J., Chernikova, M., Molinario, E., &
Pierro, A. (2018). A structural model of intrinsic motivation: On the psychology of means-
ends fusion. Psychological Review, 125(2), 165182. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000095
Latham, G. P., Brcic, J., & Steinhauer, A. (2017). Toward an Integration of Goal Setting Theory
and the Automaticity Model. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 66(1), 2548.
https://doi.org/10.1111/apps.12087
Locke, E. A. (2007). What is Capitalism? Some Comments on Kasser, Cohn, Kanner, and Ryan.
Psychological Inquiry, 18(1), 3842. https://doi.org/10.1080/10478400701386603
Locke, E. A. (2008). The prime movers: Traits of the great wealth creators. Ayn Rand
Bookstore.
Locke, E. A. (2015). Theory building, replication, and behavioral priming. Perspectives On
Psychological Science, 10(3), 408414. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614567231
Locke, E. A. (2018a). Long range thinking. In G. Oettingen (Ed.), The psychology of thinking
about the future. New York: Guilford Press.
Locke, E. A. (2018b). The illusion of determinism: Why free will is real and causal. Edwin A.
Locke.
Locke, E. A. & Kenner, E. (2016). Burnout and the battle for your own happiness. In A-S
Antoniou & C. L. Cooper (Eds.), Coping, personality and the workplace. Burlington, VT:
Gower.
Locke, E. A. & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Locke, E. A. & Latham, G. P. (2013). New developments in goal setting and task performance.
New York: Routledge.
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 30
Masuda, A. D., Locke, E. A., & Williams, K. J. (2015). The effects of simultaneous learning and
performance goals on performance: An inductive exploration. Journal of Cognitive
Psychology, 27(1), 3752. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2014.982128
McClelland, D. C. (1985). How motives, skills, and values determine what people do. American
Psychologist, 40(7), 812825. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.40.7.812
McClelland, D. C. (1987). Human Motivation. New York: Cambridge University Press.
McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., & Lowell, E. L. (1953). The achievement
motive. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Mento, A. J., Locke, E. A., & Klein, H. J. (1992). Relationship of goal level to valence and
instrumentality. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77(4), 395405.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.77.4.395
Morisano, D. (2013). Goal setting in the academic arena. In E. A. Locke & G. P. Latham (Eds.),
New developments in goal setting and task performance. New York: Routledge.
Morisano, D., Hirsh, J. B., Peterson, J. B., Pihl, R. O., & Shore, B. M. (2010). Setting,
elaborating, and reflecting on personal goals improves academic performance. The Journal
of Applied Psychology, 95(2), 255264. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018478
Pang, J. S. (2010). Content coding methods in implicit motive assessment: Standards of
measurement and best practices for the picture story exercise. In O. C. Schultheiss & J. C.
Brunstein (Eds.), Implicit motives (pp. 119150). Oxford: University Press.
Pinder, C. C. (1998). Work motivation in organizational behavior. Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Prentice Hall
Rand, A. (1957). Atlas shrugged. New York: Signet
Rand, A. (1969). The romantic manifesto. New York: World
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 31
Rawolle, M., Kehr, H. M., & Glaser, J. (2008). Why self-set goals may sometimes be non-
motivating. In C. Wankel (Ed.), 21st century management. A reference handbook. Thousand
Oaks, CA: Sage.
Rheinberg, F., & Engeser, S. (2018). Intrinsic Motivation and Flow. In J. Heckhausen & H.
Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and action (3rd ed., pp. 579622). Berlin: Springer.
Rheinberg, F., & Vollmeyer, R. (2018). Motivation (9th ed.). Stuttgart, Germany: Kohlhammer.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic
motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 6878.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68
Schattke, K., Brandstätter, V., Taylor, G., & Kehr, H. M. (2014). Flow on the rocks: Motive-
incentive congruence enhances flow in rock climbing. International Journal of Sport
Psychology, 45(6), 603620. https://doi.org/10.7352/IJSP2014.45.603
Schiepe-Tiska, A., & Engeser, S. (2012). Flow in Nonachievement Situations. In S. Engeser
(Ed.), Advances in flow research (pp. 87107). New York: Springer.
https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-2359-1_5
Schneider, K. (1996). Intrinsisch (autotelisch) motiviertes Verhalten dargestellt an den
Beispielen des Neugierverhaltens sowie verwandter Verhaltenssysteme (Spielen und
leistungsmotiviertes Handeln). In J. Kuhl & H. Heckhausen (Eds.), Enzyklopädie der
Psychologie: C/IV/4. Motivation, Volition und Handlung (pp. 119153). Göttingen,
Germany: Hogrefe.
Schultheiss, O. C., Kordik, A., Kullmann, J. S., Rawolle, M., & Rösch, A. G. (2009). Motivation
as a natural linchpin between person and situation. Journal of Research in Personality,
43(2), 268269. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2008.12.018
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 32
Seijts, G. H., Latham, G. P., & Woodwark (2013). Learning goals: A qualitative and quantitative
review. In E. A. Locke & G. P. Latham (Eds.), New development in goal setting and task
performance. New York: Routledge.
Smith, T. (2003). Money can buy happiness. Reason papers. Summer, 7-20.
Spangler, W. D. (1992). Validity of questionnaire and TAT measures of need achievement: Two
meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1). https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.140
Spangler, W. D., Tikhomirov, A., Sotak, K. L., & Palrecha, R. (2014). Leader motive profiles in
eight types of organizations. The Leadership Quarterly, 25(6), 10801094.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2014.10.001
Spence, K. W. (1956). Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Spence, K. W. (1960). Behavior Theory and Learning: Selected Papers. Englewood Cliffs:
Prentice-Hall, Inc.
Srivastava, A., Locke, E. A., & Bartol, K. M. (2001). Money and subjective well-being: It's not
the money, it's the motives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(6), 959971.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.959
Thibault Landry, A., Gagné, M., Forest, J., Guerrero, S., Séguin, M., & Papachristopoulos, K.
(2017). The Relation Between Financial Incentives, Motivation, and Performance. Journal
of Personnel Psychology, 16(2), 6176. https://doi.org/10.1027/1866-5888/a000182
Thibault Landry, A., Kindlein, J., Trépanier, S.-G., Forest, J., Zigarmi, D., Houson, D., &
Brodbeck, F. C. (2016). Why individuals want money is what matters: Using self-
determination theory to explain the differential relationship between motives for making
money and employee psychological health. Motivation and Emotion, 40(2).
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-015-9532-8
Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation 33
Wegner, M., & Teubel, T. (2014). The implicit achievement motive predicts match performances
and the explicit motive predicts choices for target distances in team sports. International
Journal of Sport Psychology, 45(6), 621638. https://doi.org/10.7352/IJSP2014.45.621
Weibel, A., Wiemann, M., & Osterloh, M. (2014). A behavioral economics perspective on the
Overjustification Effect. In M. Gagné (Ed.), Oxford handbook of work engagement,
motivation, and self-determination theory. New York: Oxford.
Woodworth, R. S. (1918). Dynamic psychology. New York: Columbia.
... Koch, 1956;White, 1959). Intrinsic motivation is described as involving internal, personal factors such as enjoyment, interest and attaining a sense of accomplishment (Locke & Schattke, 2019). Students who are intrinsically motivated engage in an activity because they find the activity itself rewarding. ...
... Students who are extrinsically motivated undertake an activity in order to gain something or to avoid being punished. The focus in extrinsic motivation is on external outcomes rather than genuine interest in the activity itself (Locke & Schattke, 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
The positive impacts of parental involvement on academic achievement and student motivation are well established. However, little is known about the association between parental involvement and academic passion, particularly in an online learning context. This study investigates the causal relationship between parental involvement, academic passion, and motivation for learning English online among 270 students in grades 7–11 in Oman. We also examine the role of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation as mediating variables in this association and explore gender differences. Results indicate that parental involvement significantly promotes both intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, enhancing academic passion, study habits and English language grades. Notably, males exhibited higher extrinsic motivation and academic passion, while females demonstrated superior English proficiency and longer study hours. These findings highlight the critical role of parental engagement in online education and suggest the need for gender-specific educational strategies and the development of initiatives that promote parental involvement.
... Practitioners may have an occasional experiences of themselves beyond their fluctuating thoughts, feelings, and sensations, however, for this to stabilise, there is much to purify. If however there is an growing shift from an external to an internal source of well-being [194,195], individuals can feel less dependent on life events needing to go to plan to maintain well-being. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
The journey to actualise our full potential is a complex one but there is research to suggest it is marked with signs along the way. Following an investigation of the literature, a theoretical framework made up of five major stages and their markers is proposed. It is suggested that in Stage 1, the conventional sense of self, well-being pivots on the experiences of pleasure and pain. In Stage 2 (Mindfulness) a new level of nonjudgmental objectivity develops where thoughts can be seen as passing mental events. With practice, Stage 3 awakening events may occur, leading to an interruption of the sense of a continuous self. Over time, these intermittent states may stabilise, leaving a default of the ongoing awakened state of Stage 4, an altruistic, peaceful resting state with no discernible sense of self, and increased functionality. Finally, after a seemingly infinite amount of practice, the rare uninterrupted blissful state of perfected enlightenment, along with unlimited capacities, occurs (Stage 5). With the increase now in evidence-based mindfulness research, we are able to evaluate the psychology of enlightenment and its associated stages. If we can translate methods and outcomes into psychological language and find common ground with western concepts of happiness and human potential, we may find antidotes to our mental health inquiry, that can raise the bar from coping to contentment.
... Typically, people in a confused or frustrated state encounter a deadlock in decision-making (Molina et al., 2023). On the contrary, research has established a link between fear of failure, boredom, anxiety, and challenges directing people to achieve certain goals (Bakker et al., 2020;Locke & Schattke, 2019), aligning with the Flow Theory (Nakamura & Csikszentmihalyi, 2009), which posits that optimal engagement occurs when task difficulty matches skill level. In a similar context, competition was linked to negative emotions such as anger, fear/afraid/insecurity, sadness/not excited, and disgust/dislike (Sekhavat, 2019). ...
Article
Full-text available
Persuasive games, designed and tailored to change users' behavior, have enhanced user experience across various domains. However, their role in emotion regulation remains understudied, despite the increasingly immersive environments they create that trigger emotional responses. Sensors have emerged as key artificial intelligence (AI) tools in persuasive games supporting emotion regulation compared to other AI technologies, such as chatbots. Yet the effectiveness, challenges , and applications of these sensors lack comprehensive exploration. Our scoping review analyzed 32 articles published from 2013 to 2023 on persuasive games using AI-based sensing technologies for emotion regulation. Results from 27 articles (84.8%) reporting the effectiveness of sensors revealed mostly large to moderate effects (d ≥ 0.8 ≤ 0.4). Common challenges with integrated sensors (camera systems, facial recognition, smartwatches, and altimetric pressure sensors) included emotion detection accuracy and ethical concerns. We provide design recommendations for developing ethically sound and effective persuasive games for emotion regulation in the future.
... This type of motivation refers to the motivation within an individual. This type of motivation is driven by personal interests, values, and a sense of enjoyment or satisfaction derived from the activity (Locke & Schattke, 2019). Educational motivation, which includes the desire to learn agriculture, having an inquisitive mindset, satisfaction with the teaching methods in agriculture and fisheries, and alignment with the current educational track and career and job aspirations, can become intrinsic motivations when they align with an individual's interests and passions. ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite the critical role of agriculture and fisheries in the Philippines' economy and food security, enrolment in related educational programs remains low compared to more commercially popular fields. Our study explores the motivational factors influencing Filipino senior high school students' decisions to pursue higher education degrees in agriculture and fisheries. We employed an explanatory sequential mixed-method design, beginning with a cross-sectional survey followed by interviews. The study used multi-stage random sampling, including 907 respondents and 12 interviewees. The quantitative data was collected with questionnaires, and qualitative information was collected through interview guides. The findings reveal three main types of motivation impacting educational choices: intrinsic motivation, linked to personal interest and satisfaction; extrinsic motivation, influenced by family and perceived career prospects; and amotivation, driven by misconceptions about career potential and societal devaluation of these fields. Recommendations are presented for improving the visibility and value of agriculture and fisheries programs through enhanced educational strategies.
... Intrinsic motivation is the enjoyment of a specific task because the activity itself brings one pleasure (traditionally referred to as interests, see Locke & Schattke, 2019). Presumably, those who find a task enjoyable or interesting are more motivated to perform well. ...
Article
Full-text available
The present study examined whether individual differences in memory self-efficacy (MSE)-one's perception and evaluation of their memory abilities-predict actual learning and memory ability among younger adults. Across three studies, participants completed a delayed free recall task along with measures of working memory, episodic long-term memory (LTM), task-specific motivation, strategy use, and strategy knowledge. Study 1 focused on global MSE (perceived general memory ability across various memory domains), whereas Studies 2 and 3 assessed concurrent MSE (perceived current memory ability for a specific task one is about to complete). Overall, results revealed concurrent MSE, but not global MSE, consistently correlated with delayed free recall accuracy (our index of learning ability). Individuals who believed they were better capable of learning the free recall task tended to perform better on the task, and the strength of this relationship increased with task experience. Both concurrent MSE and overall learning were positively associated with working memory, broad episodic LTM abilities, motivation to perform well, and the use of more effective encoding strategies. Critically, concurrent MSE (assessed pre-task) did not explain unique variance in learning when these additional variables were accounted for. Taken altogether, the present study suggests that among younger adults, efficacy beliefs can reliably predict learning, so long as these beliefs consider contextual features and specific task demands. That said, the efficacy-performance relationship appears to be largely driven by associations with other meaningful "third" variables, particularly broader cognitive abilities like working memory and general episodic LTM.
... There are several authors who have adopted the classic definition of intrinsic motivation based on Woodworth, one of which is Locke and Schattke (2018). According to Locke and Schattke (2018), intrinsic motivation is about liking or wanting an activity for its own sake, enjoying pleasure with the aim of enjoying the experience. The word intrinsic job satisfaction according to Garg et al, (2018), is when employees understand the type of work they do, the tasks that make up their job. ...
Article
Employee turnover has been viewed as a severe issue, notably in the field of human resources management and has been feared by organizations. This study aimed to see the affect of the role of motivation, job insecurity as well as job satisfaction and work engagement on turnover intentions towards Private Bank Frontline Employees (PBFE) which simultaneously has not been widely discussed in Indonesia, especially in the Banten area. Therefore, the contribution of this research is expected to add theoretical to the human resources literature and reduce the occurrence of turnover intentions. Referring to empirical theory and the results of previous studies related to the role of motivation and job insecurity towards turnover intentions, this study was designed by distributing the questionnaires online as a survey method for collecting data. Factor analysis and Lisrel’s Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) were used for analysis. The findings of this research showed that the factor that has affected job satisfaction and work engagement is intrinsic motivation however the one that could significantly affect turnover intentions is job insecurity.
... One advantage is the ability of this strategy to increase student motivation [38,44] by allowing students to learn in a novel and totally experiential way [46,99]. Strategies that give a central role to the learner have a positive impact on extrinsic motivation while having the potential to generate interest and appreciation for topics that might initially be abstract or unfamiliar [100]. At the same time, these active practices contribute to counteracting the eminently theoretical character with which Social Sciences are still associated and the consequent disinterest of students in these subjects [101,102]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Heritage education has been marked over the years using expository methodologies, as well as by a heritage education that hardly considers intangible assets. In the face of these limitations, there are continuous calls for a transformation of heritage education, which requires significant changes in teacher training. This transformation requires the incorporation of active methodologies that increase student participation, stimulate their interest and facilitate the understanding of heritage from an integrative perspective. In this context, the results of a didactic intervention that implements a role-playing strategy with teachers in training are presented. The objective is to validate the potential of this didactic strategy in heritage education. Specifically, the activity addresses the current social problems related to the loss of traditional crafts, as a representative part of intangible heritage. For this purpose, five situations were selected for the students to represent after a group research process: old Mesta cattle breeders, cork extractors, charcoal burners, the problem of large landowners and the possibilities of rural tourism for the knowledge of intangible heritage. Eighty-seven interviews were conducted to learn about their perception of the methodology. The results show that role-playing is an effective strategy for learning about heritage. It proved to be a motivating and effective strategy for understanding complex historical issues, as well as for developing the capacity for reflection and critical thinking.
... This type of motivation is driven by external factors such as praise, money, or recognition. Locke and Schattke (2019) describe intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. They define intrinsic motivation as "liking or wanting an activity for its own sake divorced from any specific outcome level" (p. ...
Article
Full-text available
Despite ongoing efforts towards gender equity, the gender gap in STEM (Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics) remains significant today. This article explores the motivations and perceptions of women in different professional roles within STEM fields regarding the importance of mentoring in fostering interest and participation in STEM careers, thus contributing to continuing engineering education. Based on qualitative data from 19 semi-structured interviews with women in managerial, research, teaching, and external academic and professional roles, the study delves into their motivations for pursuing STEM careers, their interest in promoting diversity, and the role of mentoring in supporting their professional development. The thematic analysis results are grouped into a hierarchical structure comprising one meta-theme, four primary, and six subthemes. The participants emphasized that their primary motivation for STEM involvement was contributing to society and promoting economic growth. Additionally, they advocated for greater diversity and challenged traditional gender roles in these areas. The participants highlighted the importance of closing the gender gap and recognizing the capabilities and new perspectives that women brought. Although these women faced obstacles such as glass ceilings, having a mentorship opportunity was identified as a critical tool for women’s empowerment and training. The insights contribute to advancing strategies for promoting gender equity and diversity in STEM fields, with implications for researchers, universities, and organizations seeking to support women’s participation and advancement in STEM careers. Further research is recommended to explore the perspectives of women in other roles and the effectiveness of mentoring programs in fostering gender diversity in STEM.
... Y por último la libertad e iniciativa de una persona cuando realiza una actividad sin presión externa con respecto a sus objetivos personales se relaciona con la necesidad de autonomía (Ryan y Deci, 2000). Estudios previos, (Locke y Schattke, 2019;Medina, 2022) diferenciación entre motivación extrínseca e intrínseca. Esta última se relaciona con la propia satisfacción y genera interés, iniciativa o curiosidad por aprender. ...
Chapter
Full-text available
Los estudiantes universitarios muestran poca motivación en sus estudios, quizá parte se deba al medio social en el que vivimos, al avance de las tecnologías y la facilidad para alcanzar lo deseado sin necesidad de demasiado esfuerzo. Por ello, en este estudio se busca conocer más sobre la opinión, que los estudiantes del Grado en Turismo tienen sobre las asignaturas de contabilidad y de esta manera reflexionar sobre qué acciones se pueden realizar para fomentar y mejorar el proceso enseñanza-aprendizaje. La metodología se desarrolla en tres subsecciones. Primero se habla del diseño, para ello se divide el estudio realizado a los estudiantes en dos partes, el análisis cualitativo con entrevistas e imágenes proporcionadas por ellos y el análisis cuantitativo, con un cuestionario. La investigación reveló características comunes en sus opiniones, en un porcentaje elevado de alumnos, en relación con la asignatura. Las principales conclusiones a las que se ha llegado, según la investigación y resultados obtenidos han sido las siguientes: 1. Un cambio en la metodología, con la incorporación de herramientas informáticas, podría aumentar la atención del alumno, al relacionar estás prácticas con las realizadas en las empresas. Mejorando el aprendizaje de estos y aumentando su motivación 2. Se confirmó el bajo interés de los estudiantes en la asignatura, al considerar la misma poco importante en su futuro. 3. Se resalta la importancia en la relación directa entre docente y alumno, para alcanzar una buena motivación. 4. La cooperación como herramienta del aprendizaje para la mejora de su rendimiento. 5. Con respecto a la metodología aplicada, se puede decir que es fundamental para el éxito del alumno, pudiéndose conocer propuestas concretas para mejorar los métodos empleados al impartir la asignatura
Article
Introduction: This research aims to investigate the influence of motivation on employee performance at the Indomaret retail company. As a leading company in the retail industry in Indonesia, employee performance at Indomaret is an important factor in maintaining competitiveness and meeting dynamic market demands. The concept of motivation, which has long been a focus of attention in the human resource management literature, plays an important role in shaping employee behavior and productivity. Methods: Data collection was carried out offline by distributing questionnaires to Indomaret employees in Malang. The number of research samples used purposive techniques and a sample of 32 samples was obtained. The research data analysis method uses simple linear regression, then the research instrument is tested using product moment for validity testing and Cronbach's alpha for reliability testing. Results: The results of this research show that work motivation has a positive effect on employee performance. Conclusion and suggestion: It is hoped that the results of this research can provide concrete input for Indomaret management in designing more effective employee motivation policies. It is also hoped that these findings can contribute to human resource management literature, especially in the context of the retail industry in Indonesia. In addition, it is hoped that this research can pave the way for further, more in-depth research regarding motivation strategies that can improve employee performance in the competitive retail industry.
Article
Full-text available
Two studies examined the importance of motive dispositions in determining the extent to which the pursuit of personal goals accounts for interindividual differences in emotional well-being. Within the domains of agency and communion, motives were assessed with a picture–story test, whereas self-report measures were used to assess goal attributes. Study 1 found that progress toward motive-congruent goals, in contrast to progress toward motive-incongruent goals, accounted for students’ daily experiences of emotional well-being. Study 2 found that the combination of high commitment to and high attainability of motive-congruent goals predicted an increase in students’ emotional well-being over 1 semester. In contrast, high commitment to motive-incongruent goals predicted a decline in emotional well-being. Results are discussed with reference to a 2-system approach to human motivation.
Chapter
Full-text available
The performance of an activity can have positive incentives per se and individuals may engage in an activity purely for the enjoyment of it. The engagement due to the enjoyment of an activity is often called intrinsic motivation. Beside this understanding of intrinsic mo-tivation other conceptions are presented (self-determination, experience of competence, interest and involvement, mean-end-correspondence, learning-goal orientation). In doing so, the problem became evident, that the term intrinsic motivation refers to different, even conflicting conceptions. With the “Extended Cognitive Model of Motivation” different as-pects of motivation are theoretically integrated. Instead of using the term intrinsic motiva-tion, we use the term activity-related motivation. Qualitative and quantitative ways to measure activity-related incentives are outlined. Finally we present an intensively studied activity-related incentive, i.e. the experience of flow.
Article
Full-text available
The term intrinsic motivation refers to an activity being seen as its own end. Accordingly, we conceptualize intrinsic motivation (IM) as (perceived) means-ends fusion and define an intrinsicality continuum reflecting the degree to which such fusion is experienced. Our Means-Ends Fusion (MEF) theory assumes four major antecedents of activity-goal fusion: (1) Repeated pairing of the activity and the goal, (2) Uniqueness of the activity-goal connection, (3) Perceived similarity between the activity and its goal, and (4) temporal immediacy of goal attainment following the activity. MEF theory further identifies two major consequences of the activity-goal fusion (i.e., manifestations of intrinsic motivation): (1) Perceived instrumentality of the activity to goal attainment and consequent activity engagement; (2) goal-related affective experience of the activity. Empirical evidence for MEF theory comes from diverse fields of psychological inquiry, including animal learning, brain research, and social cognition.
Article
Available at: https://www.reasonpapers.com/archives/
Chapter
Work motivation is an important determinant of the success of individuals, teams, and organizations. Researchers and professionals are therefore interested in motivation as a key variable that can explain performance, commitment, and work satisfaction. This chapter provides an introduction to classic theories of employee motivation and current applied research. Finally, the chapter will introduce the 3C-model of motivation as a framework for the comprehensive classification of the determinants of employee motivation.
Chapter
This chapter provides an overview of the various different historical branches of research on motivation. In general, research on motivation can be divided into four conceptually different approaches based on the problems they address: first, volitional approaches which conceptualize volition as externally caused (heterogenetic position) or internally driven (autogenetic position) and examine them phenomenologically or experimentally; second, approaches of instinct theory, which describes the content of motivation with more or less comprehensive lists of instincts and tries to assess motivational processes with concepts of behavioral ethology such as innate causal mechanisms; third, approaches of personality theory, which can be distinguished based on whether their orientation lies in motivational, cognitive, or personality theory; and, lastly, the approaches of association theory which are divided into approaches based on learning or activation.