ArticlePDF Available

"Toxic Masculinity” in the Age of #MeToo: Ritual, Morality, and Gender Archetypes Across Cultures’.

Authors:
  • Centre intégré universitaire de santé et de services sociaux de l'Ouest-de-l'Île-de-Montréal.

Abstract

Purpose This paper aims to take the “toxic masculinity” (TM) trope as a starting point to examine recent cultural shifts in common assumptions about gender, morality and relations between the sexes. TM is a transculturally widespread archetype or moral trope about the kind of man one should not be. Design/methodology/approach The author revisits his earlier fieldwork on transnational sexualities against a broader analysis of the historical, ethnographic and evolutionary record. The author describes the broad cross-cultural recurrence of similar ideal types of men and women (good and bad) and the rituals through which they are culturally encouraged and avoided. Findings The author argues that the TM trope is normatively useful if and only if it is presented alongside a nuanced spectrum of other gender archetypes (positive and negative) and discussed in the context of human universality and evolved complementariness between the sexes. Social implications The author concludes by discussing stoic virtue models for the initiation of boys and argues that they are compatible with the normative commitments of inclusive societies that recognize gender fluidity along the biological sex spectrum. Originality/value The author makes a case for the importance of strong gender roles and the rites and rituals through which they are cultivated as an antidote to current moral panics about oppression and victimhood.
Society and Business Review
“Toxic Masculinity” in the age of #MeToo: ritual, morality and gender archetypes
across cultures
Samuel Paul Louis Veissière,
Article information:
To cite this document:
Samuel Paul Louis Veissière, (2018) "“Toxic Masculinity” in the age of #MeToo: ritual, morality
and gender archetypes across cultures", Society and Business Review, https://doi.org/10.1108/
SBR-07-2018-0070
Permanent link to this document:
https://doi.org/10.1108/SBR-07-2018-0070
Downloaded on: 12 October 2018, At: 12:06 (PT)
References: this document contains references to 72 other documents.
To copy this document: permissions@emeraldinsight.com
Access to this document was granted through an Emerald subscription provided by
Token:Eprints:HREMDWVKJ5TVDXKDCAJ7:
For Authors
If you would like to write for this, or any other Emerald publication, then please use our Emerald
for Authors service information about how to choose which publication to write for and submission
guidelines are available for all. Please visit www.emeraldinsight.com/authors for more information.
About Emerald www.emeraldinsight.com
Emerald is a global publisher linking research and practice to the benefit of society. The company
manages a portfolio of more than 290 journals and over 2,350 books and book series volumes, as
well as providing an extensive range of online products and additional customer resources and
services.
Emerald is both COUNTER 4 and TRANSFER compliant. The organization is a partner of the
Committee on Publication Ethics (COPE) and also works with Portico and the LOCKSS initiative for
digital archive preservation.
*Related content and download information correct at time of download.
Downloaded by McGill University At 12:06 12 October 2018 (PT)
Toxic Masculinityin the age of
#MeToo: ritual, morality and
gender archetypes across cultures
Samuel Paul Louis Veissière
Division of Social, Transcultural Psychiatry, Culture, Mind, and Brain Program,
McGill University, Montreal, Canada
Abstract
Purpose This paper aims to take the toxic masculinity(TM) trope as a starting point to examine recent
cultural shifts in common assumptions about gender, morality and relations between the sexes. TM is a
transculturally widespread archetype or moral trope about the kind of man one should not be.
Design/methodology/approach The author revisits his earlier eldwork on transnational sexualities
against a broader analysis of the historical, ethnographic and evolutionary record. The author describes the
broad cross-cultural recurrence of similar ideal types of men and women (good and bad) and the rituals
through which they are culturally encouraged and avoided.
Findings The author argues that the TM trope is normatively useful if and only if it is presented alongside
a nuanced spectrum of other gender archetypes (positive and negative) and discussed in the context of human
universality and evolved complementariness between the sexes.
Social implications The author concludes by discussing stoic virtue models for the initiation of boys
and argues that they are compatible with the normative commitments of inclusive societies that recognize
gender uidity along the biological sex spectrum.
Originality/value The author makes a case for the importance of strong gender roles and the rites and
rituals through which they are cultivated as an antidote to current moral panics about oppression and
victimhood.
Keywords Morality, Masculinity, Archetypes, Ritual, Sex and gender, Virtue-signalling
Paper type Research paper
1. Introduction: in and out of sex work
In the introduction to his ethnography of drug-dealing in Spanish Harlem, Bourgois (2003)
jokes that he fell into crack by accident. I used to joke that I fell into sex work by accident.
From 2006 to 2009, I conducted ethnographic eldwork in the streets of Salvador da Bahia in
Brazil. The way I tell the story now, I was studying social ontology, resilience, and emerging
modes of sociality among street kids and sex workers. I did not use that language then. I had
been trained in political anthropology and postcolonial theory, and I wanted to learn how
people facing extreme marginalization (oppressionwas the word I used them) learned to
make sense of the social, cultural, economic and political structures in which they were
embedded. The Field turned out to offer many surprises not all of which were pleasant (see
Veissière, 2009,2010). I never found the noble victims and bad perpetrators I had been
looking for. I did nd people who were almost invariably more competent than I was at
making critical sense of and moving within structures that still did not make sense to me,
despite (or because of) the esoteric social science concepts I had been equipped with. I also
found that my informants aimed at and often attained a different kind of social mobility than
what I had expected. The strange book that came out of my rst Brazil cycle[1], thus,
Ritual,
morality, and
gender
archetypes
Received 10 July2018
Accepted 20 July2018
Society and Business Review
© Emerald Publishing Limited
1746-5680
DOI 10.1108/SBR-07-2018-0070
The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available on Emerald Insight at:
www.emeraldinsight.com/1746-5680.htm
Downloaded by McGill University At 12:06 12 October 2018 (PT)
describes the lives of poor black women and their attempt to escape the lowest ranks of a
pigmentocratic, highly stratied society by marrying foreign gringotourists (Veissière,
2011).
The transactions I observed did not look like sex work in the typically patterned sense of
the term. The bar patio seduction rituals were no more explicitly performative than other
kinds of effortful social interactions anywhere (such as job interviews or meeting ones in-
laws). Seduction rituals, when well executed, must produce the impression of not being
seduction rituals. In the transnational Brazilian bar version of the ritual, gender roles are
reinforced by being underplayed. The woman saves face by never explicitly asking for
money, and the man saves face by never explicitly offering to pay. The women who were
most successful at this game were those who were most adept at picking men who could
also play the game well. The women who were best at signalling that they were not like
those other women who go after gringoswere best at nding the men who were good at
signalling that they were not like those other gringos.
The stories that I documented, my own included, did not always end well. From the
womens lives in Europe (most of my informants ended up in or had returned from Italy,
Germany, France and Spain), I collected many stories of dreary small towns, bigoted
families, horny priests and brothers-in-law, evil-eyed mothers-in-law, miscarriages,
deportations and the occasional truck-stop brothel. Save for my own, I left most of the mens
stories out of my ethnography. There were some happy ones. But the bad stories (of which
there were more) showcased recurring themes of estrangement from suspicious families and
departed children and a downward curve towards nancial, social and physical ruin.
It took some effort to disentangle myself from sex work. As the post-colonial theory
which I had laboured over in my juvenilia was morphing into mandatory anti-oppression
trainingon college campuses, I found myself weary of returning to predictably scripted
arguments about gender, victimhood and sex trafcking[2]at academic conferences
arguments which, as a white man (by, 2014 a cis white man; by, 2016, a cis-hetero white-
presenting man), I was now told lacked the experiential authorityto comment on. As I
moved on from postmodern autoethnography to third-person cognitive anthropology, I
turned to more general questions about the psychological underpinnings and embodied
effects of culture, sociality and ritual behaviour (Ramstead et al., 2016;Veissière, 2016a,
2018).
These general questions and varied approaches from the masculine stories I left out of
my rst-person ethnography to third-person generalization of evolutionary anthropology
inform about my efforts to make sense of the current crisis in gender relations. They also
assist me in examining the meta-ethical foundations of gender normativities across cultures.
In the end, what I learned from these stories also assists me in making tentative
recommendations on the kinds of gendered rituals, rites and archetypes that might work
best for the human species.
1.1 Masculinity in 2018: Whats in a camping trip?
The stories we tell ourselves are often more normative that descriptive. They usually tell of
how the world should be according to our cultural norms. They are also performative. Stories
have strange ways of bringing the world they describe into being (Hacking, 2002). When
stories change, so do our innermost assumptions and experiences.
In a recent class discussion on the experience of bliss in nature, my anthropology
students shared stories of their favourite childhood moments. A young American woman in
her early 20s smiled as she evoked memories of fatherdaughter camping trips. This yearly
event, she recounted, had been something of a family ritual deemed worthy of its own name.
SBR
Downloaded by McGill University At 12:06 12 October 2018 (PT)
Picking up on an awkward pause in the class conversation, she commented with a hint of
irony that in todays age, the sight of a father and daughter sharing a small tent would
seem a little creepy.
That comment gave me pause to wonder. The intuitive ease with which the father and
daughter concepts could be associated with creepmark the widespread normalization of a
rapid cultural shift in common assumptions about gender, sexuality and social interactions
at large. This much, the cultural anthropologist in me could recognize. This shift is now
described by social scientists as being concurrent with a growing culture of victimhood, and
an emphasis on adverse mental health outcomesstemming from processes of oppression
along with increasingly pervasive social justiceand intersectionalmoral culture on
north American college and university campuses (Campbell and Manning, 2014;Lilienfeld,
2017;Haidt, 2017). Commenting on these shifts at the level of gender ideology, cultural critic
Laura Kipnis (2017), elicited a erce controversy by pointing out that the emphasis on
womens victimhood and mens aggression resembled the kind of patriarchal narrative that
rst- and second-wave feminists had sought to overturn. Kipnis remarked that as she was
coming of age in the 1970s, she had been culturally primed to associate female sexuality
with freedom and liberation. By her account, sex in the late 2010s is now primarily
associated with risk, passive, vulnerable women and predatory men.
As a man and father of two boys, another part of me this one, closer to the heart felt
more troubled by my studentscomments. I thought of my young boys and wondered what
it might be like to be discovering ones manhood in a culture that actively preaches against
toxic masculinity(TM); a culture, to be precise, that overwhelming associates masculinity
with risk, violence and an inner essence tainted with sexual aggression. I also knew (the
evolutionary anthropologist in me now) that these associations were not entirely unfounded.
1.2 Stereotypes and archetypes in cognition and culture
The human mind is not well-equipped to reason about to counter-intuitive facts that violate
our expectations. Counter-intuitive facts tend to produce strong responses. They will be
dismissed as nonsense when they depart too far from common stories (Boyd and Richerson,
1998), and will be highly attention-grabbing and memorable when they combine common
stories in unusual ways (Boyer, 2008). Such facts can elicit automatic responses and even
violence when they contradict the moral core of implicit social norms (Greene and Haidt,
2002;Haidt, 2012).
Our expectations are heavily modulated by cultural norms. These are norms we all know
and obey, often without knowing that we know them (Ramstead et al.,2016). In a culture
where one version of feminism has become an obligatory moral norm (Kipnis, 2017),
pointing out that men fare much worse than women in many indicators of well-being (see for
example, Bilsker and White, 2011) is likely to be interpreted as misogynist. Any talk of
mens issues is also likely to be read as a call for victimhood. In this version of the
victimhood story, some men get to claim that women are the realoppressors. It is both
interesting and alarming to note that competition for victim status is found on both sides of
gender equality debate (Campbell and Manning, 2014).
Stereotypes about genders and other categories of persons are found in all cultures.
Those stereotypes have likely been around for as long as we have been symbolic species
(Hrdy, 2011). In general, stereotypes co-evolved (in cognition and culture) for the purpose of
informational efciency. They describe, however crudely, patterns of behaviours and
statistical regularities in the world that can be gured out with no explicit instruction. To
cognize, in other words, is to categorize (Harnad, 2017). Babies recognize patterns in the
world, and they form mental templates to organize information into stereotypes.
Ritual,
morality, and
gender
archetypes
Downloaded by McGill University At 12:06 12 October 2018 (PT)
A stereotypy is also the hallmark of patterned behaviour, in both animal and cultural forms
of life (Boyer and Liénard, 2006).
Stereotyped behaviour along sexed lines in the animal kingdom looks roughly like
stereotyped gendered behaviour among humans. This is why, in addition to roughly similar
gender archetypes found across cultures, we know that human behaviour and the cultural
patterning of behaviour are rooted in our evolved biology (Eagly and Wood, 1990).
Stereotypes can also be plain wrong. Because the human mind is not good at handling
complexity, it tends to simplify the world and infer patterns where there are none. This is
why we are prone to gamblers fallacy, superstition and conspiracy theories (Swami et al.,
2014). Cultural groups need to produce efcient stories to promote and enforce social norms
that are good for the survival of group. Lay people call this kind of story-telling morality.
Psychologists call this as rule-governed behaviour (Schmidt et al.,2011).
All cultures actively promote different stereotypes and ideal types. Some cultural stories
are nuanced and others less so. Some are simplied to the point of promoting conspiracy
theories. Archetypes that promote crowd madness, witch-hunts, public hangings and
genocides are like mind viruses. The human minds vulnerability to zero-in on single actors
and intentional culprits and its propensity to give in to collective pressure has been
recognized for a long time. Take, for example, an old story from the Talmud: When a man
is unanimously condemned to death, he must be released at once.
1.3 Gender archetypes as normative ethics
Gender archetypes usually describe worst-case and best-case ideal types of men and women.
The archetypes propagate and promote moral stories about the kinds of men and women we
should and should not be. Across cultures and throughout histories, these archetypes have
proven to be highly similar. For a cultural anthropological view on morality across cultures,
(see Keane, 2015); for a psychological, evolutionary view (see Tomasello, 2016); and for a
biocultural view on sexand gender roles, (see Fuentes, 2015).
The worst-case ideal type for men is usually too aggressive, selsh and not caring
enough. The worst-case ideal type for women is usually coddling and manipulative (Eagly
and Wood, 1999). Similarly, cultures do not differ very much in where the best-case ideal
type should t on this spectrum (Schmitt et al., 2008).
The best-case archetype for a man is usually strong, protecting and generous. The ideal
woman is usually beautiful, caring and generous. There is broader cultural difference in the
next ideal sub-type, but the general picture looks something like this: traditionally, mens
role in care-giving is to protect the family. In childrearing, men usually toughen up children
and socialize them to face the challenges of the outside world. Women typically attend to
minute needs that men are not very good at noticing. Both boys and girls are generally
recognized to need masculine types to toughen them and feminine types to make them more
delicate and attentive. Evolutionary and cross-cultural research has shown that fathers
usually favour rough-and-tumble play over ne-motor subtle play (Hrdy, 2011;Lamb, 2000;
Lamb and Goldberg, 1982). Universally, most cultures have devised some version of the
childfather camping or hunting trip ritual.
All cultures recognize the complementarities of men and women, from the anatomical
complementarity that makes coitus and reproduction possible, to the complementary ways
in which men and women use their strengths to help keep the species alive. The spectrum of
masculine and feminine ideal types is also universally recognized as porous (Fuentes, 2015).
Men can indeed, by most accounts, should embody some feminine traits, and women can
and should be masculine in some domains. All cultures, thus, have an archetype for
effeminate men and masculine women. Some cultures, like the Amerindian berdache type
SBR
Downloaded by McGill University At 12:06 12 October 2018 (PT)
(Trexler, 1999, the Filipino bakla type (Manalansan, 2003) type, or the Brazilian travesti type
(Kulick, 1997) have fully accepted social roles that oppositescan integrate. Many cultures
identify very masculine women and very effeminate men as unbalanced, or negative types
(Trexler, 1999).
Sexual preference does not always correlate with the sex-and-gender spectrum.
Warrior cultures that promoted aggressive men types, like Ancient Greece or Japan, also
permitted and encouraged homoeroticism and man-on-man love (Trexler, 1999). Many
cultures have permitted homoeroticism and love-making among men without any
implication of homosexuality. In most of Melanesia and Polynesia, boy-on-boy love was
tolerated as a normal part of transgressive child-play (Mead, 1939). In this cultural
package, boy-on-boy love was discouraged once one became a seriousmarried person. In
some parts of Brazil, the act of activelypenetrating men can be seen as a sign of hyper-
masculinity the homosexualrole in such instances is reserved for the passiveman
who becomes feminized through penetration (Kulick, 1997;Hecht, 2006). This kind of
macho homoeroticism is also found, albeit in a more covert way, in many athletic, gang
and prison cultures that promote aggressive, hyper-masculinemale archetypes
(Lancaster, 2005).
1.4 Negative gender archetypes across cultures
The risk of over-aggression in males and over-nurturing in women is recognized in all
societies. The same is true of social cluelessness in males and social manipulation in women
(Stoller and Herdt, 1982). Robust brain and cross-cultural psychological ndings support the
view that male and female behaviour traits are normally distributed along biological lines.
Males are more aggressive and impulsive on average and not as good as females at paying
attention to other peoples needs (Soutschek et al.,2017;Rand et al., 2016;Buss, 1995;Schmitt
et al., 2008). As in all normal distribution curves, there is a considerable amount of overlap
and individual differences.
Cultural differences in masculine and feminine types often mirror the idealized
archetypes elevated as moral models in different societies. Philosopher Ian Hacking calls
this phenomenon looping effects: human biology, experience and personality traits can be
malleable and tend to t the stories we tell ourselves to make sense of the world (Hacking,
2007). One, however, should remain attentive to the strong limits of this malleability. One
might think of cultural malleability as a spectacular add on, rather than the founding core
of symbolically-enriched human biology. This nal point bears retelling and helps clear the
muddied conceptual waters surrounding the contemporary archetype of toxic masculinity.
I address these epistemological contradictions in the next sections.
1.4.1 Virtue or virtue-signalling? the sacred and the taboo in evolutionary perspective. In
his introduction to psychologist Jordan Petersons (2018) controversial book on 12 Rules for
Life, the Canadian psychiatrist Norman Doidge raised concerns about the deep, invisible
contradiction that underpins the current culture of social justice on university campuses.
On the one hand, Doidge pointed out, most young people schooled after the 1990s have
been taught that all morality is relative, and that everything from sex to power to success is
socially constructed. This leaves little room for identifying evolutionarily stable patterns of
behaviour and places the locus of or the blame for”–all problems on nebulous social
forces(e.g. colonialism) that appear to possess intentions of their own. In more extreme,
conspiracy-like versions of this story, the blame, agency and intentions are placed on
malec, usually white male single social actors.
The next set of contradictions is harder to discern. It takes a more minute conceptual look
to notice that the intersectional worldview moves from:
Ritual,
morality, and
gender
archetypes
Downloaded by McGill University At 12:06 12 October 2018 (PT)
Ultra-constructivist ontological commitments (all behaviours are the result of
socialization);
to a radically individualist, standpointepistemology (Harding, 2004) granted to
some, but not all social actors (reported rst-person experience is the ultimate
authority on all matters of truth and justice, but if and only if one speaks from the
position of the oppressed); and
universalist ethical commitments to the undoingof gender and most social norms,
to be led by those who hold marginalized standpoints.
In spite of its relativist claims, thus, the culture of social justice actively promotes one kind of
highly rigid normative morality about power, sex and human relations. This moral story is
then grounded in highly stereotyped models and ideal types of identity. In this model,
gender and ethnic identities are typically cast on the same plane of social construction, but
gender and not ethnicityis seen a violentpatriarchal imposition that must be undone.
Ethnicity, in turn, is described as a virtuous marker of colonial victimhood that must be
valourized and accommodated but not undone. All those who are not identied with the cis-
heteronormative white supremacist patriarchy, then, get to leverage their disempowered
identity status as a marker of virtue.
Doidge calls this model virtue-signallingand sharply contrasts it with the Aristotelian
model of virtue ethics, which emphasizes depth of moral character (MacIntyre, 2013). From
an anthropological perspective, virtue-signalling is found in all cultures as a form of
reputational management (Tomasello, 2008). In any given culture and context, one must
invariably signal how one abides by and excels at the terms of social norms that regulate
good social and moral standing (Leary and Kowalski, 1990).
Psychologists have pointed out that social norms that regulate virtue aim to promote a
symbolic ideal of purity while marking those who do not play by the rules as polluted
(Haidt, 2012). Across cultures, things, substances, persons, ideas and practices that are
deemed sacred typically signal purity, while taboos signal pollution (Douglas, 2003). The
sacred and the taboo, nally, tend to elicit the most automatic, often virulent responses. While
the sacred, by denition, cannot be questioned or desecrated, the taboo cannot be touched or
defended. When individuals or ideas are symbolically marked as taboo, they typically elicit
strong disgust responses from purity-seekers. It is pertinent to note that disgust-sensitivity
responses, which recruit subcortical brain structures like the insula and amygdala, are
evolutionarily ancient mechanisms optimized to deal with threats and poisons in the
environment. The presence of taboo among Homo sapiens, in turn, activates disgust systems
by applying a poison-detection formula to symbolic stimulus (Phillips et al.,1997;Rozin et al.,
2009;Tybur et al.,2013). While disgust-sensitivity is a normal brain function among
vertebrates (particularly mammals and humans), some individuals and cultural groups are
known to be more sensitive than others. Conservatives, for example, have often been shown
to score higher in disgust-sensitivity that liberals (Haidt, 2012) A recent study, however,
found that those who espoused authoritarian left-wing views of political correctness scored
were as high as conservatives in disgust-sensitivity (Haidt, 2012;Brophy, 2015).
This amicable cultural separation from biology, which accounts for the symbolic
activation of most deep-brain functions, has often been described favourably to account for
humanitys unique evolutionary successover other species. An important addition to this
cultural intelligence hypothesis(Henrich, 2015), however, could cast this mechanism as a
potential problem.One might term this the cultural stupidity hypothesis.
This evolutionary detour helped me re-ground my argument in a general meta-ethics of
interpersonal relations: people invariably form intuitive moral judgements and
SBR
Downloaded by McGill University At 12:06 12 October 2018 (PT)
performatively manage theirreputations around ideas of purityand pollution consistent with
those of their time and sub-culture. How different social norms capture and enforce this
spectrum, thus, will largely determine the experience of different classes of people marked as
a clean or dirty.
If masculinity is increasingly marked as a disgust-eliciting taboo in the new moral
culture, we should now examine the TM trope in broader historical, cross-cultural and
evolutionary contexts.
1.4.2 Toxic masculinity in comparative focus. Toxic masculinityis a highly salient but
awkwardly-tting feature in the conceptual architecture of the social justicemoral culture
I discussed above. In the grander scheme of human structures of myth, TM is simply a
worst-case ideal type, i.e. a fairy tale with some basis in biology and broad cross-cultural
relevance. The TM myth serves the useful purpose of promoting socially desirable
behaviour among males: men should not be bullies, and men should not rape. What healthy
mind would disagree with that?
In the current scheme of twenty-rst century social justice mythology, the TM story is
also the master archetype in an archetype-hungry culture that pretends not to use
archetypes.
Invoking the Toxic Femininity(TF for shorts, a worst-case female ideal type) in a
twenty-rst century discussion is not likely to be well received. TF archetypes, however, are
also universal. When psychoanalysis still dominated the psychological science scene, a slew
of children personality traits from autism to introversion were routinely blamed on a
spectrum of feminine bad-mother types, from hysterical, castrating harpies to refrigerator
mothers(Severson et al.,2008).
In spite of the demise of TF tropes in public culture, female gender archetypes still
permeate everyday cultural assumptions on many levels. Many female professors, for
example, report that their male students are culturally ill prepared to respect female
pedagogical authority. In this argument, most men and boys are said to perceive women
leaders through a binary archetypical lens in which one can either be a sexy girlfriendor a
bitchy mother(Hay, 2015, for a discussion). It may be, then, that many boys do not know
how to interact with and perceive women outside of these cultural templates.
I suggest that current Ur-socioconstructivist gender ideology (which provides no clear
bearings on what to expect from and how to behave towards other sexes) perpetuates, rather
than addresses this problem. On an extreme end of this misgendering spectrum, confusing
expectations and an obscurantist denial of biology may also accentuate tensions
surrounding so-called rape culture. While the act of rape constitutes a universally
recognizable wrongdoing, what I call rape culture-culturepresents a contradictory and,
ultimately, unexplained story on the aetiology of male sexual aggression. As Doidge points
out, the advent of third-wave feminism which denies masculine and feminine essences
outside of socialization processes (Butler, 2011)has now sacralized the denial of nature on
the one hand and the invisible imbalance of essentialising men only for their worst-case
traits.
If we take cross-culturally recurrent tropes about good persons as a reliable indicator of
moral wealth, the TM story certainly has a place in the human record. A normative ethics of
gender relations, thus, may begin with a recognition of biology. From this lens, an analysis
of the distribution of sex-based selected traits would undeniably lead to the conclusion that
men do require strong cultural nurturing to balance their aggression, temper their
domination and cultivate protecting roles. This is an old evolutionary story. On average,
fatherchild interaction is almost entirely absent among our closest cousins the great apes
(Hrdy, 2011), but a rough outline of healthy masculinityis found in the primate record.
Ritual,
morality, and
gender
archetypes
Downloaded by McGill University At 12:06 12 October 2018 (PT)
Some chimpanzee males, thus, have been observed to step up and occasionally rescue, adopt
and raise orphaned baby chimps on their own (Boesch et al.,2010).
Successfully securing paternal investment an essential predictor of quality offspring in
humans has been and remains an important biocultural challenge for human females.
There is always a strong risk that men, after impregnating women, will take off forever into
their cultural equivalent of the long hunting trip and binge-drinking session. This is also
why, on average, human females have evolved to be uniquely attractive to males and signal
their attractiveness in highly effective ritual ways. Contrary to popular belief in, 2018,
women across cultures are much more likely than men to use their attractiveness and
genetic status to gain socioeconomic mobility (Henrich et al.,2012). This is a phenomenon I
routinely observed in the context of my eldwork in Brazil (Veissière, 2011). For men of low
social status, genetic status (in the form of health and attractiveness) matter little. In highly
stratied societies, low-status males are most often excluded from reproductive rites and
rituals, as high-status males monopolize the high-quality females. This is particularly true in
polygamous, polygynous societies. Predictably, societies that produce packs of sex-starved,
mate-less, purposeless males run into a lot of trouble (Veissière, 2011). This is when the
camping-drinking sessions run out of hand. All cultures, thus, have produced stories about
the tragicomedy of this challenge. The bachelor party rite found among Anglo-Saxons is
likely an important ritual way to keep bidding farewell tothis evolutionary problem.
A common worst-case female archetype across cultures, thus, warns against the femme
fatale who secures social success through her attractiveness and sexual favours and leads
men to social, nancial and emotional ruin (Forouzan and Cooke, 2005;Hanson and ORawe,
2010). In the traditional mens huts of the Amazon and Papua New Guinea, ethnographers
report that common talk among men centres on the awe-inspiring, terrifying power of the
all-swallowing vagina. Gossip from the female hut usually centres on the gullibility of
baby like men who think with their penis (Rival, 2011). This is the cultural equivalent of
the men-in-the-sauna and women-at-the-hair-salon rituals.
Like the TM story, these TF stories serve an important social purpose. In the pragmatic,
bio-culturally-based meta-ethics of gender relation I am advocating here, both TM and TF
stories should be told.In turn, the good versions of both stories must be actively promoted.
It may be for this reason that cultures have crafted elaborate, costly and highly sought-
after rites of initiation for boys and girls (Turner et al., 1987). In addition to promoting
solidarity, meaning and an identity (Whitehouse et al., 2014), the no-nonsense pragmatic
logic behind such rituals is that girls fare better when they initiated by older women into the
arts of womanhood, while boys also benet from initiation led by older men.
Complementariness and good models of gender relations are similarly socialized through
different rituals. Across cultures, there are opportunities for boys to learn from women role
models, from their girl peers to learn to relate to and to seduce and be respectful to the other
sex. The same is true for girls. Boys and girls, nally, need rites of passages to get to know
one another and learn to consume and cultivate their need for seduction. All cultures, in
other words, need their equivalent of Bar mitzvahs, Bat mitzvahs, prom nights, marriages,
father-son, father-daughter and mixed-family camping trips with strong aunt, uncle and
grandparent role models (Hrdy, 2011). In the language of public health, having access to
varied kinds of gender-specic and mixed-genders seduction, interaction and initiation
rituals grounded in a rich cultural folklore is an immense protective factor against poor life
outcomes. In strength-based language (Smith, 2006), these gendered rituals are crucial
mediators and indicators of community well-being. Contemporary psychologists recognize
the importance of same-gender role models for women, particularly in the context of
educational and career achievement, but often argue that mens success do not require same-
SBR
Downloaded by McGill University At 12:06 12 October 2018 (PT)
sex models (Lockwood, 2006;Young et al.,2013). I suggest that this reects a contemporary
bias against masculinity proper rather than a universalpsychological nding.
Rites of boyhood and manhood, such as the ones cultivated in fraternities and athletic
cultures are now overly associated with the toxicarchetypes. Those who promote the
importance of initiation into manhood, like the poet Bly (2001), psychologist Peterson (2018)
or motivational speaker Deida (1997), are typically dismissed as quack mystics, misogynists
or ALT-rightconservatives (see Burns, 2017, for an example of this view).
Consider this essay a rational call for the importance of such rites and for the return of
masculinity as a good ideal type.
2. Conclusion: How should we raise our boys?
I began this essay with reections on my intellectual journey as anthropologist who has
examined complex questions of gender and sex differences from ethnographic and
evolutionary perspectives. I grounded my effort to present a general meta-ethics of
gender relations on stable patterns of recognized ideal types in the cross-cultural record.
Drawing of these transculturally stable models, I proposed that toxicmale and female
archetypes were pedagogically important to provide boys and girls with clear counter
examples of the kinds of good moral persons they should become. As such, I argued that
TM and TF archetypes were most useful when presented together along with a
discussion of evolved differences and opportunities for same-gender and mixed-gender
rites and rituals of initiation.
Returning to what I understand best as a man and a father of two boys, I conclude this
essay with a tentative call for a normative, character-virtue-based ethics for the imitation of
young boys.
The following story, which is often attributed to the US military folklore and discussed in
light of its toxicity(Sparrows, 2015), presents what I take to be a fairly universal trope in
the education of boys. The story describes three ideal types: wolves, sheep and sheepdogs.
Wolves are painted as lonely, strong and mean. Wolves prey on sheep and weak wolves
alike. Sheep are described as naïve and weak. They are eaten by wolves. Sheepdogs, who
protect the sheep against the wolves, are elevated as strong and reliable. Boys in this story
(like my own boys) are then encouraged to be sheepdogs (the good ideal type) but not wolves
or sheep (the ideal types for weak and aggressive men).
This emphasis on strength, autonomy, altruism and reliability has been found in many
cultures, from Western stoicism to Vedic and Buddhist wisdom and huntergatherer
pedagogies (Haidt, 2007). On an interpersonal level, men in this story are encouraged to be
caring and protecting. On a personal level, character strength is taken to entail a sense of
ownership over ones feelings and actions. You will never be well, as the transcultural story
goes, if you expect the world to conform to your desires.
One may further extract the following set of principles from this stoic model:
You should be strong and caring and should protect the weak.
You should respect your elders and hierarchies question them when you are being
encouraged to be weak or selsh.
You should offer your services to women, children, elders and the sick.
As a rule, your needs always come last. If you do not take care of yourself well, you
will not be able to take care of others.
You should never be a victim; when bad things happen to you, the onus is on you to
create the right mindset to recover and thrive.
Ritual,
morality, and
gender
archetypes
Downloaded by McGill University At 12:06 12 October 2018 (PT)
You should never blame others for your own feelings.
You should not encourage victimhood in others.
It is good to cry for the joys and pains of others. You should never cry out of pity for
yourself.
You should always be rm, kind and generous and know your boundaries.
Rights and obligations are given, but privileges are earned.
You should try new things and learn new skills that will make you a good protector.
The simplest stories are often the best ones; the simplest solutions are not always
best; you should choose stairs over elevators.
Be open to changing your mind; do not change your mind too much.
Go on a walkabout at least twice in your life.
When about to give up, try just a little longer; try a little longer after that as well.
You should have women friends you will not seduce and spend time with aunties
and grandmothers.
You should observe and study the mysteries and beauties of femininity.
In this version of the stoic tenets, which I borrow from my teachings as a father, the story is
gendered rst and foremost because I am a man. I have no daughter of my own, but I teach
the same story to my sistersand friendsgirls when I take them outside for initiation into
the world. This story is similarly applicable to children who may feel a stable sexual
attraction towards their own sex.
Where the new archetype of gender uidityts in this picture is a difcult question.
This archetype is still under construction in its current form, and it is still counter-intuitive
to many people. For parents of children who cannot t into a clear side of typically gendered
stories and archetypes, it may simply be useful to teach the full story the story of
masculine and feminine types, good and bad.
Notes
1. For the second, sexless Brazil cycle, see Veissière, 2016b
2. For an ethnographically and statistically grounded critique of the sex trackingpanic, see
Agustin, 2007; see also Lancaster, 2011.
References
Agustin, L.M. (2007), Sex at the margins: Migration, labour markets and the rescue industry, Zed
books.
Bilsker, D. and White, J. (2011), The silent epidemic of male suicide,BC Med J, Vol. 53 No. 10, pp. 529-534.
Bly, R. (2001), Iron John: Men and Masculinity, Random House, New York, NY.
Boesch, C., Bole, C., Eckhardt, N. and Boesch, H. (2010), Altruism in Forest chimpanzees: the case of
adoption,PLoS One, Vol. 5 No. 1, pp. e8901.
Bourgois, P. (2003), In search of respect: Selling crack in El Barrio, Vol. 10, Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge.
Boyd, R. and Richerson, P.J. (1988), Culture and The Evolutionary Process, University of Chicago press.
Boyer, P. (2008), Religion Explained, Random House, New York, NY.
SBR
Downloaded by McGill University At 12:06 12 October 2018 (PT)
Boyer, P. and Liénard, P. (2006), Precaution systems and ritualized behavior,Behavioral and Brain
Sciences, Vol. 29 No. 6, pp. 635-641.
Brophy, C.A. (2015), Political correctness: social-scal liberalism and left-wing authoritarianism,
Masters thesis, Department of Psychology, University of Toronto.
Burns, J. (2017), Biopolitics, toxic masculinities, disavowed histories, and youth radicalization,Peace
Review, Vol. 29 No. 2, pp. 176-183.
Buss, D.M. (1995), Psychological sex differences: origins through sexual selection.
Butler, J. (2011), Gender trouble: Feminism and the Subversion of Identity, Routledge, Abingdon.
Campbell, B. and Manning, J. (2014), Microaggression and moral cultures,Comparative Sociology,
Vol. 13 No. 6, pp. 692-726.
Deida, D. (1997), The way of the superior man. Boulder: Sounds True, 2004.
Douglas, M. (2003), Purity and Danger: An Analysis of Concepts of Pollution and Taboo, Routledge,
Abingdon.
Eagly, A.H. and Wood, W. (1999), The origins of sex differences in human behavior: evolved
dispositions versus social roles,American Psychologist, Vol. 54 No. 6, p. 408.
Forouzan, E. and Cooke, D.J. (2005), Figuring out la femme fatale: conceptual and assessment
issues concerning psychopathy in females,Behavioral Sciences and the Law, Vol. 23 No. 6,
pp. 765-778.
Fuentes, A. (2015), Race, Monogamy, and Other Lies They Told You: Busting Myths About Human
Nature, Univ of CA Press.
Greene, J. and Haidt, J. (2002), How (and where) does moral judgment work?,Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, Vol. 6 No. 12, pp. 517-523.
Hacking, I. (2002), Historical ontology,In the Scope of Logic, Methodology and Philosophy of Science,
Springer, Dordrecht, pp. 583-600.
Hacking, I. (2007), Kinds of people: moving targets,InProceedings-British Academy, Vol. 151, Oxford
University Press, p. 285.
Haidt, J. (2007), The Happiness Hypothesis: Putting Ancient Wisdom to the Test of Modern Science,
Random House.
Haidt, J. (2012), The Righteous Mind: Why Good People are Divided by politics and Religion, Vintage,
New York, NY.
Haidt, J. (2017), The unwisest idea on campus: commentary on Lilienfeld,Perspectives on Psychological
Science : a Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, Vol. 12 No. 1, pp. 176-177.
Hanson, H. and ORawe, C. (2010), The femme fatale: images, histories, contexts, Springer, Berlin.
Harding, S.G. (Ed.). (2004), The feminist standpoint theory reader: Intellectual and political controversies,
Psychology Press, London.
Harnad, S. (2017), To cognize is to categorize: cognition is categorization,In Handbook of
Categorization in Cognitive Science, 2nd ed., pp. 21-54.
Hay, C. (2015), Girlfriend, Mother, Professor?,New York Times, available at: https://opinionator.blogs.
nytimes.com/2016/01/25/girlfriend-mother-professor/
Hecht, T. (2006), After life: An Ethnographic Novel, Duke University Press.
Henrich, J. (2015), The Secret of Our Success: How Culture is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating
Our Species, and Making us Smarter, Princeton University Press.
Henrich, J., Boyd, R. and Richerson, P.J. (2012), The Puzzle of Monogamous Marriage, Phil.
Hrdy, S.B. (2011), Mothers and others, Harvard University Press.
Keane, W. (2015), Ethical life: Its Natural and Social Histories, Princeton University Press.
Kipnis, L. (2017), Unwanted Advances: Sexual Paranoia Comes to Campus, Harper Collins.
Ritual,
morality, and
gender
archetypes
Downloaded by McGill University At 12:06 12 October 2018 (PT)
Kulick, D. (1997), The gender of Brazilian transgendered prostitutes,American Anthropologist,
Vol. 99 No. 3, pp. 574-585.
Lamb, M.E. (2000), The history of research on father involvement: an overview,Marriage and Family
Review, Vol. 29 Nos 2/3, pp. 23-42.
Lamb, M.E. and Goldberg, W.A. (1982), The fatherchild relationship: a synthesis of biological,
evolutionary, and social perspectives,Parenting: Its Causes and Consequences, pp. 55-73.
Lancaster, R. (2005), Tolerance and Intolerance in Sexual Cultures in Latin America, Sexuality and
Immigration, Passing Lines, 14, 255.
Lancaster, R.N. (2011), Sex panic and the Punitive State, University of CA Press.
Leary, M.R. and Kowalski, R.M. (1990), Impression management: a literature review and two-
component model,Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 107 No. 1, p. 34.
Lilienfeld, S.O. (2017), Microaggressions: strong claims, inadequate evidence,Perspectives on
Psychological Science, Vol. 12 No. 1, pp. 138-169.
Lockwood, P. (2006). Someone like me can be successful: do college students need same-gender role
models?, Psychology of Women Quarterly.
MacIntyre, A. (2013), After Virtue, A&C Black.
Manalansan, M.F. IV, (2003), Global Divas: Filipino Gay Men in the Diaspora, Duke University Press.
Mead, M. (1939), From the South Seas; studies of adolescence and sex in primitive societies, Morrow,
New York, New York, NY.
Peterson, J.B. (2018), 12 Rules for Life: An Antidote to Chaos, Random House, Toronto.
Phillips, M.L., Young, A.W., Senior, C., Brammer, M., Andrew, C., Calder, A.J. and Gray, J.A. (1997), A
specic neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust,Nature, Vol. 389 No. 6650,
p. 495.
Ramstead, M.J., Veissière, S.P. and Kirmayer, L.J. (2016), Cultural affordances: scaffolding local
worlds through shared intentionality and regimes of attention,Frontiers in Psychology,
Vol. 7, p. 1090.
Rand, D.G., Brescoll, V.L., Everett, J.A., Capraro, V. and Barcelo, H. (2016), Social heuristics and social
roles: intuition favors altruism for women but not for men,Journal of Experimental Psychology:
General, Vol. 145 No. 4, p. 389.
Rival, L. (2011), What Kind of Sex Makes People Happy?in Astuti, R., Parry, J. and Stafford, C. (? 1).
(2007). Questions of Anthropology, Berg.
Rozin, P., Haidt, J. and Fincher, K. (2009). From oral to moral, Science.
Schmidt, M.F., Rakoczy, H. and Tomasello, M. (2011), Young children attribute normativity to
novel actions without pedagogy or normative language,Developmental Science,Vol.14
No. 3, pp. 530-539.
Schmitt, D.P., Realo, A., Voracek, M. and Allik, J. (2008), Why cant a man be more like a woman? Sex
differences in big ve personality traits across 55 cultures,Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, Vol. 94 No. 1, p. 168.
Severson, K., Aune, J. and Jodlowski, D. (2008), Bruno bettelheim, autism, and the rhetoric of scientic
authority,Autism and Representation, Vol. 65-77.
Smith, E.J. (2006), The strength-based counseling model,The Counseling Psychologist, Vol. 34 No. 1,
pp. 13-79.
Soutschek, A., Burke, C.J., Beharelle, A.R., Schreiber, R., Weber, S.C., Karipidis, I.I. and Tobler, P.N.
(2017), The dopaminergic reward system underpins gender differences in social preferences,
Nature Human Behaviour, Vol. 1 No. 11, p. 819.
Sparrows, J. (2015), American sniper and the sheepdog trope,the Bafer, avilable at: https://
thebafer.com/latest/american-sniper
SBR
Downloaded by McGill University At 12:06 12 October 2018 (PT)
Stoller, R.J. and Herdt, G.H. (1982), The development of masculinity: a cross-cultural contribution,
Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, Vol. 30 No. 1, pp. 29-59.
Swami, V., Voracek, M., Stieger, S., Tran, U.S. and Furnham, A. (2014), Analytic thinking reduces
belief in conspiracy theories,Cognition, Vol. 133 No. 3, pp. 572-585.
Tomasello, M. (2016), A Natural History of Human Morality, Harvard University Press.
Trexler, R.C. (1999), Sex and conquest: Gendered violence, political order, and the European conquest of
the Americas, Cornell University Press.
Turner, V., Mahdi, L.C., Foster, S. and Little, M. (1987), Betwixt and between: Patterns of masculine and
feminine initiation, Open Court, Lasalle, IL.
Tybur, J.M., Lieberman, D., Kurzban, R. and DeScioli, P. (2013), Disgust: evolved function and
structure,Psychological Review, Vol. 120 No. 1, pp. 65-84.
Veissière, S. (2009), Notes and queries for an activist street anthropology: street resistance,
gringopolitica, and the quest for subaltern visions in Salvador da Bahia, Brazil,Education,
Participatory Action Research, and Social Change, Palgrave Macmillan, New York, NY,
pp. 209-222.
Veissière, S.P. (2010), Making a living: the gringo ethnographer as pimp of the suffering in the late
capitalist night,Cultural Studies? Critical Methodologies, Vol. 10 No. 1, pp. 29-39.
Veissière, S. (2011), The Ghosts of Empire: Violence, Suffering and Mobility in The Transatlantic
Cultural Economy of Desire, Vol. 3, LIT Verlag, Münster.
Veissière, S. (2016a), Varieties of Tulpa experiences: the hypnotic nature of human sociality,
personhood, and interphenomenality,Hypnosis and meditation: Towards an integrative science
of conscious planes, pp. 55-78.
Veissière, S. (2016b), The internet is not a river: space, movement and personhood in a wired world,in
Friedman, M. and Schultermandl, S. (Eds), Click and Kin: Transnational Identity and Quick
Media, University of Toronto Press, Toronto.
Veissière, S. (2018), Cultural Markov blankets? Mind the other minds gap!,Physics of Life Reviews,
Vol. 24, pp. 47-49.
Whitehouse, H., Lanman, J.A., Downey, G., Fredman, L.A., Swann Jr, W.B., Lende, D.H. and Whitehouse,
H. (2014). The ties that bind us: ritual, fusion, and identication.Current Anthropology,55(6),
000-000.
Young, D.M., Rudman, L.A., Buettner, H.M. and McLean, M.C. (2013), The inuence of female role
models on womens implicit science cognitions,Psychology of Women Quarterly, Vol. 37 No. 3,
pp. 283-292.
Further reading
Henrich, J., Boyd, R. and Richerson, P.J. (2008), Five misunderstandings about cultural evolution,
Human Nature (Hawthorne, N.Y.), Vol. 19 No. 2, pp. 119-137.
Corresponding author
Samuel Paul Louis Veissière can be contacted at: samuel.veissiere@mcgill.ca
For instructions on how to order reprints of this article, please visit our website:
www.emeraldgrouppublishing.com/licensing/reprints.htm
Or contact us for further details: permissions@emeraldinsight.com
Ritual,
morality, and
gender
archetypes
Downloaded by McGill University At 12:06 12 October 2018 (PT)
... Furthermore, there is also a connection between social expectations and cultural ideas about masculinity and how men express their emotions. Some societal norms may emphasize emotional control, potentially leading to concerns about being judged or rejected for expressing vulnerability (Veissière, 2018;Butler, 2023;Harianti, 2023). This focus on emotional control may influence their readiness to share their experiences with mental health challenges, potentially contributing to the association between mental health and a lack of openness (Huggett et al., 2018;Martinez et al., 2020). ...
... Interventions that address these societal norms and create supportive environments are crucial. (Huggett et al., 2018;Veissière, 2018;Martinez et al., 2020;Butler, 2023;Harianti, 2023). ...
... These statements reveal how the pervasive cultural narrative of toxic masculinity constrains male students' emotional expression, as they fear being perceived as weak (Butler, 2023;Harianti, 2023). The result highlights the subjective experience of male students who internalize societal expectations of stoicism, impacting their mental health by discouraging openness and vulnerability (Veissière, 2018;Cebreros, 2023). Moreover, the societal power structures perpetuating these harmful norms suggest that systemic change is necessary to dismantle these stereotypes (Barillas & Aguirre, 2024). ...
Article
Full-text available
Mental health is crucial throughout life and significantly influences overall well-being, yet it is often overlooked and stigmatized. Mental health issues among men are a significant public health concern due to societal norms that discourage emotional vulnerability and seeking help, leading to underreporting and inadequate access to necessary services. While research has explored these connections, a gap exists in understanding how these factors influence the mental health of young men specifically. The study was conducted in the college department of an institution in Malaybalay City, Bukidnon. It utilized an explanatory sequential research design, beginning with quantitative data collection to measure 106 male students' experiences with mental health expression and stigma, followed by qualitative methods to explore the experiences of 5 male students in-depth regarding their mental health expression. This study examines the factors influencing male students' willingness to communicate mental health expression. The study shows a strong positive correlation between adverse experiences and higher-level stigma, underscoring how societal attitudes discourage open communication. Moreover, the qualitative findings reveal that societal norms, personal experiences, and coping mechanisms play a role. Themes like stereotyping, social anxieties, and feelings of isolation emerged, highlighting students' challenges in seeking support. These qualitative insights support the quantitative data, which shows a high prevalence of stigma surrounding mental health. The study emphasizes the need to address these cultural and structural barriers to create a more supportive environment that reduces stigma and encourages help-seeking behavior among male students.
... Namun, bagi mereka Katon Dicken Adi Wicaksono, Fitrinanda An Nur Toxic Masculinity Tokoh Ken Pada Film Barbie Live Action 2023 yang tidak sesuai dengan norma-norma ini, sering kali dianggap kurang "maskulin" atau bahkan dianggap tidak mencapai standar maskulinitas yang dianggap ideal, dan ini dikenal sebagai maskulinitas toksik. (Veissière, 2018). Film ini mengkaji bagaimana Ken yang awalnya hanya menjadi peran pendukung dalam kehidupan barbie berusaha membawa sistem patriarki ke dalam barbie land, sehingga posisi yang awalnya perempuan yang memimpin dan mengatur semuanya berbubah menjadi laki-laki yang berkuasa atas segalanya. ...
Article
Penelitian ini bertujuan untuk menjelaskan karakteristik maskulinitas toksik yang berasal dari karakteristik maskulinitas yang disalahartikan pada sosok Ken. Toksik maskulinitas adalah karakteristik gagah yang pada dasarnya, namun hal ini menjadi toxic atau salah arah ketika pria dituntut harus memiliki dan menunjukkan maskulinitas secara berlebihan hingga merugikan orang di sekitarnya. Hal ini tercermin dalam film ini melalui tokoh Ken yang selalu mengedepankan konsep patriarki. Pendekatan yang digunakan adalah kualitatif dengan metode analisis semiotika John Fiske, yang terdiri dari tiga tingkatan pengkodean: tingkat realitas (lingkungan dan perilaku), tingkat representasi (setting dan angle kamera), dan tingkat ideologi (realitas dan representasi sebagai kode ideologi). Hasil analisis mencakup tanda-tanda verbal dan nonverbal, seperti ekspresi wajah, gerakan tubuh, dan percakapan tokoh Ken. Karakter Ken juga sering menunjukkan memiliki kekuasaan dan status sosial yang tinggi agar bisa dihormati oleh orang lain, berperilaku kasar dan agresif, serta mendominasi orang lain, serta berusaha tampil macho untuk menyembunyikan ketidaksempurnaannya, yang merupakan contoh negatif dari dominasi maskulinitas yang dipaksakan, disalahpahami, dan disalahgunakan. Selain itu, film ini mencerminkan ideologi individualisme yang kurang memperhatikan lingkungan sekitarnya.
... The extreme set of physical and behavioural traits that were once seen as typically male are now linked to toxic masculinity. Toxic masculinity is a transculturally widespread archetype or moral trope that represents the kind of man one should not aspire to be (Veissière 2018). The concept is often used to describe the negative aspects of exaggerated masculine traits. ...
... Ao trazer como personagem principal um garoto adolescente, é comum que se espere um reflexo do machismo estrutural presente no contexto histórico global, que ensina os meninos a não chorarem, não sofrerem, não manifestarem seus sentimentos, configurando o que a ciência tem chamado de "masculinidade tóxica" (Sculos, 2017;Veissière, 2018;Paula e Rocha, 2019). Ao retomar o histórico da "construção social da masculinidade" (Oliveira, 2004) -que, ao longo dos anos, atrelou ao homem a imagem do indivíduo forte, viril, chefe de família, líder político e religioso, no topo da hierarquia da sociedade -, percebe-se que Anything Could Happen pode contribuir para uma mudança nesse imaginário coletivo. ...
Article
Full-text available
Tudo pode acontecer é um romance juvenil norte-americano escrito por Will Walton em 2015 e que, no Brasil, foi publicado pela editora V&R. Neste trabalho, objetivou-se identificar o potencial comunicativo-cultural da obra, por meio de uma análise estrutural e contextual da narrativa. Para isso, adotou-se a metodologia de abordagem qualitativa e interpretativo-analítica, partindo do roteiro de Gancho (2006) e expandindo para dimensões culturais. A fundamentação teórica ancorou-se nos estudos de narrativa, tendo como base Todorov (2011), Genette (2011) e Barthes (2011), entre outros autores. Os resultados apontaram que, para além das categorias convencionais da narrativa (enredo, temporalidade, ambientação, narrador e discurso), foi possível detectar referências linguísticas, sociais, históricas e artístico-culturais.
Book
Full-text available
Celem książki jest przybliżenie najbardziej popularnych i najczęściej stosowanych teorii w obrębie krytycznych studiów nad mężczyznami i męskościami. Wybrane teorie zostały przybliżone, przeanalizowane i zobrazowane przykładami badań. W rozdziale pierwszym opisano rozwój studiów nad mężczyznami i męskościami w Polsce na tle kultury Zachodu. Ponadto wskazano perspektywy oraz teorie wykorzystywane w polskich socjologicznych studiach nad mężczyznami i męskościami. W kolejnych rozdziałach scharakteryzowano założenia kolejnych koncepcji, przedyskutowano i zniuansowano ich specyfikę, omówiono krytykę. Przybliżono: teorię męskości hegemonicznej, założenia męskości toksycznej, teorię męskości hybrydowych, męskości inkluzywnej, męskości opiekuńczych, kobiecych męskości i koncepcję „otwartych” i „zamkniętych” męskości.
Article
Full-text available
En los últimos años, las series de televisión han apostado por mujeres protagonistas que, además de tener un rol principal en las tramas, ponen sobre la palestra cuestiones que importan a nivel social como la violencia de género o la maternidad. La cadena de ámbito público, Televisión Española (TVE), es partícipe de esta tendencia ofreciendo ficciones a la audiencia que articulan interesantes debates en torno a la desigualdad de género. El presente artículo ofrece un análisis textual de dos series de televisión recientes: La otra mirada (TVE: 2018-2019) y Néboa (TVE: 2020), en las que existe un protagonismo femenino incuestionable. Desde una perspectiva feminista, se concluye que ambas ficciones españolas articulan personajes complejos, caracterizados por su personalidad y capacidad de decisión, y enfrentados a situaciones sexistas.
Chapter
This chapter provides broader contextualsation of the incel community. It explores how incels use ‘pill’ metaphors, particularly in relation to other communities within the manosphere. Furthermore, the differences between ‘red’, ‘blue’, and ‘black’ pill philosophies are explored and incels are discussed in relation to these philosophies. This chapter highlights how incels conform to ideals of hegemonic masculinity, and as such I also explore this concept and related theories of masculinity. In a similar vein, I argue that incels view themselves as non-normative because they are unable to conform to hegemonic masculine norms, and as such as explore previous research into normativities. Incels view themselves as nonnormative due to both essentialist and deterministic thinking. I discuss these concepts and highlight how sometimes this kind of thinking is contradictory. The chapter discusses extensive academic work on masculinity, the alt-right, and cognate topics as they relate to the incel community.
Article
Full-text available
Here I summarize the main points in my 2016 book, A Natural History of Human Morality. Taking an evolutionary point of view, I characterize human morality as a special form of cooperation. In particular, human morality represents a kind of we > me orientation and valuation that emanates from the logic of social interdependence, both at the level of individual collaboration and at the level of the cultural group. Human morality emanates from psychological processes of shared intentionality evolved to enable individuals to function effectively in ever more cooperative lifeways.
Article
Full-text available
Ramstead et al’s variational neuroethology framework elegantly combines recent developments in evolutionary systems theory, machine learning, and theoretical biology to explain the tendency of living systems to resist disintegration. They expand on a formal model of neuronal self-organization based on the free-energy principle (FEP) to articulate a meta-theory of perception, action, and biobehaviour that they extend from the human brain and mind to body and society. This paper applies the Cultural Affordances model to answer a question raised by the authors on the mechanisms of free-energy minisation applied to cultural ensembles in the Homo Sapiens niche. I take up Ramstead et al’s notion of the markov blanket (the ‘veil’ of perception through which organisms process internal states and statistical priors about external states), to clarify the importance of other minds as a crucial level of species-wide information processing. I argue that for humans, the markov blanket serves as a buffer to exploit statistical regularities in human psychology at least as much, if not more than in external states of the world. I clarify that the nestedness of these inferences should be primarily conceptualized at the level of recursive mindreading – or inferences about other humans’ internal states. I conclude with comments on the relevance of the Extended Evolutionary Synthesis (EES) to the author’s description of cognitive function on evolutionary, developmental, and real-time scales, and apply the authors’ free-energy model to speculate on the role of simplifications in the evolution of social complexity.
Article
Full-text available
Women are known to have stronger prosocial preferences than men, but it remains an open question as to how these behavioural differences arise from differences in brain functioning. Here, we provide a neurobiological account for the hypothesized gender difference. In a pharmacological study and an independent neuroimaging study, we tested the hypothesis that the neural reward system encodes the value of sharing money with others more strongly in women than in men. In the pharmacological study, we reduced receptor type-specific actions of dopamine, a neurotransmitter related to reward processing, which resulted in more selfish decisions in women and more prosocial decisions in men. Converging findings from an independent neuroimaging study revealed gender-related activity in neural reward circuits during prosocial decisions. Thus, the neural reward system appears to be more sensitive to prosocial rewards in women than in men, providing a neurobiological account for why women often behave more prosocially than men.
Chapter
We organisms are sensorimotor systems. Things in the world come into contact with our sensory surfaces, and we interact with them based on what that sensorimotor contact “affords.” All of our categories consist of ways in which we behave differently toward different kinds of things—things we do or do not eat, mate with, or flee from; or the things that we describe, through our language, as prime numbers, affordances, absolute discriminables, or truths. Categorization—doing the right thing with the right kind of thing—is largely what cognition is about, and for.