ArticlePDF Available

Cohesion is Associated with Perceived Exertion and Enjoyment during Group Running

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Carnes AJ, Mahoney SE. Cohesion is Associated with Perceived Exertion and Enjoyment during Group Running. JEPonline 2016; 19(6):24-39. The purpose of this study was to determine if interval running with a group affects average speed, perceived exertion (RPE), and/or enjoyment in recreational runners, and if these variables are associated with cohesion and/or social support. Twenty adult runners performed two trials under different social conditions (alone, group), consisting of high intensity intervals. Average interval time, enjoyment, and RPE were compared between trials. Social support and cohesion were assessed separately. There were no main or interaction effects on average speed (P>0.87), RPE (P>0.08), or enjoyment (P>0.26). Task cohesion (r =-.58, P=0.01) and social support (r =-.73, P=0.001) were negatively associated with RPE in the group condition only, and positively associated with enjoyment. Running with a group did not affect speed, enjoyment, or RPE during an interval workout. However, higher perceived task cohesion and social support were associated with lower perceived exertion and greater enjoyment during group running. While the group environment did not augment the subjects' average running speed during a high intensity interval workout, group training may nonetheless furnish psychological benefits that could aid in the completion of challenging, high intensity training sessions.
Content may be subject to copyright.
24
Journal of Exercise Physiology
online
December 2016
Volume 19 Number 6
Editor
-
in
-
Chief
Tommy Boone, PhD, MBA
Review Board
Todd Astorino, PhD
Julien Baker, PhD
Steve Brock, PhD
Lance Dalleck, PhD
Eric Goulet, PhD
Robert Gotshall, PhD
Alexander Hutchison, PhD
M. Knight-Maloney, PhD
Len Kravitz, PhD
James Laskin, PhD
Yit Aun Lim, PhD
Lonnie Lowery, PhD
Derek Marks, PhD
Cristine Mermier, PhD
Robert Robergs, PhD
Chantal Vella, PhD
Dale Wagner, PhD
Frank Wyatt, PhD
Ben Zhou, PhD
Official Research Journal
of the American Society of
Exercise Physiologists
ISSN 1097-9751
Official Research Journal of
the American Society of
Exercise Physiologists
ISSN 1097-9751
JEPonline
Cohesion is Associated with Perceived Exertion and
Enjoyment during Group Running
Andrew J. Carnes, Sara E. Mahoney
Bellarmine University, Department of Exercise Science, Louisville,
KY, United States
ABSTRACT
Carnes AJ, Mahoney SE. Cohesion is Associated with Perceived
Exertion and Enjoyment during Group Running. JEPonline 2016;
19(6):24-39. The purpose of this study was to determine if interval
running with a group affects average speed, perceived exertion
(RPE), and/or enjoyment in recreational runners, and if these
variables are associated with cohesion and/or social support. Twenty
adult runners performed two trials under different social conditions
(alone, group), consisting of high intensity intervals. Average interval
time, enjoyment, and RPE were compared between trials. Social
support and cohesion were assessed separately. There were no
main or interaction effects on average speed (P>0.87), RPE
(P>0.08), or enjoyment (P>0.26). Task cohesion (r = -.58, P=0.01)
and social support (r = -.73, P=0.001) were negatively associated
with RPE in the group condition only, and positively associated with
enjoyment. Running with a group did not affect speed, enjoyment, or
RPE during an interval workout. However, higher perceived task
cohesion and social support were associated with lower perceived
exertion and greater enjoyment during group running. While the
group environment did not augment the subjects’ average running
speed during a high intensity interval workout, group training may
nonetheless furnish psychological benefits that could aid in the
completion of challenging, high intensity training sessions.
Key Words: Group Running, Cohesion, Recreational Runners,
Social Support, Social Facilitation
25
INTRODUCTION
Distance running is a highly popular fitness and athletic activity among US adults of both
sexes and a wide range of ages. Over 17 million individuals participate in organized running
events from 5 km to marathon (42.2 km) distances, and more recently ultramarathon (>42.2
km) distances annually (45). Among competitive and recreational runners alike (32), training
in a group is commonly viewed as a simple yet effective strategy to boost performance.
Various elements of a group environment are thought to encourage harder training effort and,
therefore, produce greater improvements in fitness than when training alone (24,30). This
strategy reflects similar recommendations by fitness professionals to increase the motivation
to exercise (19,33).
Knowledge of the potential benefits of exercising with others is not new. Ample research
points to social support as a strong correlate of physical activity (55), and the phenomenon of
“social facilitation” – that is, enhanced performance in the presence of others – was proposed
over a century ago (53,54,62). Although the social environment appears to have a positive
influence on total exercise behavior as well as cognitions and attitudes associated with
exercise (13), it is not a foregone conclusion that every individual who trains with others for
an athletic event (e.g., a footrace) will exert a greater effort than he or she would alone during
training sessions. For instance, while social support has been shown to increase the amount
of physical activity and promote adherence to exercise programs in adults who are not
competitive athletes (57), such results cannot be generalized to the voluntary effort of
athletes during individual training sessions.
Social facilitation, by contrast, has a stronger basis of examination in the athletic population.
Studies on cyclists (16,61), swimmers (59), and weightlifters (42) suggest that the presence
of an audience and/or competition can improve maximal physical performance. Yet, despite
the positive effect on maximal athletic performance shown in these studies (16,42,59,61),
only a small number of studies by our research group (7-10) have examined the potential role
of social factors during athletes’ submaximal training sessions. Many athletes, especially
those in endurance sports, perform a substantial amount of training below maximal effort
(22,49). Recent evidence supports a “polarized” endurance training scheme comprised of a
large volume of low intensity training, interspersed with a smaller proportion of structured,
high intensity efforts (29,49). Increased intensity in the presence of others during either type
could augment the total training stimulus (52,37). However, increased effort could also be
detrimental if intended low intensity training becomes excessively strenuous, which could
inhibit recovery (35,37), and/or the intensity of structured sessions (6). Therefore, a better
understanding of factors which could modulate the volume or intensity of submaximal training
(such as a partner or group) is warranted.
While a small number of experimental studies on non-athlete adults (27,39) and children
(1,43,47,48) suggest that the presence of others (i.e., peer influence) can increase the
intensity, amount of physical activity (via accelerometer), or enjoyment of self-regulated
exercise, our recent studies on competitive and recreational adult runners (7-10) raise the
question: Is the link between training with others and individual effort consistent across
different types of exercisers (e.g., non-athletes vs. athletes). Our prior studies diverge from
previous findings of positive effects, showing null (9,10) or negative (7,8) responses to the
presence of a peer. The unexpected findings of these preliminary studies on adult runners,
26
compared with the enhanced maximal athletic performance in previous social facilitation
literature (16,42,59,61), highlight the need for further experimental research on the behavioral
and psychological effects of performing submaximal training coactively with others.
Local running clubs and groups offer the most common avenue to train with others. Also, they
are widely available throughout the US (44), and provide scheduled training sessions in a
group environment (24). However, no existing research has examined the effect of training in
a regularly meeting group (vs. alone) on voluntary intensity or volume, or the perceived effort
(i.e., ratings of perceived exertion (RPE), and enjoyment of a bout of training. Within this
context, an individual’s perception of the group’s social environment and collective goals may
be more important as the mere presence of the other group members. In this regard, an
established body of research has investigated group cohesion, which is characterized by a
sense of common unity in the pursuit of specific collective goals and/or objectives and a
tendency to “stick together” in the face of adversity or obstacles (11). Cohesion has been
shown to be a robust predictor of the adherence behavior of exercise group (15,50) and sport
team (14) members, but we are aware of only one study that has empirically examined the
effect of perceived cohesion on physical exertion in athletic individuals.
Prapavessis and Carron (40) found that individual sport team members’ perception of the
team’s task cohesiveness had a positive impact on exertion relative to individual maximum in
a running task. However, the authors did not test for a difference in exertion between
performing the task simultaneously with teammates and performing it alone. Another
proposed benefit of group exercise, the provision of social support, has also been
consistently identified as a strong correlate of physical activity and a predictor of adherence
behavior (13,23). Yet, despite the extensive research showing the utility of social support in
promoting regular physical activity, it has seldom received attention as a possible moderating
influence on acute exercise intensity (28).
The existing experimental research on the effect of partner or group influence on acute
training intensity in athletic individuals is limited, and the idea of universally increased
motivation and intensity when running in a group relies on anecdotal support. Although
previous studies on peer influence during voluntarily paced continuous running showed
inconsistent results (7-10), high intensity intervals within the polarized training model
(29,37,49) require effort more similar (25,36) to the maximal effort shown to be enhanced in
previous social facilitation studies on athletes (16,42,59,61). Furthermore, this type of training
can have a powerful effect on endurance performance (25,36,37). If social facilitation or
perceived cohesion in a group setting augments the rate of work sustained during high
intensity training, running in a group may indeed be an effective means to enhance the
adaptive response (25,52) and ultimately, competitive performance (37). However, no
empirical evidence currently exists to support this potential outcome of training in a group.
The purpose of this study was to determine if running with a group affects average speed,
perceived exertion, and enjoyment in members of an established public running group during
a structured, high intensity interval session. Secondly, we sought to determine if any changes
in these variables were related to individuals’ perceptions of group cohesion or social
support. Based on the previous social facilitation research (16,42,59,61) showing enhanced
maximal performance in athletes, we hypothesized that runners performing a structured
interval running session coactively with familiar group members would increase average
27
speed and report greater enjoyment compared to running the same workout alone. From
previous cohesion research showing a positive effect on relative running effort (40), we also
hypothesized that the magnitude of any changes in average speed and/or enjoyment would
be positively associated with perceived cohesion and social support.
METHODS
Subjects
Twenty habitual adult runners (11 women, 9 men), all belonging to the same local running
group, were recruited through fliers and verbal announcements by the principal investigator at
group training sessions. The principal investigator had no other affiliation or involvement with
the group. Three participants withdrew from the study, one due to knee pain and two due to
scheduling conflicts, leaving data from 17 subjects (10 women, 7 men) to be analyzed. To be
eligible for the study, subjects were required to report running at least 3 d·wk-1 and at least 20
km·wk-1 for the prior 6 months, compete in 3 or more organized running events during the
prior 12 months, and free from medical complications (such as metabolic, orthopedic, and
cardiovascular disorders). The subjects took part in the group to train for a series of local
road racing events ranging from 5 km to the marathon (42.2 km). Each subject reported
training with the group for at least 2 sessions·wk-1 for the last 4 wks. All subjects were
provided a written informed consent, and all study procedures were approved by the
University Institutional Review Board. Table 1 presents the subjects’ physical characteristics.
Table 1. Descriptive Data of the Subjects. Values are reported as means ± SD (N = 17).
Men Women
Age (yr) 45.3 ± 6.52 44.5 ± 14.9
Body Mass (kg) 80.7 ± 10.4 64.5 ± 8.87
Height (cm) 178 ± 2.28 162 ± 5.62
Weekly Volume (km·wk-1) 32.6 ± 10.6 30.4 ± 9.89
Procedures
Each subject completed two experimental trials. Each trial was a different social condition
(group, alone) and in a randomized order. In the group condition, the subjects were observed
while performing a previously scheduled “speed” session coactively with the entire running
group, including those who were not study participants. This session was part of the group’s
prearranged schedule and was selected for examination due to its intermittent, high intensity
nature. In order to observe the subjects’ customary training habits, they were allowed to run
with anyone with whom they normally did during group sessions, but were not required to run
alongside any other specific group member. Because the group included members with
varying ability, individual runners progressed through the session at varying rates. While
many of the training group’s members simultaneously completing the session were not
subjects in the study, participants were observed within this environment in order to mimic as
closely as possible an actual group training environment and preserve the usual interactions
the subjects had with other accustomed group members.
28
Initial Session
Prior to the data collection sessions, the principal investigator met individually with each
subject in the university laboratory to explain the study procedures, obtain written informed
consent, and collect anthropometric measurements. Height and weight were measured to the
nearest millimeter using an analog stadiometer (Seca, Chino CA) and to the nearest 0.2 kg
using a calibrated balance beam scale (Seca, Chino CA), respectively. During this session,
the subject also completed validated questionnaires to assess perceived cohesion of the
running group and the provision of social support from the group. Perceived cohesion and
social support were assessed using the Group Environment Questionnaire (GEQ) (14) and
the Exercise Social Provisions Scale (EXSPS) (18), respectively. The subject was given
standardized instructions on the completion of each questionnaire, and was permitted to ask
questions at any time. All questionnaires were completed privately and the order in which the
subjects completed the GEQ and EXSPS was counterbalanced. Lastly, the subject was
introduced to the Borg 6-20 RPE scale (5) and provided standardized instructions on its use.
Group Interval Session
Each interval running session consisted of 8 uphill 400 m intervals. The hill was part of a
paved pedestrian path closed to vehicular traffic, and had an average inclination of 7.6%, as
calculated using Google Earth. The same hill was used for all intervals under both social
conditions. Upon arrival to the data collection site, subjects were individually fitted by
research staff with a heart rate monitoring chest strap and wrist unit. The instructions for the
session were explained by the principal investigator to the subjects, who were then given the
opportunity to ask any questions before the session began. Then, the subjects performed a
10-min light intensity warm-up, on a flat section of the path used for the warm-up in both
sessions. The subjects were instructed to run each interval at a “hard” but not maximal effort,
correspondent to 90 to 95% of maximal effort and 16 to 18 on the Borg RPE scale (37,38).
The recovery between each interval was dictated by the time required for the subject to jog
down the hill at a light effort. The subsequent interval began as soon as the subject returned
to the starting point. Any timing or monitoring devices worn by the subject were obscured by
black nontransparent tape, and group members not participating in the study were asked not
to share any temporal feedback with the subjects until the conclusion of the workout. At the
end of each interval, the elapsed time, average heart rate, and rating of perceived exertion
(RPE) for that interval were recorded. The elapsed time of each recovery period was also
recorded between intervals. The data recorded for each of these variables was then
averaged over the 8 intervals. Each subject’s enjoyment (i.e., liking) of the entire session was
assessed at the conclusion of the workout in a private area via visual analog scale.
Alone Interval Session
The alone trial took place at the same time of day, 5 to 7 days before or after the group trial.
The subjects were given identical directions to the group trial, and completed the same
training session on the same course. Measures were identical to those in the group trial.
Measurements
Group Environment Questionnaire
The Group Environment Questionnaire (GEQ) is a four scale instrument that provides a valid
and reliable measure of perceived cohesion in sport teams (14) and exercise groups
(15,50,51). The questionnaire is based on a recognized conceptual model (21) describing
29
cohesion as a construct consisting of four elements, as illustrated in Figure 1. The model
distinguishes between individual and group aspects of cohesion, which are each
subcategorized into task and social orientations. The individual aspect is divided into
Individual Attraction to Group-Task (ATG-T) and Individual Attraction to Group-Social (ATG-
S), while the group aspect is divided into Group Integration-Task (GI-T) and Group
Integration-Social (GI-S).
Figure 1. Conceptual Model of Group Cohesion (14).
Individual Attraction to Group-Task (ATG-T) is a measure of the individual’s perception of his
or her personal involvement in the group’s common task and/or goals, while Individual
Attraction to Group-Social (ATG-S) is a measure of the individual’s perception of his or her
acceptance, inclusion, and involvement in the group’s social atmosphere. Group Integration-
Task (GI-T) is a measure of the individual’s perception of the group’s unity around the shared
pursuit of common goals, while Group Integration-Social (GI-S) is a measure of the
individual’s perception of the group’s closeness as a social entity (21). The GEQ assesses an
individual’s perception of the four separate elements of group cohesion using an 18-item
questionnaire to which responses are given on a 9 point Likert scale ranging from 1 (“strongly
disagree”) to 9 (“strongly agree”).
Exercise Social Provisions Scale
The Revised Exercise Group Social Provisions Scale (EXSPS) is a validated questionnaire
developed by Cutrona and Russell (18) to assess six distinct components of social support,
as described by Weiss (56): attachment, social integration, reassurance of worth, reliable
alliance, guidance, and opportunity for nurturance. The questionnaire consists of 24 items to
which responses are given on a 4 point Likert scale ranging from 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 4
(“strongly agree”).
Enjoyment
Subjects rated their enjoyment of each interval session using a visual analog scale (VAS) that
consisted of a continuous horizontal 100-mm line anchored by “do not like it at all” on the left
30
and “like it very much” on the right (4). The subject was shown the scale and was instructed
to make a mark on the line to indicate his or her level of enjoyment of the session, with a
higher millimeter measurement indicating greater liking.
RPE
Undifferentiated, whole-body RPE was assessed at the conclusion of each interval using the
validated Borg RPE scale (5). The subject was shown a large placard of the scale and
allowed to point to or verbally express the number (6 to 20) indicating his or her level of
perceived exertion.
Statistical Analyses
Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 21 (IBM Inc, Armonk, IL) with an alpha level
of P≤0.05. Means and measures of variability were calculated for the primary dependent
variables (average interval time, average interval RPE, and enjoyment) in each social
condition. Assumptions of normality were tested and confirmed for each dependent variable
using Shapiro-Wilk tests. Because the running group included multiple study participants,
interdependence occurred between the subjects who completed the group condition
coactively. Therefore, mixed-effects regression models were used to examine main and
interaction effects of social condition and sex for each dependent variable. Mixed models
assume that the data within subjects are dependent among the observations and can
therefore be used to account for interdependence (26). Separate models were performed for
each of the dependent variables. All regression analyses utilized the following model:
Dependent variable = α + β1 (social condition) + β2 (sex) + β3 (social condition*sex)
Post-hoc paired t-tests were performed for any significant main or interaction effects.
Individual ratings of cohesion and social support were compared between sexes using
unpaired t-tests. Pearson’s correlations were used to detect associations between perceived
cohesion and social support and the primary dependent variables (interval time, enjoyment,
and RPE) in each social condition.
RESULTS
The data for all dependent variables, grouped by sex, are presented in Table 2 (Values are
reported as means ± SD, N = 17; *Significant main effect of sex, P≤0.05). The heart rate data
were incomplete for numerous subjects. Therefore, the data were eliminated from the final
analysis. Mixed model regression analysis showed a main effect of sex on average interval
time (F = 12.15, P=0.002) in which the men (122 ± 16.1 sec) completed the intervals in less
time than the women (155 ± 27.8 sec) across both social conditions. No main effect of social
condition or sex by condition interaction occurred on average interval time (P>0.87). There
were no main or interaction effects on RPE (P>0.08) or enjoyment (P>0.26). Data for ratings
of perceived social support and individual aspects of cohesion are shown in Table 3 (means
± SD, N = 17). No aspect of cohesion nor social support differed between the sexes (P>0.11).
31
Table 2. Average Interval Time, Enjoyment, and RPE Across Social Conditions
Men Women
Alone Group Alone Group
Time (sec)* 122.9 ± 16.1 121.1 ± 15.8 151.0 ± 27.7 151.8 ± 28.9
Enjoyment (mm) 79.4 ± 20.9 86.0 ± 13.5 71.9 ± 21.9 80.9 ± 12.9
RPE 14.6 ± 2.12 15.2 ± 1.95 15.8 ± 1.41 16.1 ± 1.47
Table 3. Perceived Cohesion and Social Support.
Men Women
ATG-T 8.14 ± 0.76 8.20 ± 0.57
GI-T 7.17 ± 1.54 7.50 ± 1.12
ATG-S 6.65 ± 1.13 7.46 ± 1.38
GI-S 6.00 ± 2.07 6.42 ± 1.72
EXSPS 3.21 ± 0.47 3.18 ± 0.45
Both individual and group aspects of task cohesion, Individual Attraction to Group-Task
(ATG-T) and Group Integration-Task (GI-T), were negatively associated with RPE in the
group condition (ATG-T: r = -0.58, P=0.01; GI-T: r = -0.54, P=0.03), but not RPE in the alone
condition (P>0.25). The association between ATG-T and group condition RPE is shown in
Figure 2. Neither measure of social cohesion (i.e., Individual Attraction to Group-Social (ATG-
S), Group Integration-Social (GI-S)) was associated with RPE in either condition.
Figure 2. Task Cohesion (ATG-T) and RPE. Perceptions of task cohesion (ATG-T shown) were
negatively associated with RPE only during the group condition (r = -0.58, P=0.01).
32
The subjects’ perceived social support was negatively associated with RPE in the group
condition (r = -0.73, P=0.001) but not RPE in the alone condition (P=0.17). The association
between perceived social support and group condition RPE is shown in Figure 3.
Figure 3. Perceived Social Support and RPE. Perceived social support was negatively associated
with RPE only during the group condition (r = -0.73, P=0.001).
DISCUSSION
The primary finding of this study was that completing an interval running workout with familiar
group members did not significantly influence the recreational runners’ average speed,
perceived exertion, or enjoyment of the workout. However, perceptions of both types of task
cohesion (ATG-T and GI-T) and social support were negatively associated with perceived
exertion during the group running condition, but not during the alone condition. This
association, in concurrence with a lack of difference in average speed between conditions,
may suggest that individuals who perceived higher task cohesion and social support reported
lower effort in the group environment to run the same average speed. In addition, perceived
task cohesion was positively associated with ratings of enjoyment of both interval running
sessions.
Interestingly, the runners’ average speed was not different between the two social conditions.
Numerous studies on athletes have shown that an audience or competition can improve
maximal performance (16,42,59,61) through a “social facilitation” process (53). However, in
studies on recreational and competitive runners performing submaximal exercise (7-10), such
an effect was not observed. During a self-paced, submaximal training session, collegiate
male runners ran slower in the presence of teammates versus running alone (7). In a similar
study on recreational runners, female runners decreased speed in the presence of an
unfamiliar peer while male runners showed a positive change in speed (8). A later study
showed no differences in voluntary speed, duration, perceived exertion, or enjoyment
between running alone, with a familiar peer, or with an unfamiliar peer in recreational runners
(9). The intensity in these studies was self-selected, but runners were advised to approach
33
the experimental trials as they would an unstructured, light to moderate intensity training
session, which comprises a majority of endurance athletes’ training (22).
The present study investigated the possibility that a group setting, through social facilitation
(16,42,59,61) or the influence of cohesion (40) would stimulate volitional intensity during a
strenuous interval training session. While such workouts represent a small proportion of
endurance athletes’ training regimen, they are nonetheless an integral part of structured
endurance training programs (49) and deliver large adaptive benefits (25,36). Given that they
involve markedly higher intensity than continuous, moderate running and more closely
resemble the maximal effort (25) shown elsewhere to be enhanced in the presence of others
(16,42,59,61), we hypothesized that the stimulatory effect absent from submaximal running
studies (7-10) would occur. Yet, despite the higher intensity involved in such an interval
workout, no consistent difference in average speed occurred between performing the workout
alone or amidst a familiar group in the present study. While the potential for competition to
enhance physical performance cannot be discounted, the present results more closely align
with the findings of Bath et al. (2), who showed that the 5-km time trial performance of trained
runners was not affected by a second runner acting as a pacer, but the runners subjectively
rated the “pacer” condition as easier than running alone. Our findings reflect the conclusion of
Bath et al. (2) that a runner’s pacing strategy appears to be robust and not altered by the
presence of another runner. The present similarity in average interval speed between social
conditions limits the likelihood of an augmented volitional intensity by a group environment.
However, concurrence with Bath’s (2) proposal of robust pacing strategy may have the
benefit of allowing runners to train in a group environment while adhering to individually
appropriate training paces (35,37) even at high intensities.
While average speed was not altered between social conditions, measures of perceived task
cohesion (ATG-T and GI-T) and social support were negatively associated with RPE only in
the group condition. Past research on cohesion, particularly individual attraction to group task
(ATG-T) has shown positive effects on individual adherence behavior, including reduced
withdrawal and absenteeism (15,50). Similarly, social support has consistently been identified
as a robust correlate of physical activity behavior (55). However, these relationships do not
allow conjecture on the effect that cohesion or social support may have on acute individual
effort in a group setting. Evidence does exist to support a positive relationship between
cohesion and performance in sport (12), although the index used to gauge performance has
not been consistent across reports. For example, performance in interactive team sports has
been gauged using win-loss records (34,58), and in a coactive sport using individual golf
scores (60). Only Prapavessis and Carron (40) have empirically examined the effect of
cohesion on physical exertion during a running task. Athletes in various team sports that
benefit from cardiovascular fitness (rugby, basketball, soccer, netball, water polo) were
grouped by their reported perceptions of task cohesion (ATG-T, GI-T) before a maximal effort
3-min run. Athletes in the “high cohesion” group exerted significantly greater effort, as
indicated by percentage of maximal aerobic capacity, than those in the “low cohesion” group
(40). Runners in the present study were not separated into groups based on reported
cohesion, but did not change average speed between the alone and group interval sessions,
regardless of perceived cohesion. Although cohesion did not appear to impact voluntary
running speed, the negative association between group session RPE and perceived task
cohesion reflects the findings of Courneya (17), who reported higher feeling states in relation
to higher perceptions of task cohesion in exercise class participants. Furthermore, in addition
34
to its inverse relation with group session RPE, task cohesion was positively associated with
ratings of enjoyment of both running sessions. Although affect was not directly assessed in
the present study, a relationship between enjoyment and positive effect has been previously
reported (41).
Notably, group session RPE was related only to measures of task cohesion (ATG-T, GI-T),
but not to those of social cohesion (ATG-S, GI-S). Past work showing that cohesion positively
affects performance (12), adherence (15,50), and effort (40) suggests that task cohesion
most consistently exerts a positive effect on these variables than social aspects of cohesion
(21). Here, while group influence did not produce the greater effort widely thought to occur in
such a setting (24,30,32), the association between task cohesion and group session RPE
suggests that an individual’s perception of a group environment may have important effects
other than altering the intensity of exercise. Similar to the positive cohesion – affective
relationship in exercise class participants (17), Bath et al. (2) showed that even without a
performance effect from a second runner, trained runners felt that a maximal effort 5-km time
trial was easier in the “pacer” condition than alone.
In the present study, the lower RPE and greater enjoyment reported for a group workout by
those with higher perceptions of task cohesion could be especially valuable during high
intensity interval training sessions. This format of training provides a potent stimulus for
enhanced fitness (25), but requires much higher effort and discomfort than continuous
moderate intensity training (20), and may produce negative affective states (31,46). Despite
the lack of an effect on running speed, it is possible that the lower RPE and greater
enjoyment associated with the perceived task cohesiveness of the group could potentially
mitigate the discomfort and/or negative feelings often experienced during intense training.
This could be of benefit to experienced runners aiming to enhance performance through the
inclusion of higher intensity training, or help make such training more approachable for novice
runners (36,37). However, further examination under experimental conditions is necessary to
support this possibility.
The present study is not without limitation. Foremost, the small sample size (N = 17)
necessitates that the results, while novel, be interpreted as preliminary. While all participants
fulfilled the criteria for participation, their wide ranging ability levels likely contributed to
considerable variability in average running speed. However, public running groups most
frequently welcome all levels of ability, which may contribute to their growing popularity (3).
Thus, the subjects’ varying ability in the present study may reflect the variation present in
many running groups, making the present findings highly relevant to these groups.
Conversely, these results cannot be generalized to highly competitive runners in elite training
groups or collegiate teams. Despite the moderate and significant correlations between
measures of task cohesion and RPE in the group condition, the range of task cohesion
ratings in this sample was fairly homogeneous, which restricts the assessment of these
associations with lower levels of perceived cohesion. Most importantly, the significant
correlations between perceptions of cohesion, social support, exertion, and enjoyment are
compelling, but cannot be used to infer causation without further controlled empirical study.
Future research should involve larger samples of both recreational and highly competitive
runners with a wider range of perceived cohesion who can be stratified into groups of low and
high perceived cohesion and/or social support.
35
CONCLUSIONS
The present findings are the first to suggest that performing a challenging running workout
coactively with a highly cohesive and supportive group may positively affect the perception of
effort and enjoyment of the workout. While the group environment did not augment the
subjects’ average running speed during the high intensity interval workout, the present study
provides preliminary evidence that group training may nonetheless furnish psychological
benefits that could aid in the completion of challenging, high intensity training sessions.
However, such benefits likely depend on the perception of strong group cohesion, without
which the group environment may have little impact. Additional research is necessary to
elucidate the behavioral and psychological responses to group training in a variety of athletic
settings.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors wish to thank members of the Fleet Feet Distance Project of Louisville,
Kentucky, for their enthusiastic participation and effort, without which this study would not
have been possible.
Address for correspondence: Andrew J. Carnes, PhD, Department of Exercise Science,
Bellarmine University, Louisville, KY, United States, 40242, Email: acarnes@bellarmine.edu
REFERENCES
1. Barkley JE, Salvy SJ, Sanders GJ, Dey S, Von Carlowitz KP, Williamson ML. Peer
influence and physical activity behavior in young children: An experimental study. J
Phys Act Health. 2014;11(2):404-409.
2. Bath D, Turner L, Bosch A, Tucker R, Lambert E, Thompson K, St. Clair Gibson A.
The effect of a second runner on pacing strategy and RPE during a running time trial.
Int J Sports Physiol Perfor. 2012;7(1):26-32.
3. Beverly J. Come run with us. Competitor. 2016;June:30-34.
4. Bond A, Lader M. The use of analog scales in rating subjective feelings. Br J Med
Psychol. 1974;47:211-218.
5. Borg G. Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1982;
14(5):377-381.
6. Bruin G, Kuipers H, Keizer H, Vandewalle H. Adaptation and overtraining in horses
subjected to increasing training loads. J Appl Physiol. 1994;76(5):1908-1913.
7. Carnes AJ, Barkley JE. The effect of peer influence on exercise intensity and
enjoyment during outdoor running in collegiate distance runners. J Sport Behav.
2015;38(3):257-271.
36
8. Carnes AJ, Barkley JE, Glickman E. Gender differences in the effect of peer influence
on outdoor running in recreational runners. Med Sci Sport Exerc. 2014;46(5):S461-
466.
9. Carnes AJ, Petersen J, Barkley JE. The effect of peer influence on exercise behavior
and enjoyment in recreational runners. J Strength Cond Res. 2016;30(2):497-503.
10. Carnes AJ, Barkley JE, Williamson M, Sanders G. The presence of a familiar peer
does not affect intensity or enjoyment during treadmill exercise in male distance
runners or non-runners. J Athl Enhanc. 2013;2(4):1-6.
11. Carron AV. Cohesiveness in sport groups: Interpretations and considerations. J Sport
Psychol. 1982;4:23-138.
12. Carron AV, Colman MM, Wheeler, J. Cohesion and performance in sport: a meta-
analysis. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2002;24:168-188.
13. Carron AV, Hausenblaus HA, Mack D. Social influence and exercise: A meta-analysis.
J Sport Exerc Psychol. 1996;18:1-16.
14. Carron AV, Widmeyer WN, Brawley LR. The development of an instrument to assess
cohesion in sport teams: The group environment questionnaire. J Sport Psychol.
1985;7(3):244-266.
15. Carron AV, Widmeyer WN, Brawley LR. Group cohesion and individual adherence to
physical activity. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 1988;10(2):127-138.
16. Corbett J, Barwood MJ, Ouzounogluo A, Thelwell R, Dicks M. Influence of competition
on performance and pacing during cycling exercise. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2012;44
(3):509-515.
17. Courneya KS. Cohesion correlates with affect in structured exercise classes. Percept
Mot Skills. 1995;8:1021-1022.
18. Cutrona CE, Russell DW. The provisions of social relationships and adaptation to
stress. Adv Pers Relat. 1987;1(1):37-67.
19. Dolan S. Benefits of group exercise. ACSM Fit Society Page. 2008;Winter, 2008 /
2009.
20. Ekkekakis P, Acevedo EO. Affective responses to acute exercise: toward a
psychobiological dose-response model. In: Psychobiology of Physical Activity,
Acevedo E.O. and Ekkekakis P. (Eds.). Human Kinetics, Champaign, 2006:91-110.
21. Estabrooks PA. Sustaining exercise participation through group cohesion. Exerc
Sport Sci Rev. 2000;28(2):63-67.
37
22. Esteve-Lanao J, San Juan A, Earnest CP, Foster C, Lucia A. How do endurance
runners actually train? Relationship with competition performance. Med Sci Sports
Exerc. 2005;37(3):496-504.
23. Fraser SN, Spink KS. Examining the role of social support and group cohesion in
exercise compliance. J Behav Med. 2002;25(3):233-249.
24. Galloway J. Join the pack. Runners World. 2012;47(6).
25. Gibala MJ, Little JP, Macdonald MJ, Hawley JA. Physiological adaptations to low
volume, high-intensity interval training in health and disease. J Physiol. 2012;590
(5):1077-1084.
26. Gibbons RD, Hedeker D. Application of random-effects prohibit regression models. J
Consult Clin Psychol. 1994;62:285-296.
27. Grindrod D, Paton CD, Knez WL, O’Brien BJ. Six-minute walk distance is greater
when performed in a group than alone. Br J Sports Med. 2006;40(10):876-877.
28. Hüffmeier J, Wessolowski K, van Randenborgh A, Bothin J, Schmid-Loertzer N, Hertel
G. Social support from fellow group members triggers additional effort in groups. Eur J
Soc Psychol. 2014;44:287-296.
29. Hydren JR, and Cohen BS. Current scientific evidence for a polarized cardiovascular
endurance training model. J Strength Cond Res. 2015; 29(12):3523-3530.
30. Jarvis L. Buddy system. Runner’s World. 2012;47(2):35.
31. Jung ME, Bourne JE, Little JP. Where does HIT fit? An examination of the affective
response to high-intensity intervals in comparison to continuous moderate-and
continuous vigorous-intensity exercise in the exercise intensity-affect continuum. PLoS
One. 2014;9(12):e114541.
32. Kolata G. To train harder, consider a crowd. New York Times. 2009;September 16:
E1.
33. Kravitz L. What motivates people to exercise? Reasons and strategies for exercise
adherence. IFJ. 2011;8:25-27.
34. Landers DM, Wilkinson MO, Hatfield BD, Barber H. Causality and the cohesion-
performance relationship. J Sport Psychol. 1982;4(2):170-183.
35. Lehmann M, Dickhuth HH, Gendrisch G, Lazar W, Thum M, Kaminski R, Aramendi JF,
Peterke E, Wieland W, Keul J. Training-overtraining: A prospective, experimental
study with experienced middle and long distance runners. Int J Sports Med. 1991;12:
444-452.
38
36. Little JP, Safdar A, Wilkin GP, Tarnopolsky MA, Gibala MJ. A practical model of low-
volume high-intensity interval training induces mitochondrial biogenesis in human
skeletal muscle: Potential mechanisms. J Physiol. 2010;588:1011-1022.
37. Midgley AW, McNaughton LR, Jones AM. Training to enhance the physiological
determinants of long-distance running performance. Sports Med. 2007;37(10):857-
880.
38. Norton K, Norton L, Sadgrove D. Position statement on physical activity and exercise
intensity terminology. J Sci Med Sport. 2010;13(5):496-502.
39. Plante TG, Madden M, Mann S, Lee G, Hardesty A, Gable N. Effects of perceived
fitness level of exercise partner on intensity of exertion. J Soc Sci. 2010;6:50-54.
40. Prapavessis H, Carron AV. Cohesion and work output. Small Gr Res. 1997;28(2):
294-301.
41. Raedeke TD. The relationship between enjoyment and affective responses to
exercise. J Appl Sport Psychol. 2007;19:105-115.
42. Rhea MR, Landers DM, Alvar BA, Arent SM. The effects of competition and the
presence of an audience on weight lifting performance. J Strength Cond Res. 2003;
17:303-306.
43. Rittenhouse M, Salvy SJ, Barkley JE. The effect of peer influence on the amount of
physical activity performed in 8- to 12-year-old boys. Pediatr Exerc Sci. 2011;23
(1):49-60.
44. Road Runners Club of America. Start a Running Club. 2016. (Online). Retrieved from
http://www.rrca.org/resources/club-directors/start-a-club
45. Running USA. 2015 state of the sport: US race trends. (Online). Available at:
http://www.runningusa.org/state-of-sport-us-trends-2015?returnTo=running-usa-blog.
Accessed June 13, 2016.
46. Saanijoki T, Nummenmaa L, Eskelinen JJ, Savolainen AM, Vahlberg T, Kalliokoski,
KK, Hannukainen JC. Affective responses to repeated sessions of high-intensity
interval training. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2015;47(12):2604-2611.
47. Salvy SJ, Bowker JW, Roemmich JN. Peer influence on children’s physical activity: An
experience sampling study. J Pediatr Psychol. 2008;33(1):39-49.
48. Sanders GJ, Peacock CA, Williamson ML, Wilson K, Carnes AJ, Barkley JE. The
effect of friendship groups on children’s physical activity: An experimental study. J
Behav Health. 2014;3:95-100.
49. Seiler S. What is best practice for training intensity and duration distribution in
endurance athletes? Int J Sports Psychol Perform. 2010;5(3):276-291.
39
50. Spink KS, Carron AV. Group cohesion and adherence in exercise classes. J Sport
Exerc Psychol. 1992;14(1).
51. Spink KS, Carron, AV. Group cohesion effects in exercise classes. Small Gr Res.
1994;25(1):26-42.
52. Swain DP, Franklin BA. VO2 reserve and the minimal intensity for improving
cardiorespiratory fitness. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2002;34(1):152-157.
53. Titus LJ, Bond CF. Social facilitation: A meta-analysis of 241 studies. Psychol Bull.
1983;94(2):265.
54. Triplett N. The dynamogenic factors in pacemaking and competition. Am J Psychol.
1898;4:507-533.
55. Trost SG, Owen N, Bauman AE, Sallis JF, Brown W. Correlates of adults' participation
in physical activity: Review and update. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2002;34:1996-2001.
56. Weiss R. The provisions of social relationships. In: Z. Rubin (Ed.), Doing Unto Others
(pp. 17-26). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, 1974.
57. Wendel-Vos W, Droomers M, Kremers S, Brug J, Van Lenthe F. Potential
environmental determinants of physical activity in adults: A systematic review. Obes
Rev. 2007;8(5):425-440.
58. Williams JM, Hacker CM. Causal relationships among cohesion, satisfaction, and
performance in women's intercollegiate field hockey teams. J Sport Psychol. 1982;
4(4):324-337.
59. Williams KD, Nida SA, Baca LD, Latane B. Social loafing and swimming: Effects of
identifiability on individual and relay performance of intercollegiate swimmers. Basic
Appl Soc Psych. 1989;10:73-81.
60. Williams JM, Widmeyer WN. The cohesion-performance outcome relationship in a
coacting sport. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 1991;13(4):364-371.
61. Wilmore J. Influence of motivation on physical work capacity and performance. J Appl
Physiol. 1968;24(4):459-463.
62. Zajonc R.B. Social facilitation. Science. 1965;149(3681):269-274.
Disclaimer
The opinions expressed in JEPonline are those of the authors and are not attributable to
JEPonline, the editorial staff or the ASEP organization.
... A recent review indicated that additional research is needed to determine which factors are most important [45]. In this paper, we selected a common conceptualization of group cohesion that allows us to examine subcomponents of group cohesion [34] that may have differential effects on mental health [10,[48][49][50]. ...
... Research shows that these dimensions of group cohesion predict staying with the group, depending on how long the group has existed. However, more evidence is needed to develop a solid pattern [50][51][52]. In the context of exercise groups, Estabrooks and Carron (2000) found that people with higher GI-T scores were more likely to stay with the group long term (prioritizing doing the work of exercising), and Loughead et al. (2001) found that people who had higher levels of GI-S were more likely to only stay for the short term (prioritizing socializing). ...
... For codes that mismatched, both raters discussed their reasoning and determined a final code. • The Group Environment Questionnaire (GEQ) [34] is a typical measure of group cohesion [45,[50][51][52]77], most often applied to a sports team. We modified it to refer to participants' identified social group. ...
Article
Full-text available
During the 2020 COVID-19 pandemic, governments around the world mandated shutdowns and social distancing, limiting how much people could see other people outside of their household. Because of this, people had negative mental health outcomes, and many people turned to technology to maintain connections and create new ones. In this paper, we examine the relationship between technology, mental health, and group cohesion with existing groups ( N = 202) and new groups ( N = 74). We surveyed U.S. participants in June 2020, two to three months after the start of mandated social distancing. Results indicated that, as predicted, higher levels of reported group cohesion typically related to better reported mental health; however, the relationship occurred differently for existing groups compared to new groups. Further, higher levels of affinity for technology did not relate to group cohesion for existing groups, but did relate to more perceived cohesion for new groups. Researchers and mental health practitioners can use these results to help people develop a sense of group cohesion with new and existing groups and improve mental health during relative social isolation; technology may be especially beneficial for people to connect with new groups compared to existing groups.
... , and greater adherence to the sport (Spink, Ulvick, Crozier, & Wilson, 2014). Yet, few studies have examined cohesion in individual sport or recreational sport contexts (Carnes & Mahoney, 2016;Carron & Spink, 1992;Curry, Jenkins, & Weatherford, 2015;Kwak, Kremers, Walsh, & Brug, 2006;Spink & Carron, 1994;Spink et al., 2014). Since understanding and developing cohesion within groups can optimize interactions among group members, we sought to examine if the same benefits from cohesion in team sport settings could be experienced and cultivated in individual sports. ...
... Teams encouraging and facilitating the development of cohesion and shared leadership can outperform teams without these critical factors. Yet, while multiple studies have outlined the benefits of cohesion in team sports, few have examined cohesion in individual sports (Carnes & Mahoney, 2016;Curry et al., 2015;Kwak et al., 2006;Spink & Carron, 1994;Spink et al., 2014). This limited focus overlooks the potential for cohesion among individual sport participants who take part in group settings such as recreational runners in running groups. ...
... In one of the few studies examining cohesion and recreational running, Carnes and Mahoney (2016) explored the effects of task cohesion and social cohesion on perceived exertion while completing high intensity running workouts. They found an inverse relationship between the cohesion types and perceived effort. ...
Article
Full-text available
Sixty percent of recreational runners in the United States use running groups to participate in the sport. Within these groups, cohesion, or the process of working together to achieve goals and objectives, can develop. The purpose of this study was to investigate the levels of cohesion occurring among recreational runners using running groups to train for a running event. This study used social network analysis to assess task cohesion (achieving collective goals) and social cohesion (developing relationships among group members) in two running groups. The groups exhibited cohesion; task cohesion exceeded social cohesion. Informal leaders emerged alongside formal leaders in both groups. Running group organizers can use these findings to increase task cohesion and shared leadership in their groups. The results add to the few studies examining cohesion in individual sports and extend the application of social network analysis in sport research. © 2018
... We anticipate that a team member's perceived team cohesion positively influences his or her perceived enjoyment. Carnes and Mahoney (2016) showed that cohesion is associated with perceived enjoyment during group running. They further found that performing a challenging running workout coactively with a highly cohesive team may positively influence the perception of enjoyment of the workout (Carnes & Mahoney, 2016). ...
... Carnes and Mahoney (2016) showed that cohesion is associated with perceived enjoyment during group running. They further found that performing a challenging running workout coactively with a highly cohesive team may positively influence the perception of enjoyment of the workout (Carnes & Mahoney, 2016). Also, challenging and participative tasks are often structured to prompt intrinsic motivation, which is associated with high levels of interest and enjoyment of the task (Gagné & Deci, 2005). ...
Pedagogy using gamification has recently received much attention as a way of enhancing student learning and retention. Additionally, academic institutions are making extensive use of online resources to expand teaching beyond the traditional classroom setting. Both academic institutions and companies utilize virtual teams to accomplish remote teamwork, particularly in a post-COVID environment. Despite the growing interest in incorporating gamification into teaching for business school courses, prior researchers have paid little attention to team-based gamification in the online learning environment. The purpose of this study is to examine if team members’ perceived team cohesion influences their perceptions of flow. Also, we investigate the relationships among three flow dimensions: concentration, perceived control, and perceived enjoyment. We tested our research model using an enterprise resource planning simulation game involving 187 business students. The results of structural equation modeling with partial least squares analysis support all of our hypotheses.
... al (2017) found that stronger identification with the parkrun running group positively predicted participation, life satisfaction, exercise-specific satisfaction, and group cohesion. These and other studies (Carnes & Mahoney, 2016;Graupensperger et al., 2019) (Carnes & Mahoney, 2016) offer support for the idea that individuals' perceptions of the social group as cohesive and supportive, and with which they can strongly identify as group members, can promote positive affective exercise experiences, increase participation in physical activity, and facilitate performance via socially-mediated mechanisms other than arousal, evaluation apprehension and distraction. This literature has elucidated the types, causes and consequences of group cohesion in sports. ...
... al (2017) found that stronger identification with the parkrun running group positively predicted participation, life satisfaction, exercise-specific satisfaction, and group cohesion. These and other studies (Carnes & Mahoney, 2016;Graupensperger et al., 2019) (Carnes & Mahoney, 2016) offer support for the idea that individuals' perceptions of the social group as cohesive and supportive, and with which they can strongly identify as group members, can promote positive affective exercise experiences, increase participation in physical activity, and facilitate performance via socially-mediated mechanisms other than arousal, evaluation apprehension and distraction. This literature has elucidated the types, causes and consequences of group cohesion in sports. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
There is growing academic, civic and policy interest in the public health benefits of community-based exercise events. Shifting the emphasis from competitive sport to communal activity, these events have wide appeal. In addition to physical health benefits, regular participation can reduce social isolation and loneliness through opportunities for social connection. Taking a broad evolutionary and social psychological perspective, we develop and test the hypothesis that social reward and support are associated with more positive exercise experiences and greater performance outputs. Using a repeated-measures design, we examine the influence of social perceptions and behaviour on subjective enjoyment, energy, fatigue, effort, and objective performance (run times) among a UK sample of parkrun participants. Social factors were associated with greater subjective enjoyment and energy. Higher subjective energy, in turn, was associated with faster run times, without any corresponding increase in perceived effort. No significant main effects of social factors on fatigue, performance or effort were detected. The role of social structural factors has long been recognised in public health approaches to physical activity. Our results motivate greater research attention on how positive and rewarding social behaviours and experiences – particularly subjective enjoyment and energy, and perceptions of community social support and belonging - influence exercise-related behaviour, psychology and physiology, and promote health through collective physical activity. The research also supplements traditional emphases on social facilitation and team sport that have dominated sport and exercise psychology and offers new avenues for understanding the deep connections among psychological, social and physical function in everyday health.
... Being part of a group is particularly valued among recreational runners for increasing enjoyment and persistence. [5][6][7] Beginner programs typically involve walk-run intervals where the amount of running is incrementally increased over an 8-12-week period with the goal of running continuously for a minimum distance or time (e.g., 5 kilometers or 30 minutes). ...
Article
Full-text available
Running is an example of vigorous activity that leads to important health benefits if maintained. Beginner running groups provide supportive training programs to help people progress from walking to sustained running. This study explored the characteristics of individuals joining beginner running groups and the outcomes they achieve. New members of beginner running groups (n = 141; mean age 43 years, 122 female) completed online assessments at the start of their group program with 63 participants (45%) also completing a follow-up assessment at the end of the program. Validated scales were used to assess exercise behavior, mental wellbeing, self-efficacy, running identity and social physique anxiety. The majority of participants had low exercise levels at the start of the program (63%, n = 89). By the program end, 47 participants (75% of those completing the follow-up assessment) reported meeting the training goal (running for 30 minutes continuously) with self-efficacy, program adherence and younger age representing significant predictors of success. Significant improvements in exercise levels, mental wellbeing, self-efficacy, running identity and social physique anxiety were observed by the end of the program. In conclusion, beginner running programs attract low active individuals and may lead to improved levels of exercise and psychological outcomes. Additional research is needed to examine the extent to which improvements are sustained longer term.
... For example, in a recent parkrun study, Stevens et al. [38] found that stronger identification with the parkrun running group positively predicted participation, life satisfaction, exercise-specific satisfaction, and group cohesion. These and other studies [39,40] offer support for the idea that individuals' perceptions of the social group as cohesive and supportive, and with which they can strongly identify as group members, can promote positive affective exercise experiences, increase participation in physical activity, and facilitate performance via socially mediated mechanisms other than arousal, evaluation apprehension and distraction. ...
Article
Full-text available
There is growing academic, civic and policy interest in the public health benefits of community-based exercise events. Shifting the emphasis from competitive sport to communal activity, these events have wide appeal. In addition to physical health benefits, regular participation can reduce social isolation and loneliness through opportunities for social connection. Taking a broad evolutionary and social psychological perspective, we suggest that social factors warrant more attention in current approaches to physical (in)activity and exercise behavior. We develop and test the hypothesis that social reward and support in exercise are associated with positive exercise experiences and greater performance outputs. Using a repeated-measures design, we examine the influence of social perceptions and behavior on subjective enjoyment, energy, fatigue, effort, and objective performance (run times) among a UK sample of parkrun participants. Social factors were associated with greater subjective enjoyment and energy. Higher subjective energy, in turn, was associated with faster run times, without any corresponding increase in perceived effort. No significant main effects of social factors on fatigue, performance or effort were detected. The role of social structural factors has long been recognized in public health approaches to physical activity. Our results indicate that there should be greater research attention on how positive and rewarding social behaviors and experiences—particularly subjective enjoyment and energy, and perceptions of community social support and belonging—influence exercise-related behavior, psychology and physiology, and promote health through collective physical activity. The research also supplements traditional emphases on social facilitation and team sport that have dominated sport and exercise psychology and offers new avenues for understanding the deep connections among psychological, social and physical function in everyday health.
... al. [43] identi ed links between subjective evaluations of groupness (i.e., the perception of a group as an interdependent unit in which members adopt roles and group norms) and exertion, enjoyment and affective valence in a tness class context. In a preliminary study with recreational runners (n = 17), Carnes and Mahoney [44] also found that higher perceived task cohesion (de ned as individual involvement in and group unity around common goals) and social support and were associated with lower perceived exertion during group running, and higher enjoyment in both interval and group running. These studies offer support for the idea that individuals' perceptions of the social group as cohesive and supportive, and with which they can strongly identify as group members, can promote positive affective exercise experiences, increase participation in physical activity, and facilitate performance via socially-mediated mechanisms other than arousal, evaluation apprehension and distraction. ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background: There is growing academic, civic and policy interest in the public health benefits of community-based exercise events. Shifting the emphasis from competitive sport to communal activity, these events have wide appeal, including among those new to sport or exercise. In addition to health benefits of physical activity, regular participation can reduce social isolation and loneliness through opportunities for social connection. Taking a broad evolutionary and social psychological perspective, we suggest that social factors warrant more attention in current approaches to physical (in)activity and exercise behaviour. We develop and test the hypothesis that social reward and support in exercise are associated with positive exercise experiences and greater performance outputs. Methods: Using a repeated-measures survey design, we examine the influence of social behaviour (e.g., attending with others, pre-event socialising) and perceptions of social support and belonging on subjective enjoyment, energy, fatigue, effort, and objective performance (run times) among a UK sample of parkrun participants. Results: Social factors were associated with greater subjective enjoyment and energy. Higher subjective energy, in turn, was associated with faster run times, without any corresponding increase in perceived effort. No significant main effects of social factors on fatigue, performance or effort were detected. Conclusions: The role of social structural factors has long been recognised in public health approaches to physical activity. Our results motivate greater research attention on how positive and rewarding social behaviours and experiences – particularly subjective enjoyment and energy, and perceptions of community social support and belonging - influence exercise-related behaviour, psychology and physiology, and promote health through collective physical activity. Approaching community exercise as a powerful context for social collaboration, reward and belonging also supplements the traditional focus on social facilitation and team sport that has dominated social psychological approaches within the sport and exercise sciences, and offers new avenues for understanding the deep connections among psychological, social and physical function in everyday health.
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: This study longitudinally compared changes in running performance (5-km TT) and fitness (VO2max, body composition) between polarized training and CrossFit Endurance© in recreational runners. Methods: Participants (N=21) completed 12 weeks of CrossFit Endurance (CFE) or polarized endurance training (POL). Both groups trained 5 d·wk-1. POL ran 5 d·wk-1 while CFE ran 3 d·wk-1 and performed CrossFit© 3 d·wk-1 (run + CF 1 d·wk-1). Intensity was classified as low, moderate, or high (Zone 1, 2, or 3) according to ventilatory thresholds. POL was prescribed greater volume (295±67 min·wk-1), distributed as 85/5/10% in Z1/Z2/Z3. CFE emphasized a lower volume (110±18 min·wk-1) distribution of 48/8/44%. Results: POL ran 283±75.9 min·wk-1 and 47.3±11.6 km·wk-1, both exceeding the 117±32.2 min·wk-1 and 19.3±7.17 km·wk-1 in CFE (p<0.001). The POL distribution (74/11/15%) had greater total and percent Z1 (p<0.001) than CFE (46/15/39%), which featured higher percent Z3 (p<0.001). TT improved -93.8±40.4s (-6.21±2.16%) in POL (p<0.001) and -84.2±65.7s (-5.49±3.56%) in CFE (p=0.001). Body composition improved by -2.45±2.59 %fat in POL (p=0.02) and -2.62±2.53% CFE (p=0.04). The magnitude of improvement was not different between groups for TT (p=0.79) or BC (p=0.88). Both groups increased VO2 max (p≤0.01), but with larger magnitude (p=0.04, d=0.85) in POL (4.3±3.6 ml·kg·min-1) than CFE (1.78±1.9 ml·kg·min-1). Conclusions: Recreational runners achieved similar improvement in 5-km performance and body composition through polarized training or CFE, but POL yielded a greater increase in VO2 max. Extrapolation to longer distances requires additional research.
Article
Full-text available
Although it is a common practice for competitive athletes to train together with fellow athletes and/or teammates, experimental research on the effect that training partners may have on athletes ’ submaximal exercise behavior is limited. The purpose of this study was to determine if, relative to an alone condition, exercising with a familiar peer or in a group of familiar peers affects average running speed and/or liking of the exercise during a self-paced outdoor run in a sample of competitive collegiate distance runners. Collegiate male distance runners (N=12, 20.5 ± 1.98 years old) completed running trials on separate days under three different, randomized order social conditions: running alone, with a single peer, and in a group with two additional runners (three total). Trials consisted of a self-paced 6.4-km run on a measured outdoor trail. The primary dependent variables were average speed and liking (assessed via visual analog scale) of the run. Mixed model regression analysis revealed a significant (P < 0.016) main effect of condition for average speed and liking. Participants ran faster in the alone condition than in the peer or group conditions and enjoyed running in a group more than running alone. The presence of peers, relative to the alone condition, increased the liking of a bout o f exercise in collegiate male distance runners. However, both peer conditions reduced average running speed in this group of athletes.
Article
Full-text available
This article investigates whether there is currently sufficient scientific knowledge for scientists to be able to give valid training recommendations to longdistance runners and their coaches on how to most effectively enhance the maximal oxygen uptake, lactate threshold and running economy. Relatively few training studies involving trained distance runners have been conducted, and these studies have often included methodological factors that make interpretation of the findings difficult. For example, the basis of most of the studies was to include one or more specific bouts of training in addition to the runners’ ‘normal training’, which was typically not described or only briefly described. The training status of the runners (e.g. off-season) during the study period was also typically not described. This inability to compare the runners’ training before and during the training intervention period is probably the main factor that hinders the interpretation of previous training studies. Arguably, the second greatest limitation is that only a few of the studies included more than one experimental group. Consequently, there is no comparison to allow the evaluation of the relative efficacy of the particular training intervention. Other factors include not controlling the runners’ training load during the study period, and employing small sample sizes that result in low statistical power. Much of the current knowledge relating to chronic adaptive responses to physical training has come from studies using sedentary individuals; however, directly applying this knowledge to formulate training recommendations for runners is unlikely to be valid. Therefore, it would be difficult to argue against the view that there is insufficient direct scientific evidence to formulate training recommendations based on the limited research. Although direct scientific evidence is limited, we believe that scientists can still formulate worthwhile training recommendations by integrating the information derived from training studies with other scientific knowledge. This knowledge includes the acute physiological responses in the various exercise domains, the structures and processes that limit the physiological determinants of long-distance running performance, and the adaptations associated with their enhancement. In the future, molecular biology may make an increasing contribution in identifying effective training methods, by identifying the genes that contribute to the variation in maximal oxygen uptake, the lactate threshold and running economy, as well as the biochemical and mechanical signals that induce these genes. Scientists should be cautious when giving training recommendations to runners and coaches based on the limited available scientific knowledge. This limited knowledge highlights that characterising the most effective training methods for long-distance runners is still a fruitful area for future research.
Article
Carnes, AJ, Petersen, JL, and Barkley, JE. Effect of peer influence on exercise behavior and enjoyment in recreational runners. J Strength Cond Res 30(2): 497-503, 2016-Fitness professionals and popular media sources often recommend exercising with a partner to increase exercise motivation, adherence, intensity, and/or duration. Although competition with peers has been shown to enhance maximal athletic performance, experimental research examining the impact of peer influence on submaximal exercise behavior in adults is limited. The purpose of this study was to determine the effects of the presence of familiar and unfamiliar peers, vs. running alone, on recreational runners' voluntary running duration, distance, intensity, liking (i.e., enjoyment), and ratings of perceived exertion (RPEs). Recreational runners (n 12 males, n 12 females) completed 3 experimental trials, each under a different social condition, in a randomized order. Each trial consisted of self-paced running for a duration voluntarily determined by the participant. The 3 social conditions were running alone, with a sex-and fitness-matched familiar peer, or with a sex-and fitness-matched unfamiliar peer. A wrist-worn global positioning system was used to record running duration, distance, and average speed. Liking and RPE were assessed at the end of each trial. Mixed model regression analysis showed no significant effects of social condition (p ≥ 0.40) for any of the dependent variables. The presence of a familiar or unfamiliar peer did not alter recreational runners' running behavior, liking, or perceived exertion during submaximal exercise. However, exercising with others may have other benefits (e.g., reduced attrition) not examined herein.
Article
Vigorous exercise feels unpleasant and negative emotions may discourage adherence to regular exercise. We quantified the subjective affective responses to short term high-intensity interval training (HIT) in comparison with moderate-intensity continuous training (MIT). Twenty-six healthy middle-aged (mean age 47±5 years, mean VO2peak: 34.2±4.1 ml/kg/min) sedentary men were randomized into HIT (n = 13, 4-6 x 30 s of all-out cycling efforts at ∼ 180 % of peak workload with 4 min recovery) and MIT (n = 13, 40-60 min continuous cycling at 60 % of peak workload) groups, performing six sessions within two weeks. Perceived exertion, stress and affective state were recorded before, during and after each session. Perceived exertion and arousal were higher and affective state more negative during the HIT than MIT sessions (p<0.001). HIT versus MIT exercise acutely increased the experience of stress, tension and irritation and decreased positive affect (p<0.05). Additionally, satisfaction was lower and pain and negative affect were higher in the HIT than in the MIT group (p<0.05). However, perceived exertion and displeasure experienced during exercise alleviated similarly in response to HIT and MIT over the six days of training. Peak oxygen consumption increased (p<0.001) after intervention (HIT 34.7±3.9 vs. 36.7±4.5; MIT 33.9±4.6 vs. 35.0±4.6) and not differently between HIT and MIT (p=0.28 for group*training). Short-term HIT and MIT are equally effective in improving aerobic fitness, but HIT increases experience of negative emotions and exertion in sedentary, middle-aged men. This may limit the adherence to this time-effective training mode, even though displeasure lessen over time and suggests similar mental adaptations to both MIT and HIT.
Article
Maintains that operational measures of cohesion based on attraction underrepresent the concept because goals and objectives relating to performance are also important in the study of cohesion. Measures based on attraction fail to explain cohesion in situations characterized by negative affect. Better ways to study cohesion might include the direct observation of group behavior (e.g., the number of passes exchanged between teammates as related to friendship choices) or the use of statistics of change and commonality. The linear structure of a conceptual model of cohesion is discussed with regard to factors that are environmental, personal, leadership-based, and team-based. It is noted that cohesion has been found to influence productivity, conformity, individual satisfaction, behavior change, role clarity among group members, and group stability. (60 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
A solution is suggested for an old unresolved social psychological problem.