ArticlePDF Available

Frequency of Visual Deficits in Children With Developmental Dyslexia

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Importance Developmental dyslexia (DD) is a specific learning disability of neurobiological origin whose core cognitive deficit is widely believed to involve language (phonological) processing. Although reading is also a visual task, the potential role of vision in DD has been controversial, and little is known about the integrity of visual function in individuals with DD. Objective To assess the frequency of visual deficits (specifically vergence, accommodation, and ocular motor tracking) in children with DD compared with a control group of typically developing readers. Design, Setting, and Participants A prospective, uncontrolled observational study was conducted from May 28 to October 17, 2016, in an outpatient ophthalmology ambulatory clinic among 29 children with DD and 33 typically developing (TD) children. Main Outcomes and Measures Primary outcomes were frequencies of deficits in vergence (amplitude, fusional ranges, and facility), accommodation (amplitude, facility, and accuracy), and ocular motor tracking (Developmental Eye Movement test and Visagraph eye tracker). Results Among the children with DD (10 girls and 19 boys; mean [SD] age, 10.3 [1.2] years) and the TD group (21 girls and 12 boys; mean [SD] age, 9.4 [1.4] years), accommodation deficits were more frequent in the DD group than the TD group (16 [55%] vs 3 [9%]; difference = 46%; 95% CI, 25%-67%; P < .001). For ocular motor tracking, 18 children in the DD group (62%) had scores in the impaired range (in the Developmental Eye Movement test, Visagraph, or both) vs 5 children in the TD group (15%) (difference, 47%; 95% CI, 25%-69%; P < .001). Vergence deficits occurred in 10 children in the DD group (34%) and 5 children in the TD group (15%) (difference, 19%; 95% CI, –2.2% to 41%; P = .08). In all, 23 children in the DD group (79%) and 11 children in the TD group (33%) had deficits in 1 or more domain of visual function (difference, 46%; 95% CI, 23%-69%; P < .001). Conclusions and Relevance These findings suggest that deficits in visual function are far more prevalent in school-aged children with DD than in TD readers, but the possible cause and clinical relevance of these deficits are uncertain. Further study is needed to determine the extent to which treating these deficits can improve visual symptoms and/or reading parameters.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Frequency of Visual Deficits in Children
With Developmental Dyslexia
Aparna Raghuram, OD, PhD; Sowjanya Gowrisankaran, PhD; Emily Swanson, BS;
David Zurakowski, MS, PhD; David G. Hunter, MD, PhD; Deborah P. Waber, PhD
IMPORTANCE Developmental dyslexia (DD) is a specific learning disability of neurobiological
origin whose core cognitive deficit is widely believed to involve language (phonological)
processing. Although reading is also a visual task, the potential role of vision in DD has been
controversial, and little is known about the integrity of visual function in individuals with DD.
OBJECTIVE To assess the frequency of visual deficits (specifically vergence, accommodation,
and ocular motor tracking) in children with DD compared with a control group of typically
developing readers.
DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS A prospective, uncontrolled observational study was
conducted from May 28 to October 17, 2016, in an outpatient ophthalmology ambulatory
clinic among 29 children with DD and 33 typically developing (TD) children.
MAIN OUTCOMES AND MEASURES Primary outcomes were frequencies of deficits in vergence
(amplitude, fusional ranges, and facility), accommodation (amplitude, facility, and accuracy),
and ocular motor tracking (Developmental Eye Movement test and Visagraph eye tracker).
RESULTS Among the children with DD (10 girls and 19 boys; mean [SD] age, 10.3 [1.2] years)
and the TD group (21 girls and 12 boys; mean [SD] age, 9.4 [1.4] years), accommodation
deficits were more frequent in the DD group than the TD group (16 [55%] vs 3 [9%];
difference = 46%; 95% CI, 25%-67%; P< .001). For ocular motor tracking, 18 children in the
DD group (62%) had scores in the impaired range (in the Developmental Eye Movement test,
Visagraph, or both) vs 5 children in the TD group (15%) (difference, 47%; 95% CI, 25%-69%;
P< .001). Vergence deficits occurred in 10 children in the DD group (34%) and 5 children in
the TD group (15%) (difference, 19%; 95% CI, –2.2% to 41%; P= .08). In all, 23 children in the
DD group (79%) and 11 children in the TD group (33%) had deficits in 1 or more domain of
visual function (difference, 46%; 95% CI, 23%-69%; P< .001).
CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE These findings suggest that deficits in visual function are far
more prevalent in school-aged children with DD than in TD readers, but the possible cause
and clinical relevance of these deficits are uncertain. Further study is needed to determine
the extent to which treating these deficits can improve visual symptoms and/or reading
parameters.
JAMA Ophthalmol. 2018;136(10):1089-1095.doi:10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2018.2797
Published online July 19, 2018.
Invited Commentary
page 1096
Supplemental content
CME Quiz at
jamanetwork.com/learning
and CME Questions page 1212
Author Affiliations: Author
affiliations are listed at the end of this
article.
Corresponding Author: Aparna
Raghuram, OD, PhD,Depar tment of
Ophthalmology, Boston Children’s
Hospital, 300 Longwood Ave,
Boston, MA 02115 (aparna.raghuram
@childrens.harvard.edu).
Research
JAMA Ophthalmology | Original Investigation
(Reprinted) 1089
© 2018 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Downloaded From: by a Harvard University User on 10/12/2018
Developmental dyslexia (DD) is a reading disorder that
emerges in childhood. The primary deficit involves im-
paired single-word decoding, word recognition, and
spelling, which may affect the a child’s reading rate, compre-
hension, and written expression later on. The disorder is gen-
erally viewed as stemming from a core linguistic deficit in pho-
nological processing; the recommended interventions are
educational and are theoretically guided by this premise.
1
Al-
though experts have historically dismissed claims that visual
processing might contribute meaningfully to the deficits
present in DD,
2,3
vision therapy has been recommended for
decades by behavioral optometrists to improve purported
visual symptoms.
4-6
Vision therapy is based on the premise that correctingperiph-
eral visual deficits (specifically vergence, accommodation, and
ocular motor tracking) will improve reading performance, yet
there is a dearth of controlled studies documenting that such defi-
cits are more common in children with DD, much less whether
treating them could improve reading. A well-documented ac-
counting of whether visual deficits are more prevalent in chil-
dren with DD is thus a necessary prerequisite to any controlled
evaluation of interventions to correct such deficits.
The primary goal of this study, therefore, was to assess the
prevalence and nature of visual deficits in children with dys-
lexia and compare them with a control group of typically devel-
oping (TD) readers. Wesought to provide initial estimates of what
visual functions specifically are affected in children with DD, the
frequency and magnitude of any abnormal findings, and whether
these deficits cluster in a subgroup of readers with dyslexia or
are more evenly distributed across the population.
Methods
Study Design and Participants
A prospective, uncontrolled, group comparison observational
study was conducted from May 28 to October 17, 2016. Chil-
dren who received a diagnosis of DD were recruited from a hos-
pital-based clinic, and a control group of TD readers were re-
cruited from patients receiving standard eye examinations in
the same hospital, as well as by flyers and word of mouth. In-
formed written assent was obtained from each participant, and
informed written consent was obtained from each parent or
guardian. The ethics committee of Boston Children’s Hospital
approved the research protocol. The entire study was con-
ducted according to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.
7
Inclusion criteria for the DD group were diagnosis of DD, 7
to 11 years of age, best-corrected visual acuity better than or equal
to 20/25 in each eye, and residing within 120 km (75 miles) of
the research site. Exclusion criteria were diagnosis of attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder documented by questionnaires,
IQ less than 70, diagnosis of a comorbid neurodevelopmental
disorder (eg, autism spectrum disorder, brain malformation,or
neurogenetic disorder) or medical illness, history of eye sur-
gery, structural anomalies of the anterior or posterior segment
of the eye or medical condition that could affect the ability to
participate in the study, or other ocular pathologic conditions.
The inclusion and exclusion criteria for the TD group were the
same except that the child must not have received a diagnosis
of DD or extra reading support within either general or special
education. Of 50 potentially eligible children identified with
DD, 29 agreed to participate and completed the evaluation.
Thirty-three children participated in the TD group.
Procedure
Participants were invited to the laboratory for a visit of ap-
proximately 2 hours, during which psychoeducational test-
ing, a comprehensive eye examination, and visual function
measures were performed as detailed herein.
Measures
Psychoeducational Testing
Psychoeducational testing included documentation of IQ
(2-subtest version of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intel-
ligence–II) and of single-word reading and spelling (Wood-
cock Reading Mastery Test, Word Identification and Word
Attack subtests, and Test of Written Spelling).
Vision Testing
Vision testing included a standard eye examinationincluding
pupil dilation and cycloplegic refraction. A detailed descrip-
tion of the assessments of visual function—vergence, accom-
modation, and ocular motor tracking—is provided in the
eAppendix in the Supplement. In brief, vergence was as-
sessed for amplitude, fusional ranges, and facility. Accommo-
dation was assessed for amplitude, facility, and accuracy.
Ocular motor tracking was assessed using 2 methods: a printed
test involving numbers oriented vertically and horizontally(D e-
velopmental Eye Movement [DEM] test) and an infrared eye
tracking test performed during reading activities (Visa-
graph). These assessments were performed with appropriate
refractive correction in place and in free space.
eTable 1 in the Supplement details the diagnoses for ver-
gence and accommodation deficits and their corresponding
criteria.
8-11
For the ocular motor tracking parameters, DEM and
Visagraph, scores below the 25th percentile for the TD group
were considered abnormal in the absence of well-established
criteria in the literature. Formeasures of accommodation per-
formed monocularly, the value from the eye with the lowest
performance was used for analysis.
Statistical Analysis
Group differences in demographic characteristics, psychoedu-
cational performance, vision, and visual function assessments
Key Points
Question Are deficits in visual function more frequent in children
with developmental dyslexia than in typically developing readers?
Findings In this cohort study, school-aged children with
developmental dyslexia exhibited more deficits in visual function—
vergence, accommodation, and/or ocular motor tracking—than did
a nonrandomized control group of typically developing children.
Meaning These findings suggest that visual function deficits contribute
to reading acquisition in children with developmental dyslexia.
Research Original Investigation Frequency of Visual Deficits in Children With Developmental Dyslexia
1090 JAMA Ophthalmology October 2018 Volume 136, Number 10 (Reprinted) jamaophthalmology.com
© 2018 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Downloaded From: by a Harvard University User on 10/12/2018
were assessed by ttests for continuous variables and χ
2
tests for
categorical variables. For continuous variables, Ftests based on
univariate analysis of variance and 2-way analysis of covari-
ance (ANCOVA) were performed to evaluate group differences,
adjusting for age, sex, and IQ differences as appropriate.All 95%
CIs were calculated using the Wilson method.
12
A 2-sided P<.01
was used to address type I errors due to multiple outcome analy-
ses and testing.
13
Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS
Statistics, version 23.0 (IBM Corp).
Results
Participant Characteristics
The Table shows the demographic and psychometric charac-
teristics of the participants in both groups. The DD group was
somewhat older (mean [SD] age, 10.3 [1.2] years vs 9.4 [1.4]
years) and had a higher proportion of boys than the TD group
(19 [66%] vs 12 [36%]), as is typical; the TD group had a higher
proportion of Asian children than the DD group (17 [52%] vs
0). The mean (SD) IQ for the DD group was in the average range
(104.97 [12.05]), but the mean (SD) IQ for the TD group was in
the high average range (117.88 [10.83]). There were group dif-
ferences in reading and spelling (Word Identification, t= 12.32;
WordAttack, t= 9.2; and Spelling, t= 8.45; all P< .001), which
remained highly significant after adjusting for IQ (WordIden-
tification, F= 101.07; Word Attack, F= 48.92; and Spelling,
F= 39.65; all P< .001). All parametric analyses for the vision
measures were adjusted for age and sex. Using ANCOVA, we
did not find IQ to be a significant covariate or to change the
findings in a meaningful way, so it was not included as a co-
variate for the vision measures.
Vision Measures
Refractive Error and Eye Alignment
The median cycloplegic refractive error did not differ between
the 2 groups (DD group: 0.75 diopters [D] [range, 2.25 to –4.75
D]; TD group: 0.50 D [range, 1.75 to –3.75 D]). The distribution
of eye alignment (based on the cover test) did not differ by
group for distance or near (eTable 2 in the Supplement).
Vergence
Individual vergence measures, including the near point of con-
vergence test and near fusional convergenceranges, were sig-
nificantly reduced in the DD group (eTable 3 in Supplement).
Furthermore, full criteria for a diagnosis of vergence deficit
were met in twice as many children in the DD group (10 [34%];
7 convergence excess and 3 convergence insufficiency) as in
the TD group (5 [15%]; 4 convergence excess and 1 conver-
gence insufficiency). This latter difference did not reach sta-
tistical significance (χ
2
= 3.15; difference, 19%; 95% CI, –2.2%
to 41%; P= .08).
Accommodation
The proportion of children who met the criteria for a diagno-
sis of accommodation deficiency was higher in the DD group
(16 [55%]; 9 accommodative insufficiency, 6 accommodative
dysfunction, and 1 accommodative infacility) than in the TD
group (3 [9%]; all accommodative insufficiency) (χ
2
= 15.42;
difference, 46%; 95% CI, 25%-67%; P< .001). An uncor-
rected refractive error greater than 0.75 D was present in 5 of
9 children in the DD group and 2 of 3 children in the TD group
with accommodative insufficiency. The amplitude of accom-
modation (F= 13.67) and monocular accommodative facility
(F= 18.11) also differed by group (P< .001 for both; eTable 4
in the Supplement). Overall, the number of children who had
vergence and/or accommodation deficits was substantially
higher in the DD group than in the TD group (18 [62%] vs 7
[21%]), as detailed in Figure 1.
14
Ocular Motor Tracking
DEM Test|One child with DD was overwhelmed by the DEM task
and could not complete it. For the DEM parameters (horizon-
tal and errors), the DD group performed more poorly than the
TD group, as seen from results of 2-way ANCOVA (Figure 2).
The proportion of children from the DD group obtaining scores
Table. DemographicCharacteristics and Performance
of Psychoeducational Measures of Developmental Dyslexia
and Typical Reading Groups
Characteristic
Individuals With
Developmental
Dyslexia
(n = 29)
Typically
Developing
Readers
(n = 33)
Age, mean (SD), y 10.3 (1.2) 9.4 (1.4)
Male, No. (%) 19 (66) 12 (36)
Race, No. (%)
White 29 (100) 16 (48)
Asian 0 17 (52)
Psychoeducational Test Scores at Time of Study
WRMT Word Identification
Mean (SD) 87.55 (15.65) 122.58 (15.94)
Median (range) 89 (55-112) 127 (95-145)
WRMT Word Attack
Mean (SD) 87.55 (9.43) 111.33 (11.73)
Median (range) 86 (63-112) 110 (92-134)
Test of written spelling
Mean (SD) 80.93 (13.44) 110.91 (14.36)
Median (range) 82 (45-105) 111 (83-140)
WASI-II estimated IQ
Mean (SD) 104.97 (12.05) 117.88 (10.83)
Median (range) 105 (83-133) 118 (95-138)
Psychoeducational Test Scores at Time of Clinical Evaluation
Elapsed time since
evaluation, y
Mean (SD) 1.68 (1.17) NA
Median (range) 1.72 (0.16-4.15) NA
WRMT Word Identification
a
Mean (SD) 80.62 (10.68) NA
Median (range) 81.00 (55-102) NA
WRMT Word Attack
b
Mean (SD) 83.88 (7.11) NA
Median (range) 83.50 (70-98) NA
Abbreviations: NA, not applicable; WASI-II, Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of
Intelligence–second edition; WRMT,Woodcock Reading Mastery Test.
a
For 29 children.
b
For 26 children.
Frequency of Visual Deficits in Children With Developmental Dyslexia Original Investigation Research
jamaophthalmology.com (Reprinted) JAMA Ophthalmology October 2018 Volume 136, Number 10 1091
© 2018 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Downloaded From: by a Harvard University User on 10/12/2018
in the failing range for the DEM test was elevated for both the
error (66%; χ
2
=10.70;P= .001)and horizontal (55%; χ
2
= 6.22;
P= .01) scores (eFigure 1A and B in the Supplement). Nearly
half (13 of 29 [45%]) of the children in the DD group per-
formed in the failing range for both the error and horizontal
scores compared with only 1 child in the TD group (3%).
Visagraph |Data could not be obtained for 2 children in the DD
group (1 child reading below the first grade reading level; the
other owing to examiner error). The text was chosen based on
the grade equivalent level from the Woodcock Reading Mastery
Test Word Identification. Forall 33 children in the TD group, the
grade equivalent was at or abovetheir c urrentgrade placement.
Of the 27 children in the DD group, 16 had a grade equivalent at
least 1 year below their current grade placement, and 11 had a
grade equivalent at their current placement. Datawere retained
for analysis only if the reading comprehension questions were
answered at or above 60% accuracy; all children except 1 in the
DD group met this criterion. The mean (SD) reading comprehen-
sion was comparable across the groups (DD group below their
grade placement, 85% [13.6%]; DD group at their grade place-
ment, 81% [14.5%]; and TD group, 89% [10.1%]).
Both DD subgroups performed more poorly than the TD
group on all Visagraph parameters but did not differ from each
other as seen from the results of 2-way ANCOVA (Figure 3). Nine
children in the DD group also completed the task with a text 1
level below their grade equivalent; their readingrate (F= 16.97),
fixations (F= 18.40), and regressions (F= 13.65) werestill poorer
than those of controls (P< .01 with 2-way ANCOVA). As illus-
trated in eFigure 2A to C in the Supplement, the proportion of
individuals with Visagraph impairments was greater in the DD
group for reading rate (21 of 26 [81%]), fixations (16 of 26 [62%]),
and regressions (15 of 26 [58%]) (reading rate, χ
2
= 18.59; fixa-
tions, χ
2
= 8.38; regressions, χ
2
= 8.28; all P< .01). A total of 13
children (50%) in the DD group achieved scores in the impaired
range for both fixations and regressions compared with only 5
children (15%) in the TD group. The DEM Horizontal score had
a moderate correlation with all 3 Visagraph parameters (r= 0.49-
0.51; P<.001;eTable5intheSupplement). Overall, 18 children
in the DD group (62%) had scores in the impaired range on the
DEM and/or Visagraphcompared w ithonly 5 children in the TD
group (15%) (difference, 47%; 95% CI, 25%-69%; P< .001).
Distribution of Visual Function Deficits Within the DD Group
Of the 29 children with DD, 23 (79%) had a deficit in 1 or more
domain compared with only 11 (33%) in the TD group. Among
the children in the DD group, deficits were observed in more
than 1 domain in 15 children (52%) and in 1 domain in 8 chil-
dren (28%) (Figure 4).
14
Discussion
The goals of this study were to determine whether deficits in
peripheral visual function (vergence, accommodation, and ocu-
lar motor tracking) are more prevalent in children with DD.Spe-
cifically, it addressed which functions were most promi-
nently affected, the frequency and magnitude of abnormal
findings, and whether deficits clustered in a subgroup or were
more evenly distributed across the DD group.
The prevalence of visual deficits was high among children
with DD; 23 (79%) met the criteria for a diagnosis of deficit in 1
or more domains of vergence, accommodation, and/or track-
ing compared with only 11 children in the TD group (33%). Ac-
commodation and ocular motor tracking were significantly im-
paired in children in the DD group, as were differences in 3
specific measures of vergence (but not in overall impairment
of vergence.) These deficits were not equally distributed across
the DD group. A total of 15 of the children in the DD group (52%)
had deficits in more than 1 domain, 8 (28%) had deficits in 1 do-
main, and 6 (21%) did not have deficits.
The potential role of vision deficits in DD has been con-
troversial, and it has been argued that specific vision-related
deficits are no more prevalent in individuals with DD than in
Figure 2. Mean (SE) Developmental EyeMovement Test Scores
for the Typically Developing(TD) Readers and Individuals
With Developmental Dyslexia (DD)
120
110
100
90
80
70
60
Standard Score
Vertical Horizontal Ratio Error
DD group
TD group
Vertical, F
1,61
=5.77;P= .02;horizontal, F
1,61
= 12.09; P= .001; ratio, F
1,61
= 4.72;
P= .03; and error, F
1,61
= 18.63; P< .001.
Figure 1. Distributionof Vergence and Accommodation Deficits Between
Individuals With Developmental Dyslexia and TypicallyDeveloping Readers
Vergence
4-Convergence
excess
1-Convergence
insufficiency
Accommodation
3-Accommodative insufficiency
Accommodation
9-Accommodative insufficiency
6-Accommodative dysfunction
1-Accommodative infacility
7-Convergence excess
3-Convergence
insufficiency
Vergence
2
1
4
2
8
8
Typically developing
readers (n
=
33)
Individuals with developmental dyslexia (n
=
29)
Overlapping circles indicate patients who had both vergence and
accommodation deficits. The numerals in the circles indicate number of
patients. The specific types of vergence and accommodation deficits are also
detailed (χ
2
= 15.6; difference, 41%; 95% CI, 18%-69%; P< .001).
Research Original Investigation Frequency of Visual Deficits in Children With Developmental Dyslexia
1092 JAMA Ophthalmology October 2018 Volume 136,Number 10 (Reprinted) jamaophthalmology.com
© 2018 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Downloaded From: by a Harvard University User on 10/12/2018
the general population.
3
The lack of a control group in prior
clinical studies has also been criticized.
15
In the present pro-
spective controlled study, however, we documented a higher
prevalence of vision deficits in a well-characterized group of
children with DD. Nevertheless, the contribution of these
deficits remains uncertain.
Vergence
Although convergence insufficiency has been viewed as a po-
tential problem in children with reading deficits,
16,17
the fre-
quency of children who met the criteria for a diagnosis of ver-
gence deficit was unexpectedly similar in our DD and TD
groups. Moreover, in both groups, the deficit was much more
likely to involve convergence excess than convergence insuf-
ficiency, which was infrequent in our sample. Prior studies of
vergence in DD have mostly assessed performance on ver-
gence tests rather than documenting a clinical diagnosis; hence,
their findings are not directly comparable to ours.
18-20
Con-
sistent with such studies,
18,19,21,22
we did find statistically
significant differences on individual vergence tests, includ-
ing the near point of convergence, positive fusional ranges at
near, and vergence facility. Several studies reported a diagno-
sis of vergence deficit in children who had a generic learning
disability but not necessarily dyslexia.
22,23
Ygge et al,
20
how-
ever, did not document diagnoses of vergence deficit even on
individual tests for children with dyslexia.
Accommodation
More than half the children in the DD group (16 [55%]) met the
criteria for a diagnosis of accommodation deficit compared with
only 3 (9%) in the TD group. Approximately two-thirdsof these
children in the DD group had accommodative insufficiency, and
the rest had generalized accommodative dysfunction. At the
level of individual tests, the amplitude of accommodation was
lower in the DD group than in the TD group, consistent with
other studies.
18,24
Children in the DD group recorded fewer
cycles on the monocular and binocular accommodative facil-
ity test, indicating that their accommodation dynamics were
slower to react than those of the TD group.
Ocular Motor Tracking
More than half of the children in the DD group (18 [62%]) ex-
hibited ocular motor tracking deficits when reading nonlin-
guistic stimuli (DEM). They also showed elevated deficits in
rate, fixations, and regressions on the Visagraph, even when
reading text at their grade-equivalent level with adequate
Figure 3. Mean (SE) Visagraph EyeTracker Parameters in TypicallyDeveloping (TD) Readers and Individuals
With Developmental Dyslexia (DD)
250
200
150
100
50
0
Reading Rate, Words/min
TD Group DD Group
at Grade
Level
DD Group
Below Grade
Level
Visagraph reading rate
A
300
250
200
150
100
50
0
Fixations, per 100 Words
TD Group DD Group
at Grade
Level
DD Group
Below Grade
Level
Visagraph fixations
B
70
60
50
40
30
20
10
0
Regressions, per 100 Words
TD Group DD Group
at Grade
Level
DD Group
Below Grade
Level
Visagraph regressions
C
A, Reading rate, F
2,59
= 15.63;
P< .001. B, Fixations, F
2,59
= 16.12;
P< .001. C, Regressions,
F
2,59
= 14.64; P< .001. In the DD
group, 15 children had a reading
proficiency at grade level, and 11
children had a reading proficiency
below grade level. For all 3
parameters, both DD subgroups
performed more poorly than the TD
group (P< .001) but not different
from each other.
Figure 4. Distribution of Visual Function Deficits Between Individuals With DevelopmentalDyslexia (DD) and
Typically Developing(TD) Readers
3
22
11
24
2
3
5
6
5
26
Ocular motor tracking
A
ccommo
d
atio
n
Accommo
d
ation
Ocular motor tracking
V
ergence
V
ergenc
e
No deficits
No deficits
Individuals with developmental dyslexia (n
=
29) Typically developing readers (n
=
33)
The numerals in the circles indicate
number of patients. The DD group
had more deficits (79%) than the TD
control group, and the deficits (33%)
clustered in subgroups of individuals
with DD (χ
2
= 13.18; difference, 46%;
95% CI, 23%-69%; P< .001).
Frequency of Visual Deficits in Children With Developmental Dyslexia Original Investigation Research
jamaophthalmology.com (Reprinted) JAMA Ophthalmology October 2018 Volume 136, Number 10 1093
© 2018 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Downloaded From: by a Harvard University User on 10/12/2018
comprehension. Some have questioned whether DEM is a valid
measure of saccadic eye movements.
25,26
In our study, DEM
horizontal gaze parameters had a moderate correlation with
Visagraph parameters, supporting its utility as a clinical
screener of ocular motor tracking in children.
Visual Function and Reading
Although this study documents a higher frequency of im-
paired visual function in children with DD, the association with
the reading process is uncertain. Clinically, patients with ac-
commodation deficits can report blurry vision at near, words
coming in and out of focus, difficulty maintaining clear vi-
sion while reading, and difficulty switching focus from dis-
tance to near.
27-29
Although the amplitude of accommoda-
tion of the DD group was not within a range expected to cause
near-vision work-related blur or asthenopic symptoms, the
push-up method we used overestimates amplitude of
accommodation.
30
Hence, the decrease in accommodation ob-
served in the DD group could have been greater had objective
measures been used, within a range that could cause diffi-
culty sustaining clear near vision.
30
Accommodative facility has been correlated with reading
prowess in children in the first grade,
31
and vergence facility
correlated significantly with the reading rate among children
with learning disabilities,
22
but, again, the direction of causa-
tion and functional significance is uncertain. The Conver-
gence Insufficiency Treatment Trials showed that eye exer-
cises improved near work-related symptoms in children with
reduced accommodation and convergence.
8,32
In children with
learning disabilities with nonstrabismic binocular vision
anomalies, eye exercises improved vergence, accommoda-
tion, and tracking, but their effect on reading was not studied.
33
Horizontal vergence training improved reading speed and
positive fusional vergence in an uncontrolled trial in children
with dyslexia and normal binocular vision.
24
More evidence
is needed to further establish the association between
vergence and accommodation deficits and reading metrics.
Tracking has also been associated with reading. Horizon-
tal DEM scores are correlated with the reading rate amongchil-
dren with dyslexia and poor readers.
34,35
In addition, beginning
readers exhibit more fixations, longerfixation duration, shorter
saccades, and more regressions than skilled readers, but these
deficits persist in children with dyslexia and are specific to
reading.
32-37
Because eye movements can be influenced by
higher-order cognitive and linguistic demands, such visual defi-
cits in children with dyslexia have been understood as effects
of the reading deficits rather than as a low-order pure ocular
motor deficit that might affect reading. Similarly, deficits in
ocular motor skills in children with DD are comparable to those
of reading age–matched controls (but inferior to those of
typically reading chronological age controls), suggesting an
immature ocular motor system in children with DD. Again,
whether this result is a cause or an effect of reading impair-
ment is uncertain.
36
In our study, children in the DD group read texts corre-
sponding to their single-word reading grade-equivalent
level. Although their comprehension was adequate, they still
made more fixations and regressions than children in the TD
group. These results do not necessarily support a causal role
for ocular motor deficits in children with dyslexia; instead, they
document the presence of ocular motor deficits even when the
children with dyslexia read passages at their readinglevel. Chil-
dren with dyslexia with short visual spans had more right-
ward fixations in reading text but performed similarly to con-
trols on a visual search task; these short visual spans influence
ocular motor tracking in dyslexia.
37
Such a mechanism could
also account for the ocular motor deficits in our DD group, even
when reading at their proficiency level.
Strengths and Limitations
The strengths of the study include the prospective, group
comparison design and the well-defined group with dyslexia
to ensure objective comparisons. The limitations are the
relatively small sample size and unmasked examiners. The
latter could be mitigated, in part, by the fact that most of the
data were collected electronically and not amenable to
examiner bias or influence.
Conclusions
This study demonstrates deficits in the visual functioning of
children with dyslexia. Although this comparative study does
not address any causal association of these deficits with read-
ing performance, the findings support further investigation of
visual function in dyslexia. We propose that assessmentof ver-
gence, accommodation, and eye movements may be helpful
in the initial evaluation of children with dyslexia and will
supplement the findings of a comprehensive ophthalmologic
examination and a detailed literacy evaluation. The study fur-
ther provides evidence that supports the design of controlled
cohort studies to determine whether treatment of these defi-
cits can bring about changes in visual symptoms and/or read-
ing parameters.
ARTICLE INFORMATION
Accepted for Publication: May 25, 2018.
Published Online: July 19, 2018.
doi:10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2018.2797
Author Affiliations: Department of
Ophthalmology, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston,
Massachusetts (Raghuram, Gowrisankaran,
Swanson, Hunter); Department of Ophthalmology,
Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts
(Raghuram, Hunter); Department of
Anesthesiology, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston,
Massachusetts (Zurakowski); Department of
Surgery, Boston Children’s Hospital, Boston,
Massachusetts (Zurakowski); Department of
Anesthesia, Harvard Medical School, Boston,
Massachusetts (Zurakowski); Division of
Psychology,Depar tment of Psychiatry, Boston
Children’s Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts
(Waber); Department of Psychiatry,Harvard
Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts (Waber).
Author Contributions: Drs Raghuram and Waber
had full access to all the data in the study and take
responsibility for the integrity of the data and the
accuracy of the data analysis.
Concept and design: Raghuram, Zurakowski,
Hunter, Waber.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All
authors.
Drafting of the manuscript: Raghuram, Zurakowski,
Waber.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important
intellectual content: All authors.
Statistical analysis: Raghuram, Zurakowski.
Obtained funding: Raghuram, Hunter.
Research Original Investigation Frequency of Visual Deficits in Children With Developmental Dyslexia
1094 JAMA Ophthalmology October 2018 Volume 136, Number 10 (Reprinted) jamaophthalmology.com
© 2018 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Downloaded From: by a Harvard University User on 10/12/2018
Administrative, technical, or material support:
Raghuram, Swanson.
Supervision: Raghuram, Hunter, Waber.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: All authors have
completed and submitted the ICMJE Form for
Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest and
none were reported.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by a
Knights TemplarCareer Development Grant and a
Discovery Award from the Children’sHospital
Ophthalmology Foundation (Dr Raghuram).
Role of the Funder/Sponsor:The funding sources
had no role in the design and conduct of the study;
collection, management, analysis, and
interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or
approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit
the manuscript for publication.
REFERENCES
1. Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of
Child Health and Human Development. Report of
the National Reading Panel: TeachingChildren to
Read (00-4769). Washington,DC: US Government
Printing Office; 2000.
2. Creavin AL, Lingam R, Steer C, Williams C.
Ophthalmic abnormalities and reading impairment.
Pediatrics. 2015;135(6):1057-1065.doi:10.1542/peds
.2014-3622
3. Handler SM, Fierson WM. Reading difficulties
and the pediatric ophthalmologist. J AAPOS.2017;
21(6):436-442. doi:10.1016/j.jaapos.2017.09.001
4. Garzia RP, Nicholson SB. Visual function and
reading disability: an optometric viewpoint. JAm
Optom Assoc. 1990;61(2):88-97.
5. Hoffman LG. Incidence of vision difficulties in
children with learning disabilities. JAmOptomAssoc.
1980;51(5):447-451.
6. Rosner J, Rosner J. Comparison of visual
characteristics in children with and without learning
difficulties. Am J Optom Physiol Opt. 1987;64(7):
531-533. doi:10.1097/00006324-198707000
-00008
7. World Medical Association. World Medical
Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical
principles for medical research involving human
subjects. JAMA. 2013;310(20):2191-2194. doi:10
.1001/jama.2013.281053
8. Convergence Insufficiency Treatment Trial
(CITT) Study Group. The Convergence Insufficiency
Treatment Trial: design, methods, and baseline
data. Ophthalmic Epidemiol. 2008;15(1):24-36.
doi:10.1080/09286580701772037
9. Pediatric Eye Disease Investigator Group.
Home-based therapy for symptomatic convergence
insufficiency in children: a randomized clinical trial.
Optom Vis Sci. 2016;93(12):1457-1465. doi:10.1097
/OPX.0000000000000975
10. Scheiman M, Mitchell GL, Cotter S, et al;
Convergence Insufficiency Treatment Trial Study
Group. A randomized clinical trial of treatments for
convergence insufficiency in children. Arch
Ophthalmol. 2005;123(1):14-24.doi:10.1001/archopht
.12 3.1 .14
11. Scheiman M, Wick B. Clinical Management of
Binocular Vision: Heterophoric, Accommodative,
and Eye Movement Disorders.3rd ed. Philadelphia,
PA: Wolters Kluwer Health/Lippincott Williams &
Wilkins; 2008.
12. Brown LD, Cai TT, DasguptaA . Interval
estimation for a binomial proportion. Stat Sci. 2001;
16(2):101-117. doi:10.1214/ss/1009213286
13. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false
discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to
multiple testing. J R Stat Soc B. 1995;57(1):289-300.
14. Hulsen T, de Vlieg J, AlkemaW. BioVenn—a web
application for the comparison and visualization of
biological lists using area-proportional Venn
diagrams. BMC Genomics. 2008;9:488. doi:10.1186
/1471-2164-9-488
15. American Academy of Pediatrics, Section on
Ophthalmology, Council on Children with
Disabilities; American Academy of Ophthalmology;
American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology
and Strabismus; American Association of Certified
Orthoptists. Joint statement—learning disabilities,
dyslexia, and vision. Pediatrics. 2009;124(2):837-844.
doi:10.1542/peds.2009-1445
16. Borsting E, Mitchell GL, Kulp MT, et al; CITT
Study Group. Improvement in academic behaviors
after successful treatment of convergence
insufficiency. Optom Vis Sci. 2012;89(1):12-18. doi:
10.1097/OPX.0b013e318238ffc3
17. Scheiman M, Mitchell GL, Cotter SA, et al;
CITT-ART Investigator Group. Convergence
Insufficiency Treatment Trial–Attention and
Reading Trial(CIT T-ART):design and methods. Vis
Dev Rehabil. 2015;1(3):214-228.
18. Evans BJ, Drasdo N, Richards IL. Investigation
of accommodative and binocular function in
dyslexia. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 1994;14(1):5-19.
doi:10.1111/j.1475-1313.1994.tb00550.x
19. Latvala ML, Korhonen TT, PenttinenM, Laippala
P. Ophthalmic findings in dyslexic schoolchildren. Br
J Ophthalmol. 1994;78(5):339-343.doi:10.1136/bjo
.78.5.339
20. Ygge J, Lennerstrand G, RydbergA , Wijecoon S,
Pettersson BM. Oculomotor functions in a Swedish
population of dyslexic and normally reading
children. Acta Ophthalmol (Copenh). 1993;71(1):
10-21. doi:10.1111/j.1755-3768.1993.tb04953.x
21. Kapoula Z, Bucci MP. Posturalcontrol in dyslexic
and non-dyslexic children. J Neurol. 2007;254(9):
1174-1183. doi:10.1007/s00415-006-0460-0
22. Quaid P, Simpson T. Association between
reading speed, cycloplegic refractive error, and
oculomotor function in reading disabled children
versus controls. Graefes Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol.
2013;251(1):169-187. doi:10.1007/s00417-012
-2135-0
23. Dusek W, Pierscionek BK, McClelland JF.
A survey of visual function in an Austrian
population of school-age children with reading and
writing difficulties. BMC Ophthalmol. 2010;10:16.
doi:10.1186/1471-2415-10-16
24. Wahlberg-Ramsay M, Nordström M, Salkic J,
Brautaset R. Evaluation of aspects of binocular
vision in children with dyslexia. Strabismus. 2012;
20(4):139-144. doi:10.3109/09273972.2012.735335
25. Ayton LN, Abel LA, Fricke TR, McBrien NA.
Developmental eye movement test: what is it really
measuring? Optom Vis Sci. 2009;86(6):722-730.
doi:10.1097/OPX.0b013e3181a6a4b3
26. Webber A, Wood J, Gole G, Brown B. DEM test,
Visagraph eye movement recordings, and reading
ability in children. Optom Vis Sci. 2011;88(2):295-302.
doi:10.1097/OPX.0b013e31820846c0
27. Daum KM. Accommodative dysfunction. Doc
Ophthalmol. 1983;55(3):177-198. doi:10.1007
/BF00140808
28. Hennessey D, Iosue RA, Rouse MW. Relation of
symptoms to accommodative infacility of
school-aged children. Am J Optom Physiol Opt.
1984;61(3):177-183. doi:10.1097/00006324
-198403000-00005
29. Levine S, Ciuffreda KJ, Selenow A, Flax N.
Clinical assessment of accommodative facility in
symptomatic and asymptomatic individuals. JAm
Optom Assoc. 1985;56(4):286-290.
30. Anderson HA, Stuebing KK. Subjective versus
objective accommodative amplitude: preschool to
presbyopia. Optom Vis Sci. 2014;91(11):1290-1301.
doi:10.1097/OPX.0000000000000402
31. Kulp MT, Schmidt PP. Visual predictors of
reading performance in kindergarten and first grade
children. Optom Vis Sci. 1996;73(4):255-262.doi:10
.1097/00006324-199604000-00007
32. Convergence Insufficiency Treatment Trial
Study Group. Randomized clinical trial of
treatments for symptomatic convergence
insufficiency in children. Arch Ophthalmol. 2008;
126(10):1336-1349.doi:10.1001/archopht.126.10.1336
33. Hussaindeen JR, Shah P, Ramani KK,
Ramanujan L. Efficacy of vision therapy in children
with learning disability and associated binocular
vision anomalies. J Optom. 2018;11(1):40-48. doi:10
.1016/j.optom.2017.02.002
34. Palomo-Alvarez C, Puell MC. Relationship
between oculomotor scanning determined by the
DEM test and a contextual reading test in
schoolchildren with reading difficulties. Graefes
Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2009;247(9):1243-1249.
doi:10.1007/s00417-009-1076-8
35. Northway N. Predicting the continued use of
overlays in school children—a comparison of the
Developmental Eye Movement testand the Rate of
Reading test. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2003;23(5):
457-464. doi:10.1046/j.1475-1313.2003.00144.x
36. Bucci MP, Nassibi N, Gerard CL, Bui-Quoc E,
Seassau M. Immaturity of the oculomotor saccade
and vergence interaction in dyslexic children:
evidence from a reading and visual search study.
PLoS One. 2012;7(3):e33458. doi:10.1371/journal
.pone.0033458
37. Prado C, Dubois M, Valdois S. The eye
movements of dyslexic children during reading and
visual search: impact of the visual attention span.
Vision Res. 2007;47(19):2521-2530. doi:10.1016/j
.visres.2007.06.001
Frequency of Visual Deficits in Children With Developmental Dyslexia Original Investigation Research
jamaophthalmology.com (Reprinted) JAMA Ophthalmology October 2018 Volume 136, Number 10 1095
© 2018 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.
Downloaded From: by a Harvard University User on 10/12/2018
... Dyslexics have issues with spelling, word recognition, and decoding words. These deficits manifest themselves as difficulty with reading comprehension, with writing, and slower reading [6][7][8]. Despite being so common, it has been traditionally very difficult to present a clear pathologic origin for the disorder. ...
... Outside of the behavioral model for dyslexia, others have postulated there may be a physiologic basis for dyslexia as well. For example, dyslexics have demonstrated abnormalities in visual processing, saccadic and vergence eye movements, and visual rehabilitation is widely used to improve symptoms in dyslexics [7,[11][12][13][14][15]. Despite these controversies, there has never been a longitudinal or randomized control trial to further investigate these relationships between these physiologic and behavioral differences. ...
Article
Full-text available
Dyslexic children have been shown to be more creative than their non-dyslexic counterparts. They have also been shown to have an abnormal oculomotor profile while viewing targets in free space, making vergence or saccadic eye movements while reading or when viewing Op art. They show a slower deceleration of their eye movements and a difficulty in coordinating their two eyes to obtain single fused vision in depth. Interestingly, their abnormal oculo-motor profile is exacerbated while reading more difficult texts. Given these differences, we postulate that dyslexics’ increased creativity may be related to their different eye movement control affecting how they perceive the world. Therefore, we decided to measure adolescent dyslexics’ creativity, oculomotor profile, and subjective responses while they viewed three paintings by Magritte. These were chosen to stimulate the perception of hidden conceptual spaces or stimulate conflict between the perception of the figural and textural content. For the first time to our knowledge, dyslexic adolescents were demonstrated to be more creative in terms of flexibility and fluidity than their non-dyslexic peers. Subjectively, while viewing the Magritte paintings, dyslexics reported fewer conceptual spaces and fewer hidden words than their non-dyslexic peers; thus, they confabulated less than non-dyslexics. Dyslexics also demonstrated an abnormal oculomotor profile similar to those that we have shown when reading, viewing randomized targets, and while perceiving illusions of depth in Op art paintings, in that they demonstrated difficulty with disconjugation and abnormalities in their eye velocity profiles. We propose there may be a link between dyslexic increased creativity and their eye movement abnormalities. Similar to reading nonsense text, we propose that Magritte’s contradictory paintings exacerbate dyslexics’ eye movement abnormalities. These eye movement abnormalities while viewing these particular paintings might provide a physiological signature suggesting a contribution of their unusual eye control to their higher creativity scores.
... According to Stein & Fowler (1981), 63% of children with dyslexia showed unstable eye dominance while only 1% of typical readers had such problems. More recently, Raghuram et al. (2018) reported that dyslexic participants have difficulties in making vergence eye movements and showed abnormal accommodation and ocular training effects. It is therefore not surprising that many dyslexic readers who have these symptoms report that occluding one eye can improve reading (Stein et al., 2000). ...
... Nearly 30 years ago, Slaghuis et al. (1993) wrote: "…despite evidence for the involvement of perceptual factors in the etiology of dyslexia the prevailing view is that the disorder is almost entirely due to language related difficulty". It is interesting to compare this to what Raghuram et al., say in 2018(Raghuram et al., 2018: "…little is known about the integrity of visual function in [developmental dyslexia]". This sounds as if little progress has been made in 25 years. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
What are the causes of dyslexia? Decades of research reflect a determined search for a single cause where a common assumption is that dyslexia is a consequence of problems with converting phonological information into lexical codes. But reading is a highly complex activity requiring many different mechanisms, and a number of different visual problems have been documented in dyslexic readers. We review evidence from various sources for the role of visual factors in dyslexia, highlighting among others recent proposals that problems with high-level vision contribute to dyslexia. We believe that the role of visual problems in dyslexia has been underestimated in the literature, to the detriment of the understanding and treatment of dyslexia. We propose that rather than focusing on a single cause of dyslexia, the questions that need to be answered involve: When does each dysfunction apply, how do these dysfunctions interact, and what symptoms can each account for?
... In fact, DDiVAT's operator-assisted mode could be used for visual acuity examination of preschool minors, with his/her guardian or teacher as the application's operator. Therefore, it may contribute to the prevention of amblyopia and vision-related learning disabilities, by increasing the awareness of the parents, especially in vulnerable populations [10,27]. ...
Article
Full-text available
(1) Background: While smartphones are among the primary devices used in telemedical applications, smart TV healthcare apps are not prevalent despite smart TVs’ penetrance in home settings. The present study’s objective was to develop and validate the first smart TV-based visual acuity (VA) test (Democritus Digital Visual Acuity Test (DDiVAT)) that allows a reliable VA self-assessment. (2) Methods: This is a prospective validation study. DDiVAT introduces several advanced features for reliable VA self-testing; among them: automatic calibration, voice recognition, voice guidance, automatic calculation of VA indexes, and a smart TV-based messaging system. Normal and low vision participants were included in the validation. DDiVAT VA results (VADDiVAT) were compared against the ones from: (a) the gold-standard conventional ETDRS (VAETDRS), and, (b) an independent ophthalmologist who monitored the self-examination testing (VARES). Comparisons were performed by noninferiority test (set at 2.5-letters) and intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs). DDiVAT’s test-retest reliability was assessed within a 15-day time-window. (3) Results: A total of 300 participants (185 and 115 with normal and low vision, respectively) responded to ETDRS and DDiVAT. Mean difference in letters was −0.05 for VAETDRS–VARES, 0.62 for VARES–VADDiVAT, and 0.67 for VAETDRS–VADDiVAT, significantly lower than the 2.5 letter noninferiority margin. ICCs indicated an excellent level of agreement, collectively and for each group (0.922-0.996). All displayed letters in DDiVAT presented a similar difficulty. The overall accuracy of the voice recognition service was 96.01%. ICC for VADDiVAT test-retest was 0.957. (4) Conclusions: The proposed DDiVAT presented non-significant VA differences with the ETDRS, suggesting that it can be used for accurate VA self-assessment in telemedical settings, both for normal and low-vision patients.
... 32 45 46 It is therefore surprising that some researchers investigate the effect of coloured filters on populations selected solely because they have a diagnosis of dyslexia or reading difficulties. Several studies are not included in this book (e.g., [47][48][49][50][51] ) because one of the factors in the tables in this chapter means their results are unlikely to be meaningful. ...
Chapter
This chapter is aimed at researchers and clinicians. It starts with reflections on visual stress. The multifactorial nature of reading difficulties is noted. Recommendations are made for research on both these topics, priorities for research, both concerning vision and reading difficulties and noting some of the pitfalls of research in this field that are relevant to different research designs. Suggestions for eye care practitioners are then addressed, noting the importance of research evidence, patient (and parent) preferences, and clinical state and circumstances. The chapter concludes with comments for educational professionals, parents, and students.
... 32 45 46 It is therefore surprising that some researchers investigate the effect of coloured filters on populations selected solely because they have a diagnosis of dyslexia or reading difficulties. Several studies are not included in this book (e.g., [47][48][49][50][51] ) because one of the factors in the tables in this chapter means their results are unlikely to be meaningful. ...
Chapter
In this chapter we review studies in which precision tints have been used in a variety of neurological disorders, including photosensitive epilepsy, autism, migraine, cluster headache, visual snow, stroke, multiple sclerosis and concussion. Whilst the evidence regarding these topics is preliminary and indicative rather than conclusive, the effects of coloured filters in these conditions may help to further elucidate the mechanisms underlying the benefit from colour. In particular, the extent to which these findings provide convergent support for the cortical hyperexcitability hypothesis is discussed.
... Typically, dyslexia is considered a primary learning disability. Usually, the typical deficit in dyslexia consists of difficulty with word recognition, decoding, and spelling, which presents as trouble with reading (in terms of decreased comprehension and reading speed) and writing [24,25]. Dyslexia is therefore often considered to be a behavioral disability. ...
Article
Full-text available
Op art was created, in part, to produce illusions of movement. Given that dyslexics have been shown to have impaired visuo-postural axis deficits, it may be possible that dyslexics see illusions different than their non-dyslexic peers. To test this theory, we measured eye movement and posture in 47 dyslexic (18 female, 29 male; mean age 15.4) and 44 non dyslexic (22 female, 22 male; mean age 14.8) adolescents while they viewed three works of art by Op artist Bridget Riley. They then responded to a questionnaire about how they felt while viewing the artworks. Dyslexics demonstrated significantly slower saccades in terms of average velocity that was particularly disturbed in paintings that manipulated depth. Subjectively, dyslexics felt much more destabilized compared to their peers; however, there was not a significant difference in objective postural measurements between the two groups. The sensation of destabilization was positively correlated with appreciation in non-dyslexic adolescents. These subjective results suggest that dyslexics may be more sensitive to movement in depth, which could be related to the instability in vergence movements. Whereas this instability represents a hinderance in relation to reading, it could be an advantage while viewing paintings such as these.
Article
Significance: The high frequency of vergence and accommodation deficits coexisting in patients with a vestibular diagnosis merits a detailed visual function examination. Purpose: Deficits in vergence and saccades have been reported in patients with vestibular symptomatology. We retrospectively evaluated visual function deficits in adolescents with vestibular diagnoses and concussion. Methods: Inclusion criteria: vestibular and optometric evaluations between 2014 and 2020; 6 to <22 years old; 20/25 best-corrected vision or better. Clinical criteria assigned vestibular diagnoses and concussion diagnoses. Vestibular diagnoses included vestibular migraine, benign paroxysmal positional vertigo, and persistent postural perceptual dizziness. Visual function deficits were compared to a pediatric control group (30). Non-parametric statistics assessed differences in group distribution. Results: 153 patients were included: 18 had vestibular diagnoses only, 62 had vestibular diagnoses related to concussion, and 73 had concussion only. Vergence deficits were more frequent in patients with vestibular diagnoses and concussion (42%) and concussion only (34%) compared to controls (3%; all P = .02). Accommodation deficits were more frequent in patients with vestibular diagnoses only (67%), vestibular diagnoses and concussion (71%), and concussion (58%) compared to controls (13%; all P = .002). Patients with vestibular migraine and concussion (21) had more vergence deficits (62%) and accommodation insufficiency (52%) than concussion only patients (47%; P = .02; 29%; P = .04). Patients with benign paroxysmal positional vertigo and concussion (20) had lower positive fusional vergence and failed near vergence facility (35%) more than concussion only patients (16%; P = .03). Conclusions: Visual function deficits were observed at a high frequency in patients with a vestibular diagnosis with or without a concussion and particularly in vestibular migraine or benign paroxysmal positional vertigo. Visual function assessments may be important for patients with vestibular diagnoses.
Article
Whether oculomotor deficits are common in reading disorders (RD), and related to reading ability in general, has been debated for decades. Since RD are frequently comorbid with the Neurofibromatosis type1 (NF1), children with NF1 were used as a comparison group for children with dyslexia in this study. Eye movements were recorded while 21 dyslexic, 20 NF1, and 20 typically developing children performed an oculomotor lateralized bisection task. The type of stimulus - discrete (words and strings of hashes) versus continuous (solid lines) - and the stimulus presentation side (left vs. right) were manipulated. The results showed that (1) only proficient readers (TD and NF1 without RD) showed fully developed oculomotor mechanisms for efficient reading, with a clear preferred viewing location located halfway between the beginning and the middle of the word in both visual fields, and fine-tuned saccade targeting guided by the between-character space information and (2) NF1 poor readers mirrored the dyslexic eye movement behaviour, with less accurateness and more variability in saccadic programming, no sensibility to the discreteness of the stimuli, particularly in the left visual field. We concluded that disruption to oculomotor behaviour reflects the fact that many of the component processes in reading have not become automatized for children with RD, independently of NF1. This suggests that the differences in saccade targeting strategy between children with and without RD would be secondary consequences of their reduced reading experience.
Chapter
There are several visual pathways that convey different types of information to the brain. We review evidence that in dyslexic individuals there is a deficit in one of these pathways, the magnocellular pathway-dorsal (transient; M-D) pathway. A large body of research supports an M-D deficit as a correlate of dyslexia, but the evidence that the deficit plays a causal role in reading difficulty is less compelling. The M-D hypothesis is linked to impaired visuospatial attention in reading difficulty, and to difficulties in processing crowded contours. Preliminary evidence concerning the treatment of these deficits with action video games is discussed.
Article
Background. The ability to read is the key to successful education of schoolchildren and students. At the same time, according to various authors, from 5–10 to 20–30% of the population experience difficulties, characterized as “reading below the level of language proficiency”, “reading difficulties” and “dyslexia”. This work is devoted to the analysis of modern Russian and foreign literature in the field of aetiopathogenesis, diagnosis, clinical manifestations and functional correction of visual disorders in dyslexia. Aim: to study the literature data on visual impairment in patients with dyslexia. Materials and methods . The literature analysis of publications available on PubMed, eLibrary, CyberLeninka and Crossref Metadata Search was carried out. Results. The conducted analysis demonstrates the importance of effective detection of visual disorders in patients with dyslexia, which is necessary for a correct comprehensive assessment of this condition, as well as understanding the causes and nature of difficulties experienced by a child with dyslexia in the process of learning to read. The results of the analysis of studies reveal the possibilities of positive dynamics in the development of reading skills in patients with dyslexia in the process of functional treatment aimed at developing binocular visual functions, improving the functioning of the oculomotor system and the accommodation. Conclusion. The authors conclude that an in-depth ophthalmic examination and treatment of the identified ophthalmic pathology is of great importance in the overall complex of diagnostic and corrective measures in patients with dyslexia.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: To report the frequency of binocular vision (BV) anomalies in children with specific learning disorders (SLD) and to assess the efficacy of vision therapy (VT) in children with a non-strabismic binocular vision anomaly (NSBVA). Methods: The study was carried out at a centre for learning disability (LD). Comprehensive eye examination and binocular vision assessment was carried out for 94 children (mean (SD) age: 15 (2.2) years) diagnosed with specific learning disorder. BV assessment was done for children with best corrected visual acuity of ≥6/9 - N6, cooperative for examination and free from any ocular pathology. For children with a diagnosis of NSBVA (n=46), 24 children were randomized to VT and no intervention was provided to the other 22 children who served as experimental controls. At the end of 10 sessions of vision therapy, BV assessment was performed for both the intervention and non-intervention groups. Results: Binocular vision anomalies were found in 59 children (62.8%) among which 22% (n=13) had strabismic binocular vision anomalies (SBVA) and 78% (n=46) had a NSBVA. Accommodative infacility (AIF) was the commonest of the NSBVA and found in 67%, followed by convergence insufficiency (CI) in 25%. Post-vision therapy, the intervention group showed significant improvement in all the BV parameters (Wilcoxon signed rank test, p<0.05) except negative fusional vergence. Conclusion: Children with specific learning disorders have a high frequency of binocular vision disorders and vision therapy plays a significant role in improving the BV parameters. Children with SLD should be screened for BV anomalies as it could potentially be an added hindrance to the reading difficulty in this special population.
Article
Full-text available
Objective: To describe the design and methodology of the Convergence Insufficiency Treatment Trial: Attention and Reading Trial (CITT-ART), the first randomized clinical trial evaluating the effect of vision therapy on reading and attention in school-age children with symptomatic convergence insufficiency (CI). Methods: CITT-ART is a multicenter, placebo-controlled, randomized clinical trial of 324 children ages 9 to 14 years in grades 3 to 8 with symptomatic CI. Participants are randomized to 16 weeks of office-based vergence/accommodative therapy (OBVAT) or placebo therapy (OBPT), both supplemented with home therapy. The primary outcome measure is the change in the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test-Version 3 (WIAT-III) reading comprehension subtest score. Secondary outcome measures are changes in attention as measured by the Strengths and Weaknesses of Attention (SWAN) as reported by parents and teachers, tests of binocular visual function, and other measures of reading and attention. The long-term effects of treatment are assessed 1 year after treatment completion. All analyses will test the null hypothesis of no difference in outcomes between the two treatment groups. The study is entering its second year of recruitment. The final results will contribute to a better understanding of the relationship between the treatment of symptomatic CI and its effect on reading and attention. Conclusion: The study will provide an evidence base to help parents, eye professionals, educators, and other health care providers make informed decisions as they care for children with CI and reading and attention problems. Results may also generate additional hypothesis and guide the development of other scientific investigations of the relationships between visual disorders and other developmental disorders in children.
Article
Full-text available
To explore associations between specific learning disorder with impairment in reading (dyslexia) and ophthalmic abnormalities in children aged 7 to 9 years. Cross-sectional analysis was performed on cohort study data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Reading impairment was defined according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition criteria. Children who achieved >2 SD below the mean in the Neale Analysis of Reading Ability Scale II and level <4 in nonmathematical national key stage 2 tests were defined as having severe reading impairment (SRI). Children with blindness or IQ <70 were excluded. Data were available for 5822 children, of whom 172 (3%) met the criteria for SRI. No association was found between SRI and strabismus, motor fusion, sensory fusion at a distance, refractive error, amblyopia, convergence, accommodation, or contrast sensitivity. Abnormalities in sensory fusion at near were mildly higher in children with SRI compared with their peers (1 in 6 vs 1 in 10, P = .08), as were children with stereoacuity worse than 60 seconds/arc (1 in 6 vs 1 in 10, P = .001). Four of every 5 children with SRI had normal ophthalmic function in each test used. A small minority of children displayed minor anomalies in stereoacuity or fusion of near targets. The slight excess of these children among those with SRI may be a result of their reading impairment or may be unrelated. We found no evidence that vision-based treatments would be useful to help children with SRI. Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics.
Article
Approximately 20% of children have dyslexia, a language-based reading disability. A variation in language processing in the brain leads to a deficit in phonological (auditory) processing, which leads to problems in learning to read, write, and spell. Myths continue to exist regarding dyslexia and vision, and although eye and vision problems may coexist with dyslexia, they are not more prevalent than in the general population. Rarely vision problems may make reading at near very difficult and may masquerade as a learning problem or attention deficit disorder/attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. The pediatric ophthalmologist can play a valuable role in determining whether any eye or vision problems exist that might interfere with learning or reading. Treatments to improve these eye conditions may help make reading more comfortable, but they are not a therapy for coexisting dyslexia. The use of vision therapy has never been shown scientifically to be effective and may prevent the application of effective interventions during the critical period of development when reading disorders can best be remediated. The pediatric ophthalmologist should educate parents about reading and dyslexia and provide prompt referral to professionals who have expertise in evaluating and treating learning disabilities.
Article
Purpose. To compare the effectiveness of home-based (HB) computer vergence/accommodative therapy (HB-C) to HB near target push-up therapy (HB-PU) and to HB placebo treatment (HB-P) among children aged 9 to G18 years with symptomatic convergence insufficiency (CI). Methods. In this multicenter randomized clinical trial, participants were randomly assigned to computer therapy, near target push-ups, or placebo. All therapy was prescribed for 5 days per week at home. A successful outcome at 12 weeks was based on meeting predetermined composite criteria for the CI Symptom Survey, near point of convergence, and positive fusional vergence at near. Results. A total of 204 participants were randomly assigned to HB-C (n = 75), HB-PU (n = 85), or HB-P (n = 44). At 12 weeks, 16 of 69 (23%, 95% CI: 14Y35%) in the HB-C group, 15 of 69 (22%, 95% CI: 13Y33%) in the HB-PU group, and 5 of 31 (16%, 95% CI: 5Y34%) in the HB-P group were classified as having a successful outcome. The difference in the percentage of participants with a successful outcome in the HB-C group compared with the HB-PU group was j4% (two-sided 97.5% CI: j19 to +11%; p = 0.56) and with the HB-P group was +5% (two-sided 97.5% CI: j12 to +22%; p = 0.52), adjusted for baseline levels of the composite outcome components. Conclusions. The majority of participants with symptomatic CI did not have a successful outcome at 12 weeks. Some participants treated with placebo were successful.With recruitment reaching only 34% of that originally planned and differential loss to follow-up among groups, estimates of success are not precise and comparisons across groups are difficult to interpret. (Optom Vis Sci 2016;93:00Y00)
Article
Objective: This report describes the design and methodology of the Convergence Insufficiency Treatment Trial (CITT), the first large-scale, placebo-controlled, randomized clinical trial evaluating treatments for convergence insufficiency (CI) in children. We also report the clinical and demographic characteristics of patients. Methods: We prospectively randomized children 9 to 17 years of age to one of four treatment groups: 1) home-based pencil push-ups, 2) home-based computer vergence/accommodative therapy and pencil push-ups, 3) office-based vergence/accommodative therapy with home reinforcement, 4) office-based placebo therapy. Outcome data on the Convergence Insufficiency Symptom Survey (CISS) score (primary outcome), near point of convergence (NPC), and positive fusional vergence were collected after 12 weeks of active treatment and again at 6 and 12 months posttreatment. Results: The CITT enrolled 221 children with symptomatic CI with a mean age of 12.0 years (SD = +2.3). The clinical profile of the cohort at baseline was 9Delta exophoria at near (+/- 4.4) and 2Delta exophoria (+/-2.8) at distance, CISS score = 30 (+/-9.0), NPC = 14 cm (+/- 7.5), and near positive fusional vergence break = 13 Delta (+/- 4.6). There were no statistically significant nor clinically relevant differences between treatment groups with respect to baseline characteristics (p > 0.05). Conclusion: Hallmark features of the study design include formal definitions of conditions and outcomes, standardized diagnostic and treatment protocols, a placebo treatment arm, masked outcome examinations, and the CISS score outcome measure. The baseline data reported herein define the clinical profile of those enrolled into the CITT.
Article
Learning disabilities, including reading disabilities, are commonly diagnosed in children. Their etiologies are multifactorial, reflecting genetic influences and dysfunction of brain systems. Learning disabilities are complex problems that require complex solutions. Early recognition and referral to qualified educational professionals for evidence-based evaluations and treatments seem necessary to achieve the best possible outcome. Most experts believe that dyslexia is a language-based disorder. Vision problems can interfere with the process of learning; however, vision problems are not the cause of primary dyslexia or learning disabilities. Scientific evidence does not support the efficacy of eye exercises, behavioral vision therapy, or special tinted filters or lenses for improving the long-term educational performance in these complex pediatric neurocognitive conditions. Diagnostic and treatment approaches that lack scientific evidence of efficacy, including eye exercises, behavioral vision therapy, or special tinted filters or lenses, are not endorsed and should not be recommended. Pediatrics 2009; 124: 837-844
Article
This study compared subjective and objective accommodative amplitudes to characterize changes from preschool to presbyopia. Monocular accommodative amplitude was measured with three techniques in random order (subjective push-up, objective minus lens stimulated, and objective proximal stimulated) on 236 subjects aged 3 to 64 years using a 1.5-mm letter. Subjective push-up amplitudes were the dioptric distance at which the target first blurred along a near-point rod. Objective minus lens stimulated amplitudes were the greatest accommodative response obtained by Grand Seiko autorefraction as subjects viewed the stimulus at 33 cm through increasing minus lens powers. Objective proximal stimulated amplitudes were the greatest accommodative response obtained by Grand Seiko autorefraction as subjects viewed the stimulus at increasing proximity from 40 cm up to 3.33 cm. In comparison with subjective push-up amplitudes, objective amplitudes were lower at all ages, with the most dramatic difference occurring in the 3- to 5-year group (subjective push-up, 16.00 ± 4.98 diopters [D] vs. objective proximal stimulated, 7.94 ± 2.37 D, and objective lens stimulated, 6.20 ± 1.99 D). Objective proximal and lens stimulated amplitudes were largest in the 6- to 10-year group (8.81 ± 1.24 D and 8.05 ± 1.82 D, respectively) and gradually decreased until the fourth decade of life when a rapid decline to presbyopia occurred. There was a significant linear relationship between objective techniques (y = 0.74 + 0.96x, R = 0.85, p < 0.001) with greater amplitudes measured for the proximal stimulated technique (mean difference, 0.55 D). Objective measurements of accommodation demonstrate that accommodative amplitude is substantially less than that measured by the subjective push-up technique, particularly in young children. These findings have important clinical implications for the management of uncorrected hyperopia.
Article
We revisit the problem of interval estimation of a binomial proportion. The erratic behavior of the coverage probability of the standard Wald confidence interval has previously been remarked on in the literature (Blyth and Still, Agresti and Coull, Santner and others). We begin by showing that the chaotic coverage properties of the Wald interval are far more persistent than is appreciated. Furthermore, common textbook prescriptions regarding its safety are misleading and defective in several respects and cannot be trusted. This leads us to consideration of alternative intervals. A number of natural alternatives are presented, each with its motivation and context. Each interval is examined for its coverage probability and its length. Based on this analysis, we recommend the Wilson interval or the equal-tailed Jeffreys prior interval for small n and the interval suggested in Agresti and Coull for larger n. We also provide an additional frequentist justification for use of the Jeffreys interval.
Article
Background: Prevalence of dyslexia is 5%-10% of the population. Opinions differ on how binocular function affects dyslexia. The aim of the present study was to evaluate the binocular function in dyslexic children and compare it with a group of age-matched control children. Methods: The study was performed at Ängkärrskolan and at Kungsholmen elementary schools in Stockholm, Sweden. Sixty-three children with dyslexia and 60 control children between fourth and ninth grade participated. Monocular and binocular visual acuity, refractive error, best corrected visual acuity at distance and near, near point of convergence, amplitude of accommodation, stereopsis, phorias, and fusional reserves were evaluated in all of the children. Results: The results show that there was no difference in the visual functions tested except for the amplitude of accommodation, which was found to be reduced both monocularly and binocularly in the dyslexic children. Conclusion: This study showed that only the amplitude of accommodation seems to differ in children with dyslexia as compared with the control children; however, the ability to accommodate was still good and is unlikely to hamper reading and learning ability. The results therefore support that the recent findings of binocular deficits in dyslexic children are a result of the phonological deficit of dyslexia and not an underlying cause of dyslexia.